You are on page 1of 14

Essay

Biological; Conservation, and Ethical Implications of


Exploiting and Controlling Wolves
G O R D O N C. HABER
P.O. Box 64, Denali National Park, Alaska 99755, U.S.A.

A b s t r a c t : The w i d e s p r e a d c l a i m t h a t w o l f p o p u l a t i o n s can w i t h s t a n d 2 5 - 5 0 % o r g r e a t e r a n n u a l r e d u c t i o n s
w i t h o u t m a j o r biological c o n s e q u e n c e s is b a s e d p r i m a r i l y o n the o b s e r v a t i o n t h a t p o p u l a t i o n s often m a i n -
tain their size f r o m y e a r to y e a r as h a r v e s t o r control c o n t i n u e s o r recover w i t h i n a f e w y e a r s afterward. This
e m p h a s i s o n n u m e r i c a l s t a t u s o v e r l o o k s the l i k e l i h o o d o f major, lingering i m p a c t s o n the size, n u m b e r , stabil-
ity, a n d p e r s i s t e n c e o f f a m i l y - g r o u p social units, o n reproductive, h u n t i n g , a n d territorial behavior, o n the
role o f l e a r n i n g a n d related traditions, o n w i t h i n - a n d b e t w e e n - g r o u p p a t t e r n s o f g e n e t i c variation, a n d o n
overall m o r t a l i t y rates. The t e n d e n c y o f biologists a n d agencies in n o r t h e r n N o r t h A m e r i c a to p r o m o t e u , o l f
h a r v e s t s t h a t a r e f o u r to eight t i m e s g r e a t e r t h a n u n g u l a t e harvests, in accord w i t h the w o l f versus u n g u l a t e
difference in r e p r o d u c t i v e rates b u t c o n t r a d i c t o r y to a b r o a d a r r a y o f differences in social o r g a n i z a t i o n a n d
related behavior, is r e a s o n e n o u g h to q u e s t i o n the logic o f this p r e v a i l i n g m a n a g e m e n t view. True sustained-
y i e l d m a n a g e m e n t requires m o r e e m p h a s i s o n q u a l i t a t i v e biological f e a t u r e s to d e t e r m i n e the e x t e n t to
w h i c h w o l v e s a n d o t h e r species w i t h e v o l u t i o n a r y histories a s p r e d a t o r s rather t h a n as p r e y s h o u l d be har-
vested. M o s t recent g o v e r n m e n t - s p o n s o r e d w o l f control p r o g r a m s a n d proposals, i n c l u d i n g sterilization, relo-
cation, a n d "redirected" killing, h a v e been b a s e d o n q u e s t i o n a b l e c l a i m s a b o u t u n g u l a t e o r Hvestock p r o b -
l e m s a n d h a v e n o t a d e q u a t e l y c o n s i d e r e d p o t e n t i a l biological costs (especially to the target w o l f p o p u l a t i o n s ) ,
benefits, o r m a n a g e m e n t alternatives. The high s e n t i e n c e o f w o l v e s j u s t i f i e s o v e r l a p p i n g biological-ethical
concerns a b o u t s u c h p r o g r a m s a n d especially a b o u t the heavy, i n d i s c r i m i n a t e , deceptively r e p o r t e d public"
h u n t i n g a n d t r a p p i n g o f w o l v e s t h a t is c u r r e n t l y p e r m i t t e d t h r o u g h o u t m o s t o f A l a s k a ( U . S . A . ) - - i n d u d i n g in
n a t i o n a l p a r k s - - a n d elsewhere.

Implicaciones Biologicas, Conservacionistas y Eticas de la Explotaci6n y Control de Labos


R e s u m e n : La a f i r m a c i 6 n de q u e las p o b l a c i o n e s d e lobos p u e d e n s o p o r t a r r e d u c c i o n e s de 2 5 - 5 0 % o m S s sin
c o n s e c u e n c i a s biol6gicas m a y o r e s se basa p r i n c i p a l m e n t e e n la observaci n d e q u e las p o b l a c i o n e s b a j o ex-
p l o t a c i 6 n o control a m e n u d o m a n t i e n e n s u t a m a ~ o a g o o se r e c u p e r a n p o c o s a ~ o s despu~s. E1 ~nfasis en el
estatus n u m ~ r i c o p a s a p o r alto la p o s i b i l i d a d de i m p a c t o s m a y o r e s sobre el t a m a ~ o , el n ~ m e r o y la estabil-
i d a d d e u n i d a d e s sociales; sobre la c o n d u c t a reproductiva, d e caceria y territorial; sobre el p a p e l del apren-
d i z a j e y tradiciones relacionadas; sobre los p a t r o n e s d e v a r i a c i 6 n gendtica i n t e r e i n t r a g r u p a l e s y sobre las
tasas de m o r t a l i d a d en general. La t e n d e n c i a de bi61ogos y a g e n c i a s en el norte de N o r t e A m d r i c a de pro-
m o v e r sacrificios d e Iobos 4-8 veces m ~ s altas q u e de u n g u l a d o s , de a c u e r d o con las d i f e r e n c i a s de tasa repro-
d u c t i v a s p e r o e n c o n t r a d i c c i 6 n con la alta g a m a de d i f e r e n c i a s e n o r g a n i z a c i 6 n social y c o n d u c t a rela-
cionada, es m o t i v o suficiente p a r a c u e s t i o n a r la lckqica de dsta visi6n p r e v a l e c i e n t e de m a n e j o , el v e r d a d e r o
m a n e j o s u s t e n t a b l e r e q u i e r e d e m a y o r ~nfasis en caracteristicas biol6gicas p a r a d e t e r m i n a r el grad() e n q u e
el lobo y otras especies, con historias e v o l u t i v a s c o m o d e p r e d a d o r e s y n o presas, d e b e n ser cosechados. Los
p r o g r a m a s y p r o p u e s t a s g u b e r n a m e n t a l e s m ~ s recientes p a r a el control del lobo, i n c l u y e n d o esterilizaci6n,
relocaci6n y m u e r t e "redirigida" se b a n b a s a d o en a f i r m a c i o n e s cuestionables acerca de p r o b l e m a s con un-
g u l a d o s o g a n a d o y n o h a n c o n s i d e r a d o a d e c u a d a m e n t e los p o t e n c i a l e s costos biol6gicos ( e s p e c i a l m e n t e
p a r a las p o b l a c i o n e s d e lobos), n i los beneficios o a l t e r n a t i v a s de m a n a j o . La g r a n s e n s i b i l l d a d de los lobos
j u s t i f i c a el t r a n s l a p e de p r e o c u p a c i o n e s biol6gico-dticas acerca de tales p r o g r a m a s e s p e c i a l m e n t e acerca de la

Paper submitted April 27, 1995; revised manuscript accepted January 4, 1996.

1068

ConservationBiology,Pages 1068-1081
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications of Killing Wolves 1069

intensa, indiscrimiinada y enga~osamente reportada caceria y trampeo de lobos que actualmente se permite
en casi todo Alaska (EUA), incluyendo parques nacionales y otras regiones.

Introduction to a b o u t t h e s a m e size. M u c h t h e s a m e s t a n d a r d is ap-


