You are on page 1of 10

Cognitive Concepts of Craving

Stephen T. Tiffany, Ph.D.

Traditional models of craving have been based primarily on the concept of classical
conditioning. In recent years, however, researchers increasingly have introduced cognitive
concepts, such as memory, expectancies, interpretation, and automatic behavior, into their
conceptualizations of craving. These efforts have culminated in the development of four
cognitive models of craving: cognitive labeling, outcome expectancy, dual-affect, and
cognitive processing. The cognitive processing model posits that although many alcohol use
behaviors have become automatized processes in the course of an alcoholic’s drinking
career, craving is a nonautomatic process that requires mental effort and is limited by a
person’s cognitive capacity. This model also implies that alcohol use and alcohol-seeking
behavior can occur in the absence of craving. In addition to introducing various new
concepts and models into craving research, the cognitive sciences also offer well-established
methodologies for testing these models and analyzing craving processes. KEY WORDS: AOD
(alcohol and other drug) craving; scientific model; cognition; research method and evaluation;
conditioning; expectancy; emotion; memory; AOD use behavior; literature review

I
magine that you are an alcoholic races and your hands sweat; you realize mechanisms (for a review of the classi-
trying to quit drinking. You have that this is craving at its worst. You are cal conditioning model, see sidebar, p.
not had a drink in a month, but drawn inexorably into the bar. There is 216, and Tiffany 1995a).
during the past several days, you have no way you can fight it any longer; you Numerous components of the sce-
thought about alcohol constantly. These must have a drink. nario described previously, however,
thoughts occupy your mind, making it Although fictional, this situation is go beyond simple conditioning pro-
nearly impossible to concentrate on not farfetched. In fact, many alcoholics cesses. For example, the fictional scene
anything else. Everything around you will describe in vivid detail similar sto- includes descriptions of alcohol-specific
seems to invoke memories of how pleas- ries about craving and relapse (Ludwig memories, positive expectancies about
ant and satisfying drinking can be. You 1988). The conventional explanation alcohol use, difficulties in concentration,
have wrestled with the idea of having a for this scenario, based on classical con- decisions about drinking, attention
drink, but you have decided to wait at ditioning models, is relatively straight- focused on alcohol cues, interpretations
least a little longer. Today, however, forward: Over a long history of drinking, of physiological reactions, and auto-
after leaving work, you find yourself stimuli and events routinely paired with matic behavior (i.e., automaticity),
somewhat mindlessly driving by your alcohol consumption (e.g., the sight of all of which are cognitive concepts.
favorite bar. You cannot help but notice a bar) become conditioned stimuli—
the front door of the bar propped open, that is, they induce the same responses
seeming to beckon you inside. You pull that are produced by alcohol itself. These STEPHEN T. TIFFANY, PH.D., is a professor
over to the curb, park your car, and conditioned stimuli activate conditioned in the Department of Psychological Sciences,
find yourself standing at the door. As motivational states1 that produce crav- Purdue University, and a member of the
you look through the doorway, it is all ing experiences, physiological reactions, Purdue University Neurosciences Program,
so familiar: The bar stools, the televi- and alcohol-seeking behaviors. Thus, West Lafayette, Indiana.
sion flickering in the corner, and even all the events described in the opening
the smell of stale cigarette smoke are paragraph could be viewed as the con- 1
For a definition of this and other technical terms
comfortable and inviting. Your heart sequences of classical conditioning used in this article, see glossary, p. 223.

Vol. 23, No. 3, 1999 215


Craving researchers increasingly apply biology, computer science, and engi- led to a conditioned withdrawal syn-
these concepts in their attempts to under- neering. Modern cognitive science gen- drome, which the alcoholic, through
stand the processes underlying craving. erally describes the operation of mental cognitive processes, would experience
What does the term “cognitive functions in terms of information-pro- as craving for alcohol. In another major
approach” mean when applied to craving cessing systems—hypothesized mecha- conditioning model, Wise (1988)
or to other areas of research? Cognitive nisms that control the acquisition and described craving as the memory of the
approaches investigate the processes manipulation of information and positively reinforcing effects of alcohol
that control mental functions, such as translate that information into action. and other drugs (AODs). Finally,
communication, learning, classification, During the past 25 years, even con- Berridge and Robinson (1995) argued
knowledge representation, problem- ventional conditioning models of craving that conditioned drug motivational
solving, planning, remembering, and often have invoked cognitive processes. states were largely unconscious and
decisionmaking. To this end, cognitive For example, an influential craving the- resulted in conscious experiences of
science incorporates the contributions ory presented by Ludwig and Wikler craving only through mechanisms of a
of several disciplines, including psy- (1974) (see sidebar, below) hypothesized process called cognitive interpretation.
chology, philosophy, linguistics, neuro- that exposure to withdrawal-related cues Although these influential theories all

