You are on page 1of 22

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/37623241

Social Identity and Self-Categorization Processes in Organizational Contexts

Article  in  The Academy of Management Review · January 2000


DOI: 10.2307/259266 · Source: OAI

CITATIONS READS

2,095 22,707

2 authors, including:

Deborah J. Terry
The University of Queensland
201 PUBLICATIONS   13,305 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Diabetes & Cardiovascual Intervention Trial (DACIT) View project

The emotional face View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Deborah J. Terry on 04 June 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Social Identity and Self-Categorization Processes in Organizational Contexts
Author(s): Michael A. Hogg and Deborah J. Terry
Source: The Academy of Management Review, Vol. 25, No. 1 (Jan., 2000), pp. 121-140
Published by: Academy of Management
Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/259266
Accessed: 05/01/2009 12:36

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at
http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless
you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you
may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at
http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=aom.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed
page of such transmission.

JSTOR is a not-for-profit organization founded in 1995 to build trusted digital archives for scholarship. We work with the
scholarly community to preserve their work and the materials they rely upon, and to build a common research platform that
promotes the discovery and use of these resources. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org.

Academy of Management is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Academy
of Management Review.

http://www.jstor.org
? Academy of Management Review
2000, Vol. 25, No. 1, 121-140.

SOCIAL IDENTITYAND SELF-


CATEGORIZATION PROCESSESIN
ORGANIZATIONAL CONTEXTS
MICHAEL A. HOGG
DEBORAH J. TERRY
University of Queensland

Although aspects of social identity theory are familiar to organizational psycholo-


gists, its elaboration, through self-categorization theory, of how social categorization
and prototype-based depersonalization actually produce social identity effects is less
well known. We describe these processes, relate self-categorization theory to social
identity theory, describe new theoretical developments in detail, and show how these
developments can address a range of organizational phenomena. We discuss cohe-
sion and deviance, leadership, subgroup and sociodemographic structure, and merg-
ers and acquisitions.

Organizations are internally structured 1998. The new interest in groups is different.
groups that are located in complex networks of There is less emphasis on interactive small
intergroup relations characterized by power, groups, group structure, and interpersonal rela-
status, and prestige differentials. To varying de- tions within groups, and there is more emphasis
grees, people derive part of their identity and on the self concept: how the self is defined by
sense of self from the organizations or work- group membership and how social cognitive
groups to which they belong. Indeed, for many processes associated with group membership-
people their professional and/or organizational based self-definition produce characteristically
identity may be more pervasive and important "groupy" behavior. This revival of interest in
than ascribed identities based on gender, age, group processes and identity has been influ-
ethnicity, race, or nationality. It is perhaps not enced significantly by the development within
surprising that social psychologists who study social psychology of social identity theory and
groups often peek over the interdisciplinary self-categorization theory. A search of PsychLit
fence at what their colleagues in organizational in mid 1997 for the key terms social identity and
psychology are up to. Some, disillusioned with self-categorization resulted in a list of almost
social cognition as the dominant paradigm in 550 publications since 1991.
mainstream social psychology, vault the fence, In this article we introduce social identity the-
thus fueling recent and not so recent laments ory as a platform from which to describe in de-
within social psychology that the study of tail how social categorization and prototype-
groups may be alive and well, but not in social based depersonalization actually produce
psychology (e.g., Levine & Moreland, 1990; social identity phenomena. We explain how
Steiner, 1974). these processes, which are the conceptual core
Over the past 10 or 15 years, however, there of self-categorization theory, relate to the origi-
has been a marked revival of interest among nal and more familiar intergroup and self-
social psychologists in the study of groups and enhancement motivational perspective of social
group processes (e.g., Abrams & Hogg, 1998; identity theory. We show how recent conceptual
Hogg & Abrams, 1999; Hogg & Moreland, 1995; advances based largely, although not exclu-
Moreland, Hogg, & Hains, 1994), even spawning sively, on self-categorization theory have great
two new journals: Group Dynamics in 1996 and but as yet largely unexplored potential for our
Group Processes and Intergroup Relations in understanding of social behaviors in organiza-
tional contexts. We have tried to energize this
This article was written while Michael Hogg spent a year potential by describing various speculations,
as visiting professor in the Department of Psychology at hypotheses, and propositions that can act as a
Princeton University. framework for empirical research.
121
122 Academy of Management Review January

Some of the key theoretical innovations we press; Riordan & Shore, 1997; Tsui, Egan, &
promote are based on the ideas that (1) social O'Reilly, 1992).
identity processes are motivated by subjective This work, however, often touches only the
uncertainty reduction, (2) prototype-based de- surface of social identity theory. It focuses on
personalization lies at the heart of social iden- some aspects but does not systematically incor-
tity processes, and (3) groups are internally porate significant theoretical developments
structured in terms of perceived or actual group made since 1987 that focus on self-categoriza-
prototypicality of members. After introducing so- tion, group prototypicality, contextual salience,
cial identity theory and describing self-catego- and depersonalization processes (see Pratt, in
rization mechanisms, we discuss cohesion and press). These developments have enabled social
deviance, leadership, group structure, sub- identity theorists to extend the theory's concep-
groups, sociodemographic groups, and mergers tual and empirical focus on intergroup phenom-
and acquisitions. We have chosen these group ena to incorporate a focus on what happens
phenomena because they particularly benefit within groups; it has become what could be
from the self-categorization-based extension of called an extended social identity theory. For
social identity theory. They capture the inter- example, in recent work on social psychology,
play of intergroup and intragroup relations and researchers have explored social influence and
the conceptual importance of prototypicality, de- norms (e.g., Turner, 1991); solidarity and cohe-
personalization, and uncertainty. They are also sion (e.g., Hogg, 1992); attitudes, behavior, and
particularly organizationally relevant phenom- norms (e.g., Terry & Hogg, 1999); small groups
ena, where social identity theory can make a (e.g., Hogg, 1996a); group motivation (Hogg, in
press a,b; Hogg & Abrams, 1993a; Hogg & Mullin,
contribution.
1999); and group structure and leadership (e.g.,
Before we begin, we underscore two caveats.
Hogg, 1996b, 1999).1
First, consistent with social identity theory's
group level of analysis and cognitive definition
of the social group (e.g., Turner, 1982; Turner, SOCIALIDENTITYAND
Hogg, Oakes, Reicher, & Wetherell, 1987), we SELF-CATEGORIZATION
consider organizations to be groups, units or
Tajfel first introduced the concept of social
divisions within organizations to be groups, pro-
identity-"the individual's knowledge that he
fessions or sociodemographic categories that
belongs to certain social groups together with
are distributed across organizations to be some emotional and value significance to him of
groups, and so forth-all with different social this group membership" (1972: 292)-to move
identities and group prototypes. Thus, inter- from his earlier consideration of social, largely
group relations can exist between organiza- intergroup, perception (i.e., stereotyping and
tions, between units or divisions within an or- prejudice) to consideration of how self is concep-
ganization, between professions that are within tualized in intergroup contexts: how a system of
but transcend organizations, and so forth. Sa- social categorizations "creates and defines an
lience mechanisms, described below, determine individual's own place in society" (Tajfel, 1972:
which group and, therefore, intergroup relation- 293). Social identity rests on intergroup social
ship is psychologically salient as a basis for comparisons that seek to confirm or to establish
self-conceptualization in a given context. ingroup-favoring evaluative distinctiveness be-
Second, social identity theory is not entirely tween ingroup and outgroup, motivated by an
new to organizational psychologists. Although underlying need for self-esteem (Turner, 1975).
already adopted to some extent by organization- Tajfel (1974a,b) quickly developed the theory
al researchers, Ashforth and Mael (1989) first to specify how beliefs about the nature of rela-
systematically introduced the theory to organi-
zational psychology (also see Ashforth & Hum-
phrey, 1993, and Nkomo & Cox, 1996) and subse- 1 For general developments, see books by Abrams and
quently published some related empirical work Hogg (1990, 1999); Hogg and Abrams (1988); Hogg and Terry
(in press); Oakes, Haslam, and Turner (1994); Robinson (1996);
(e.g., Mael & Ashforth, 1992, 1995). Others have
Spears, Oakes, Ellemers, and Haslam (1997); Terry and Hogg
also applied it to organizational settings (e.g., (1999); Turner et al. (1987); and Worchel, Morales, Pdez, and
Dutton, Dukerich, & Harquail, 1994; Pratt, in Deschamps (1998).
2000 Hogg and Terry 123

tions between groups (status, stability, perme- 1994; Spears et al., 1997) but has hitherto at-
ability, legitimacy) influence the way that indi- tracted little attention in organizational psy-
viduals or groups pursue positive social chology (Pratt, in press).
identity. Tajfel and Turner (1979) retained this
emphasis in their classic statement of social
SELF-CATEGORIZATIONTHEORY
identity theory. The emphasis on social identity
as part of the self-concept was explored more Self-categorization theory specifies the opera-
fully by Turner (1982). In a comprehensive cov- tion of the social categorization process as the
erage of relevant research, Hogg and Abrams cognitive basis of group behavior. Social cate-
(1988) then integrated and grounded intergroup, gorization of self and others into ingroup and
self-conceptual, and motivational emphases. At outgroup accentuates the perceived similarity of
about the same time, Turner and his colleagues the target to the relevant ingroup or outgroup
(Turner, 1985; Turner et al., 1987) extended social prototype (cognitive representation of features
identity theory through the development of self- that describe and prescribe attributes of the
categorization theory, which specified in detail group). Targets are no longer represented as
how social categorization produces prototype- unique individuals but, rather, as embodiments
based depersonalization of self and others and, of the relevant prototype-a process of deper-
thus, generates social identity phenomena. sonalization. Social categorization of self-self-
Social identity theory and/or self-categoriza- categorization- cognitively assimilates self to
tion theory has been described by social identity the ingroup prototype and, thus, depersonalizes
theorists in detail elsewhere (e.g., Hogg, 1992, self-conception. This transformation of self is
1993, 1996b; Hogg & Abrams, 1988; Hogg, Terry, & the process underlying group phenomena, be-
White, 1995; Tajfel & Turner, 1986; for historical cause it brings self-perception and behavior in
accounts see Hogg, in press c; Hogg & Abrams, line with the contextually relevant ingroup pro-
1999; Turner, 1996). Because the original form of totype. It produces, for instance, normative be-
social identity theory is familiar to organization- havior, stereotyping, ethnocentrism, positive in-
al psychologists, we do not redescribe it here. group attitudes and cohesion, cooperation and
Instead, we focus on self-categorization theory, altruism, emotional contagion and empathy,
which is less familiar to and less accessible for collective behavior, shared norms, and mutual
organizational psychologists in terms of its pro- influence. Depersonalization refers simply to a
cesses, its relationship to social identity theory, change in self-conceptualization and the basis
and its potential for explicating organizational of perception of others; it does not have the neg-
processes (Pratt, in press). ative connotations of such terms as deindividu-
Self-categorization theory clearly evolves ation or dehumanization (cf. Reicher, Spears, &
from Tajfel's and Turner's earlier ideas on social Postmes, 1995).
identity. We view it as a development of social
identity theory or, more accurately, as that com-
ponent of an extended social identity theory of Representation of Groups As Prototypes
the relationship between self-concept and group The notion of prototypes, which is not part of
behavior that details the social cognitive pro- the earlier intergroup focus of social identity
cesses that generate social identity effects (e.g., theory, is absolutely central to self-categoriza-
Abrams & Hogg, in press; Hogg, 1996a, in press c; tion theory. People cognitively represent the de-
Hogg & Abrams, 1988, 1999, in press; Hogg & fining and stereotypical attributes of groups in
McGarty, 1990; Hogg, Terry, & White, 1995). We the form of prototypes. Prototypes are typically
see no incompatibility between self-categoriza- not checklists of attributes but, rather, fuzzy sets
tion theory and the original form of social iden- that capture the context-dependent features of
tity theory but view self-categorization theory, group membership, often in the form of repre-
rather, as an important and powerful new con- sentations of exemplary members (actual group
ceptual component of an extended social iden- members who best embody the group) or ideal
tity theory. The self-categorization component of types (an abstraction of group features). Proto-
social identity theory has been very influential types embody all attributes that characterize
in recent developments within social psychol- groups and distinguish them from other groups,
ogy (e.g., Abrams & Hogg, 1999; Oakes et al., including beliefs, attitudes, feelings, and behav-
124 Academy of Management Review January

