You are on page 1of 13

Attitudes of Children With Dysphonia

*†Nadine P. Connor, *†Stacy B. Cohen, *†Shannon M. Theis,


‡Susan L. Thibeault, †Diane G. Heatley, and *†Diane M. Bless
*†Madison, Wisconsin and ‡Salt Lake City, Utah

Summary: Because voice disorders in childhood may have a negative im-


pact on communicative effectiveness, social development, and self-esteem,
the objective was to determine the impact of voice disorders on lives of chil-
dren from the perspective of chronically dysphonic children and their parents.
This study consisted of focused interviews with chronically dysphonic chil-
dren and their caregivers. Focused interviews were conducted with 10 chil-
dren in each of the following age groups: Toddler (2–4 years old), Young
Child (5–7 years old), School-Aged Child (8–12 years old), and Adolescent
(13–18 years old). Interview questions were formulated to elicit attitudes in
the following conceptual domains: emotional, social/functional, and physical.
Interviews were transcribed and subjected to systematic qualitative analyses
that identified common themes within each age group for each conceptual do-
main. For Toddlers, interviews relied heavily on parents and the biggest con-
cerns were found in the physical and functional domains. Young Children
expressed that their biggest issues related to voice were physical (‘‘run out
of air,’’ ‘‘sometimes voice does not work’’). Ninety percent of Young Chil-
dren were repeatedly asked to use a quieter voice. Emotional factors and
physical factors were prominent in the interviews of School-Aged Children
and Adolescents. Children and Adolescents often felt that their dysphonic
voice received undue attention and also limited their participation in impor-
tant events. Anger, sadness, and frustration were also expressed. Chronic
dysphonia negatively affects the lives of children. This work will serve as
the basis for development of a valid, reliable, and age-appropriate measure
of voice-related quality of life in children.
Key Words: Dysphonia—Children—Attitudes—Quality of life—Voice
disorders.

Accepted for publication September 15, 2006. University of Wisconsin, Clinical Science Center K4/711,
From the *Department of Communicative Disorders, 600 Highland Avenue, Madison, WI 53792-7375. E-mail:
University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin; connor@surgery.wisc.edu
†Department of Surgery, Otolaryngology – Head & Neck Sur- Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, pp. 197–209
gery, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, Wisconsin; 0892-1997/$34.00
and the ‡Department of Otolaryngology – Head & Neck Sur- Ó 2008 The Voice Foundation
gery, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah. doi:10.1016/j.jvoice.2006.09.005
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Nadine P.
Connor, PhD, Assistant Professor, Department of Surgery,

197
198 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

INTRODUCTION pathology. That is, it may be believed that children


Dysphonia may adversely impact a child’s gen- become dysphonic due to vocal misuse behaviors,
eral health, communicative effectiveness, social but are unaware of the resultant dysphonia and
and educational development, self-esteem, self-im- thus unmotivated to change their behaviors. How-
age, and participation in school group activities. To ever, this appealing line of reasoning is not based
this point, based on voice recordings alone, children upon systematic investigations of the attitudes and
with dysphonia were rated negatively on physical, levels of awareness of dysphonic children or their
social, and personality factors by naı̈ve, young families regarding their voices. Because phonatory
adult judges, when compared to children without disorders in children may have lasting negative ef-
dysphonia.1 Specifically, dysphonic children were fects, studies geared toward the understanding of
judged more negatively than their nondysphonic the potential challenges of childhood dysphonia
peers on all but two bipolar comparisons of appear necessary toward the goal of developing
physical and personality characteristics, such as appropriate assessment tools for voice-related
‘‘dirty-clean,’’ ‘‘bad-good,’’ ‘‘cruel-kind,’’ ‘‘worth- quality of life (voice-related QOL) that are valid,
less-valuable,’’ ‘‘dishonest-honest,’’ ‘‘sick-healthy,’’ reliable, age-appropriate, and sensitive to change.
‘‘sad-happy,’’ and ‘‘wrong-right.’’1 Very similar re- The paucity of research concerning voice-related
sults were found when normal-speaking children QOL in children may also be a reflection of the
(ages 9–11 years) served as judges,2 and also for view that children ‘‘outgrow’’ their dysphonia at
children with Down syndrome.3 It can be inferred puberty. However, recent data show that vocal nod-
from these reports that voice disruptions have a neg- ules and voice complaints persist past puberty in at
ative effect on how children are perceived by adults least 21% of adolescents.13 Accordingly, the clini-
and by their peers. It follows, then, that dysphonia cal problem of childhood dysphonia is potentially
may have a substantial negative effect on children’s long lasting and children should be included in
lives. However, this issue has not been adequately models and studies of voice-related QOL.
addressed in the medical, social science, or educa- Health-related quality of life (HR-QOL) is a mul-
tional literature. Therefore, the true impact of tidimensional, subjective concept that broadly de-
dysphonia on the lives of children is currently scribes an individual’s well-being and satisfaction
unknown and warrants study. with their health and health-related functioning in
Childhood dysphonias have an estimated inci- daily life. For measurement purposes, HR-QOL
dence of 6–24%.4–7 Accordingly, over 1 million is generally partitioned into conceptual domains
children in the United States may have voice disor- that include physical, emotional, functional, or
ders.5 Although the clinical problem of childhood other factors.14–17 Questionnaire instruments based
dysphonia appears significant, attitudes of dys- upon the conceptual domains allow access to the
phonic children or their families concerning patient’s perspective of their health. Prior to devel-
voice-related physical, social/functional, and emo- oping items for a questionnaire, however, it is crit-
tional challenges have not been systematically ical to obtain an unbiased view of the attitudes,
examined. feelings, and experiences of patients who live
The dearth of information concerning the atti- with the health-related signs and symptoms of
tudes of children with dysphonia may be a function interest.
of the commonly held belief that dysphonic chil- A great deal of research has been performed con-
dren do not have an awareness8 or concern about cerning HR-QOL and voice-related QOL in adult
their vocal quality, and thus are not motivated to populations.18–22 However, the general HR-QOL
seek or follow through on behavioral treatment.9,10 of children has been understudied23–26 and voice-
This belief may be based upon reports of a high rate related QOL research has followed a similar trend
of voice misuse behaviors among children,9–12 and of excluding children. The relative ‘‘invisibility’’24
an unawareness on the part of children of the puta- of children in models and studies of voice-related
tive linkage of phonotraumatic behaviors to vocal QOL may be due to the same types of factors that
affect the study of general HR-QOL. Among these