Classical Conditioning and Drug Craving

The concepts of classical conditioning have had a


major impact on theories of alcohol and other drug
Food Salivation (AOD) craving. Perhaps the most influential model of
Unconditioned Unconditioned conditioned craving was developed by Wikler (1948),
stimulus response
who hypothesized that stimuli paired repeatedly with
AOD withdrawal could become conditioned stimuli
Bell Salivation
that elicited conditioned withdrawal effects, which,
Conditioned Conditioned in turn, would generate craving. Addicts experiencing
stimulus response craving would be motivated to seek out and use AODs
to relieve the conditioned withdrawal effects. Although
The classical conditioning model. If an unconditioned this model was originally developed to explain heroin
stimulus (e.g., food) that elicits a certain response is addiction, researchers have also applied it to other drugs,
repeatedly paired with a neutral stimulus (e.g., the ringing
including alcohol (e.g., Ludwig and Wikler 1974).
of a bell), the neutral stimulus eventually becomes a
conditioned stimulus that elicits the same response as
With respect to alcohol craving, a drop in blood alcohol
the unconditioned stimulus. levels would be considered the unconditioned stimulus
that activates the unconditioned response (i.e., alcohol
withdrawal). Stimuli or situations reliably paired with
a decline in blood alcohol levels would become condi-
Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov (1849–1936) was the
tioned stimuli. For example, an alcoholic might rou-
first researcher to study the learning procedure called
tinely experience withdrawal in a certain location (e.g.,
classical conditioning. Pavlov and his coworkers discov-
a therapist’s office). After repeated withdrawal episodes
ered that animals could learn to display certain responses
(i.e., repeated pairings), seeing or being in this location
to stimuli that had been paired with other stimuli already
would be sufficient to trigger conditioned withdrawal
eliciting those responses. For example, a dog salivates if
reactions and craving.
food is placed in its mouth. In the terminology of classi-
—Stephen T. Tiffany
cal conditioning, the food is called an unconditioned
stimulus that reflexively elicits an unconditioned response
(i.e., salivation) (see figure). If a neutral stimulus (e.g.,
the sound of a bell) is repeatedly presented at the same
time as the unconditioned stimulus (i.e., the food), the References
neutral stimulus by itself eventually will elicit the same LUDWIG, A.M., AND WIKLER, A. “Craving” and relapse to drink.
response (i.e., salivation). At that point, the neutral stim- Quarterly Journal of Studies on Alcoholism 35:108–130, 1974.
ulus has become a conditioned stimulus that elicits a WIKLER, A. Recent progress in research on the neurophysiological basis
conditioned, or learned, response. of morphine addiction. American Journal of Psychiatry 105:329–338, 1948.

216 Alcohol Research & Health


Cognitive Concepts of Craving

cited cognitive processes as central to cognitive science paradigm, which focuses craving exist (see table, p. 218). These
the development of craving, they did on information processing, cognitive include two cognitive-behavioral mod-
not elaborate on how those cognitive architectures, memory, and decision- els—the cognitive labeling model and
processes might operate. making, is more clearly representative of the outcome expectancy model—and
Other more recent models describe contemporary cognitive psychology. Until two cognitive science models—the dual-
the role of cognition in craving somewhat recently, however, this paradigm did not affect model and the cognitive process-
more specifically. This article reviews substantially influence craving research. ing model. Each model offers distinct
selected cognitive models of craving and Both the cognitive-behavioral and examples of how various cognitive con-
discusses their implications for craving information-processing approaches to cepts may help explain craving and
research and assessment. Although this craving differ from the conventional may therefore complement each other.
review focuses on alcohol craving, nei- conditioning perspectives in several ways. The four models differ considerably in
ther cognition-based nor conditioning- Most important, traditional condition- the extent to which they utilize con-
based contemporary craving models ing models conceptualize craving as a temporary cognitive science theories.
are unique to alcoholism research. somewhat biologically primal, homoge- Indeed, one approach—the cognitive
Consequently, this review also draws neous state that directly represents the labeling model—uses concepts that are
from craving research on other drugs. fundamental motive for AOD use no longer widely accepted in the cogni-
in the addict. Conversely, cognitive tive sciences. Nevertheless, this model is
approaches consider craving the prod- included here because it still is invoked
Cognition and Craving uct of higher order mental functions. occasionally when researchers speculate
Research Thus, from the cognitive perspective, about the cognitive features of craving.
craving is not a primitive motivational
The increasing emphasis on cognition state but a complex, multidimensional
in craving research reflects the aftermath process that reflects how AOD-relevant
The Cognitive Labeling Model
of the “cognitive revolution” that swept information controls an addict’s behavior. One problem associated with conven-
the social sciences, particularly psychol- Furthermore, cognitive models contend tional conditioning models is explain-
ogy, in the 1970s. Since then, cognitive that craving processes can be measured ing how conditioned responses are
perspectives have dominated many of in ways not envisioned by traditional translated into craving experiences. For
the major fields of psychological research. conceptualizations of craving. (For example, assume that an alcoholic pre-
Even basic models of classical condi- more information on cognition-based sented with the sight of his or her
tioning, which are frequently invoked approaches to measuring craving, see favorite bar responds to the situation
in craving research, have increasingly the section “Craving and Cognitive with a racing heart and sweaty palms.
incorporated cognitive perspectives (e.g., Methodology,” p. 222.) How can these supposed conditioned
Rescorla 1988). For example, many mod- These two perspectives on craving autonomic responses generate craving?
ern theories of classical conditioning (i.e., cognitive-behavioral and informa- Some craving theorists have suggested
have adopted an information-process- tion processing) also have somewhat that conditioned responses are con-
ing perspective. This approach posits different implications for the development verted, in some unspecified way, into
that the learning of an association of interventions to prevent or treat craving. experiences of craving (e.g., Poulos et al.
between stimuli depends on the extent For example, if, as suggested by the 1981). Other researchers have hypothe-
to which the occurrence of one stimu- conventional view, craving is a biologi- sized that craving experiences arise
lus provides information about the cally primal state, it makes sense to seek from some form of cognitive interpre-
occurrence of the other stimulus. biological treatments (e.g., pharmaco- tation of conditioned reactions (e.g.,
When considering the impact of logical therapy) that directly target this Ludwig and Wikler 1974; Melchior
cognitive concepts on craving research, motivational state. Conversely, if crav- and Tabakoff 1984). This latter
it is useful to distinguish between two ing arises from the operation of dynamic, hypothesis has resulted in the formula-
types of cognitive theories: cognitive- multidimensional information-processing tion of the cognitive labeling model.
behavioral models and the cognitive systems, attempts to reduce craving might The cognitive labeling model of
science paradigm. Cognitive-behavioral be most successful if they target the craving is a variant of Schachter and
models (also called social learning cognitive processes regulating craving. Singer’s (1962) cognition-arousal theory
models) emphasize such constructs as of emotion. According to that theory,
expectancies, attributions, imitation, an emotional experience results from
and self-efficacy in the control of drink- Cognitive Models the interaction between physiological
ing behavior (e.g., Marlatt and Gordon of Craving arousal and a cognitive interpretation
1985). These models, which draw heav- of the arousal. The interpretation pro-
ily from the social and personality psy- Many researchers have speculated that vides an emotional label that deter-
chology of the 1960s and 1970s, are cognition plays an important role in mines the quality (e.g., pleasant or
relatively well represented in studies of generating craving. To date, however, unpleasant) of the emotional state. The
alcohol dependence. Conversely, the only four detailed cognitive models of intensity of that emotion is determined

