You are on page 1of 8

Professional Psychology: Research and Practice Copyright 2006 by the American Psychological Association

2006, Vol. 37, No. 1, 51–58 0735-7028/06/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0735-7028.37.1.51

Perceptions of the Validity and Utility of Criminal Profiling Among


Forensic Psychologists and Psychiatrists
Angela N. Torres, Marcus T. Boccaccini, and Holly A. Miller
Sam Houston State University

Criminal profiling is the process of using crime scene evidence to make inferences about potential
suspects, including personality characteristics and psychopathology. An exploratory Internet survey of
forensic psychologists and psychiatrists was conducted to examine their experiences with and opinions
about profiling and to determine whether referring to profiling as “criminal investigative analysis” had
any impact on these opinions. About 10% of the 161 survey respondents had profiling experience,
although more than 25% considered themselves knowledgeable about profiling. Fewer than 25% believed
that profiling was scientifically reliable or valid, and approximately 40% felt that criminal investigative
analysis was scientifically reliable or valid. Although the scientific aspects of profiling lacked support,
respondents viewed profiling as useful for law enforcement and supported profiling research.

Keywords: profiling, criminal investigative analysis, general acceptance, admissibility, forensic

The image of a criminal profiler sorting through crime scene by these practitioners were based largely on clinical judgment and
evidence and definitively identifying a guilty suspect is popular in prevailing theories of personality and psychopathology. For exam-
novels, television shows, and movies. However, this popular im- ple, many early profiles from mental health practitioners were
age is more fiction than fact, and the process and limits of real rooted heavily in psychoanalysis (Grubin, 1995). These profiles
profiling work are often misunderstood. Criminal profiling is the focused on the probable interpersonal functioning and psychopa-
process of using behavioral evidence left at a crime scene to make thology of the person responsible for committing the crime (Wil-
inferences about the offender, including inferences about person- son, Lincoln, & Kocsis, 1997). Psychiatrist William Langer devel-
ality characteristics and psychopathology. In its most basic form, oped one of the most famous early profiles— a profile of Adolf
profiling is simply the postdiction of behavior; an action has taken Hitler for the U.S. Office of Strategic Service during World War II
place that allows investigators to make inferences about the person (Pinizzotto & Finkel, 1990). Langer’s psychiatric approach led to
responsible (Davis & Follette, 2002). Despite popular images of a profile of Hitler’s personality and mental disorder as well as to
criminal profiling, the main goal of profiling in real investigations Langer’s prediction of suicide as Hitler’s response to defeat (Pin-
is to narrow the scope of a suspect pool rather than to identify a izzotto, 1984). The most lauded and cited example of early pro-
single guilty criminal (Douglas & Olshaker, 1995). filing comes from the work of another psychiatrist. In 1956, David
Brussel constructed a profile of New York’s “Mad Bomber” that
Background was accurate in many details (see Weinerman, 2004a).
Mental Health Practitioners and Criminal Profiling Although some criminal profiling work is done by mental health
professionals (e.g., psychologists and psychiatrists), most of it is
Psychiatrists and psychologists made significant contributions
done by trained law enforcement agents. It is difficult to get a clear
to the early development of criminal profiling. The profiles created
estimate of how many mental health professionals are involved in
profiling. The best data come from a survey of 152 police psy-
ANGELA N. TORRES is currently a student in the clinical psychology PhD chologists, who reported spending approximately 3% of their time
program at Sam Houston State University. Her areas of interest in research
profiling offenders (Bartol, 1996). However, Bartol did not report
include criminal profiling; sexual offenders; eating disorders; body image;
a standard deviation value for this finding, making it impossible to
female offenders; lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender issues; and advocacy.
MARCUS T. BOCCACCINI received his PhD in clinical psychology from The infer whether most psychologists spent 3% of their time conduct-
University of Alabama. He is an assistant professor in the Department of ing profiles or whether a few professionals spent a large proportion
Psychology at Sam Houston State University and conducts research in the of their time conducting profiles while others avoided this work
areas of witness preparation, expert testimony, and psychological assess- completely. Although Bartol’s published survey results cannot
ment practices. help researchers determine the number of respondents who did and
HOLLY A. MILLER received her PhD in clinical psychology from Florida State did not participate in profiling, Bartol did find that 70% of the
University. She is an associate professor in the College of Criminal Justice at police psychologists “seriously” questioned the validity of profil-
Sam Houston State University. She teaches, consults, and conducts research in
ing work (p. 79). This combination of findings from the Bartol
the areas of malingered psychopathology, assessment and treatment of offend-
ers, psychopathy, and law enforcement training, assessment, and evaluation. study suggests that many respondents were not actively involved
CORRESPONDENCE CONCERNING THIS ARTICLE should be addressed to Marcus in generating profiles. It is possible, but unlikely, that the same
T. Boccaccini, Psychology Department, Box 2447, Sam Houston State psychologists who expressed negative views about profiling were
University, Huntsville, TX 77341. E-mail: boccaccini@shsu.edu actually participating in a significant amount of profiling-related
51
52 TORRES, BOCCACCINI, AND MILLER