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 199

factors is the complexity of examining patients who years old, (2) Young Child: 5–7 years old, (3)
undergo such rapid developmental changes in such School-Aged Child: 8–12 years old, and (4) Adoles-
a short period of time.24,25 Childhood is a period cent: 13–18 years old. These age groupings coin-
of dynamic change and necessitates the use of cided with those from the PedsQL 4.0, a validated
age-appropriate items in questionnaire instru- measure of general HR-QOL.23,30 Inclusion criteria
ments.17,24,25 When developing an instrument to included: (1) presence of at least mild dysphonia,
study HR-QOL in children, a single instrument ver- as assessed by a speech-language pathologist, for at
sion with static questions or questions designed for least the previous 6 months, (2) cognitive and/or lan-
an adult audience cannot be applied uniformly guage skills that allow responses to structured inter-
across the childhood age range. Doing so would views or written questionnaires, although limited
be problematic because different components of responses by toddlers were considered acceptable,
HR-QOL may be manifested or emphasized by (3) effective speakers of English, and (4) free from
a toddler, a teenager, or an adult.24,25 However, other comorbid conditions at time of interview. Prior
the task of developing multiple versions of an in- voice treatment or medical/surgical management for
strument for different ages is inherently complex laryngeal diseases or disorders was not exclusionary.
and must begin with interviews or focus groups of Subject characteristics for each age group are found
children and their caregivers across the childhood in Table 1.
age range. Previous studies of voice-related QOL The Toddlers and Young Children were inter-
in children geared toward instrument development viewed together with their caregivers. Caregivers
did not incorporate the essential step of interview- consisted of a parent or guardian for 37 of the 40
ing children with voice disorders and their care- children interviewed. For one child in the Young
givers, relied solely on parent proxy reporting, Child group, a teacher, rather than a parent, was in-
and used a single set of questions for all children terviewed. In the Adolescent group, the parents of
between the ages of 2 and 18 years.27–29 These lim- two adolescents were not available to participate
itations must be considered when interpreting the in the interviews.
results of these previous studies, and when using School-Aged Children and Adolescents were in-
the instruments derived from the research. terviewed independently from caregivers. Children
Our hypothesis was that voice-related functional, were: (1) patients who presented to the Pediatric
emotional, and physical challenges would be ex- Otolaryngology Clinic at the University of Wiscon-
pressed by dysphonic children and their families. sin or the University of Utah with chronic hoarse-
Furthermore, it was hypothesized that attitudes ness, (2) children referred via the Madison
and feelings expressed by Toddlers or their care- Metropolitan School District (MMSD; Madison,
givers (ages 2–4 years), Young Children (ages 5–7 WI), or (3) children who were recruited via flyers
years), School-Aged Children (ages 8–12 years), placed in the waiting room of a general pediatrics
and Adolescents (ages 13–18 years) would be quali- clinic. All children agreed to participate after in-
tatively distinct from each other. Accordingly, the formed consent was obtained from their parents/
aim of our study was to determine the impact of voice guardians. Interviewers were speech-language pa-
disorders on lives of children from the perspective of thologists at the University of Wisconsin or Univer-
children and their caregivers. sity of Utah. Prior to the start of data acquisition,
a presentation and training session was provided to
all involved clinicians regarding study procedures,
METHODS interview techniques, and recording methods. Eight
Focused interviews of 40 dysphonic children speech-language pathologists participated in the
(ages 2–18 years) and their parents or caregivers interviews. Seven of the eight speech-language
were performed to determine the main components pathologists specialized in voice disorders, while
affecting voice-related challenges. Ten children and one interviewer was a student clinician.
their parents/caregivers from each of the following Medical diagnosis and etiology of the dysphonia
age groups were interviewed: (1) Toddler: 2–4 were not controlled or stratified, and were not

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


200 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

TABLE 1. Subject Characteristics in Each Age Group


Mean Age Gender Diagnoses Dysphonia Severity
(SD) Distribution (Number of Subjects) (Number of Subjects)

Toddler (ages 2–4 years) 3.6 (0.82) Six males, Vocal nodules: 7 Mild: 2
four females Unspecified chronic Mild-moderate: 1
dysphonia: 3 Moderate: 6
No rating: 1
Young Child 6.3 (0.91) Seven males, Vocal nodules: 6 Mild: 2
(ages 5–7 years) three females Papillomatosis: 2 Mild-moderate: 1
Unspecified chronic Moderate: 2
dysphonia: 2 Moderate-severe: 4
Severe: 1
School-Aged Child 9.9 (1.4) Five males, Vocal nodules: 4 Mild: 3
(ages 8–12 years) five females Papillomatosis: 2 Mild-moderate: 2
Chronic tonsillitis: 1 Moderate: 4
Vocal fold cyst: 1 Moderate-severe: 1
Reflux laryngitis: 1
Unspecified chronic
dysphonia: 1
Adolescent 15.6 (1.5) One male, Vocal nodules: 4 Mild: 5
(ages 13–18 years) nine females Papillomatosis: 1 Moderate-severe: 4
Vocal fold paralysis: 2 Severe: 1
Mixed connective
tissue disease: 1
Unspecified chronic
dysphonia: 1
Diagnoses, when available, were provided by the pediatric otolaryngologists involved in the clinical care of the patients. Dysphonia
ratings were performed by the speech-language pathologist performing the interview during the live interview session.

