You are on page 1of 7

The Consortium to

Establish a Registry for


Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD).
Part V.A normative study of the neuropsychological battery
K.A. Welsh, PhD; N. Butters, PhD; R.C. Mohs, PhD; D. Beekly, BS;
S. Edland, MS; G. Fillenbaum, PhD; and A. Heyman, MD

Article abstract-The neuropsychological tests developed for the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s
Disease (CERAD) are currently used to measure cognitive impairments of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) in clinical investi-
gations of this disorder. This report presents the normative information for the CERAD battery, obtained in a large
sample (n = 413) of control subjects (ages 50 to 89) who were enrolled in 23 university medical centers in the United
States participating in the CERAD study from 1987 to 1992. We compared separately the performance of subjects with
high (212) and low ( d 2 ) years of formal education. For many of the individual cognitive measures in the highly edu-
cated group, we observed significant age and gender effects. Only the praxis measure showed a significant age effect in
the low-education group. Delayed recall, when adjusted for amount of material acquired (savings), was relatively unaf-
fected by age, gender, and level of education. Our findings suggest that the savings scores, in particular, may be useful
in distinguishing between AD and normal aging.
NEUROLOGY 1994;44:609-614

There are many brief mental status instruments approximately) and relative ease of administration
used to measure the degree and types of cognitive and scoring, have suggested a broader clinical ap-
impairments in the Most of these instru- plicability than its original intended use in re-
ments were validated on institutionalized patients search. In clinical settings where factors such as
and differentiate definite cases of dementia from patient fatigue, motivation, or incapacity prohibit
normal controls. Because many mental status tests more extensive neuropsychological examination of
have a restricted range of values in normal elderly cognitive capacities, the CERAD battery would ap-
subjects (ie, ceiling effects), they have limited sensi- pear to be a favorable alternative. However, in clin-
tivity and specificity in detecting mild cognitive syn- ical settings, as well as in research studies de-
d r o m e ~ .In
~ ,addition,
~~ there are high false-positive signed to detect dementia, use of the CERAD bat-
tendencies for many of these measures when used tery has been limited by the lack of normative val-
in community samples of normal elderly subjects ues needed for clinical interpretation.
with less education and of lower socioeconomic sta- Normative information is critically important for
tus.11 deciding who, on the basis of performance on the
The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alz- battery, may be demented and who is not. Without
heimer’s Disease (CERAD) developed a neuropsy- a clear indication of the normal range of perfor-
chological battery that is more extensive than a mance on the CERAD measures and the effects of
simple mental status screen, is sensitive to early- age and education on performance, interpretation
stage dementia,12J3and has substantial interrater is subjective and prone to error. For patients with
agreement (0.92 to L O ) , high test-retest reliability, early-stage dementia or those who are very old or
and longitudinal validity.14J5These findings, along who have limited formal education, the need for ob-
with the instrument’s brevity (20 to 30 minutes, jective information on the normal range of perfor-

From the Duke University Medical Center (Drs. Welsh, Fillenbanm, and Heyman), Durham, NC; Veterans Administration Medical Center and Univer-
sity of California at San Diego (Dr. Butters), La Jolla, CA; Mount Sinai School of Medicine (Dr. Mohs), New York, NY;and University of Washington (D.
Beekly and S. Edlandf, Seattle, WA.
Supported by NIA grants AGO6790 and AGO5128 to Duke University Medical Center by funds from the Veterans Administration, Washington, DC, and
NIA grants AGO8024 and AGO5131 to the University of California at San Diego, CA.
Received August 4, 1993. Accepted for publication in final form September 15, 1993.
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Kathleen Welsh, Bryan Alzheimer’s Disease Research Center, Duke University Medical Center,
2200 West Main Street, Suite A-230, Durham, NC 27705.