activities. The applicability of Bartol’s findings to other groups of examinations of the reliability of profiling are limited to a study of
mental health professionals is not clear. Larger groups of profes- six Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI) profilers’ agreement in
sionals have not been surveyed about their participation in profil- classifying crime scenes as organized, disorganized, mixed, or
ing work. unknown (Ressler & Burgess, 1985, as reported in Homant &
The pattern suggested by the Bartol (1996) survey—that is, that Kennedy, 1998). The identification of a crime scene as organized
a small number of psychologists participate in profiling work and or disorganized has important implications in some profiling ap-
the vast majority avoid it because of concerns about validity—fits proaches. Agreement among the agents in classifying crime scenes
well with our perception of the relationship between psychology in 64 cases was reported to be 74%. The researchers concluded that
and criminal profiling during the past 20 years. However, this the overall level of agreement found in the study indicated ade-
pattern may be changing. A recent issue of the American Psycho- quate reliability; however, there was notable variation in the ac-
logical Association’s (APA) monthly professional magazine, Mon- curacy of the agents. For instance, the classification accuracy of
itor on Psychology, highlighted the role of psychology and psy- one rater was only 51.7%.
chologists in criminal profiling (Weinerman, 2004a, 2004b). Several studies have examined the accuracy of profiles gener-
Several psychologists who are active in profiling work were in- ated by professional profilers. The general design used in these
terviewed, and, although the articles clearly acknowledged that studies has been to compare profiles generated by profilers with
many concerns about the scientific validity of profiling still exist, those generated by nonprofilers (e.g., students, psychologists, po-
the reader is left with the impression that interest in profiling work lice detectives). One important limitation of this research was that
by psychologists is on the rise. This trend is also evident in the most of these studies used fewer than five profilers because the
recent publication of profiling research by psychologists (e.g., researchers had difficulty finding profilers willing to participate in
Canter, Alison, Alison, & Wentink, 2004; Canter & Wentink, the research (see Kocsis, 2004; Pinnizzotto & Finkel, 1990). Nev-
2004; Kocsis, 2004; Salfati, 2003; Salfati & Canter, 1999) and ertheless, the existing studies in this area seem to converge in
coverage of profiling in recent editions of psychology–law text- finding that trained profilers write longer and more detailed reports
books (Bartol & Bartol, 2004; Constanzo, 2004; Wrightsman & that contain more information and predictions about the offender
Fulero, 2005), suggesting that professional interest and practice in (Kocsis, 2003b; Pinizzotto & Finkel, 1990).
this area also may be on the rise. Indeed, the use of psychological Existing accuracy studies also provide some support for profil-
profiles in criminal investigations has been described as “burgeon- ers making predictions that are more accurate than those of non-
ing” (Kocsis & Hayes, 2004, p. 149). profilers, although results are somewhat mixed and are based on a
small number of profilers and studies (see Kocsis, 2003a, for a
Profiling Research review). For example, Pinizzotto and Finkel (1990) examined the
profiles generated by four current or former FBI profilers, six
When reviewing profiling research, one must recognize that police detectives with profiling training, six police detectives with
multiple approaches to profiling exist, each with different under- no profiling training, six clinical psychologists with no profiling
lying theories, methods, and assumptions (see Homant & training, and six undergraduate students with no profiling training.
Kennedy, 1998, for a concise overview; see also Turvey, 2002). Each participant generated profiles for one murder case and one
Most profiling studies examine the scientific merit of aspects of arson case on the basis of actual police investigation materials
these specific approaches as opposed to the profiling process in from the two cases. Participants also answered 15 multiple-choice
general. These studies can be thought of as examinations of con- questions about the suspect, including age, race, occupation, the
struct validity in that they gauge support for the theories and relationship between the victim and the offender, and the likeli-
concepts underlying various components of profiling approaches. hood that the offender had committed similar crimes in the past.
For example, one focus of recent profiling research has been the Researchers knew the correct response to each question. In the
validity of the offender typologies underlying some profiling ap- profiling of the murder case, all the participants performed better
proaches. For instance, in several studies Canter and colleagues than chance, with the average number of correct responses for each
coded information from police files and found limited support for group ranging from 5.3 (35%) to 7.0 (47%); however, no signif-
typologies sometimes used to classify serial killers, such as the icant differences were observed between the groups of partici-
classification of crime scenes as organized or disorganized and use pants. Thus, the accuracy of the professional profilers was similar
of the Holmes and Holmes system that includes visionary, mission, to that of untrained psychologists and students. In the profiling of
hedonistic, and power or control killers (see Canter et al., 2004; the sexual assault case, the 10 profilers performed significantly
Canter & Wentink, 2004). These findings raise serious questions better than the other three groups of participants. Again, all groups
about the validity of approaches that rely on these types of dis- performed at an above-chance rate, but the average number of
tinctions for generating profiles. However, researchers also have correct responses for profilers was 10.0 out of 15.0 (67%), com-
used data from real cases to develop more empirically supported pared with correct response averages ranging from 5.5 (37%) to
typologies of murderers and rapists (Canter, Bennell, Alison, & 8.5 (57%) for the other three groups.
Reddy, 2003; Salfati, 2003; Salfati & Canter, 1999; see also Kocsis (2003a) summarized the results of three studies con-
Kocsis, Cooksey, & Irwin, 2002). Future research in this area ducted by his research team comparing the accuracy of profes-
clearly is needed to determine whether psychologists can use these sional profilers (N ⫽ 11, across studies), psychologists, self-
typologies to accurately classify offenders in future cases and to identified psychics, college students, and various groups of law
provide useful information for investigators. enforcement officers. In each study, participants reviewed crime
A second line of profiling research has focused on the reliability descriptions and case materials from actual solved cases (i.e.,
and accuracy (criterion validity) of professional profilers. Formal murder, arson) and completed multiple-choice questionnaires
CRIMINAL PROFILING 53