Vol. 23, No. 3, 1999 217


by the extent of the arousal (i.e., the quent important modifications of that The Outcome Expectancy Model
greater the arousal, the more intense is theory (e.g., London and Nisbett 1974),
This cognitive model contends that
the emotion). As applied to craving, which have replaced the model’s some- environmental cues can trigger power-
this model implies that alcohol cues what vague concept of labeling with a ful expectations about alcohol’s effects
(e.g., the sight of a bar) can generate more precise proposal—that the experi- and that those expectations will pro-
conditioned physiological arousal (e.g., ence of emotion requires causal beliefs foundly influence the alcoholic’s behav-
a racing heart). In addition, the cues or attributions to connect emotional ior. The expectancies have two essential
activate mental processes (e.g., memo- cognitions with arousal. components: an informational compo-
ries of previous drinking occasions) Any attempt to generate a fully artic- nent and a motivational component.
that identify the situation as a setting ulated cognition-arousal model of crav- The informational component repre-
for drinking. As a result of this cogni- ing, however, may be of little relevance sents specific beliefs about alcohol’s
tive response, the alcoholic interprets for two reasons. First, the cognition- various effects. For example, Marlatt
the physiological reactions as craving arousal theory of emotion is generally (1985) proposed that alcohol-paired
(i.e., provides the reactions with the no longer considered an accepted model stimuli presented to an alcoholic may
label “craving”). Stated simply, the of emotional processing (Reisenzein generate an expectation or anticipation
alcoholic thinks, “I feel aroused and 1983), because research findings have that alcohol use may produce pleasure,
I am in a situation in which I usually not supported this theory. Second, data relaxation, or relief from withdrawal.
drink alcohol. Therefore, this feeling from the craving literature suggest that The motivational component of
must be craving.”2 the cognition-arousal model cannot expectancies reflects the desire for expe-
Many researchers have speculated adequately explain all aspects of crav- riencing the positive outcome of alco-
that labeling may be crucial to craving ing. For example, according to this hol consumption. It is as though the
experiences (for a review, see Tiffany model, the intensity of craving should alcoholic were thinking, “When I see
1995b), although no one has generated be determined by the magnitude of my friends drinking, I think about how
a fully developed cognition-arousal theory the physiological reaction generated by much I enjoy the effects of alcohol,
of craving. At best, cognitive labeling AOD-related stimuli. In other words, and I want to experience those effects.”
explanations of craving are only loose the stronger the physiological reaction The outcome expectancy model also
approximations of Schachter and to drinking cues is, the stronger the distinguishes between “craving” and
Singer’s (1962) original theory. Further- craving generated should be. In gen- “urges.” According to this definition,
more, these explanations ignore subse- eral, however, no significant correla- craving is the desire for positive out-
tions exist between the extent of cue- comes. This desire triggers urges—that
2
Some emotion theorists have hypothesized that elicited physiological reactions and the is, the intent to engage in alcohol use.
such thought processes probably occur uncon- extent of cue-elicited craving (Tiffany This intent, in turn, precipitates drink-
sciously in the everyday experience of emotion. 1990). ing. Thus, craving by itself may not be

Cognitive Models of Craving and Their Major Characteristics

Model Major Characteristics

Cognitive labeling model Craving is an emotion generated after exposure to alcohol-related cues that lead to both physical
arousal and a cognitive response identifying the arousal as craving; the extent of craving depends
on the extent of the arousal.

Outcome expectancy model Craving is generated after exposure to environmental alcohol-related cues that trigger positive
expectations about alcohol’s effects.

Dual-affect model Craving can be generated by both negative emotional systems (e.g., negative emotional states,
aversive events, and withdrawal) and positive emotional systems (e.g., positive emotional states
and consumption of small alcohol doses). Positive- and negative-affect craving are mutually
exclusive; the extent of craving depends on the extent to which positive- or negative-affect systems
are activated.

Cognitive processing model Craving represents a nonautomatic cognitive process that is activated when the execution of
automatized drinking behavior is voluntarily or involuntarily blocked. Craving-inducing situations
require cognitive processing and mental efforts and may thereby interfere with other cognitively
demanding tasks. Craving is not required for either alcohol seeking or alcohol use.

