You are on page 1of 13

Children's Responses to Gender-Role

Stereotyped Advertisements

AYSEN BAKIR Research indicates that adult's gender-role perceptions can influence their responses
Illinois State University toward different types of advertising appeals; however, it is not known whether the
abaKir2@ilstu.edu
same is true for young children. Given that children's gender-role stereotypes vary
JEFFREY G. BLODGETT across both age and gender, it is possible that their responses toward different types
North Carolina A&T
of advertisements might also vary. Accordingly, this study examines whether
State University
jgblodge@ncat.edu préadolescent boys and girls differ in their attitudes toward advertisements that
incorporate agentic or communal gender-role attributes. The results are managerially
GREGORY M. ROSE
University of relevant. The findings suggest that marketers can target young boys and giris with a
Washington. Tacoma common set of advertisements. However, advertisements should convey agentic
rosegm@u.washi ngton.edu
themes when targeted toward older préadolescent girls, and communal themes when
targeted toward younger préadolescent girls. Considering that children represent a
large and growing consumer segment this issue deserves greater attention.

YOUNG CHILDREN CONSTITUTE a substantial mar- expansion in the number and type of children's
ket segment. It has been estimated that children media outlets. Companies can now advertise their
account for more than $30 billion in direct pur- products via numerous children's television net-
chases (Schor, 2004) and indirectly influence more works (e.g.. Nickelodeon, Disney Channel, Car-
than $600 billion of U.S. household spending— toon Network, National Geographic Kids, FOX
from snack foods to automobiles (Piperato, 2005). Kids) and a plethora of traditional and online
Due to the increasing involvement of children in magazines for kids of different ages (e.g.. Sports
family decision making, spending on children's Illustrated for Kids, Highlights For Kids, American
advertising has risen to over $15 billion a year Girl, KidsWorld, Ranger Rick, U.S. Kids, Dogs for
(Piperato, 2005). Indeed, a myriad of companies— Kids, etc.). Marketers have certainly been taking
such as Toys "R" Us, Kraft, and Kellogg's—have advantage of these children's media; for example.
been Investing significant sums of money on ad- Nickelodeon forecasted advertising revenues of
vertisements and promotions targeting children approximately $935 million in 2005 (Klaassen, 2005).
(Thompson, 2005). LA Gear, for example, was These facts and figures clearly indicate that chil-
allocating approximately 95 percent of its adver- dren's advertising is a topic worthy of additional
tising budget to children's media, and Burger research.
King has been spending over $100 million per Although researchers have examined a multi-
year on a series of promotions tied to new movies tude of issues related to advertising, most of
(e.g.. Beauty and the Beast, Aladdin, Toy Story, The these studies have been conducted with adult
Hunchback of Notre Dame, Lord of the Rings, Star subjects. Consequently, our understanding of
Wars) that are aimed primarily at children (Thomp- how young boys and girls react to different types
son, 2003). The tremendous growth in the chil- of advertisements is limited, ln particular, little
dren's market has been accompanied by an is known about the underlying factors that

DOt; 10.2501/S002184990808029X June 2 0 0 8 JOURHilL OF HDUEHTISIIIG RESEHHCH 2 5 5


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

It has been estimated that children account for more These stereotypes focus on and contrast
the personality traits, predispositions, so-
than $30 billion in direct purchases and indirectly influence cial orientations, and behavioral tenden-
cies of the two sexes. Bakan (1966) and
more than $600 billion of U.S. household spending—from Parsons and Bales (1955) describe male
stereotypes as having an "agentic" or "in-
snack foods to automobiles. strumental" orientation (a primary con-
cern for the self and a cognitive focus on
problem solving), and associate female
stereotypes with a "communal" or "ex-
determine the effectiveness of different sent a particular type of person or thing" pressive" orientation (a focus on relation-
types of advertising appeals. One vari- (Collins, 1994). Stereotypes embody (some- ships and group harmony, and a concern
able that has received considerable atten- times accurately, sometimes inaccurately) for others). Although many of the agentic/
tion in the developmental psychology one group's perceptions of the personal- instrumental and communal/expressive
literature, but has largely been over- ity traits, predispositions, attitudes, and gender-role attributes are positive, others
looked in marketing, is children's gender- behaviors of another group of people. describe the focal gender in a more neg-
role perceptions (or stereotypes). Prior Although stereotypes oftentimes reflect a ative manner. For males, those attributes
research indicates that adults' gender- negative bias, their use is common in that have a more positive connotation in-
role stereotypes influence their responses popular culture. Over the years, market- clude independence, leadership, decisive-
to advertising and purchase behavior ers have relied on different stereotypes ness, active, strong, and skillful; whereas
(Gilly, 1988; Jaffe and Berger, 1994); how- and the perceptions, connotations, and the more negative aspects include being
ever, little is known about the relation- meanings conveyed in order to target their aggressive, dominant, stubborn, and self-
ship between children's gender-role products to various consumer segments. centered (Meyers-Levy, 1988). For fe-
stereotypes and their reactions to differ- Common stereotypes revolve around the males, those attributes that have a positive
ent types of advertising appeals. Al- differences between older versus youn- connotation include empathy, nurturance,
though a few studies have documented ger people, urban dwellers versus rural social grace, and a concern for others;
the use of stereotypes in children's ad- folks, and jocks versus geeks. Another whereas attributes with a more negative
vertising (Hoek and Laurence, 1993; Mack- pervasive stereotype revolves around the connotation include weakness, indecisive-
lin and Kolbe, 1984) and have examined various roles and traits of men versus ness, dependence, and emotional.
the effects of gender on children's evalu- women. In general, females have been
ation of an advertisement (Kolbe and described as weak, nurturing, depen-
Influence of stereotypes across age
Muehling, 1995), our knowledge is incom- dent, indecisive, and emotional; whereas
groups and gender
plete. Given the substantial sums of money males have been characterized as strong,
Gender-role stereotypes are oftentimes used
being spent by marketers, it would be independent, competent, and stubborn
by advertisers when targeting adult con-
beneficial to know whether young boys' (Meyers-Levy, 1988). Although it is widely
sumers. By early adulthood most individ-
and girls' gender-role stereotypes influ- recognized that these stereotypes are not
uals' perceptions of gender-appropriate
ence their attitudes toward different types altogether accurate, they have been per-
behavior have reached a steady state. Given
of advertisements. The resulting knowl- petuated throughout the popular culture—
that adults' gender-role perceptions re-
edge could aid in developing more ef- e.g., in movies, television, and commercials
main fairly constant over time, advertisers
fective advertisements when targeting (Lovdal, 1989)—and in general have been
can target broad age segments with a com-
younger children. accepted to be indicative of gender-
mon set of stereotyped advertisements.
typical behavior.
Unlike adults, research indicates that chil-
Stereotypes dren's perceptions of gender-appropriate
The use of stereotypes throughout soci- Components of gender-role stereotypes behavior vary by age. During the develop-
ety is pervasive. A stereotype is defined Gender-role stereotypes highlight and of- mental process children typically go through
as a "fixed general belief, characteristic, tentimes exaggerate common attributes and certain stages in their understanding and
etc. that a lot of people believe to repre- differences among men versus women. application of gender-role stereotypes,