about the suspect. Across these studies, the profilers provided the Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals (1993) departs from
largest number of correct predictions about the actual perpetrators, Frye v. United States (1923) by asking judges to consider the
with accuracy rates ranging from 46% (Kocsis, Irwin, Hayes, & scientific merit of the theories, procedures, and research on which
Nunn, 2000) to 70% (Kocsis, 2004). However, Kocsis (2003b) potential experts will base their testimony. The Supreme Court
cautioned that a notable amount of variability was observed within noted in Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals that general
the profiler group, indicating that some profilers were much more acceptance is one criterion that can be used for evaluating scien-
accurate than others. It is unclear whether this variation was due to tific merit but also recommended that judges consider other
overall differences in the profilers’ skills or to specific aspects of sources of evidence, including whether the theory or technique on
the cases. which the testimony is based has been subjected to peer review,
whether that theory or technique can be falsified, and whether it
has a known and acceptable error rate. Under Daubert v. Merrell
Profiling Admissibility in Court Dow Pharmaceuticals, any theory or procedure upon which ex-
perts rely should be based on a falsifiable theory that has been
When profiles are created, both the prosecution and the defense tested, be subjected to peer review, be published, have a known
may attempt to have the profiler testify as an expert witness. error rate, and be generally accepted in the field (Goodman-
Profiling evidence has been admitted and denied admission into Delahunty, 1997).
court. Many professionals who engage in profiling work believe
that profiling testimony is more likely to be admitted into court
when it is called something other than profiling (Cooley & Turvey, Survey Results as Evidence for General Acceptance
2002; Risinger & Loop, 2002). For example, in United States v. Error rates of profiling methods, the falsifiability of profiling
Meeks (1992), testimony that was based on motivational analy- theories, and the peer review status of profiling theories and
sis—the name sometimes used by the Federal Bureau of Investi- methods, are, for the most part, objective criteria that can be
gation (FBI) for profiling—was admitted because it was found to identified at any point in time through the current scholarly liter-
be a generally accepted investigative technique that is based on ature. Although arguments concerning the general acceptance of
specialized knowledge (Cooley & Turvey, 2002). Profiling also profiling work can be informed by findings from existing empir-
has been referred to as behavioral evidence analysis, linkage ical research, general acceptance can be examined directly by
analysis, signature analysis, and investigative analysis. The FBI, surveying the opinions of those in the relevant profession or field.
which originally coined the term profiling, now uses criminal For example, researchers have used surveys to systematically
investigative analysis to refer to its profiling work (Turvey, 2002). examine general acceptance within the psychology field of the
Although motivational analysis testimony was allowed in validity of polygraph testing (Honts & Quick, 1995; Iacono &
United States v. Meeks (1992) because it was found to be a Lykken, 1997) and factors influencing the accuracy of eyewitness
generally accepted investigative tool, profiling testimony has been memory (Kassin, Ellsworth, & Smith, 1989; Kassin, Tubb, Hosch,
found inadmissible because it was not perceived as being generally & Memon, 2001).
accepted (see Grezlak, 1999). A defense attorney in one case in The purpose of the current study is to use a survey approach to
which a psychologist’s profiling testimony was barred from being explore perceptions of the validity and utility of criminal profiling
entered into evidence openly referred to profiling as “voodoo” among forensic psychologists and psychiatrists. This study also
(Grezlak, 1999, p. 3). seeks to provide information about the proportion of mental health
One likely reason for the discrepant decisions concerning the professionals in these fields who are active in profiling work, to
admissibility of profiling testimony is that there is little documen- explore similarities and differences in opinions about profiling
tation one way or the other about whether profiling, regardless of from forensic psychologists and psychiatrists, and to examine
what it is called, is a generally accepted investigative technique. whether changing the term criminal profiling to criminal investi-
Just as perceptions of profiling are often based on lore, so are gative analysis has an impact on perceptions of its acceptance and
opinions about its acceptance. The general acceptance of profiling validity.
has not been systematically examined among mental health pro-
fessionals, and arguments that support the acceptance of profiling
in the law enforcement community typically are based on the Exploratory Profiling and Criminal Investigative
rationale that profiling must be accepted if it continues to be used Analysis Survey
(see Kocsis & Hayes, 2004).
Identifying Appropriate Mental Health Professionals
In all states and federal jurisdictions, general acceptance is a
criterion that judges consider when making decisions about the Because there is no professional society or organization dedi-
admissibility of expert testimony. General acceptance in the field cated to mental health professionals involved in profiling work, we
is the main criterion on which decisions are to be based in states asked members of professional organizations with a more general
relying on the Frye (Frye v. United States, 1923) standard for focus on mental health and law issues to complete an Internet
determining the admissibility of expert testimony. In Daubert v. survey about the scientific merit and utility of profiling. These
Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals (1993), the Supreme Court ruled organizations were Division 41 (American Psychology–Law So-
that the Federal Rules of Evidence, and not the Frye v. United ciety [AP-LS]) of the APA, the American Academy of Forensic
States standard, should be used for determining the admissibility of Psychology (AAFP), the American Academy of Psychiatry and
expert testimony in federal courts. Many states have since adopted Law (AAPL), and the Police and Public Safety section in APA
Daubert-like standards for admitting expert testimony. Division 18, Psychologists in Public Service. AP-LS member
54 TORRES, BOCCACCINI, AND MILLER

