You are on page 1of 9

Revista_A5_N2.

qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 29

volumen 2 año 2007

THE ST UDY OF MODAL VERBS FROM A PEDAGOGICAL


PE R SPE C T I VE: AN ANALYSIS OF TEXTBOOKS AND
GRAMMARS 1

Ramiro Durán
Pilar Alonso
María Jesús Sánchez
Universidad de Salamanca

Abstract: This paper shows how the indiscriminate combination of the form-function criterion in the tra-
ditional presentation of modal verbs brings more confusion than light to the subject due mostly to the
fact that the grammatical simplicity of modal verbs clashes with their semantic complexity. In order to
verify the treatment of modals in the EFL classroom, both a reduced but representative sample of textbo-
oks and English grammar books will be analysed. Based on the findings of research conducted in this
field, this article concludes that when studying modals in the EFL classroom the pragmatic uses of modal
verbs should be primed over potential polysemic often indeterminate semantic values and/or grammati-
cal criteria based on a higher or lower rank of graded modality.

Key words: Modal verbs, EFL classroom, grammars, textbooks, semantic values, pragmatic uses.

1. SOME GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS of this double criterion brings confusion to


the subject, for the formal simplicity of

W
hen modals are tackled in modal verbs clashes with the semantic
the EFL classroom follo- complexity of the modality phenomenon.
wing the design of most What is more, most textbooks and grammars
textbooks, they are treated generally opt for a sentential environment or,
as a grammatical category and their form at best, a much-cont rolled discourse cotext
and function are presented on a pair with, to practise questions concerning meaning
let’s say, the form and function of other auxi- and use, thus increasing the feeling of
liaries (e.g., be for the construction of the imprecision during the learning process.
passive voice, or progressive aspect; have This widespread situation gives evidence of
for the construction of perfective aspect, the strong indeterminate grammatical-
etc.). However, the criteria used to classify semantic tie that is at work when modals
and introduce them are not based on their are taught in the classroom and of the com-
grammatical behaviour but on their seman- plications that may derive from this situa-
tic capacity to express notions such as tion. To verify the state of the art in the
possibility, certainty, obligation, permission, treatment of modals in the EFL classroom,
and so on. The indiscriminate combination we will analyse a reduced but representati-
1
Research for this article has been financed by the Ministerio Español de Ciencia y Tecnología, grant BFF2002-01315,
and by the Junta de Castilla y León, grant SA062/02.

Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas | 29


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 30

volumen 2 año 2007

ve sample of coursebooks which will enable ria, as has been said above. In modal verbs
us to show some of the problems that may 1 they consider those forms expressing
derive from the inappropriate unconscious possibility and certainty; in modal verbs 2
use on the part of teachers of undoubtedly they deal with those expressing obligation
valuable pedagogical material. and n e c e s s i t y, permission and prohibition.
Capel and Sharp follow a similar organiza-
tion and present a section entitled modals 1
2. THE ANALYSIS OF TEXTBOOKS where they tackle modals dealing with o b l i-
g a t i o n, necessity and permission and a sec-
All textbooks selected are first certifica- tion entitled modals 2 where they focus on
te level and have been chosen at random modals expressing speculation and deduc-
among a number of books that have been tion. They include under the double label
recently used as textbooks in our first-year speculation and deduction can and might
English Philology classes or that have been for probability, could for deduction and must
considered potentially valuable for use in for certainty. While the values covered in
these classes. In all cases, explanations both books are basically coincident, it
and practice on the use of modals are found seems of interest to illustrate the potential
scattered throughout the grammar sections instability of the semantics of modals that
of these books; they appear together with Capel and Sharp choose to include the
other varied grammatical aspects such as common values of probability andc e r t a i n t y
tenses, passive voice, the use of the article, under the more general denomination of
transitive and intransitive constructions, and speculation and deduction, which may be
the like. However, in the case of modals, seen as an indication of the subjectivity and
this grammatical affiliation is invariably trea- flexibility that is found not only in the actual
ted on the basis of their meaning, as little use of modals in real discourse but also in
mention is made, with very few exceptions, the theoretical basis adopted for their des-
of their grammatical behaviour and formal cription. Capel and Sharp also describe
peculiarities. Some of the books consulted could as expressing deduction, a concept
do not even identify modal verbs as such; that has not been found in any of the other
they turn directly to the semantic values texts analysed.
they represent although this is invariably Contrary to Fried-Booth (1997) and
done in the grammar parts of the units. Capel and Sharp (2000), Haines and Ste-
Out of the six textbooks that have been wart (1996) and O´Neill et al. (1997) never
contrasted, only two of them (Fried-Booth, use the term modal when they discuss
1997 and Capel and Sharp, 2000) consis- these auxiliary verbs; in this sense, they
tently talk about modal verbs in the sections indirectly draw attention away from their
they devote to them. Fried-Booth distribu- grammatical status as a distinctive formal
tes the uses they study into two blocks: category. O´Neill et al. (1997) refer to modals
modal verbs 1 and modal verbs 2, nonethe- merely as forms, sometimes even mixing
less attending to customary semantic crite- them with other subjectively-proned non-

