You are on page 1of 15

Guidelines and Ethical Considerations for

Housing and Management of Psittacine Birds Used in Research

Isabelle D. Kalmar, Geert P.J. Janssens, and Christel P.H. Moons

Abstract most popular companion animal, estimated at 10.1 million

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


individuals in 2002 (AVMA 2002). Although their confine-
The Psittaciformes are a large order of landbirds comprising ment in laboratory settings for research purposes is believed
over 350 species in about 83 genera. In 2009, 141 published to be limited, it is a matter of concern because scientifically
studies implicated parrots as research subjects; in 31 of these based species-specific housing guidelines are scarce.
studies, 483 individuals from 45 different species could be According to the Universities Federation for Animal
considered laboratory animals. Amazons and budgerigars were Welfare Handbook on the Care and Management of Labora-
by far the most represented psittacine species. The laboratory tory and Other Research Animals (UFAW 2010), a labora-
research topics were categorized as either veterinary medicine tory animal in the broadest sense is “any non-human member
and diagnostics (bacteriology, hematology, morphology, and of the animal kingdom which is kept in captivity for experi-
mental or observational purposes.” Accurate information on
reproduction; 45%) or behavioral and sensory studies (behav-
the extent of psittacine bird use in laboratory research is
ior, acoustics, and vision; 17%). Confinement of psittacine
lacking because official statistics on laboratory animals do
species for research purposes is a matter of concern as scien-
not specify psittacine birds.
tifically based species-specific housing guidelines are scarce.
For this review we performed an ISI Web of Knowledge
The aim of this article is to provide scientific information rel- literature search in January 2010, using the search string
evant to the laboratory confinement of Psittaciformes to pro- “Topic=(psittaci*) OR Topic=(parrot*)” and including all 2009
mote the refinement of acquisition, housing, and maintenance references. This search yielded 141 database entries, which we
practices of these birds as laboratory animals. We briefly dis- categorized into type of research and research field as shown in
cuss systematics, geographical distribution, legislation, and Table 1. The main research fields in laboratory experiments
conservation status as background information on laboratory were veterinary medicine and diagnostics (bacteriology, hema-
parrot confinement. The following section presents welfare tology, morphology, and reproduction; 45%) and behavior and
concerns related to captive containment (including domestica- sensory studies (behavior, acoustics, and vision; 17%).
tion status) and psittacine cognition. We then discuss consider- As shown in Figure 1, studies published in 2009 in which
ations in the acquisition of laboratory parrots and review parrots were used as laboratory animals involved 483 indi-
important management issues such as nutrition, zoonoses, viduals of 45 different psittaciform species. Because it was
housing, and environmental enrichment. The final section re- not possible to determine the number of animals used in
views indications of distress and compromised welfare. three studies, the total number shown is a slight underrepre-
sentation of the actual count of laboratory Psittaciformes.
Key Words: avian welfare; bird; enrichment; housing; Most animals (83%) belonged to the Psittacidae family, and
laboratory animal; nutrition; parrot; psittacine 12% and 5% of individuals belonged to the families Cacatuidae
and Nestoridae, respectively. Within the Psittacidae, most of
Introduction the subjects (97%) were true parrots (only 12 individuals were
lorikeets), and of these Amazons (38%) and budgerigars

T
he Psittaciformes or parrots form a large order of land (32%) were by far the most represented.
birds comprising over 350 species in about 83 genera We also summarized information on the type of housing
(Collar 1997). In the United States they are the third described in the 31 publications of 2009 that cited the use of
parrots in laboratory experiments (i.e., not field studies or
Isabelle D. Kalmar, Msc Vet Med, MSc LAS, is an assistant in the Laboratory
other types of research). We found that 42% (n = 13) of the
of Animal Nutrition; Christel P.H. Moons, PhD Vet Med, MSc AS Lic, is an parrots were housed at on-site facilities, 23% (n = 7) at off-
assistant professor in the Laboratory of Ethology; and Geert P.J. Janssens, site facilities, and for 35% of cases (n = 11) housing infor-
PhD Vet Med, agr eng, is a professor in the Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, mation was unavailable. The off-site facilities were all
all at Ghent University in Belgium. outdoor aviaries; most studies conducted at on-site facilities
Address correspondence and reprint requests to Isabelle Dominique
Kalmar, Laboratory of Animal Nutrition, Department of Nutrition, Genetics,
used indoor cages (59% or n = 4), only 23% (n = 2) used
and Ethology, Ghent University, Heidestraat 19, 9820 Merelbeke, Belgium outdoor confinement, and in 18% of the studies (n = 1) this
or email isabelle.kalmar@ugent.be. information was unavailable. Information on group housing

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 409


Table 1 Number of peer-reviewed studies published during 2009 involving Psittaciformes as subjects
sorted by research type (rows) and research field (columns)

VMD BS EC GEN NUTR Total (%)

Field study 4 6 32 4 1 47 (33%)


Laboratory experiment 11 16 0 2 2 31 (22%)
Opportunistic sampling 17 1 0 10 0 28 (20%)
Case report 23 1 0 0 0 24 (17%)
Undetermined 8 0 1 2 0 11 (8%)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


Total 63 24 33 18 3 141 (100%)

BS, behavior and sensory; EC, ecology; GEN, genetics; NUTR, nutrition; VMD, veterinary medicine and diagnostics

was provided in 61% (n = 19) of the studies: 19% (n = 6) Background Information


used individual housing, 13% (n = 4) pair housing, and 29%
(n = 9) group housing. Systematics
The aim of this article is to provide scientific informa-
tion concerning laboratory confinement of Psittaciformes, Psittaciform birds have distinct morphological traits, such as a
with the intention of refining arrangements for their acqui- stout curved beak topped by a cere (the bump where the
sition, housing, and maintenance. We begin with an over- nostrils are located), zygodactyl feet (two toes pointing
view of systematics, geographic distribution, and specific forward and two backward), and colorful plumage. Tradition-
legislation and conservation status. The next section pres- ally, psittacines are phylogenetically placed between the
ents a discussion of welfare concerns related to captive Columbiformes (pigeons) and Cuculiformes (cuckoos). Using
containment, including psittacines’ domestication status molecular techniques, Sibley and Ahlquist (1990) suggested a
and cognition. We then discuss factors to consider in the closer relationship with Apodiformes (swifts), but this would
acquisition of laboratory parrots, followed by a review of be inconsistent with the plesiomorphic or primitive distal cen-
important management issues related to nutrition, housing, triole in swift sperm (Jamieson and Tripepi 2005).
and environmental enrichment. The final section concerns Within the order of Psittaciformes, two distinct groups
zoonotic diseases and indications of distress and compro- are generally recognized as separate families: Cacatuidae
mised welfare. (cockatoos) and Psittacidae (Collar 1997; Forshaw and Cooper

Figure 1 Total number of Psittaciform individuals (n) and species (N) included as study subjects in peer-reviewed studies published during
2009. For one study using Amazon parrots and for two from the “other” category the exact sample size could not be derived; the resulting
underrepresentation of the actual count is shown in italic font.

410 ILAR Journal


2002). External morphological features of cockatoos that wild-caught birds from Mexico, where the species is still
distinguish them from other parrots include an erectile crest extant, have resulted in a limited number of surviving, trans-
and lack of Dyck texture1 in feathers (as a result of the latter, located birds and some reproductive success in Arizona
cockatoos never display blue or green colors in their plum- (Snyder et al. 1994).
age). Internal differences include the presence in cockatoos
of a gallbladder, which is absent in other parrots (Rowley
1997). Recent phylogenetic data (Tokita et al. 2007) suggest Conservation and Legislation
a third family in the order of Psittaciformes, the Nestoridae,
Parrots have become the most endangered birds in the
which includes the New Zealand parrots: kakapo (Strigops
world—almost all species in the order of Psittaciformes are
habroptilus), kea (Nestor notabilis), and kaka (N. meridio-
listed by the Convention on International Trade in Endan-
nalis). Lories and lorikeets are a separate group, distinguished
gered Species (CITES2), an international treaty that classifies

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


by certain anatomical features and feeding preferences, but their
endangered species and regulates the import and export of
classification into a subfamily within the Psittacidae (loriinae)
wild flora and fauna, including animals (alive or dead) and
or into a separate family (Loriidae) is controversial (Russell
their products and tissues. CITES Appendix I (also called
1987; Forshaw and Cooper 2002). The most prominent anatom-
CITES I) lists species at risk of global extinction; Appendix
ical characteristic of lories is their long, brush-tipped tongue
II includes (1) species that may become threatened with ex-
and slender beak, reflecting a high degree of diet specialization
tinction in the absence of international trade restrictions, (2)
toward nectar and pollen (Cornejo and Clubb 2005).
nonendangered species that may be in danger because of
their resemblance to specimens listed in the appendices, and
Geographic Distribution (3) second-generation captive-born offspring of Appendix I
species (CITES 1973, 2009); and Appendix III lists species
Most Psittacines are native to the southern hemisphere and are for which trade is regulated in specific countries. Appendix I
predominantly, but not exclusively, found in tropical regions. lists 51 psittacine species, which account for about one-third
Lories, lorikeets, and cockatoos occur naturally only in Austra- of all avian species in this CITES appendix; nearly the entire
lia and surrounding Indonesian islands. Genera in the family of remaining two-thirds of psittacine species appear in Appen-
Psittacidae, on the other hand, are indigenous to Asia, Central dix II (CITES 2009). Only four species of Psittaciformes are
and South America, Africa, and the Australian continent. There not considered at risk: peach-faced lovebirds (Agapornis ro-
are only two genera of Asiatic parrots: hanging parrots (Loricu- seicollis), budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus), cockatiels
lus spp.) and ring-necked parakeets (Psittacula spp.). Pionus (Nymphicus hollandicus), and rose-ringed parakeets (Psit-
parrots (Pionus spp.), macaws (e.g., Ara, Cyanopsitta, and An- tacula krameri).
odorhynchus spp.) and conures (e.g., Aratinga, Cyanoliseus, The acquisition, transport, housing, and care of animals
and Pyrrhura spp.) are native to South America, and the Ama- kept for research purposes (and the reporting thereof) are
zons (Amazona spp.) originate from Central and South Amer- subject to national legislation. In the United States, any ver-
ica. African greys (Psittacus spp.), Poicephalus parrots tebrate animal use in research, teaching, or testing is consid-
(Poicephalus spp.), and lovebirds (Agapornis spp.) originate ered experimental and therefore subject to legal requirements.
from Africa; grass parakeets (Neophema spp.), budgerigars But coverage of birds in the US Animal Welfare Act (AWA),
(Melopsittacus sp.), and rosellas (Platycercus spp.) are indige- which regulates the treatment of laboratory animals in the
nous to Australia (Russell 1987). United States, has been a subject of debate as the Act’s orig-
Irrespective of geographical origin, descendants of es- inal definition of “animal” did not include birds. Subsequent
caped cage birds have been establishing self-sustaining amendments only partly addressed this exclusion: the Act
populations in regions other than their native habitat. For now covers birds not bred for research purposes, but explic-
instance, monk parakeets (Myopsitta monachus), which are itly excludes birds bred for research (Cohen 2006). US re-
indigenous to South America, have formed feral popula- search facilities that receive federal funding also have to
tions on four other continents (Russello et al. 2008), and in comply with the Public Health Service policy, outlined in the
the United States the American Ornithologists’ Union Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (NRC
(www.aou.org) has identified seven parrot species as self- 1996). The stipulations of the latter, in contrast to the AWA,
sustaining exotic species, with another seven likely to be also include purpose-bred birds (Federal Register 2004) but
recognized as such in the near future. But the two native lack species-specific guidelines on housing and manage-
US parrot species, the Carolina parakeet (Conuropsis caro- ment.3 In contrast to US guidelines and restrictions, Euro-
lensis) and the thick-billed parrot (Rhynchopsitta pachy- pean legislation excludes noninvasive procedures as well as
rhyncha), disappeared during the 20th century (Butler 2005). humane euthanasia, classifying only procedures that inflict
Although efforts to reestablish the latter have not resulted in
self-sustaining wild populations, release experiments with
2Abbreviation used in this article: CITES, Convention on International
Trade of Endangered Species
1Blue feather color is formed by nanostructures in the feather called Dyck 3The recent update of the Guide (8th ed., NRC 2010) includes references for