April 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 609


mance on the CERAD battery is essential. The maximum score on the test remains 30.
This study reports performance on the CERAD Word-list memory (Trial 1, Trial 2. Trial 3 learning).
neuropsychological battery by 413 normal white This is a free-recall memory test that is also included in
adults, 50 to 89 years of age, enrolled in the large the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS; 20)
and assesses learning ability for new verbal information.
multicenter CERAD study. All subjects were exam- Three trials of a 10-item word list are presented. On each
ined by physicians to assess their neurologic and trial, the 10 words are presented in a different order. The
functional status independent of the results of the subject is instructed to read aloud each word as it is pre-
neuropsychological examination. Thus, this study sented. Immediately following each trial, the subject is
provides the normative data on the CERAD battery asked to recall as many items as possible. The maximum
needed for clinical diagnosis. Information from score on each trial is 10.
other important demographic groups (blacks and Constructional praxis (Praxis). This task is also de-
Hispanics) are not included here but are the subject rived from t h e ADAS.20 It measures visuospatial and
of the expanded enrollment in the CERAD project. constructional abilities and requires the subject to copy
four line drawings presented in order of increasing com-
plexity (circle, diamond, overlapping rectangles, cube).
Methods. Subjects. Control subjects were recruited from The total possible score is 11.
23 participating US C E W sites. The sample of 413 in- Word-list recall (Delav). This test assesses the ability
dividuals included in these analyses consists of approxi- to recall, after a 5-minute delay, the 10 words given in
mately e q u a l n u m b e r s of spouses of p a t i e n t s w i t h the word-list memory test. The maximum number of cor-
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) enrolled in the CERAD study rect responses is 10. For each subject, a savings score
and community volunteers. Because the number of en- (Savings) is computed and is presented as a percentage,
rolled black and Hispanic subjects was too small at the reflecting the relative amount of verbal information re-
time of data analysis to support normative inferences, tained over the delay interval ([DelayPTrial 31 X 100 =
only the normative data for the white participants are Savings).
presented here. All subjects satisfied strict entry criteria Word-list recomition. The last test in the battery as-
excluding serious neurologic, medical, and psychiatric dis- sesses recognition of the words presented in the word-list
orders that could affect mentation. All were 50 years of memory task (Rec-Yes) when presented among 10 dis-
age or older at entry into the study, and their first lan- tractor words (Rec-No). The maximum score for correct
guage was English. Informed consent was obtained from recognition of the targets is 10. The maximum score for
each subject by methods approved by the institutional re- correct discrimination of the distractors is also 10.
view board at each center. Procedures. The CERAD control subjects tended to
Clinical assessment. Each of the control subjects en- have high levels of education, similar to other research
rolled in the CERAD project was examined by physicians studies of this nature.21v22For this reason, the sample
participating in CERAD. The clinical assessment, de- was subdivided into two groups, those with less than a
scribed elsewhere,14 consists of a semistructured inter- high school education and those with 12 years of school-
view of cognitive and functional status, drug inventory, ing or more. Although the number of lower-education in-
depression a n d medical history, t h e six-item S h o r t dividuals was small, it was nevertheless retained to
Blessed test,16 and a general physical and neurologic ex- allow exploratory analyses of the effects of educational
amination, usually carried out by a neurologist or psychi- level on t h e various cognitive measures. These two
atrist. Stage of disease was measured by the Clinical De- groups were further subdivided by age (50 to 69 years
mentia Rating ~ c a 1 e . l ~ and 70+ years) for an evaluation of the effects of age on
Neuropsychologicat testing. The CERAD neuropsycho- performance. The sample of control subjects with high
logical assessment battery was administered at each of education was further divided into two groups by gender.