obtained for the MMSD participants. We did not about 15 minutes. What we would like to talk about
limit participation to particular dysphonia etiolo- is your (your child’s) scratchy (hoarse) voice and
gies because our intention was to allow acquisition how that might or might not be a problem for you
of a broad range of perspectives. (your child) and your family. Please feel free to
A preliminary conceptual model was incorpo- give us lots of examples and descriptions of how
rated into this work that was similar to a model you feel because that is what we are interested in
used by others in voice-related outcomes in dys- here – your opinions and experiences.’’
phonic adults.20 The conceptual model included An interview script, shown in Table 2, was devel-
three domains: (1) impact of voice on child’s phys- oped to guide the focused interviews, but questions
ical functioning, (2) impact of voice on child’s so- were not read to subjects and the exact wording and
cial/functional aspects combined, and (3) impact of emphasis within the questions varied. Throughout
voice on child’s emotional functioning. Initial the interviews, the speech-language pathologist in-
structured interviews of dysphonic children and terviewers developed a rapport with the subjects
their parents focused on these empirical domains and encouraged children and parents to provide ap-
as related to voice symptoms, difficulties faced in propriate responses whether ‘‘yes/no,’’ narrative-
various activities and with various people due to style scenarios, anecdotes, or other descriptions of
the voice disorder, other related problem areas, how they felt about the voice disorder and how it
and environmental or behavioral covariables. affected their function, school performance, and
A constant set of instructions was read to all sub- family interactions. After each subject’s response,
jects: ‘‘We are going to have a conversation for the interviewer probed further with follow-up

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 201

TABLE 2. Interview Script Items by Conceptual Domain


Physical Social/Functional Emotional

 Does your throat hurt when you talk?  Do people ask what is ‘‘wrong’’  Does your scratchy (hoarse) voice make
(or, Do you get a sore throat when with your voice? you feel sad or angry?
you talk?)  Do kids tease you or comment on  Does your voice make you feel badly or
 If a sore throat is a problem for you your voice? frustrated or embarrassed?
when you talk, what do you do?  Do people, like your parents or  Does your scratchy voice make you
 Is your voice better if you eat certain teachers, ask you to use a quieter worry that you are sick?
foods, voice or ‘‘bug’’ you about using a  What is the biggest problem you have
or if you keep yourself from eating or quieter voice? related to your voice? (sore throat,
drinking certain things?  If people ask you to use a quieter run out of air, use a lot of energy to
 Do you have a hot or burning sensation voice, how does this make you feel? talk, can’t depend on my voice
in your throat sometimes? How often?  Does your voice sound different than working, feel ‘‘different’’
 What is the biggest problem you have your friends or other kids at school? from other kids)
related to your voice? (sore throat,  Do you have speech or voice therapy?  Is there anything that you would
run out of air, use a lot of energy to Do you have speech or voice like to tell me about your voice?
talk, can’t depend on my therapy at school?
voice working, feel ‘‘different’’ from  Does it make you feel good or
other kids) bad to get extra help from your
 Is there anything that you would like teacher or speech-language
to tell me about your voice? pathologist?
 What is the biggest problem you have
related to your voice? (sore throat,
run out of air, use a lot of energy to
talk, can’t depend on my voice
working, feel ‘‘different’’ from
other kids)
 Is there anything that you would
like to tell me about your voice?
When parents were interviewed, ‘‘your child’’ was substituted for ‘‘your.’’ After each subject’s response, the interviewer probed fur-
ther with follow-up questions, including ‘‘How does this make you feel?’’ ‘‘How do you usually react to this (What do you do)?’’
‘‘Has this changed the way you do things at school (or home, or specific environment being discussed)?’’ Questions in italics were
general questions that fell into each of the 3 conceptual domains.

questions, including ‘‘How does this make you represented in columns, and respondents in rows.
feel?’’ ‘‘How do you usually react to this (What Separate spreadsheets were created for parents
do you do)?’’ ‘‘Has this changed the way you do and child/adolescent respondents for each of the
things at school (or home, or specific environment four age groups, resulting in eight separate spread-
being discussed)?’’ sheets. Responses from children and parents to each
All interviews were audiorecorded on a Sony question were read from the individual transcripts
portable minidisc recorder (Model # MZ-B50; and then charted on the spreadsheet either as affir-
Sony, New York, NY) placed on the examination mative or negative when ‘‘yes/no’’ questions were
room desk with an omnidirectional microphone. In- asked (for example, ‘‘Does your throat hurt when
terviews were then transcribed. Transcripts were re- you talk?’’) or with key content words when narra-
viewed by the investigators to develop a content tive answers were requested (for example, ‘‘If
analysis of the major themes revealed by the chil- a sore throat is a problem for you when you talk,
dren and their parents. To perform the content anal- what do you do?’’; a frequent response was ‘‘drink
ysis, a chart was created using commercially water,’’ which was entered into the spreadsheet in
available spreadsheet software (Microsoft Excel the appropriate row and column). Three of the in-
2003; Microsoft, Redmond, WA) that had questions vestigators (NPC, SBC, DGH) performed the