218 Alcohol Research & Health


Cognitive Concepts of Craving

sufficient for causing alcohol consump- both alcoholics and nonalcoholics to thought to be mutually inhibitory—
tion, because a person can desire alco- drinking cues and subsequently mea- that is, stimulation of one system
hol without intending to drink. sured the respondents’ changes in out- would suppress activation of the other
Since Marlatt’s (1985) development come expectancies and craving. The system. Consequently, the model
of the original outcome expectancy study found some evidence that cue assumes that a person cannot experi-
model, researchers have further elabo- exposure enhanced craving and increased ence both positive- and negative-affect
rated on the cognitive aspects of this expectancies of positive outcomes in craving simultaneously.
theory (e.g., Goldman and Rather 1993). both subject groups. However, the According to the dual-affect model,
For example, these researchers use sev- expectancy effects were not found con- craving systems are structured at a cog-
eral contemporary concepts of memory sistently across all measures, and some nitive level into propositional net-
and information processing to describe aspects of the findings could even be works—memory networks that store
the mental processes that control alco- interpreted as being inconsistent with information on the stimuli that trigger
hol expectancies. These reformulations the expectancy model (see Tiffany the craving system, AOD-related
have not, however, specifically addressed 1995b). Nevertheless, to date, too little responses, and the meaning or interpre-
the craving aspects of Marlatt’s original research exists on expectancies and tation of stimuli and responses. A given
theory.3 Thus, expectancy research gen- craving for AODs to allow meaningful network is activated when environmental
erally has focused on the beliefs that conclusions about the validity of the stimuli match the stimulus information
people have about alcohol’s effects and outcome expectancy model. stored in that network.
on the associations of those beliefs with The extent of the activation (and,
drinking behavior. Few studies have consequently, craving) depends on the
examined the relationship between
The Dual-Affect Model extent to which multiple cues match
craving and situation-specific outcome Baker and colleagues (1987) proposed the triggering stimuli stored in the
expectancies. that craving is controlled by complex propositional network. For example,
In Marlatt’s theory, positive outcome emotion-processing systems that influ- an alcoholic in a positive, celebratory
expectancies about alcohol should be ence physiological responses, self-reports mood may experience some alcohol
more readily triggered in some settings of craving and emotion, and drug-seek- craving. The same alcoholic also may
than in others. Thus, certain situations ing behavior. The researchers posited experience craving when presented
that are routinely associated with alco- that craving can reflect the operation of with the opportunity to consume an
hol consumption, such as being in a both negative and positive emotion, or alcoholic beverage. The intensity of the
bar setting, would be much more likely affect, systems. craving, however, may differ in each
to activate positive outcome expectan- Negative-affect craving can be trig- situation. Furthermore, the person’s
cies about the effects of alcohol than, gered by a negative emotional response craving will be considerably stronger
for example, being in church. Further- or feeling (e.g., depression or anger), if he or she is in a positive mood and
more, researchers have not investigated aversive events (e.g., having to give a is given the opportunity to drink.
specifically the proposed distinction presentation to a group of strangers), The activation level of a network
between craving and urges (Tiffany AOD withdrawal, cues paired with also determines the consistency of the
1995b). previous withdrawal episodes, and various responses generated by that
If the outcome expectancy model is information that the drug is not avail- network. That is, as a network becomes
correct in assuming that expectancies able. When activated, the negative- more fully engaged, stronger correla-
control alcohol consumption, then a affect system induces craving experi- tions should exist between the extent of
correlation should exist between the ences, drug-seeking behavior, negative self-reported craving and the extent of
extent of the expectancies and the extent affect, and physiological reactions that drug-seeking behavior. Finally, the
of alcohol intake. Indeed, several stud- mimic withdrawal. dual-affect model predicts that partial
ies have demonstrated that expectancies Conversely, positive-affect craving is activation of a propositional network
about alcohol’s effects tend to correlate associated with positive emotions and should lower the threshold for further
significantly with general and situation- with pleasurable or positively reinforc- activation of that network. For exam-
specific alcohol consumption levels ing AOD effects. This craving system is ple, an alcoholic in a celebratory mood
(Goldman and Rather 1993). activated by positive emotional states, a (i.e., whose craving system is partially
Cooney and colleagues (1987) con- small AOD dose, cues paired with AOD activated) should respond more
ducted a study in which they exposed use, and information that the drug is strongly to the presentation of alcohol
available. Activation of this system cues than would an alcoholic in a neu-
3
Indeed, in their elaboration of expectancy theory, induces craving, drug-seeking behavior, tral mood (i.e., whose craving system is
Goldman and Rather (1993) have adopted the positive affect, and physiological responses not activated).
position suggested by Tiffany (1990) that the moti- that mimic the stimulating AOD effects. Unlike other cognitive models of
vational control of alcohol use in the highly practiced
alcoholic may be automatized and not reflected in An important feature of the dual- craving, the dual-affect model has been
reports of craving (also see section “The Cognitive affect model is that both positive- and developed sufficiently to allow
Processing Model,” p. 220). negative-affect craving systems are researchers to generate and test fairly

Vol. 23, No. 3, 1999 219


precise predictions. For example, the What is an automatic process? Many The cognitive processing model of
model predicts that negative affect and activities are controlled by cognitive craving proposes that over a long his-
stress should trigger craving more effec- processes that operate quickly and tory of drinking, many of the actions
tively in drug-deprived than in non-drug- effortlessly and require little focused involved in acquiring and consuming
deprived addicts. Furthermore, the attention. For example, people gener- alcohol become automatized for alco-
model proposes that positive- and neg- ally eat, groom, dress, walk, drive, talk, holics (see figure below). For example,
ative-affect craving cannot be activated and read while paying little or no following years of drinking at a particular
simultaneously. These propositions can attention to exactly how they perform bar every day after work, an alcoholic
readily be tested. Moreover, investigators these functions. In fact, many daily may unthinkingly walk into the bar
can easily assess predictions on whether activities have become so automatic even though he or she has resolved to
partial activation of one network lowers that people may have difficulty remem- quit drinking. Consequently, the actions
the threshold at which the person bering what their performance was of a highly practiced addict, during both
responds to other relevant stimuli. like when they first acquired those regular use and relapse, may be viewed
Several studies have provided partial skills. In contrast, when learning a not as a consequence of craving but as
evidence for some of the model’s pre- new skill, its execution requires consid- an example of the behaviors exhibited
dictions. For example, Zinser and col- erable effort and focused attention. during the execution of any automa-
leagues (1992) found that negative affect With practice, however, performance tized skill (Tiffany and Carter 1998).
correlated with craving levels in smok- improves, and what once was a As with any activity that is performed
ers undergoing nicotine withdrawal but demanding and clumsy activity repeatedly, alcohol consumption by an
not in continuing smokers. Certain becomes effortless and highly coordi- alcoholic can be seen as readily triggered
other predictions, however, have not nated. According to cognitive psychol- by certain stimuli—stereotyped, effort-
been well supported (Tiffany 1995b). ogists, this kind of transformation less, difficult to control, and regulated
Most important, only limited evidence marks the transition from nonauto- largely outside of awareness. Ludwig
suggests that positive- and negative-affect matic to automatic functioning (1988, p. 92) provided an excellent
craving are mutually exclusive (Maude- (Shiffrin and Schneider 1977). description of automatic drinking:
Griffin and Tiffany 1996; Tiffany and
Drobes 1991). In addition, research
with cigarette smokers has not yielded
any evidence that deprived smokers are Automatic
specifically sensitized to smoking cues processing
(Maude-Griffin and Tiffany 1996;
Drobes and Tiffany 1997). The avail- Stimulus Automatized
able evidence therefore suggests that triggers drug use
craving networks may not be organized
as described by the dual-affect model.
Most of those studies, however, have Obstacle
been conducted with cigarette smokers. Stimulus
Almost no research has applied the triggers
model specifically to alcohol craving.