iors. A critical feature of prototypes is that they Abrams, 1990, 1993b; Hogg & Mullin, 1999; Long &
maximize similarities within and differences Spears, 1997; Rubin & Hewstone, 1998). Self-
between groups, thus defining groups as dis- categorization theory's focus on the categoriza-
tinct entities. Prototypes form according to the tion process hints at an additional (perhaps
principle of metacontrast: maximization of the more fundamental), epistemic, motivation for so-
ratio of intergroup differences to intragroup dif- cial identity, which has only recently been de-
ferences. Because members of the same group scribed-the uncertainty reduction hypothesis
are exposed to similar social information, their (Hogg, in press a,b; Hogg & Abrams, 1993b; Hogg
prototypes usually will be similar and, thus, & Mullin, 1999). In addition to being motivated
shared. by self-enhancement, social identity processes
Prototypes are stored in memory but are con- are also motivated by a need to reduce subjec-
structed, maintained, and modified by features tive uncertainty about one's perceptions, atti-
of the immediate or more enduring social inter- tudes, feelings, and behaviors and, ultimately,
active context (e.g., Fiske & Taylor, 1991). They one's self-concept and place within the social
are highly context dependent and are particu- world. Uncertainty reduction, particularly about
larly influenced by what outgroup is contextu- subjectively important matters that are gener-
ally salient. Enduring changes in prototypes ally self-conceptually relevant, is a core human
and, therefore, self-conception can arise if the motivation. Certainty renders existence mean-
relevant comparison outgroup changes over ingful and confers confidence in how to behave
time-for instance, if Catholics gradually define and what to expect from the physical and social
themselves in contradistinction to Muslims environment within which one finds oneself.
rather than to Protestants, or if a car manufac- Self-categorization reduces uncertainty by
turer compares itself to a computer software transforming self-conception and assimilating
manufacturer rather than to another car manu- self to a prototype that describes and prescribes
facturer. Such changes are also transitory in perceptions, attitudes, feelings, and behaviors.
that they are tied to whatever outgroup is sa- Because prototypes are relatively consensual,
lient in the immediate social context. For in- they also furnish moral support and consensual
stance, a psychology department may experi- validation for one's self-concept and attendant
ence a contextual change in self-definition if it cognitions and behaviors. It is the prototype that
compares itself with a management school actually reduces uncertainty. Hence, uncer-
rather than with a history department. Thus, so- tainty is better reduced by prototypes that are
cial identity is dynamic. It is responsive, in type simple, clear, highly focused, and consensual,
and content, to intergroup dimensions of imme- and that, thus, describe groups that have pro-
diate comparative contexts. nounced entitativity (Campbell, 1958; also see
Brewer & Harasty, 1996; Hamilton & Sherman,
Proposition 1: Changes in the inter-
1996; Hamilton, Sherman, & Lickel, 1998; Sher-
organizational comparative context
man, Hamilton, & Lewis, 1999), are very cohesive
affect the content of organizational
(Hogg, 1992, 1993), and provide a powerful social
prototypes.
identity. Such groups and prototypes will be at-
As we will see, the content of prototypes tractive to individuals who are contextually or
strongly influences the group phenomena dis- more enduringly highly uncertain, or during
cussed later in the article. times of or in situations characterized by great
uncertainty.
Self-Enhancement and Uncertainty Reduction Proposition 2: Subjective uncertainty
Motivations may produce a prototypically homog-
enous and cohesive organization or
According to social identity theory, social
work unit with which members iden-
identity and intergroup behavior are guided by
the pursuit of evaluatively positive social iden- tify strongly.
tity, through positive intergroup distinctiveness, Uncertainty reduction and self-enhancement
which, in turn, is motivated by the need for pos- are probably independent motivations for social
itive self-esteem-the self-esteem hypothesis identity processes, and in some circumstances it
(e.g., Abrams & Hogg, 1988; see also Hogg & may be more urgent to reduce uncertainty than
2000 Hogg and Terry 125

to pursue self-enhancement (e.g., when group ceptual accentuation of intragroup similarities


entitativity is threatened), whereas in others it and intergroup differences, thereby maximizing
may be the opposite (e.g., when group prestige separateness and clarity. Self-categorization in
is threatened). However, uncertainty reduction terms of the activated ingroup category then
may be more fundamentally adaptive because it depersonalizes behavior in terms of the ingroup
constructs a self-concept that defines who we prototype.
are and prescribes what we should perceive, Salience is not, however, a mechanical prod-
think, feel, and do. uct of accessibility and fit (Hogg, 1996b; Hogg &
The uncertainty reduction hypothesis has Mullin, 1999). Social interaction involves the mo-
clear relevance for organizational contexts. In- tivated manipulation of symbols (e.g., through
deed, the hypothesis is not inconsistent with speech, appearance, and behavior) by people
Lester's (1987) uncertainty reduction theory that who are strategically competing with one an-
plays an important role in Saks and Ashforth's other to influence the frame of reference within
(1997) multilevel process model of organization- which accessibility and fit interact. People are
al socialization. Although in both cases uncer- not content to have their identity determined by
tainty motivates group socialization behaviors, the social cognitive context. On the contrary,
the uncertainty reduction hypothesis specifies they say and do things to try to change the
self-categorization as the social cognitive pro- parameters so that a subjectively more mean-
cess that resolves uncertainty through proto- ingful and self-favoring identity becomes sa-
type-based self-depersonalization. lient. For instance, a mixed-sex conversation
about the communication of feelings is likely to
make sex salient, because the chronically ac-
Salience of Social Identity
cessible category "sex" is situationally accessi-
The responsiveness of social identity to imme- ble and has good structural and normative fit.
diate social contexts is a central feature of so- Male interactants who find the self-evaluative
cial identity theory-and self-categorization implications of gender stereotypes about feel-
theory within it. The cognitive system, governed ings unfavorable might change the topic of con-
by uncertainty reduction and self-enhancement versation to politics so that sex becomes situ-
motives, matches social categories to properties ationally less accessible and now has poor
of the social context and brings into active use structural and normative fit. In this way a differ-
(i.e., makes salient) that category rendering the ent, and self-evaluatively more favorable, iden-
social context and one's place within it subjec- tity may become salient.
tively most meaningful. Specifically, there is an This dynamic perspective on identity and self-
interaction between category accessibility and conceptual salience has clear implications for
category fit so that people draw on accessible organizational contexts. Manipulation of the in-
categories and investigate how well they fit the tergroup social comparative context can be a
social field. The category that best fits the field powerful way to change organizational identity
becomes salient in that context (e.g., Oakes et (self-conception as a member of a particular or-
al., 1994; Oakes & Turner, 1990). ganization) and, thus, attitudes, motives, goals,
Categories can be accessible because they and practices. Organizations or divisions within
are valued, important, and frequently employed organizations that have poor work practices or
aspects of the self-concept (i.e., chronic accessi- organizational attitudes can be helped to recon-
bility) and/or because they are perceptually sa- struct themselves, through surreptitious or overt
lient (i.e., situational accessibility). Categories changes in the salience of relevant intergroup
fit the social field because they account for situ- comparative contexts (different levels of catego-
ationally relevant similarities and differences rization or different outgroups at the same level
among people (i.e., structural fit) and/or because of categorization). Such changes affect contex-
category specifications account for context- tual self-categorization and, therefore, people's
specific behaviors (i.e., normative fit). Once fully internalized attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Terry
activated (as opposed to merely "tried on") on & Hogg, 1996).
the basis of optimal fit, category specifications One way in which organizations may deliber-
organize themselves as contextually relevant ately manipulate the intergroup social compar-
prototypes and are used as a basis for the per- ative context is by "benchmarking." An organi-
126 Academy of Management Review January