e-mail addresses were obtained from the list of attendees posted on selves as psychiatrists, 3.1% (n ⫽ 5) reported having a doctorate in
the 2004 AP-LS conference Web site. We cross-checked names medicine and a doctorate in philosophy, and 2.5% (n ⫽ 4) did not
from the AP-LS Web site with the APA membership directory to provide information about their profession.
ensure that those asked to participate were psychologists, as op-
posed to graduate students, attorneys, or other affiliate members.
Survey Format and Questions
We e-mailed AAFP members directly, using addresses available
through the organization’s Web site. The presidents of AAPL and A major goal of this study was to examine whether perceptions
of the Police and Public Safety section of APA Division 18 of the validity, utility, and acceptance of profiling methods varied
requested that we first send e-mails soliciting participation to them, depending on whether these methods were described as “profiling”
which they would then forward to their respective members. or “criminal investigative analysis.” Two versions of the study
E-mails soliciting participation were distributed to 1,637 foren- survey were created that were identical in all aspects except one:
sic mental health professionals: 175 AP-LS members, 147 AAFP one version used the term profiling in the instructions to partici-
members, 840 AAPL members, and 475 members of the Police pants and in the study questions, whereas the other version used
and Public Safety section of APA Division 18. Only 9.9% (N ⫽ criminal investigative analysis. The latter is the term currently
161) of those who received an e-mail solicitation completed the used by the FBI to refer to profiling. The definition of profiling
study. Although this response rate was admittedly lower than we provided to mental health professionals was “Profiling is the
had hoped for and clearly places limits on the applicability of the process of using physical and behavioral evidence available at a
findings to forensic mental professionals as a whole, the completed crime scene to make inferences about the personality characteris-
surveys provide information about the perceptions of profiling by tics of the individual(s) responsible for committing the criminal
a sizable group of forensic mental health professionals (N ⫽ 161). act.” In the alternate version of the survey, criminal investigative
Moreover, a low response rate was anticipated for several reasons. analysis was substituted for profiling, but the definitions were
Few practitioners in these four organizations were expected to be otherwise identical.
active in profiling, and many were expected to avoid answering The first five survey questions asked participants to provide yes
questions about profiling because they felt it was outside the scope or no responses to questions about their training, knowledge, and
of their field. Although the Internet can be a cost-effective method experience with profiling methods or criminal investigative anal-
for collecting survey data, response rates for Internet surveys are ysis (see Table 1). The second five questions asked participants to
often markedly lower (ranging from 7% to 44%) than those for provide yes or no responses to questions about the scientific and
mail and telephone surveys (Schonlau, Fricker, & Elliot, 2002). In professional status of profiling or criminal investigative analysis
addition, response rates for Internet surveys of academic and (see Table 2).
professional groups tend to be lower than those of other groups The final five questions assessed respondents’ knowledge of
(Cook, Heath, & Thompson, 2000). The expected low response standards used for admitting expert testimony into court, including
rate was the main reason for soliciting participation from such a Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals (1993), Frye v. United
large group of forensic mental health professionals (N ⫽ 1,637). States (1923), and the Federal Rules of Evidence.1 Each item was
Although the survey data reported in this article provide infor- scored as correct or incorrect. We totaled the scores on the five
mation about the practices and beliefs of 161 forensic mental items to form an admissibility knowledge composite score, which
health professionals, the low response rate raises legitimate ques- we found to have acceptable internal consistency (Kuder–
tions about whether the sample is representative and, thus, whether Richardson 20 ⫽ .72).
the findings can be generalized to forensic mental health profes- For each professional organization, mental health practitioners
sionals as a whole. The low response rate also suggests that it is with last names beginning with A–M were directed to one version
unlikely that respondents were able to accurately express the of the survey, and those with last names beginning with N–Z were
amount of variation in beliefs and practices that is present among directed to the other version. The order was counterbalanced for
forensic mental health professionals as a whole. It may be that the different organizations. For two organizations, those with
those who responded were strong proponents or advocates of names beginning with A–M were directed to the profiling survey.
profiling and that the survey findings represent either an overly For the other two organizations, those with names beginning with
positive or an overly negative view of profiling. For this reason, A–M were directed to the criminal investigative analysis survey.
we strongly caution readers against interpreting the findings re-
ported here as clear evidence either for or against the acceptance of
profiling in the field. We consider this study to be an early Experiences and Attitudes of Psychologists and
exploratory step in moving the fields of psychology and psychiatry Psychiatrists
toward a more empirically based understanding of the acceptance Preliminary analyses revealed only one significant difference
or nonacceptance of profiling. Readers must interpret the survey between psychologists and psychiatrists on the 10 main survey
findings with this important limitation in mind. questions concerning training, knowledge, experience, and atti-
Of the responding forensic mental health professionals, 44.7% tudes about profiling. This one effect revealed that psychiatrists
(n ⫽ 72) reported AP-LS membership, 19.3% (n ⫽ 31) reported were somewhat more likely to say that profiling was a useful tool
AAFP membership, 44.1% (n ⫽ 71) reported AAPL membership, for law enforcement (95.4%) than were psychologists (84.9%), ␹2
and 8.1% (n ⫽ 13) reported Police and Public Safety section
membership. Many participants reported membership in multiple
organizations. With respect to profession, 50.9% (n ⫽ 82) identi- 1
A copy of the five-item questionnaire assessing knowledge of admis-
fied themselves as psychologists, 43.5% (n ⫽ 70) identified them- sibility standards is available from Marcus T. Boccaccini.
CRIMINAL PROFILING 55