the participating sites by trained, certified psychometri- The number of subjects in the lower education sample,
cians, independent of the physician’s clinical evaluation. however, was too small to allow further subdivision by
The battery was designed to assess the basic cognitive gender. This would have resulted i n cell sizes of fewer
functions affected in AD and includes the various mea- t h a n five individuals i n some cases, which could give
sures listed below, presented in the order of administra- misleading findings. Thus, analyses of performance on
tion within the battery. The subtest abbreviations that t h e various cognitive measures in t h e low-education
will be used throughout the text are also provided. groups were collapsed across gender.
Verbal Fluencv Test (Fluencv). This test measures Statistical analysis. Means and SDs were computed
verbal production, semantic memory, and language.18 It for each of the above subdivided groups in the high- and
requires the subject to name as many examples of the low-education strata. Comparisons of performance on
category “animal” as possible in 1minute. each of the cognitive measures were made with ANOVA
Boston Naming Test-modified (Naming). This test and post hoc t tests. Some cognitive measures proved
measures visual naming and presents 15 line drawings limited in range (eg, Naming). In these situations, equiv-
of common objects from the Boston Naming Test.lg These a l e n t nonparametric statistical t e s t s were used (eg,
items are stratified into three groups of five items each, Wilcoxon’s). To better define performance on the various
representing objects of high (easy to name), medium, and measures, the groups were collapsed and the distribution
low (hard to name) frequency of occurrence in the En- of scores was determined. All statistical analyses were
glish language. The maximum score is 15. carried out by the CERAD Methodology and Data Man-
Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE). This is a agement Center using SAS (SAS, version 6.07, SAS In-
slightly modified version of the well-known screen of cog- stitute, Cary, NC, 1991).
nitive function3 t h a t measures orientation, language,
concentration, constructional praxis, and memory. The
serial subtraction subtest has been replaced by the alter- Results. Characteristics o f the sample. Demo-
native test procedure, ie, spelling “world” backwards. graphic data for the high and low education groups
610 NEUROLOGY 44 April 1994
are presented in table 1.The average level of educa- tend t o do better than older subjects), the differ-
tion for men and women was similar within each of ences in the high-education group suggest that
the education strata (212 years, <12 years). How- women might be expected t o slightly outperform
ever, the ages of the men and women in the educa- men (who are older) on the various neuropsychologi-
tion groups differed significantly. In the highly edu- cal measures. In the low-education sample, in which
cated group, the men were significantly older than the groups are collapsed across gender, differences
the women, i n contrast with the low-education between men and women need t o be considered
group, in which the women were older than the men when interpreting the data. Normative results in
(p’s <0.001). Because age is often a significant modi- the low-education group may be largely driven by
fier of cognitive performance (ie, younger subjects the performance of elderly women.
Distribution of scores. The distribution of scores
on each measure for the entire sample is presented
Table 1. Demographic variables for total sample in table 2. In some instances, particularly on the
(N = 413) Naming, Rec-Yes, and Rec-No tasks, the distribu-
tion of scores clustered close t o the ceiling score.
212 Years education <12 Years education These limitations in range should be considered in
Men Women Men Women interpreting the data because the clinical implica-
(N= 127) (N= 240) (N= 16) (N= 30) tions of very small, albeit statistically significant,
Age (years) differences are likely to be minor. Use of cut-scores
Mean (SD) 70.5 (6.5) 67.0 (7.9) 67.4 (7.5) 71.6 (8.8) or percentile ranks may facilitate interpretation of
Range 55.6-84.9 50.5-88.3 55.5-80.5 53.5-86.8 individual performance outcomes.
Education (years) High-education group. The neuropsychological
Mean (SD) 14.9 (2.6) 14.5 (2.4) 9.1 (1.9) 9.1 (1.4) performance in the sample with 212 years of educa-
Range 12.0-21.0 12.0-21.0 5.0-11.0 6.0-11.0 tion (presented in table 3) should be considered
with the above caveats in mind. The analyses indi-