The Cognitive Processing Model


Nonautomatic
The models previously described assume processing
that craving and AOD use are tightly
coupled—in other words, that craving
is at the motivational core of AOD use Craving
in the addict. Conversely, the cognitive
processing model of AOD use and crav-
ing proposes that the regulation of AOD The cognitive processing model. In an alcoholic who is not trying to quit drinking,
use in experienced addicts can function alcohol use is controlled by automatic cognitive processes. Under these circumstances,
independently of the processes that con- “stimulus triggers” activate automatic processes that result in automatized drug use,
trol craving (Tiffany 1990). According to and craving plays no role in the control of drinking. When the automatized alcohol-
the cognitive processing model, addictive use sequences (e.g., driving to a favorite bar, entering, sitting down at the bar, and
AOD use is regulated by what cognitive ordering a drink) are blocked by an environmental obstacle (e.g., the bar is closed),
psychologists refer to as automatic pro- the alcoholic must activate nonautomatic processes to cope with that problem. These
cesses, whereas craving represents the nonautomatic processes generate craving for alcohol.
operation of nonautomatic processes.

220 Alcohol Research & Health


Cognitive Concepts of Craving

“Others essentially think instinctively, they are faced with cues and situations have strongly supported the core assump-
short circuiting both imagery and cog- that trigger portions of their automatic tion of this model—that craving is not
nitions, and are inclined to act without drinking behaviors; consequently, they necessary for drug seeking or AOD use
knowing why. When alcohol becomes must exert considerable mental effort (Tiffany 1990, Tiffany and Carter 1998;
readily available, they drink before they to keep themselves from drinking. The Tiffany and Conklin in press). Neither
think.” constant battles with craving may leave studies of craving and alcohol con-
In contrast to automatic processing, the abstinent alcoholic mentally exhausted sumption in the laboratory nor studies
nonautomatic processing can be char- and unable to cope effectively with the of craving and relapse in the real world
acterized as slow, flexible, intention cognitive demands of everyday life. The have provided strong evidence that
dependent, requiring cognitive effort, cognitive effort associated with absti- craving is directly responsible for alcohol
and restricted by limited cognitive nence may be sustainable as long as no use in alcoholics (Tiffany and Conklin
capacity. Nonautomatic processes occur additional problems arise (e.g., in the in press). For example, the alcohol
under three kinds of circumstances: (1) workplace or in relationships with fam- amounts consumed by alcoholics in
when a person first learns a skill, (2) ily and friends). In stressful, challeng- laboratory studies are only weakly cor-
when a highly automatized sequence is ing situations (e.g., when faced with a related with the alcoholics’ descriptions
activated but some environmental tight deadline at work), however, the of craving. Moreover, studies of relapse
obstacle blocks the completion of that abstinent alcoholic may find it easier to episodes indicate that addicts rarely
sequence, and (3) when a person wants succumb to alcohol craving and take a identify craving as a major direct trig-
to prevent the execution of activated drink rather than struggle with both ger for resuming AOD use (Tiffany
automatized sequences (Shiffrin and craving and work-related pressure. and Carter 1998).
Schneider 1977). The cognitive pro- Overt behavior, self-reports of craving Numerous studies also have sup-
cessing model hypothesizes that craving and emotional distress, and autonomic ported the cognitive processing model’s
represents nonautomatic processes physiological responses are typical reac- prediction that craving activation
that are activated simultaneously with tions that result from the activation of should disrupt cognitive functioning
automatized AOD use sequences. The craving processes. The cognitive pro- (Sayette in press). For example, Sayette
nonautomatic processes are activated cessing model, however, proposes that and colleagues (1994) exposed people
either to overcome obstacles to success- craving may influence a fourth category receiving inpatient alcoholism treat-
fully completing automatized AOD use of responses not typically addressed in ment to both alcohol-related cues (e.g.,
or to prevent the execution of an autom- craving studies—that is, responses to a glass of their favorite alcoholic bever-
atized sequence. These two situations the cognitive demands of craving. age) and neutral cues (e.g., a glass of
generate, respectively, the craving observed According to the model, a craving- water). During cue exposure, the sub-
in alcoholics who are not attempting to inducing situation presents a problem jects were asked to press a button when-
quit drinking and the craving observed in that demands a solution. Furthermore, ever they heard a brief sound, and their
alcoholics who are attempting abstinence. the model posits that solving this prob- reaction times were recorded. The study
The first scenario is exemplified by lem requires nonautomatic cognitive found that reaction times were slower
the alcoholic who goes to his or her processes that function at the expense in the presence of alcohol-related cues
favorite bar and discovers that it is closed of other activities that also necessitate than in the presence of neutral cues,
for remodeling. Resolution of this unex- nonautomatic processing. Because suggesting that the alcoholics’ ability to
pected inconvenience requires nonau- nonautomatic processes involve mental perform the button-pressing task was
tomatic processes, causing changes in effort that exhausts limited cognitive disrupted by the cognitive processing
overt behavior, craving and emotional resources, the alcoholic trying to solve triggered by the alcohol cues. These
distress, and physiological responses. craving-associated problems will have findings support the cognitive process-
For example, the alcoholic will take little mental capacity left for coping ing model’s hypothesis that craving is
action to overcome the obstacle (e.g., with other cognitively demanding situ- cognitively disruptive and therefore
drive to another bar). In addition, the ations. Thus, the nonautomatic cogni- exerts a toll on other cognitively
alcoholic will express desire for alcohol, tive demands of craving processes can demanding tasks.
intentions to find and consume alco- account for the disruptive impact of Beyond their implications for the
hol, and frustration and anger at being craving on daily functioning. cognitive processing model, this and
thwarted in his or her attempt to drink. The cognitive processing model similar studies demonstrate how the
The alcoholic also will likely display allows for several explicit predictions cognitively intrusive and disruptive fea-
physiological responses (e.g., changes in about the cognitive organization of tures of craving—features that addicts so
heart rate or sweat gland activity) that craving and AOD use, the behavioral often complain about—can be studied
reflect either the physical or cognitive manifestations of craving, and the role under controlled laboratory conditions.
demands of the craving situation. of automatic and nonautomatic pro- More generally, the studies illustrate
The second scenario represents the cesses in relapse. Many of these predic- the usefulness of measures and meth-
situation confronting alcoholics who tions have not yet been evaluated sys- ods derived from cognitive sciences for
are trying to quit drinking. Every day tematically; however, various studies investigating craving processes. These