256 flESEññCH June 2 0 0 8


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

being more rigid (as to what is perceived Prior research indicates that aduits' gender-roie
as appropriate) during the preschool years
and becomijig more flexible during mid- stereotypes influence their responses to advertising and
dle childhood (Trautner, 1992). These de-
velopmental changes could have important purchase behavior; however, iittie is known about the
implications for children's advertising. For
example, the type of appeal that might be relationship between children's gender-role stereotypes
more effective when targeting a younger
age group (e.g., children ages 5-6) might and their reactions to different types of advertising
differ from the appeals that are more ef-
fective when targeting older children (e.g., appeals.
ages 8-10).
Research also indicates that children's
gender-role stereotypes vary by gender.
Due to developmental differences, boys
and girls—of the same age group—hold children go through various stages in their appropriate behavior (Bussey and Bandura,
somewhat different views of gender- cognitive development, e.g., from infancy, 1984). Interestingly, previous findings in-
appropriate behavior. In general, girls have through preschool and kindergarten, and dicate that parents put more pressure on
been shown to hold more flexible views on through adolescence (e.g., Piaget, 1954). boys in regard to gender-appropriate be-
regarding gender-roles. Compared to boys, This research indicates that children's un- havior, and thus boys have been found to
younger females are more accepting of derstanding of gender is fluid and devel- be more likely than girls to imitate same-
both genders' participation in household ops gradually over time (Weinraub et al., gender models and to reject behaviors
tasks (Hageman and Gladding, 1983). Ac- 1984). As children mature they progress and objects associated with the other gen-
cordingly, the type of appeals that might from a reliance on physical features to a der (Bussey and Perry, 1982).
be effective in targeting girls might not be more complex understanding of their en-
as effective when targeting boys. vironment. Over time, children develop The effects of age and gender on
schémas regarding gender-appropriate be- stereotyping
Purpose havior (Martin, Wood, and Little, 1990). Previous research indicates that children's
The purpose of this research is to examine These schémas progress from the simple awareness of gender stereotypes begins
whether the age and gender of préadoles- (e.g., soft things are feminine and rough as early as two and a half years of age
cent children influence their attitudes to- things are masculine; girls play with dolls (Cowan and Hoffman, 1986). Indeed, Ur-
ward advertisements that incorporate whereas boys play with trucks) to more berg (1982) found that children between
stereotypical male (agentic) or female (com- complex concepts of masculinity and fem- the ages of two to four differentiate be-
munal) attributes. Given that previous re- ininity (Martin, Wood, and Little, 1990). tween males and females on the basis of
•.f.irch has shown that attitude toward the During children's formative years par- gender-stereotyped behaviors (e.g., boys
odvertisement is a precursor of one's at- ents and other socialization agents present are cruel and hit people; girls are fearful
titude toward the brand (MacKenzie and a framework for "gender appropriate" be- and cannot fix things). Similarly, Huston
Lutz, 1989), it is logical to examine those havior, modeling and reinforcing the de- (1983) found that preschool age children
variables that might influence children's sired behaviors. Researchers generally associate boys with attributes such as
responses to various types of advertising agree that children begin to be aware of strong, fast, aggressive, and cruel; whereas
appeals. This knowledge can help market- gender as they approach one year of age girls are described with attributes such as
ers develop more effective advertise- (Ruble and Martin, 1998). Gradually, chil- helplessness, cries a lot, nice, and affec-
ments, in a more efficient manner. dren begin to develop abstract models of tionate. During this period, their prefer-
gender appropriate behavior. As children ences are oriented toward same-gender
THEORETICAL FOUNDATION mature they are more likely to imitate playmates and activities as they gain more
A rich body of literature in the develop- same-sex models (e.g., mother, father, older detailed knowledge about the differences
mental psychology literature indicates that sister, or brother) in order to ensure gender- between masculinity and femininity.