p l i e d u n d e r t h e E n d a n g e r e d Species Act in j u d g i n g
Wildlife scientists a n d m a n a g e r s are f o n d o f calling Aldo w h e t h e r o r n o t w o l v e s a n d o t h e r s p e c i e s are biologically
L e o p o l d (1949) t h e f a t h e r o f wildlife m a n a g e m e n t . How- t h r e a t e n e d o r e n d a n g e r e d . Little m o r e t h a n t h e n u m b e r
ever, m a n y p r o f e s s i o n a l s o v e r l o o k o r i g n o r e o n e o f his o f animals is c o n s i d e r e d , a n d s o m e t i m e s n o t e v e n this
c e n t r a l messages, as e m b o d i e d in o n e o f his m o s t fa- s t a n d a r d is m a i n t a i n e d (Tear et al. 1993).
m o u s q u o t a t i o n s : "A t h i n g is right w h e n it t e n d s to pre- R e c o m m e n d e d h a r v e s t levels for w o l v e s are com-
serve t h e integrity, stability, a n d b e a u t y o f t h e b i o t i c m o n l y four to e i g h t times h i g h e r t h a n for m o s t u n g u l a t e
c o m m u n i t y . It is w r o n g w h e n it t e n d s o t h e r w i s e . " I sug- a n d o t h e r p o p u l a t i o n s s i m p l y b e c a u s e w o l v e s have a
gest L e o p o l d s a w a d i s t i n c t i o n b e t w e e n true sustained- h i g h e r r e p r o d u c t i v e capability. An a p p l i c a t i o n o f this
yield m a n a g e m e n t , in w h i c h wildlife s y s t e m s are har- thinking, w i t h r e g a r d to t h e a l l e g e d relative biological
v e s t e d o n l y selectively in a w a y that tries to e n s u r e t h e i r costs o f w o l f c o n t r o l versus b e a r control, is illustrated in
u n d e r l y i n g structural a n d f u n c t i o n a l integrity, a n d w h a t a r e c e n t w o l f c o n t r o l p r o p o s a l f r o m Alaska (Alaska De-
I w o u l d refer to as "farming" o f t h e s e systems. p a r t m e n t o f Fish and G a m e 1995a: 2, 1995b: 8):
T w o basic t e n e t s o f t h e p r e v a i l i n g f a r m i n g a p p r o a c h • . . unlike w o l v e s , b e a r s h a v e v e r y l o w r e p r o d u c t i v e
are that (1) virtually all m a j o r wildlife p o p u l a t i o n s / s p e - rates, m a k i n g t h e m v u l n e r a b l e to o v e r h a r v e s t and m u c h
s l o w e r to recover• Thus, w e s h o u l d r e d u c e b e a r preda-
cies c a n a n d s h o u l d b e h a r v e s t e d , a n d (2) m a n y o f t h e m
tion only if d e c r e a s i n g p r e d a t i o n by w o l v e s d o e s not in-
c a n b e h a r v e s t e d to t h e i r full r e p r o d u c t i v e p o t e n t i a l (to c r e a s e calf survival.
t h e limit o f n e t a n n u a l i n c r e m e n t s for u n g u l a t e s a n d cer-
tain o t h e r s ) o n a m o r e o r less c o n t i n u i n g basis. In Alaska (U.S.A.), w o l v e s c a n b e h u n t e d a n d t r a p p e d
In this p a p e r , I e x a m i n e o n e a p p l i c a t i o n o f this think- for almost 9 m o n t h s a y e a r - - 1 0 A u g u s t - 3 0 April in m o s t
ing w i t h r e g a r d to t h e m a n a g e m e n t o f w o l v e s (Canis lu- areas, i n c l u d i n g w i t h i n m o s t o f t h e national p a r k areas
pus). I question the prevailing way impacts of harvesting a n d national wildlife refuges (Alaska Board o f G a m e
( h u n t i n g / t r a p p i n g ) a n d c o n t r o l r e d u c t i o n s are e v a l u a t e d 1995; Federal Subsistence Board 1995; H a b e r 1992,
a n d suggest that m u c h m o r e e m p h a s i s s h o u l d b e given 1995a). W o l f p e l t s are c o n s i d e r e d almost w o r t h l e s s until
to t h e qualitative f e a t u r e s o f w o l f biology. I t h e n pro- late fall o r w i n t e r , a n d f e w if any h u n t e r s eat w o l f meat;
p o s e a g e n e r a l f r a m e w o r k for d e t e r m i n i n g t h e relative n e v e r t h e l e s s b o t h t h e s p o r t and "subsistence" w o l f hunt-
e x t e n t to w h i c h w o l v e s a n d o t h e r s p e c i e s s h o u l d b e har- ing seasons b e g i n o n 10 August to c o i n c i d e w i t h t h e
vested, b a s e d o n t h e i r e v o l u t i o n a r y histories and social- o p e n i n g o f t h e c a r i b o u a n d s h e e p h u n t i n g seasons to
ity, a n d for a d a p t i n g h a r v e s t p o l i c i e s to o t h e r natural pat- m a x i m i z e t h e p o t e n t i a l kill o f w o l v e s via i n c i d e n t a l en-
terns a n d p r o c e s s e s . I e n d w i t h s o m e t h o u g h t s a b o u t t h e c o u n t e r s . A n n u a l h u n t i n g limits g e n e r a l l y range f r o m 5
direction of w o l f conservation ~md the ethics of w o l f killing. to 15 w o l v e s p e r h u n t e r . Some areas, i n c l u d i n g a b o u t
half o f t h e Arctic National Wildlife Refuge, have n o hunt-
ing limit. T h e t r a p p i n g s e a s o n e x t e n d s f r o m 15 O c t o b e r
Biological Impacts and Wolf Social Organization o r 1 N o v e m b e r t h r o u g h March o r April. T h e r e is n o limit
to t h e n u m b e r o f w o l v e s that m a y b e t a k e n w i t h a trap-
W o l f biologists a n d wildlife a g e n c i e s regularly a s s u m e p i n g license, w h i c h allows m a n y m e t h o d s o f killing (ac-
n o r t h e r n w o l f p o p u l a t i o n s can b e h a r v e s t e d at o n g o i n g tual t r a p p i n g is n o t r e q u i r e d e x c e p t in p a r k areas), in-
a n n u m rates o f u p to 25-50%; o r c a n w i t h s t a n d shorter- c l u d i n g w i t h t h e use o f s e m i - a u t o m a t i c assault rifles,
t e r m c o n t r o l p r o g r a m s o f u p to 8 0 - 9 0 % w i t h o u t signifi- airplanes, a n d s n o w m a c h i n e s . Saturation snaring is a
c a n t b i o l o g i c a l i m p a c t (Elliot 1982; Keith 1983; P e t e r s o n c o m m o n t r a p p i n g m e t h o d : w i r e snares are set virtually
et al. 1984; Ballard et al. 1987, 1991; Alaska D e p a r t m e n t in walls, b y t h e d o z e n s p e r site, at n u m e r o u s brushy,
o f Fish a n d G a m e 1989, 1991a, 1991b, 1992a, 1992b, b a i t e d sites a l o n g well-used t r a v e l routes.
1992c, 1995c; M e c h 1970, 1991, 1994; G a s a w a y et al. T h e r e are no restrictions, i n c l u d i n g in m o s t o f t h e na-
1992; Boertje et al. 1995; J. M o r e h e a d l e t t e r 10 J a n u a r y tional p a r k areas, against killing adult w o l v e s w i t h de-
1992 to D. K e l l e y h o u s e , National Park Service files, An- p e n d e n t y o u n g o r t h e y o u n g themselves. At t h e s e lati-
chorage). t u d e s w o l f p u p s ar c usually c o m p l e t e l y d e p e n d e n t o n
"Biological i m p a c t " is typically d e f i n e d solely in t e r m s t h e c a r e p r o v i d e d b , adults t h r o u g h at least late Septem-
o f n u m e r i c a l status: T h e i m p a c t is c o n s i d e r e d negligible b e r o r early O c t o b e r (the e n d o f t h e h o m e s i t e p e r i o d ) ,
if t h e w o l f p o p u l a t i o n e i t h e r maintains o r s o o n r e c o v e r s a n d typically for at least several m o n t h s after that ( H a b e r
I

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
1070 Implications of Killing Wolves Haber

1977). Young are not allowed to hunt or travel regularly havior that is unusual even for social mammals. Commu-
with the adults until the end of the homesite period. nal nonparental and parental provisioning of the young
Thus, when adults are killed--especially from a small in multiple as well as single litters at the same and differ-
family group or a pair with a relatively large litter--the ent homesites is common. There are complex divisions
result can be much the same as if the pups were killed of labor and other sophisticated forms of cooperation,
directly. including extraordinary cooperation during hunting, with
More than 1600 wolves, equal to nearly all of the wolves elaborate spatial coordination between individuals some-
in Minnesota, were killed in Alaska during the 1993- times located (in visual contact) miles apart from each
1994 reporting period. The annual kill typically ranges other (Haber 1977). Between well-established groups there
from at least 800 to 1200 and averages about 1000 is extreme hostility, and intense inbreeding appears to
(Alaska Department of Fish and Game 1995c). The state- be routine under natural conditions, resulting in healthy,
wide population of wolves has been variously estimated sometimes exceptionally large litters of young with high
by the Alaska Department of Fish and Game at 5000- survival rates (Haber 1977; Peterson 1977; Allen 1979).
10,000 since 1992 (Peterson 1995; Alaska Department For long intervals, when my primary Denali study
of Fish and Game 1995c) but I estimate that it is 15-40% groups did not suffer any significant hunting/trapping
lower based on my aerial surveys in several of the Alaska losses, there was impressive social stability and continu-
Department of Fish and Game's highest priority count- ity. For example, in one group (Toklat/East Fork) the
ing areas (Game Management Units 13, 20A, 20B, 20C, same female maintained her position as alpha-female for
20D, 20E; Haber 1993b, 1994, 1995b, Haber letter 26 13-14 years, until she died naturally at about 18 years
October 1995 to S. Martin, Denali National Park files, old. Murie (1944 and personal communication) ob-
Haber letters 23 January and 28 February 1996 to F. Rue, served similar indications of social stability and continu-
Alaska Dept. of Fish and Game files; and unpublished ity in this group from 1939 to 1966. Our intensive
data). Included in my counts were large regions where it ground and aerial observations of identifiable individuals
has been possible since 1993 for the Maska Department indicate that this same family lineage has persisted for at
of Fish and Game or a cooperating agency and me to least 56 years, and Murie (personal communication) felt
monitor most of the same resident wolf groups via aerial it probably extended to at least 8-10 years earlier.
radio tracking (in 20D, 20E, and the southern half of 20C). The adjacent Savage family (Haber 1977, 1987, 1992)
To understand the impact of heavy, ongoing public was well established when I began observing it inten-
killing and shorter-term government control on wolf sively in 1966. It persisted with relatively little social
populations and why, in general, this makes little biolog- change until the winter of 1982/1983, when (based on
ical sense, requires an understanding of wolf social orga- strong circumstantial evidence) it was eliminated by
nization. Social behavior is strongly influenced by inher- hunting/trapping. The Headquarters family colonized
itance (Plomin 1990; Bouchard et al. 1990; Kelner and this territorial vacancy in 1984. It persisted until May
Benditt 1994) and is therefore very much within the 1995, when the last wolf, the alpha-female, was snared.
realm of biology. Hunting and trapping began taking a major toll on this
I have spent more than 11,000 hours observing wolves group in the winter of 1993/1994 (Haber letter 30 May
during 30 years of field research in Alaska. This research 1995 to Barbee/Martin, Denali National Park files). The
(e.g., Haber 1977, 1992, 1993b, 1994, 1995b) includes Headquarters alpha-female had maintained her alpha sta-
14 established groups of wolves observed via ground tus for at least 6-7 years, and remained mated to the
and aerial observation in Denali National Park since same (alpha) male until hc was killed during National
1966. I have observed 11 of these groups with aerial and Park Service helicopter darting activities in November
ground radio tracking. I have observed 37 groups, 31 of 1993. A new ("Sanctuary") family--two adults and two
these via aerial radio tracking, in other regions extending of three pups they produced in May 1995--has n o w col-
eastward from Denali to the Yukon border since 1993. I onized this area.
have also undertaken woff research and reviewed wolf Likewise, the Cottonwood family group of Yukon-
management programs in northern British Columbia and Charley Rivers National Preserve has maintained a late
the Yukon Territory (Habcr 1979, 1988, 1993a). winter size of 12-14 wolves (6-7 new pups annually),
All the groups I have studied intensively (with the ex- stable social relations among high ranking, radio col-
ception of one or two non-reproducing pairs) were, or lared and other identifiable individuals, the same large
soon became, family groups, in most cases extended territory and natal sites, and the same basic pattern of
families. Murie (1944), Peterson (1977), Mlen (1979), coat colors since at least 1992, w h e n this group was first
Packard (1980), Harrington and Mech (1982), and Mess- radio-collared (Haber 1994, 1995b, unreported 1995-
ier (1985) similarly concluded that the groups they stud- 1996 data).
ied were primarily families, especially extended families. Unlike most other eusocial species, wolves feature
These families were characterized by year-round integra- one of the hallmarks of advanced vertebrate societies:
tion between the sexes and among adult age classes, be- prolonged dependency of the young, for up to 25-30%