zation selects specific other organizations as a that liking, can change dramatically when an
legitimate comparison set, which threatens the aggregate becomes a salient group (e.g., when
group's prestige. This motivates upward redefi- uncertainty or entitativity are high, or when the
nition of organizational identity and work prac- group is under threat or is engaged in inter-
tices, to make the group evaluatively more com- group competition over a valued scarce re-
petitive. source). Social and personal attraction are not
Self-categorization theory's focus on proto- isomorphic (see Mullen & Copper, 1994). These
types allows some important conceptual devel- predictions have been supported repeatedly by
opments in social identity theory, which have a program of research with laboratory, quasi
direct implications for organizational contexts. naturalistic, sports, and organizational groups
When group membership is salient, cognition is
(Hogg, Cooper-Shaw, & Holzworth, 1993; Hogg &
attuned to and guided by prototypicality. Thus,
Hains, 1996; Hogg & Hardie, 1991, 1992, 1997;
within groups people are able to distinguish
Hogg, Hardie, & Reynolds, 1995; see overviews
among themselves and others in terms of how
by Hogg, 1992, 1993).
well they match the prototype. An intragroup
One practical implication of the idea of deper-
prototypicality gradient exists-some people
are or are perceived to be more prototypical sonalized social attraction in an organizational
than others (Hogg, 1996a,b, 1999). This idea al- setting is that organizational or workgroup sol-
lows social identity theorists to now explicate idarity and, thus, adherence to group norms are
social identity-based intragroup processes, unlikely to be strengthened by activities that
such as cohesion and social attraction, deviance strengthen only personal relationships or
and overachievement, and leadership and intra- friendships. Indeed, such activities may compro-
group structural differentiation. mise solidarity and norm adherence by frag-
menting the group into friendship pairs or
cliques that show interpersonal dislike for other
COHESIONAND DEVIANCE
pairs or cliques. To increase social attraction
A development of social identity theory made and solidarity within an organization, managers
possible by focusing on how social categoriza- might, among other things, create uncertainty
tion produces prototype-based depersonaliza- (this motivates identification), focus on interor-
tion is the social attraction hypothesis, which ganizational competition (this makes the group
approaches group solidarity and cohesion as a salient), and emphasize desirable attributes of
reflection of depersonalized, prototype-based in- the organization (this provides positive distinc-
terindividual attitudes (Hogg, 1987, 1992, 1993). A tiveness).
distinction is drawn between interindividual
evaluations, attitudes, and feelings that are Proposition 3: Social attraction may
based on and generated by being members of foster organizational cohesion, and
the same group or members of different groups
thereby identification and adherence
(depersonalized social attraction) and those that
to organizational norms; conversely,
are based on and generated by the idiosyncra-
interpersonal attraction may fragment
sies and complementarities of close and endur-
the organization and disrupt identifi-
ing interpersonal relationships (personal attrac-
tion). cation and adherence to norms.
When a group is salient, ingroup members are
liked more if they embody the ingroup prototype. Cohesion and solidarity, and the feelings peo-
Where the prototype is consensual, certain peo- ple have for one another within a group, hinge
ple are consensually liked, and where all mem- on the perceived group prototypicality of others.
bers are highly prototypical, there is a tight net- We now discuss two organizationally relevant
work of social attraction. Of course, outgroup implications of this idea: (1) the dynamic inter-
members generally are liked less than ingroup play of group and demographic prototypes that
members. When a group is not salient, liking is affects cohesion within an organization (rela-
based on personal relationships and idiosyn- tional demography) and (2) the perception and
cratic preferences. The prediction is that pat- treatment of nonprototypical group members
terns of liking in an aggregate, and the bases of (negative outliers and high flyers).
2000 Hogg and Terry 127

Relational Demography salience; organizational salience and


behavior are enhanced by demo-
The social attraction analysis of cohesion has
graphic similarity, if organizational
relevance for recent organizational research on
and demographic norms are consis-
relational demography (e.g., Mowday & Sutton,
tent, and by demographic diversity, if
1993; Riordan & Shore, 1997; Wesolowski & Moss-
there is societal harmony among de-
holder, 1997). Relational demography theorists
mographic categories.
propose that people in organizations or work
units compare their own demographic charac-
teristics (e.g., race, gender, ethnicity) with those
Negative Outliers and High Flyers
of individual other members or the group as a
whole, and that perceived similarity enhances A further implication of the social attraction
work-related attitudes and behavior. The organ- hypothesis is that prototypically marginal in-
ization or unit provides the context within which group members will be liked less than prototyp-
similarity comparisons are made. Social iden- ically central members and that this process
tity theory and the social attraction hypothesis will be accentuated under high salience so that
provide a much more textured analysis, based marginal members may be entirely rejected as
on the relative salience of the demographic or "deviants." A program of laboratory research by
organizational group and on the correspon- Marques and his associates provides good evi-
dence between demographic and organization- dence for this process (Marques, 1990; Marques
al norms/prototypes (see the discussion of group & Paez, 1994; Marques & Yzerbyt, 1988; Marques,
structure below). Yzerbyt, & Leyens, 1988). By being aprototypical,
Demographic homogeneity may strengthen particularly in a direction that leans toward a
organizational ingroup prototypes, social at- salient outgroup, a marginal ingrouper jeopar-
traction, and identification and, thus, adher- dizes the distinctiveness and prototypical clar-
ence to norms, particularly if group norms are ity and integrity of the ingroup. This may intro-
not inconsistent with demographic category duce the threat of uncertainty. Thus, fellow
norms. If the organizational group's norms ingroupers, especially those for whom uncer-
clash with those of the wider demographic tainty is particularly threatening, will strongly
category, then demographic homogeneity may reject the deviant in order to consolidate a clear
make the wider category and its norms salient prototype to which they can strongly assimilate
and, thus, weaken adherence to the organiza- themselves through self-categorization.
tion's norms. Demographic diversity may So-called black sheep studies focus on "neg-
weaken the impact of demographic group ative" deviants: ingroup members who are in-
membership, make the organizational group clined toward the outgroup prototype. But what
itself more salient, and, thus, strengthen ad- about "positive" deviants? These are group
herence to organizational norms, particularly members who are aprototypical, but in evalu-
if societal relations between demographic
atively favorable ways-for example, over-
groups are harmonious. If, however, relations achievers or high flyers. On the one hand, over-
between demographic groups are conflictual
achievers should be socially unattractive
and are emotionally charged, diversity will
because they are aprototypical, but, on the
highlight intergroup relations outside the or-
other, they should be socially attractive because
ganization or unit, thus making demographic
the group can bask in their reflected glory (cf.
membership salient and strengthening adher-
Burger, 1985; Cialdini et al., 1976; Cialdini & de
ence to demographic-not organizational-
Nicholas, 1989; Sigelman, 1986; Snyder, Lasseg-
norms. This analysis revolves around the con-
ard, & Ford, 1986; Wann, Hamlet, Wilson, &
textual salience of demographic or organiza-
Hodges, 1995). There is some evidence that peo-
tional identity-not just the degree of per-
ple are evaluatively particularly harsh on over-
ceived demographic similarity.
achievers who suffer a setback or experience a
Proposition 4: Intraorganizational de- fall (e.g., Feather, 1994), but this research does
mographic similarityldiversity will not differentiate between overachievers who are
impact organizational behavior via or- members of a salient ingroup and those who
ganizational or demographic identity are not.
128 Academy of Management Review January

To investigate this, researchers are conduct- 1998). From this perspective, leadership-the fo-
ing a series of laboratory experiments (Fielding cus is largely on emergent leaders-is a struc-
& Hogg, 1998). From social identity theory we tural feature of ingroups (i.e., leaders and fol-
predict that the immediate and intergroup so- lowers), which is produced by the processes of
cial context of overachievement determines the self-categorization and prototype-based deper-
evaluation of positive ingroup deviants. There sonalization. As group membership becomes
are two dimensions to the model: more salient, being a prototypical group mem-
(1) A functional dimension. Where solidarity ber may be at least as important for leadership
and consensual prototypicality are important to as having characteristics that are widely be-
the group, perhaps owing to uncertainty con- lieved to be associated with a particular type of
cerns, positive deviants are dysfunctional for leader (i.e., being stereotypical of a nominal
the group; they will be evaluatively down- leader category; see leader categorization the-
graded, much like negative deviants. Where sol- ory: Lord, Foti, & De Vader, 1984; Nye & Forsyth,
idarity is less critical and prototypicality less 1991; Nye & Simonetta, 1996; Rush & Russell,
consensual but self-enhancement is important, 1988). There are three aspects of the process:
positive deviants are functional for the group; (1) Self-categorization constructs a gradient of
they will be upgraded as they contribute to a actual or perceived prototypicality within the
favorable redefinition of ingroup identity. group so that some people are more prototypical
(2) A social attribution dimension. Where pos- than others, and they act as a focus for attitudi-
itively deviant behavior can be "owned" by the nal and behavioral depersonalization. The per-
group, the deviant will be favorably evaluated; son who occupies the contextually most proto-
this would be likely if the deviant modestly at- typical position embodies the behaviors that
tributed the behavior to the support of the group others conform to and, thus, appears to have
rather than to personal ability and if the deviant exercised influence over other group members.
had little personal history of overachievement If the social context remains stable, the proto-
(i.e., was a "new" deviant). Where positively de- type remains stable, and the same individual
viant behavior cannot readily be "owned" by the appears to have enduring influence. However,
group, the deviant will be unfavorably evalu- the process is automatic. The "leader" merely
ated; this would be likely if the deviant took full embodies the aspirations, attitudes, and behav-
personal credit for the behavior without ac- iors of the group but does not actively exercise
knowledging the group's support (i.e., "boasted") leadership.
and if the deviant had a long personal history of (2) Social attraction ensures that more proto-
overachievement (i.e., was an enduring devi- typical members are liked more than less proto-
ant). typical members; if the prototype is consensual,
more prototypical members are consensually
Proposition 5: Organizations will re- liked. There are a number of important implica-
ject negative organizational deviants. tions of this. First, being socially attractive fur-
Positive deviants will be accepted nishes the leader with the capacity to actively
where organizational prestige is im- gain compliance with his or her requests-
portant but will be rejected where or- people tend to agree and comply with people
ganizational solidarity and distinc- they like. Second, this empowers the leader and
tiveness are important. publicly confirms his or her ability to exercise
influence. Third, the prototypical leader is likely
to identify strongly with the group and, thus,
LEADERSHIP
exercise influence in empathic and collectively
In contrast to deviants, prototypical group beneficial ways, which strengthens his or her
members are reliably and consensually favor- perceived prototypicality and consensual social
ably evaluated when group membership is sa- attractiveness. Fourth, consensual attractive-
lient. This idea has recently been extended in ness confirms differential popularity and public
order to develop a social identity model of lead- endorsement of the leader, imbues the leader
ership processes in groups (Hogg, 1996b, 1999; with prestige and status, and instantiates an
also, see Fielding & Hogg, 1997; Hains, Hogg, & intragroup status differential between leader(s)
Duck, 1997; Hogg, 1996a; Hogg, Hains, & Mason, and followers.
2000 Hogg and Terry 129

