You are on page 1of 14

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57:1 (2016), pp 4–17 doi:10.1111/jcpp.

12435

Research Review: Use of EEG biomarkers in child


psychiatry research – current state and future
directions
Sandra K. Loo,1 Agatha Lenartowicz,1 and Scott Makeig2
1
Semel Neuropsychiatric Institute, David Geffen School of Medicine, UCLA, Los Angeles, CA; 2Swartz Center for
Computational Neuroscience, UCSD, San Diego, CA, USA

Background: Electroencephalography (EEG) and related measures have a long and productive history in child
psychopathology research and are currently experiencing a renaissance in interest, particularly for use as putative
biomarkers. Method and Scope: First, the recent history leading to the use of EEG measures as endophenotypes and
biomarkers for disease and treatment response are reviewed. Two key controversies within the area of noninvasive
human electrophysiology research are discussed, and problems that currently either function as barriers or provide
gateways to progress. First, the differences between the main types of EEG measurements (event-related potentials,
quantitative EEG, and time–frequency measures) and how they can contribute collectively to better understanding of
cortical dynamics underlying cognition and behavior are highlighted. Second, we focus on the ongoing shift in
analytic focus to specific cortical sources and source networks whose dynamics are relevant to the clinical and
experimental focus of the study, and the effective increase in source signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) that may be obtained
in the process. Conclusions: Understanding of these issues informs any discussion of current trends in EEG
research. We highlight possible ways to evolve our understanding of brain dynamics beyond the apparent
contradictions in understanding and modeling EEG activity highlighted by these controversies. Finally, we
summarize some promising future directions of EEG biomarker research in child psychopathology.
Keywords: EEG, event-related potential, brain imaging, neurophysiology, psychopathology.

ics. Over time, scientific interest in EEG, particu-


Introduction
larly as applied to child psychopathology research
Electroencephalography (EEG) has a long history in
and practice, has waxed or waned depending on
child psychopathology research and was the first
experimental questions of the day. Currently, EEG
methodology used for examining human cortical
is experiencing a renaissance, as suggested by the
brain activity among children. EEG recordings from
number of Pubmed citations with keywords ‘EEG,
the human scalp were first successfully recorded by
“ERP”, and ‘brain oscillation’, for which Figure 1
Hans Berger (1929), whose observations regarding
shows some interesting trends: the early importance
prominent brain oscillations in the 8–12 Hertz (Hz)
of EEG brain imaging before the development of
‘alpha’ band range still hold true today. Soon,
fMRI, the emergence of ERP studies since the
thereafter, Jasper, Solomon, and Bradley (1938)
introduction of this term and measure in the early
reported increased slow-wave activity among chil-
1960s, leading to the predominance of the ‘ERP’
dren who were institutionalized for problem behav-
keyword over the keyword ‘EEG’ itself in published
iors, many of whom might today qualify for child
reports during a nearly 30-year period (1980–2007),
psychiatric diagnoses. Since those first qualitative
the difference peaking about 1989. Also, note the
observations, EEG and metrics derived from it,
continuing increase over the last 40 years in studies
including trial-averaged event-related potentials
using the keywords ‘brain oscillations’, which may
(ERPs), have been widely used in child psychopa-
reflect the growing recognition that brain dynamic
thology studies. This is likely because of the many
phenomena are incompletely captured by ERP and
advantages of using EEG for functional brain imag-
static spectral ‘quantitative EEG’ (qEEG) measures
ing – including its flexibility and portability, its
(Makeig, 1993; Pfurtscheller, Aranibar, & Maresch,
noninvasive nature and wide subject acceptance,
1979). Ongoing applications of new EEG measures
and its excellent temporal resolution and relatively
are revealing information about cortical brain
low cost.
dynamics in EEG data that go well beyond tradi-
In recent decades, new mathematical, technolog-
tional ERP and qEEG measures.
ical, and neuroscientific advances have allowed
EEG measures to become an important data source Focus of the current review
for a new quantitative science of macroscopic or
‘mesoscopic’ (Freeman, 2000) cortical brain dynam- Currently, many applications of EEG research
within child psychopathology are being proposed
and interpreted within the context of endopheno-
types and biomarkers for disease and treatment
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared.

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 Garsington Road, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main St, Malden, MA 02148, USA
doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 5

psychopathology. This paradigmatic shift started in


large part due to advances made by the Human
Genome Project and subsequent focus on identifying
genetic factors underlying complex traits. Because of
the significant heterogeneity that is present across
nearly all Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM;
APA, 2013) psychiatric disorders, the need arose for
greater use of intermediate phenotypes or ‘endophe-
notypes’ between the diagnostic phenotypes and the
underlying causal (i.e., genetic and environmental)
factors (Gottesman & Gould, 2003). Theoretically,
endophenotypes were meant to be genetically ‘sim-
pler’ phenotypes that lay along the same etiologic
pathway but were closer to the underlying gene
Figure 1 Relative percent of PubMed citations retrieved by ‘All
action. It was commonly believed that endophenoyt-
Fields’ search terms: ‘EEG’ + ‘ERP’ + ‘Brain Oscillations’. The
percent of citations for each search term relative to the total pes would confer more power to detect gene variants
number of citations returned by a search for any of the three signifying increased risk for disorders, thereby
terms is plotted relative to the other two search terms. For visual reducing etiologic heterogeneity and allowing delin-
clarity, ‘Brain Oscillations’ citations are graphed with a dotted eation of more homogeneous subgroups within a
line according to the Y-axis labels on the right; ‘EEG’ with a solid
diagnosis.
line and ‘ERP’ with a dashed line according to the Y-axis labels on
the left Electroencephalography and ERP measures were
considered excellent candidate endophenotypes
given they are quantitative neurobiological measures
response. Biomarkers are defined as, ‘a character- that can be much less expensive to collect than
istic that is objectively measured and evaluated as magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) measures
an indication of normal biological processes, path- (McLoughlin, Makeig, & Tsuang, 2014) and have
ogenic processes, or pharmacologic responses to a high heritability estimates: ~0.80 for spectral power
therapeutic intervention’ (Biomarkers Definition and ~0.50 for (some) ERP peak amplitudes within
Working Group, 2001). Because child psychiatric healthy populations (Smit, Posthuma, Boomsma, &
disorders involve significant clinical heterogeneity Geus, 2005). Heritability of EEG measures is simi-
in etiology, symptomatic presentation, developmen- larly high within psychiatric populations including
tal course, and treatment response, the identifica- ADHD (Loo et al., 2010; McLoughlin, Palmer,
tion of biomarkers to detect early risk factors, Rijsdijk, & Makeig, 2014), Bipolar Disorder (Bestel-
facilitate diagnosis, and potentially predict treat- meyer, Phillips, Crombie, Benson, & St Clair, 2009),
ment outcome would be of great value to the field. and Schizophrenia (Hall et al., 2011), particularly
Accordingly, this topic will be the primary focus of when recorded during cognitive activation conditions
the review. First, we review the recent history (Loo et al., 2010).
leading up to this development. Then, we discuss Many early endophenotype studies were based on
two key controversies within the area of noninvasive overly simplistic models of gene action and ignored
human electrophysiology research that currently the influence of environmental variables on gene
function either as gateways or barriers to progress. expression (Valdar et al., 2006). For example, early
Understanding of these issues informs any discus- genetic investigations of psychiatric disorders were
sion of current trends in EEG research. We then designed to detect the effects of a relatively small set
highlight possible ways to evolve our understanding of putative risk genes with moderate to large effects.
of brain dynamics beyond the apparent contradic- In addition, it was assumed that most cognitive and
tions in understanding and modeling EEG oscilla- neurobiologic endophenotypes had a simpler
tory activity as highlighted by these controversies. genetic architecture than psychiatric diagnoses,
Finally, we summarize some promising future which now appears to not be the case (Flint &
directions of EEG biomarker research in child Munafo, 2007). Finally, the majority of genetic
psychopathology. variance was thought to be captured by common
genetic variants (or polymorphisms) rather than
rare variants or copy number variations. In current
Use of EEG endophenotypes for gene models of psychiatric genetics, however, complex
discovery traits (and associated endophenotypes) are thought
In the past 10 to 15 years, our understanding of to be the result of hundreds, perhaps thousands of
psychopathology (in both child and adult forms) has genetic variations, each of small effect. As a result,
evolved from a focus on unique factors (genetic, the endophenotype approach for identifying genetic
environmental, neurobiologic, and cognitive) under- loci for psychiatric disorders has failed to deliver the
lying specific psychiatric disorders to one aimed at previously hoped for successes, with few notable
examining shared factors across different forms of exceptions.