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications of Killing Wolves 1071

o f t h e i r n o r m a l lifespan ( H a b e r 1977). This is l o n g e r c o u l d h a r d l y b e e x p e c t e d n o t to r e d u c e a n d f r a g m e n t its


t h a n in h u m a n societies. T o g e t h e r w i t h t h e i r sophisti- sociality a n d e v e n t u a l l y p r o d u c e less selectivity in o t h e r
c a t e d l e a r n i n g abilities, p r o l o n g e d d e p e n d e n c y p r o v i d e s i m p o r t a n t a s p e c t s o f behavior, as P e t e r s o n et al. (1984)
t h e basis for m a j o r social as w e l l as g e n e t i c transfer o f in- also c o n c l u d e d . Similarly, it is difficult to i m a g i n e that
f o r m a t i o n b e t w e e n g e n e r a t i o n s . [Eusocial s p e c i e s fea- s u c h killing w o u l d n o t a d d greatly to t h e natural varia-
t u r e g r o u p s o r c o l o n i e s in w h i c h at least t w o genera- tions in t e r r i t o r y b o u n d a r i e s a n d overall territorial mosa-
tions live t o g e t h e r , r e p r o d u c t i o n is r e s t r i c t e d to a f e w ics a n d that it w o u l d n o t simplify h o m e s i t e traditions
individuals, a n d n o n b r e e d e r s c o o p e r a t e to c a r e for t h e w i t h i n t h e s e territories.
offspring o f b r e e d e r s ( S h e r m a n et al. 1991, 1992)]. In G r e a t e r variability in m o o s e kill rates a n d t e r r i t o r y
short, wolves, along w i t h o n l y a f e w o t h e r species, dis- s i z e - g r o u p size r e l a t i o n s h i p s is p r o b a b l y also e x p l a i n e d
p l a y a rare f o r m o f sociality that r e p r e s e n t s a p i n n a c l e o f in large m e a s u r e b y social c h a n g e s a n d b y t h e i n c r e a s e d
v e r t e b r a t e social d e v e l o p m e n t . availability o f t e m p o r a r y v a c a n c i e s w i t h i n territorial mo-
It is difficult to dissect t h e i m p a c t s o f w o l f exploita- saics, w h i c h allows for m o r e e x p a n s i o n o f b o t h surviv-
tion b e c a u s e d e t a i l e d c o m p a r a t i v e i n f o r m a t i o n o n be- ing a n d n e w territories. U n d e r natural c o n d i t i o n s at
havior from both exploited and protected wolf popula- g r o u p sizes o f s e v e n to n i n e o r m o r e in Denali National
tions is scarce. N e v e r t h e l e s s , s o m e o f t h e k n o w n o r Park ( H a b e r 1977), t h e highest-ranking family m e m b e r s
likely effects o f r e m o v i n g 15-20% o r m o r e o f a p o p u l a - w e r e g e n e r a l l y t h e m o s t assertive a n d a p p e a r e d to eat
tion annually are s u m m a r i z e d as follows (see also A p p e n - the m o s t o r at least s h o w e d t h e least variation in con-
d i x 1). C o m p a r i s o n s are r e s t r i c t e d p r i m a r i l y to similar s u m p t i o n f r o m o n e m o o s e kill to t h e next, e v e n t h o u g h
s y s t e m s in Alaska a n d n o r t h w e s t e r n C a n a d a in w h i c h t h e entire g r o u p typically c r o w d e d a r o u n d e a c h kill to-
m o o s e are available as p r e y , to m i n i m i z e t h e i n f l u e n c e o f gether. O t h e r family m e m b e r s usually f o l l o w e d w h e n
o t h e r variables (e.g., m a j o r d i f f e r e n c e s in p r e y t y p e s c a n t h e high-ranking w o l v e s b e g a n a n e w hunt. T h e r e w a s
greatly i n f l u e n c e t h e d e g r e e o f sociality [ B o w e n 1981 ]): relatively little variation in t h e r e f r a c t o r y p e r i o d o f t h e
high-ranking wolves. T h e r e f o r e , s u c c e s s i v e h u n t s b e g a n
(1) A v e r a g e late-winter g r o u p sizes d e c r e a s e a n d t h e after relatively c o n s t a n t intervals for e a c h p r e y type, a n d
n u m b e r o f r e s i d e n t singles a n d g r o u p s c o m b i n e d kill rates r e m a i n e d fairly c o n s t a n t r e g a r d l e s s o f variations
in t h e p o p u l a t i o n i n c r e a s e s o r r e m a i n s relatively in (large) g r o u p sizes at t h e s a m e p r e y densities.
h i g h e v e n at r e d u c e d area-wide p o p u l a t i o n densi- U n d e r c o n d i t i o n s o f m o d e r a t e to h e a v y e x p l o i t a t i o n ,
ties (i.e., m o r e p o p u l a t i o n f r a g m e n t a t i o n ) at least w i t h f r e q u e n t r e p l a c e m e n t o f k e y individuals, it w o u l d
until t h e h i g h e s t h a r v e s t o r c o n t r o l intensities are b e difficult for w o l v e s to maintain stable, w e l l - d e f i n e d
reached. d o m i n a n c e r e l a t i o n s h i p s s u c h as I o b s e r v e d in well-
(2) Mating is less selective. T h e r e are m o r e litters p e r e s t a b l i s h e d Denali family groups. T h e r e w o u l d likely b e
p o p u l a t i o n ( m o r e a l p h a pairs c r e a t e d ) a n d h i g h e r m o r e overall variation in m o s t relationships, d o m i n a n c e
mid- to l a t e - w i n t e r p u p ratios, f o l l o w e d b y a s h a r p a n d o t h e r w i s e . Territorial b e h a v i o r a n d h u n t i n g w o u l d
d e c r e a s e at t h e h i g h e s t h a r v e s t o r c o n t r o l intensi- b e d e t e r m i n e d m o r e b y i n t e r a c t i o n s o f various w o l v e s
ties in p a r t b e c a u s e o f p a i r b o n d i n g difficulties. t h a n b y t h e b e h a v i o r o f t h e s a m e c o r e o f high-ranking
(3) M o o s e kill rates are m o r e variable, s o m e t i m e s in- g r o u p m e m b e r s . C o m p e t i t i o n w o u l d b e less restrained,
c r e a s i n g as a f u n c t i o n o f g r o u p size o v e r a m u c h a n d w h e n g r o u p size c o n t i n u e d to i n c r e a s e t h e r e w o u l d
h i g h e r ( t w o to t h r e e t i m e s ) r a n g e o f g r o u p sizes, p r o b a b l y b e less o f a d i f f e r e n c e in c o n s u m p t i o n rates
a n d b e c o m i n g m o r e erratic at t h e smallest sizes a m o n g adults, l o w e r average per-adult rates o f c o n s u m p -
(e.g., w o l f pairs s o m e t i m e s kill as o f t e n as g r o u p s tion p e r m o o s e kill, a n d s h o r t e r r e f r a c t o r y p e r i o d s lead-
o f six o r seven). ing to m o r e t i m e s p e n t hunting, a c o n t i n u i n g i n c r e a s e in
(4) T e r r i t o r i e s a n d h o m e s i t e p a t t e r n s o f use are m o r e kill rates, a n d p r o b a b l y m o r e o f a t e n d e n c y for t h e g r o u p
variable, w i t h r e p l a c e m e n t b y substantially differ- to try to e x p a n d its territory.
e n t territorial m o s a i c s at t h e h i g h e s t harvest o r A d d i t i o n a l kill rate d i f f e r e n c e s r e p o r t e d for e x p l o i t e d
c o n t r o l intensities. p o p u l a t i o n s m i g h t involve m o r e s u b t l e social o r o t h e r
(5) T h e r e is a m o r e d i r e c t r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n terri- t y p e s o f distortions. Hayes et al. (1991) s u g g e s t e d that
t o r y size a n d r e s i d e n t w o l f g r o u p size (versus terri- t h e s h a r p i n c r e a s e in kill rates t h e y o b s e r v e d for g r o u p s
t o r y size a n d p r e y availability), s o m e t i m e s w i t h an r e d u c e d to pairs f o l l o w i n g h e a v y c o n t r o l w a s d u e to t w o
i n v e r s e r e l a t i o n s h i p d u r i n g initial c o l o n i z a t i o n o f factors: (1) T h e unusually large territories that coloniz-
large vacancies. ing pairs w e r e able t o o c c u p y initially p r o v i d e d t h e m
(6) Overall annual, natural m o r t a l i t y rates ( e x c l u d i n g w i t h an i n c r e a s e d s e l e c t i o n o f p o t e n t i a l l y v u l n e r a b l e
t h e w o l v e s killed via h a r v e s t o r c o n t r o l ) increase. m o o s e . (2) H i g h e r per-kill losses to scavengers, espe-
cially ravens, p r o m p t e ~ t h e m to s p e n d m o r e t i m e hunt-
T h e first t w o i m p a c t s are n o t surprising; h e a v y indis- ing. Heavy b r o a d c a s t h~rvest o r c o n t r o l i m p l i e s a g r e a t e r
c r i m i n a t e h a r v e s t o r c o n t r o l o f a h i g h l y social s p e c i e s l i k e l i h o o d that pairs can settle into an area a n d h u n t siz-

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
1072 Implications of Killing Wolves Haber