Table 2. Raw scores and cumulative percentages on CERAD measures:Distribution for total population
T = 413)

Raw
score MMSE Naming Fluency Praxis Trial 1 Trial2 Trial3 Delay Rec-Yes Rec-No

231 5 (100%)
30 169(100%) 4 (99%)
29 127(61%) 3 (99%)
28 66(30%) 6 (99%)
27 32(14%) 6 (98%)
26 14(6%) 7 (97%)
25 2(<3%) 7 (95%)
24 2(<2%) 15 (93%)
23 1(<1%) 16 (89%)
22 26 (85%)
21 19 (79%)
20 28 (74%)
19 28 (67%)
18 34 (60%)
17 32 (52%)
16 48 (44%)
15 284 (100%) 36 (32%)
14 100 (32%) 23 (23%)
13 22 (8%) 20 (17%)
12 4 (3%) 22 (12%)
11 2 (<2%) 15 (6%) 221 (54%)
10 1(<1%) 6(2%) 94 (23%) 3 (100%) 34 (100%) 86 (100%) 41 (100%) 323 (100%) 381 (100%)
9 6 (1%) 55 (13%) 9 (99%) 52 (92%) 105 (78%) 54 (89%) 64 (21%) 24 (8%)
8 - 24 (6%) 30 (97%) 111 (79%) 112 (53%) 105 (76%) 14 (6%) 8 (2%)
7 1(<l%) 14 (3%) 47 (90%) 104 (52%) 66 (26%) 81 (51%) 7 (3%)
6 5 (1%) 85 (79%) 66 (26%) 29 (10%) 62 (31%) 4 (1%)
5 109 (58%) 32 (11%) 11(3%) 39 (16%) 1 (<l%)
4 75 (31%) 8 (3%) 4 (<l%) 14 (7%)
3 39 (13%) 4 (1%) - 12 (4%)
2 13 (3%) 2 (<1%) - 2 (1%)
1 3 (<l%) - - 1 (<l%)
0 2 (<1%)

The number of individuals scoring a t each level of the various psychometric measures is indicated. Values in parentheses indicate the percentage
of subjects in the sample scoring a t that level or lower.

April 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 611


Table 3. Means and SDs on the CERAD measures by age and gender (N = 413)
I 1

n=61 n=151 n = 66 n=89 factors n = 23 n = 23 factors

MMSE 28.9 (1.1) 29.4 (0.8) 28.6 (1.3) 28.9 (1.3) 28.4 (1.3) 27.6 (2.2) ED*
Fluency 18.4 (5.8) 19.0 (4.8) 18.3 (4.5) 17.2 (4.2) 16.6 (4.4) 14.4 (3.7)
Naming 14.7 (0.7) 14.7 (0.6) 14.6 (0.7) 14.5 (0.7) 14.4(1.1) 14.3 (1.1) ED**
Praxis 10.3 (0.9) 10.3 (1.0) 10.3 (1.0) 10.0 (1.3) 9.9 (1.2) 8.8 (1.9) A*, ED**
Trial 1 5.1 (1.5) 6.1 (1.5) 4.6 (1.5) 5.1 (1.6) 4.1 (1.2) 4.8 (1.6)
learning
Trial 2 7.1 (1.5) 8.0 (1.4) 6.6 (1.5) 7.3 (1.2) 6.4 (1.7) 6.6 (1.7)
learning
Trial 3 8.0 (1.3) 8.8 (1.0) 7.7 (1.5) 8.2 11.4) 8.1 (1.3) 7.6 (1.9) ED*
learning
Delay 7.0 (2.1) 7.9 (1.6) 6.3 (1.8) 6.9 (1.7) 7.0 (1.9) 6.7 (1.9)
recall
Savings 86.7 (21.7) 89.9 (15.5) 81.5 (19.6) 85.3 (18.9) 86.8 (17.8) 89.7 (17.3)
Rec-Yes 9.7 (0.8) 9.8 10.5) 9.5 (0.8) 9.7 (0.7) 9.3 (1.0) 9.3 (1.2) ED**
Rec-No 9.9 (0.5) 9.9 (0.2) 9.8 (0.5) 9.9 (0.3) 9.9 (0.3) 9.9 (0.3)
Means and SDs (presented in parentheses) for each of the CERAD measures in the high- (N = 367) and low- ( N = 46) education groups. For the
high-education strata (left side of table), the data for men and women are presented separately, and age, gender, and age X gender effects were
explored. In the low-education strata (right side of table), only age effects could be explored because of limited sample size. Overall education
effects were examined in the entire sample, composed of both high- and low-educated subjects.
A = age factor. G = gender factor. ED = education factor. Statistically significant overall group differences (ANOVA) are indicated (*I for each
factor. * p < 0.05, * * p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