Table 1
Percentage of Surveyed Forensic Mental Health Professionals With Profiling Knowledge,
Training, and Experience

Procedure

Profiling CIA
Survey item (n ⫽ 92) (n ⫽ 69)

Do you consider yourself knowledgeable about the field of ? 31.5% 26.1%


Do you have any training in ? 16.3% 20.3%
Have you ever generated a for an investigation? 10.9% 10.1%
Have you ever testified in court about a ? 9.8% 10.1%
Have you ever been asked about your opinion of in court? 10.9% 13.0%

Note. CIA ⫽ criminal investigative analysis. Percentage values in this table reflect the proportion of “yes”
responses to survey items. All differences between profiling and CIA failed to reach statistical and clinical
significance, p ⬎ .05, phi coefficients (␾) ⬍ .06.

(1, N ⫽ 172) ⫽ 4.12, p ⬍ .05, ␾ ⫽ .17. Although this difference Attitudes About Profiling and Criminal Investigative
is large enough to reach statistical significance, the majority of Analysis
respondents from both professions felt that profiling was a useful
tool for law enforcement. Because the overall pattern of findings Forensic mental health professionals’ attitudes about the scien-
across the 10 main survey questions was similar for psychologists tific merit and utility of profiling and criminal investigative anal-
and psychiatrists, findings are reported below for the entire sample ysis are reported in Table 2. Fewer than half of the forensic mental
of forensic mental health professionals. health professionals felt that either profiling or criminal investiga-
tive analysis was reliable, was valid, or had enough scientific
Training, Knowledge, and Experience With Profiling support to be admitted into court. Perceptions were especially
negative among those who received the version of the survey using
Table 1 provides information concerning the proportion of fo-
rensic mental health professionals who reported having training, the term profiling. These forensic mental health professionals rated
knowledge, and experience with profiling and criminal investiga- profiling to be significantly less reliable and valid than those who
tive analysis. A similar pattern of findings was observed for were given the term criminal investigative analysis for the same
participants responding to questions about profiling and criminal process. These findings support the position that profiling is likely
investigative analysis. Although a sizable minority of forensic to be viewed more favorably if it is referred to by another name.
mental health professionals reported having knowledge of profil- Recall that the definitions provided for profiling and criminal
ing and criminal investigative analysis (31.5% and 26.1%, respec- investigative analysis were identical. Only the name of the process
tively) and training in these techniques (16.3% and 20.3%, respec- varied.
tively), only about 10% reported ever having generated a profile or Although forensic mental health professionals questioned the
criminal investigative analysis for an actual case. Similarly, only scientific merit of profiling and criminal investigative analysis,
about 10% reported having testified in court about their use of there was general agreement that these techniques were useful
these techniques or their opinion of these techniques in general. tools for law enforcement. In addition, nearly all forensic mental

Table 2
Percentage of Surveyed Forensic Mental Health Professionals Who Identified Profiling and Criminal Investigative Analysis (CIA) as
Being Valid and Useful

Procedure

Profiling CIA
Survey item (n ⫽ 92) (n ⫽ 68) ␹2(1, N ⫽ 160) ␾

Is scientifically valid for linking a defendant to a crime? 23.0% 43.3% 6.84* .22
Is scientifically reliable? 17.1% 40.0% 9.69** .26
Do you think that is scientifically supported enough to be admitted into court under 27.0% 31.1% 0.31 .05
any circumstances?
Do you think that is a useful tool for law enforcement? 86.4% 95.1% 3.03 .14
Do you think that should be researched empirically? 97.8% 96.9% 0.10 ⫺.02

Note. Percentage values in this table reflect the proportion of “yes” responses to survey items. ␾ ⫽ phi, effect size for ␹2 analyses that can be interpreted
as a correlation coefficient. In this table, ␾ provides an index of the strength of the association between the label given to the profiling process and positive
perceptions of scientific merit and utility. Larger positive values of ␾ indicate more positive evaluations for CIA as opposed to those evaluations for
profiling.
* p ⬍ .05. ** p ⬍ .01.
56 TORRES, BOCCACCINI, AND MILLER