able territories with less risk of attack from nearby, Even under natural conditions there is significant dis-
much larger, established groups. ruption of family groups, n e w group formation, and
Other likely impacts of harvest and control include much dispersal usually ending in mortality (Haber 1977;
disruption of learning, increased population-wide mix- Mech 1977). However, the natural area-wide pattern is
ing, and different within- and between-group genetic likely to be one of larger, well-established, genetically
pattems, all of which would be difficult to identify with distinct family groups in prime prey areas with smaller,
the sketchy comparative information available. How- less-stable groups in surrounding or nearby marginal
ever, these impacts might be among the most important. areas, rather than of almost exclusively stable or unsta-
Disruption in the flow of learning from generation to ble groups, as Meier et al. (1995) have portrayed the
generation would result in fewer, simpler learned behav- choices. Evidence for this kind of mixed mosaic of estab-
ioral traditions and in general a diminished role of these lished and satellite or other unstable groups, in which
behaviors (Haber 1977, 1979, unpublished data) that many of the latter colonize nearby areas by "budding"
help adapt individual family groups to the specific re- from the former and continue to reassociate with them
sources and other unique features of each area. periodically at least for the short term, can be seen in
Wolves commonly live 7-10 years or more in well- both Meier et al.'s (1995) and my (Haber 1977; unpub-
established family groups subjected to little or no hunting lished) data for Denali and for the upper Tanana-For-
and trapping in Denali National Park. As indicated, one tymile-Yukon-Charley region of Alaska (Haber 1994,
alpha-female lived to about 18 years old. In contrast, few 1995b, unpublished).
wolves live more than 5-7 years in exploited populations The problem comes in separating out the effects of re-
(Stephenson & Sexton 1974; Hayes et al. 1991). Hayes et al. cent exploitation from the effects of natural instability
for example, found that 77% of all adults were only 2-4 and turnover, such as could b e anticipated for newly
years old, only 9% w e r e / > 5 years old, and only 3% were formed groups in marginal prey areas (e.g., Wonder
/>7 years old. With such a high rate of turnover and Lake family of Denali; Haber 1977). With heavy exploita-
young age structure, there is much less opportunity for tion, especially ongoing harvest, there is bound to be
wolves in these populations to accrue and transfer informa- much less opportunity for well-established groups to
tion from one generation to the next via the prolonged de- persist in relative isolation from each other. It is this dis-
pendency period and complex learning sequences impor- tinction that sets the stage for most of the other behav-
tant to wolves under natural conditions (Haber 1977). ioral and genetic changes I discuss here.
Fewer large, well-established family groups implies Sometimes it is claimed that heavy killing leads to the
less intergroup hostility and more population-wide mixing renewal of wolf populations, with the implication that
and thus, almost certainly, some fundamental changes in this constitutes positive biological change. Such claims
genetic patterns. Under natural conditions, at least are questionable enough when applied to short-term con-
where moose are important prey for mainland wolf pop- trol programs, but they are almost meaningless when ap-
ulations, the available behavioral evidence points to plied to the heavy, much more widespread ongoing
more overall genetic variation between groups and less forms of exploitation via public hunting and trapping
within groups than some researchers have suggested is that predominate in the north, especially in Alaska. For
present based on blood and tissue samples taken primar- example, of the more than 1600 wolves killed in Alaska
ily from exploited populations (Brewster and Fritts during the August 1993-April 1994 reporting period,
1992) or from populations, including Denali, where un- only 98 were killed via government control. It is impor-
derlying genetic patterns may not have hilly recovered tant to understand h o w frequent the "renewals" and
from earlier years and decades of exploitation in certain h o w short-lived the intervening "recoveries" of wolf
areas (Lehman et al. 1992; Meier et al. 1995). Observa- populations subject to heavy, ongoing exploitation are
tions in Denali (Murie 1944; Haber 1977) of eusocial be- likely to be. In Alaska it is only from the 30 April closing
h a v i o r - i n t e n s e inbreeding without obvious problems, of one hunting/trapping season to the 10 August open-
extreme intergroup hostility, low rates of alien accep- ing of the next that most populations are protected. Bi-
tance, and histories of distinct coat color differences for ologists and managers w h o dismiss concerns about the
well established groups during long intervals of minimal impacts of heavy killing on the qualitative aspects of
human disruption--strongly suggest that normally the wolf biology have not addressed this critical difference
wolves of these groups share a high proportion of their between formal control and ongoing exploitation, with
genes, that there are major between-group genetic dif- regard to the duration and extent of these impacts.
ferences, and that kin selection plays an important role Note the indications (Appendix 1) of lingering higher
in the development of adaptive traits in such popula- overall natural mortality rates for wolves that survive
tions. These observations are consistent with Meier et harvest and control, w h e n compared to populations
al.'s (1995) observation from this population that be- where there has been little or no recent harvest or con-
tween-group genetic relatedness was lowest among trol. This would appear to be a predictable result of the
"longer-established" groups. increased fragmentation, higher turnover, and greater

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, Nu. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications of Killing Wolves 1073

overall social disruption that accompanies heavy killing. yearlings), would be relocated, which implies both indi-
This by itself provides a warning that the biological im- vidual and group impacts.
portance of a sophisticated, natural social structure is
being greatly underrated. It may represent a simple
quantitative indication of the impact of human killing on An Evolutionary Perspective
a range of qualitative features of wolf biology.
Sterilization and other forms Of fertility control, "redi- Wolf social behavior is remarkably adaptable, but the
rected killing," and relocation of juveniles are emerging adaptations are primarily for cooperative hunting, not
as n e w approaches to wolf control in Alaska and the defense against sustained, heavy predation. It does not
Yukon Territory. Control advocates are again arguing follow that wolves will be able to survive heavy exploita-
that there will be little more than temporary numerical tion and control just because they have held their own
reductions without significant biological costs. A cur- numerically against heavy killing for the past 40-50
rent wolf sterilization-trapping-relocation proposal for years in places like Alaska. A few decades of heavy kill-
the Fortymile region of Alaska (Alaska Department of ing amount to the blink of an eye compared with the far
Fish and Game 1995a; 1995b) is illustrative. At least 13 longer period of evolutionary time over which wolves
family groups of wolves (Alaska Department of Fish and have evolved in response to the opposite pressures.
Game 1995b: 8,13) would be reduced to alpha pairs if Modern wolves have been present for at least 1-2 mil-
possible, via trapping, snaring, and relocation. The alpha lion years (Mech 1970) and for at least 500,000 years in
pairs would be spared to maintain existing territories in Alaska.
order to prevent other wolves from moving into the re- The relationship between total population size (i.e.,
gion. Then, up to 30 males and/or 15 females would be numerical status) and the integrity of c o m p o n e n t social
sterilized. Potential biological impacts are described as systems appears to be subtle and nonlinear, just as we
follows: are n o w finding to be the case for many natural relation-
ships. For example, a social breakdown can lead in the
Sterilization is a m i n o r s u r g i c a l p r o c e d u r e c a u s i n g lim-
ited t r a u m a . C u r r e n t r e s e a r c h i n d i c a t e s t h e r e is n o short term to more successful matings in the population
c h a n g e in w o l f b e h a v i o r . . . . W i t h i n 10 years, t h e w o l f and thus relatively stable or even higher total numbers
p o p u l a t i o n s h o u l d b e b a c k to c u r r e n t levels a n d s h o u l d (Appendix 1). Nevertheless, it is foolish to ignore the
c o n t i n u e to i n c r e a s e . . . . Local tr~appers c o u l d assist this
p l a n b y s h i f t i n g t h e i r efforts to w o l v e s w h o s e t e r r i t o r i e s possibility that after some further lag, and lags are common
encompass the [caribou] calving and summer range, in nature, there will finally come a dramatic collapse in
w h e r e little o r n o t r a p p i n g c u r r e n t l y o c c u r s . T h e area- total ntLmbers. Serious problems with social organization
w i d e [wolf] h a r v e s t in t h e F o r t y m i l e is n o t e x p e c t e d to
i n c r e a s e s i n c e t r a p p e r s will b e shifting t h e i r efforts and other important qualitative biological features must
[ f r o m p e r i p h e r a l areas], n o t i n c r e a s i n g t h e m . . . . Dis- ultimately translate into a major, long-term decline in
p e r s a l o f y o u n g w o l v e s is c o m m o n a n d r e l o c a t i o n s numbers, but the linkages are likely too indirect to rely on
w o u l d m i m i c this b e h a v i o r . (Alaska D e p a r t m e n t o f Fish
a n d G a m e 1 9 9 5 a : 1,4; 1 9 9 5 b : 7, 8). the numerical signal for a warning before the underlying
qualitative problems become impossible to reverse.
Given the unusual fanlily-based social structure of In population viability simulations based on informa-
wolves, it is simplistic to imply that reducing these com- tion from the Isle Royale wolf population, including so-
plex societies to sterile pairs will not have significant be- cial structure, Vucetich and Peterson (1995, personal
havioral or other biological consequences. The 5- to lO- communication) found that mean time to extinction was
year distortion in age structures and disruption in the independent of population size for all populations above
flow of genetic and cultural information alone imply a a threshold of only eight wolves. The number of social
likelihood of important changes. units was of much greater importance than total num-
The claim that there would be no increased trapping bers in predicting the population's viability.
impacts because trappers would shift their efforts from It is questionable as to whether a normally Ultra-social
the wolves they are already exploiting in peripheral species "survives" if its social organization is continually
areas to lightly or untrapped wolves again illustrates the shredded by heavy exploitation. Heavy, ongoing exploi-
assumption that little more than area-wide numbers are tation implies a high potential for eventually reducing
important biologically. Similarly, relocation of juvenile these complex societies to much simpler, more primi-
wolves might mimic natural dispersal to some extent, in- tive forms, particularly w h e n it is so likely to scramble
cluding in its most c o m m o n outcome for the dispersers, their unusual genetic and cultural information transfer
mortality. However, many juveniles of this and nearby processes. Nonlethal forms of control, such as steriliza-
regions do not disperse. They remain in their family tion and relocation, could easily end up producing the
groups through adulthood and ultimately contribute in same or similar results.
important ways (Haber 1977, 1994, 1995b, unreported Natural selection leading to further speciation or the
1995-1996 data). It is unavoidable that significant num- maintenance of a particular adaptive state probably op-
bers of these juveniles, especially pups of the year (short erates on only a tiny portion of a species' genome in

Conservation Biology
Volume 1.0, No. 4, August 1996
1074 Implications of Killing Wolves Haber