texture (Dyck 1971a,b). readers interested in this information for birds.

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 411


pain, suffering, distress, or lasting harm as animal experi- other parrot species, however, has become truly successful
ments (European Community 1986). only in the past 40 years. Aside from their late maturity and
Additional legislation governs the import and export of sexual monomorphism to the human eye, other contributing
endangered Psittaciform species as well as their scientific factors to this delay are the animals’ strict monogamy and
use. The US Fish and Wildlife Service, through a permit need for prolonged periods of parental care. A major im-
system, governs the import, export, and interstate trade of provement in captive breeding was the development of endo-
species listed as endangered or threatened in the United scopic and genetic sexing, as well as histological appraisal
States or elsewhere under the Endangered Species Act and is of breeding potential from testis biopsies. Thanks to these
also responsible for CITES administration (NRC 2006). Im- developments, about 85% of parrot species have bred in cap-
port or export of Appendix I specimens is allowed under tivity (Crosta et al. 2002; Gartrell 2002; Harcourt-Brown
very strict permitting requirements only for scientific pur- 2000; Silva 1991).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


poses, education, or conservation, but not commercial Yet domestication—the process of adaptation to captiv-
trade (ARENA/OLAW 2002; NRC 2006). In Europe, CITES ity through genetic changes—is still in its infancy for parrots
I specimens may be used in animal research only if (1) the and other psittacines (Meehan et al. 2003b). Although par-
objective is to preserve the species or (2) the species is the rots have been bred for generations and can become very
only available model in fundamental biological studies (Eu- docile and adapted to captive environments within their
ropean Community 1986). lifespan, they still share natural behavior and response
A number of regulations govern the use of birds, both thresholds with their wild counterparts and should be con-
wild caught and captive bred. In Europe, the scientific use of sidered wild animals (with the possible exceptions of bud-
CITES-listed wild-caught birds or their eggs is permissible gerigars and cockatiels; Bergman and Reinisch 2006). In
only if they are essential to the research and all necessary contrast to dogs and cats, for example, parrots are still in
licenses have been granted by the appropriate authorities their first few captive-bred generations and have not yet un-
(European Community 1979). In the United States, importa- dergone selection for behavioral traits that facilitate adapta-
tion of CITES-listed avians (with some exceptions, includ- tion to life in captivity (Davis 1999). They are therefore
ing 37 captive-bred parrot species) requires a Wild Bird likely to experience frustration when confined in conditions
Conservation Act permit. The Lacey Act, on the other hand, that limit the expression of species-specific behaviors, espe-
which concerns injurious birds (those that cause damage to cially those associated with high levels of motivation.
indigenous flora and fauna), does not apply to either birds
imported for scientific research or psittacines imported for
any purpose. Irrespective of purpose, US importation of Cognition
birds requires a 30-day quarantine period (except for imports
The remarkable cognitive abilities in psittacine birds, which
from Canada); importantly, if a single bird in the lot is diag-
have been intensively studied and documented (e.g., Giret
nosed with highly pathogenic avian influenza or Newcastle
et al. 2009; Pepperberg 2009; Shuck-Paim and Borsari 2009),
disease during the quarantine period, the whole lot must be
can be a source of welfare concern in the laboratory and ne-
destroyed (Paul 2005). Further information about legislation
cessitate careful consideration of the choice of species and
is available (NRC 2006; Paul 2005).
housing conditions. Parrots’ vocalizations, and especially
their ability to mimic the human voice, are a distinctive man-
ifestation of their cognition,4 and they exhibit lifelong vocal
Main Areas of Welfare Concern learning, whereas in most songbirds this feature is limited to
a sensitive period in early development (Marler 1970; Scarl
Domestication
and Bradbury 2009).
In the oldest known writings about parrots, dating from
Parrots, commonly admired for their vocal abilities and ex-
500 BCE, Ctesius described a plum-headed parakeet (Psit-
otic appearance, have been kept in captivity for at least 2500
tacula cyanocephala) saying Indian and Greek words
years. The first parrots introduced into Europe were probably
(Collar 1977). Scarl and Bradbury (2009) suggest that the
Alexander parakeets (Psittacula krameri), brought from the
ability in some parrot species to mimic vocal calls in a single
Far East by Alexander the Great during the 4th century BCE;
interaction is an efficient vocal technique for mediating fission-
North African and Indian species were spread by the Ro-
fusion flocks (constantly changing social groups). And
mans. In the 15th and 16th centuries, European explorers
Schachner and colleagues (2009) demonstrated the ability to
brought back American and Asian parrot species as living
align synchronized motor actions (head movements and foot
proof of their voyages (Collar 1997; Forshaw and Cooper
lifting) in response to music in an African grey parrot (Psittacus
1989; Silva 1991), and in the 18th century colonists brought
small Australian parakeets to Europe by (Silva 1991).
Budgerigars were successfully bred in captivity by the 4This ability in psittacines is distinctive but not unique: songbirds (Passeriformes)
end of the 19th century, largely because of their early repro- and hummingbirds (Trochilidae) also display vocalization through imitation
ductive maturity and sexual dimorphism (Alderton 2001; (Matsunaga and Okanoya 2009), in contrast to the vocalizations of most avian
Earle and Clarke 1991; Silva 1991). Captive breeding in groups, which depend on the production of innate sounds.

412 ILAR Journal


erithacus erithacus) and a sulfur-crested cockatoo (Cacatua caretakers, enhances the performance of experimental pro-
galerita). cedures, and reduces the risk of development or aggravation
In addition, numerous scientifically sound laboratory of welfare issues.
studies of parrot cognition have demonstrated that several In addition to all of the above factors, it is advisable to
parrot species display a much higher intellectual capacity consider the following species traits when acquiring labora-
than the ability merely to mimic the human voice. The Afri- tory parrots: species size, ecology, behavior, anticipated
can grey parrot Alex is probably the best-known individual longevity, and suitability for the intended research. Bird size,
in this research. Pepperberg (2006) worked extensively with ecology, and behavior can affect the ethics of a decision about
Alex and reviewed his trained cognitive abilities, which in- the housing of certain species in the available space; solitary
cluded the labeling of over 50 objects and the functional use housing, which is necessary in some research, can be deleteri-
of short queries that demonstrated accurate comprehension ous to the well-being of certain species. Further, the loudness