cated significant age and gender effects on some cation effects emerged, the more highly educated
measures where no serious restrictions in range group, as expected, outperformed the lower-edu-
were present (MMSE, p < 0.01; each of the three cated group. In contrast with these measures, there
learning trials and the Delay measure, p's < 0.001). were no significant differences between the low-
In each of these instances, the younger groups out- and high-education groups on the Fluency measure
performed the older groups and women tended to or on most of the learning and memory measures,
score higher than men did. Similar but milder de- including Trial 1 and Trial 2 learning, Delay, Sav-
clines with age were noted on the Savings measure ings, and recognition of distractors (Rec-No). The
and on the Naming test and one of the recognition only memory measures affected by education were
memory measures (Rec-Yes);no gender differences, Trial 3 learning and Rec-Yes.
however, were seen on any of these measures. Fi-
nally, performance on the Fluency and Praxis mea- Discussion. The results of these analyses indicate
sures were not influenced by age or gender in this that the neuropsychology measures in the CERAD
highly educated group. battery are differentially affected by age, education,
Low-education group. The neuropsychological and gender. This is particularly evident in the sub-
performance of individuals with <12 years of edu- jects with 12 years' or more education, of whom
cation is presented in the far-right columns of table there are sufficient numbers to evaluate all the fac-
3. Because of the small size of this sample, the ef- tors. In this group, tests of verbal learning and
fects of gender were not analyzed. Unlike the re- memory were most affected by age, education, and
sults in the highly educated group, no significant gender. Performance scores on the memory mea-
differences emerged between the age groups on s u r e s decreased with age a n d were higher in
tests of learning or memory. The only measure af- women. The memory measures were largely unaf-
fected by age in this sample was Praxis ( p < 0.05). fected by education, a finding consistent with other
Education effects. To explore the effects of educa- reports.23There were similar results with the
tion on the various neuropsychological measures, MMSE, confirming many previous s t ~ d i e s . ~ ~ In- ~ O
each education stratum (<12 years, 212 years) was contrast to these results, several measures of cogni-
collapsed across age and gender, and performance tive function were affected by some but not all de-
on the measures was then compared between the mographic factors. For example, Praxis was affected
two education groups. The results of these analyses by education but not by age or gender; Naming was
are presented in the far-right column of table 3. affected by age and education but not gender; and
Analysis of MMSE scores across groups revealed Savings, although slightly vulnerable to age, was
significant differences ( p < 0.051, and highly signifi- not affected by either education or gender. In the
cant differences were also seen on Naming and subjects with fewer than 12 years' education, of
Praxis (p's < 0.01). In each instance, whenever edu- whom the sample size is small, we did not analyze
612 NEUROLOGY 44 April 1994
gender. The Praxis measure was the only measure sure). The level of education did not affect the Delay
affected by age in this group. or Savings memory measures. The amount of edu-
The lack of an age effect in either the high- or cation did affect performance on the Naming and
low-education groups on the Fluency measure was the Praxis procedures. When applying these find-
somewhat surprising and appears to conflict with ings to the clinical evaluation of the elderly, particu-
other reports that indicate age-related declines in lar attention should be directed to the Delay mea-
verbal p r o d u ~ t i o n .Those
~ ~ , ~ studies,
~ however, in- sure and the Savings scores. In the early stages of
cluded a broader range of ages. Thus, the apparent dementia (such as AD), cognitive loss caused by this
conflict between studies may actually indicate that disorder should be suspected if both the Delay score
little additional age-related change in fluency oc- and the Savings score are significantly decreased.
curs after age 67 or so (the average age of our Modest impairments in delayed recall, when associ-