(3) The final process is an attribution one, in the self-serving ingroup prototype, and they can
which members make the fundamental attribu- elevate uncertainty to ensure that members are
tion error (Ross, 1977) or show correspondence motivated to identify strongly with a group that
bias (Gilbert & Jones, 1986; see also Gilbert & is defined as the leader wishes (uncertainty can
Malone, 1995, and Trope & Liberman, 1993). be managed as a resource by people in power;
Members overattribute or misattribute the lead- e.g., Marris, 1996).
er's behavior to personality rather than to his or The most basic prediction from this model is
her prototypical position in the group. Because that as group salience increases, perceived
the behavior being attributed, particularly over leadership effectiveness becomes more deter-
an enduring period, includes the appearance or mined by group prototypicality and less deter-
actuality of being influential over others' atti- mined by possession of general leadership
tudes and behaviors, being consensually so- qualities. This prediction has been confirmed in
cially attractive, and gaining compliance and a series of three laboratory studies of emergent
agreement from others, this constructs a charis- leadership (Hains et al., 1997; Hogg et al., 1998,
matic leadership personality for the leader. Experiments 1 and 2) and replicated in a field
A number of factors accentuate this process. study of outward-bound groups (Fielding &
First, because prototypicality is the yardstick of Hogg, 1997). The social attraction and attribution
group life, it attracts attention and renders aspects of the model remain to be investigated,
highly prototypical members figural against the as do the many implications described in this
background of the group, thus enhancing the section.
fundamental attribution error (Taylor & Fiske, We now suggest three organizationally rele-
1978). Second, the emerging status-based struc- vant leadership consequences of excessively
tural differentiation between leader(s) and fol- high group cohesiveness. Such groups may
lowers further enhances the distinctiveness of (1) produce leaders who are prototypical but do
the leader(s) against the background of the rest not possess task-appropriate leadership skills
of the group. Third, to redress their own per- (cf. groupthink); (2) consolidate organizational
ceived lack of power and control, followers seek prototypes that reflect dominant rather than
individualizing information about the leader, minority cultural attributes and, thus, exclude
because they believe that such information is minorities from top leadership positions; and
most predictive of how the leader will behave in (3) produce an environment that is conducive to
many situations (Fiske, 1993; Fiske & D6pret, the exercise, and perhaps abuse, of power by
1996). Fourth, cultural theories of causes of lead- leaders.
ership behavior (e.g., the "great person" theory
of leadership) may accentuate the fundamental
Prototypical Leadership and Groupthink
error (e.g., Morris & Peng, 1994). And fifth, the
correspondence bias may be strengthened be- This research may help cast light on
cause followers perceive the leader's behavior groupthink: suboptimal decision-making proce-
to be relatively extreme and distinctive and be- dures in highly cohesive groups, leading to poor
cause they then fail to properly consider situa- decisions with potentially damaging conse-
tional causes of the behavior (e.g., Gilbert & quences (e.g., Janis, 1982). There is now some
Malone, 1995; Trope & Liberman, 1993). evidence that the critical component of "cohe-
Together, these three processes transform pro- siveness" associated with groupthink is social
totypical group members into leaders who are attraction, rather than interpersonal attraction
able to be proactive and innovative in exercis- (Hogg & Hains, 1998; see also Turner, Pratkanis,
ing influence. This also equips leaders to main- Probasco, & Leve, 1992). If we assume that group
tain their tenure. They can simply exercise prototypes do not necessarily embody optimal
power (more of this below), but they can also procedures for group decision making, then
manipulate circumstances to enhance their per- group prototypical leaders are quite likely to be
ceived prototypicality: they can exercise self- less effective leaders of decision-making groups
serving ideological control over the content of than are leadership-stereotypical leaders (i.e.,
the prototype, they can pillory ingroup deviants leaders who, in this case, possess qualities that
who threaten the self-serving prototype, they most people believe are appropriate for group
can demonize outgroups that clearly highlight decision making). This suggests that groupthink
130 Academy of Management Review January

may arise because overly cohesive groups through self-categorization. It is unnecessary to


"choose" highly prototypical and, thus, perhaps, exercise power to gain influence, and there are
task-inappropriate members as leaders. strong mutual bonds of liking and empathy be-
tween prototypically united leaders and follow-
Proposition 6: Strong organizational
ers that would inhibit the exercise of power in
identification may hinder endorse-
ways that might harm members of the group.
ment of effective leaders, because
However, once charisma and status-based
leadership is based on group proto-
structural differentiation gather pace, the leader
typicality, and group prototypes may
becomes increasingly psychologically and ma-
not embody effective leadership prop-
terially separated from the group. This severs
erties.
the empathic and social attraction bonds that
previously guarded against abuse of power. A
Minorities As Organizational Leaders consensually endorsed, status-based intergroup
relationship between leader(s) (probably in the
Another implication of this analysis of leader-
form of a power elite) and followers has effec-
ship relates to evidence that minorities (e.g.,
tively come into existence; thus, typical inter-
women and people of color) can find it difficult
group behaviors are made possible. The leader
to attain top leadership positions in organiza-
can discriminate against followers, favor self
tions (e.g., Eagly, Karau, & Makhijani, 1995). If
and the leadership elite, and express negative
organizational prototypes (e.g., of speech, dress,
social attitudes against and develop negative
attitudes, and interaction styles) are societally
cast so that minorities do not match them well, stereotypes of followers (e.g., Goodwin & Fiske,
1996; Goodwin, Gubin, Fiske, & Yzerbyt, in
minorities are unlikely to be endorsed as lead-
press). Under these conditions leaders are likely
ers under conditions where organizational pro-
to exercise power (in Yukl & Falbe's, 1991, sense
totypicality is more important than leadership
of personal power or in Raven's, 1965, sense of
stereotypicality-that is, when organizational
reward power, coercive power, or legitimate
identification and cohesion are very high. This
power) and are able to abuse power-for exam-
might arise under conditions of uncertainty
ple, when they feel their position is under threat.
when, for example, organizations are under
This rigidly hierarchical leadership scenario is
threat from competitors or when there is an eco-
most likely to emerge when conditions encourage
nomic crisis-situations where leaders, rather
than managers, may be badly needed. groups to be cohesive and homogenous, with ex-
tremitized and clearly delimited prototypes that
Proposition 7: Minorities may find it are tightly consensual. In an organizational con-
difficult to attain top leadership posi- text, extreme societal or organizational uncer-
tions in organizations because they do tainty might produce these conditions (Hogg, 1999;
not fit culturally prescribed organiza- see also Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle,
tional prototypes. 1994, and Tyler, 1990).
Proposition 8: Subjective uncertainty
Leadership and the Exercise of Power may produce a prototypically and de-
An important feature of the model is that the mographically homogenous organiza-
processes of social attraction and prototypical tion or work unit that has a hierarchi-
attribution decouple the leader from the group; cal leadership structure with a
they create a status-based structural differenti- powerful leader and that has rigid,
ation of leaders(s) and followers, which is en- entrenched, and "extremist" attitudes
dorsed by both leader(s) and followers. This has and practices.
implications for the role of power in leadership The progression from benign influence to the
(Hogg, 1998, 1999; Hogg & Reid, in press). Tradi- possibly destructive wielding of power may not
tionally, social identity theorists have said little be inevitable. Conditions that inhibit the attri-
about power, preferring to talk of influence. bution of charisma and the process of structural
Where leaders are merely prototypical, they differentiation, and that reground leadership in
have influence over followers by virtue of being prototypicality, may curb the exercise of power.
prototypical; followers automatically comply For example, if a group becomes less cohesive,
2000 Hogg and Terry 131

more diverse, and less consensual about its pro- work is framed by the "contact hypothesis," to
totype, followers are less likely to agree on and investigate the conditions under which contact
endorse the same person as the leader. The in- between members of different groups might im-
cumbent leader's power base is fragmented, prove enduring relations between the groups
and numerous new "contenders"' emerge. This (e.g., Brown, 1996; Gaertner, Dovidio, Anastasio,
limits the leader's ability to abuse power and Bachman, & Rust, 1993; Gaertner, Dovidio, &
renders the exercise of power less effective. Par- Bachman, 1996; Gaertner, Rust, Dovidio, Bach-
adoxically, a rapid increase in cohesiveness, man, & Anastasio, 1995; Hewstone, 1994, 1996;
caused, for example, by imminent external Pettigrew, 1998).
threat to the group, may, through a different Subgroups often resist attempts by a superor-
process, have a similar outcome. Cohesion may dinate group to dissolve subgroup boundaries
make the group so consensual that leader and and merge them into one large group. This can
group become temporarily re-fused. The em- be quite marked where the superordinate group
pathic bond is re-established so that the leader is very large, amorphous, and impersonal. Thus,
does not need to exercise power to gain influ- assimilationist strategies within nations, or
ence, and any abuse of power would be akin to large organizations, can produce fierce sub-
abuse of self. group loyalty and intersubgroup competition.
Subgroup members derive social identity from
Proposition 9: Emergent leaders may their groups and, thus, view externally imposed
tend to abuse their power unless the assimilation as an identity threat. The threat
organization is highly diverse or
may be stronger in large superordinate groups
highly cohesive. because of optimal distinctiveness consider-
ations (Brewer, 1991, 1993). People strive for a
GROUP STRUCTURE balance between conflicting motives for inclu-
sion/sameness (satisfied by group membership)
Leadership is only one way in which groups and for distinctiveness/uniqueness (satisfied by
can be internally structured. Groups, such as individuality). So, in very large organizations,
organizations, are also structured, in various people feel overincluded and strive for distinc-
ways, into functional or demographic sub- tiveness, often by identifying with distinctive
groups. In this section we discuss the relevance subunits or departments.
of social identity theory to the analysis of rela- Some research suggests that an effective
tions among subgroups within organiza- strategy for managing intersubgroup relations
tions-in particular, sociodemographic sub- within a larger group is to make subgroup and
groups based on gender, race, ethnicity, and so superordinate group identity simultaneously sa-
forth and organizational subgroups within a su- lient. For example, Hornsey and Hogg (1999, in
perordinate organization formed by a merger or press a,b) conducted a series of experiments in
acquisition. which they found intersubgroup relations to be
more harmonious when the subgroups were sa-
lient within the context of a salient superordi-
Subgroup Structure
nate group than when the superordinate group
Almost all groups are vertically organized to alone or the subgroups alone were salient. This
contain subgroups, while they themselves are may re-create, in the laboratory, the policy of
nested within larger groups. Sometimes sub- multiculturalism, adopted by some countries to
groups are wholly nested within a superordi- manage ethnic diversity at a national level (cf.
nate group (e.g., a sales department within an Prentice & Miller, 1999).
organization), and sometimes subgroups are The implication for organizations is clear.
crosscut by the superordinate group (e.g., pilots To secure harmonious and cooperative rela-
within an airline). Social identity theorists and tions among departments or divisions within a
those with more general social categorization large organization, it may be best to balance
perspectives make predictions about the nature loyalty to and identification with the subunit
of relations between subgroups as a function of with loyalty to and identification with the su-
the nature of the subgroups' relationship to the perordinate organization, and not overempha-
superordinate group. Much of these scholars' size either one to the detriment of the other.
132 Academy of Management Review January