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


6 Sandra K. Loo, Agatha Lenartowicz, and Scott Makeig J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2016; 57(1): 4–17

With the growing feasibility, cost-effectiveness, Within the research community, organizations
and use of whole-genome sequencing, however, a including the U.S. National Institute of Mental
more complete account of genetic variation underly- Health (NIMH) have proposed a move away from
ing complex traits and psychiatric conditions will characterizing psychopathology based on psychiat-
soon be available. With this will come a renewed ric diagnoses but rather according to translational
need for endophenotypes to assist with functional dimensions of functioning such as cognitive pro-
interpretation of the genetic sequence variants cesses, positive and negative valence, arousal and
(de Geus, 2010). This ‘reverse endophenotype’ self-regulation (Research Domain Criteria; RDOC).
approach is a strong justification for increased use Each of these dimensions can be characterized by a
of EEG measures in genetic investigations. Decou- translational, multilevel pathway (i.e., genes, neural
pled from examining the genetic pathways associ- circuitry, brain dynamics, and behavior) that can
ated with disease and neurobiological functions, inform searches for both underlying neural mecha-
EEG measures shown to be useful for disease nisms and potential treatments. Consistent with this
prediction, diagnosis, characterization, and treat- approach, NIMH has more recently issued guidance
ment monitoring are increasingly referred to as for preclinical and intervention trials (e.g., NIH
biological markers or ‘biomarkers’ rather than end- FAST-Fail trials) that makes central the need for
ophenotypes. measurement of biomarkers in addition to behav-
ioral symptoms in gauging treatment response.
Collectively, this broader focus on etiologic and
Shift to shared genetic and mechanistic factors symptomatic heterogeneity within psychiatric disor-
across disorders ders and new focus on examination of domains of
As genetic studies failed to pinpoint the expected function across disorders has implications for efforts
risk genes underlying psychiatric disorders, the to develop more robust, highly cost effective EEG-
concept of disorder-specific genetic risk factors and based biomarkers for psychiatric disorders. It seems
mechanisms started to shift to shared etiologic and unlikely that a single biomarker measure of any type
neurobiologic factors that may be operant across can capture all of the variance within a diagnostic
disorders. Early studies in this area demonstrated category (Lenartowicz & Loo, 2014). Instead, finding
that genetic linkage findings across several child biomarkers for neural circuits associated with
psychiatric disorders including autism spectrum domains of functioning that are abnormal across
disorder (ASD), attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- several disorders seems a potentially more fruitful
order (ADHD), and dyslexia overlapped at a rate approach.
significantly greater than chance (Smalley, Loo,
Yang, & Cantor, 2005). Theories regarding common
genetic architecture for broad band internalizing Current controversies
and externalizing disorders have been proposed, Interest in EEG research is again on the rise in large
with a small proportion of disorder unique genes part because refined EEG measures can now
and environmental experience factors determining increase the amount of relevant information about
the specific manifestation and diagnosis (Lahey, brain function that can be derived from the collected
Van Hulle, Singh, Waldman, & Rathouz, 2011). data. Below we discuss two controversies in EEG
Recent genetic findings from the Psychiatric Genet- analytic approaches that may serve as barriers to
ics Consortium across five psychiatric disorders: progress in using EEG in child psychopathology.
ADHD, ASD, Bipolar Disorder, Schizophrenia, and First, we discuss differences between the most
Major Depressive Disorder validate the idea that common types of EEG measures (ERP, qEEG, and
shared heritability and genetic loci underlie multi- event-related time–frequency measures) and how
ple psychiatric disorders (Cross-Disorder Group of their combination may contribute to better under-
the Psychiatric Genomics et al., 2013). standing of cortical dynamics underlying cognition
Simultaneous with the shift to common genetic and behavior. Second, we highlight the ongoing shift
factors came a recognition of common or shared in analytic focus to identifying specific cortical
neurobiologic mechanisms that underlie different sources and source networks whose dynamics are
psychiatric disorders. Although this multidimen- relevant to the clinical and experimental focus of the
sional view of psychopathology has always had study, and the effective increase in biomarker signal-
proponents, the concept currently has much wider to-noise ratio (SNR) that may be obtained using
and growing acceptance in the scientific and clinical source-resolved EEG measures.
communities. Within the clinical community, greater
recognition of psychopathology dimensions have
Controversy I: Use of averaged event-related
been incorporated into the DSM diagnostic criteria
potentials versus power spectra (qEEG) versus time–
as dimensional specifiers, which have replaced some
frequency measures
diagnostic subtypes, as well as elimination of exclu-
sion criteria prohibiting the diagnosis of comorbid Of the many possible outcome variables that can be
disorders such as ADHD and ASD. extracted from EEG-recorded scalp data, the two

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 7

that have most frequently been applied in psychiat- not in the other 25%. Thus, activity sequences
ric research are mean EEG power spectra during appearing in ERPs typically do not occur in all trials,
some specified time period(s) (often called ‘quantita- and indeed might even not occur in entirety in any
tive EEG’ or qEEG measures), and peak amplitudes single trial. Nonetheless, the amplitudes and laten-
and latencies of ERPs extracted from EEG data by cies of several ERP peaks (e.g., error-related nega-
averaging across data epochs identically time-locked tivity, P3, slow cortical potential, etc.) are typically
to some set of experimental events. Although the examined in ERP studies without any attention to
same hardware, software, and scalp signals are used their (possibly systematic) trial-to-trial variability –
to compute these different measures, there has or to accompanying EEG phenomena that do not
historically been a deep divide (with little cross-over) appear in the ERPs (for example, periods of alpha
between researchers who use ERP or qEEG mea- power ‘blocking’ or ‘flooding’ time-locked but not
sures. The principal difference between these mea- phase-locked to the same events).
sures is the signal processing methods used to derive Preliminary efforts to study single-trial ERP vari-
them. Below, we briefly describe these and then ability have yielded a more refined understanding of
discuss potential paths to bridge the divide in future ERP features and their association with underlying
research. neural and cognitive mechanisms. Several studies