30 | Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 31

volumen 2 año 2007

modal verbal forms such as suggest or sented simply under the heading of p r o b a-
manage. And so, they speak directly of may, bility and possibility with no mention made
should, must and will in chapter 2; should this time of the term modals or of the verbal
have, must have or might have in 14; sug- forms themselves. Furthermore, Haines and
gest doing, suggest that and should do in Stewart associate the notion of probability
15; could, managed to, couldn’t and m u s t in with the idea of near certainty expressed
17; and finally had better, had better not in through must and can, and possibility
18. Haines and Stewart (1996) classify through could, may, and might. If we com-
modals by combining values and forms. For pare this distribution of meaning functions
example, in chapter 9 they talk about sug- with that offered by Capel and Sharp
gestions, a d v i c e, warnings and mention the (2000), where could was associated with
values regrets and criticism on account of deduction, at least one thing is obvious:
the forms should, ought to and could; while there is either clear conflict with the seman-
in chapter 10 they introduce obligation and tic values both books assign to modal forms
necessity in relation to must, have to, need. or, what is more acceptable and likely, the
Spratt and Obee (2001) and Haines and meaning frontiers of modals are far from
Stewart (2004) fuse both approaches. On being as clear-cut and well-differentiated as
the one hand, Spratt and Obee (2001) use students are, rather erroneously, made to
the term modal in two of the three sections believe, when modals are taught in the
they dedicate to these verbs, dividing them classroom, and for perfectly understandable
into more specific sections labelled: ways of pedagogical reasons.
combining modals and past tense of some
modals, where grammatical aspects are
covered. In the third section, where seman- 3. THE ANALYSIS OF GRAMMARS
tic values are focused on, they choose ins-
tead the heading obligation, possibility, The treatment given to modals by those
certainty without any direct reference to the grammars which often serve as a basis for
modal realization of these functions. Haines teachers in their preparation of the class-
and Stewart (2004), on the other hand, use content or which are used to instruct pros-
the tag modal verbs in only one of the seve- pective teachers of English is not very
ral grammar sections where they deal with d i fferent from that found in the textbooks.
d i fferent aspects of modality. They include a Some grammars introduce the study of
section entitled modal verbs of obligation to modals by placing them in a short textual
discuss the uses of don’t need to, needn’t environment (e.g. Murphy and Altman,
have, be allowed to and can (curiously 1989; Downing and Locke, 1992; Bolton
enough, with the exception of can, these and Goodey, 1996) and others work with dif-
forms could hardly be considered central ferent kinds of authentic texts (e.g. Free-
modals). And quite unsystematically, they born, 1987; Thornbury, 1997; 2004) putting
label a second section can, be able to, the emphasis on the actual behaviour of
other ability structures, while a third is pre- modals in discourse. Nevertheless, the majo-

Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas | 31


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 32

volumen 2 año 2007

rity of the grammars consulted stick to sen- tic function. H o w e v e r, there is once more no
tential contexts and cl assify them according general agreement about the sub-catego-
to the double criteria of form and meaning. ries distinguished in this form-function
This is a fact that has varied little over the dichotomy.
years and that applies equally to the more Regarding their grammatical require-
classic grammars (e.g. Thomson and Marti- ments, we can mention, amongst others,
net, 1968; Quirk and Greenbaum, 1973; the following classifications. Thomson and
Close, 1975; Greenbaum and Q uirk, 1990), Martinet (1968) distinguish between modal
the more modern (e.g. Swan and Wa l t e r, auxiliaries (can, could, may, might, must,
1997; Parrot, 2000), and to those written ought, will, would, shall and should) and
specifically for Spanish learners of EFL (e.g. semi-modals (n e e d, dare and used). Free-
Alcaraz and Moody, 1980; Cruz and Trainor, born (1987) talks about modals (the same
1989). A partial exception to the form and verbs as Thomson and Martinet plus u s e d
meaning combination, is Quirk, Greenbaum, to and had better) and semi-auxiliaries
Leech and Svartvik (1985) which perhaps (have to, be to, be about to, be bound to, be
due to its extensiveness and thoroughness going to, etc.) Parrot (2000) divides modals
deals separately with the grammatical into two groups: pure modals (the same
aspects of modal verbs (135-143) and with verbs as Thomson and Martinet except
their semantic values (219-239), although o u g h t, plus n e e d and d a r e) and semi-modal
the examples they produce to illustrate all verbs (ought to, had better, have (got) to
cases are once again exclusively sentences and be able to). As can be seen, there are
devoid of context. In addition, some of the significant variations in the classifications
grammars which are intended specifically used in relation to both the grammatical
for use by university students go further into requirements they follow (even with respect
the theory and discuss the epistemic condi- to the traditional criterion regarding the
tion of modals (e.g. Cruz and Tr a i n o r, 1989; absence or presence of infinitival to), and to
Downing and Locke, 1992). Furthermore, the central or peripheral use of these forms.
Quirk et al. (1985) and Cruz and Tr a i n o r There are also a wide variety of classifi-
(1989) go beyond the confines of form and cations based on semantic criteria. For
de-contextualized meaning and also refer to example, Close (1975) differentiates betwe-
the pragmatic function of modals as vehi- en the primary and the secondary use of
cles for expressing speech acts. modal verbs (except shall), while Green-
The particular behaviour of modal verbs baun and Quirk (1990) establish two kinds
gives rise in the grammars studied to many of uses in every modal: intrinsic modality
d i fferent subdivisions among modals, which (which includes p e r m i s s i o n, obligation or
make their analysis even more complex and volition and involves some intrinsic human
may consequently hinder the teaching-lear- control over events) and extrinsic modality
ning process. In general terms, these subdi- (which includes p o s s i b i l i t y, necessity or p r e-
visions are based on two different criteria: diction and involves human judgement of
their grammatical behaviour and their seman- what is or is not likely to happen). Langac-

32 | Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 33

volumen 2 año 2007

ker (1991) talks about epistemic modality may also be expressed through adverbials,
(its sole import is to indicate the likelihood of thus echoing the tendency in late studies to
a designated process) and root modality reflect the importance that the adverb sate-
(there is additionally some conception of llites of modals, in Hoye’s terms (1997), play
potency directed towards the realization of in the construction of modality. This is a
that process). And using a similar termino- symptom as well of the role that other cotex-
logy, Downing and Locke (1992) also distin- tual components play in the realization of
guish two basic kinds of modal meaning: those subjective aspects of meaning that
epistemic (the speaker comments on the were traditionally associated with modals.
content of the clause or evaluates it: i.e. it A new perspective in the analysis of
may be raining) and non-epistemic (the spe- modal verbs can be found in Thornbury
aker intervenes in the speech event and (1997), where modals, integrated in authen-
brings about changes in events). tic texts, are studied in three different chap-
Along the same line, it is nearly impos- ters: Modality, Futurity and Hypothetical
sible to find any consensus about the impor- meaning and conditionals. Thornbury (1997:
tance of the semantic values attributed to 215) gives valuable information about the
every modal verb when each form is dealt frequency of occurrence of modals in
with in particular. There has been, however, English, highlighting the importance of their
a certain evolution in the quantity and qua- frequency in common use with view to
lity of the uses of the modal verbs studied in assign priorities when teaching this kind of
the grammars over the years as a result verbs. Thornbury, who obtained these data
perhaps of the importance increasingly from the Birmingham Corpus (20 million
given to the communicative dimension of words), ranks the modals in the following
language over a more prescriptive descrip- order:
tion of its normativity. The more traditional
would 44
grammars (e.g. Thomson and Martinet,
can 60
1968; Quirk and Greenba um, 1973; Close, could 71
1975) tend to be much more exhaustive in will 77
their expositions of the semantic values of may 118
modal verbs and so they include complete should 133
lists of central and peripheral modals and a must 141
fully detailed revision of their potential mea- might 155
nings, some of which may be considered shall (does not occur in the first 200
highly infrequent in common use. On the words)
contrary, more recent approaches (e.g. Bol-
ton and Goodey, 1996; Swan and Wa l t e r, The number represents the position
1997) tend to abandon the description of each word occupies in the ranking order of
the less habitual uses and opt for the study frequency. Thus can, which is 60, is the
of the more prototypical ones. Bolton and 60th most frequent word in the corpus.
Goodey stress as well the fact that modality Thornbury (1997) regrets that the corpus

Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas | 33


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 34

volumen 2 año 2007

data does not specify which meanings of dents of English Philology. In order to obtain
each modal are most frequent, though he a network representation of the students’
links the high frequency of occurrence (i.e. conceptual organization of English modal
would) with the extensive range of important verbs, we used the Pathfinder algorithm
meanings they convey. (Schvanevel dt et al., 1985). The Pathfinder
generated a network of relations between
the standard modal forms showing signifi-
4. PEDAGOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS IN cant differences in both groups of subjects
THE ANALYSIS OF MODAL VALUES (cf. Sánchez and Alonso, 2004-5), the main
one being that native speakers organized
In a recent article, Leech has drawn the modal forms according to what could be
attention to the changing status of modals in considered semantic criteria, while the non-
present day use. He says: “According to an native showed a tendency to prioritize for-
exploratory investigation we have underta- mal and grammatical relations over
ken, the English modal auxiliaries as a semantic closeness.
group have been declining significantly in To illustrate the nature of this difference
their frequency of use” (2003: 223), and we may consider how should was treated by
explicitly recommends “to those involved in both groups of subjects. In the native net-
the teaching of English as a second or work, should was directly linked to ought to
foreign language” not to “waste hours of (presumably due to the fact that the two
valuable classroom time teaching s h a l l and forms express weak obligation); but the
ought to” (ibid: 235-236). This appreciation non-native group directly related should to
is in agreement with extensive research we would and established a remarkable distan-
have conducted and whose results have ce between should and ought to. Thus, in
been published elsewhere (Alonso and the non-native network, the connection star-
Sánchez, 2005, Sánchez and Alonso, 2004- ted with would which was linked successi-
2005, Sánchez and Alonso, forthcoming, vely to should—must—have to—had to and
and also Alonso, Sánchez and Durán, forth- then finally to ought to. This organization
coming). As a part of this research, an expe- seemed to obey, first, a grammatical crite-
riment was carried out among native and rion as in formal teaching should and would
non-native speakers of English in order to are often present ed as 1st and 3rd persons
see the degree of agreement they showed of the same auxiliary verb; second, it see-
in the assignment and recognition of med to respond to a graded temporal arran-
semantic modal values to the different gement alternating forms marked for
modal verb forms. present and past, as in the string must-have
All subjects participating in the experi- to-had to-ought to. In this way, aspects rela-
ment were students at the University of ted to the quality, degree and/or intensity of
Salamanca: the group of native speakers obligation expressed by each of these
studied at the University International Pro- modal forms were disregarded by non-nati-
gram; the non-natives were first-year stu- ve speakers. Later researches on the

34 | Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 35

volumen 2 año 2007

Permission: can, could


recognition of modal verbs by native and
Suggestion: might
non-native subjects and on the use of these
Offer: would
forms in their own discourse production pro- Request: would
ved that students of EFL actually had no Advice: had better
problem in the grammatical identification of Volition: would rather
these verbal forms, but encountered serious Promise: should
d i fficulties when they had to deal with their
semantic values; the opposite was found to As can be seen, the list disregards cer-
be true of the native subjects (cf. Alonso tain aspects concerning modal verbs and
and Sánchez, 2005). modality which have been traditionally con-
According to our findings, we have sidered decisive by most grammatical and
grounds to affirm that the unquestionably pedagogic approaches, as is the difference
identifiable semantic values of modals, i.e. between central and peripheral modals.
those around which there seems to be However, we consider the list may provide a
general agreement among native language good and quite realistic starting point to
users and which may therefore be conside- introduce the study of modal values, as it is
red prototypical, are much fewer and conse- rooted in the point of view of actual native
quently more manageable than those listed speakers of the English language and it
in grammars and textbooks, and should the- reflects the present day state of the art, pri-
refore be much easier to teach and to learn. ming regular pragmatic uses over polysemic
The same can be said of the use of modals often indeterminate semantic values and/or
in the actual production of native and non- grammatical criteria based on a higher or
native subjects, also tested in our research lower rank of graded modality. Part of the
project, which proved to be considerably problem learners have to face when dealing
more reduced and specific than could have with modal verbs concerns not only their
been expected in abstract terms (for a detai- ability to know and detect the semantic con-
led study of all these aspects cf. Alonso and tent of modals according to the values
Sánchez, 2005). currently taught, but also the high amount of
Drawing from the data obtained in the indeterminacy and flexibility that these ver-
experiment mentioned above, we are in a bal forms present in their actual pragmatic
position to propose a skeletal but highly use in real discourse samples coupled with
practical list of modal values which contains the degree of subjectivity that this flexibility
all the values that were agreed upon by all brings into the interpretative process (cf.
subjects that took part in the activity and Alonso, Sánchez and Durán, forthcoming).
which were the following: Evidently, the values covered in the list
proposed above are not the only ones which
Obligation: must, have got to, had to modals help to express, and students of
Possibility: may EFL should be made aware of this directly
Certainty: must or indirectly. We believe, however, that this
Probability: should, ought to
should be done by way of an extensive con-
Ability: be able to

Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas | 35


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 36

volumen 2 año 2007

tact with real instances of language which that primary level we find it wise to opt for a
should be presented to students naturally, simplified approach which favours perfor-
inserted into a complete authentic discourse mance over competence. This may help
environment rather than an intensive explo- acquisition and develop a sense of confi-
ration of uses and functions in a reduced dence while a premature exposition to the
sentential context. We agree that at a cer- wide range of meanings that may be cove-
tain stage in the course of the teaching pro- red by modals and to the variations of mea-
cess, it is advisable, even unavoidable, to ning which they may undergo in context
tackle the double question of the form and may result in confusion and prove a hin-
meaning of English modals jointly, but at drance.

REFERENCES

Alcaraz, E. & B. Moody. (1980). Morfosintaxis inglesa para hispanohablantes. Alcoy: Marfil.
Alonso, P. & M. J. Sánchez. (2005). “Estudio contrastivo del uso de los verbos modales
ingleses y de su organización semántica”. Porta Linguarum 3: 95-110.
Alonso, P., M. J. Sánchez, & R. Durán (Forthcoming). “Discourse as a disambiguating tool in
the teaching of modals to EFL students”.
Bolton, D. & N. Goodey. (1996). English Grammar in Steps. London: Richmond Publishing.
Capel A. & W. Sharp. (2000). Objective First Certificate. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Close, R. A. (1975). A Reference Grammar for Students of English. London: Longman.
Cruz, J. de la & P. Trainor. (1989). Gramática inglesa. Madrid: Cátedra.
Downing, A. & P. Locke. (1992). A University Course in English Grammar. New York: Prenti-
ce Hall.
Freeborn, D. (1987). A Course Book in English Grammar. London: MacMillan.
Fried-Booth, D. (1997). Richmond First Certificate Course. London. Richmond Publishing.
Greenbaum, S & R. Quirk. (1990). A Student´s Grammar of the English Language. London:
Longman.
Haines S. & B. Stewart. (1996). New First Certificate Masterclass. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Haines S. & B. Stewart. (2004). First Certificate Masterclass. Oxford: Oxford University
Press.
Hoye, L. (1997). Adverbs and Modality in English. London: Longman.
Langacker, R. (1991). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Vol. 2. Stanford: Stanford Univer-
sity Press.
Leech, G. (2003). “Modality on the move: The English modal auxiliaries 1961-1992”, in R.
Facchinetti, M. Krug & F. Palmer (eds.). Modality in Contemporary English. Berlin:
Mouton de Gruyter.

36 | Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas


Revista_A5_N2.qxd 3/5/07 12:43 Página 37

volumen 2 año 2007

Murphy, R. & R. Altman. (1989). Grammar in use. Reference and Practice for Intermediate
Students of English. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
O´Neill, R., M. DuckWorth, & K. Gude. (1997). New Success at First Certificate. Oxford:
Oxford University Press.
Parrot, M. (2000). Grammar for English Language Teachers. Cambridge: Cambridge Univer-
sity Press.
Quirk, R. & S. Greenbaum. (1973). A University Grammar of English. London: Longman.
Quirk, R., S. Greenbaum , G. Leech, & J. Svartvik. (1985). A Comprehensive Grammar of
the English Language. London: Longman.
Sánchez, M. J. and P. Alonso (2004-2005). “Análisis descriptivo de la organización cognitiva
de los verbos modales ingleses”. Resla, 17-18: 241-255.
Sánchez, M. J. & P. Alonso. (forthcoming). “Los verbos modales ingleses. Organización en
contexto oracional libre y restringido”.
Schvaneveldt, R. W., F. T. Durso, T. E Goldsmith, T. J. Breen, N. M. Cooke, R. G. Tucker, &
J. C. De Maio. (1985). “Measuring the structure of experise”. International Journal of
Man-Machine Studies, 23: 699-728.
Spratt M. & B. Obee. (2001). First Certificate Direct. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.
Swan, M. & C. Walter. (1997). How English Works. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Thomson, A. J. & A. V. Martinet. (1968). A Practical English Grammar. Oxford: Oxford Uni-
versity Press.
Thornbury, S. (1997). About Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thornbury, S. (2004). How to Teach Grammar. London: Longman.

Revista de Lingüística y Lenguas Aplicadas | 37

You might also like