From a social identity perspective, managers stance, if there are relatively few female em-
might achieve this balance by having a dis- ployees and they are all employed in secretarial
tinct departmental or divisional structure, in- or clerical positions. In such circumstances, cat-
volving, for example, departmental activities egorization in terms of the employment classifi-
and friendly interdepartmental rivalry, care- cation is facilitated, because it covaries with a
fully balanced against a clear interorganiza- salient demographic categorization.
tion orientation and organization-wide activi- According to Brewer (1996; Brewer et al., 1999;
ties that emphasize positive distinctiveness see also Brewer & Miller, 1996), differentiations
and positive organizational identity. within categories are more likely to be made
when minority status does not correlate with
Proposition 10: Harmonious relations employment classification. If minority group
among subgroups within an organiza- status is not diagnostic of employment catego-
tion are often best achieved by simul- rization, employees will find it necessary, in or-
taneous recognition of subgroup and der to function within the organization, to ac-
organizational identity. knowledge differences within both the minority
group and the employment classification. One
way in which convergence between minority
Sociodemographic Structure
group status and employment classification can
Intragroup dynamics and structure also are be avoided, and hence stereotyped responses to
influenced by the sociodemographic structure of the minority group can be reduced, is to crosscut
society. Most groups, including organizational organizational roles and social group member-
groups, have a membership that is diverse in ship. In a crosscutting structure, minority group
terms of race, ethnicity, gender, (dis)ability, and memberships and employment classifications
so forth (e.g., Chung, 1997; Cox, 1991; Ibarra, 1995; are independent of each other; knowing a per-
Kandola, 1995). Organizations are a crucible in son's group membership is undiagnostic of
which wider intergroup relations, often evalu- employment role or classification. Marcus-
atively polarized and emotionally charged, are Newhall, Miller, Holtz, and Brewer (1993) found
played out; conflict, disadvantage, marginaliza- that when category membership and role as-
tion, and minority victimization can arise (e.g., signment were not convergent (i.e., they were
Williams & Sommer, 1997; cf. the expectation crosscut), category members were less likely to
states theory notion of diffuse status character- favor their own category on post-test ratings,
istics [de Gilder & Wilke, 1994] and our earlier and they were less likely to differentiate among
discussion of relational demography). the categories than in a convergent role struc-
As a theory of intergroup relations, social ture.
identity theory has direct relevance for the study From a social identity perspective, a crosscut-
of sociodemographic diversity within organiza- ting structure is one way to manage diversity
tions (Brewer, 1996; Brewer, von Hippel, & effectively in organizations. Another strategy is
Gooden, 1999; see also Alderfer & Thomas, 1988; to create a pluralistic or multicultural normative
Brewer & Miller, 1996; Kramer, 1991; Oakes et al., environment within the organization (Cox, 1991;
1994). Intraorganizational minority status rests Kandola, 1995). As discussed above, this in-
on the dominant composition of the organiza- volves minority members' balancing subgroup
tion-for example, gender may be a minority (i.e., demographic minority) and superordinate
status in some organizations but not others. Be- group (i.e., demographic majority or organiza-
cause of the salience of their minority status in tion) identification, and majority members' ex-
the organizational context, members of such hibiting normative acceptance and support for
groups are likely to be classified and perceived cultural diversity within the organization.
in terms of this status, thus occasioning stereo- To summarize, a crosscutting structure will
typical expectations and treatment from mem- assist the development of a pluralistic organi-
bers of the dominant group. The likelihood of zational environment, as will reduced marginal-
stereotyped responses increases if the demo- ization of minority group members, through co-
graphic minority categorization (e.g., gender or operative intergroup contact (Hewstone &
ethnicity) converges with a role or employment Brown, 1986; see also Deschamps & Brown, 1983)
classification within the organization-for in- and through intergroup contact that changes
2000 Hogg and Terry 133

members' cognitive representation of the inter- worked together in their own dyad and, hence,
group structure from the perception of separate were only nominal groups.
groups to one that acknowledges plural identi- Social identity theorists make clear predic-
ties or a common ingroup identity (e.g., Gaertner tions about the success of a merger. The behav-
et al., 1996). iors that group members adopt to pursue self-
enhancement through positive social identity
Proposition 11: Conflict arising from are influenced by subjective belief structures:
sociodemographic diversity within an beliefs about the nature of relations between the
organization can be moderated by ingroup and relevant outgroups (Tajfel & Turner,
crosscutting demography with role as- 1979; see also Ellemers, 1993; Ellemers, Doojse,
signments or by encouraging a strat- van Knippenberg, & Wilke, 1992; Ellemers, van
egy of cultural pluralism. Knippenberg, de Vries, & Wilke, 1988; Tajfel,
1975; Taylor & McKirnan, 1984; van Knippenberg
Finally, drawing on the uncertainty reduction
& Ellemers, 1993). These beliefs concern (1) the
hypothesis, we would expect organizational un-
stability and legitimacy of intergroup status re-
certainty to generally work against diversity.
lations (i.e., whether one's group deserves its
Organizations facing uncertainty would strive
status, and the likelihood of a change in status)
for homogeneity and consensual prototypicality
and (2) the possibility of social mobility (psycho-
that might marginalize sociodemographic mi-
logically passing from one group to another) or
norities within the organization. The effect
social change (changing the ingroup's evalua-
would be amplified under conditions of wider
tion). Social change can involve direct conflict
societal uncertainty that encourages ethnic, ra-
but also socially creative behavior, such as in-
cial, religious, and national identification, and
group bias on dimensions that are not related
concomitant xenophobia and intolerance.
directly to the basis for the status differentiation
(e.g., Lalonde, 1992; Terry & Callan, 1998).
At the interorganizational level, an organiza-
Mergers and Acquisitions
tion that believes its lower-status position is
A special case of group structure is the merg- legitimate and stable and believes that it is
ing of two organizations or the acquisition of possible for members to pass psychologically
one organization by another. Mergers and ac- into the more prestigious organization (i.e., ac-
quisitions pose special problems of intragroup quire a social identity as a member of the pres-
relations for organizations (e.g., Hakansson & tigious organization) will be unlikely to show
Sharma, 1996; Hogan & Overmyer-Day, 1994). organizational solidarity or engage in interor-
When two organizations merge or, more com- ganizational competition. Instead, members will
monly, one acquires the other, the postmerger attempt, as individuals, to disidentify and gain
entity embraces premerger intergroup relations psychological entry to the other organization.
between the merger "partners." These relations This will increase their support for the merger
are often competitive and sometimes bitter and and their commitment to and identification with
antagonistic. Indeed, negative responses and the new, merged organization.
feelings toward the employees of the other or- In contrast, an organization that believes its
ganization may jeopardize the success of the lower-status position is illegitimate and unsta-
merger. ble, that passing is not viable, and that a differ-
Case studies of mergers confirm this. There ent interorganizational status relation is achiev-
are many examples of mergers failing because able will show marked solidarity, engage in
of "us" versus "them" dynamics that prevail if direct interorganizational competition, and ac-
employees do not relinquish their old identities tively attempt to undermine the success of the
(e.g., Blake & Mouton, 1985; Buono & Bowditch, merger. Although members of low-status organ-
1989). In a laboratory study Haunschild, More- izations are likely to respond favorably to con-
land, and Murrell (1994) found similar results. ditions of high permeability (see Zuckerman,
People who had worked on a task together in a 1979), an opposite effect is likely for employees
dyad showed stronger interdyad biases when of the higher-status premerger organization (see
different dyads were subsequently required to Vaughan, 1978). Permeable boundaries pose a
merge than did people who had not previously threat to the status they enjoy as members of a
134 Academy of Management Review January

higher-status premerger organization, so they reduced anxiety, to low intergroup bias),


are likely to respond negatively to permeable whereas the perception that the organization
intergroup boundaries. felt like two subgroups was related positively to
work-related bias. Thus, in contrast to the opti-
Proposition 12: Lower-status merger mization of subgroup relations in an organiza-
partners will respond favorably to a
tional context (and the management of sociode-
merger, if they believe their status is
mographic diversity; see above), a dual identity
legitimate and that the boundary be-
model does not appear to be useful in the con-
tween the premerger partners is per-
text of a merger, presumably because height-
meable, and unfavorably, if they be-
ened salience of premerger group identities
lieve their status is illegitimate and
may threaten the success of this type of organi-
boundaries are impermeable. Higher-
zational change.
status merger partners will respond
One lacuna in social identity research on
unfavorably to permeable bound-
mergers is the temporal dimension (which is
aries.
absent from most social psychological research
In a recent study of employees involved in a on social identity processes). Mergers take time
merger between two airlines, Terry, Carey, and and move through stages, during which differ-
Callan (in press) found some support for these ent social identity processes may operate. It
predictions. Perception of permeable intergroup would be valuable to track social identity and
boundaries in the new organization was associ- self-categorization processes in mergers over
ated positively with identification with the new time, perhaps within the framework of Levine
organization and both job-related (organization- and Moreland's (1994; Moreland & Levine, 1997,
al commitment and job satisfaction) and person- in press) diachronic group socialization model.
related (emotional well-being and self-esteem) Another lacuna is uncertainty. Mergers and
outcomes among employees of the low-status takeovers often produce enormous uncertainty,
premerger organization, but negatively with the which can instantiate precisely the conditions
person-related outcomes among employees of that work against a successful merger. To re-
the high-status premerger organization. Analy- duce self-conceptual uncertainty, merger part-
ses showed that these effects were significant ners resist change and may polarize and consol-
after controlling for the type of individual-level idate interorganizational attitudes around
constructs that have been considered in previ- narrowly prescriptive norms and fierce pre-
ous merger research (e.g., perceived positive- merger organizational identification.
ness of the change process and the use of both
problem- and emotion-focused coping respons-
SUMMARY,CONCLUSIONS,AND PROSPECTS
es)-a pattern of results that reflects the impor-
tance of considering group-level variables in The aim of this article has been to describe
merger research. recent theoretical developments within social
Gaertner and colleagues (Anastasio, Bach- identity theory that focus, via self-categorization
man, Gaertner, & Dovidio, 1997; Gaertner et al., theory, on how social categorization produces
1996) also found support for a social categoriza- prototype-based depersonalization, which is re-
tion approach, in the context of a bank merger. sponsible for social identity phenomena. These
Perception of successful contact between the developments extend social identity theory.
premerger organizations (e.g., contact between They advance our understanding of social iden-
equal-status partners, positive interdependence tity processes in intergroup contexts and the
between the groups, and many opportunities for way in which people may internalize group
interaction) reduced intergroup bias, through norms and align their behavior with these
employees' cognitive representations of the norms. They also have produced a new concep-
merged group and through low intergroup anx- tualization of motivation associated with social
iety. For intergroup evaluative bias on both identity, a better understanding of salience pro-
work-related and sociability dimensions, the be- cesses, and a new focus on intragroup processes
lief that the merged organization felt like one that is now producing social identity models of,
group (see van Knippenberg, 1997) was related for example, cohesion, deviance, group struc-
negatively to intergroup anxiety (and, through ture, and leadership.
2000 Hogg and Terry 135