most cases. This means many of the important natural medial wolf control programs for ungulate population
forces of selection that distinguish one species from an- management may be warranted biologically in excep-
other are inherently small and subtle. For the wolf natu- tional cases, for example, w h e n natural patterns and
ral forces are likely to be swamped by the artifical, ran- processes have been seriously disrupted in unnatural
dom forces associated with heavy annual killing, forces ways at much larger scales and wolf predation is pre-
to which this species has not had time to adapt. venting recovery. A determination as to what "natural,"
Those w h o tiT to defend heavy, ongoing killing and "unnatural" and other such criteria mean in these situa-
even some more limited forms of control, including ster- tions will always be difficult and will require some sub-
ilization, seem to view natural forms of wolf social orga- jectivity but in general should be possible. I predict that
nization as lacking intrinsic value. But, as with the basic only rarely will such biological justification for control
organizational state of any well-established species, be found, even for major ungulate declines.
the sophisticated, highly developed sociality of wolves is Haber (1977) and Haber & Waiters (1980) provide an
the product of past selection forces and thus represents the example of a set of circumstances where wolf control
level of organization most environmentally fit for this for ungulate management purposes was warranted.
species. Overharvesting had triggered a premature major decline
A related biological view could help guide our exploi- in the Western Arctic caribou h e r d - - t h e primary center
tation of wild vertebrates in general and could sharpen of abundance in the then depressed Alaska-Yukon cari-
the thinking that underlies the U.S. Endangered Species bou population--and wolf predation appeared to be a
Act and similar legislation. This view emphasizes the dif- critical variable preventing a timely recovery. This was
ferences among species in their adaptations for exploit- not merely a major decline of caribou in the western
ing versus being exploited. Eusocial cooperative hunt- Arctic, which by itself would not necessarily be a matter
ers, such as wolves and African wild dogs, represent one of concern. It was a decline that created what appeared
extreme, for which there is no biological rationale for to be a premature, unnatural condition of low syn-
harvesting and no way to undertake most control pro- chrony in the Alaska-Yukon system of caribou herds as a
grams without major biological costs. Herbivores such whole, quite unlike the present condition of high sys-
as the ungulates represent virtually the opposite ex- tem-wide abundance and asynchrony.
treme. For ungulates, the interactions among individuals Biologists, agencies, legislators, and others have subse-
and generations are simple enough so that the survivors quently proposed wolf control programs in response to
can quickly reorganize and carry on in about the same much less obvious ungulate problems, where there are
way w h e n many others are removed. no problems, or where there is little prospect of resolv-
Species of this kind are well adapted to exploitation ing such problems with wolf control (especially with re-
within carefully def'med limits (Haber 1977, 1980; Haber & gard to caribou). There has been minimal consideration
Waiters 1980; Waiters et al. 1981), having persisted as prey of the potential biological costs, especially for the target
throughout their long evolutionary histories. There are wolf populations. Most of the recent ungulate-related
familiar scenes from the East African plains and elsewhere Alaska and northwestern Canada wolf control proposals
of hoofed grazers either ignoring kills by predators in and programs have been frivolous from these and other
their midst or resuming feeding activity within minutes. standpoints (Haber 1987, 1988, 1992, 1993a). My initial
Between the cooperative hunter and ungulate/herbi- review of several new (October 1995) Alaska wolf con-
vore prey extremes there is a "gradient of sociality" that trol proposals, including Alaska Department of Fish and
might suggest differences in the way we exploit other Game (1995a, 1995b), indicates the same for them
species as well. This approach and more emphasis on (Haber letter 28 September 1995 to S. Todd, Alaska De-
protecting the integrity of underlying natural system, partment of Fish and Game files, Fairbanks).
population, and group patterns and processes in general Ungulate-related wolf (and bear) control proposals
(Haber 1992:15-24) w h e n harvesting is biologically jus- should be evaluated on a case-by-case basis and sub-
tiffed (e.g., for ungulates) represents the kind of selectiv- jected to rigorous scientific review. Decision-makers and
ity that true sustained-yield management implies. This the public should be provided with more information
merely acknowledges that there are some key differ- on the potential biological (and other) costs, benefits,
ences among species and that harvestable populations and alternatives for each proposal. There should be an a
are components of dynamic systems rather than sepa- priori assumption that control is not warranted. This
rate crops. would help to ensure a more scientifically defensible ap-
proach, as demonstrated in hypothesis testing. The em-
phasis would shift from trying to find support for pro-
Implications for Conservation and Management posals to "falsifying" them. P r o p o n e n t s - - a g e n c y and
otherwise--should not oversee the review process.
Although, in my view, there is no biological rationale for Thus, I disagree with Mech's (1995) view that some
routine harvesting of wolf populations, short-term, re- form of wolf control will generally be necessary. Mech

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications of Killing Wolves 1075

falls to allow that there is disagreement as to what is nec- stead of government agencies, in the interest of promot-
essary. His view as to the inevitability of control seems ing more widespread recovery of wolves to areas of
to reflect his belief that wolves do not socially limit their former range. He argues that agencies have become in-
o w n population. On the contrary, social limitation ap- creasingly reluctant to promote recovery because of the
pears to be of major importance under natural condi- controversy they fear in later trying to control the same
tions. This is indicated by the relative stability in size of wolves.
established, vigorously reproducing family groups even Mech's argument again assumes that control is gener-
during periods of minimal hunting and trapping losses ally necessary, which I believe more rigorous review
and ample prey (Murie 1944; Haber 1977, 1992, 1994, would reveal not to be the case. I suggest that wolf advo-
1995b; Peterson 1977; Allen 1979) and by related annual cates would be much more willing to accept control if it
variations in patterns of temporary group splitting and were proposed only w h e n actually needed and carried
the dispersal or mortality of juveniles (Haber 1977). Al- out m u c h more selectively. For ungulate-related wolf
though wolf social organization probably represents an control, a more rigorous process would do much to pre-
adaptation for self regulation, human exploitation proba- vent unrealistic user expectations about potential re-
bly selects against self-regulating traits. turns (Haber 1992: 43-44). In this sense, I agree with
Mech dismisses opposition to ungulate-related wolf Mech (1995) that there are serious misconceptions, ex-
control as "politically" motivated and the result of misin- cept that I attribute a large share of these to a failure by
formation by "animal-rights groups," despite negative management agencies to provide adequate professional
evaluations by other professionals that focus exclusively guidance for users.
on the biology of recent control proposals, to which he Mech (1995: 272-273) portrays wolves as "inherently
has not responded. He similarly dismisses opposition to adaptable," such that, "In Spain wolves live like coyotes
wolf control for livestock depredation, without noting in wheat and sunflower fields" and (in Italy, Spain, and
that in at least some major cases protest about this kind Portugal) obtain much of their food by scavenging gar-
of control arises because it appears to be much less se- bage and livestock remains in and near rural villages. I
lective than is necessary and the law requires (Friends of agree that wolves are highly adaptable and that there are
Animals Inc. v. Babbitt et al., U.S. District Court, District varying degrees of sociality, depending, for example, on
of Connecticut, 7/10/95). In Minnesota (U.S.A.) for ex- the type of prey animals hunted (e.g., moose generally
ample, a suspected wolf kill of a livestock animal n o w require more cooperation to kill than caribou; Haber
often prompts federal officials to try to kill all or most of 1977). And I agree that scavenging of dead ungulates is
the wolves that subsequently visit the original carcass or an important supplemental foraging activity for wolves.
supplemental baits a half mile or more from the original Nevertheless, Mech (1995) seems willing to accept al-
site for up to 30 days afterward, even though wolves most any behavioral variation as "inherent." Should Mech's
scavenge dead ungulates (Haber 1977) and only one garbage-eating, largely solitary, sunflower-field canids re-
wolf may have been involved in the livestock kill. ally be regarded as wolves? Or are they the product of a
Mech (1995) suggests that there is less protest about lengthy, subtle process of "unwolfmg" via human perse-
the m u c h heavier kill of wolves in Alaska via public cution and habitat/prey displacement? Far from support-
hunting and trapping than by government control be- ing his position, the examples he provides of "adaptabil-
cause there is more political advantage to be gained in ity" may instead serve as a warning about the pitfalls of
stirring up opposition to the government. He ignores watching for numerical signals of endangerment while
the fact that leading opponents have long attempted to ignoring virtually all else about a creature's biology.
call attention to the public kill as being of at least as More detailed comparisons between exploited and un-
much biological impact as government control (Haber exploited wolf populations are needed. There is little
1985, 1993b, 1993c, 1995a; Haber versus Mech debate: problem in obtaining information about currently or re-
"Biological Impacts on Wolves of Exploitation and Con- cently exploited populations, where most of the re-
trol," at First Annual Conference of The Wildlife Society, search effort lies. However, opportunities to do research
9/24/94). He seems unaware of the deceptive way in in areas that have remained free of harvest and control
which the public kill is reported (Haber 1985, 1993b, for a long period are rare. Even in such world-renowned
1995a). For example, m o s t of this kill is reported as due wolf research areas as Algonquin Provincial Park, On-
to trapping. But as noted earlier, state and federal regula- tario, and Denali National Park, Alaska, established resi-
tions define trapping to include direct hunting with fire- dent family groups of wolves are still exploited to some
arms in most areas (semi-automatic weapons are com- extent via legal hunting and trapping, inside park bound-
monly used for wolves, legally), without requiring any aries as well as outside (J. Theberge, personal communi-
actual trapping, to permit the virtually direct use of air- cation; Haber 1995a; Federal Subsistence Board 1995;
planes and snowmachines, and to allow saturation snaring. Alaska Board of Game 1995).
Mech (1995) recommends that control programs For Denali the responsible agency biologists and man-
should be carried out by public hunting and trapping in- agers continue to defend current policies that allow non-

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
1076 Implicationsof Killing Wolves Haber