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


of concepts such as color, shape, material, relative size, and of normal vocalization varies by species (African grey parrots,
quantity. Studies of other African greys have demonstrated for instance, are quieter than most Amazons and cockatoos)
mirror-mediated discrimination, spatial location, and spon- and may require thoughtful consideration if the animal hous-
taneous categorization into unlabeled edible or nonedible ing area is near research offices (Wilson 2006).
items (Giret et al. 2009; Pepperberg 1995). Although most It is especially critical to bear in mind that all parrots are
cognitive research in parrots has involved African grey par- long-lived animals (Carpenter et al. 2001), and some species
rots, advanced cognitive processing has been evaluated on more so than others; budgerigars, for instance, have a rela-
other parrot species as well. Werdenich and Huber (2006), tively short lifespan (8 to 10 years) in contrast to the larger
for instance, showed successful performance of complex parrots (50 to 80 years). Longevity may thus be an ethical
string-pulling tasks in keas, and Shuck-Paim and Borsari consideration when choosing a parrot species, as most are
(2009) demonstrated this ability in neotropical parrot spe- very likely to outlive the duration of a research project. We
cies (two macaw and one Amazon species). recently reviewed possible fates of laboratory parrots after
The cognition of parrots is consistent with several char- the completion of research projects and found the following
acteristics generally associated with advanced cognitive pro- options: relocation to private care or zoological collections,
cessing, such as a relatively large brain, slow development, humane euthanasia, or reintroduction to the natural habitat
longevity, and a long period of parental association (Shuck- (Kalmar et al. 2007a). The first option is considered the most
Paim and Borsari 2009). ethical choice, but may not be feasible if responsible research
personnel did not socialize the birds properly during their
laboratory confinement (Kalmar et al. 2007a).
Factors to Consider in the Selection of Finally, the selection of appropriate parrot species for
Laboratory Parrots laboratory use can depend on their susceptibility to a partic-
ular infectious or metabolic disease. For example, African
The acquisition of animals that will be confined in labora- grey parrots may develop hypocalcemic tetany syndrome in
tory facilities for research purposes should always be based response to an absolute or relative calcium deficiency; other
on careful consideration of the species, source, and proposed parrot species respond to such a deficiency with calcium mo-
use and should take into account species, age, gender, back- bilization from their bones, thus maintaining blood calcium
ground, and homogeneity of these features among the birds level but developing metabolic bone disease (Kollias 1995;
to be used. Purchases should not depend simply on ease of Levine 2003). The peculiar calcium metabolism in African
availability or proximity of a retailer. Most importantly, greys makes this species an outstanding model to investigate
whenever possible, the least endangered and least cogni- its underlying mechanisms.
tively developed species should be the species of choice con- In conclusion, the selection of appropriate birds for labo-
sistent with achieving the experimental goals. ratory research should take into consideration not only the
In addition, several other factors are important to con- most suitable species but also the individual’s features for
sider. First, a heterogeneous group of laboratory animals is the intended research to optimize both the experimental data
likely to increase variability of research or test results and and animal welfare.
thus decrease the strength of experimental setups (and pos-
sibly require the use of additional animals). Second, good
knowledge of the animals’ background improves interpreta- Management Considerations
tion of research data and promotes sound decisions on nec-
essary health measures, which may considerably reduce the Nutrition
likelihood of introducing infectious agents into the resident
colony. Third, poor socialization may influence the animals’ Obesity and nutritional deficiencies are a common cause
aptitude to undergo laboratory confinement or may hamper of disease in captive parrots. Without a well-considered
the ease of handling. In our opinion, appropriate socializa- feeding strategy, nutritional imbalances will occur and ad-
tion of birds is of utmost importance in their laboratory versely affect animal health and welfare as well as experi-
confinement. Socialization facilitates handling by animal mental results.

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 413


Parrots are commonly fed a multicomponent whole-seed plant compounds, because geophagy typically occurs before
diet, which is perceived as a natural diet because parrots are foraging. Brightsmith and Muñoz-Najar (2004) posit the pro-
classified as seed eaters. But commercial parrot food mixes vision of additional sodium and other minerals when the di-
of cultivated seeds and nuts differ significantly from the nat- etary supply is insufficient as a further function of geophagy,
ural diet of wild parrots, which consume a wide range of suggesting a degree of innate nutritional wisdom.
plant parts as well as invertebrates and sometimes meat from
animal carcasses. Their diet also changes based on seasonal
availability (Collar 1997; Ullrey et al. 1991). Moreover, the Seed Diets
typical ingredients in commercial seed mixes for parrots are
often not indigenous to the natural habitat of all parrot spe- Commercial seed mixtures commonly provided to, and readily
cies for which these mixes are intended. Sunflower seed, ingested by, parrots are deficient in several minerals and vita-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


pumpkinseed, and peanuts, for instance, originate from the mins, and contain a deteriorated calcium:phosphorus ratio (e.g.,
American continent and thus are not natural food items for Harrison 1998; Ullrey et al. 1991; Wolf et al. 1998). Manifesta-
African, Australian, or Asiatic species. Additionally, physi- tion of resulting deficiencies remains subclinical for prolonged
cal constraints would hamper wild parrots’ foraging on pea- periods of time, and when clinical symptoms do emerge they
nuts or pumpkinseeds, which captive parrots regard as are often unspecific and remain undiagnosed (Wolf et al. 1998).
preferred food items (Kalmar et al. 2010). It is thus not surprising that malnutrition constitutes the primary
Regardless of the type of diet, fresh food should be cause of up to 90% of all clinical problems in pet birds pre-
provided daily, and clean drinking water must be available at sented to veterinarians (Harrison 1998).
all times and refreshed at least daily. In addition, because parrots dehusk seeds (thereby con-
suming only the kernel) and display a profoundly selective
feeding behavior when provided a multicomponent diet, pre-
Effect of Nutrition on Feather Color
ferring oilseeds (e.g., pumpkin and sunflower seeds) over
carbohydrate-rich seeds, the fraction they actually consume
In contrast to several other bird orders, where red, yellow,
is significantly different from the intended diet. They also
and orange feather pigment is a function of dietary-derived
consume inadequate amounts of supplements (e.g., oyster
carotenoids or metabolites thereof (McGraw et al. 2004), the
shells or vitamin-, amino acid-, and mineral-rich pellets) in-
feather coloration of psittacine birds cannot be influenced by
corporated in these seed-based diets (Kalmar et al. 2010).
dietary supplements (although aberrant pigmentation often
Nutritional supplementation through drinking water is
reflects chronic malnutrition or toxicity; for review, Koski
not recommended as vitamins are unstable in aqueous solu-
2002). Over 80% of psittaciform species display red in their
tions and reduce water palatability, possibly decreasing wa-
feathers; in parrots this color is generated by unique red pig-
ter intake and introducing a risk of dehydration (Donoghue
ments, called polyenal lipochromes or psittacofulvins, that
and Stahl 1997; Hawley 1997).
are locally synthesized de novo at maturing feather follicles
(Forshaw and Cooper 1978; Stradi et al. 2001). Blues, on
the other hand, are formed by feather nanostructures (Dyck
texture) and greens evolve from a mixture of the blue struc- Pellet Diets: Advantages
tural color and endogenously produced yellow pigments
(Dyck 1971a,b; McGraw and Nogare 2005; Rowley 1997). Pellet diets rather than seed mixtures are strongly recom-
Black, brown, and grey shades result from melanin pigmen- mended for birds used in laboratory research because they
tation, whereas unpigmented feather keratin is white offer two important advantages: they prevent selective feed-
(McGraw et al. 2005; Prum et al. 1999). ing behavior and they can be precisely formulated to comply
with available nutrient guidelines (e.g., AAFCO 1998). As a
result, first, the pellet diet can contribute to long-term health
Psittacine Strategies in Detoxification and help reduce biased research data that may result from
of Foodstuffs (undiagnosed) dietary deficiencies. Second, with a multi-
component diet, management and/or animal factors may re-
Parrots’ inherent tendency to peel food items is thought to be sult in a significant divergence between the nutrient supply
a method to safely broaden their repertoire of forageable food (known) of the offered diet and that of the actual consumed
items (to include, for example, seeds that are toxic to other fraction (unknown unless specifically calculated based on
species) as the outer layers of certain plant products often con- analysis of food intake, nutrient content of offered diet, and
tain the highest concentration of toxins (Gilardi et al. 1999). a subsample from the total collection of all food remainders).
Other natural methods of detoxification include ingestion of Such analyses are costly, time intensive, and inevitably inac-
clay or charcoal, which adsorb potential dietary toxins and curate when birds are group housed. Seeds, nuts, fruit, and
may confer cytoprotection of the gastrointestinal lining some vegetables can be provided as treats to complement the
(Collar 1997; Gilardi et al. 1999). Burger (2003) and Mee and pellet diet; fruit can be offered ad libitum as a safe measure
colleagues (2005) further substantiate intake of clay in parrots to reduce voluntary energy intake and prevent obesity with-
as a deliberate defense mechanism against secondary toxic out compromising sufficient protein intake (Kalmar et al.

414 ILAR Journal


2010). However, avocado,5 for instance, is highly toxic to omnivorous and the kaka forages on diverse plant items as
parrots (Hargis 1989; Schulte and Rupley 2004). well as large numbers of invertebrates (Diamond and Bond
1991; Moorhouse 1997). The Loriidae or loriinae are necta-
rivorous birds (i.e., they feed predominantly on nectar and
Pellet Diets: Responses to Concerns
pollen), supplementing their intake with fruit, flowers, and
invertebrates as minor components of their diet (Cannon
Despite the advantages of pelleted food in terms of nutrition
1984; Collar 1997; Gartrell 2000). In captivity, lorikeets are
as well as easy dosing and delivery of oral test substances,
usually fed a commercially available nectar powder that is di-
several concerns about such diets persist. We address these
luted before use; the degree of dilution affects voluntary intake
based on published reports and our own observations.
and thus energy and nutrient supply (Kalmar et al. 2009).
First, there is concern that the lack of a husk on pellets
might compromise parrots’ natural feeding behavior, which