We introduce these new developments to an standing of how identity-related constructs and


organizational readership that is familiar with processes impact organizational phenomena,
some aspects of social identity theory, but less and it should lead, in turn, to extensions of and
familiar with more recent self-categorization refinements to social identity theory itself. We
theory-based developments, in order to show are confident that the extent of theoretical inter-
how these developments are relevant to under- change between organizational and social psy-
standing a range of social behaviors in organi- chologists studying social identity mechanisms
zational contexts. One of our main aims has can increase (e.g., Hogg & Terry, in press), and
been to derive, from these developments, a va- we are extremely optimistic about the potential
riety of more or less specific, but testable, spec- that this interchange has for the development of
ulations and propositions, in order to help frame both disciplines.
future research directions in the study of social
identity processes and social behavior in organ-
izational contexts. REFERENCES
The challenge for the future is to integrate Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. 1988. Comments on the motiva-
new social identity mechanisms centrally into tional status of self-esteem in social identity and inter-
theories of organizational behavior. To date, group discrimination. European Journal of Social Psy-
such mechanisms have played a relatively chology, 18: 317-334.
small role in the literature on organizational Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (Eds.). 1990. Social identity theory:
behavior. Thus, the important role that identifi- Constructive and critical advances. London: Harvester
Wheatsheaf.
cations with the workgroup, organization, and
profession, as well as those that emanate from Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. 1998. Prospects for research in
group processes and intergroup relations. Group Pro-
people's sociodemographic background, may cesses and Intergroup Relations, 1: 7-20.
play in organizational behavior has yet to be
Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (Eds.). 1999. Social identity and
articulated fully.
social cognition. Oxford: Blackwell.
We suggest that identity-related constructs
Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. In press. Self, group and identity:
and processes have the potential to inform our
A dynamic model. In M. A. Hogg & R. S. Tindale (Eds.),
understanding of organizational behavior. Com- Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Group pro-
bined with multilevel approaches to organiza- cesses. Oxford: Blackwell.
tional research, the use of both individual-level Alderfer, C. P., & Thomas, D. A. 1988. The significance of race
and group-level constructs in models of organi- and ethnicity for understanding organizational behav-
zational phenomena could mark the beginning ior. In C. L. Cooper & I. Robertson (Eds.), International
of a new phase of research in organizational review of industrial and organizational psychology:
1-41. New York: Wiley.
behavior. By acknowledging the importance of
work-related identities to people's sense of self, Anastasio, P. A., Bachman, B. A., Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio,
J. F. 1997. Categorization, recategorization, and common
a social identity perspective adds to our under-
ingroup identity. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, &
standing of organizational attitudes and behav- S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyp-
ior by drawing on the important link between ing and group life: 236-256. Oxford: Blackwell.
such identities and the person's sense of self. Ashforth, B. E., & Humphrey, R. H. 1993. Emotional labor in
Such a perspective should improve explanation service roles: The influence of identity. Academy of
and understanding of intergroup relations, both Management Review, 18: 88 -115.
within and between organizations. Ashforth, B. E., & Mael, F. A. 1989. Social identity theory and
To maximize the usefulness of the social iden- the organization. Academy of Management Review, 14:
tity perspective in the organizational arena, 20-39.
there must be significant interchange between Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. 1985. How to achieve integration
social and organizational psychologists. When on the human side of the merger. Organizational Dy-
one is deriving predictions from a social identity namics, 13: 41-56.
perspective on organizational behavior, one Brewer, M. B. 1991. The social self: On being the same and
finds the results from laboratory-based social different at the same time. Personality and Social Psy-
chology Bulletin, 17: 475-482.
psychological research invaluable, as are the
Brewer, M. B. 1993. The role of distinctiveness in social iden-
insights that have been gained from field re-
tity and group behaviour. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams
search in the organizational context. It is this (Eds.), Group motivation: Social psychological perspec-
type of interchange that will further our under- tives: 1-16. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
136 Academy of Management Review January

Brewer, M. B. 1996. Managing diversity: The role of social Ellemers, N., Doojse, B. J., van Knippenberg, A., & Wilke, H.
identities. In S. Jackson & M. Ruderman (Eds.), Diversity 1992. Status protection in high status minority groups.
in work teams: 47-68. Washington, DC: American Psy- European Journal of Social Psychology, 22: 123-140.
chological Association.
Ellemers, N., van Knippenberg, A., de Vries, N., & Wilke, H.
Brewer, M. B., & Harasty, A. S. 1996. Seeing groups as entities: 1988. Social identification and permeability of group
The role of perceiver motivation. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. boundaries. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18:
Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition. 497-513.
Volume 3: The interpersonal context: 347-370. New York:
Guilford. Feather, N. T. 1994. Attitudes towards high achievers and
reactions to their fall: Theory and research concerning
Brewer, M. B., & Miller, N. 1996. Intergroup relations. Milton tall poppies. Advances in Experimental Social Psychol-
Keynes, UK: Open University Press. ogy, 26: 1-73.
Brewer, M. B., von Hippel, W., & Gooden, M. P. 1999. Diversity Fielding, K. S., & Hogg, M. A. 1997. Social identity, self-
and organizational identity: The problem of entree after categorization, and leadership: A field study of small
entry. In D. A. Prentice & D. T. Miller (Eds.), Cultural interactive groups. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research,
divides: Understanding and overcoming group conflict: and Practice, 1: 39-51.
337-363. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Fielding, K. S., & Hogg, M. A. 1998. Positive and negative
Brown, R. J. 1996. Tajfel's contribution to the reduction of
deviance in groups: A social identity analysis. Unpub-
intergroup conflict. In W. P. Robinson (Ed.), Social groups
lished manuscript, University of Queensland.
and identities: Developing the legacy of Henri Tajfel:
169-189. Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann. Fiske, S. T. 1993. Controlling other people: The impact of
power on stereotyping. American Psychologist, 48: 621-
Buono, A. F., & Bowditch, J. L. 1989. The human side of merg-
628.
ers and acquisitions: Managing collisions between peo-
ple, cultures, and organizations. San Francisco: Jossey- Fiske, S. T., & D6pret, E. 1996. Control, interdependence and
Bass. power: Understanding social cognition in its social con-
Burger, J. M. 1985. Temporal effects on attributions for aca- text. European Review of Social Psychology, 7: 31-61.
demic performances and reflected-glory basking. Social Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. 1991. Social cognition (2nd ed.).
Psychology Quarterly, 48: 330-336. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Campbell, D. T. 1958. Common fate, similarity, and other Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Anastasio, P. A., Bachman, B. A.,
indices of the status of aggregates of persons as social & Rust, M. C. 1993. Reducing intergroup bias: The com-
entities. Behavioral Science, 3: 14-25. mon ingroup identity model. European Review of Social
Chung, W. V. L. 1997. Ethnicity and organizational diversity. Psychology, 4: 1-26.
New York: University Press of America. Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., & Bachman, B. A. 1996. Revisit-
Cialdini, R. B., Borden, R. J., Thorne, A., Walker, M. R., Free- ing the contact hypothesis: The induction of a common
man, S., & Sloan, L. R. 1976. Basking in reflected glory: ingroup identity. International Journal of Intercultural
Three (football) field studies. Journal of Personality and Relations, 20: 271-290.
Social Psychology, 34: 366-375. Gaertner, S. L., Rust, M. C., Dovidio, J. F., Bachman, B. A., &
Cialdini, R. B., & de Nicholas, M. E. 1989. Self-presentation by Anastasio, P. A. 1995. The contact hypothesis: The role of
association. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- a common ingroup identity in reducing intergroup bias
ogy, 57: 626-631. among majority and minority group members. In J. L.
Cox, T. 1991. The multicultural organization. Academy of Nye & A. Brower (Eds.), What's social about social cog-
Management Executive, 5: 34-47. nition: 230-260. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

de Gilder, D., & Wilke, H. A. M. 1994. Expectation states Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. 1986. Perceiver-induced con-
theory and the motivational determinants of social in- straint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal
fluence. European Review of Social Psychology, 5: 243- of Personality and Social Psychology, 50: 269-280.
269. Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. 1995. The correspondence bias.
Deschamps, J.-C., & Brown, R. J. 1983. Superordinate goals Psychological Bulletin, 117: 21-38.
and intergroup conflict. British Journal of Social Psychol- Goodwin, S. A., & Fiske, S. T. 1996. Judge not, lest ...: The
ogy, 22: 189-195. ethics of power holders' decision making and standards
Dutton, J. E., Dukerich, J. M., & Harquail, C. V. 1994. Organi- for social judgment. In D. M. Messick & A. E. Tenbrunsel
zational images and member identification. Administra- (Eds.), Codes of conduct: Behavioral research into busi-
tive Science Quarterly, 39: 239-263. ness ethics: 117-142. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Eagly, A. H., Karau, S. J., & Makhijani, M. G. 1995. Gender and Goodwin, S. A., Gubin, A., Fiske, S. T., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. In
the effectiveness of leaders: A meta-analysis. Psycho- press. Power can bias impression formation: Stereotyp-
logical Bulletin, 117: 125-145. ing subordinates by default and by design. Group Pro-
Ellemers, N. 1993. The influence of socio-structural variables cesses and Intergroup Relations.
on identity management strategies. European Review of Hains, S. C., Hogg, M. A., & Duck, J. M. 1997. Self-categoriza-
Social Psychology, 4: 27-57. tion and leadership: Effects of group prototypicality and
2000 Hogg and Terry 137