June 2 0 0 8 JOÜRllflL OF flDyERTISlliG 257


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

Differences across age. Previous re- Previous studies have found that boys hold more
search indicates that children's gender-
related stereotypes vary by age. Early on, traditional gender-role beliefs and have stronger
as children become avv'are of gender-role
differences, their preference for same-
preferences for same-gender activities compared to
gender activities and objects increases
(Edelbrock and Sugawara, 1978; Fagot,
girls.
1985), and they become somewhat rigid
in their views. However, at later ages
they become more flexible and acknowl-
edge a wider range of gender-appropriate tween their preschool years and early ad- HIB: Préadolescent girls will report
activities and occupations. In general, re- olescence, and that young boy's and girl's a more favorable attitude for
searchers have found that children be- perceptions of gender-appropriate behav- an advertisement embedded
tween the ages of four and eight embrace ior vary. Accordingly, it is reasonable to with communal attributes, as
gender stereotypes more strongly {Katz hypothesize that children's advertise- compared to an advertisement
and Boswell, 1986; Trautner, 1992); and ments that incorporate various gender- that is embedded with agentic
that between the ages of eight and ten role stereotypes might be perceived attributes.
children seem to recognize that a wider differently by young boys as compared
to young girls, and might be viewed dif- HlC: Préadolescent boys will report
range of behaviors, occupations, objects,
ferently by préadolescent children of dif- a more favorable attitude for
and characteristics are acceptable for their
ferent ages. an advertisement embedded
gender.
with agentic attributes than will
HYPOTHESES préadolescent girls.
Differences across gender. Previous stud-
This study examines whether young chil-
ies have found that boys hold more tra- HID: Préadolescent girls will have a
dren's attitudes toward gender-stereotyped
ditional gender-role beliefs and have more favorable attitude for an
advertisements vary by age and gender.
stronger preferences for same-gender ac- advertisement embedded with
As previously discussed, gender-role ste-
tivities as compared to girls (Bussey and communal attributes than will
reotypes have been described using an
Perry, 1982; Martin and Little, 1990). Al- préadolescent boys.
agentic/instrumental versus communal/
though young boys and girls alike ex-
expressive orientation (Bakan, 1966; Par- Furthermore, given that studies focus-
pand their views of gender-appropriate
sons and Bales, 1955). Previous research ing on age (Trautner, 1992) indicate that
behavior between preschool and middle
regarding gender differences (e.g., Etaugh children become more accepting of a wider
school, girls typically become more flexi-
and Liss, 1992; Huston, 1983; Ruble and range of gender-typed behaviors as they
ble in their views (Plumb and Cowan,
Martin, 1998) suggests that young boys, progress from their kindergarten to middle-
1984). Interestingly, Etaugh and Liss (1992)
in general, will perceive agentic advertise- school years, it is hypothesized that:
found that girls' interest in female sex-
ments more favorably than communal ad-
typed activities decreased during this same
vertisements, and that young girls will H2A: Préadolescent boys who are in
time period, although boys' interest in
perceive communal advertisements more a younger age group will ex-
male sex-typed activities did not change.
favorably than agentic advertisements. Ac- hibit a more favorable attitude
cordingly, it is hypothesized that: for an advertisement embed-
Gender stereotypes and children's ded with agentic attributes than
advertising HIA: Préadolescent boys will have will préadolescent boys who are
Very little is known as to how children's a more favorable attitude for in an older age group.
gender-role perceptions influence their re- an advertisement embedded
sponses toward different types of adver- with agentic attributes, as com- H2B: Préadolescent boys who are in
tising appeals. It is well established that pared to an advertisement that an older age group will show a
children go through different stages of is embedded with communal more favorable attitude for an
cognitive and social development be- attributes. advertisement embedded with

2 5 8 JDUHHHL DF HDDEHTISID6 HCSEHRCK June 2 0 0 8


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

communal attributes, as com- ical appearances or overt behavior. By tion, strength, and decisiveness; whereas
pared to préadolescent boys who ages 8-10, children begin to form impres- the communal storyboard was operation-
are in a younger age group. sions of others based on abstract or psy- alized to reflect nurturance, empathy, har-
chological terms. The sample chosen for mony, and need for affiliation. The first
H2C: Préadolescent girls who are in this study corresponds to these age groups, frame of both storyboards consisted of a
a younger age group will ex- thus strengthening the validity of the ex- picture of the focal product. For the agen-
hibit a more favorable attitude perimental design. tic storyboard, the second frame showed
for an advertisement embed- Data were collected at an elementary a single child (thus depicting indepen-
ded with communal attributes school located in the southern United dence) in a kitchen taking "crackers" out
than will préadolescent girls States. The principal and teachers pro- of a box. The third frame consisted of a
who are in an older age group. vided valuable cooperation. To partici- collage showing children engaged in dif-
pate, students had to have the written ferent activities—swimming in a pool, run-
H2D: Préadolescent girls who are in consent of their parents. In return for their ning, and playing soccer—in which the
an older age group will show a participation, each student was allowed goal was to win, thus depicting competi-
more favorable attitude for an to pick a prize (e.g., a toy or coloring tiveness and aggressiveness. The final
advertisement embedded with book); in addition, the elementary school frame depicted four children running, with
agentic attributes, as compared was provided with a new digital camera one child passing the finish line and win-
to préadolescent girls who are and color printer. The 280 participants ning the race. In contrast to the agentic
in a younger age group. were evenly mixed between boys and girls; storyboard, the communal storyboard de-
in addition, half were in kindergarten and picted children in noncompetitive group
METHODOLOGY half were in 3rd or 4th grade; 68 percent activities. In the second fnime four chil-
Subjects were Caucasian, 30 percent were African- dren were shown in the kitchen snacking
To test the hypotheses a sample of pré- American, and 2 percent were Asian or on crackers, thus reflecting a communal
adolescent boys and girls, of two distinct Hispanic. theme. The next frame consisted of a col-
age groups, was required. Accordingly, lage showing children swimming, run-
the younger préadolescent age group was Stimulus materials ning, and playing soccer with a group of
comprised of children in kindergarten (who To manipulate the independent variable friends—in which the focus is on friend-
typically are 5 to 6 years of age), whereas (agentic versus communal gender-role be- ship, having fun, and being together, thus
the older préadolescent group was com- haviors), advertising storyboards were used emphasizing nurturance and affiliation.
prised of children in the 3rd and 4th grades as stimulus materials. It Is commonly ac- Similarly, the final frame showed a group
(who typically are 9 to 10 years old). To cepted that the use of storyboards is valid of children sitting around in the yard talk-
establish a clear demarcation between age for this type of research (Belch and Belch, ing, eating crackers, and relaxing, thus
groups, children in the first and second 2004). One set of advertising storyboards reflecting group harmony.
grades (who typically are 7 to 8 years old) conveyed an agentic theme, while the other Accompanying both storyboards was a
were not included in the study. Accord- set conveyed a communal theme. The story- voice-over. The scripts were written to
ing to previous research, kindergarteners boards were drawn and colored by a free- reinforce the various dimensions being
should be more rigid as to what they lance artist, and a voice-over was used to depicted in the advertisements. In the agen-
consider to be gender-appropriate, whereas present the audio message. The two story- tic advertisement, the voice-over empha-
children who are in the 3rd and 4th grades boards contained both male and female sizes winning and independence; in the
should be more flexible in their view- characters. Based on interviews and pre- communal advertisement, the voice-over
points (Trautner, 1992), Furthermore, the tests conducted with graduate students and emphasizes affiliation, harmony, and em-
age groups chosen for this study are con- faculty in the behavioral sciences, and with pathy. To avoid any confounding effects,
sistent with seminal work by Selman (1980) young children, crackers were found to be both a male and a female voice-over were
and Barenboim (1981). They assert that a "gender neutral" product and thus were used. The gender of the voice-over was
during the preschool and kindergarten selected as the focal product. counterbalanced across the agentic and
years (ages 3-6) children describe other The agentic storyboard was operation- communal storyboards (which in turn were
people in concrete terms, based on phys- alized to reflect independence, competi- counterbalanced across subjects). The two