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


includes manipulation of food with their feet and beak. But Food Storage
parrots intensively manipulate pellets before ingestion,
which is consistent with their behavior when they eat seeds The method of storage can vastly alter the nutritional com-
or nuts (Rozek et al. 2010). Second, a potential reduction in position of the manufacturer’s formulation because food is
the animals’ time spent feeding might result in boredom and often stored in large containers in which different-sized in-
behavioral disorders. However, Wolf and colleagues (2002) gredients segregate. It is therefore advisable to regularly stir
demonstrated similar feeding time budgets when parrots the food and completely empty storage containers of multi-
were fed either seeds or pellets; and increasing the size of the component diets before refilling to ensure that all compo-
pellets can contribute to a rise in total time spent feeding nents are eventually fed to the animals. An alternative option
(Rozek et al. 2010). Third, parrots are notably neophobic, is to store food in the original bags (Kalmar et al. 2010), al-
especially when wild caught, single housed, or raised in bar- though closed feed containers offer the advantage of im-
ren environments (Fox and Millam 2004; Meehan and proved protection against vermin.
Mench 2002; Meehan et al. 2003a), so it is in their nature to Food should be properly stored and consumed (or dis-
be resistant to novel food items (i.e., a change from a seed posed of) by the manufacturer’s indicated date, as improper
diet to a pellet diet). But most parrots can be readily and (i.e., suboptimal temperature or relative humidity) or pro-
safely converted to pellet diets with a proper conversion longed storage can promote the formation of mycotoxins
schedule and careful monitoring of their food intake. Con- (Samour 2004). Exposure to air or light also negatively af-
version of food type is not recommended for birds housed in fects the nutritional quality of foods, for example by reducing
large groups in which individual monitoring is impeded and the content of several vitamins (Reddy and Love 1999). Ac-
must be avoided with sick birds (Ghysels 1997). Last, the cording to Bavelaar and Beynen (2003), oxidation of polyun-
fact that pellet feeding has been shown to inflict gastric le- saturated fatty acids due to improper storage is likely to be
sions in other granivorous birds (i.e., chickens; Bird et al. more extensive in pelleted foods compared to seeds as the
1937) raised concerns that the same might be true for par- husks of the latter protect fatty acids from exposure to air.
rots. However, parrots use their beaks to reduce the particle
size of ingested foodstuffs, whereas chickens ingest grains
whole and reduction of ingredient size normally occurs in
Zoonoses
the gizzard (through its strong musculature and thick koilin
layer, assisted by the mechanical action of ingested grit). Like many laboratory animals psittacine birds can be carriers
Hence, it is likely that particle size reduction through food of infectious agents with zoonotic potential (i.e., transmissible
processing during the manufacture of pellets (which are sim- to humans). A complete overview of such zoonotic agents
ply compressed or extruded forms of ground ingredients) falls outside the scope of this article; instead we offer general
has far less effect on the gizzard in parrots compared to guidelines, with a brief description of psittacosis (also called
chickens (Kalmar et al. 2007b). ornithosis), which is possibly the most significant zoonosis
associated with parrots (Scott 1995; Turner 1987).
Diet Specialization in Minor Parrot Groups All personnel should be well informed of the possible
risk of zoonotic diseases, which can be contracted either di-
Some groups of parrots have particular feeding specializa- rectly from psittacine birds or indirectly from their excreta
tions. Among the Nestoridae, the nocturnal and flightless or contaminated materials and air. Adequate personal and
kakapo is entirely herbivorous and feeds mostly on tree environmental hygiene measures—including vermin con-
leaves, grasses, and herbs, ingesting only the plant juices and trol, as the latter also can be vectors of disease—should be in
spitting out squeezed pellets containing coarse fibers (Eason place at animal facilities. Personnel with impaired immunity,
et al. 2006; Houston et al. 2007). In contrast, the kea is including pregnant women, should strictly avoid any direct
or indirect contact with the birds.
5The avocado pit contains the toxin persin, which leaks into the fruit and is Newly acquired parrots should be subject to appropriate
cardiotoxic to birds (Schulte and Rupley 2004). quarantine and screening procedures. Such measures enable

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 415


early identification of possibly zoonotic agents and may Housing
prevent the transmission to resident birds of infectious
agents (e.g., psittacine circovirus, which causes beak and Despite their widespread geographic distribution, most par-
feather disease). rot species have similar behavior repertoires and thus similar
To protect the health of both personnel and birds, animal housing and management requirements in captivity (Russello
health checks, including diagnostic screening for important et al. 2008). Parrots are generally diurnal prey animals
zoonotic agents, should be repeated regularly. Table 2 pro- and, with few exceptions, highly social birds that establish
vides a partial list of zoonotic agents that can be con- strong, monogamous pair bonds and often form gregarious
tracted from parrots (Scott 1995). Of particular importance flocks (Evans 2001; Seibert 2006). Inconsistent with envi-
is Chlamydophila psittaci, which is the etiologic agent of ronmental enrichment recommendations (discussed below;
the human disease psittacosis (also called parrot fever and also see Bateson and Feenders 2010 and Schmidt 2010, both

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


ornithosis). Parrots can be asymptomatic carriers and shed- in this issue), laboratory confinement during research trials
ders of chlamydiosis, and the strains found in Amazon par- often requires solitary housing. However, it is probably un-
rots seem to be particularly virulent to humans (Turner common that parrots are actively involved in trials for the
1987). In birds the disease is usually a subclinical infection duration of their laboratory confinement and more likely that
but may be associated with nonspecific clinical signs such trials account for only a small share of their time in labora-
as dyspnea, anorexia, lethargy, diarrhea, biliverdinuria (yel- tory housing. An ethical approach is to provide two types of
low to green urates), and oculonasal discharge (Gould 1995; enclosures, one that complies with the restrictive require-
Greenacre 2003; Turner 1987). In humans the infection usu- ments of the experiment and another that accommodates ex-
ally also passes subclinically, but clinical signs may vary pression of the animal’s natural behavior.
from mild respiratory disease to pneumonia, nausea, vomit- Appropriate confinement construction and design should
ing, hepatitis, myocarditis, disorientation, mental depres- safeguard the health and well-being of both the birds and their
sion, delirium, and even death. Possible transmission routes caretakers. Depending on the nature and anticipated duration
of C. psittaci from birds to humans include, in order of im- of the research project, it may be prudent to consider the use
portance, inhalation of contaminated air; direct contact with of resident animals at an off-site setting, such as a zoo or orni-
birds, feathers, excreta, oculonasal discharge, or infected thological collection, instead of establishing on-site capacity
tissue; and bites or other open wounds. The incubation pe- in the research institution. Consideration of alternatives is par-
riod is 1 to 4 weeks (Turner 1987). Diagnostic tests for C. ticularly relevant in light of the substantial investments in-
psittaci in parrots include culture, cytology, polymerase volved in the acquisition of birds, appropriate housing and
chain reaction, and the enzyme-linked immunosorbent as- daily care associated with the setup of an on-site parrot facil-
say, but none of these has proven to be 100% accurate ity, and the discrepancy between the lifespan of the research
(Gould 1995; Turner 1987). subjects and the projected duration of research projects.

Table 2 Potential zoonotic diseases that can be contracted from contact with psittacine birds
(adapted from Scott 2005)

Predominant Effect on host


route of Control
Disease Causal organism transmission Bird Human measures

Salmonellosis Salmonella spp. Orofecal Subclinical to Gastrointestinal Hygiene, RHCa


acute disease signs, fever
Psittacosis Chlamydophila Inhalation Subclinical to Subclinical, respiratory Screening,
(ornithosis) psittaci acute disease signs (mortality) hygiene, RHC
Yersiniosis Y. pseudotuberculosis, Orofecal Subclinical to Gastrointestinal Hygiene, rodent
Y. enterocolitica acute disease signs control

Tuberculosis Mycobacterium spp. Orofecal Cutaneous lesions, Local lesions, Hygiene, RHC
systemic lesions respiratory signs,
urinary tract disorders
Ectoparasite Dermanyssus Contact Subclinical, pruritus, Pruritus, skin lesions Hygiene, RHC
infestation gallinae anemia

aRHC, routine health checks

416 ILAR Journal


Environmental Conditions birds require bigger enclosures (Dilger 1982a; Lightfoot and
Nacewicz 2006; Turner 1992). The appropriateness of cage
Parrots require a circadian light-dark schedule, in which the dimensions is determined by volume and shape, and both in
dark period should be of adequate duration (about 12 hours) turn dictate the number and kind of birds within. To facilitate
and without disruptions to facilitate sleep; an inappropriate flight, cages should be rectangular in shape and of sufficient
photoperiod (i.e., a dark period that is either too long or too length (and width) to accommodate airborne movement
short) or poor night’s rest adversely affects their physical and (Hawkins 2001; Luescher and Wilson 2006).
emotional health (Bergman and Reinisch 2006; Lightfoot and
Nacewicz 2006). The lighting schedule in animal rooms for Design
parrots should try to mimic their natural habitat, with a year-
round day length of 12 hours and a gradual transition between The internal design and complexity of an enclosure are

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


darkness and light (Dilger 1982b; Wilson 2005). Some birds highly relevant to the birds’ usable space and comfort. Ap-
(e.g., chickens) experience low-frequency light as strobo- propriate perches are a must in a psittacine enclosure (for a
scopic (Nuboer et al. 1992; Prescott et al. 2003). review of the advantages and disadvantages of different
Other environmental considerations include temperature, types of perches, Kalmar et al. 2007a). Favorable perch traits
humidity, and noise levels. Extreme temperatures and high include good grip texture and suitable diameter (to ensure
relative humidity should be avoided, although parrots tolerate secure footing), flexibility (to stimulate balance and exer-
a relatively wide range of these environmental parameters cise), and varying diameter (to promote foot health); ideal
(Dilger 1982a). During the daytime parrots, which are noisy, perches are also chewable (environmental enrichment), non-
gregarious animals themselves, are surrounded by noise in slippery and not cold (in contrast to aluminum bars, for ex-
their natural habitat (complete silence often indicates the pres- ample), nonabrasive, and nontoxic (Kalmar et al. 2007a).
ence of a predator), but if the environment is too noisy, it can Parrots are prey animals that roost in trees and the height of
render the birds nervous or even noisier (Davis 1999). The perches is important to ensure their sense of safety and com-
provision of soft background music can help avoid continuous fort. As a general guideline, perch height should approxi-
apprehensive states in parrots (Evans 2001). mate human shoulder height (i.e., by suspending or elevating
the enclosure) (Luescher and Wilson 2006; Wilson 2006).
Type of Enclosure Other measures to promote a sense of security in parrots
include the provision of a hiding place (e.g., a dark, cavity-
There are several options for bird housing: cage or aviary, soli- like wooden box) or visual barriers in, on top of, or adjacent
tary or nonsolitary, and indoor or outdoor. Each type has ad- to the cage (Evans 2001). To provide clambering opportuni-
vantages and limitations. A cage is a relatively small enclosure ties, the enclosure should have horizontal bars or mesh wire
for solitary or pair housing, whereas an aviary is a large, most (Hawkins 2001); solid cage sides and vertical bars are defi-
often outdoor enclosure that can accommodate a flock of nitely discouraged.
birds. Solitary housing is inadvisable because of the social na-
ture of most parrot species (see the section below on Environ- Cage Position
mental Enrichment), but group housing can give rise to
intraspecies aggression and so requires careful monitoring When positioning cages in an animal room, one should keep
(Romagnano 2006). Indoor housing is usually more constrain- in mind that parrots are a prey species, so a preferred posi-
ing in dimension, but outdoor housing increases the risk of tion is next to an opaque wall and/or away from doors or
infectious diseases contracted from wild birds (Lightfoot and windows as the sudden passage of humans or vehicles out-
Nacewicz 2006). Wilson (2006) advises not housing species side the latter could startle the animal (Dilger 1982a; Evans
that originated on different continents in the same airspace. 2001; Wilson 2005). There should be sufficient space or
Outdoor confinement offers the advantage of full-spectrum double wiring between enclosures to prevent birds from bit-
sunlight, through which ultraviolet B radiation enables endog- ing the toes of adjacently housed conspecifics (Harcourt-
enous vitamin D3 synthesis and thus promotes calcium me- Brown 2000; Stoodley et al. 1992). Large, adjacent hanging
tabolism (Stanford 2006; Wilson 2006). However, challenges cages should allow easy passage along at least two free ends
inherent in such housing include higher risks of escape at- of the cages: the passage at the front side of the cages should
tempts by the birds and theft of the animals. be kept clear and used for daily husbandry procedures; the
rear end of the cages ideally offers a protective barrier (e.g.,
through dense plant covering), with access only for facilitat-
Dimensions and Shape
ing nonroutine procedures such as capture of the birds.
Scientifically based recommendations for species-specific
cage dimensions are scarce. Enclosures should at least enable Accessibility and Safety
the birds to spread their wings and turn around while perched
without touching the cage floor or walls with their tail or Wide door-like openings should permit easy access to all cor-
wings. Larger birds, long-tailed birds, or a greater number of ners of the cage through which the birds as well as all enclosure