youngest subgroup), an interpretation supported by ated with adequate Savings scores, would be less di-
other studies using similar fluency procedure^.^^-^^ agnostic. These considerations, however, should de-
Of special importance in this study are the find- pend on the results of the clinical examination and
ings obtained with the Savings scores. Unlike the other neuropsychological findings.
other learning and memory measures, including
Delay, the Savings scores showed only minimal ef-
fects of age, and then only in the highly educated
Acknowledgments
group. This finding is consistent with other reports We would like to acknowledge the various CERAD centers and
u s i n g s i m i l a r memory m e a s u r e s i n t h e el- the many investigators (neurologists, psychiatrists, neuropsy-
chologists, and clinical coordinators) a t each site who con-
derly,23,31,36-38 which indicate that Delay, particu- tributed data to this work. The centers include Burke Rehabili-
larly when adjusted for acquisition, is fairly resis- tation Center, White Plains, Ny, Baylor University College of
tant to decline with age. This measure, which is Medicine, Houston, TX; Case Western Reserve University,
very sensitive to early AD12J3fortunately is rela- Cleveland, OH; Columbia University, New York, Ny, Duke Uni-
versity Medical Center, Durham, NC; Emory University School
tively unaffected by age, education, and gender. of Medicine, Atlanta, GA; Graduate Hospital, Philadelphia, PA,
Since the learning and Delay measures are some- Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, MD; Massachusetts Gen-
what depressed as a function of normal aging (and eral Hospital, Boston, M A Mount Sinai Medical Center, New
to a greater extent as a function of AD), the pres- York, Ny; University of Alabama, Birmingham, AL; University
ence of ambiguous performance, such as partial for- of Kentucky Medical Center, Lexington, Ky; University of Pitts-
burgh Medical School, Pittsburgh, PA; University of Rochester
getting, poses a difficult diagnostic dilemma. How- Medical Center, Rochester, Ny, University of Southern Califor-
ever, since the ratio of the measures (Savings) de- nia, Los Angeles, CA; University of Texas, Southwestern Medi-
creases only slightly with age and is unaffected by cal Center, Dallas, Tx; University of Washington, Seattle, WA;
gender and education, which often confound inter- Veterans Administration Medical Center and University of Cali-
fornia at S a n Diego, La Jolla, CA, Veterans Administration
pretation, the decrease in Savings may be particu- Medical Center, Minneapolis, MN; and Washington University
larly helpful in diagnosing cognitive impairment. School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO. We also wish to acknowledge
Performance on both Delay and Savings should be the contributions to this work of the various members of the
impaired in cases of AD. Thus, considering both CERAD steering committee: Leonard Berg, MD, coprincipal in-
measures together should increase the likelihood of vestigator of the CERAD study, and Mokhtar Gado, MD (Wash-
ington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO); Patricia
detecting and differentiating this disorder from the Davis, MD, and Suzanne Mirra, MD (Emory University School
benign cognitive impairment in aging. of Medicine, Atlanta, GA); Gerald van Belle, PhD (University of
Some caveats are necessary, however, when ap- Washington, Seattle, WA); and Marilyn Albert, PhD (Harvard
plying the above findings to clinical practice. The Medical School, Boston, MA). We appreciate the helpful com-
ments of Florence Nash and William Wilkinson, PhD, and the
present sample consists entirely of white subjects technical assistance of Kathleen Keating and Ron Nelson of the
who are, for the most part, well educated. The Bryan Alzheimer's Disease Research Center.
number of subjects with lower educational attain-
ment was very small, and for this reason, the
norms for this subgroup must be considered only References
preliminary. Because of these small numbers, the 1. Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M. The association between
power to detect education effects was reduced, and quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in
* the cerebral erev matter of elderlv subiects. Br J Psvchiatrv
the influence of low education on the individual 1968;114:79fSl"l.
" . I