Event-related potential. An ERP is computed from


EEG activity within a number of data epochs (A)
selected as being time-locked to the occurrence of
some class of events of interest, such as stimulus
onsets or subject responses. The time-locked data
epochs (or ‘trials’) are averaged, producing a single
trial-average waveform consisting of a series of
positive and negative peak deflections. The brief
(~1-s) ERP time series are typically quantified by
measuring the latencies and amplitudes of the major
peaks, and tested for statistical robustness using (B)
standard means (T-tests, ANOVAs, or nonparametric
bootstrap methods). After averaging many trial
epochs, only event-related EEG deflections that are
both time-locked (occur at fixed latencies relative to
the time-locking events) and phase-locked (i.e., at
least somewhat consistently positive- or negative-
going across trials) at some set of latencies following
(or sometimes preceding) the time-locking events
(C)
remain in the trial-average ERP waveform. Similarly,
only those ERP data features that arise more or less
consistently across subjects are reflected in the
group ‘grand average’ ERP. If oscillatory EEG com-
plexes following the time-locking events do not occur
at a consistent latency or with a constituent (e.g.,
positive or negative) phase, nothing of these features
will remain in the ERP after averaging across many
trials. Thus, analyzing only average ERPs extracted
from the recorded EEG signals typically leaves many
Figure 2 Insufficiency of the ERP model. (A) Color-indicated
of its features effectively unexamined.
single trials time locked to participant button presses in a
Importantly, ERPs do not reveal how many or cognitive task paradigm (channel: Cz to average reference). The
which of the trials contain deflections with the same trials are sorted (bottom to top) in increasing order of mean
phase (positive or negative) as the trial-average ERP potential in a (dashed-line indicated) postresponse latency
at that latency. For most ERP features, this figure is window and lightly smoothed with a 20-trial moving average.
The trial-average ERP is shown below the ERP-image panel. The
quite a bit less than 100%, as can be ascertained by
prominent postresponse positivity in the ERP actually occurs in
plotting the single trials sorted by potential at a less than 80% of the single trials. (B) The trial-average ERP
latency of interest. Figure 2A, from Makeig and regressed on each trial activity, plotted in the same trial order.
Onton (2012), shows an ‘ERP-image’ view of such a The model trial average, shown below, matches the actual trial
collection of trials whose ERP average contains a ERP. (C) The difference between the data in panels A and B shows
that activity in the postresponse latency window, for a subset of
prominent positivity peaking about 110 ms after
most positive-going and negative-going (top and bottom) trials,
subject button presses. However, sorting trials by is not proportional to the expression of the whole ERP in those
potential in this latency window shows that this trials. (Reprinted from Makeig & Onton, 2012, which also shows
positivity only occurs in about 75% of the trials and many other phenomena in these data not evident in this figure)

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 7

that have most frequently been applied in psychiat- not in the other 25%. Thus, activity sequences
ric research are mean EEG power spectra during appearing in ERPs typically do not occur in all trials,
some specified time period(s) (often called ‘quantita- and indeed might even not occur in entirety in any
tive EEG’ or qEEG measures), and peak amplitudes single trial. Nonetheless, the amplitudes and laten-
and latencies of ERPs extracted from EEG data by cies of several ERP peaks (e.g., error-related nega-
averaging across data epochs identically time-locked tivity, P3, slow cortical potential, etc.) are typically
to some set of experimental events. Although the examined in ERP studies without any attention to
same hardware, software, and scalp signals are used their (possibly systematic) trial-to-trial variability –
to compute these different measures, there has or to accompanying EEG phenomena that do not
historically been a deep divide (with little cross-over) appear in the ERPs (for example, periods of alpha
between researchers who use ERP or qEEG mea- power ‘blocking’ or ‘flooding’ time-locked but not
sures. The principal difference between these mea- phase-locked to the same events).
sures is the signal processing methods used to derive Preliminary efforts to study single-trial ERP vari-
them. Below, we briefly describe these and then ability have yielded a more refined understanding of
discuss potential paths to bridge the divide in future ERP features and their association with underlying
research. neural and cognitive mechanisms. Several studies

Event-related potential. An ERP is computed from


EEG activity within a number of data epochs (A)
selected as being time-locked to the occurrence of
some class of events of interest, such as stimulus
onsets or subject responses. The time-locked data
epochs (or ‘trials’) are averaged, producing a single
trial-average waveform consisting of a series of
positive and negative peak deflections. The brief
(~1-s) ERP time series are typically quantified by
measuring the latencies and amplitudes of the major
peaks, and tested for statistical robustness using (B)
standard means (T-tests, ANOVAs, or nonparametric
bootstrap methods). After averaging many trial
epochs, only event-related EEG deflections that are
both time-locked (occur at fixed latencies relative to
the time-locking events) and phase-locked (i.e., at
least somewhat consistently positive- or negative-
going across trials) at some set of latencies following
(or sometimes preceding) the time-locking events
(C)
remain in the trial-average ERP waveform. Similarly,
only those ERP data features that arise more or less
consistently across subjects are reflected in the
group ‘grand average’ ERP. If oscillatory EEG com-
plexes following the time-locking events do not occur
at a consistent latency or with a constituent (e.g.,
positive or negative) phase, nothing of these features
will remain in the ERP after averaging across many
trials. Thus, analyzing only average ERPs extracted
from the recorded EEG signals typically leaves many
Figure 2 Insufficiency of the ERP model. (A) Color-indicated
of its features effectively unexamined.
single trials time locked to participant button presses in a
Importantly, ERPs do not reveal how many or cognitive task paradigm (channel: Cz to average reference). The
which of the trials contain deflections with the same trials are sorted (bottom to top) in increasing order of mean
phase (positive or negative) as the trial-average ERP potential in a (dashed-line indicated) postresponse latency
at that latency. For most ERP features, this figure is window and lightly smoothed with a 20-trial moving average.
The trial-average ERP is shown below the ERP-image panel. The
quite a bit less than 100%, as can be ascertained by
prominent postresponse positivity in the ERP actually occurs in
plotting the single trials sorted by potential at a less than 80% of the single trials. (B) The trial-average ERP
latency of interest. Figure 2A, from Makeig and regressed on each trial activity, plotted in the same trial order.
Onton (2012), shows an ‘ERP-image’ view of such a The model trial average, shown below, matches the actual trial
collection of trials whose ERP average contains a ERP. (C) The difference between the data in panels A and B shows
that activity in the postresponse latency window, for a subset of
prominent positivity peaking about 110 ms after
most positive-going and negative-going (top and bottom) trials,
subject button presses. However, sorting trials by is not proportional to the expression of the whole ERP in those
potential in this latency window shows that this trials. (Reprinted from Makeig & Onton, 2012, which also shows
positivity only occurs in about 75% of the trials and many other phenomena in these data not evident in this figure)

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 9

ena that occur or evolve at 0.1-s and finer time scale, ciation between ADHD and the theta/beta ratio
such as perception, cognition, event evaluation, measure, a conceivable suggested reason being a
action planning, and motor action itself. However, general increase in this ratio among normal control
the misconception that data features appearing in populations in recent years (Arns et al., 2013). While
the EEG data not reflected in an average ERP are this theta/beta ratio measure may be elevated in a
‘task-irrelevant EEG noise’, can be a conceptual subgroup of individuals with ADHD, neither the
barrier to achieving a more complete understanding characteristics of this subgroup nor what brain state
of the meso- and macroscopic brain dynamic pro- is indexed by the theta/beta ratio have been deter-
cesses by making use of more of the information mined.
contained in the recorded EEG data. In contrast, EEG resting spectral power has been
shown to be a robust marker of neurodevelopment.
Quantitative EEG. Electroencephalography spec- Age-related maturation of the EEG power spectrum
tral power is estimated by separating the EEG has long been noted, consisting of attenuated slower
recordings using mathematical methods including wave activity (delta & theta bands) and enhanced
the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) and its variants faster wave activity (alpha & beta bands) (Gasser,
into a sum of activities within narrow frequency Jennen-Steinmetz, Sroka, Verleger, & Mocks, 1988;
bands. Power spectra represent the magnitude or John et al., 1980; Luchinger, Michels, Martin, &
magnitude-squared power of complex activity pat- Brandeis, 2012). In a recent study, Buyck and
terns as a function of EEG frequency (measured in Wiersema (2014) reported using several EEG mea-
cycles per second, Hertz [Hz]). Because EEG signals sures for predicting age classification (i.e., children,
themselves occupy a wide spectrum of frequencies adults) with high accuracy (theta/beta ratio, 97%;
(from 0.1 Hz and below to 250 Hz and above), power absolute and relative theta power, 91%–94%; relative
spectral averages can capture information about beta power, 90%). These developmental shifts have
parallel EEG processes occurring in distinct fre- been compared with MRI measures to help interpret
quency ranges that contribute to different aspects of the EEG findings. Combining concurrent resting-
cognitive, sensory, or motor processing. While defi- state fMRI and EEG measures, Luchinger et al.
nitions vary, mean amplitude or power within well- (2012) suggested that EEG changes during develop-
known frequency bands, including delta (1–4 Hz), ment may reflect age-related changes in emphasis
theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (9–12 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and integration of local and long-range networks, as
and gamma (>30 Hz), are often reported. Estimation inferred from spatial coherency in BOLD signals
of the mean power spectrum at each scalp channel or during instructed rest periods.
of average power across several electrodes has been While the use of the mean qEEG power spectrum
most frequently performed across the whole duration as a primary dependent variable does not capitalize
of a continuous recording period. This has some- on the temporal resolution of EEG, it does make use
times been referred to as qEEG measurement (to of its other advantages including its noninvasive-
contrast with still earlier nonquantitative analysis ness, relatively low cost, and flexibility of use with a
methods based solely on visual inspection, still wide range of subject and patient populations. New
widely used in neurology). Mean spectral power signal processing methods and widely available
estimation is most appropriate for conditions in computing power (see below) make it more feasible
which EEG signals might be expected to be relatively to extract information from analyses extending
stationary; the extent to which this is the case across large datasets, making large-scale studies
deserves detailed examination. computationally tractable. And, like average ERP
The power spectrum at rest is often modeled as a measures, mean power spectral measures greatly
marker of trait-like brain function indexing variables reduce the dimensionality of the recorded EEG data,
such as developmental level, baseline level of affect facilitating analysis including comparisons with
reactivity, or arousal. The best known example of other measures of interest such as fMRI BOLD
using EEG spectral power as a biomarker for diag- changes or behavioral performance. While reducing,
nosis is in ADHD, where high values of the theta to the (temporal and spatial) dimensionality of the data
beta ratio, recorded from the vertex or top of the is often necessary to achieve a coherent result, both
head, have been claimed to be strongly associated ERP and qEEG approaches result in a vast reduction
with diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- in information content about EEG signals generated
order (ADHD). Early studies (Monastra, Lubar, & by the many cortical processes contributing to the
Linden, 2001; Snyder et al., 2008) suggested high recorded EEG during the experiment.
accuracy in identifying people with ADHD; a meta- qEEG and ERP measures share relatively little
analysis reported a large effect size of 3.08 (Snyder & redundant information, since variations in the
Hall, 2006). However in the past 5 years, empiric and amplitude and latency of an ERP peak (representing
meta-analytic studies (Arns, Conners, & Kraemer, only a tiny fraction of the power in the whole EEG)
2013; Buyck & Wiersema, 2014; Liechti et al., 2013; may have little correlation with variations in the EEG
Loo et al., 2013; Ogrim, Kropotov, & Hestad, 2012; power spectrum during the same recording period.
Williams et al., 2010) have not supported the asso- qEEG power spectral measures may therefore be