June 2008 l OF RESKII 259


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

advertising storyboards, along with the descriptors "great," "like," "fun," and "bor- two genders (male versus female), the
script, are shown in the Appendix. ing," and were measured with a 5-point two age groups (kindergarten versus 3rd
The storyboards and scripts were exten- "smiley face" scale. The scale was moder- and 4th grade), and the two gender-role
sively pretested. The first stage of pretest- ately reliable, with an alpha of 0.72. (For depictions (agentic versus communal) re-
ing was conducted with graduate students, parsimony, this hybrid measure will herein suited in eight cells. The final data set
who were asked to evaluate the extent to be referred to as attitude toward the consisted of 280 subjects, for a total of 35
which each frame of the storyboards de- advertisement.) subjects per cell. A summary of cell and
picted an agentic or communal orienta- group means for the dependent variable
tion. Based upon their feedback, the Procedure (attitude toward the advertisement) is
stimulus materials were modified. A sec- The préadolescent subjects participated in shown in Tables lA-lC.
ond stage of pretesting then took place, in the experiment one at a time, at pre-
which undergraduate students were asked arranged times during the school day. The Confound checks
to rate the extent to which the stimulus young boys and girls were ushered be- Before testing the hypotheses, confound
materials reflected the underlying agentic tween their classrooms and the research checks were conducted. Given that the
and communal dimensions. Additional station by one of the researchers. The re- design is completely crossed and there
modifications were made until it was clear searcher explained that the purpose of the are equal numbers of boys and girls in
that there was a significant difference be- "assignment" was to watch an advertise- each cell, there should be no significant
tween the two storyboards. ment for a product and to indicate how difference in attitude across each of the
much they liked it (i.e., using the smiley two levels of age, gender, and ad type.
Dependent variable face scale). The storyboards were pre- Indeed, the main effects of gender (f 1,272 =
The "attitude toward the advertisement" sented on a laptop computer screen, one 0.190, p < 0.663), ad type (f,,372 = 1.344,
construct is well established in the mar- frame at a time, accompanied by a voice- p £ 0.247), and age (fi,272 = 0.097, p <
keting literature and has been shown to over. The advertisements were evenly bal- 0,756) were not significant. In other words,
he a precursor of "attitude toward the anced across each of the four groups; boys' attitudes (aggregated across both
brand" (MacKenzie and Lutz, 1989). How- one-half of the kindergarten boys and one- ad types) did not significantly differ from
ever, the majority of this research has uti- half of the 3rd/4th grade boys viewed the girls' attitudes. Likewise, the attitudes of
lized aduit subjects, and hence it is not agentic advertisement, whereas the others kindergarten children (across both ad
clear whether préadolescent children are were exposed to the communal advertise- types) did not differ from that of 3rd and
able to discern between these two con- ment. Likewise, one-half of the 3rd/4th 4th graders; and children's attitude (across
structs (see Ward, Wackman, and War- grade girls and one-half of the kindergar- both ages and genders) toward the agen-
tella, 1977). This concern was particularly ten girls watched the communal adver- tic advertisement did not differ from their
relevant in this study, given that chil- tisement, while the others viewed the attitude toward the communal advertise-
dren's only exposure to the advertise- agentic advertisement. After viewing the ment. These findings provide evidence of
ment and to the brand would be one and advertisement children were asked to re- discriminant validity (Perdue and Sum-
the same. The dependent variable was spond to the attitudinal items. The re- mers, 1986); hence, any effects of the in-
therefore operationalized as a hybrid of searcher read each question aloud and dependent variables on the dependent
children's attitude toward the advertise- asked children to point to the smiley face measure can be interpreted in an unambig-
ment, and their attitude toward the brand. that best represented their response. Chil- uous manner.
To ensure that scale items were appropri- dren seemed to enjoy the experiment and
ate for the age groups being sampled, had little difficulty in responding to the Hypothesis testing
teachers at the elementary school helped questions. The first set of hypotheses focuses on
the researchers modify items previously the effects of gender on préadolescent
used by MacKenzie and Lutz (1989) and RESULTS children's attitudes toward agentic and
Mitchell and Olson (1981), These items A between-subjects ANOVA design was communal advertisements. The results
were then pretested with several préado- used to assess the effects of age, gender, revealed no significant interaction across
lescent children and were further modi- and ad type on children's attitude toward gender and ad type (Fi 272 = 2.59,
fied. The final set of items used the the advertisement. The combination of the p < 0.109). Contrary to HIA and HIB,