health professionals felt that profiling and criminal investigative Mental Health Professionals’ Involvement in Profiling
analysis should be the focus of empirical research.
Because many forensic mental health professionals reported that Few forensic mental health professionals responding to the
they did not consider themselves knowledgeable about profiling survey had ever conducted a profile or criminal investigative
and criminal investigative analysis, it can be argued that only the analysis, yet a somewhat larger minority considered themselves
perceptions of those who are knowledgeable should be considered knowledgeable about these techniques. Why would those who
for gauging general acceptance. To address this concern, we com- claim to be knowledgeable about profiling not conduct profiles?
pared beliefs about the validity, reliability, and scientific support Three possible interpretations for this pattern of findings are of-
for profiling and criminal investigative analysis for those who did fered here: (a) one that argues against mental health professionals’
and did not consider themselves knowledgeable about these meth- involvement in the practice of profiling, (b) one that is more
ods. Small cell sizes precluded separate analyses for profiling and positive about involvement in profiling, and (c) one that is open
criminal investigative analysis. No significant differences between minded about involvement in profiling.
those with and without knowledge were revealed, with effect sizes The first interpretation is that the knowledge possessed by some
(␾) ranging from .01 to .05. For the 47 forensic mental health respondents suggests to them that profiling lacks scientific merit.
practitioners who reported being knowledgeable about profiling or Support for this interpretation is provided by the finding that both
criminal investigative analysis, 27.7% (n ⫽ 13) felt that these profiling and criminal investigative analysis were seen as lacking
procedures were scientifically valid, 25.5% (n ⫽ 12) felt that they adequate reliability, validity, and scientific support by more than
were reliable, and 25.5% (n ⫽ 12) felt that they had adequate half of all respondents. This pattern of findings was similar for
scientific support for being admitted into court. those who did and did not consider themselves knowledgeable
about profiling, suggesting that many with knowledge about pro-
filing felt that profiling and criminal investigative analysis lacked
Admissibility Knowledge
scientific merit. This perspective may be especially common
Knowledge about standards for admitting expert testimony into among psychologists who are active practitioners and who are
evidence was assessed to examine whether those with higher levels knowledgeable about standards for admitting expert testimony into
of admissibility knowledge were more likely to see profiling and court. Psychologists who said that they were more knowledgeable
criminal investigative analysis as lacking adequate scientific merit. about admissibility standards tended to have the most negative
The average number of correct responses to the admissibility views about the scientific merit of profiling and criminal investi-
questions across the 158 mental health professionals who com- gative analysis.
pleted all five questions was 3.65 (SD ⫽ 1.48). Psychologists (n ⫽ The second, more positive interpretation is that some forensic
80, M ⫽ 3.99, SD ⫽ 1.35) exhibited significantly more knowledge mental health professionals have become familiar with profiling
about admissibility standards on the five-question scale than psy- methods or research (or both) because they have been asked about
chiatrists did (n ⫽ 69, M ⫽ 3.26, SD ⫽ 1.46), t(147) ⫽ 3.15, p ⬍ profiling by attorneys, journalists, or students. Some of these
.02, Cohen’s d ⫽ 0.52. forensic mental health professionals may be willing to undertake
A scientific merit composite score was created by totaling the profiling work but have never been hired to do so.
“yes” responses to the survey questions about the reliability, va- The third and more open-minded interpretation is that some
lidity, and scientific support for profiling (see Table 1). Scores on forensic mental health professionals are keeping a watchful eye on
this composite could range from 1 to 3, with high scores reflecting developments in profiling but are not yet comfortable practicing in
positive attitudes about the scientific merit of profiling. These this area. During the past 5 years, many of the top peer-reviewed
scores were then correlated with scores on the admissibility knowl- psychology–law journals have published articles concerning pro-
edge scale using Spearman rank-order correlations (rs). Among filing. These journals include Psychology, Public Policy, and Law
forensic mental health professionals as a whole, admissibility (Alison, West, & Goodwill, 2004; Canter et al., 2004), Law and
knowledge was negatively related to beliefs about the scientific Human Behavior (Davis & Follette, 2002; Wells, 2003), Behav-
merit of profiling and criminal investigative analysis, rs ⫽ ⫺.16, ioral Sciences and the Law (Canter et al., 2003; Salfati & Canter,
p ⬍ .05. Those with higher levels of admissibility knowledge 1999; Snook, Canter, & Bennell, 2002), and Criminal Justice and
tended to view profiling as having less scientific merit. However, Behavior (Canter & Wentink, 2004; Kocsis, 2004). Those involved
further examination revealed that this trend was almost completely in forensic practice or research may have seen these articles or
accounted for by psychologists (rs ⫽ ⫺.26, p ⬍ .05). Admissibility been given information about the latest developments in profiling
knowledge and beliefs about the scientific merit of profiling were through continuing education outlets. This interpretation also is
not significantly correlated for psychiatrists, rs ⫽ ⫺.03, p ⬎ .05. supported by the finding that nearly all forensic mental health
practitioners believed that profiling was worthy of scientific study.
Implications for Practice Many respondents who questioned the scientific merit of profiling
still believed that more research should be conducted.
The purpose of conducting this exploratory survey was to pro- These three interpretations are not mutually exclusive. All or
vide an estimate of the acceptance of profiling among forensic none of these rationales may apply to a single forensic mental
mental health professionals. Although the low response rate nec- health professional who is knowledgeable about profiling but is
essarily limits the strength of the conclusions that can be drawn not involved in profiling work. Those individuals who are consid-
from the survey findings, this survey should be considered an early ering becoming involved in profiling work should be aware that
step in moving the field toward a more empirically based under- some forensic mental health professionals who are knowledgeable
standing of the acceptance of profiling. about profiling avoid working in this area. At this point in time,
CRIMINAL PROFILING 57