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 417


items can be reached (Luescher and Wilson 2006). But water environments that lack resemblance to their natural living
and feeding bowls should be easily accessible without conditions, they face a number of challenges.
the need to enter or open the enclosure (Luescher and Wilson
2006). The same is true for solid cage bottoms, for which
Constraints on the Expression of
drawer-like designs facilitate removal for cleaning and disin-
Species-Specific Behavior
fection (Kalmar et al. 2007a,b). In hanging cages or aviaries,
a mesh bottom enables removal of excrement and food spill-
In captivity, parrots are often deprived of social contact and
age with minimal disturbance to the birds. These features
have limited opportunities for exploration and foraging. The lat-
also minimize the risk to animal caretakers of being bitten by
ter is the appetitive phase of feeding behavior and precedes ac-
the parrots, which can inflict serious flesh wounds. Because
tual consumption of food (the consummatory phase). Under
animal research often requires manual restraint, cage design
natural conditions, time spent performing behaviors from the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


and accessibility, coupled with personnel training in proper
appetitive phase amounts to several hours per day whereas in
handling and catching techniques, can minimize stress and
captivity, when “free food” is provided in a dish, the time in-
risk of injury to both the birds and the animal caretakers. Lit-
volved in this activity decreases substantially (e.g., 30-72 min
erature on techniques to manually restrain parrots is available
per day for orange-winged Amazon parrots), thereby decreas-
(Kalmar et al. 2007a; Schulte and Rupley 2004).
ing the overall time spent feeding (reviewed in Meehan et al.
Materials used to construct cages must be noncorrosive
2003b). Furthermore, the reduction in (or even omission of) the
and nontoxic. Galvanized wire cages have to be washed with
appetitive phase reduces the diversity of the parrots’ behavioral
diluted acetic acid and thoroughly brushed to remove all
repertoire, as does the restricted space available in enclosures,
“white rust” (lumps of oxidized zinc) before use in order to
which limits the opportunity for locomotory behaviors. The
prevent “new wire disease,” a form of zinc poisoning
constraint of these and other highly motivated behaviors may
(Harcourt-Brown 2000; Howard 1992).
lead to frustration, which after prolonged exposure, unless the
animal adapts, becomes chronic stress (Ödberg 1978) and can
Species-Specific Considerations lead to the development of abnormal behaviors such as stereo-
typies (invariant, apparently useless behaviors that are repeated
Species-specific characteristics must be taken into account regularly over time; Ödberg 1978). There is much debate, how-
in all types of laboratory confinement. For example, certain ever, about whether a stereotypy is an adaptive response (i.e., a
parrot species are considerably more prone to aggressive means to cope with the stress caused by the limited environ-
behavior toward conspecifics than others. Male cockatoos ment) or a pathological manifestation (Mason 2006).
in particular, but also macaws and Amazons, often inflict
serious mate trauma when housed in pairs, although this
Inanimate versus Animate Environmental
apparently does not occur in free-ranging specimens
Enrichment
(Romagnano 2006). Another example of a species-specific
behavior applies to a minority of parrot species (e.g., many
It has long been accepted that every effort should be made to
cockatoo species) that spend much of their active time in
enhance the quality of life for laboratory animals by reduc-
terrestrial activity (Wilson 2006). Cockatiels, for instance,
ing the stresses of a stimulus-poor environment (NRC 1996).
normally display a distinctive running behavior; suspended
One broad definition of environmental enrichment is “an
wire cages are therefore discouraged for this species in fa-
animal husbandry principle that seeks to enhance the quality
vor of standing aviaries or cages with a solid (but not slip-
of captive animal care by identifying and providing the envi-
pery or abrasive) bottom (Dilger 1982a; Hawkins 2001;
ronmental stimuli necessary for optimal psychological and
Wilson 2006).
physiological well-being” (Shepherdson et al. 1998). And
for parrots, too, notwithstanding the limitations inherent in
Environmental Enrichment the captive confinement of psittacine species, behavioral re-
search has shown that environmental enrichment is effective
Free-ranging parrots are accustomed to an environment in in preventing the development of abnormal repetitive behav-
which they have been evolving for centuries and where they iors (Lumeij and Hommers 2008; Meehan et al. 2003b).
have developed a behavioral repertoire whose context- Enrichment for psittacines typically falls into inanimate
appropriate expression increases their chances of survival and and animate categories (for reviews, Evans 2001; Kalmar
reproduction. Psittacine species have been reported to forage et al. 2007a; Meehan and Mench 2006). Inanimate enrichment
for 4 to 6 hours per day (Snyder et al. 1987) and to spend comprises (1) adaptations to the physical cage environment
most of their active time in close proximity to conspecifics. (e.g., a swinging ladder, suspended plastic coil, or cotton
They typically move about the canopy or between shelters in rope wound around a spiral ring, for balancing, manipulat-
open woodland areas to be less conspicuous to predators and ing, and climbing exercises), (2) provision of manipulanda
have a considerable amount of control over their environ- (e.g., objects and toys that can be manipulated with the beak
ment and the diversity of stimuli. When such birds, which as and feet), and (3) foraging opportunities (e.g., items to pro-
mentioned above are largely undomesticated, are housed in long the appetitive phase of feeding such as barriers to chew

418 ILAR Journal


through, food puzzles, and other containers that require the bird’s teristics of the individual animal before implementing envi-
manipulation to obtain the food) (Meehan and Mench 2006). ronmental enrichment, as long as individual accommodations
Animate enrichment includes social contact with both do not interfere with experimental standardization.
conspecifics and humans.6 Social parrot species, such as A fourth approach to environmental enrichment draws on
orange-winged Amazons, display differences in behavior when the idea that laboratory animals lack appropriate challenges.
housed as same-sex pairs compared to their counterparts in A recent theoretical framework states that presenting animals
solitary housing (Meehan et al. 2003a). The pair-housed birds with problems that are potentially stressful but which they pos-
more frequently made use of inanimate enrichment devices, sess the capacity to resolve may have a positive effect on behav-
were less fearful of novelty (inanimate and animate), spent ioral and physiological animal welfare indicators (Meehan and
less time screaming, and remained more active. More impor- Mench 2007). Of course, the challenge should be carefully
tantly, they did not develop any stereotypy throughout the 12- designed to ensure that the animal is indeed able to solve the

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


month study. They did, however, show mild aggression problem. In human-animal social interaction, challenges may
toward one another (it was never severe enough to necessitate be introduced to the bird through training sessions in which the
separation of pairs). Intraspecific aggression usually develops animal masters a behavioral task (Pepperberg 2007).
at the age of sexual maturity (Carpenter et al. 2001; Romagnano Last, after implementing an enrichment scheme, it is im-
2006) (between the ages of 4 and 6 years in Amazons), portant to thoroughly evaluate its ergonomic costs to animal
whereas the birds in the study of Meehan and colleagues caretakers (e.g., additional cleaning of enrichment objects),
(2003a) were only 2½ years old at the end of the experiment. its effectiveness with regards to the intended goals for the
With respect to interaction with humans, another study on animals, and potential undesired side effects, and to make
orange-winged Amazons showed that, without access to in- adaptations accordingly.
animate enrichment in their home cage, they were more mo-
tivated to interact with humans (Meehan and Mench 2002).
Fear-Minimizing Strategies
Effectiveness of Environmental Enrichment
Efforts to adapt parrots’ physical environment should in-
clude consideration of fear-minimizing strategies. Captive
The development of enrichment schemes requires consider-
birds cannot escape when they are exposed to unpleasant
ation of a number of elements. First, enrichment objects with
stimuli, which may not be perceived as such by their human
biological relevance will be most successful at stimulating
handlers and/or caretakers. It is therefore advisable to pro-
behaviors from the species-specific ethogram. In orange-
vide some type of refuge in the cage or at least respect the
winged Amazons, for instance, researchers discovered that
safety of a sheltered cage wall, so the bird can learn that
the color and hardness of preferred enrichment objects were
staying close to that wall gives it some control over the de-
similar to those of preferred food objects (Kim et al. 2009).
gree of stimulus exposure. It could be argued that providing
More recently, research showed that in the same species, cer-
shelter may decrease habituation to humans and thus com-
tain characteristics of manipulanda (rope) were preferred if
plicate animal handling during experimentation. To our
they allowed expression of biologically relevant behavior
knowledge, this has not been systematically investigated in
(preening), although this effect depended on gender (Webb
parrots used for research purposes, but conflicting evidence
et al. 2010). Second, novel objects may not only stimulate
exists in other laboratory animal species (e.g., mice) (Hess
exploration but also induce avoidance due to neophobia,
et al. 2008; Moons et al. 2004).
both of which are somewhat related to psittacine habitat and
diet characteristics (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002). Third, en-
vironmental enrichment in young parrots can reduce the
likelihood of neophobia at a later age (Fox and Millam
Indications of Distress and
2007). The degree of neophobia in juveniles is not necessar-
Compromised Welfare
ily constant as hand-raised birds showed decreased neopho-
Excessive Vocalization
bia at 7 months, but similar levels of neophobia compared to
parent-reared birds at 12 months (Fox and Millam 2004). Almost all psittacine species are highly social and flock-
Rotating the enrichment objects increases their efficiency, dwelling animals, displaying a wide variety of vocalizations—
but individual differences modulate this effect: Fox and calm contact calls, loud alarm calls, food-begging calls, and
Millam (2007) demonstrated an increase in neophobia in the interspecific agonistic calls—that help to promote flock cohe-
most fearful individuals of a colony of Amazons in response sion (Bergman and Reinisch 2006). Vocalization is thus an
to frequent change of enrichment objects, whereas the inherent component of the normal repertoire of parrot behav-
opposite was true for less fearful individuals. For this reason ior. It may become excessive in both loudness and frequency,
it may be useful to take into account the behavioral charac- and humans often consider any loud vocalization in parrots
6However, introducing social contact for species that are known to be undesirable, excessive, or abnormal. But while such vocaliza-
solitary, such as adult kakapos, may abolish the beneficial effects of tions may indicate disturbed well-being, they may also simply
environmental enrichment (Diamond et al. 2006). reflect unintentionally learned attention-seeking behavior.