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


202 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

content analysis separately. Reliability analyses to both parents and children, and thus did not allow
were performed by a fourth author not involved in acquisition of responses from both parents and chil-
the initial review (DMB) by reviewing two ran- dren to a common question. For the Toddlers and
domly selected interviews from each of the age Young Children, the conversational transcripts did
groups. As such, eight interviews were reviewed not allow the opportunity to reliably assess corre-
again for reliability. Percent agreement of the reli- spondence between child and parent responses to
ability analysis with the original review was 87.5%. questions. This was a limitation of the interview
Trends across responses were evaluated as the methodology not found for the School-Aged Chil-
percentage of ‘‘yes’’ responses to the ‘‘yes/no’’ dren and Adolescents, because interviews with par-
questions asked of each child and parent. Percent ents or caregivers were performed separately from
agreement between parent-child dyads was calcu- those conducted with the child or adolescent. The
lated for each yes/no question for the School- result of performing separate interviews was that
Aged Children and Adolescents. Due to the nature all questions were asked, the child’s/adolescent’s
of this qualitative analysis, no statistical tests were perspective could be obtained without involvement
performed. from the parents/caregivers and correspondence of
responses could be assessed.
As shown in Table 3, the interviews yielded a se-
RESULTS ries of consistent themes within each of the concep-
Children within the Toddler and Young Child tual domains. For the physical domain, consistent
groups were interviewed together with their parents themes were focused on the increased effort used
or caregivers. We performed these interviews to produce voice and limitations in activities due
jointly to reduce any potential anxiety within the to voice. Across age groups, 32.5% of children
children and adults participating in the study. As drank water to help alleviate the voice problem or
such, the same questions were often not repeated associated sore throat. Social/functional domain

TABLE 3. Results: Summary of Themes by Conceptual Domain Across Age Groups


Physical Social/Functional Emotional

 Can’t be heard  Tired of answering questions about voice  People ask what is wrong with child’s voice
 Can’t sing (high notes)  Voice is different from peers  Reports of anger, sadness, depression,
 Sore throat associated with voice  Not wanting to sound like a smoker irritation, nervous (re: not being able to
 Voice ‘‘cuts out’’  Social exclusion because people can’t participate in activities)
 Drinking water helps voice understand them  Reports of embarrassment due to voice
 ‘‘Runs out of air’’  Afraid or hesitant to participate in class  Reports of reductions in self-esteem
 Increased effort for voicing (reading aloud is bothersome)  People laugh when voice is ‘‘squeaky’’
(must ‘‘push to get voice out’’);  Fear of ‘‘sounding dumb’’  Does not like the sound of own voice
strain  Other kids tease the dysphonic child by  Frustration experienced due to voice
 Hot burning sensation in throat attempting to imitate
 Sometimes voice ‘‘does not work’’  ‘‘Missing out on things’’ due to voice
 Receives undue attention due to voice
 Toddlers, Young Child, and School-Aged
groups asked to use quieter voice
 Adolescents asked to ‘‘speak up’’
 Children and others comment that the
child’s voice sounds hoarse or scratchy
 Others can’t understand the child, which
leads to frustration, behavior problems
 Talking on the phone is uncomfortable

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 203

themes were centered on altered peer interactions we give her [the child] too many Camels [ciga-
associated with the dysphonia and fears of appear- rettes] – that she [the child] sounds like a smoker.’’
ing different from peers, which may contribute to As shown in Figure 1, the most frequently ex-
hesitation in participating in classroom activities. pressed concerns appeared to be in the emotional
Within the emotional domain, feelings of anger, and social/functional domains for toddlers. Specif-
sadness, embarrassment, depression, irritation, ner- ically, 70% of toddlers and parents felt that their
vousness, and reduced self-esteem were expressed. child’s voice was different from other children,
Results specific to each age group are discussed while feelings of sadness, anger, frustration, or em-
below. barrassment related to the voice were shown by
30% of children.
Toddler (ages 2–4 years) All of the parents of toddlers (100%) reported
In the Toddler interviews, we relied to a large ex- that they and the child’s day care providers fre-
tent on the parent interview. For one subject, the quently asked the toddlers to be quieter or to use
parent was the only respondent although the child a quieter voice. The toddlers confirmed this obser-
was directly addressed. For the remaining nine tod- vation with 78% of respondents indicating that they
dler subjects, short responses (range of 1–20 words were frequently asked to use a quiet voice. Yelling
per response) were received and integrated into our reportedly resulted in greater impairments in voice
findings. quality. One parent noted, ‘‘He strains his voice and
Most parents (60%) felt that their children were that’s one thing that’s been recommended – that we
not aware of their dysphonia. When asked to iden- try to have [him] use a quiet voice.’’
tify the biggest problem or concern associated with Parents expressed that listeners appear to have
their child’s voice, parents’ responses fell into the difficulty understanding the child and that the voice
physical domain with reports of sore throats and disorder appears to contribute to frustration and be-
running out of breath when speaking. Parents also havior problems. One parent was frustrated that the
expressed concern that their child’s voice sounded child’s pediatrician was not responsive to questions
hoarse or scratchy and that other people tended to regarding the dysphonia, indicating that she could
comment on the voice disorder. One mother of not get any ‘‘attention’’ or ‘‘airplay’’ directed to-
a toddler commented, ‘‘Grandma teases us that ward the voice disorder. Another parent commented

Responses to Interview Questions


Toddler
Young
100% School-Age
Adolescent
80%
Percent "Yes"

60%

40%

20%

0%
E E E P P SF SF SF SF
Sad? Angry? Voice different Worry you are Throat hurt? Hot, burning Certain foods People ask Kids tease or Do people
Frustrated? from other sick because Sore? sensation? affect voice? about voice? comment? want you to
Embarassed? kids? of voice? use quieter
voice?