selective 8- to %month annum harvests of up to 10 hours of wolf observations in Denali since 1966, I am
wolves per hunter and no-limit trapping, and to actively convinced that most of this habituation involves a form of
oppose creation of a protective buffer on state lands trust by the wolves. For the most part, they seem n o w to
along critical boundary areas, with arguments about the view people in a friendly, sometimes playful way. I have
health of total numbers ("numbers generally range be- yet to see or hear of any obvious aggressive behavior.
tween 100 and 130") and their view that "not many" As of 10 August each year whenever the same wolves
wolves are likely to be taken (National Park Service step across the park boundary or enter the 1980 park ad-
1995; S. Martin, testimony to Alaska Board of Game, ditions they become legal quarry for hunters and as of 1
March 1996). This is despite aerial radio-tracking surveys November they become legal quarry for trappers. We allow
by the National Park Service and me that consistently them to trust us inside the park and then look the other
yielded total counts of only 55-77 wolves in 9-11 family way w h e n they become easier hunter-trapper fodder be-
groups from November 1995 through April 1996, and cause of this diminished wariness. This was the predica-
despite the complete hunting/trapping loss of the Head- ment of the Headquarters family and of the alpha-male
quarters family group as of May 1995, the trapping loss and others of the Toklat family who were shot, snared, and
of the alpha-male and at least three other wolves from trapped recently. I knew these wolves well. They had al-
the Toklat family group in November 1992, the likely most no fear of people. They were an easy mark for the
hunting/trapping loss of the heavily viewed, intensively few hunters and trappers who were allowed to kill them.
studied Savage family group (Haber 1977, 1987) in win- In her 1958 classic, Arctic Wild, Lois Crisler wrote
ter 1982/1983, and other such hunting/trapping losses with great sensitivity about the wolves she knew during
through recent years. Nor are wolf hunters and trappers the early 1950s in northern Alaska. This is always one of
even required to report their kills until well after the the first publications I recommend to aspiring biologists
factmhunters not until 30 days afterward, trappers not and laypeople w h o are interested in wolves because it
until 30 May. In contrast, the 2000-3000 caribou of the portrays them as the marvelously intelligent, expressive,
Denali herd are off limits to all hunting. There are about emotional creatures they are. This was the account that
2000 moose in Denali, with a harvest limit of one bull first stirred my interest in wolves. Almost 40 years later I
per hunter, a season of only 60 days, a ban against hunt- am obliged to also read the sterile National Park Service
ing in the most accessible areas, and no hunting of summaries (e.g., Adams & Stephenson 1986, 1988; Ad-
white-phased or albinos (Federal Substance Board 1995; ams et al. 1989) of the 15-20% annual wolf harvests that
Alaska Board of Game 1995). are currently allowed within the same a r e a - - n o w Gates
of the Arctic National P a r k - - b y native "subsistence"
hunters with high-speed snowmachines.
Ethical Considerations I recognize that my strong opposition to the way
wolves are managed in Alaska and elsewhere involves
High intelligence, expressiveness, and unusual emo- more than pure biology. I receive frequent criticism for
tional depth enable wolves to maintain sophisticated so- this position from my peers. Nevertheless, Aldo Leopold
cial bonds, to work together as highly skilled coopera- did not hesitate to venture into such areas of overlap be-
tive hunters (Haber 1977). This same extraordinary tween biology and ethics, to distinguish between right
sentience that is so integral to their basic biology also and wrong in advocating improved management of natu-
provides an ethical reason for not allowing them to be ral systems. Other wildlife scientists w h o regard his
harvested and for considering remedial short-term con- ideals as a guiding light for the profession should not
trol only in the rarest of circumstances, w h e n there are hesitate to do the same.
solid, irrefutable biological and cost-benefit arguments
and no other reasonable alternatives. To treat them oth-
erwise is wrong. Such higher standing is n o w generally Acknowledgments
accorded to other creatures of obvious high sentience,
including whales, dolphins, gorillas, and chimpanzees, I thank Friends of Animals, Wolf Haven International,
and it is time to extend it fully to wolves. The Alaska Wildlife Alliance, and the Conservation Soci-
I have described some of the details of the heavy, in- ety for Wolves and Whales for their support, and Paul
discriminate killing of wolves that is still permitted in Joslin, John Theberge, Jeff Augustine, Bill Clark, and three
Alaska. Consider an additional problem for the wolves of anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on ear-
Denali National Park: Although the Denali wolves still lier drafts and symposia presentations leading to this paper.
hunt and seem to behave socially and individually as
they did w h e n I first began studying them in 1966, over
Literature Cited
recent years they have become strongly habituated to
people because of numerous close, friendly contacts Adams, L. G. and R. O. Stephenson. 1986. Wolf survey, Gates of the
with park visitors each summer. Based on my 10,000+ Arctic National Park and Preserve--1986. Natural resources survey

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications of Killing Wolves 1077

and inventory report AR-86/04. National Park Service, Alaska Re- Federal Subsistence Board. 1995. Subsistence m a n a g e m e n t regulations
gional Office, Anchorage. for the harvest offish and wildlife o n federal public lands in Alaska,
Adams, L. G. a n d R. O. Stephenson. 1988. Population ecology of effective July 1, 1995-June 30, 1996. U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service,
wolves in Gates of the Arctic National Park and Preserve, Alaska-- Federal Subsistence Board, Anchorage.
1987. Natural resources progress report AR-88/07. National Park Gasaway, W. C., R. O. Stephenson, J. L. Davis, P. E. K. Shepherd, and
Service, Alaska Regional Office, Anchorage. O. E. Burris. 1983. Interrelationships of wolves, prey, and m a n in
Adams, L. G., R. O. Stephenson, B. W. Dale, and B. Shults. 1989. Popu- Interior Alaska. Wildlife Monographs 84.
lation ecology of wolves in Gates of the Arctic National Park and Gasaway, W. C., R. D. Boertje, D. V. Grangaard, D. G. KeUeyhouse,
Preserve, Alaska--1988. Natural resources progress report AR-89/ R. O. Stephenson, and D. G. Larsen. 1992. The role of predation in
15. National Park Service, ALaska Regional Office, Anchorage. limiting m o o s e at low densities in Alaska and Yukon and implica-
Alaska Board of Game. 1995. Alaska state hunting and trapping regula- tions for conservation. Wildlife Monographs 120.
tions (no. 36), effective July 1, 1995-June 30, 1996. Alaska Depart- Haber, G. C. 1977. Socio-ecological dynamics of wolves and prey in a
m e n t of Fish and Game, Juneau. subarctic ecosystem. Ph.D. dissertation. University of British Co-
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1989. Wolf. Federal aid in wild- lumbia, Vancouver.
life restoration, annual report of survey-inventory activities, 1 July Haber, G. C. 1979. The u p p e r Stikine-Spatsizi wolf-ungulate system,
1987-30 June 1988. Volume XIX, part XV, project W-23-1, study northwestern British Columbia--a field reconnaissance and prelim-
14.0. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Juneau. inary evaluation, 1978. Report n u m b e r 1. Spatsizi Association for
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 199 la. Wolf. Federal aid in wild- Biological Research, Smithers, British Columbia. (Also available
life restoration, survey-inventory m a n a g e m e n t report, 1 July 1989- from Arctic Environmental Information and Data Canter, University
30 June 1991. Project w-23-3,4, study 14.0. Alaska Department of of Alaska, Anchorage.)
Fish and Game, Juneau. Haber, G. C. 1980. The balancing act of m o o s e and wolves. Natural
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1991b. Wolf. Federal aid in wild- History 89: 38-51.
life restoration, annual report of survey-inventory activities, 1 July Haber, G. C. 1985. Aerial wolf hunting in Alaska. Three-part series: part
1990-30 June 1991. Volume XXII, part XV, project W-23-4, study I--Aerial wolf hunting in Alaska: still underway in force; part II--
14.0. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Juneau. "Landand shoot" hunters take heavy toll of wolves; part HI--Am-
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1992a. Alaska's wolves. Alaska's biguous laws let wolf hunters do almost anything. Anchorage Daily
wildlife magazine (supplement), January-February, 1992. Alaska News, March 13 (c-11), 18 (c-9), 19 (c-13).
Department of Fish and Game, Juneau. Haber, G. C. 1987. Exploitation of wolf-moose s y s t e m s - - l e s s o n s from
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1992b. Area-specific wolf man- Interior Alaska. The Alaska Wildlife Alliance, Anchorage. (Also
agement plan, game m a n a g e m e n t units 12, 20, and 25C (second available from Arctic Environmental Information and Data Center,
draft--March 1992). Alaska D e ~ e n t o f f i s h and Game, Fairbanks. University of Alaska, Anchorage.)
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1992c. Area-specific wolf man- Haber, G. C. 1988. Wildlife m a n a g e m e n t in northern British Columbia:
agement plan for south central/interior Alaska (final draft--August Kechika-Muskwa wolf control and related issues. Wolf Haven Inter-
1992). Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Fairbangs. national, Tenino, Washington. (Also available from Arctic Environmen-
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1995a. Draft Fortymile caribou tal Information and Data Center, University of Alaska, Anchorage.)
herd recovery plan (September 1995). Alaska Department of Fish Haber, G. C. 1992. Wildlife m a n a g e m e n t in Alaska: Southcentral-inte-
and Game, Fairbanks. rior wolf control and related issues. Wolf Haven International,
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1995b. Fortymile caribou herd Tenino, Washington. (Also available from Arctic Environmental In-
m a n a g e m e n t plan (October 1995). Alaska Department of Fish and formation and Data Center, University of Alaska, Anchorage.)
Game, Fairbanks. Haber, G. C. 1993a. Wildlife m a n a g e m e n t in the Yukon Territory: Aish-
Alaska Department of Fish and Game. 1995c. Wolf m a n a g e m e n t in ihik wolf control and related issues. Wolf Haven International,
Alaska is not simple. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Fair- Tenino, Washington. (Also available from Arctic Environmental In-
banks (media release). formation and Data Center, University of Alaska, Anchorage.)
Allen, D. L. 1979. The wolves of Minong: their vital role in a wild com- Haber, G. C. 1993b. Wolves and wolf-prey-human interactions, interior
munity. Houghton-Mifflin, Boston. and southcentral Alaska. Research report 1. The Alaska Wildlife Al-
Ballard, W. B., J. s. Whitman, and C. L. Gardner. 1987. Ecology of an liance, Anchorage; Friends of Animals, Darien, Connecticut; Wolf
exploited wolf population in south-central Alaska. Wildlife Mono- Haven International, Tenino, Washington. (Also available from Arc-
graphs 98. tic Environmental Information and Data Center, University of
Bailard, W. B., J. s. Whitman, and D. L. Reed. 1991. Population dynam- Alaska, Anchorage.)
ics of moose in south-central Alaska. Wildlife Monographs 114. Haber, G. C. 1993c. The great Alaska wolf kill. The New York Times.
Boertje, R. D., C. L. Gardner, and P. Valkenburg. 1995. Factors limiting 2 October.
the Fortymile caribou herd. Federal aid in wildlife restoration, re- Haber, G. C. 1994. Wolves and wolf-prey-human interactions, interior
search progress report 1 July 1994-30 June 1995. Study 3.38, grant and southcentral Alaska. Summary of research activities, J u n e -
W-24-3. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Juneau. December 1993 (Alaska Department of Fish and Game scientific
Bouchard, T. J., D. T. Lykken, M. McGue, N. L. Segal, and A. Tellegen. permit no. 93-117). Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Juneau.
1990. Sources of h u m a n psychological differences: The Minnesota Haber, G. C. 1995a. Park wolves need protection. Fairbanks Daily
study of twins reared apart. Science 250:223-228. News-Miner. 11 September.
Bowen, W. D. 1981. Variation in coyote social organization: the influ- Haber, G. C. 1995b. Wolves and wolf-prey-human interactions, interior
ence of prey size. Canadian Journal of Zoology 59:639-652. and sonthcentral Alaska. Summary of research activities, January-
Brewster, W. G. and S. H. Fritts. 1992. Taxonomy, genetics, and status December1994 (Alaska Department of Fish and Game scientific re-
of the gray wolf, Canis lupus, in western North America: a review. search permit no. 94-25). Alaska Department ofFish and Game, Ju-
Pages 3-33-3-93 in Wolves for Yellowstone? A report to the Ulaited neau.
States Congress, volume IV, research and analysis. National Park Haber, G. C., and C. J. Waiters. 1980. Dynamics of the Alaska-Yukon
Service, Yellowstone National Park, Wyoming. caribou herds and m a n a g e m e n t implications. Pages 645-663 in E.
Elliot, J. P. 1982. Muskwa project: draft problem analysis/working Reimers, E. Gaare, and S. Skjenneberg, editors. Proceedings of the
plan. Report. British Columbia Ministry of Environment and Parks, 2nd international reindeer/caribou symposium, Roros, Norway,
Fort St. John. 1979. Direktoratet for vilt og ferskvannsfisk, Trondheim. (Also