Vol. 23, No. 3, 1999 221


methods are discussed more fully in the whenever they perceive a probe stimulus interfering with the addict’s ability
following section. (e.g., hear a tone). The investigator then to complete the secondary task.
measures the subject’s reaction time in
response to the stimulus. Slowed reac- • AOD-related cues may conjure up
Craving and Cognitive tion times during craving are assumed upsetting memories about alcohol,
Methodology to reflect the processing demands of the and those negative recollections may
craving task. This approach allows for reduce the addict’s motivation to
The cognitive sciences bring not only the quantification of the old adage that perform.
a fresh perspective to the conceptualiza- it is hard to do two things at once, par-
tion of craving but also a variety of ticularly if one of the things (i.e., cop- • Craving may prime motor systems
well-established procedures and measures ing with craving) is especially difficult. organized to seek and use drugs,
for studying craving processes. This As noted earlier in this article, many and the activation of those systems
cognitive methodology has considerable studies have demonstrated that craving may conflict with the motor demands
potential for enriching the traditional induction can disrupt performance of a of the button-pressing task.
assessment approaches generally used secondary task. These studies also suggest,
in craving studies. Most laboratory stud- however, that reaction-time measures • Craving may reflect a problem-
ies of craving expose addicts to AOD- and craving reports may not be con- solving activity that exhausts
related cues and then measure self- trolled entirely by the same underlying resources that might otherwise be
reported craving; autonomic reactions psychological processes. For example, devoted to responding quickly on
(e.g., heart rate); and, occasionally, AOD some analyses determined only weak or the secondary task.
seeking or use. In those studies, alco- even statistically insignificant correla-
holics confronted with alcohol-related tions between slowed reaction times Cognitive science offers many refined
cues typically exhibit increases in self- and craving levels (e.g., Cepeda-Benito techniques for identifying the source(s)
reported craving, heart rate, sweat gland and Tiffany 1996). This observation is of disruption in dual-task procedures
activity, and salivation (Carter and Tiffany similar to the finding that craving levels (e.g., Wickens 1984). For example,
in press; Tiffany et al. in press). Thus, do not correlate strongly with physio- craving researchers can use secondary
those studies demonstrate (for alcohol logical responses or measures of AOD tasks that vary systematically along sev-
as well as for other drugs) that both use. The weak relationships among self- eral critical dimensions (e.g., motor
nonautomatic processes, such as crav- reported craving, cognitive measures, demands, cognitive complexity, and
ing, and autonomic reactions can be physiological reactions, and assessments ease of detecting the probe stimulus).
manipulated in the laboratory (Carter of AOD use underscore the growing Cognitive scientists have repeatedly
and Tiffany in press). Nevertheless, recognition among craving researchers used systematic manipulations of these
such analyses are unlikely to reveal much that responses elicited in craving situa- dimensions to analyze the processing
about the dynamics of the psychologi- tions reflect the combined effects of demands of cognitive tasks. The results
cal mechanisms that regulate craving. multiple psychological processes. of similarly designed craving studies
Cognitive scientists have developed Although initial studies of craving could offer fascinating insights into the
highly sophisticated tasks and measures and cognitive processing have yielded cognitive dynamics of craving pro-
to dissect the operation of complex promising results, these studies repre- cesses. Moreover, such research could
cognitive processes. One technique, the sent only a small part of the potential provide a model of how the conceptual
dual-task procedure, has been used in of cognitive technology for revealing and methodological contributions of
several investigations into the cognitive the complexities of craving processes cognitive sciences could be exploited to
demands of craving (Tiffany 1990) and (Sayette in press). Thus, the studies discover how alcoholics and other addicts
is a well-established approach for inves- have produced additional questions process information about AODs.
tigating the extent to which the process- that remain unanswered, including In addition to providing techniques
ing demands of one task interfere with which aspect of craving disrupts pro- for assessing cognitive aspects of crav-
performance of a second task (Sayette cessing or depletes cognitive resources. ing, cognitive sciences offer immense
in press). In craving research, the primary Several potential explanations exist for potential for craving research, because
task is a procedure designed to activate this observation, including the following: modern theories of cognitive process-
the nonautomatic processes that gener- ing often are so well developed that
ate craving. For example, smokers might • AOD-related stimuli may be so they can be used to generate mathe-
be asked to imagine a situation in which “attention grabbing” that they pre- matical or computer models of mental
they have a strong desire to smoke vent addicts from focusing on a sec- functioning. Such models offer a level
(Cepeda-Benito and Tiffany 1996). ondary task. of predictive precision and testability
The secondary, or probe, task generally that cannot be achieved with less for-
is a simple reaction-time task in which mally developed theories. To date, the-
subjects are asked, for example, to gen- • AOD-related cues may induce some ories of craving or of addictive behavior
erate a response (e.g., press a button) degree of general arousal, thereby in general have not been developed suf-