FIGURE 1. Percent of subjects who answered ‘‘yes’’ to questions are summarized on the x-axis (complete
questions may be found in Table 2). For toddlers, combined responses from parents and children were
counted, whereas only responses from children are shown for the Young Child, School-Aged Child, and
Adolescent groups. E 5 emotional domain; P 5 physical domain; and SF 5 social/functional domain.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


204 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

that she was told that her child had to have general indicated that other people asked them about or
anesthesia for an endoscopic examination of the vo- commented on their voices. Approximately 50%
cal folds, and that she considered general anesthe- of the children felt that their voice was different
sia to be a barrier to obtaining an examination. from that of their peers. With regard to using
She was frustrated when she learned that there a loud voice, 80% reported that adults routinely
were other options because she had invested in asked them to use a quieter voice. A hot or burning
three previous physician appointments for her child sensation in the throat was reported by 40% of chil-
before the endoscopic examination was performed. dren in this age group. Five children saw a speech-
Three children had seen a speech-language pathol- language pathologist for their dysphonia or other
ogist for the voice problem, or for other communi- communicative disorder. Use of medication for gas-
cative disorders, such as language impairment. It is troesophageal or extraesophageal reflux was not
possible that participation in voice treatment may noted for any child.
have affected or altered perception of the voice dis-
order in these participants. School-Aged Child (ages 8–12 years)
Two children were taking medication for gastro- Children and parents were interviewed sepa-
esophageal or extraesophageal reflux. rately. When asked to list their biggest concerns re-
lated to voice, children listed physical concerns,
Young Child (ages 5–7 years) such as ‘‘getting the voice out,’’ soreness in the
Much greater interview participation was re- throat associated with voice use, and running out
ceived from the children within this age group dur- of air. One child with a moderate voice disruption
ing the interviews. Utterance length ranged from 1 and vocal fold nodules commented, ‘‘I didn’t like
to 47 words across children and responses were al- the sound of it and how it sounded when I talk.
ways received when requested in the conversational When I breathe and [when I am] talking it just stops
give and take. Parents and children were inter- right in the middle of a sentence.’’ When asked how
viewed together for this age group but very little in- he felt when his voice ‘‘stops,’’ this child re-
put was received from the parents during interview sponded, ‘‘I feel mad.’’ Another child noted,
sessions, presumably due to adequate participation ‘‘.sometimes I lose my voice completely and I
from the children themselves. In one instance, get frustrated because I’m like ‘ahhhhhh,’ and noth-
a teacher rather than a parent was interviewed ing comes out.’’ However, as shown in Figure 1,
with a child. An emerging awareness of the dyspho- only 40% of these children reported feeling sad-
nia was observed by one parent, who commented, dened, angered, frustrated, or embarrassed by their
‘‘. he brings it up once in a while, but he is voice disorder.
more curious saying, ‘Sometimes my voice does As shown in Figure 1, 70% of children reported
not work right.’’’ a sore throat associated with voice use. When asked
The biggest concerns in this age group were what strategies were used for the sore throat, 60%
physical, including running out of air and difficulty reported drinking water to help improve their voice
initiating voice without a great deal of effort (de- quality or make their throat feel better. A hot or
scribed as ‘‘hard to push the voice out’’). Children burning sensation in the throat was noted by 60%
also noted that sometimes the voice ‘‘didn’t work,’’ of children in this age group. Three children were
and they experienced a sore throat associated with taking medications for gastroesophageal or laryng-
voicing. One young boy commented, ‘‘. some- opharyngeal reflux. Sixty percent had seen
times when I talk, I can’t even talk.’’ Four of the a speech-language pathologist for treatment of the
children received relief from some of these factors voice disorder.
by drinking water. Fifty percent of the children re- In the social/function domain, 60% of children in
ported a sore throat associated with voice use. this age group reported that others commented on
As shown in Figure 1, 50% of the children indi- their voices. Questions from their peers included in-
cated that they were saddened, angered, or frus- quiries about whether vocal nodules were conta-
trated by their voice. Half of the children gious, or simply ‘‘Why can’t you talk?’’ For the