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
1078 Implicationsof KiUingWolves Haber

available from Arctic Environmental Information and Data Center, ulation. Pages 293-302 in L. D. Carbyn, S. H. Fritts, and D. R. Seip,
University of Alaska, Anchorage.) editors. Ecology and conservation of wolves in a changing world.
Harrington, F. H. and L. D. Mech. 1982. Patterns of homesite atten- Canadian Circumpolar Institute, Occasional Papers 35, Edmonton,
dance in two Minnesota wolf packs. Pages 81-105 in F. H. Har- Alberta.
rington and P. C. Paquet, editors. Wolves of the world: perspec- Messier, F. 1985. Social organization, spatial distribution, and popula-
tives of behavior, ecology, and conservation. Noyes Publications, tion density of wolves in relation to moose density. Canadian Jour-
Park Ridge, New Jersey. nal of Zoology 63: 1068-1077.
Hayes, R. D., A. M. Baer, and D. G. Larsen. 1991. Population dynamics Murie, A. 1944. The wolves of Mount McKinley. U.S. National Park Ser-
and prey relationships of an exploited and recovering wolf popula- vice Fauna Series 5.
tion in the southern Yukon. Final report TR-91-1, Yukon Fish and National Park Service. 1995. Fact sheet: Wolf protection and manage-
Wildlife Branch, Whitehorse, Yukon Territory. ment at Denali National Park and Preserve. National Park Service,
Keith, L. B. 1983. Population dynamics of wolves. Pages 66-77 in L. N. Denali National Park.
Carbyn editor. Wolves in Canada and Alaska: their status, biology, Packard, J. M. 1980. Deferred reproduction in wolves. Ph.D. disserta-
and management. Canadian Wildlife Service Report Series 45. tion. University of Minnesota, Minneapolis.
Kelner, K. andJ. Benditt, editors. 1994. Genes and behavior. A special Peterson, R. O. 1977. Wolf ecology and prey relationships on Isle Roy-
report. Science 264:1685-1739. ale. U.S. National Park Service Scientific Monograph Series 11.
Lehman, N., P. Ciarkson, L. D. Mech, T. J. Meier, and R. K. Wayne. Peterson, R. O., J. D. Woolington, and T. N. Bailey. 1984. Wolves of the
1992. A study of the genetic relationships within and a m o n g wolf Kenai Peninsula, Alaska. Wildlife Monographs 88.
packs using DNA finger-printing and mitochondrial DNA. Behav- Peterson, S. 1995. Estimated statewide wolf population status (1994-
ioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30:83-94. 95). Memorandum (12 June). Alaska Department of Fish and Game,
Leopold, S. A. 1949. A Sand County almanac and sketches here and Juneau.
there. Oxford University Press, New York. Plomin, R. 1990. The role of inheritance in behavior. Science 248:
McNay, M. E. 1993. Development and testing of a general predator- 183-188.
prey c o m p u t e r model for use in making m a n a g e m e n t decisions Rausch, R. A. 1967. Some aspects of the population ecology of wolves,
(Fig. 7). Federal aid in wildlife restoration, research progress re- Alaska. American Zoologist 7: 253-265.
port, project W-24-1, study 1.46. Alaska Department of Fish and Sherman, P. W., J. U. M. Jarvis, and R. D. Alexander. 1991. The biology
Game, Juneau. of the naked mole rat. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New
Mech, L. D. 1970. The wolf: the ecology and behavior of an endan- Jersey.
gered species. Natural History Press, Doubleday, New York. Sherman, P. W., J. U. M. Jarvis, and S. H. Braude. 1992. Naked mole
Mech, L. D. 1977. Population trend and winter deer consumption in a rats. Scientific American 267: 72-78.
Minnesota wolf pack. Pages 55-83 in R. L. Phillips, and C. Jonkel, Stephenson, R. O. and J. J. Sexton. 1974. Wolf report. Federal aid in
editors. Proceedings of the 1975 predator symposium. University of wildlife restoration progress report, projects W-17-5, W-17-6.
Montana, Forest Conservation Experiment Station Bulletin, Missoula. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, Juneau.
Mech, L. D. 1991. C o m m e n t s to Alaska wolf m a n a g e m e n t planning Tear, T. H., J. M. Scott, P. H. Hayward, and B. Griffith. 1993. Status and
team. In Alaska wolf m a n a g e m e n t planning team, meeting sum- prospects for success of the Endangered Species Act: a look at re-
mary, March 21-23, 1991. Alaska Department of Fish and Game, covery plans. Science 262: 976-977.
Anchorage. Vucetich, J. A., and R. O. Peterson. 1995. Population viability analysis
Mech, L. D. 1995. The challenge and opportunity of recovering wolf of Isle Royale wolves incorporating species interactions and social
populations. Conservation Biology 9: 270-278. structure. Paper (and abstract) presented at, Wolves and h u m a n s
Mech, L. D., T.J. Meier, J. W. Burch, and L. G. Adams. 1991. Demogra- 2000, Duluth, Minnesota, March 1995. International Wolf Center,
phy and distribution of wolves, Denali National Park and Preserve, Ely, Minnesota.
Alaska--1986-1990. Natural resources progress report AR-91/O1. Waiters, C. J., M. Stocker, and G. C. Haber. 1981. Simulation and opti-
National Park Service, Alaska Regional Office, Anchorage. mization models for a wolf-ungulate system. Pages 317-337 in
Meier, T., J. Butch, L. D. Mech, and L. Adams. 1995. Pack structure and C. W. Fowler, and T. D. Smith, editors. Dynamics of large mammal
genetic relatedness a m o n g wolf packs in a naturally regulated pop- populations. John Wiley & Sons, New York.

I i

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications o f KiUing Wolves 1079

Appendix 1
Known or likely impacts of heavy ( 15-20 + % annual) exploitation and control on wolves.

Impact Area Source


1. Smaller late winter average group sizes; more population fragmentation.
Wolves/group Groups/l O00 k m 2 Wolves/l O00 k m 2
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before
or during observation.
7-14 0.77 5.4-10.8 Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977, 1992 a
6.2-10.9 0.82 4.0-11.2 Denali N.P., Alaska NPS, unpublished; Mech et
al. 1991 °
4.7-8.7 1.0-1.1 4.1-8.1 Denali N.P., Alaska Meier et al., 1995 b
5-8 0.85 4.2-6.8 Denali N.P., Alaska Haber, NPS, 1993-1995,
unpublished"
No known heavy killing immediately before
observation; heavy killing during observation.
11.3 to 5.6 0.79 to 2.20 9.0 to 12.31 Kenai Pen., Alaska Peterson et al. 1984
(annual exploitation: 7-18% to 25-31%) a
8.6 to 3.6 to 5.5 1.3 to 1.4to 1.6 12.4 to 3.6 to 10.8 SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
(annual control rates: 38=39% to 66% to 23-29%) e
12.0 to 4.5-7.0 to 7.6 0.83 to 1.03-1.15 10.0 to 5.19-7.12 Horseranch area, Haber 1988
(annual control rates: 61% to 85-86% to 0): to 1.92 to 14.62 northern B.C.
Heavy killing during observation; k n o w n or likely heavy
killing immediately before.
2.9-4.7 0.75-1.52 2.6-7.1 Nelchina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987
(X = 1.14) Alaska
5.1 1.50-3.07 15.67 GMU 20A, Alaska Gasaway et al. 1983
2.0-3.5 1.10-1.61 2-5 Fortymile region, Gassaway et al. 1992
Alaskag
3.5-4.7 0.86 3-4 Fortymile/upper Haber 1994/1995 b,
Tanana/Charley unpublished 1995 data
region, Alaskag
2.0-5.1 0.84-1.44 1.68-7.31 Minto area, Alaska McNay 1993
4.5-5.7 0.65-0.85 3.7-3.9 Gates of the Arctic Adams & Stephenson 1986,
N.P., Alaska 1988; Adams et al. 1989
6.2 to 3.4-4.1 to 3.6 2.53 to 3.65 15.73 to 12.50- Kechika area, Haber 1988
(annual control rates: 79% to 73-83% to 65%) ° to 3.11 15.02 to 11.13 northern B.C.
5.32 to 3.16-3.69 to 4.03 7.38 to 3.5-6.0 39.30 to 12.93- Muskwa area, Haber 1988
(annual control rates: 60% to 77 + 0% to 60%) l to 4.52 18.95 to 18.25 northern B.C.
Probably some heavy killing immediately before and/or
during observation.
7.7-8.4 1.85-2.01 15.44-15.60 j Blue-Jenningsarea, Haber 1988
northern B.C.
2. Mating less selective; more litters per population and generally higher mid-late winter p u p ratios,
except at highest exploitation intensities.
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before or during observation.
Typically one litter per group; 0-15% of groups produced 2-4 litters annually (simultaneously). Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977a; Haber, NPS
Fewer than 25-50% of adult females produce young. X = 37% pups (18-60'%). unpublished 1978-
1995c; Mech et al. 1991~;
Meier et al., 1995 ~
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
One litter per group (two in one group). 67% of adult females in estrus or previously pregnant. Kenai Pen, Alaska Peterson et al. 1984
Percentage pups in population increased 26 to 54% as hunting and trapping increased 7-
18% to 25-31%. °
No more than one litter per group. 82% of adult females (from before and during control SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
samples combined) in estrus or previously pregnant. Percentage pups in population
increased 36 to 45% (78%+ of groups reproduced annually) during two years of 38-39%
control; percentage pups decreased to 16% the next year (39% of groups reproduced)
following 66% controL*
Heavy killing during observation; k n o w n or likely heavy killing immediately before.
89% of adult females in estrus or previously pregnant. X = 45% p u p s in population (39-60%). Interior Alaska Rausch 1967
Typically one litter per group; 7-10% of groups produced two litters annually. X = 57% pups Neichina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987
in population (42-74%). * Alaska
71% of adult females in estrus or previously pregnant. X = 30% p u p s in population (25-33%). GMU 2OA, Alaska Gasaway et al. 1983
One litter per group. X = 32% p u p s in population (31-33%). Fortymile/upper Haber 1994, 1995 b,
Tanana/Charley unpublished 1995 data
region, Alasl~
Typically one litter per group. Approximately 41% p u p s in population. Gates of the Adams & Stephenson 1988
Arctic, N.P.,
Alaska

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
1080 Implications of Killing Wolves Haber

Appendix 1
Continued.