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 419


It is important to try to distinguish between normal, evaluate the animal’s behavior, proximity to the object, and
distress-signaling, and learned vocalization in parrots. The un- bite marks resulting from manipulation (Fox and Millam
derlying motivation should be considered by taking into ac- 2004). Assessments of fear toward familiar or unfamiliar peo-
count both the animals and elements in their environment. ple involve a handler response test that scores birds’ reactions
Normal vocalization is displayed according to a daily pattern (e.g., flight distance, bites, screams) to the following stimuli:
that includes extensive morning and evening vocalization as extending a finger toward the bird, touching the bird on differ-
well as several 15- to 20-minute loud vocalizations through- ent parts of the body, offering food, and standing or sitting
out the day (Wilson 2005). In contrast, prolonged and repeti- near the bird (Meehan and Mench 2002).
tive screaming, especially if it occurs throughout the day,
may indicate boredom, alarm and fear (Alderton 2001;
Davis 1991), a welfare concern related to a distressing environ- Abnormal Repetitive Behavior

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


ment, or, as mentioned, a desire for increased attention from
According to Garner (2007) abnormal repetitive behaviors
caretakers (Bergman and Reinisch 2006).
are those that “are inappropriately repetitive in goal or motor
pattern, and are functionless, maladaptive, or self-injurious.”
They comprise stereotypies and impulsive/compulsive be-
Fear Behavior haviors (e.g., feather picking; for review, Van Zeeland et al.
2009). Meehan and Mench (2006) classify parrot stereotyp-
Fear is generally considered an unpleasant emotional state
ies into three main categories: (1) locomotor (e.g., route trac-
(Jones 1997), so animal caretakers and research personnel
ing, pacing, corner flipping), (2) oral (e.g., spot pecking,
should be able to readily recognize species-specific signs of it.
sham chewing, bar biting, tongue rolling), and (3) object-
Parrots’ fear of humans is often directed at the hands; hu-
directed (repetitive, invariant manipulation of objects). In
man faces rarely elicit fear reactions, but if the sight or prox-
rare cases, parrots have been known to develop vocal stereo-
imity of a friendly face does provoke fear, it could indicate
typies (screaming) (Bergman and Reinisch 2006).
extreme fright (Wilson 2007). As parrots are prey animals,
The development of a stereotypy is often linked to frus-
they may respond to fear by biting (Welle and Luescher 2006);
tration caused by constraint of a highly motivated behavior
more often, however, they instinctively attempt to avoid the
(Mason 1991; Meehan and Mench 2006). The stereotypic
threat by flying away (although panic flights can cause them
behavior can become disengaged from its original cause and
injury). Other fear-induced behaviors include reluctance to
continue to be performed even after environmental condi-
approach (novel objects, humans, conspecifics), withdrawal,
tions are improved, and may thus not necessarily indicate
hiding, or alarm calls. If such signals are ignored, the animal
currently compromised welfare but rather provide informa-
may express stronger responses, such as fearful vocalization
tion about the animal’s history. Conditions known to elicit
or a passive-aggressive posture (i.e., erection of all contour
stereotypic behavior in parrots include impaired foraging
feathers) (Welle and Luescher 2006; Wilson 2007). As a last
opportunities, insufficient opportunity for locomotion, and
resort, the animal may bite, which usually leads to the desired
lack of (physical) social contact with conspecifics (Meehan
outcome: cessation of interaction with the handler. Repetitive
et al. 2003a, 2004; Meehan and Mench 2006). Meehan and
exposure to fearful situations in which humans ignore a bird’s
colleagues (2004) demonstrated that environmental enrich-
warning signals increases the risk of apparently impulsive fear
ment in the form of foraging opportunities and increased
biting (Welle and Luescher 2006; Wilson 2006). In general, all
level of cage complexity significantly reduced—but did not
sources of fear should be kept to a minimum and causal fac-
completely eliminate—the performance of stereotypies in
tors of seemingly unfounded dread actively be sought, fol-
single-housed Amazons. In pair-housed Amazons, however,
lowed by an attempt to overcome or minimize them. As
the same environmental enrichment protocol completely in-
discussed above, the use of diverse environmental enrichment
hibited development of stereotypies (Meehan et al. 2003a).
strategies can reduce the likelihood of fear responses.
Several standardized methods are available to objectively
evaluate the level of fear in parrots. For instance, it is possible Disease
to assess an animal’s fear toward a novel object by measuring
its latency to feed on a highly favored food item in the pres- A healthy bird colony requires quarantine units, separate
ence of the object (Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002). A second housing and infectious disease monitoring for newly ac-
test measures both the latency of first contact with a novel quired birds, and thorough health checks of all birds at regu-
object and the duration of interaction within a fixed time inter- lar intervals. Aside from obvious signs of trauma or infectious
val (Meehan and Mench 2002; Mettke-Hofmann et al. 2002); disease, animal care and research personnel should also
a longer latency does not necessarily reflect greater fear, how- be attentive to more subtle indications of impaired health
ever, but may indicate a low motivation for exploration, which (including malnutrition), such as abnormal plumage,7 bone
is likely if the duration of interaction is also low (Meehan and dimorphism, emaciation, clinical signs of metabolic disease,
Mench 2002). A third method to test fear toward a novel ob-
ject is to introduce it to the parrot in a familiar environment— 7Examples of abnormal plumage are stress bars, weakened feathers, broken
for example, by placing or hanging it in the home cage—and or damaged feathers, and depigmentation (reviewed in Koski 2002).

420 ILAR Journal


or infectious disease resulting from impaired immunological Bergman L, Reinisch US. 2006. Parrot vocalisation. In: Luescher AU,
function. Inadequate management or housing conditions, ed. Manual of Parrot Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.
p 219-223.
such as abrasive perches, suboptimal environmental tem-
Bird HR, Oleson JJ, Elvehjem CA, Hart EB. 1937. Relation of grit to the
perature, or unsatisfactory hygiene, can also lead to lesions development of the gizzard lining in chicks. Poultry Sci 16:238-242.
or other indications of impaired health. Brightsmith DJ, Muñoz-Najar RA. 2004. Avian geophagy and soil charac-
Most diseases and conditions are accompanied by dis- teristics in Southeastern Peru. Biotropica 36:534-543.
comfort, distress, and eventually severe pain or even death. Burger J. 2003. Parrot behaviour at a Rio Manu (Peru) clay lick: Temporal
patterns, associations, and antipredator responses. Acta Ethol 6:23-34.
Animal caretakers and research personnel should therefore
Butler CJ. 2005. Feral parrots in the continental United States and United
carefully monitor all individual birds on a regular basis, ac- Kingdom: Past, present and future. J Avian Med Surg 19:142-149.
tively looking for signs of compromised health, which are Cannon CE. 1984. The diet of lorikeets Trichoglossus spp. in the Queensland-
often subtle and go unnoticed to the inattentive eye. These New South Wales border region. Emu 84:16-22.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