leader stereotypicality. Personality and Social Psychol- Hogg, M. A. In press a. Subjective uncertainty reduction
ogy Bulletin, 23: 1087-1 100. through self-categorization: A motivational theory of so-
cial identity processes. European Review of Social Psy-
Hakansson, H., & Sharma, D. D. 1996. Strategic alliances in a
chology.
network perspective. In D. Iacobucci (Ed.), Networks in
marketing: 108-124. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Hogg, M. A. In press b. Self-categorization and subjective
uncertainty resolution: Cognitive and motivational fac-
Hamilton, D. L., & Sherman, S. J. 1996. Perceiving persons and
ets of social identity and group membership. In J. P.
groups. Psychological Review, 103: 336-355.
Forgas, K. D. Williams, & L. Wheeler (Eds.), The social
Hamilton, D. L., Sherman, S. J., & Lickel, B. 1998. Perceiving mind: Cognitive and motivational aspects of interper-
social groups: The importance of the entitativity contin- sonal behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
uum. In C. Sedikides, J. Schopler, & C. A. Insko (Eds.),
Hogg, M. A. In press c. Social identity and social comparison.
Intergroup cognition and intergroup behavior: 47-74.
In J. Suls & L. Wheeler (Eds.), Handbook of social com-
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
parison: Theory and research. New York: Plenum.
Haunschild, P. R., Moreland, R. L., & Murrell, A. J. 1994.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. 1988. Social identifications: A
Sources of resistance to mergers between groups. Jour-
social psychology of intergroup relations and group pro-
nal of Applied Social Psychology, 24: 1150-1178.
cesses. London: Routledge.
Hewstone, M. R. C. 1994. Revision and change of stereotypic
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. 1990. Social motivation, self-
beliefs: In search of the illusive subtyping model. Euro-
esteem and social identity. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg
pean Review of Social Psychology, 5: 69-109.
(Eds.), Social identity theory: Constructive and critical
Hewstone, M. R. C. 1996. Contact and categorization: Social advances: 28-47. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
psychological interventions to change intergroup rela-
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. (Eds.). 1993a. Group motivation:
tions. In C. N. Macrae, C. Stangor, & M. R. C. Hewstone
Social psychological perspectives. London: Harvester
(Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping: 323-368. London:
Wheatsheaf.
Guilford.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. 1993b. Towards a single-process
Hewstone, M. R. C., & Brown, R. J. 1986. Contact is not enough:
uncertainty-reduction model of social motivation in
An intergroup perspective on the "contact hypothesis."
groups. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), Group moti-
In M. R. C. Hewstone & R. J. Brown (Eds.), Contact and vation: Social psychological perspectives: 173-190. Lon-
conflict in intergroup encounters: 1-44. Oxford: Black- don: Harvester-Wheatsheaf.
well.
Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. 1999. Social identity and social
Hogan, E. A., & Overmyer-Day, L. 1994. The psychology of cognition: Historical background and current trends. In
mergers and acquisitions. International Review of In- D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity and social
dustrial and Organizational Psychology, 9: 247-282. cognition: 1-25. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hogg, M. A. 1987. Social identity and group cohesiveness. In Hogg, M. A., & Abrams, D. In press. Social categorization,
J. C. Turner, M. A. Hogg, P. J. Oakes, S. D. Reicher, & M. S. depersonalization and group behavior. In M. A. Hogg &
Wetherell, Rediscovering the social group: A self- R. S. Tindale (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psy-
categorization theory: 89-116. Oxford: Blackwell. chology: Group processes. Oxford: Blackwell.
Hogg, M. A. 1992. The social psychology of group cohesive- Hogg, M. A., Cooper-Shaw, L., & Holzworth, D. W. 1993. Group
ness: From attraction to social identity. London: Har- prototypicality and depersonalized attraction in small
vester Wheatsheaf. interactive groups. Personality and Social Psychology
Hogg, M. A. 1993. Group cohesiveness: A critical review and Bulletin, 19: 452-465.
some new directions. European Review of Social Psy- Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. 1996. Intergroup relations and
chology, 4: 85-111. group solidarity: Effects of group identification and so-
Hogg, M. A. 1996a. Social identity, self-categorization, and cial beliefs on depersonalized attraction. Journal of Per-
the small group. In E. H. Witte & J. H. Davis (Eds.), sonality and Social Psychology, 70: 295-309.
Understanding group behavior. Volume 2: Small group Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. 1998. Friendship and group iden-
processes and interpersonal relations: 227-253. Mahwah, tification: A new look at the role of cohesiveness in
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. groupthink. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28:
Hogg, M. A. 1996b. Intragroup processes, group structure and 323-341.
social identity. In W. P. Robinson (Ed.), Social groups and Hogg, M. A., Hains, S. C., & Mason, I. 1998. Identification and
identities: Developing the legacy of Henri Tajfel: 65-93. leadership in small groups: Salience, frame of reference,
Oxford: Butterworth-Heinemann. and leader stereotypicality effects on leader evalua-
Hogg, M. A. 1998. Group identification, leadership, and the tions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75:
exercise of power. Paper presented at the 1998 conven- 1248-1263.
tion of the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Hogg, M. A., & Hardie, E. A. 1991. Social attraction, personal
Issues, Ann Arbor, MI. attraction and self-categorization: A field study. Person-
Hogg, M. A. 1999. A social identity theory of leadership. ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17: 175-180.
Unpublished manuscript (submitted for publication), Hogg, M. A., & Hardie, E. A. 1992. Prototypicality, conformity
University of Queensland. and depersonalized attraction: A self-categorization
138 Academy of Management Review January

analysis of group cohesiveness. British Journal of Social Cummings & B. M. Staw (Eds.), Research in organization-
Psychology, 31: 41-56. al behavior, vol. 13: 191-228. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Hogg, M. A., & Hardie, E. A. 1997. Self-prototypicality, group Lalonde, R. N. 1992. The dynamics of group differentiation in
identification and depersonalized attraction: A polariza- the face of defeat. Personality and Social Psychology
tion study. In K. Leung, U. Kim, S. Yamaguchi, & Bulletin, 18: 336-342.
Y. Kashima (Eds.), Progress in Asian social psychology,
Lester, R. E. 1987. Organizational culture, uncertainty reduc-
vol. 1: 119-137. Singapore: Wiley.
tion, and the socialization of new organizational mem-
Hogg, M. A., Hardie, E. A., & Reynolds, K. 1995. Prototypical bers. In S. Thomas (Ed.), Culture and communication:
similarity, self-categorization, and depersonalized at- Methodology, behavior, artifacts, and institutions: 105-
traction: A perspective on group cohesiveness. Euro- 113. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
pean Journal of Social Psychology, 25: 159-177.
Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. 1990. Progress in small group
Hogg, M. A., & McGarty, C. 1990. Self-categorization and research. Annual Review of Psychology, 41: 585-634.
social identity. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social
Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. 1994. Group socialization:
identity theory: Constructive and critical advances: 10-
27. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf. Theory and research. European Review of Social Psy-
chology, 5: 305-336.
Hogg, M. A., & Moreland, R. L. 1995. European and American
influences on small group research. Paper presented at Long, K., & Spears, R. 1997. The self-esteem hypothesis re-
the small groups preconference of the Joint Meeting of visited: Differentiation and the disaffected. In R. Spears,
the European Association of Experimental Social Psy- P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social
chology and the Society for Experimental Social Psy- psychology of stereotyping and group life: 296-317. Ox-
chology, Washington, DC. ford: Blackwell.

Hogg, M. A., & Mullin, B.-A. 1999. Joining groups to reduce Lord, R. G., Foti, R. J., & DeVader, C. L. 1984. A test of lead-
uncertainty: Subjective uncertainty reduction and group ership categorization theory: Internal structure, informa-
identification. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social tion processing, and leadership perceptions. Organiza-
identity and social cognition: 249-279. Oxford: Blackwell. tional Behavior and Human Performance, 34: 343-378.
Hogg, M. A., & Reid, S. In press. Social identity, leadership, Mael, F. A., & Ashforth, B. E. 1992. Alumni and their alma
and power. In J. Bargh & A. Lee-Chai (Eds.), The use and mater: A partial test of the reformulated model of organ-
abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on the causes of izational identification. Journal of Organizational Be-
corruption. Philadelphia: Psychology Press. havior, 13: 103-123.
Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (Eds.). In press. Social identity Mael, F. A., & Ashforth, B. E. 1995. Loyal from day one: Bio-
processes in organizational contexts. Philadelphia: Psy- data, organizational identification, and turnover among
chology Press. newcomers. Personnel Psychology, 48: 309-333.
Hogg, M. A., Terry, D. J., & White, K. M. 1995. A tale of two Marcus-Newhall, A., Miller, N., Holtz, R., & Brewer, M. B. 1993.
theories: A critical comparison of identity theory with Crosscutting category membership with role assign-
social identity theory. Social Psychology Quarterly, 58: ment: A means of reducing intergroup bias. British Jour-
255-269. nal of Social Psychology, 32: 124-146.
Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. 1999. Subgroup differentiation Marques, J. M. 1990. The black-sheep effect: Out-group ho-
as a response to an overly-inclusive group: A test of mogeneity in social comparison settings. In D. Abrams &
optimal distinctiveness theory. European Journal of M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social identity theory: Constructive
Social Psychology, 29: 543-550. and critical advances: 131-151. London: Harvester
Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. In press a. Subgroup relations: Wheatsheaf.
A comparison of mutual intergroup differentiation and Marques, J. M., & Paez, D. 1994. The "black sheep effect":
common ingroup identity models of prejudice reduction. Social categorization, rejection of ingroup deviates and
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. perception of group variability. European Review of So-
Hornsey, M. J., & Hogg, M. A. In press b. Intergroup similarity cial Psychology, 5: 37-68.
and subgroup relations: Some implications for assimi- Marques, J. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. 1988. The black sheep effect:
lation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. Judgmental extremity towards ingroup members in
Ibarra, H. 1995. Race, opportunity, and diversity of social inter- and intra-group situations. European Journal of
circles in managerial networks. Academy of Manage- Social Psychology, 18: 287-292.
ment Journal, 38: 673-703.
Marques, J. M., Yzerbyt, V. Y., & Leyens, J.-P. 1988. The black
Janis, I. L. 1982. Groupthink: Psychological studies of policy sheep effect: Extremity of judgements towards in-group
decisions and fiascoes (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin. members as a function of group identification. European
Kandola, R. 1995. Managing diversity: New broom or old hat. Journal of Social Psychology, 18: 1-16.
International Review of Industrial and Organizational Marris, P. 1996. The politics of uncertainty: Attachment in
Psychology, 10: 131-168. private and public life. London: Routledge.
Kramer, R. M. 1991. Intergroup relations and organizational Moreland, R. L., & Levine, J. M. 1997. Work group socializa-
dilemmas: The role of categorization processes. In L. L. tion: A social identity approach. Paper presented at the
2000 Hogg and Terry 139