Impact Area Source


3. More variable (moose) kill rates.
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before or during observation.
Kill rates increased with increased group size over a range of 2 to 7-9 wolves, then leveled off Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977
through a group size of at least 19. a
No known heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
Kill rates of pairs often the same as for groups of 6-7 wolves. No significant correlation between SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
kill rate and group size through a group size of at least 7 wolves, even w h e n pairs are excluded, e
Heavy killing during observation; k n o w n or likely heavy killing immediately before.
Kill rates increased with increased group size over a range of 2-9 wolves, then apparently Nelchina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987 (includes
continued increasing more slowly through a group size of at least 20. Kenai Pen, one data point from
Alaska Peterson et ai. 1984).
4. More variable territories
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before or during observation.
At least 3 of 15 territories in 1987-1988 were still occupied by the same groups in 1995, in Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977a; Mech et al.
approx, the same areas or (for one) in the core of the same area; 1-2 of these 3 territories/ 1991b; Meier et al.,
groups extend back to at least 1966-1974. Of the remaining 12 territories at least 6 were 19950; Haber, NPS
approx, the same in 1995 as in 1987-1988 but were apparently occupied by n e w groups; 2 unpublished
of these turnovers were probably caused by hunting and trapping. The other 6 territories 1978-1995 c
were not monitored adequately through 1995, although as of 1991-1992 at least one was
occupied by the same group in aprox, the same area, another was occupied by 2
neighboring groups via expansion of their territories (the previous group disappeared
naturally), and a third was occupied by 3 smaller groups. A group occupying another, stable
territory for 16+ years was eliminated in 1983, probably due to hunting/trapping. Most of
this vacancy was occupied by another group for 11 years, until it was eliminated by hunting
and trapping in 1993-1995. A n e w group began occupying essentially the same area in 1995.
No known heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
2 of the 3 territories covering most of the study area in 1976-1977 were occupied by the same Kenai Pen, Alaska Peterson et al. 1984
groups in 1980-1981. One of the 3 territories was approx, the same size in 1980-1981, and
another was centered in approx, the same location. There were 4 additional, occupied
territories within the overall 1976-1977 area in 1980-1981. a
None of 3 closely monitored "sample" territories was occupied by the same group in 1986- SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
1987 as in 1982-1984. One of the 3 territories was of approx, the same size and location in
1986-1987. Within the overall wolf control area, "suspected" territory boundaries ( ~ 18- 20
territories) suggested a substantially different mosaic in 1987-1988 versus 1982-1984.
Control increased from 38-39% in 1982-1984 to 66% in 1984-1985, then decreased to 23-
29% in 1986-1987. e
Heavy killing during observation; k n o w n or likely heavy killing immediately before.
3-4 of 15 territories in 1975-1976 were occupied by the same groups in 1980-1982.0-2 of Nelchina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987
these territories and 3-7 of the other 11-12 were of approx, the same size and location in Alaska
1980-1982. There were 2-4 additional, occupied territories within the overall 1975-1976
area in 1980-1982.
5. Less use of established dens.
No known heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
76% and 64% of known 1966-1982 and 1966-1993 within-summer homesite moves (following Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977a; Haber, NPS,
abandonment of the natal den; n = 17,59) by 2 family groups occupying approx, the same unpublished
adjacent territories for 16+ and 50+ years, respectively, were to various established dens 1978-1995 c
(versus rendezvous sites). 33% of known moves (n = 12) by wolves that colonized the "76%"
territory in 1984, after the 16+ year residents were eliminated (probably by hunting/
trapping) were to a den, in each case to the same established den. This group was eliminated
by hunting/trapping in 1993-1995.
6. Territory size varies more directly with resident group size, sometimes with disproportionately
much larger initial territories for colonizers.
No known heavy killing immediately before or during observation.
No significant relationship between territory size and group size over a range of at least 5-20 Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977a; Mech et al.
wolves/group. Territory size varied inversely with moose density. 19910
No known heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
Territory size varied significantly with group size (i.e., larger groups-larger territories) over a Kenai Pen, Alaska Peterson et al. 1984
range of at lease 5-20 wolves/group. Smaller, recolonizing groups, especiallypairs, initially
occupied disproportionately much larger territories (3-4 × area/wolD than were occupied
by larger groupsfl
Smaller, recoionizing groups, especially 2-3 wolves, initially occupied disproportionately SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
much larger territories (5 × area/wolD than were occupied by larger groupsY
Heavy kilting during observation; known or likely heavy killing immediately before.
Territory size varied significantly with group size over a range of at least 2-15 wolves/group, Nelchina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987
and inversely with moose density. Alaska

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996
Haber Implications o f Killing Wolves 1081

Appendix 1
Continued.

Impact Area Source


7. Higher overall natural mortality rates.
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before or during observation
16-31% annual "losses'--i.e., natural mortality and dispersal combined; closely related to early Denali N.P., Alaska Haber 1977 a
winter group size for at least one established family group--this was the primary means by
which the size of this group was limited.
= 13% (7-18%) annual natural mortality. Denali N.P., Alaska Mech et al. 19910
= 20% annual natural mortality. Denali N.P., Alaska Meier et al., 19950
No k n o w n heavy killing immediately before observation; heavy killing during observation.
= 25% annual natural mortality; increased from 14-21% to 25-38% as hunting and trapping Kenai Pen, Alaska Peterson et al. 1984
increased from 7-18% to 25-31%. Population limited primarily by exploitation, a
= 60% annual natural mortality; increased from 32-36% to 79% as control increased from 38- SW Yukon Hayes et al. 1991
39% to 66%; decreased to 49% the next year as control decreased to 29%, then increased to
78-84% for at least the next 2 years as control decreased to ~23%. e
Heavy killing during observation; k n o w n or likely heavy killing immediately before.
= 29% annual natural mortality. Population limited primarily by exploitation and control. Nelchina Basin, Ballard et al. 1987
Alaska
a 1 9 6 6 - 1 9 9 2 f o r g r o u p sizes, groups/area, a n d w o l f densities; 1 9 6 6 - 1 9 7 4 f o r reproductive info, kill rates, territory size versus g r o u p size, a n d
mortality rates. Data f o r up to f i v e established f a m i l y groups p r i m a r i l y f r o m periods w h e n these g r o u p s suffered f e w i f a n y hunting~trapping
losses.
b 1986-1992. Data f o r up to 16 groups, including s o m e in areas o f the park~preserve w h e r e there were moderate to h e a v y h u n t i n g a n d trapping
losses over earlier y e a r s a n d decades a n d sporadic light to moderate losses during the p e r i o d o f observation.
c1978_1992 or 1993-1995. Data f o r up to 3 ( 1 9 7 8 - 1 9 9 2 ) or 11 ( 1 9 9 3 - 1 9 9 5 ) groups, including s o m e in areas o f the p a r k / p r e s e r v e w h e r e there
were m o d e r a t e to h e a v y h u n t i n g a n d trapping losses over earlier y e a r s a n d decades a n d sporadic light to moderate losses during the p e r i o d o f
observation.
a 1976-1982. Continuing h e a v y exploitation ( h u n t i n g a n d trapping) applied to a n initially lightly exploited population--i.e., exploitation in-
creased f r o m 7-18% to 25-31%. Data s h o w changes in each variable during the p e r i o d o f observation.
eHeavy g o v e r n m e n t control applied to a n initially lightly exploited population--i.e., control increased f r o m 38-39% in 1 9 8 2 - 1 9 8 4 to 66% in
1 9 8 4 - 1 9 8 5 then decreased to 23-29% in 1986-1987. Data s h o w changes in each variable d u r i n g this p e r i o d o f observation. The f i r s t values
s h o w n f o r g r o u p sizes, g r o u p s / a r e a a n d w o l f densities are f r o m 1982-1983, j u s t before the initial (38-39%) control w a s applied; the other t w o
values s h o w n f o r g r o u p sizes, groups~area, a n d w o l f densities are f r o m the subsequent heavy (66%) a n d then decreased (23-29%) ~ontrol periods.
f H e a v y g o v e r n m e n t control applied to a n initially lightly exploited population--i.e., control increased f r o m 61% in 1978 to 85-86% in 1979-
1980. There w a s no f u r t h e r control after 1980. Data s h o w changes in each variable f r o m 1978 j u s t before the initial (61%) control to 85-86%
control in 1 9 7 9 - 1 9 8 0 to 1982 ( 2 y e a r s after control h a d ended).
g G a s a w a y et al. 1992 data are f o r 1982-1989. H a b e r data are f o r 1 9 9 3 - 1 9 9 5 f r o m Gasaway's area p l u s adjoining regions.
bHeavy g o v e r n m e n t control applied to a p o p u l a t i o n p r o b a b l y subject to s o m e illegal, n o n g o v e r n m e n t control. G o v e r n m e n t control w a s applied
at rates o f 79% in 1982, 73-83% in 1983-1984, a n d 65% in 1985. Data s h o w changes in each variable f r o m 1 9 8 2 j u s t before the initial (79%)
control, to 73-83% control in 1983-1984, to 1985 j u s t before the 65% control. Data f o r a n additional y e a r (1986) c a n n o t be c o m p a r e d directly
because the survey/control area w a s almost doubled.
iH e a v y g o v e r n m e n t control applied to a p o p u l a t i o n p r o b a b l y subject to s o m e illegal, n o n g o v e r n m e n t control. G o v e r n m e n t control increased
f r o m 60% in 1984 to 77% in 1985, then decreased to no control in 1 9 8 6 a n d 60% in 1987. Data s h o w changes in each variable f r o m 1984 j a s t
before the initial (60%) control, to 77 + 0% control in 1985-1986, to 1 9 8 7 j u s t before the 60% control. No f u r t h e r data available.
J 1 9 8 5 - 1 9 8 6 No g o v e r n m e n t control b u t p r o b a b l y s o m e illegal n o n - g o v e r n m e n t control.
kThis p u p ratio estimate w a s derived in p a r t f r o m g r o u n d trapping data. G r o u n d trapping ( w i t h traps a n d snares versus airpiane-assisted
shooting/"trapping ") is usually biased to catching pups, thus the actual p u p ratio m a y he s o m e w h a t l o w e r than s h o w n here.

Conservation Biology
Volume 10, No. 4, August 1996

You might also like