profiling does not appear to be generally accepted among forensic profiling, and, thus, they were likely to view its use within law
mental health professionals as a valid and scientifically supported enforcement as important. However, this explanation seems less
technique. Whether research advancements in this area will even- plausible given that the overwhelming majority of the respondents
tually improve the opinions of profiling among mental health viewed profiling as invalid and unreliable and did not rate profiling
practitioners is not clear. The finding from the current study that positively, overall.
approximately 70% of mental health practitioners believed that One final limitation of this survey is that participants were
profiling or criminal investigative analysis was not a valid tech- provided with a general description of profiling. There are many
nique is nearly identical to Bartol’s (1996) finding from nearly 10 different approaches to profiling, with different underlying theo-
years ago that 70% of police psychologists “seriously questioned” ries, methods, and assumptions. For those knowledgeable about
the validity of profiling (p. 79). profiling, responses may have varied if we had asked about a
However, findings from the current study do suggest that some specific approach. Major approaches to profiling include the FBI’s
forensic mental health professionals harbor negative views about criminal investigative analysis (Douglas & Burgess, 1986), behav-
the term profiling rather than, or in addition to, the profiling ioral evidence analysis (Turvey, 2002), investigative psychology
process. Survey participants who were asked to express their (Canter, 1995), and geographic profiling (Rossmo, 1999). It may
opinions about criminal investigative analysis (another name for be that support for some of these approaches is stronger than
profiling), expressed much more positive views. On the basis of others, and future studies may seek to examine support for differ-
this finding, we recommend that those individuals who are asked ent profiling approaches.
to comment on or testify about profiling think carefully about the
basis for their opinion of profiling before making any statements.
Beliefs about the validity of profiling should be based on existing References
evidence rather than on negative attitudes about the term profiling.
Alison, L., West, A., & Goodwill, A. (2004). The academic and the
Many of the recent scholarly articles addressing the current status
practitioner: Pragmatists’ views of offender profiling. Psychology, Pub-
of profiling research are cited in this article. We encourage those lic Policy, and Law, 10, 71–101.
individuals who are asked to comment or testify about profiling to Bartol, C. R. (1996). Police psychology: Then, now, and beyond. Criminal
become familiar with this growing body of research literature. Justice and Behavior, 23, 70 – 89.
Bartol, C. R., & Bartol, A. M. (2004). Psychology and law (3rd ed.).
Limitations and Conclusions Wadsworth: Belmont, CA.
Canter, D. (1995). Criminal shadows: Inside the mind of the serial killer.
Although findings from the current study show that many fo- London: HarperCollins.
rensic mental health professionals have concerns about the scien- Canter, D. V., Alison, L. J., Alison, E., & Wentink, N. (2004). The
tific merit of profiling, these findings alone are insufficient for organized/disorganized typology of serial murder: Myth or model? Psy-
chology, Public Policy, and Law, 10, 293–320.
arguing that profiling evidence should not be admitted into court.
Canter, D. V., Bennell, C., Alison, L., & Reddy, S. (2003). Differentiating
Views about the acceptance of profiling as a valid technique may
sex offenses: A behaviorally based thematic classification of stranger
be higher among other professionals who do profiling work, in- rapes. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 21, 157–174.
cluding law enforcement agents and criminal justice experts. In Canter, D. V., & Wentink, N. (2004). An empirical test of Holmes and
addition, most participants felt that profiling was a useful tool for Holmes’s serial murder typology. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31,
law enforcement, suggesting that they believe someone should be 489 –515.
doing this work. Constanzo, M. (2004). Psychology applied to law. Wadsworth: Belmont,
The seemingly counterintuitive pattern of respondents reporting CA.
insufficient evidence for the validity of profiling but endorsing its Cook, C., Heath, F., & Thompson, R. L. (2000). A meta-analysis of
use for law enforcement potentially could be explained in multiple response rates in web- or Internet-based surveys. Educational and Psy-
ways. The most likely explanation is that these forensic mental chological Measurement, 60, 821– 836.
Cooley, C. M., & Turvey, B. E. (2002, June). Reliability and validity:
health professionals think about profiling in a similar way to that
Admissibility standards relative to forensic experts illustrated by crim-
of professionals in the law enforcement community by reasoning inal profiling evidence, testimony, and judicial rulings. Journal of Be-
that profiling must be at least somewhat useful if it continues to be havioral Profiling, 3(1). Retrieved August 12, 2002, from http://www
used (see Kocsis & Hayes, 2004). Indeed, it is possible that these .profiling.org/journal/index.html
forensic mental health professionals have seen, or have at least Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharmaceuticals, 113 S. Ct. 2786 (1993).
heard of, the process being effective for law enforcement and are Davis, D., & Follette, W. C. (2002). Rethinking the probative value of
willing to admit that it may be useful in some cases. A second evidence: Base rates, intuitive profiling, and the “postdiction” of behav-
explanation is that respondents may have believed that more ior. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 133–158.
information about the scientific merit of profiling is needed before Douglas, J. E., & Burgess, A. E. (1986, December). Criminal profiling: A
they are willing to assert that it is not at all useful and should be viable investigative tool against violent crime. FBI Law Enforcement
Bulletin, 55(12), 9 –13.
discontinued. In other words, given the small amount of profiling
Douglas, J. E., & Olshaker, M. (1995). Mind hunter: Inside the FBI’s elite
research available, they may have believed that the existing evi-
serial crime unit. New York: Mindhunters.
dence against profiling is not strong enough to argue that it should Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
never be used. A third explanation is that the respondents to the Goodman-Delahunty, J. (1997). Forensic psychological expertise in the
survey represent a biased sample of forensic mental health profes- wake of Daubert. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 121–140.
sionals. It may be that a large portion of the responding sample was Grezlak, H. (1999, April 12). Profiling testimony inadmissible in murder
made up of individuals who have some interest or training in trial. The Pennsylvania Law Weekly, p. 3.
58 TORRES, BOCCACCINI, AND MILLER