measures in the CERAD battery may not be sub- 2. Eslinger PJ, Damasio AR, Benton AL,Van Allen M. Neu-
stantive. Moreover, extrapolation of our findings t o ropsychological detection of abnormal mental decline i n
populations with different culturaVethnic groups35 older persons. JAMA 1985;253:670-674.
may be inadvisable. 3. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-Mental State":
a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients
In summary, the use of the CERAD battery in for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.
this sample of normal individuals ages 50 to 89 4. Jacobs JW,Bernhard MR, Delgado A, Strain JJ. Screening
shows that older persons perform less well on many for organic mental syndromes in the medically ill. Ann In-
of the subtests. The CERAD word list learning test tern Med 1977;86:40-46.
and the Delay measure particularly appear affected 5. Kahn RL, Goldfarb AL,Pollack M, Peck A. Brief objective
measures for t h e determination of mental status in t h e
by age. Importantly, however, age had only a mini- aged. Am J Psychiatry 1960;117:326-328.
mal effect on delayed recall when initial learning 6. Kendrick DC, Gibson AJ, Moyes IC. The Revised Kendrick
was taken into consideration (ie, the Savings mea- battery: clinical studies. Br J SOCClin Psycho1 1979;18:329-
April 1994 NEUROLOGY 44 613
340. 22. Ivnik RT, Malec J F , Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E,
7. Mattis S. Mental status examination for organic syndromes Kurland LT. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies:
in the elderly patient. In: Bellak L, Karasu TE, eds. Geri- updated AVLT norms for ages 56 to 97. Clin Neuropsychol
atric psychiatry. New York Grune and Stratton, 1976. 1992;6:83-104.
8. Pfeiffer E. A short portable mental status questionnaire for 23. Inouye SK, Albert MS, Mohs R, Sun K, B e r h a n LF. Cogni-
the assessment of organic brain deficit in elderly patients. J tive performance in a high functioning community-dwelling
Am Geriatr SOC1975;23:433-441. elderly population. J Gerontol 1993;48:M146-M151.
9. Anthony JC, LeResche L, Niaz U, Von Korff MR, Folstein 24. Bleecker ML, Bolla-Wilson K, Kawas C, Agnew J . Age-spe-
MF. Limits of the “Mini-Mental State” as a screening test cific norms for the Mini-Mental S t a t e Exam. Neurology
for dementia and delirium among hospital patients. Psychol 1988;38:1565-1568.
Med 1982;12:397-408. 25. Crum RM, Anthony JC, Bassett SS, Folstein MF. Popula-
10. Nelson A, Fogel BS, Faust D. Bedside cognitive screening in- tion-based norms for the Mini-Mental State Examination by
s t r u m e n t s : a critical a s s e s s m e n t . J N e r v M e n t D i s age and educational level. JAMA 1993;2692386-2391.
1986;174:73-83. 26. Cummings JL. Mini-Mental State Examination: norms, nor-
11. Fillenbaum G, Heyman A, Williams K, Prosnitz B, Burchett mals, and numbers. JAMA 1993;269:2420-2421.
B. Sensitivity and specificity of standardized screens of cogni- 27. Fillenbaum GG, Hughes DC, Heyman A, George LK, Blazer
tive impairment and dementia among elderly black and DG. Relationship of health and demographic characteristics
white community residents. J Clin Epidemiol 1990;43:651- to Mini-Mental State Examination score among community
660. residents. Psychol Med 1988;18:719-726.
12. Welsh KA, Butters N, Hughes J, Mohs R, Heyman A. Detec- 28. Jorm AF, Scott R, Henderson AS, Kay DWK. Educational
tion of abnormal memory in mild cases of Alzheimer’s dis- level differences on the Mini-Mental State: the role of test
ease using CERAD neuropsychological measures. Arch Neu- bias. Psychol Med 1988;18:727-731.
rol 1991;48:278-281. 29. O’Connor DW, Pollitt PA, Treasure FP, Brook CPB, Reiss
13. Welsh KA, Butters N, Hughes JP, Mohs RC, Heyman A. De- BB. The influence of education, social class and sex on Mini-
tection and staging of dementia in Alzheimer’s disease: use Mental State scores. Psychol Med 1989;19:771-776.
of the neuropsychological measures developed for the Con- 30. Tombaugh TN, McIntyre NJ. The Mini-Mental State Exami-