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


10 Sandra K. Loo, Agatha Lenartowicz, and Scott Makeig J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2016; 57(1): 4–17

used fruitfully in combination with ERP measures the hippocampus, based on basic animal research
although, as noted above, these measures have (Arnolds, Lopes da Silva, Aitink, Kamp, & Boeijinga,
historically been studied in isolation from one other. 1980; Buzsaki 2002), whereas alpha-band activity
As we will discuss below, using newer analytic (8–12 Hz) has a known generator mechanism in
methods to identify relevant measures of specific radially arrayed cortico-thalamic loops (Lopes da
cortical source processes contributing to the scalp Silva, 1977). This cross-species link differentiates
data may give much more robust information rele- EEG from other brain imaging modalities, such as
vant to questions of psychiatric interest than simply fMRI, since measures of oscillatory field phenomena
collapsing the whole scalp data to a few summary may be more direct measures of brain activity than,
measures at one or the mean of a few scalp channels for instance, measures used to study BOLD-signal
(Lenartowicz et al., 2014; Mullen, Acar, Worrell, & functional connectivity, which rely on statistical
Makeig, 2011). estimates (typically linear measures of co-variation)
calculated on indirect (metabolic) indices of regional
Event-related time/frequency measures. Advances brain activity.
in computer technology and signal processing soft-
ware are attracting an increasing number of research- A way forward. A straightforward (although at the
ers to performing more general time–frequency outset not simpler) way to proceed toward more
analyses that combine and extend the strengths of specific and robust models of EEG dynamics is to
both ERP and qEEG spectral power measures. Time/ combine ERP, mean qEEG spectral power, and
frequency analyses extract changes in spectral power event-related time/frequency (ERSP and other) mea-
and phase across a given set of data epochs time- sures of oscillatory activity rather than limiting
locked to a set of similar events of interest, allowing analyses to just one measure. This allows a more
sub-second resolution of changes in brain oscillatory comprehensive examination of the changes in EEG
activity. The term ‘event-related spectral perturba- activity contained in the collected EEG signals and
tion’ (ERSP) was suggested for the time/frequency more complete tests for possible relationships
measure summarizing mean spectral power changes between and across measures of interest.
at each frequency surrounding the time-locking One interesting idea illustrating this direction is
events (Makeig, 1993); various other terms have since the analysis of pre-event oscillatory dynamics, in
been offered. Event-related spectral power increases particular power spectra, as indexing context-aware
or decreases within a single frequency band (e.g., the ‘preparatory’ state differences, and postevent ERPs
8–12 Hz alpha band) have been called event-related and ERSPs as indexing temporally precise cortical
synchronization [ERS] or desynchronization [ERD], processing of events. For example, several studies
respectively, since early research with electrodes have demonstrated that prestimulus EEG spectral
implanted in animal brains suggested loss of local power may predict subsequent event-related corti-
spatial cortical field synchronization during periods of cal activation (as measured using ERPs and ERSPs)
higher-frequency activity (Pfurtscheller & Aranibar, as well as subject performance on cognitive chal-
1979). lenges including memory encoding and retrieval
ERSPs, like ERPs, can be used to compare EEG (Addante, Watrous, Yonelinas, Ekstrom, & Rang-
activity in different trial-latency windows as well as anath, 2011), motor inhibition, and visual process-
in different trial subsets. For example, Lenartowicz ing (van Dijk, Schoffelen, Oostenveld, & Jensen,
et al. (2014) used ERSP analysis to examine power 2008). Whether prestimulus spectral power EEG
spectral differences within working memory (WM) features may index attention linked to arousal and/
task trial-latency windows in ADHD, which sug- or engagement of specific attention networks
gested that WM performance deficits were specifi- remains to be tested empirically. However, more
cally related to (bottom-up) visual encoding versus attention is warranted to relationships between
(top-down) WM maintenance. prestimulus EEG dynamic differences, ensuing
In addition to the clear benefit of allowing finer stimulus-driven cortical responses, and behavioral
event-related time resolution across the full EEG performance differences.
power spectrum, time-resolved measures of brain In summary, new analytic tools are making mod-
oscillatory activity features are largely conserved and eling of ongoing EEG dynamics and its relationship
associated with similar functional significance to performance, subject status, etc., increasingly
across mammalian species (Buzsaki, Logothetis, & possible and informative. For most EEG researchers,
Singer, 2013). This enhances the use of EEG to study however, collaboration with others with expertise in
and develop translational models of neural circuits, technical areas involved (e.g., signal processing,
the importance of which is emphasized in the NIMH specific clinical populations, etc.) is advisable since
RDOC initiative. Animal models of brain oscillations each by itself is becoming its own advanced field of
have been well studied and can inform our under- study. Fruitful collaborations between basic and
standing of the functional significance of various clinical researchers may bring more computationally
brain rhythms. For example, subcortical generators complex analyses to clinical population data,
of theta-band activity during memory tasks are in thereby developing more precise biomarkers of