222 Alcohol Research & Health


Cognitive Concepts of Craving

GLOSSARY
Attributions: Inferences on the causes and effects of behavior. Expectancies: Expectations regarding the effects of a behavior
Autonomic responses: Physiological responses that are con- or a drug.
trolled by the autonomic nervous system (e.g., heart rate, Motivational state: A psychological state that initiates or modi-
blood pressure, and sweat gland activity). These responses fies a certain behavior (e.g., seeking and consuming alcohol
are not under conscious control. or other drugs).
Conditioned withdrawal syndrome: The experience of physio- Paradigm: A set of related theories or models.
logical symptoms (e.g., sweating, tremors, and anxiety) of Positively reinforcing effects: The effects of a drug (e.g., alco-
withdrawal from alcohol and other drugs that is not caused hol) that increase the probability that a person will attempt
by the actual withholding of the drug but by stimuli that to consume more of that drug.
have been associated with previous withdrawal episodes.

ficiently to allow for the generation of tions, memory organization, and per- chological characteristics of craving,
formal mathematical or computer formance-based indicators of mental alcohol researchers almost certainly will
models. The cognitive sciences, how- functions likely will elucidate many of need to apply state-of-the-art cognitive
ever, offer a standard of theorizing that the mechanisms underlying craving. processing models and methods. ■
if fully adopted by craving researchers, Although cognitive approaches to crav-
would represent a quantum leap in the ing have considerable potential, they
scientific study of craving processes. still have shortcomings. For example,
References
many of the predictions generated by BAKER, T.B.; MORSE, E.; AND SHERMAN, J.E. The
cognitive models have not been system- motivation to use drugs: A psychobiological analysis
Cognition and the Future atically evaluated. Moreover, some of of urges. In: Rivers, P.C., ed. The Nebraska Symposium
on Motivation: Alcohol Use and Abuse. Lincoln, NE:
of Craving Research the models are insufficiently developed University of Nebraska Press, 1987. pp. 257–323.
or invoke constructs that no longer
The cognitive models discussed in this represent contemporary research and BERRIDGE, K.C., AND ROBINSON, T.E. The mind
of an addicted brain: Neural sensitization of want-
article offer perspectives on craving theory in the cognitive sciences. ing versus liking. Current Directions in Psychological
processes that differ markedly from Despite these problems, the pres- Science 4:71–76, 1995.
conventional, conditioning-based con- ence of cognitive concepts, measures,
CARTER, B.L., AND TIFFANY, S.T. Meta-analysis of
cepts of craving. In some cases, such as and methods in craving research will cue-reactivity in addiction research. Addiction, in press.
the cognitive labeling hypothesis, the almost inevitably increase. This assertion
models merely place a cognitive spin is based at least in part on the observa- CEPEDA-BENITO, A., AND TIFFANY, S.T. The use of
a dual-task procedure for the assessment of cogni-
on basic conditioning approaches. tion that the theories of the behavioral tive effort associated with smoking urges. Psycho-
Conversely, some theories, such as the sciences in general increasingly incor- pharmacology 127:155–163, 1996.
cognitive processing model, depart rad- porate cognitive dimensions. Indeed,
COONEY, N.L.; GILLESPIE, R.A.; BAKER, L.H.; AND
ically from traditional assumptions the cognitive perspective has become KAPLAN, R.F. Cognitive changes after alcohol cue
about the organization and function of the dominant paradigm in most behav- exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology
craving processes. Despite their differ- ioral sciences. Craving research has 55:150–155, 1987.
ences, however, cognitive approaches always tracked—albeit with some DROBES, D.J., AND TIFFANY, S.T. Comparisons of
share common themes. Most important, delay—major trends in the behavioral smoking urges elicited through imagery and in vivo
these approaches posit that craving arises sciences. Beyond this “inevitability of cue exposure. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 106:
from the operation of information-pro- history,” however, cognitive approaches 15–25, 1997.
cessing systems. In other words, these will be welcomed into studies of alcohol GOLDMAN, M.S., AND RATHER, B.C. Substance use
models view craving not merely as a craving because of their potential for disorders: Cognitive models and architecture. In:
primitive drive state but as the product helping researchers disentangle complex Dobson, K.S., and Kendall, P.C., eds. Psycho-
pathology and Cognition. San Diego: Academic Press,
of higher order cognitive functions. craving data. For example, over the next 1993. pp. 245–292.
Cognitive models offer researchers few years, scientists will face entirely
the opportunity to study craving pro- new types of results generated by brain LONDON, H., AND NISBETT, R.E. Elements of
Schachter’s cognitive theory of emotional states. In:
cesses with measures that extend far imaging studies of craving. To under- London, H., and Nisbett, R.E., eds. Thought and
beyond the standard inventory of vari- stand the neurobiological correlates of Feeling. Chicago: Aldine, 1974.
ables suggested by conventional craving craving identified by such studies and
LUDWIG, A.M. Understanding the Alcoholic’s Mind:
theories. For example, studies of attri- to identify the association between The Nature of Craving and How to Control It. New
butions, outcome expectancies, inten- neurobiological features and the psy- York: Oxford University Press, 1988.