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 205

dysphonic school-aged child, comments by others ‘‘Do you have a hot or burning sensation in your
about the voice were sometimes associated with throat sometimes?’’ to 83.3% for the question
feelings of embarrassment, nervousness, and fear ‘‘Does your voice sound different than your friends
to participate in class because the ‘‘voice will or other kids at school?’’
give out.’’ Forty percent of the school-age children
reported those feelings. Children who expressed Adolescent (ages 13–18 years)
frustration with their voices and were hesitant to When asked to identify the biggest problem with
participate at school also felt that they were missing their voices, the adolescents focused on physical
out on things because of their voices. Children also factors, such as an inability to be heard, a sore
commented that it was bothersome to them to read throat, difficulty singing, and increased effort for
aloud in class. A parent expressed annoyance at talking. Sixty percent of the adolescents were in-
comments from the public about her daughter’s volved in aspects of vocal performance (one adoles-
‘‘sexy voice,’’ because the parent felt it was inap- cent was a cheerleader) and the dysphonia
propriate to describe a young child in that manner. interfered with their activities.
Another parent noted, ‘‘It makes him sad because As shown in Figure 1, the most frequently ex-
he wants to talk like normal kids and not be hoarse pressed concerns were in the emotional domain, in-
all of the time.’’ Some incidents were described cluding feelings of sadness, nervousness, anger,
where the child received undue attention due to embarrassment, or frustration (80%) and reports
voice or felt ‘‘humiliated’’ at school by substitute that the voice was different from that of peers
teachers who did not understand the child’s vocal (90%). A negative effect on self-esteem was espe-
limitations. Eighty-nine percent (8/9) of children cially noted by one adolescent. Frustration at hav-
reported that they felt their voice was different ing repeated surgeries was noted for the subjects
from voices of their peers. with recurrent respiratory papillomatosis. Fear of
Ninety percent of children were asked to use not being able ‘‘to get words out’’ was also ex-
a quieter voice. One child with recurrent respiratory pressed. Being left out of things or feeling isolated
papillomatosis was frequently asked to ‘‘speak up.’’ was associated with frustration for adolescents. One
Another parent mentioned that her child would not parent noted that people tended to leave her adoles-
use a voice amplifier at school due to the attention cent out of conversations because they could not
that the amplifier brought to him. One parent com- understand her when she responded. The parent
mented, ‘‘Just wave a magic wand and make it go said, ‘‘It is a big deal if you can’t communicate.
away.’’ You don’t mean to leave her out, but it is hard to
In contrast to the concerns voiced by most of the communicate with someone you can’t understand.
children, one child with a moderate voice disorder She can’t communicate with people and that makes
and vocal fold cyst was very clear that his voice her insane.’’
disorder was not important in his life. This child Social/functional factors that were expressed in-
said, ‘‘What doesn’t matter to me is cause I got cluded fear that people would laugh when the voice
something wrong in my vocal folds. And the thing was ‘‘squeaky.’’ One adolescent expressed that she
that matters to me is ‘nothing.’ It never hurts, it did not want to sound like a smoker. Eighty percent
never does anything. Frankly, I didn’t even know of the adolescents reported that people often asked
it was there before I came here and nobody said them about their dysphonia, and found it ‘‘tiring’’
it was there. It basically never mattered to me. to continue answering questions about their voice.
Well, it mattered to me, but it’s really never done One adolescent commented, ‘‘. sometimes people
anything to change me.’’ This child did not like at- will ask if I’m sick or something and sometimes I
tending voice therapy, commenting, ‘‘I really don’t say ‘yes’ because I don’t want to explain the whole
like leaving school in the middle of the day to come thing.’’
to see anybody. No offense to you.’’ Forty percent of adolescents reported a sore
Percent agreement between children and parents throat associated with voice use, and felt that drink-
to questions ranged from 33.3% for the question ing water helped to alleviate the problem. One

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


206 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

adolescent with vocal fold nodules and a mild dys- Importantly, specific dysphonia-related attitudes
phonia noted, ‘‘It just hurts right along my vocal expressed by children and parents varied across
cords, right along my throat and when I talk you the childhood age range. For instance, children in
can feel it hurting, but sometimes you get so caught the Toddler, Young Child, and School-Aged Child
up in what you are talking about you can’t feel it.’’ groups were frequently asked to speak more qui-
Only two adolescents were asked to use quieter etly, while adolescents were not. The reasons for
voice, while three adolescents were frequently asked this difference are unclear, but could possibly be at-
to ‘‘speak up.’’ One adolescent with vocal paralysis tributed to qualities that may be inherent in adoles-
indicated, ‘‘Sometimes my teachers will tell me to cents, such as shyness, or to differences in the
talk louder and I would tell them that I can’t.’’ gender or diagnosis distributions among the child
Sixty percent of the adolescents in our sample and adolescent groups. Specifically, the Adolescent
saw a speech-language pathologist for treatment group was largely female, in comparison to the
of the voice disorder. A parent mentioned that their other age groups, and also contained two partici-
adolescent would not use a voice amplifier because pants with vocal fold paralysis, that is often associ-
she did not wish to draw attention to her voice ated with a quiet, breathy voice quality. Young and
problem. Two adolescents were taking medication school-aged children also complained more fre-
for gastroesophageal or extraesophageal reflux, quently of a sore throat associated with voicing
and one adolescent practiced behavioral manage- than adolescents, or as reported by the parents of
ment techniques for these conditions. toddlers. In the adolescents, a large increase in
Percent agreement between adolescents and par- the frequency of reports of anger, sadness, embar-
ents to questions ranged from 37.3% for the ques- rassment, or nervousness due to the voice was
tion ‘‘Does your scratchy voice make you worry found relative to the other age groups. As such, re-
that you are sick?’’ with both 40% of children ductions and concerns related to physical function-
and parents answering ‘‘yes,’’ to 100% for the ques- ing were most dominant in toddlers, young
tion ‘‘Does your voice sound different than your children, and school-aged children. While physical
friends or other kids at school?’’ function was an important element in adolescents,
concern was also expressed related to social isola-
tion, anger, sadness, and embarrassment. Accord-
DISCUSSION ingly, it is clear that qualitative differences in
Through focused interviews of dysphonic chil- voice-related challenges are present across the age
dren and their families, we found that chronic dys- range. Therefore, these challenges should be
phonia negatively affects the lives of children viewed dynamically throughout childhood and
across the domains of physical, social/functional, adolescence. When developing an instrument for
and emotional performance. Across all age groups, measuring voice-related QOL, a static set of
children and adolescents expressed that their voices questionnaire items intended for all ages will not
were different from their peers, and that people properly reflect the changes in voice-related atti-
tended to comment about their dysphonic voices. tudes present across the age range. Age-appropriate
Accordingly, a large proportion of our sample of items are necessary.
40 children expressed concerns and an awareness Using accepted criteria for defining HR-QOL
of their dysphonia, including children as young as and disease-specific quality of life, it is critical to
6 years old. These findings are in contrast to the actually obtain the patient’s perspective as part of
commonly held belief that young or school-aged the validation process. The child’s perspective has
children do not appear aware of or concerned about been lacking in pediatric health services research,
how their voices sound.8,9 This belief, therefore, in general, and also specifically in the study of
appears suspect and suggests that careful examina- voice-related QOL.16,25 The differences in parent
tion, in a systemic fashion, of all elements and be- and child responses in this study imply that the
liefs in the treatment of childhood dysphonia must child’s perspective is critical to understanding
be subjected to systematic investigation. the impact of the dysphonia. The first step in