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 9

ena that occur or evolve at 0.1-s and finer time scale, ciation between ADHD and the theta/beta ratio
such as perception, cognition, event evaluation, measure, a conceivable suggested reason being a
action planning, and motor action itself. However, general increase in this ratio among normal control
the misconception that data features appearing in populations in recent years (Arns et al., 2013). While
the EEG data not reflected in an average ERP are this theta/beta ratio measure may be elevated in a
‘task-irrelevant EEG noise’, can be a conceptual subgroup of individuals with ADHD, neither the
barrier to achieving a more complete understanding characteristics of this subgroup nor what brain state
of the meso- and macroscopic brain dynamic pro- is indexed by the theta/beta ratio have been deter-
cesses by making use of more of the information mined.
contained in the recorded EEG data. In contrast, EEG resting spectral power has been
shown to be a robust marker of neurodevelopment.
Quantitative EEG. Electroencephalography spec- Age-related maturation of the EEG power spectrum
tral power is estimated by separating the EEG has long been noted, consisting of attenuated slower
recordings using mathematical methods including wave activity (delta & theta bands) and enhanced
the Fast Fourier Transform (FFT) and its variants faster wave activity (alpha & beta bands) (Gasser,
into a sum of activities within narrow frequency Jennen-Steinmetz, Sroka, Verleger, & Mocks, 1988;
bands. Power spectra represent the magnitude or John et al., 1980; Luchinger, Michels, Martin, &
magnitude-squared power of complex activity pat- Brandeis, 2012). In a recent study, Buyck and
terns as a function of EEG frequency (measured in Wiersema (2014) reported using several EEG mea-
cycles per second, Hertz [Hz]). Because EEG signals sures for predicting age classification (i.e., children,
themselves occupy a wide spectrum of frequencies adults) with high accuracy (theta/beta ratio, 97%;
(from 0.1 Hz and below to 250 Hz and above), power absolute and relative theta power, 91%–94%; relative
spectral averages can capture information about beta power, 90%). These developmental shifts have
parallel EEG processes occurring in distinct fre- been compared with MRI measures to help interpret
quency ranges that contribute to different aspects of the EEG findings. Combining concurrent resting-
cognitive, sensory, or motor processing. While defi- state fMRI and EEG measures, Luchinger et al.
nitions vary, mean amplitude or power within well- (2012) suggested that EEG changes during develop-
known frequency bands, including delta (1–4 Hz), ment may reflect age-related changes in emphasis
theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (9–12 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz), and integration of local and long-range networks, as
and gamma (>30 Hz), are often reported. Estimation inferred from spatial coherency in BOLD signals
of the mean power spectrum at each scalp channel or during instructed rest periods.
of average power across several electrodes has been While the use of the mean qEEG power spectrum
most frequently performed across the whole duration as a primary dependent variable does not capitalize
of a continuous recording period. This has some- on the temporal resolution of EEG, it does make use
times been referred to as qEEG measurement (to of its other advantages including its noninvasive-
contrast with still earlier nonquantitative analysis ness, relatively low cost, and flexibility of use with a
methods based solely on visual inspection, still wide range of subject and patient populations. New
widely used in neurology). Mean spectral power signal processing methods and widely available
estimation is most appropriate for conditions in computing power (see below) make it more feasible
which EEG signals might be expected to be relatively to extract information from analyses extending
stationary; the extent to which this is the case across large datasets, making large-scale studies
deserves detailed examination. computationally tractable. And, like average ERP
The power spectrum at rest is often modeled as a measures, mean power spectral measures greatly
marker of trait-like brain function indexing variables reduce the dimensionality of the recorded EEG data,
such as developmental level, baseline level of affect facilitating analysis including comparisons with
reactivity, or arousal. The best known example of other measures of interest such as fMRI BOLD
using EEG spectral power as a biomarker for diag- changes or behavioral performance. While reducing,
nosis is in ADHD, where high values of the theta to the (temporal and spatial) dimensionality of the data
beta ratio, recorded from the vertex or top of the is often necessary to achieve a coherent result, both
head, have been claimed to be strongly associated ERP and qEEG approaches result in a vast reduction
with diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- in information content about EEG signals generated
order (ADHD). Early studies (Monastra, Lubar, & by the many cortical processes contributing to the
Linden, 2001; Snyder et al., 2008) suggested high recorded EEG during the experiment.
accuracy in identifying people with ADHD; a meta- qEEG and ERP measures share relatively little
analysis reported a large effect size of 3.08 (Snyder & redundant information, since variations in the
Hall, 2006). However in the past 5 years, empiric and amplitude and latency of an ERP peak (representing
meta-analytic studies (Arns, Conners, & Kraemer, only a tiny fraction of the power in the whole EEG)
2013; Buyck & Wiersema, 2014; Liechti et al., 2013; may have little correlation with variations in the EEG
Loo et al., 2013; Ogrim, Kropotov, & Hestad, 2012; power spectrum during the same recording period.
Williams et al., 2010) have not supported the asso- qEEG power spectral measures may therefore be

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


12 Sandra K. Loo, Agatha Lenartowicz, and Scott Makeig J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2016; 57(1): 4–17

(Makeig, Debener, Onton, & Delorme, 2004). In et al., 2014 for details). Because of space con-
actual data, these include eye blinks, saccades, straints, we present here only the results for an
electrocardiographic activity, single-channel noise occipital source cluster centered at or near the
from a loose electrode, and individual scalp muscle occipital pole (x = 8 mm, y = -89 mm, z = -19 mm).
activities. Other sources (unmixed by ICA from The scalp projection of these occipital cluster pro-
contamination by nonbrain source activity) repre- cesses was strongest at posterior occipital scalp
sent the projections of spatially stable cortical electrodes. Thus, we first examined the ERSPs for
sources. Because the connectivity structure of cortex both the midoccipital source cluster and the overly-
is highly weighted toward local neural connections, ing posterior scalp channels (e.g.., Oz; see Figure 3).
and thalamocortical connectivity is primarily radial, By visual inspection, both the source and scalp
these brain sources are typically compatible with ERSPs have similar spectral power patterns,
local field activity within a small (cm-scale) cortical strongly suggesting that their physiological sources
source patch. The locations of each of these source are related.
patches can be estimated with either a (patch- The occipital source cluster, however, shows
equivalent) single dipole model or, when an individ- stronger signal changes, particularly in alpha band
ual electrical head model is available, with a cortical desynchronization (blue) during the stimulus encod-
patch model. Note, however, that all approaches to ing (E) period (black dashed line boxes). Examination
ICA decomposition do not perform equally well, and of event-related signal changes and overall variance
the success of the approach depends on its applica- accounted for by each suggests that the source and
tion to a sufficient amount of well-collected and channel ERSP features do not index exactly the same
adequately preprocessed data. functional source activations. The mean decrease in
alpha power (8–12 Hz) from occipital sources during
A way forward. Many EEG researchers continue the Encoding time period accounted for 34% of
to assess only scalp channel data measures of variance (SE, 2.1%) in the ERSP for scalp channel
their data, despite the logical impediments to their Oz (Figure 3), and 43% of variance (SE, 2.8%) in the
neuroscientific interpretation. Investigators in this ERSP for the more inferior channel Iz (not shown).
position may find it useful to perform side-by-side Thus, even though the ERSP patterns (in Figure 3A
comparison of scalp and source data measures to and B) look similar, the most strongly contributing
better understand the added value of using source- and most directly underlying independent brain
resolved EEG signals. Doing so may demystify the sources accounted for only less than half the vari-
source localization process and allow deeper ance in the ERSPs for the most closely related EEG
understanding of differences between channel and scalp channels.
source space measures. As an example from our We then assessed how much variance in the raw
own work, we present further analysis of data scalp channel data was accounted for by the back-
examined in a recent report in which we used projected IC source activities in the central occipital
source-level EEG analysis to discover which cogni- cluster. This tells us to what extent these cortical
tive processes (e.g., vigilance, encoding, mainte- source activities were represented in the superven-
nance) during spatial working memory (SWM) are ing scalp channel recordings. For the midoccipital
deficient in ADHD (Lenartowicz et al., 2014). To cluster, we evaluated the mean data variance
demonstrate ways of interpreting and validating accounted for across the three scalp channels
source solutions, we examined the scalp channel showing the strongest subject group differences (Iz,
and source-resolved EEG data from two subject O1, and O2). For the TD group, the IC source cluster
groups for 1) similar time/frequency patterns, 2) on average accounted for 29.0% (SE = 2.6) of scalp
amount of overlapping variance accounted for in channel data variance; for the ADHD group this
both sets of data, and 3) association with the figure was 25.5% (SE = 2.8). Thus, the scalp projec-
cognitive/behavioral phenomena of interest. These tion of this IC source cluster accounted for a low (but
comparisons illustrate the ability to compare chan- significant) portion of data variance in the EEG
nel and source measures and to test whether signals recorded at the nearest scalp channels.
source-resolved analysis can yield more statisti- These results illustrate the expected breadth of
cally robust information. contributions of a localized cortical IC source or
Participants were 43 ADHD and 37 typically source cluster across scalp channels.
developing (TD) controls, 7-14 years of age. ICA Finally, we evaluated the extent to which the
decomposition was used to identify 12 clusters of central occipital IC source cluster ERSPs, versus
source-resolved independent component (IC) pro- ERSPs from the most closely related scalp channels,
cess activities that differed significantly across clin- could account for diagnostic group differences in the
ical groups (as validated using nonparametric spatial working task. Consistent with the alpha
bootstrap statistics with false discovery rate correc- suppression scalp distribution pattern plotted in
tion for multiple comparisons). These spatial IC Figure 3B, we used an average of the supervening Iz,
clusters were localized to midfrontal and central O1, and O2 channel ERSPs (in essence, a simpler-to-
occipital cortical areas, respectively (see Lenartowicz specify but less focused spatial filter) to estimate