Vol. 23, No. 3, 1999 223


LUDWIG, A.M., AND WIKLER, A. “Craving” and SAYETTE, M.A. Cognitive theory and research. In: Behaviour: Cue Exposure, Theory and Practice. Chichester,
relapse to drink. Quarterly Journal of Studies on Leonard, K., and Blane, H., eds. Psychological England: John Wiley, 1995b. pp. 137–165.
Alcoholism 35:108–130, 1974. Theories of Drinking and Alcoholism. 2d ed. New
York: Guilford Press, in press. TIFFANY, S.T., AND CARTER, B.L. Is craving the
MARLATT, G.A. Cognitive factors in the relapse source of compulsive drug use? Journal of Psycho-
process. In: Marlatt, G.A., and Gordon, J.R., eds. SAYETTE, M.A.; MONTI, P.M.; ROHSENOW, D.J.; pharmacology 12:23–30, 1998.
Relapse Prevention. New York: Guilford Press, GULLIVER, S.B.; COLBY, S.M.; SIROTA, A.D.;
TIFFANY, S.T., AND CONKLIN, C.A. A cognitive
1985. pp. 128–200. NIAURA, R., AND ABRAMS, D.A. The effects of
processing model of alcohol craving and compul-
exposure on reaction time in male alcoholics.
MARLATT, G.A., AND GORDON, J.R. Relapse sive alcohol use. Addiction, in press.
Journal of Studies on Alcohol 55:629–633, 1994.
Prevention. New York: Guilford Press, 1985. TIFFANY, S.T., AND DROBES, D.J. The develop-
SCHACHTER, S., AND SINGER, J.E. Cognitive, social,
MAUDE-GRIFFIN, P.M., AND TIFFANY, S.T. ment and initial validation of a questionnaire of
and physiological determinants of emotional state. smoking urges. British Journal of Addiction
Production of smoking urges through imagery: Psychological Review 69:379–399, 1962.
The impact of affect and smoking abstinence. 86:1467–1476, 1991.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology SHIFFRIN, R.M., AND SCHNEIDER, W. Controlled TIFFANY, S.T.; CARTER, B.L.; AND SINGLETON,
4:198–208, 1996. and automatic human information processing: II. E.G. Challenges in the manipulation, assessment
Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a and interpretation of craving relevant variables.
MELCHIOR, C.L., AND TABAKOFF, B. A condition- general theory. Psychological Review 84:127–190, 1977. Addiction, in press.
ing model of alcohol tolerance. In: Galanter, M.,
ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. Vol. 2. New TIFFANY, S.T. A cognitive model of drug urges and WICKENS, C.D. Processing resources in attention.
York: Plenum Press, 1984. pp. 5–16. drug-use behavior: Role of automatic and nonauto- In: Parasuraman, R., and Davies, R., eds. Varieties
matic processes. Psychological Review 97:147–168, of Attention. New York: Academic Press, 1984. pp.
POULOS, C.X.; HINSON, R.; AND SIEGEL, S. The 1990. 63–103.
role of Pavlovian processes in drug tolerance and
dependence: Implications for treatment. Addictive TIFFANY, S.T. Potential functions of classical condi- WISE, R.A. The neurobiology of craving:
Behaviors 6:205–211, 1981. tioning in drug addiction. In: Drummond, D.C.; Implications for understanding and treatment of
Tiffany, S.T.; Glautier, S.P.; and Remington, B., addiction. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 97:
REISENZEIN, R. The Schachter theory of emotion: eds. Addictive Behaviour: Cue Exposure, Theory and 118–132, 1988.
Two decades later. Psychological Bulletin 94:239– Practice. Chichester, England: John Wiley, 1995a.
264, 1983. pp. 47–71. ZINSER, M.C.; BAKER, T.B.; SHERMAN, J.E.; AND
CANNON, D.S. Relation between self-reported
RESCORLA, R.A. Pavlovian conditioning: It’s not TIFFANY, S.T. The role of cognitive factors in reac- affect and drug urges and cravings in continuing
what you think it is. American Psychologist 43(3): tivity to drug cues. In: Drummond, D.C.; Tiffany, and withdrawing smokers. Journal of Abnormal
151–160, 1988. S.T.; Glautier, S.P.; and Remington, B., eds. Addictive Psychology 101:617–629, 1992.

Public Education Materials Available


A variety of public education materials are now available from NIAAA. All of these materials are free
and may be obtained in English and Spanish versions. Quantities also are available for school and
other educational programs.

Alcoholism: Getting the Facts.


A brief, research-based brochure to edu-
cate the general public about alcohol abuse
and dependence. Provides answers to
commonly asked questions; includes a list
of resources for information and assistance.

Drinking and Your Pregnancy. How to Cut Down on Your Drinking.


A short pamphlet to answer common The companion brochure to NIAAA’s The
questions about alcohol consumption during Physicians’ Guide to Helping Patients With
pregnancy. Describes the consequences of Y
Alcohol Problems. Presents tips for those who
drinking during pregnancy, focusing on are acting on medical advice to reduce their
fetal alcohol syndrome. Includes resources alcohol consumption. Includes a list of resources
for help and additional information. for treatment and additional information.

To order, write to: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, Publications Distribution Center, P.O. Box 10686,
Rockville, MD 20849–0686. Fax: (202) 842–0418. Full text is available on the World Wide Web at http://www.niaaa.nih.gov

224 Alcohol Research & Health

You might also like