precautions are necessary to protect both the animals’ wel- Carpenter JW, Mashima TY, Rupiper DJ. 2001. Exotic Animal Formulary,
2nd ed. Philadelphia: WB Saunders.
fare and the quality of the research data. The experimental
CITES. 1973. Convention on International Trade in Endangered Species of
use of healthy animals may reduce interindividual variabil- Wild Fauna and Flora. Available online (www.cites.org), accessed July
ity and improve the statistical power of experimental 29, 2010.
designs (Garner 2005), thus reducing—or at least not un- CITES. 2009. Appendices I, II, and III. Available online (www.cites.org/
necessarily increasing—the number of animal trials and eng/app/appendices.shtml), accessed July 29, 2010.
animal numbers. Cohen H. 2006. The Animal Welfare Act. J Anim Law 2:13-26.
Collar NJ. 1997. Family Psittacidae (Parrots). In: del Hoyo J, Elliott A,
Sargatal J, eds. Handbook of the Birds in the World, vol 4. Barcelona:
Lynx Editions. p 280-477.
Conclusions Cornejo J, Clubb S. 2005. Analysis of the maintenance diet offered to lories
and lorikeets (Psittaciformes; Loriinae) at Loro Parque Fundacion, Ten-
We have reviewed the current state of knowledge for the erife. Int Zoo Yb 39:85-98.
housing and management of Psittaciformes as laboratory Crosta L, Gerlach H, Bürckle HM, Timossi L. 2002. Endoscopic testicular
animals. Because parrots are long-lived, highly intelligent, biopsy technique in psittaciformes. J Avian Med Surg 16:106-110.
Davis CS. 1991. Parrot psychology and behavior problems. Vet Clin N Am
and largely nondomesticated, their confinement in labora-
Sm Anim Pract 21:1281-1288.
tory settings requires careful thought and long-term planning Davis C. 1999. Basic considerations for avian behavior modification. Semin
as well as consideration of environmental enrichment and Avian Exot Pet 8:183-195.
fear-minimizing strategies. With the exception of a number Diamond J, Bond AB. 1991. Social behaviour and the ontogeny of foraging
of scientifically based studies on inanimate and animate en- in the kea (Nestor notabilis). Ethology 88:128-144.
Diamond J, Eason D, Reid C, Bond AB. 2006. Social play in kakapo (Strig-
richment, research data on species-specific housing guide-
ops habroptilus) with comparisons to kea (Nestor notabilis) and kaka
lines for parrots are quite scarce. Researchers should acquaint (Nestor meridionalis). Behaviour 143:1397-1423.
themselves with a wide variety of aspects of parrot hus- Dilger WC. 1982a. Caging and environment. In: Petrak ML, ed. Diseases of
bandry in order to be sufficiently knowledgeable to make Cage and Aviary Birds, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. p 11-17.
sound decisions about acquisition, husbandry conditions, Dilger WC. 1982b. Behavioral aspects. In: Petrak ML, ed. Diseases of Cage
and Aviary Birds, 2nd ed. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger. p 18-20.
and potential health and welfare issues.
Donoghue S, Stahl S. 1997. Clinical nutrition of companion birds. J Avian
Med Surg 11:228-246.
Acknowledgments Dyck J. 1971a. Structure and spectral reflectance of green and blue feathers of
the rose-faced lovebird (Agapornis roseicollis). Biol Skrifter 18:1-67.
Dyck J. 1971b. Structure and color-production of the blue barbs of Aga-
Aagje Veys and Sofie Merciny are kindly acknowledged for pornis roseicollis and Cotinga maynana. Zeitschr für Zellforsch 115:
their constructive comments. 17-29.
Earle KE, Clarke NR. 1991. The nutrition of the budgerigar (Melopsittacus
undulatus). J Nutr 121:S186-S192.
References Eason DK, Elliott GP, Merton DV, Jansen PW, Harper GA. 2006. Breeding
biology of kakapo Strigops habroptilus on offshore island sanctuaries
Alderton D. 2001. Kooi- en volièrevogels. Utrecht: Veltman Uitgevers. 1990-2002. Notornis 5:27-36.
ARENA [Applied Research and Ethics National Association]/OLAW European Community. 1979. Council Directive 79/409 on the conservation
[Office of Laboratory Animal Welfare]. 2002. Institutional animal care of wild birds. Official Journal of the European Communities. Luxem-
and use committee guidebook, 2nd ed. Bethesda: OLAW. bourg: European Community (OJ L103).
AAFCO [Association of American Feed Control Officials]. 1998. Nutrition European Community. 1986. Council Directive 86/609 on the approxima-
expert panel review: New rules for feeding pet birds. Feed Mgmt tion of laws, regulations and administrative provisions of the member
49:23-25. states regarding the protection of animals used for experimental and
AVMA [American Veterinary Medical Association]. 2002. US Pet Owner- other scientific purposes. Official Journal of the European Communi-
ship and Demographics Sourcebook. Shaumburg: Center for Informa- ties. Luxembourg: European Community (OJ L 358).
tion Management. Evans M. 2001. Environmental enrichment for pet parrots. In Practice
Bateson M, Feenders G. 2010. The use of passerine bird species in labora- 23:596-605.
tory research: Implications of basic biology for husbandry and welfare. Federal Register. 2004. Animal Welfare: Regulations and standards for
ILAR J 51:394-408. birds, rats, and mice. FR 69:31537-31541.
Bavelaar FJ, Beynen AC. 2003. Qualification of commercial feeds for Forshaw JM, Cooper WT. 1978. Parrots of the World, 2nd ed. London: Blan-
parrots. Tijdschr Diergeneesk 128:726-734. ford Press.

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 421


Forshaw JM, Cooper WT. 1989. Parrots of the World, 3rd ed. London: Blan- provision of fruit on food, energy and nutrient intake in two rainbow
ford Press. lorikeet subspecies. Zoo Biol 28:98-106.
Forshaw JM, Cooper WT. 2002. Australian Parrots, 3rd ed. Robina, Queens- Kalmar ID, Veys AC, Geeroms B, Reinschmidt M, Waugh D, Werquin G,
land: Alexander Editions. Janssens GPJ. 2010. Impact of additional fruit and segregation, seed
Fox RA, Millam JR. 2004. The effect of early environment on neophobia in selection and seed dehusking of multi-component seed diets on volun-
orange-winged Amazon parrots. Appl Anim Behav Sci 89:117-129. tary nutrient and energy intake in yellow-shouldered Amazons
Fox RA, Millam JR. 2007. Novelty and individual differences influence (Amazona barbadensis). J Anim Physiol An N, doi: 10.111/j.1439-
neophobia in orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). 0396.2010.01026.x.
Appl Anim Behav Sci 104:107-115. Kim LC, Garner JP, Millam JR. 2009. Preferences of orange-winged Ama-
Garner JP. 2005. Stereotypies and other abnormal repetitive behaviors: Po- zon parrots (Amazona Amazonica) for cage enrichment devices. Appl
tential impact on validity, reliability, and replicability of scientific out- Anim Behav Sci 120:216-223.
comes. ILAR J 46:106-117. Kollias GV. 1995. Diets, feeding practices, and nutritional problems in psit-
Garner JP. 2007. Perseveration and stereotypy: Systems-level insights from tacine birds. Vet Med US 90:29-39.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


clinical psychology. In: Rushen J, Mason G, eds. Stereotypic Animal Koski MA. 2002. Dermatologic diseases in psittacine birds: An investiga-
Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to Welfare. Wallingford: tional approach. Semin Avian Exot Pet 11:105-124.
CABI. p 232-152. Levine BL. 2003. Common disorders of Amazons, Australian parakeets,
Gartrell BD. 2000. The nutritional, morphologic, and physiologic bases of and African grey parrots. Semin Avian Exot Pet 12:125-130.
nectarivory in Australian birds. J Avian Med Surg 14:85-94. Lightfoot T, Nacewicz CL. 2006. Psittacine behavior. In: Bays TB, Lightfoot
Gartrell BD. 2002. Assessment of the reproductive state in male swift par- T, Mayer J, eds. Exotic Pet Behaviour. St. Louis: Saunders. p 51-101.
rots (Lathamus discolour) by testicular aspiration and cytology. J Avian Luescher AU, Wilson L. 2006. Housing and management considerations for
Med Surg 16:211-217. problem prevention. In: Luescher AU, ed. Manual of Parrot Behaviour.
Ghysels P. 1997. Transferring birds to pellet feeding: Successful methods Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p 291-299.
for different species. Proc First Internat Symp Pet Bird Nutrition, Lumeij TJ, Hommers CJ. 2008. Foraging “enrichment” as treatment for
Hannover, October 3-4. p 98. pterotillomania. Appl Anim Behav Sci 111:85-94.
Gilardi JD, Sean SD, Munn CA, Tell LA. 1999. Biochemical functions of Marler P. 1970. A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song develop-
geophagy in parrots: Detoxification of dietary toxins and cytoprotective ment in white-crowned sparrows. J Comp Physiol Psych 71:1-25.
effects. J Chem Ecol 25:897-922. Mason GJ. 1991. Stereotypies: A critical review. Anim Behav 41:1015-1037.
Giret N, Péron F, Nagle L, Kreutzer M, Bovet D. 2009. Spontaneous catego- Mason GJ. 2006. Stereotypic behaviour in captive animals: Fundamentals
rization of vocal imitations in African grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus). and implications for welfare and beyond. In: Mason GJ, Rushen J, eds.
Behav Proc 82:244-248. Stereotypic Animal Behaviour: Fundamentals and Applications to
Gould WJ. 1995. Common digestive tract disorders in pet birds. Vet Med-US Welfare, 2nd ed. Oxfordshire: CABI. p 325-356.
90:40-52. Matsunaga E, Okanoya K. 2009. Evolution and diversity in avian vocal sys-
Greenacre CB. 2003. The avian patient. In: Ballard B, Cheek R, eds. Exotic tem: An evo-devo model from the morphological and behavioral per-
Animal Medicine for the Veterinary Technician. Ames: Blackwell Pub- spectives. Devel Growth Differ 51:355-367.
lishing. p 5-30. McGraw KJ, Nogare MC. 2005. Distribution of unique red feather pigments
Harcourt-Brown NH. 2000. Psittacine birds. In: Tully TN, Lawton MCP, in parrots. Biol Lett 1:38-43.
Dorrestein GM, eds. Avian Medicine. Oxford: Butterworth. p 112-143. McGraw KJ, Hill GE, Navara KJ, Parker RS. 2004. Differential accumula-
Hargis AM. 1989. Avocado (Persea americana) intoxication in caged birds. tion and pigmenting ability of dietary carotenoids in colorful finches.
JAVMA 94:64-66. Physiol Biochem Zool 77:484-491.
Harrison GJ. 1998. Twenty years of progress in pet bird nutrition. JAVMA McGraw KJ, Safron RJ, Wakamatsu K. 2005. How feather colour reflects its
212:1226-1230. melanin content. Funct Ecol 19:816-21.
Hawkins P. 2001. Laboratory birds: Refinements in husbandry and proce- Mee A, Denny R, Fairclough K, Pullan DM, Boyd-Wallis W. 2005. Observa-
dures. Lab Anim 35:S1-S163. tions of parrots at a geophagy site in Bolivia. Biota Neotrop 5:321-324.
Hawley SB. 1997. Nutritional comparison of diets fed to pet birds. Proc Meehan CL, Mench JA. 2002. Environmental enrichment affects the fear
First Internat Symp Pet Bird Nutrition, Hannover, October 3-4. p 55. and exploratory responses to novelty of young Amazon parrots. Appl
Hess SE, Rohr S, Dufour BD, Gaskill BN, Pajor EA, Garner JP. 2008. Anim Behav Sci 79:75-88.
Home improvement: C57BL/6J mice given more naturalistic nesting Meehan C, Mench J. 2006. Captive parrot welfare. In: Manual of Parrot
materials build better nests. JAALAS 47:25-31. Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p 301-317.
Houston D, Mcinnes K, Elliott G, Eason D, Moorhouse R, Cockrem J. Meehan C, Mench J. 2007. The challenge of challenge: Can problem solv-
2007. The use of a nutritional supplement to improve egg production in ing opportunities enhance animal welfare? Appl Anim Behav Sci 102:
the endangered kakapo. Biol Conserv 138:248-255. 246-261.
Howard BR. 1992. Health risks of housing small psittacines in galvanized Meehan CL, Garner JP, Mench JA. 2003a. Isosexual pair housing improves
wiremesh cages. JAVMA 200:1667-1674. the welfare of young Amazon parrots. Appl Anim Behav Sci 81:73-88.
Jamieson BG, Tripepi S. 2005. Ultrastructure of the spermatozoon of Meehan CL, Millam JR, Mench JA. 2003b. Foraging opportunity and in-
Apus apus (Linnaeus 1758), the common swift (Aves; Apodiformes; creased physical complexity both prevent and reduce psychogenic feather
Apodidae), with phylogenetic implications. Acta Zool-Stockholm picking by young Amazon parrots. Appl Anim Behav Sci 80:71-85.
86:239-244. Meehan CL, Garner JP, Mench JA. 2004. Environmental enrichment and
Jones RB. 1997. Fear and distress. In: Appleby MC, Hughes BO, eds. Ani- development of cage stereotypy in Orange-winged Amazon parrots
mal Welfare. New York: CAB International. p 75-87. (Amazona amazonica). Dev Psychobiol 44:209-218.
Kalmar ID, Moons CPH, Meers LL, Janssens GPJ. 2007a. Psittacine birds Mettke-Hofmann C, Winkler H, Leisler B. 2002. The significance of ecological
as laboratory animals: Refinements and assessment of welfare. JAALAS factors for exploration and neophobia in parrots. Ethology 108:249-272.
46:8-15. Moons CPH, Van Wiele P, Ödberg FO. 2004. To enrich or not to enrich:
Kalmar ID, Werquin G, Janssens GPJ. 2007b. Apparent nutrient digestibil- Providing shelter does not complicate handling of laboratory mice.
ity and excreta quality in African grey parrots fed two pelleted diets JAALAS 43:18-21.
based on coarsely or finely ground ingredients. J Anim Physiol An N Moorhouse RJ. 1997. The diet of the North Island kaka (Nestor meriodona-
91:210-216. lis septentrionalis) on Kapiti Island. New Zeal J Ecol 21:141-152.
Kalmar ID, van Loon M, Bürkle M, Reinschmidt M, Waugh D, Werquin G, NRC [National Research Council]. 1996. Guide for the Care and Use of
Janssens GPJ. 2009. Effect of dilution degree of commercial nectar and Laboratory Animals. Washington: National Academy Press.