2 6 0 JDumifiL or HDUEmisino RESEHRCH June 200s


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

TABLE 1 toward the agentic advertisement did not


Cell Means—Attitude toward the Advertisement (Full Sample, differ (f^ = -i.464,p<o.i48).
N = 280) DISCUSSION

A. Age X Ad Type "^^^ ^*"^y °^ gender-role perceptions


N - 280 70 Der Cell within the context of children's advertis-
ing addresses an important issue. A large
Agentic Communal • . r . _. ,
body or research m developmental psy-
3rd and 4th 4.28 4.06 4.17 chology indicates that gender-role stereo-
. , _ , . A Ar^ A A-j A AA types vary among children of different
Kindergarten 4.12 4.17 4.14 yy y &
ages and genders; i.e., that young boys
4 20 411 416
; .•. '• and giris have differing perceptions as to
B. Age X Gender what constitutes gender appropriate be-
N = 280, 70 per Cell havior, and that these views are not sta-
gpug Qlj|g ble, but change over time. Accordingly,
• • this study examined the effects of age and
3rd and 4th 4.23 4.10 4.17 gender on préadolescent children's atti-
Kindergarten 4.11 4.18 4.15 *"'^'-" toward agentic and communal type
advertisements. This study is not only of
4.17 4.14 4.16 , . . . . . . . . .
theoretical value, it is managerially rele-
C. Gender x Ad Type va^t as ^etl. Children constitute a large
fy = 280, 70 per Cell market segment, with considerable pur-
Agentic Communal chasing power and influence, and thus
are of interest to advertisers. A better un-
4.16 4.19 4.18 derstanding of those factors that influ-
4.24 4.04 4.14 ence children's responses to different types
^,. ..-, of advertising appeals can help marketers
develop more effective advertisements,
which in turn can have a substantial im-
pact on revenues and profits.
With regards to gender, the results in-
preadolescent boys did not report a more age and ad type was significant (f 1,272 = dicate that préadolescent boys do not nec-
favorable attitude for the agentic adver- 3.83, p < 0.05). However, contrary to H2A essarily respond more favorably to
tisement (as compared to the communal and H2B, there was no difference in the at- advertisements that are embedded with
advertisement), and préadolescent girls titudes of boys in the younger age agentic attributes {i.e., as compared to
did not exhibit a more positive attitude group—as compared to boys in the older communal attributes); and that preado-
toward the communal advertisement (vis- age group—^toward either the agentic or the lescent girls do not necessarily respond
à-vis the agentic advertisement). Further- communal advertisement (f],i36 = 0.530, p s more favorabiy to advertisements that are
more, there were no differences between 0.66). There were differences, though, in the embedded with communal attributes (i.e.,
the attitudes of young boys and young attitudesof kindergarten vis-à-vis 3rd/4th as compared to agentic attributes). Al-
girls toward either of the two ad types; grade girls (Fi,i36 = 4.55, p s 0.004) (see though these "null" findings at first glance
thus HlC and HID were also rejected Tables 2A and 2B for cell means). As hy- might not seem noteworthy, they are in-
(see Tabie IC for cell means). pothesized (H2C), the younger girls had a deed manageriaily significant. The re-
The second set of hypotheses pertains more favorable attitude toward the com- suits suggest that advertisers do not need
to the effects of age on préadolescent munal advertisement than did the older pre- to develop two different sets of advertise-
children's attitudes toward gender stereo- adolescent girls (ff,« = 2.88, p < 0.005). ments when marketing a product to both
typed advertisements. The interaction of However, contrary to H2D, their attitudes boys and girls. Instead, advertisers can