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 207

development of a validated, child-centered instru- perceptions of the child’s HR-QOL are influential
ment that addresses HR-QOL is focused interviews, in whether or not treatment for the health condition
either individually or in groups, with children who is sought in the first place, and young children or
represent the affected population, and techniques those with severe illness may not be able to com-
for performing these interviews have been de- plete the measure. As such, both patient self-report
scribed,31 including effective methods for use and parent proxies should be used in parallel.16
with young children32,33 and for addressing com- This study is the first step in developing a valid,
municative impairment in children.34 This impor- reliable, responsive, and age-appropriate instrument
tant first step was not performed in previous work for measuring voice-related QOL in children and
concerning the development of voice-related QOL adolescents. We aim to develop distinct versions
instruments for use in pediatric populations.27–29 of the instrument for both parent proxy and child
Without actually asking for and obtaining patient response for each of the age groups reported in
attitudes, a valid measure of disorder-specific qual- this study, except for the toddler instrument that
ity of life may not be obtained.17 As noted recently will rely solely on parent proxy. Interviews of dys-
by Ronen et al35 ‘‘Without including children in the phonic children and their caregivers demonstrated
main stage of HR-QOL research, we believe that that across the age range children experience reduc-
children’s fundamental beliefs, feelings, and under- tions in vocal functioning and potentially in voice-
standing of their disorders and their interface with related QOL. Because there is a dearth of research
society might not be revealed.’’ While the proce- concerning the effect of dysphonia on the lives of
dures for obtaining these attitude data are rigorous, children, the implications of these findings are sig-
reiterative, and laborious, there is no substitute or nificant. For instance, the impact of a voice disorder
shortcut. Listening to and incorporating the pa- on a child’s life may be underestimated and thus
tient’s perspective is a critical part of the process. may contribute to delays in seeking intervention
Although it is tempting to rely on our own experi- for the voice disorder. Such delays in seeking
ences as clinicians working daily with these pa- help may result in a chronic voice disorder that
tients,36 we may be surprised at the influence of limits scholastic, social, and occupational opportu-
our own biases in item development. Adults and nities and negatively influences the perceptions of
children have a fundamental difference in what others regarding the dysphonic individual. Further-
constitutes quality of life.17 more, knowledge of factors that influence the lives
Prior work examining voice-related QOL has re- of children has implications for developing focused
lied solely on use of parent reporting.27–29 The use interventions geared toward improvement of the
of parent proxy reporting alone for obtaining well-being of children.16
measures of HR-QOL is controversial because the
responses of children and their parents have not Acknowledgments: The authors are grateful for the as-
been found to match in prior research studies of sistance of Ms. Chris Gustafson, Dr. J. Scott McMurray,
HR-QOL25,37–39 or in this study. As stated in a re- Dr. Marshall Smith, Dr. Doug Montequin, Dr. James
Varni, Dr. Tasha Burwinkle, Ms. Amy Kramer, Ms.
cent review article, ‘‘the paucity of self-report data
Cara Sauder, Dr. Kristine Tanner, and Ms. Melissa
remains a problem in studying health outcomes in
Buchanan in the completion of this work. This study
younger children.’’25 Disparities have been found was supported by a grant from the National Institute of
among self-reports by children, adolescents, and Deafness and Other Communication Disorders
their parents, teachers and health care providers, (R03DC04943).
both for healthy children and for children with
chronic diseases.16,37,38 As suggested by Eiser
(1997), the basis for this cross-informant variance17
may be due to differences between adults and chil- REFERENCES
dren in their understanding of health, illness, and 1. Ruscello DM, Lass NJ, Podbesek J. Listeners’ perceptions
treatment. On the other hand, valid and reliable par- of normal and voice-disordered children. Folia Phoniatr
ent proxy responses can be valuable because parent (Basel). 1988;40:290–296.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