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 13

(A)

(B)

Figure 3 Comparison of source-resolved versus scalp channel data. (A) One cluster of Independent component (IC) source locations and
corresponding cluster mean event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) time/frequency measures for a spatial working memory task
performed by 43 ADHD and 43 typically developing (TD) children 7–14 years of age. Results for low and high memory load conditions are
shown. (B) Equivalent ERSP measures for nearest scalp electrode channel Oz. Topographical distribution of the alpha band activity (in
black dashed boxes) during the Encoding (E) time window are shown on the cartoon heads

scalp channel-level dynamics. We performed sepa- diagnostic effects that is not available from mea-
rate repeated-measure ANOVAs, with diagnostic sures computed from only one or more of the scalp
GROUP and memory LOAD as factors, first on the channel signals. Such testing of the added value of
Encoding (E) period alpha band ERD (dotted box in source-resolved versus scalp-channel data mea-
Figure 3) for the central occipital source cluster data sures may be a desirable or necessary step for
and then for the same time window for the super- current and future EEG researchers considering the
vening scalp channel data. The results are presented adoption of advanced signal processing methods
in Figure 4. For the effects of LOAD, GROUP and who may feel reluctant to abandon their practice of
LOAD x GROUP, we observed significantly higher F basing their data analysis solely on scalp channel
values for the midoccipital source cluster than for measures.
the most closely related channel data
(FLOAD(1,69) = 26.5 vs. 8.6; FGROUP(1,69) = 12.0 vs.
9.5; FLOAD 9 GROUP(1,69) = 5.2 vs. 0.6). Thus, the
unmixed EEG signals in the source-resolved central
occipital cluster were more strongly associated with
the clinical group differences in interest. These
effects were partially masked in the scalp signals
but were unmasked by ICA source separation and
spatial source clustering.
These results demonstrate that it is possible to
compare source-resolved and scalp-channel data so
as to better understand their interrelationships.
While the source cluster and scalp-channel alpha
suppression patterns were correlated, the source-
resolved IC cluster results represented a more
coherent set of (cortical) processes, projected by
Figure 4 Independent component (IC) source activities in a
volume conduction across many of the scalp EEG
midoccipital cortical source cluster are better correlated with
channels. ICA source measures can thus yield SNR ADHD diagnostic group and memory load differences than
advantage that confers a distinct advantage for spectral power changes for the most closely related scalp channel
identifying EEG associations with cognitive and signals, Iz, O1, and O2

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


14 Sandra K. Loo, Agatha Lenartowicz, and Scott Makeig J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2016; 57(1): 4–17

Analyses that examine how well individual subjects


Future directions using advanced signal
can be typified, and the sensitivity, specificity,
processing techniques for EEG and ERP analysis
positive/negative predictive power, and receiver
We believe that adoption of a more comprehensive
operating characteristics (ROC) of the single or
analysis approach of combining time frequency
combined measure must be used to evaluate the
domain measures and analyzing with advanced
discriminative validity and utility of a candidate
signal processing techniques to separate and locate
biomarker.
physiological sources of EEG signal differences has
We anticipate that the use of advanced signal
great potential for advancing child psychiatry
processing and statistical methods to identify mul-
research. Use of these techniques can allow
tivariate classifiers will result in the identification of
researchers to more precisely understand the brain
multidimensional landscapes of interpersonal dif-
origins and mechanisms represented in collected
ferences defined by brain network dynamics, cog-
scalp EEG data. Decomposition of scalp signals into
nitive processes, and behavioral symptoms. This
separate nonbrain artifact and cortical sources can
will be more likely to map onto translational
facilitate identification of the most pertinent infor-
domains of function than the current diagnostic
mation, thus enabling researchers to develop more
categories, and will define a richer landscape within
physiologically precise biomarkers of cortical dys-
which individual and group differences can be
function. Identification of more effective biomarkers
quantified. Such ‘landscapes’ can be thought of as
can in turn improve diagnostic discrimination,
multidimensional extensions of the ‘spectrum’ con-
increase understanding of pathophysiology, and
cept of individual differences currently used in
potentially aide in monitoring and prediction of
autism research. Ideally, such multidimensional
treatment response.
approaches will open a richer space of physiologi-
Increased use of advanced signal processing tools
cally defined differences in brain dynamics in which
will allow several methodological advances in future
patient heterogeneity is captured and quantified
studies of child psychopathology and treatment.
rather than ignored. Better understanding of heter-
Because child psychiatric disorders involve signifi-
ogeneity will allow more accurate understanding of
cant clinical heterogeneity in etiology, neurobiology,
each individual relative to others with respect to
symptomatic presentation, developmental course,
development, diagnosis, treatment response, and
and treatment response, the methods and measures
cognitive function.
we use need to increase in complexity. To capture
These methodological directions will not only
the heterogeneity of the underlying sources, an
allow identification of better, more effective biomar-
increased focus on multivariate classifiers is
kers and a reconceptualization of the diagnostic and
needed. The need to move beyond use of single
treatment target from being mediated by a single
measures of any type – genetic, neurologic, cogni-
clinical category label to being defined by an indi-
tive or behavioral – as biomarkers is clearly evident
vidual’s position in a varied but coherent landscape
in the current child psychopathology literature.
of individual differences. These new methods also
This makes our suggestion to combine of various
increase the feasibility of a wider range of EEG
EEG-derived measures (ERP, spectral power but
studies. EEG study paradigms have been limited by
also phase, coherence and others) increasingly
traditional methods of EEG data analyses sensitive
relevant, not only for more comprehensive under-
to differences across EEG systems and sites (limit-
standing of the data available in the signal but also
ing multisite EEG studies) and to movement-related
to more accurately represent the heterogeneity
artifacts (limiting mobile EEG studies). Refined
within the samples and populations we seek to
electrical head models based on structural MR
study. Overall, recent studies that have utilized
images (Akalin Acar & Makeig, 2010) will be more
multivariate classifiers have had higher classifica-
commonly available, allowing ever more accurate
tion rates than studies using single measures,
localization of effective EEG sources within and then
regardless of type the measure used (candidate
across individuals. This will facilitate multimodal
genes, cognitive tests, EEG/ERP, MRI). The need for
brain imaging studies and the ability to integrate the
multivariate classifiers makes sense given the diag-
excellent temporal resolution of source-resolved
nostic and population heterogeneity evident among
EEG with information derived from other imaging
child samples and the complexity of the dysfunc-
modalities such as fMRI, diffusion tensor imaging
tional brain processes that underlie behavioral
(DTI), and MR spectroscopy. Finally, the currently
differences.
scattered resource of existing, carefully collected
To model complex relationships in the data,
EEG data sets (each analyzed until now only for the
advanced statistical methods including machine
amplitudes and latencies of a few ERP peaks or
learning, graph theory, discriminant functions, and
power in frequency bands) can be collected and
logistic regression will also be increasingly required.
subjected to collective data mining efforts that make
Many current studies focus on characterizing group
use of combined sample sizes in the thousands or
mean differences only, however, this is insufficient to
more.
determine the usefulness of a candidate biomarker.