June 2 0 0 8 JDURIIHL OF HDUiHTISlOG 261


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

TABLE 2 tl^^t advertisers who are targeting preado-


Cell Means for Boys and Girls—Attitude toward the ' ^ ' " ^ " * ^ " ^ ^ ° ^ ' ^*^^ ^^^ s^oup can reiy
, , ^. , upon a common gender-typed theme. A
Advertisement , . ^ ^ . ,
note of caution, though; given that young
A. Gender = Boys *^^y^ progress through the various devel-
Age X Ad Type opmental stages more slowly than young
n = 140, 35 per Cell S^^'^s (Etaugh and Liss, 1992), it could be
that any corresponding attitudinal changes
Agentic Communal , ,
had not yet manifested. Hence, boys who
3rd and 4th 4.21 4.26 4.24 are in the 5th or 6th grades might re-
Kindergarten 4.10 4.12 4.11 ' P ° " ^ differentiy than younger boys.
These findings, while preliminary,
416 419 418
• should be of interest to marketers. Given
B. Gender = Girls that the average cost of producing a 30-
Age X Ad Type second national TV spot is approxi-
n = 140; 35 per Cell mately $350,000 (Duncan, 2005), there are
Agentic Communal significant financial implications. Further-
more, considering that the creative idea
3rd.and4th 4.35 3.85 4.10 process (from the exploration stage, to
Kindergarten 4.13 4.23 4.18 insight, execution, and on through the
evaluation and copytestine stages) can
4.24 4.04 4.14 ^^ & B /
take considerable time, it is more effi-
cient to be able to create advertisements
that can appeal to both boys and girls
appeal to préadolescent boys and girls vertisement, the older girls responded less and/or to children of a broad range of
with a single underlying theme, built upon favorably toward the communal advertise- ages,
either agentic or communal attributes. ment (i.e., as compared to the younger
Limitations and concerns
Even though young boys and girls might girls). This latter finding indicates that as
In almost all research trade-offs are made
hold somewhat different views regarding préadolescent girls become older they be-
that affect different aspects of internal or
gender-appropriate behavior, it appears gin to reject traditional female stereo-
external validity (Cook and Campbell,
that these stereotypes are more "contex- types. Considering that the 3rd/4th grade
1979); that is certainly true with this study.
tua!" or "superficial" in nature and do girls actually exhibited a more favorable
This study is limited in that only one
not extend beyond a "surface" level to attitude toward the agentic advertisement
experiment was conducted, and hence
the underlying gender-typed attributes. vis-à-vis the communal advertisement (see
the full extent to which gender-role per-
Tlius, advertisements that portray boys Table 2B), it might be beneticial for adver-
ceptions influence children's reactions
as empathetic or nurturing can often- tisers to vary their use of agentic and
to advertisements that incorporate agen-
times be just as appropriate as advertise- communal themes when targeting girls of
tic or communal attributes cannot be
ments that portray them as rambunctious different ages. Marketers might be wise to
fully assessed. It should be noted, how-
and independent; and advertisements that target older préadolescent girls with ad-
ever, that one difficulty in conducting re-
portray girls as competitive and decisive vertisements that portray females in non-
search among préadolescent subjects is
can be just as effective as advertisements traditional roles and thus capitalize upon
gaining access to large numbers of chil-
that portray them as caring and relation- their changing perceptions of gender-
dren. Without the approval and support
ship oriented. appropriate behavior. At the same time.
of local school principals and teachers, or
With regard to age, this study uncov- this study found no differences between
other large civic organizations, the logis-
ered mixed results. Although kindergar- kindergarten and 3rd/4th grade boys' at-
tics of collecting experimental data from
ten and 3rd/4th grade girls displayed titudes toward either the agentic or the
200-500 children is almost impossible.
similar attitudes toward the agentic ad- communal advertisement. It thus appears

2 6 2 JOÜflílfIL OP flDöERTISIIlG RESEflRCfl June 2 0 0 8


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

The results indicate tiiat preadoiescent boys do not attitudes toward different types of ad-
vertising appeals remains to be deter-
necessarily respond more favorably to advertisements mined. Considering that young children
influence approximately $600 billion of
that are embedded with agentic attributes (compared to purchases annually in North America
(Piperato, 2005), this is an important is-
communal attributes), and that préadolescent girls do sue. It is hoped that this study will spark
additional research into this topic. Rele-
not necessarily respond more favorably to advertisements vant issues include the following: (1) Can
children distinguish between gender (a
physical and concrete feature) and the
that are embedded with communal attributes (compared
more subtle (and hence abstract) ele-
ments of gender roles? (2) Does the in-
to agentic attributes).
fluence of gender roles manifest itself and
evolve at different ages for young boys
and girls? (3) Are certain elements of
Nonetheless, to better understand the in- over that reinforced the underlying agen- agentic and communal gender roles more
fluence of gender-role perceptions in the tic and communal attributes, stronger ma- germane and influential than others? An-
context of children's advertising, addi- nipulations might be needed to determine swers to these questions could help mar-
tional studies are needed. whether gender-role perceptions signifi- keters develop more effective advertising
Another limitation of this study is that cantly affect children's attitudes toward strategies.
the manipulations might not have been gender-typed advertisements.
strong enough to induce changes in the Demand artifacts also pose a threat to SUMMARY
dependent variable. To ensure a high de- validity (Cook and Campbell, 1979). Chil- The concept of gender-role perceptions is
gree of internal validity, various aspects dren enjoyed the experiment; viewing the a relevant issue for advertisers. Many
of the two sets of storyboards were kept advertisements on the laptop computer products are designed speciflcally for
as similar as possible. In this study, for was novel, and they knew that they would young boys or girls, whereas others are
example, it was important to distinguish be able to pick a prize when finished. It targeted to children of either gender. Sim-
between the concept of gender roles and could be that the nature of the experiment ilarly, some products are designed with
gender per se; by controlling for gender created a "halo" effect, which resulted in young children of a very specific age
any findings could more confidently be children viewing all advertisements favor- group in mind, whereas others are devel-
attributed to gender-role perceptions. Ac- ably. It is also possible that the youngsters oped to appeal to children of a wide
cordingly, the two sets of storyboards were wanted to please the experimenter, and range of ages. In the case of products
drawn so that males and females were hence responded as "good subjects" (Cook that are designed for both boys and
equally represented. In retrospect, the use and Campbell, 1979). It is difficult to as- girls—of various ages—it is certainly more
of both genders in the storyboards might cost-effective to develop a single adver-
sess the extent to which demand artifacts
have diluted the effects of the more sub- tisement that has broad appeal. Although
influenced children's responses; nonethe-
tle, underlying gender-role attributes em-
less, future research should be designed research in developmental psychology
bedded in the advertisements. Young
in such a marmer as to better rule out this indicates that boys and girls of different
children might pay more attention to su-
type of threat. ages do indeed have different percep-
perficial gender attributes such as the ap-
tions of gender-appropriate behavior, the
pearance of the characters than the more
Future research extent to which these stereotypes influ-
abstract attributes and behaviors (e.g., in-
The study of gender-role perceptions in ence their attitudes toward gender-typed
dependence and competitiveness versus
the context of children's advertising is advertisements has not been known. This
nurturance and empathy) that were ma-
in its infancy and is ripe for additional study indicates that marketers do not need
nipulated in this study. Even though the
research. Whether children's gender-role to create one set of advertisements for
storyboards were accompanied by a voice-
stereotypes substantially influence their préadolescent boys and another set for