sortium t o Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s disease n a t i o n : a comprehensive review. J Am G e r i a t r S O C
(CERAD). Arch Neurol 1992;49:448-452. 1992;40:922-935.
14. Morris JC, Heyman A, Mohs RC, et al. The Consortium to 31. Wiederholt WC, Cahn D, Butters NM, Salmon DP, Kritz-Sil-
Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part verstein D, Barrett-Connor E. Effects of age, gender and ed-
I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer’s ucation on selected neuropsychological tests in an elderly
disease. Neurology 1989;39:1159-1165. community cohort. J Am Geriatr SOC1993;41:639-647.
15. Morris JC, Edland S, Clark C, et al. The Consortium to Es- 32. Spreen 0, Strauss E. A compendium of neuropsychological
tablish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD). Part tests: administration, norms and commentary. New York:
IV. Ratings of cognitive change in the longitudinal assess- Oxford University Press, 1991.
m e n t of probable Alzheimer’s d i s e a s e . Neurology 33. Bolla KI, Lindgren KN, Bonaccorsy C, Bleecker ML. Predic-
1993;43:2457-2465. tors of verbal fluency (FAS) in the healthy elderly. J Clin
16. Katzman R, Brown T, Fuld P, et al. Validation of a short ori- Psycho1 1990;46:623-628.
entation-memory-concentration test of cognitive impair- 34. Cauthen NR. Verbal fluency: normative data. J Clin Psychol
ment. Am J Psychiatry 1983;140:734-739. 1978;34:126-129.
17. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, Coben LA, Martin RL. A 35. Ganguli M, Ratcliff G, Huff FJ, e t al. Effects of age, gender,
new clinical scale for the staging of dementia. Br J Psychia- and education on cognitive tests in a rural elderly commu-
try 1982;140:566-572. nity sample: norms from the Monongahela Valley Indepen-
18. Rosen WG. Verbal fluency in aging and dementia. J Clin dent Elders Survey. Neuroepidemiology 1991;10:42-52.
Neuropsychol1980;2: 135-146. 36. Haaland KY, Linn RT, Hunt WC, Goodwin JS. A normative
19. Kaplan EF, Goodglass H, Weintraub S. The Boston naming study of Russell’s variant of the Wechsler Memory Scale in a
test. Boston, MA Veterans Administration Medical Center, h e a l t h y e l d e r l y population. J Consult Clin Psychol
1978. 1983;51:878-881.
20. Rosen WG, Mohs RC, Davis KL. A new rating scale for 37. Ivnik R J , Smith GE, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E ,
Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Psychiatry 1984;141:1356-1364. Kurland LT. Wechsler Memory Scale: I& dependent norms
21. Ivnik RJ, Malec JF, Tangalos EG, Petersen RC, Kokmen E, for persons ages 65 to 97 years. Psychol Assess 1991;3:156-
Kurland LT. Mayo’s Older Americans Normative Studies: 161.
WMS-R n o r m s for a g e s 5 6 t o 94. Clin Neuropsychol 38. Petersen RC, Smith G, Kokmen E, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG.
1992;6:49-82. Memory function in normal aging. Neurology 1992;42:396-401.

614 NEUROLOGY 44 April 1994


The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD). Part V.
A normative study of the neuropsychological battery
K. A. Welsh, N. Butters, R. C. Mohs, et al.
Neurology 1994;44;609
DOI 10.1212/WNL.44.4.609

This information is current as of April 1, 1994

Updated Information & including high resolution figures, can be found at:
Services http://www.neurology.org/content/44/4/609.full.html

Citations This article has been cited by 91 HighWire-hosted articles:


http://www.neurology.org/content/44/4/609.full.html##otherarticle
s
Permissions & Licensing Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,tables)
or in its entirety can be found online at:
http://www.neurology.org/misc/about.xhtml#permissions
Reprints Information about ordering reprints can be found online:
http://www.neurology.org/misc/addir.xhtml#reprintsus

Neurology ® is the official journal of the American Academy of Neurology. Published continuously
since 1951, it is now a weekly with 48 issues per year. Copyright © 1994 by AAN Enterprises, Inc.. All
rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0028-3878. Online ISSN: 1526-632X.

You might also like