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 15

require comparison of source-resolved EEG with


Conclusions
other measures and models of brain function. These
EEG recording and analysis is currently experiencing
and other rapidly evolving advances in data collec-
a renaissance in interest and in both range and depth
tion, signal processing and statistical analysis will
of applications, particularly in the search for end-
allow the next generation of EEG studies (multisite,
ophenotypes and biomarkers for etiologic factors,
multimodal imaging, mobile EEG, brain-computer
individual diagnosis, risk for disorder, and treatment
interface, meta-analytic, etc.) to flourish and to
response. The field of child psychopathology research
extend our insights into both normal and pathological
would benefit from increased integration of the var-
brain processes and development.
ious EEG methods previously used in isolation to
measure and model EEG signals. Improved charac-
terization of EEG signals in terms of underlying
cortical source processes, and incorporation of their Acknowledgements
time/frequency characteristics will advance EEG as a This work was supported by National Institutes of
sensitive and physiologically specific functional brain Health grants: NS80160 (SKL) MH101282 (SKL),
NS47293 (SM), and The Swartz Foundation (SM). The
imaging modality for studying cortical activation
authors have declared that they have no competing or
patterns underlying child psychopathology. Use of potential conflicts of interest.
these methods will likely lead to the identification of
multivariate classifiers using advanced statistical
analytic techniques that will result in multidimen-
sional landscapes of interindividual differences Correspondence
defined by brain network dynamics, cognitive pro- Sandra K. Loo, Semel Institute for Neuroscience and
Human Behavior, 760 Westwood Plaza, Suite 47-406,
cesses, and behavioral symptoms. Similarly, identi-
Los Angeles, CA 90095, USA; Email: SLoo@mednet.
fying network/circuit level models underlying
ucla.edu
functional domains, necessary for RDOC models, will

Key points
• There has been a shift to common genetic and mechanistic factors across psychiatric disorders; electroen-
cephalography (EEG) measures are well suited to be used as putative biomarkers within this approach.
• To be more effective biomarkers of neural circuit activity, EEG research must successfully contend with
controversies within the field regarding: (1) the use of different EEG measurements (event-related potentials,
quantitative EEG, and time frequency measures); and (2) analysis of cortical sources rather than scalp signals.
• Advances in signal processing software are making it increasingly feasible for researchers to successfully
address both controversies within their own research.
• Future directions using advanced signal processing and statistical methods will result in identification of
better, more effective biomarkers and a reconceptualization of a diagnostic or treatment target to a map of
translational domains that will define a richer landscape within which individual and group differences can be
quantified.

Note Arns, M., Conners, C.K., & Kraemer, H.C. (2013). A decade of
EEG Theta/Beta Ratio Research in ADHD: a meta-analysis.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 17, 374–383.
1. An annotated list of programs can be obtained Arnolds, D.E., Lopes da Silva, F.H., Aitink, J.W., Kamp, A.,
from the first author. & Boeijinga, P. (1980). The spectral properties of
hippocampal EEG related to behaviour in man.
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 50,
324–328.
References Atkinson, R.J., Michie, P.T., & Schall, U. (2012). Duration
Addante, R.J., Watrous, A.J., Yonelinas, A.P., Ekstrom, A.D., mismatch negativity and P3a in first-episode psychosis and
& Ranganath, C. (2011). Prestimulus theta activity predicts individuals at ultra-high risk of psychosis. Biological
correct source memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Psychiatry, 71, 98–104.
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, Bender, S., Banaschewski, T., Roessner, V., Klein, C.,
10702–10707. Rietschel, M., Feige, B., . . . & Laucht, M. (2015). Variability
Akalin Acar, Z., & Makeig, S. (2010). Neuroelectromagnetic of single trial brain activation predicts fluctuations in
forward head modeling toolbox. Journal of Neuroscience reaction time. Biological Psychology, 106, 50–60.
Methods, 190, 258–270. Berger, H. (1929). Uber das elecktrenkephalogramm des
APA (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental meschen. Archiv Fur Psychiatrie Und Nervenkrankheiten,
disorders (5th edn). Arlington, VA: Author. 87, 527–570.