Grubin, D. (1995, September). Offender profiling. The Journal of Forensic Ressler, R. K., & Burgess, A. W. (1985, August). Violent crime. FBI Law
Psychiatry, 6, 259 –263. Enforcement Bulletin, 1–32.
Homant, R. J., & Kennedy, D. B. (1998). Psychological aspects of crime Risinger, D. M., & Loop, J. L. (2002). Three card monte, Monty Hall,
scene profiling: Validity research. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 25, modus operandi, and “offender profiling”: Some lessons of modern
319 –343. cognitive science for the law of evidence. Cardozo Law Review, 24,
Honts, C. R., & Quick, B. D. (1995). The polygraph in 1995: Progress in 193–285.
science and the law. North Dakota Law Review, 71, 987–1019. Rossmo, D. K. (1999). Geographic profiling. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.
Iacono, W. G., & Lykken, D. T. (1997). The validity of the lie detector: Salfati, C. G. (2003). Offender interaction with victims in homicide: A
Two surveys of scientific opinion. Journal of Applied Psychology, 82, multidimensional analysis of frequencies in crime scene behaviors.
426 – 433. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 18, 490 –512.
Kassin, S. M., Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, V. L. (1989). The “general Salfati, C. G., & Canter, D. V. (1999). Differentiating stranger murders:
acceptance” of psychological research on eyewitness testimony: A sur- Profiling offender characteristics from behavioral styles. Behavioral
vey of the experts. American Psychologist, 44, 1089 –1098. Sciences and the Law, 17, 391– 406.
Kassin, S. M., Tubb, V. A., Hosch, H. M., & Memon, A. (2001). On the Schonlau, M., Fricker, R. D., & Elliot, M. N. (2002). Conducting research
“general acceptance” of eyewitness testimony research: A new survey of surveys via e-mail and the Web. Santa Monica, CA: Rand.
the experts. American Psychologist, 56, 405– 416. Snook, B., Canter, D., & Bennell, C. (2002). Predicting the home location
Kocsis, R. N. (2003a). Criminal psychological profiling: Validities and
of serial offenders: A preliminary comparison of the accuracy of human
abilities. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative
judges with a geographic profiling system. Behavioral Sciences and the
Criminology, 47, 126 –144.
Law, 20, 109 –118.
Kocsis, R. N. (2003b). An empirical assessment of content in criminal
Turvey, B. E. (2002). Criminal profiling: An introduction to behavioral
psychological profiles. International Journal of Offender Therapy and
analysis (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Comparative Criminology, 47, 37– 46.
United States v. Meeks, 35 M. J. 64 (CMA 1992).
Kocsis, R. N. (2004). Psychological profiling of serial arson offenses: An
Weinerman, L. (2004a). Criminal profiling: The reality behind the myth.
assessment of skills and accuracy. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 31,
341–361. Monitor on Psychology, 35(7), 66 – 69.
Kocsis, R. N., Cooksey, R. W., & Irwin, H. J. (2002). Psychological Weinerman, L. (2004b). Does profiling really work? Monitor on Psychol-
profiling of sexual murders: An empirical model. International Journal ogy, 35(7), 67.
of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 46, 532–554. Wells, G. (2003). Murder, extramarital affairs, and the issue of probative
Kocsis, R. N., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). Believing is seeing? Investigating the value. Law and Human Behavior, 27, 623– 628.
perceived accuracy of criminal psychological profiles. International Wilson, P., Lincoln, R., & Kocsis, R. (1997). Validity, utility and ethics of
Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 48, 149 – profiling for serial violent and sexual offenders. Psychiatry, Psychology,
160. and Law, 4, 1–11.
Kocsis, R. N., Irwin, H. J., Hayes, A. F., & Nunn, R. (2000). Expertise in Wrightsman, L. S., & Fulero, S. M. (2005). Forensic psychology (2nd ed.).
psychological profiling: A comparative assessment. Journal of Interper- Wadsworth: Belmont, CA.
sonal Violence, 15, 311–331.
Pinizzotto, A. J. (1984). Forensic psychology: Criminal personality profil-
ing. Journal of Police Science and Administration, 12, 32– 40. Received August 9, 2004
Pinizzotto, A. J., & Finkel, N. J. (1990). Criminal personality profiling: An Revision received June 15, 2005
outcome and process study. Law and Human Behavior, 14, 215–233. Accepted June 16, 2005 䡲

You might also like