June 2 0 0 8 JDUfinill OF RDUERTISinG RESEHRCH 2 6 3


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

préadolescent girls of the same age, when of Retailing, the Journal of Advertising, the Journal of EDELBROCK, C, and A, I. SUGAWARA. "Acquisi-

advertising products aimed at both gen- Consumer Psychology, the Journal of Marketing, and tion of Sex-Typed Preferences in Preschool-
ders. On the other hand, it might be other journals and proceedings. Aged Children." Developmental Psychology 14, 6
prudent to create different advertise- (1978); 614-23.
ments when targeting préadolescent girls
of various ages. Advertisements target- REFERENCES ETAUGH, C , and M. B. Liss. "Home, School,
ing older préadolescent girls should and Playroom: Training Grounds for Adult Sex
embed fewer communal attributes and BAKAN, D . The Duality of Human Existence. Chi- Roles." Sex Roles 26, 3/4 (1992): 129-Í7.
more agentic attributes. In this case, the cago: Rand McNally, 1966.
additional cost of creating two different FAGOT, B. 1. "Beyond the Reinforcement Prin-
sets of advertisements might well result ciple: Another Step toward Understanding Sex-
BARENBOIM, CARL. "The Development of Per-
in enhanced revenues and profits. In Role Development." Developmental Psychology
son Perception in Childhood and Adolescence:
conclusion, these authors agree with Ty- 21, 6 (1985): 1097-104.
From Behavioral Comparisons to Psychologi-
bout (1995) that the theory is a "work in
cal Constructs to Psychological Comparisons."
progress" and suggest that marketing
Child Deudopment 52, 2 (1981): 129-44. GiLLY, M. C. "Sex Roles in Advertising: A Com-
academicians strive to extend our under-
parison of Television Advertisements in Aus-
standing of children's gender-role percep-
tralia, Mexico, and the United States." journal
tions and its influence on advertising BELCH, GEORGE E., and MICHAEL A. BELCH.
of Marketing 52, 2 (1988): 75-85.
effectiveness, Advertising and Promotion: An Integrated Market-
ing Caiumunications Perspective. New York:
HAGEMAN, M . B., and S. T. GLADDING. "The
McGraw-Hill/Irwin, 2004.
AYSEN BAKIR (Ph.D., University of Mississippi) is an Art of Career Exploration: Occupational Sex-

assistant professor of marketing at Illinois State Uni- Role Stereotyping among Elementary School
BussEY, K., and A. BANDURA. "Influence of
versity. Her research interests include children's ad- Children." Elementary School Guidance and Couii-
Gender Constancy and Social Power on Sex-
vertising, gender roles, and cross-cultural consumer seiing 17, 4 (1983): 280-87.
Linked Modeling." Journal of Personality and
behavior. She has published articles in the Journal of
Social Psychology 47, 6 (1984): 1292-302.
Marketing Theory ana Practice, the Journal of Inter- HOEK, J., and K. LAURENCE. "Television Adver-

national Consumer Marketing, the Journal of Marketing tising to Children: An Analysis of Selected New
, and D. G. PERRY. "Same-Sex Imitation:
Education, and other journals and proceedtngs. Zealand Commercials." Marketing Bulletin 4, 1
The Avoidance of Cross-Sex Models or the Ac-
(1993): 19-29.
ceptance of Same-Sex Models." Sex Roles 8, 7
JEFFREY G . BLODGETT (Ph.D., Indiana University) Is an (1982): 773-84.
HUSTON, A. C. "Sex-Typing." In Handbook of
associate professor of marketing In the College of
Child Psychology: Socialization, Personality, and
Business and Economics at North Carolina A&T State COLLINS. Collins Cobuild English Language Dic-
Social Development, Vol. 4, E. M. Hetherington,
University. He has authored papers on customer satis- tionary. London: HarperCollins Publishers, 1994.
ed. New York: Wiley, 1983.
faction and complaining behavior, advertising, culture,

and ethics in journals such as the Journal of Retailing.


COOK, T. D . , and D. T. CAMPBELL. Quasi- JAFFE, L. J., and P. D. BERGER. "The Effect of
the Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, Psy-
Experimentation: Design & Analysis Issues for Field Modem Female Sex Role Portrayals on Adver-
chology & Marketing, the Journal of Business Re-
Settings. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Com- tising Effectiveness." ¡ournal of Advertising Re-
search, and the Journal of Services Research.
pany, 1979. search 34, 4 (1994): 32-42.

GREGORY M . ROSE (Ph.D., University of Oregon) is a COWAN, G . , and C. D. HOFFMAN. "Gender Ste- KATZ, P A . , and S. BOSWELL. "Flexibility and
professor of marketing at the University of Washing- reotyping in Young Children: Evidence to Sup- Traditionality in Children's Gender Roles." Ge-
ton, Tacoma. His research interests inciude consumer port a Concept-Learning Approach." Sex Roles netic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs
sociaiization and cross-cultural consumer behavior. He 14, 3 (1986): 211-24. 112, 1 (1986): 103-47.
nas published articies in the Journal of the Academy

of Marketing Science, the Journal of Consumer Re- DUNCAN, T. Principles of Advertising & IMC. KLAASSEN, A . "Kids' Upfronl Crawls Along;
search. the Journal of Business Research, the Journal New York: McGraw-Hill/Irwin, 2005. 6% Rise Seen." Advertising Age, May 2005.