Second Australian Industrial and Organizational Psy- Riordan, C. M., & Shore, L. M. 1997. Demographic diversity
chology Conference, Melbourne. and employee attitudes: An empirical examination of
Moreland, R. L., & Levine, J. M. In press. Socialization in relational demography within work units. Journal of Ap-
organizations and work groups. In M. Turner (Ed.), plied Psychology, 82: 342-358.
Groups at work: Advances in theory and research. Mah- Robinson, W. P. (Ed.). 1996. Social groups and identities:
wah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Developing the legacy of Henri Tajfel. Oxford: Butter-
Moreland, R. L., Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. 1994. Back to the worth-Heinemann.
future: Social psychological research on groups. Journal Ross, L. 1977. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcom-
of Experimental Social Psychology, 30: 527-555. ings. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10:
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. 1994. Culture and cause: American 174-220.
and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Rubin, M., & Hewstone, M. 1998. Social identity theory's self-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67: 949- esteem hypothesis: A review and some suggestions for
971. clarification. Personality and Social Psychology Review,
Mowday, R. T., & Sutton, R. I. 1993. Organizational behavior: 2: 40-62.
Linking individuals and groups to organizational con- Rush, M. C., & Russell, J. E. A. 1988. Leader prototypes and
texts. Annual Review of Psychology, 44: 195-229. prototype-contingent consensus in leader behavior de-
Mullen, B., & Copper, C. 1994. The relation between group scriptions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
cohesiveness and performance: An integration. Psycho- 24: 88-104.
logical Bulletin, 115: 210-227. Saks, A. M., & Ashforth, B. E. 1997. Organizational socializa-
Nkomo, S. M., & Cox, T., Jr. 1996. Diverse identities in organ- tion: Making sense of the past and present as a prologue
izations. In S. R. Clegg, C. Hardy, & W. R. Nord (Eds.), for the future. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 51: 234-
Handbook of organization studies: 338-356. London: 279.
Sage.
Sherman, S. J., Hamilton, D. L., & Lewis, A. C. 1999. Perceived
Nye, J. L., & Forsyth, D. R. 1991. The effects of prototype-based entitativity and the social identity value of group mem-
biases on leadership appraisals: A test of leadership berships. In D. Abrams & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Social iden-
categorization theory. Small Group Research, 22: 360- tity and social cognition: 80-110. Oxford: Blackwell.
379.
Sigelman, L. 1986. Basking in reflected glory revisited: An
Nye, J. L., & Simonetta, L. G. 1996. Followers' perceptions of attempt at replication. Social Psychology Quarterly, 49:
group leaders: The impact of recognition-based and in- 90-92.
ference-bacsed processes. In J. L. Nye, & A. M. Bower
(Eds.), What's social about social cognition: Research on Snyder, C. R., Lassegard, M.-A., & Ford, C. E. 1986. Distancing
socially shared cognition in small groups: 124-153. Thou- after group success and failure: Basking in reflected
sand Oaks, CA: Sage. glory and cutting off reflected failure. Journal of Person-
ality and Social Psychology, 51: 382-388.
Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. 1994. Stereotyping
and social reality. Oxford: Blackwell. Spears, R., Oakes, P. J., Ellemers, N., & Haslam, S. A. (Eds.).
1997. The social psychology of stereotyping and group
Oakes, P. J., & Turner, J. C. 1990. Is limited information pro- life. Oxford: Blackwell.
cessing the cause of social stereotyping. European Re-
view of Social Psychology, 1: 111-135. Steiner, I. D. 1974. Whatever happened to the group in social
psychology? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Pettigrew, T. F. 1998. Intergroup contact theory. Annual Re- 10: 94-108.
view of Psychology, 49: 65-85.
Tajfel, H. 1972. Social categorization (English translation of
Pratt, M. G. In press. To be or not to be? Central questions in
"La cat6gorisation sociale"). In S. Moscovici (Ed.), Intro-
organizational identification. In D. Whetten & P. Godfrey
duction a la psychologie sociale, vol. 1: 272-302. Paris:
(Eds.), Identity in organizations: Developing theory
Larousse.
through conversations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Tajfel, H. 1974a. Intergroup behavior, social comparison and
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. 1994.
social change. Unpublished Katz-Newcomb lectures,
Social dominance orientation: A personality variable
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.
predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Per-
sonality and Social Psychology, 67: 741-763. Tajfel, H. 1974b. Social identity and intergroup behaviour.
Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (Eds.). 1999. Cultural divides: Social Science Information, 13: 65-93.
Understanding and overcoming group conflict. New Tajfel, H. 1975. The exit of social mobility and the voice of
York: Russell Sage Foundation. social change. Social Science Information, 14: 101-118.
Raven, B. H. 1965. Social influence and power. In I. D. Steiner Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1979. An integrative theory of inter-
& M. Fishbein (Eds.), Current studies in social psychol- group conflict. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), The
ogy: 371-382. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. social psychology of intergroup relations: 33-47.
Reicher, S. D., Spears, R., & Postmes, T. 1995. A social identity Monterey, CA: Brooks-Cole.
model of deindividuation phenomena. European Review Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. 1986. The social identity theory of
of Social Psychology, 6: 161-198. intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.),
140 Academy of Management Review January

The psychology of intergroup relations: 7-24. Chicago: Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., &
Nelson-Hall. Wetherell, M. S. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: A
self-categorization theory. Oxford: Blackwell.
Taylor, D. M., & McKirnan, D. J. 1984. A five-stage model of
intergroup relations. British Journal of Social Psychol- Turner, M. E., Pratkanis, A. R., Probasco, P., & Leve, C. 1992.
ogy, 23: 291-300. Threat, cohesion, and group effectiveness: Testing a so-
Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. 1978. Salience, attention, and cial identity maintenance perspective on groupthink.
attribution: Top of the head phenomena. Advances in Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63: 781-
Experimental Social Psychology, 11: 249-288. 796.

Terry, D. J., & Callan, V. J. 1998. Intergroup differentiation in Tyler, T. R. 1990. Why people obey the law. New Haven, CT:
response to an organizational merger. Group Dynamics: Yale University Press.
Theory, Research, and Practice, 2: 67-87. van Knippenberg, A., & Ellemers, N. 1993. Strategies in inter-
Terry, D. J., Carey, C. J., & Callan, V. J. In press. Employee group relations. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams (Eds.), Group
adjustment to an organizational merger: An intergroup motivation: Social psychological perspectives: 17-32.
perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. London: Harvester Wheatsheaf.
Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. 1996. Group norms and the attitude- van Knippenberg, D. 1997. A social identity perspective on
behavior relationship: A role for group identification. mergers and acquisitions. Paper presented at the Sec-
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22: 776-793. ond Australian Industrial and Organizational Psychol-
ogy Conference, Melbourne.
Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (Eds.). 1999. Attitudes, behavior, and
social context: The role of norms and group membership. Vaughan, G. M. 1978. Social change and intergroup prefer-
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ences in New Zealand. European Journal of Social Psy-
Trope, Y., & Liberman, A. 1993. The use of trait conceptions to chology, 8: 297-314.
identify other people's behavior and to draw inferences Wann, D. L., Hamlet, M. A., Wilson, T. M., & Hodges, J. A. 1995.
about their personalities. Personality and Social Psy- Basking in reflected glory, cutting off reflected failure,
chology Bulletin, 19: 553-562. and cutting off future failure: The importance of group
Tsui, A., Egan, T., & O'Reilly, C., III. 1992. Being different: identification. Social Behavior and Personality, 23: 377-
Relational demography and organizational attachment. 388.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 37: 549-579. Wesolowski, M. A., & Mossholder, K. W. 1997. Relational
Turner, J. C. 1975. Social comparison and social identity: demography in supervisor-subordinate dyads: Impact
Some prospects for intergroup behaviour. European on subordinate job satisfaction, burnout, and perceived
Journal of Social Psychology, 5: 5-34. procedural justice. Journal of Organizational Behavior,
18: 351-362.
Turner, J. C. 1982. Towards a cognitive redefinition of the
social group. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Social identity and inter- Williams, K. D., & Sommer, K. L. 1997. Social ostracism by
group relations: 15-40. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer- coworkers: Does rejection lead to loafing or compensa-
sity Press. tion? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23:
693-706.
Turner, J. C. 1985. Social categorization and the self-concept:
A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E. J. Worchel, S., Morales, J. F., Pdez, D., & Deschamps, J.-C. (Eds.).
Lawler (Ed.), Advances in group processes: Theory and 1998. Social identity: International perspectives. London:
research, vol. 2: 77-122. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. Sage.
Turner, J. C. 1991. Social influence. Milton Keynes, UK: Open Yukl, G. A., & Falbe, C. M. 1991. Importance of different
University Press. power sources in downward and lateral relations. Jour-
nal of Applied Psychology, 76: 416-423.
Turner, J. C. 1996. Henri Tajfel: An introduction. In W. P.
Robinson (Ed.), Social groups and identities: Developing Zuckerman, M. 1979. Attribution of success and failure revis-
the legacy of Henri Tajfel: 1-23. Oxford: Butterworth- ited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attri-
Heinemann. butional theory. Journal of Personality, 47: 245-287.

Michael A. Hogg is professor of social psychology and director of the Centre for
Research on Group Processes at the University of Queensland, Australia. He received
his Ph.D. from Bristol University. His current research interests include social identity
processes, uncertainty reduction, leadership, and attitudinal phenomena.

Deborah J. Terry is an associate professor of psychology at the University of Queens-


land, Australia. She received her Ph.D. from the Australian National University. She
has research interests in applied psychology (organizational and health psychology)
and social psychology (attitudes and group processes).

View publication stats

You might also like