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


16 Sandra K. Loo, Agatha Lenartowicz, and Scott Makeig J Child Psychol Psychiatr 2016; 57(1): 4–17

Bestelmeyer, P.E., Phillips, L.H., Crombie, C., Benson, P., & St environmental structure of prevalent forms of child and
Clair, D. (2009). The P300 as a possible endophenotype for adolescent psychopathology. Archives of General Psychiatry,
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Evidence from twin and 68, 181–189.
patient studies. Psychiatry Research, 169, 212–219. Lenartowicz, A., Delorme, A., Walshaw, P.D., Cho, A.L., Bilder,
Biomarkers Definition Working Group (2001). Biomarkers and R.M., McGough, J.J., . . . & Loo, S.K. (2014).
surrogate endpoints: Preferred definitions and conceptual Electroencephalography correlates of spatial working
framework. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 69, 89– memory deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:
95. Vigilance, encoding, and maintenance. Journal of
Buyck, I., & Wiersema, J.R. (2014). Resting electroence- Neuroscience, 34, 1171–1182.
phalogram in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Lenartowicz, A., & Loo, S.K. (2014). Use of EEG to diagnose
Developmental course and diagnostic value. Psychiatry ADHD. Current Psychiatry Reports, 16, 498.
Research, 216, 391–397. Liechti, M.D., Valko, L., Muller, U.C., Dohnert, M., Drechsler,
Buzsaki, G. (2002). Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. R., Steinhausen, H.C., & Brandeis, D. (2013). Diagnostic
Neuron, 33, 325–340. value of resting electroencephalogram in attention-deficit/
Buzsaki, G., Logothetis, N., & Singer, W. (2013). Scaling brain hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Brain
size, keeping timing: Evolutionary preservation of brain Topography, 26, 135–151.
rhythms. Neuron, 80, 751–764. Loo, S.K., Cho, A., Hale, T.S., McGough, J., McCracken, J., &
Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics, C., Lee, Smalley, S.L. (2013). Characterization of the theta to beta
S.H., Ripke, S., Neale, B.M., Faraone, S.V., Purcell, S.M., . . . ratio in ADHD: Identifying potential sources of
& Wray, N.R. (2013). Genetic relationship between five heterogeneity. Journal of Attention Disorders, 17, 384–392.
psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs. Loo, S.K., Hale, T.S., Hanada, G., Macion, J., Shrestha, A.,
Nature Genetics, 45, 984–994. McGough, J.J., . . . & Smalley, S.L. (2010). Familial
Delorme, A., Palmer, J.A., Onton, J., Oostenveld, R., & Makeig, clustering and DRD4 effects on electroencephalogram
S. (2012). Independent EEG sources are dipolar. PLoS One, measures in multiplex families with ADHD. Journal of the
i7, e30135. American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29,
van Dijk, H., Schoffelen, J.M., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. 368–377.
(2008). Prestimulus oscillatory activity in the alpha band Lopes da Silva, F.H. (1977). The cortical source of the alpha
predicts visual discrimination ability. Journal of rhythm. Neuroscience Letters, 6, 237–241.
Neuroscience, 28, 1816–1823. Luchinger, R., Michels, L., Martin, E., & Brandeis, D. (2012).
Flint, J., & Munafo, M.R. (2007). The endophenotype concept Brain state regulation during normal development: Intrinsic
in psychiatric genetics. Psychological Medicine, 37, 163– activity fluctuations in simultaneous EEG-fMRI.
180. NeuroImage, 60, 1426–1439.
Freeman, W.J. (2000). Neurodynamics: An exploration in Makeig, S. (1993). Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG
mesoscopic brain dynamics. Berlin: Springer. spectrum and effects of exposure to tones. Electroence-
Gasser, T., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., Sroka, L., Verleger, R., & phalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 86, 283–293.
Mocks, J. (1988). Development of the EEG of school-age Makeig, S., Bell, A.J., Jung, T.P., & Sejnowski, T.J. (1996).
children and adolescents. II. Topography. Electroen- Independent component analysis of electroencephalographic
cephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 69, 100–109. data. In D. Touretzky, M. Mozer, & M. Hasselmo (Eds.),
de Geus, E.J. (2010). From genotype to EEG endophenotype: A Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems (pp. 145–
route for post-genomic understanding of complex 151). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
psychiatric disease? Genome Medicine, 2, 63. Makeig, S., Debener, S., Onton, J., & Delorme, A. (2004).
Gottesman, I.I., & Gould, T.D. (2003). The endophenotype Mining event-related brain dynamics. Trends in Cognitive
concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic intentions. Sciences, 8, 204–210.
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 636–645. Makeig, S., & Onton, J. (2012). ERP features and EEG
Hall, M.H., Spencer, K.M., Schulze, K., McDonald, C., dynamics: An ICA perspective. In S. Luck, & E. Kappenman
Kalidindi, S., Kravariti, E., . . . & Rijsdijk, F. (2011). The (Eds.), Oxford handbook of event-related potential
genetic and environmental influences of event-related components (pp. 51–88). New York: Oxford University Press.
gamma oscillations on bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, Makeig, S., Westerfield, M., Jung, T.P., Enghoff, S.,
13, 260–271. Townsend, J., Courchesne, E., & Sejnowski, T.J. (2002).
Heinrich, H., Busch, K., Studer, P., Erbe, K., Moll, G.H., & Dynamic brain sources of visual evoked responses. Science,
Kratz, O. (2014). Refining the picture of reduced alerting 295, 690–694.
responses in ADHD – A single-trial analysis of event-related McLoughlin, G., Makeig, S., & Tsuang, M.T. (2014). In search
potentials. Neuroscience Letters, 582, 49–53. of biomarkers in psychiatry: EEG-based measures of brain
Jasper, H., Solomon, P., & Bradley, C. (1938). function. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B,
Electroencephalographic analyses of behavior problem Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 165B, 111–121.
children. American Journal of Psychiatry, 95, 641. McLoughlin, G., Palmer, J.A., Rijsdijk, F., & Makeig, S. (2014).
John, E.R., Ahn, H., Prichep, L., Trepetin, M., Brown, D., & Genetic overlap between evoked frontocentral theta-band
Kaye, H. (1980). Developmental equations for the phase variability, reaction time variability, and attention-
electroencephalogram. Science, 210, 1255–1258. deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a twin study.
Johnstone, S.J., Barry, R.J., & Clarke, A.R. (2013). Ten years Biological Psychiatry, 75, 238–247.
on: A follow-up review of ERP research in attention-deficit/ Meyer, A., Hajcak, G., Torpey-Newman, D.C., Kujawa, A., &
hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Neurophysiology, 124, 644– Klein, D.N. (2015). Increased ERN predicts the onset of
657. anxiety disorders between the ages of 6 and 9. Journal of
Ladouceur, C.D., Dahl, R.E., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D.A., & Abnorrmal Psychology, 124, 266–274.
Ryan, N.D. (2006). Increased error-related negativity (ERN) Meyer, A., Weinberg, A., Klein, D.N., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The
in childhood anxiety disorders: ERP and source development of the error-related negativity (ERN) and its
localization. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, relationship with anxiety: Evidence from 8 to 13 year-olds.
47, 1073–1082. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 152–161.
Lahey, B.B., Van Hulle, C.A., Singh, A.L., Waldman, I.D., & Monastra, V.J., Lubar, J.F., & Linden, M. (2001). The
Rathouz, P.J. (2011). Higher-order genetic and development of a quantitative electroencephalographic

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.


doi:10.1111/jcpp.12435 EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry 17

scanning process for attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: Shaikh, M., Dutt, A., Broome, M.R., Vozmediano, A.G.,
reliability and validity studies. Neuropsychology, 15, 136– Ranlund, S., Diez, A., . . . & Bramon, E. (2015). Sensory
144. gating deficits in the attenuated psychosis syndrome.
Mueller, A., Candrian, G., Kropotov, J.D., Ponomarev, V.A., & Schizophrenia Research, 161, 277–282.
Baschera, G.M. (2010). Classification of ADHD patients on Smalley, S.L., Loo, S.K., Yang, M.H., & Cantor, R.M. (2005).
the basis of independent ERP components using a machine Toward localizing genes underlying cerebral asymmetry and
learning system. Nonlinear Biomedical Physics, 4(Suppl 1), mental health. American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B,
S1. Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 135B, 79–84.
Mullen, T., Acar, Z.A., Worrell, G., & Makeig, S. (2011). Smit, D.J., Posthuma, D., Boomsma, D.I., & Geus, E.J. (2005).
Modeling cortical source dynamics and interactions during Heritability of background EEG across the power spectrum.
seizure. Conference Proceedings IEEE Engineering in Psychophysiology, 42, 691–697.
Medicine and Biology Society, 2011, 1411–1414. Snyder, S.M., & Hall, J.R. (2006). A meta-analysis of
Nazhvani, A.D., Boostani, R., Afrasiabi, S., & Sadatnezhad, K. quantitative EEG power associated with attention-deficit
(2013). Classification of ADHD and BMD patients using hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology,
visual evoked potential. Clinical Neurology and 23, 440–455.
Neurosurgery, 115, 2329–2335. Snyder, S.M., Quintana, H., Sexson, S.B., Knott, P., Haque,
Ogrim, G., Kropotov, J., & Hestad, K. (2012). The QEEG theta/ A.F., & Reynolds, D.A. (2008). Blinded, multi-center
beta ratio in ADHD and normal controls: Sensitivity, validation of EEG and rating scales in identifying ADHD
specificity, and behavioral correlates. Psychiatry Research, within a clinical sample. Psychiatry Research, 159, 346–
198, 482–488. 358.
Olvet, D.M., & Hajcak, G. (2008). The error-related negativity Tarasenko, M.A., Swerdlow, N.R., Makeig, S., Braff, D.L., &
(ERN) and psychopathology: Toward an endophenotype. Light, G.A. (2014). The auditory brain-stem response to
Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1343–1354. complex sounds: A potential biomarker for guiding
Pfurtscheller, G., & Aranibar, A. (1979). Evaluation of event- treatment of psychosis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 142.
related desynchronization (ERD) preceding and following Valdar, W., Solberg, L.C., Gauguier, D., Cookson, W.O.,
voluntary self-paced movement. Electroencephalography Rawlins, J.N., Mott, R., & Flint, J. (2006). Genetic and
and Clinical Neurophysiology, 46, 138–146. environmental effects on complex traits in mice. Genetics,
Pfurtscheller, G., Aranibar, A., & Maresch, H. (1979). 174, 959–984.
Amplitude of evoked potentials and degree of event-related Williams, L.M., Hermens, D.F., Thein, T., Clark, C.R., Cooper,
desynchronization (ERD) during photic stimulation. N.J., Clarke, S.D., . . . & Kohn, M.R. (2010). Using brain-
Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 47, based cognitive measures to support clinical decisions in
21–30. ADHD. Pediatric Neurology, 42, 118–126.
Saville, C.W., Feige, B., Kluckert, C., Bender, S., Biscaldi, M.,
Berger, A., . . . & Klein, C. (2015). Increased reaction time Accepted for publication: 29 April 2015
variability in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder as a First published online: 23 June 2015
response-related phenomenon: Evidence from single-trial
event-related potentials. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry, 56, 801–830.

© 2015 Association for Child and Adolescent Mental Health.

You might also like