2 6 4 JOyflflRL OFflOyERTISIflGflESEflflCH June 2 0 0 8


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

KoLBE, R. H., and D. MUEHLING. "Gender Roles MITCHELL, A. A., and J. C. OLSON. "Are Prod- SELMAN, R. L. Tlie Growth of Interpersonal Un-
and Children's Television Advertising." journal uct Attribute Beliefs the Only Mediator of Ad- derstanding: Developmental and Clinical Analyses.
of Current Issues and Research in Advertising 17, vertising Effects on Brand Attitude." journal of Academic Press: New York, 1980.
1 (1995); 49-64. Marketing Research 18, 3 (1981): 31&-32.

THOMPSON, S. "Kellogg to Spend $50 Million."


PARSONS, T., and R. BALES. Family, Socialization, Advertising Age, March 10, 2003.
LovDAL, LYNN T "Sex Role Messages in Tete-
and Interaction Process. Glencoe, IL: Free Press,
vision Commercials: An Update." Sex Roles 21,
1955. . "Cheerios Crushes Tony as Kellogg
11/12 (1989): 715-21.
Launch Flops." AdîKrtising Age, June 13, 2005.
PERDUE, B. C , and J. O. SUMMERS. "Checking
MACKENZIE, S. B., and R. ]. LUTZ. "An Empir-
the Success of Manipulations in Marketing Ex-
ical Examination of the Structural Antecedents TRAUTNER, H. M . "The Development of Sex-
periments." journal of Marketing Research 23, 4
of Attitude toward the Ad in an Advertising Typing in Children: A Longitudinal Study."
(1986): 317-26.
Pre-Testing Context." journal of Marketing 53, 2 German journal of Psychology 16, 3 (1992); 183-99.

(1989): 48-65.
PIAGET, J. Intelligence and Affectivity: Their Rela-
TYBOUT, A . "Presidential Address: The Value of
tionship During Child Development. Palo Alto,
Theory in Consumer Research." Advances in
MACKLIN, M . C , and R. H. KOLBE. "Sex Role CA: Annual Review, Inc, 1954.
Consumer Research 22 (1995): 1-8.
Stereotyping in Children's Advertising: Cur-
rent and Past Trends." joumal of Advertising 13, PIPERATO, S. "Kids as Commodities." Chrono-
URBERC, K. A . "The Development of the Con-
2 (1984): 34-42. gram, September 2005.
cepts of Masculinity and Femininity in Yoimg

PLUMB, P., and G. COWAN. "A Developmental


Children." Sex Roles 8, 6 (1982); 659-68.
MARTIN, C . L . , and JANE K. LITTLE. "The Rela-
Study of Destereotyping and Androgynous Ac-
tion of Gender Understanding to Children's
tivity Preferences of Tomboys, Nontomboys, WARD, S., D . B . WACKMAN, and E. WARTELLA.
Sex Typed Preferences and Gender Stereo-
and Males." Sex Roles 10, 9-10 (1984): 703-12. How Children Learn to Buy. Beverly Hills, CA:
types." Child Development 61, 5 (1990): 1427-39.
Sage, 1977.
RUBLE, D . N . , and C. L. MARTIN. "Gender De-
, C. H. WOOD, and J. K. LITTLE. "The
velopment." In Handbook of Child Psychology, WEINRAUB, M., L. P. CLEMENS, A. SOCKLOFF, T
Development of Gender Stereotype Compo-
Vol. 3, W. Damon and N. Eisenberg, ed. New ETHRIDGE, E . GRACELY, and B. MYERS. "The
nents." Child Deivlopment 61, 6 (1990): 1891-904.
York: Wiley, 1998. Development of Sex Role Stereotypes in the
Third Year: Relationships to Gender Labeling,
MEYERS-LEVY, J. "The Influence of Sex Roles on SCHOR, J. Bom to Buy: The Commercialized Child Gender Identity, Sex-typed Toy Preference, and
Judgement." journal of Consumer Research 14, 4 and the New Consumer Culture. New York: Scrib- Family Characteristics." Child Development 55, 4
(1988): 522-30. ner, 2004. (1984): 1493-503.

June 2 0 0 8 JDUÍIÍ1HL OF üDUtílTISlílG RESŒIICH 2 6 5


CHILDREN'S RESPONSES TO GENDER-ROLE STEREOTYPED ADVERTISEMENTS

APPENDIX
Advertising Storyboards and Scripts
Agentic Theme Storyboard Communal Theme Storyboard

Sunrise Crackers is good tasting and full of energy. Sunrise Crackers is tasty and fun to eat.

When you eat Sunrise Crackers you'll feel strong. Sunrise Crackers is a great snack to have with your friends.
When you and your friends eat the Sunrise Crackers you'll
feel friendly and happy.

You can go anywhere and do anything. You can do things You and your friends can do a iot of things together. You
without needing others to heip you. When you eat Sunrise can even give a Sunrise Cracker to a friend when they're
Crackers, you can be very active. You'ii be fast and strong. sad.

Sunrise Crackers is a great snack for you to achieve your Sharing a Sunrise Cracker together is a great way to make
goai. When you eat Sunrise Crackers, you'ii have the peopie feel happy. Sunrise Crackers is a great snack for
power to beat others in a race. Sunrise Crackers gives you you and your friends.
the power to win.

2 6 6 JOyflflflL OF ñOÜERTISIOG flESEflBCII June 2 0 0 8

You might also like