208 NADINE P. CONNOR ET AL

2. Lass NJ, Ruscello DM, Stout LL, Hoffmann FM. Peer per- handicap index (VHI): development and validation. Am J
ceptions of normal and voice-disordered children. Folia Speech Lang Pathol. 1998;6:66–70.
Phoniatr (Basel). 1991;43:29–35. 21. Deary IJ, Wilson JA, Carding PN, MacKenzie K, Voi SS.
3. Moran MJ, LaBarge JM, Haynes WO. Effect of voice qual- A patient-derived voice symptom scale. J Psychosom
ity on adult’s perspective of Down’s syndrome children. Res. 2003;54:483–489.
Folia Phoniatr (Basel). 1988;40:157–161. 22. Rosen CA, Lee AS, Osbourne J, Zullo T, Murry T. Devel-
4. Carding PN, Roulstone S, Northstone K, Study Team AL- opment and validation of the voice handicap index-10. La-
SPAC. The prevalence of childhood dysphonia: a cross- ryngoscope. 2004;114:1549–1556.
sectional study. J Voice. 2006;20:623–630. 23. Varni JW, Seid M, Rode CA. The pedsQL: measurement
5. Choi SS, Zalzal GH. Voice disorders. In: Cummings CW, model for the pediatric quality of life inventory. Med
Fredrickson JM, Harker LA, Krause CJ, Richardson MA, Care. 1999;37:126–139.
Schuller DE, eds. Pediatric Otolaryngology. St. Louis, 24. Gidwani P, Sobo EJ, Seid M, Kurtin PS. Laying the foun-
MO: Mosby CD-Online; 1999. dation: identifying major issues in applied child health ser-
6. Wilson DK. Voice Disorders in Children. 2nd ed. Balti- vices research. In: Sobo EJ, Kurtin PS, eds. Child Health
more, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1979. Services Research: Applications, Innovations, and In-
7. Wilson DK. Management of voice disorders in children sights. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2003:25–63.
and adolescents. Semin Speech Lang. 1983;4:245. 25. Christakis DA, Johnston BD, Connell FA. Methodological
8. Andrews ML. In: Voice Therapy for Children. New York, issues in pediatric outcomes research. Ambul Pediatr.
NY: Longman; 1986:7–8, 162–164. 2001;1:59–62.
9. Gray SD, Smith ME, Schneider H. Voice disorders in chil- 26. Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. The pedsQL 4.0: reliability
dren. Pediatr Clin North Am. 1996;43:1357–1384. and validity of the pediatric quality of life inventory ver-
10. Hirschberg J, Dejonckere PH, Hirano M, Mori K, Schultz- sion 4.0 generic core scales in healthy and patient popula-
Coulon HJ, Vrticka K. Voice disorders in children. Int J tions. Med Care. 2001;39:800–812.
Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 1995;32:S109–S125. 27. Hartnick CJ. Validation of a pediatric voice quality of life
11. Dobres R, Lee L, Stemple JC, Kummer AW, instrument. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2002;128:
Kretschmer LW. Description of laryngeal pathologies in 919–922.
children evaluated by otolaryngologists. J Speech Hear 28. Hartnick CJ, Volk M, Cunningham M. Establishing norma-
Disord. 1990;55:526–532. tive voice-related quality of life scores within the pediatric
12. Corbin-Lewis K, Johnson TS. Pediatric voice disorders. In: otolaryngology population. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck
Kent RD, Ball MJ, eds. Voice Quality Measurement. San Surg. 2003;129:1090–1093.
Diego, CA: Thomson Learning; 2000:431–447. 29. Boseley ME, Cunningham MJ, Volk MS, Hartnick CJ.
13. De Bodt MS, Ketelslangers K, Peeters T, et al. Evolution Validation of the pediatric voice-related quality-of-life sur-
of vocal nodules from childhood to adolescence. J Voice. vey. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2006;132:717–
In press. 720.
14. Peterman A, Cella D, Glandon G, Dobrez D, Yount S. Mu- 30. Varni JW, Seid M, Kurtin PS. The pedsQL 4.0 measure-
cositis in head and neck cancer: economic and quality-of- ment model for the pediatric quality of life inventory ver-
life outcomes. J Natl Cancer Inst Monographs. 2001;29: sion 4.0. Qual Life Newsletter. 1999;23:4–10.
45–51. 31. Krueger RA. Focus Groups: A Practical Guide for Applied
15. Schwartz S, Patrick DL, Yueh B. Quality of life outcomes Research. 2nd ed. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1994.
in the evaluation of head and neck cancer treatments. Arch 32. Rebok G, Riley A, Forrest C, Starfield B, Green B,
Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. 2001;127:673–678. Robertson J, Tambor E. Elementary school-aged children’s
16. Knight TS, Burwinkle TM, Varni JW. Health-related qual- reports of their health: a cognitive interviewing study. Qual
ity of life. In: Sobo EJ, Kurtin PS, eds. Child Health Ser- Life Res. 2001;10:59–70.
vices Research: Applications, Innovations, and Insights. 33. Pal DK. Quality of life assessment in children: a review of
San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass; 2003:209–241. conceptual and methodological issues in multidimensional
17. Eiser C. Children’s quality of life measures. Arch Dis health status measures. J Epidemiol Community Health.
Child. 1997;77:350–354. 1996;50:391–396.
18. Cohen SM, Dupont WD, Courey MS. Quality-of-life im- 34. Markham C, Dean T. Parents’ and professionals’ percep-
pact of non-neoplastic voice disorders: a meta-analysis. tions of quality of life in children with speech and lan-
Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol. 2006;115:128–134. guage difficulty. Int J Lang Commun Disord. 2006;41:
19. Hogikyan ND, Sethuraman G. Validation of an instrument 189–212.
to measure voice-related quality of life (V-RQOL). 35. Ronen GM, Rosenbaum P, Law M, Streiner DL. Health-re-
J Voice. 1999;13:557–569. lated quality of life in childhood disorders: a modified fo-
20. Jacobson BH, Johnson A, Grywalski C, Silbergleit A, cus group technique to involve children. Qual Life Res.
Jacobson G, Benniger MS, Newman CW. The voice 2001;10:71–79.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008


ATTITUDES OF CHILDREN WITH DYSPHONIA 209

36. Streiner D, Norman G. Health Measurement Scales. Ox- 38. Achenbach TM, McConaughy SH, Howell CT. Child/ado-
ford, England: Oxford University Press; 2003:15. lescent behavioral and emotional problems: implications
37. Czyzewski DI, Mariotto MJ, Bartholomew K, of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity.
LeCompte SH, Sockrider MM. Measurement of quality Psychol Bull. 1987;101:213–232.
of well being in a child and adolescent cystic fibrosis pop- 39. Guyatt GH, Feeny DH, Patrick DL. Measuring health-re-
ulation. Med Care. 1994;32:965–972. lated quality of life. Ann Intern Med. 1993;118:622–629.

Journal of Voice, Vol. 22, No. 2, 2008

You might also like