422 ILAR Journal


NRC. 2006. Guidelines for the Humane Transportation of Research Ani- Shuck-Paim C, Borsari C. 2009. Means to an end: Neotropical parrots
mals. Washington: National Academies Press. manage to pull strings to meet their goals. Anim Cogn 12:287-
Nuboer JF, Coemans MA, Vos JJ. 1992. Artificial lightening in poultry 301.
houses: Do hens perceive the modulation of fluorescent lamps as flicker? Sibley CG, Ahlquist JE. 1990. Phylogeny and Classification of Birds: A
Br Poult Sci 33:123-133. Study in Molecular Evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press.
Ödberg FO. 1978. Abnormal behaviours (stereotypies). Proc First World Silva T. 1991. Psittaculture: The Breeding, Rearing and Management of
Congr Ethol Appl Zootechnics, Madrid. p 475-480. Parrots. Surrey: Birdworld.
Oviatt LA, Millam JR.1997. Breeding behavior of captive orange-winged Snyder NFR, Wiley JW, Kepler CB. 1987. The parrots of Luquillo: Natural
Amazon parrots. Exotic Bird Rep 9:6-7. history and conservation of the Puerto Rican parrot. Los Angeles: West-
Paul E. 2005. A Guide to the Permits and Procedures for Importing Live ern Foundation of Vertebrate Zoology.
Birds into the US for Scientific Research and Display. Washington: Or- Snyder NFR, Koenig SE, Koschmann J, Snyder HA, Johnson TB. 1994.
nithological Council. Thick-billed parrot releases in Arizona. Condor 96:845-862.
Pepperberg IM. 1995. Mirror use by African grey parrots (Psittacus erith- Stanford M. 2006. Effects of UVB radiation on calcium metabolism in psit-

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/ilarjournal/article-abstract/51/4/409/676068 by guest on 17 November 2019


acus). J Comp Psychol 109:182-195. tacine birds. Vet Rec 159:236-241.
Pepperberg IM. 2006. Grey parrot cognition and communication. In: Stoodley AAJ, Hadgkiss IM, Rance LA. 1992. Feeding, housing and breed-
Luescher AU, ed. Manual of Parrot Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell ing. In: Price CJ, ed. Manual of Parrots, Budgerigars, and Other Psittacine
Publishing. p 133-145. Birds. Gloucestershire: British Small Animal Veterinary Association.
Pepperberg IM. 2007. Individual differences in grey parrots (Psittacus p 189-202.
erithacus): Effects of training. J Ornithol 148:S161-S168. Stradi R, Pini E, Celentano G. 2001. The chemical structure of pigments in
Prescott NB, Wathes CM, Jarvis JR. 2003. Light, vision and the welfare of Ara macao plumage. Comp Biochem Physiol B 130:57-63.
poultry. Anim Welf 12:269-288. Tokita M, Kiyoshi T, Armstrong KN. 2007. Evolution of craniofacial nov-
Prum RO, Torres R, Williamson S, Dyck J. 1999. Two-dimensional Fourier elty in parrots through developmental modularity and heterochrony.
analysis of the spongy medullary keratin of structurally coloured feather Evol Dev 9:590-601.
barbs. Proc R Soc Lond B 266:13-22. Turner GV. 1987. Zoonotic diseases. In: Burr EW, ed. Companion Bird
Reddy MB, Love M. 1999. The impact of food processing on the nutritional Medicine. Ames: Iowa State University Press. p 221-225.
quality of vitamins and minerals. In: Jackson LS, Knize MG, Morgan Turner WT. 1992. Handling and clinical examination. In: Price CJ, ed. Man-
JN, eds. Impact of Food Processing on Food Safety. New York: Kluwer ual of Parrots, Budgerigars, and Other Psittacine Birds. Gloucestershire:
Academic/Plenum Publishers. p 99-106. BSAVA. p 7-15.
Romagnano A. 2006. Mate trauma. In: Luescher AU, ed. Manual of Parrot UFAW [Universities Federation for Animal Welfare]. 2010. Handbook
Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p 247-253. on the Care and Management of Laboratory and Other Research
Rowley I. 1997. Family cacatuidae (cockatoos). In: del Hoyo J, Elliott A, Animals, 8th ed. Hubrecht R, Kirkwood J, eds. Oxford: Wiley-
Sargatal J, eds. Handbook of the Birds of the World, vol 4. Barcelona: Blackwell.
Lynx Editions. p 242-279. Ullrey DE, Allen MA, Baer DJ. 1991. Formulated diets versus seed mix-
Rozek JC, Danner LM, Stucky PA, Millam JR. 2010. Over-sized pellets tures for psittacines. J Nutr 121:S193-S265.
naturalize foraging time of captive orange-winged Amazon parrots van Zeeland YRA, Spruit BM, Rodenburg TB, Riedstra B, van Hierden YM,
(Amazona amazonica). Appl Anim Behav Sci 125:80-87. Buitenhuis B, Korte SM, Lumeij JT. 2009. Feather damaging behaviour
Russell WD. 1987. Common cage and aviary birds. In: Burr EW, ed. Com- in parrots: A review with consideration of comparative aspects. Appl
panion Bird Medicine. Ames: Iowa State University Press. p 2-7. Anim Behav Sci 12:75-95.
Russello MA, Avery ML, Wright TF. 2008. Genetic evidence links invasive Webb NV, Famula TR, Millam JR. 2010. The effect of rope color, size and
monk parakeet populations in the United States to international pet fray on environmental enrichment device interaction in male and female
trade. BMC Evol Biol 8:217. orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). Appl Anim Be-
Samour J. 2004. Toxicology. In: Samour J, ed. Avian Medicine. London: hav Sci 124:149-156.
Harcourt Publishers. p 180-192. Welle KR, Luescher AU. 2006. Aggressive behavior in pet birds. In:
Scarl JC, Bradbury JW. 2009. Rapid vocal convergence in an Australian cock- Luescher AU, ed. Manual of Parrot Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell
atoo, the galah Eolophus roseicapillus. Anim Behav 77:1019-1026. Publishing. p 211-218.
Schachner A, Brady TF, Pepperberg IM, Hauser MD. 2009. Spontaneous Werdenich D, Huber L. 2006. A case of quick problem solving in birds:
motor entrainment to music in multiple vocal mimicking species. Curr String pulling in keas, Nestor notabilis. Anim Behav 71:855-863.
Biol 19:831-836. Wilson L. 2005. Biting and screaming behavior in parrots. In: Rupley AE,
Schmidt MF. 2010. An IACUC perspective on songbirds and their use in ed. Veterinary Clinics of North America, Exotic Animal Practice: Avian
neurobiological research. ILAR J 51:424-430. Pet Medicine. Philadelphia: WB Saunders. p 173-179.
Schulte MS, Rupley AE. 2004. Avian care and husbandry. Vet Clin Exot Wilson GH. 2006. Behavior of captive psittacids in the breeding aviary. In:
Anim 7:315-350. Luescher AU, ed. Manual of Parrot Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Pub-
Scott P. 1995. Zoonotic, legal and ethical aspects. In: Harcourt-Brown N, lishing. p 281-289.
Chitty J, eds. BSAVA Manual of Psittacine Birds, 2nd ed. Gloucester: Wilson L. 2007. Psittacine behaviour in the examination room: Practical
British Small Animal Veterinary Association. p 280-287. applications, handling, and restraint. J Exot Pet Med 16:24-29.
Seibert LM. 2006. Social behaviour of psittacine birds. In: Luescher AU, ed. Wolf P, Bayer G, Wendler C, Kamhues J. 1998. Mineral deficiencies in pet
Manual of Parrot Behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing. p 43-47. birds. J Anim Physiol An N 80:140-146.
Shepherdson DJ, Mellen JD, Hutchins M, eds. 1998. Second nature: Envi- Wolf P, Graubohm S, Kamphues J. 2002. Experimental data on feeding ex-
ronmental enrichment for captive animals. Washington: Smithsonian truded diets in parrots. In: Proc Joint Nutrit Symp, Antwerp, August
Institution Press. 21-25. p 137.

Volume 51, Number 4 2010 423

You might also like