You are on page 1of 6

European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471

http://france.elsevier.com/direct/EURPSY/

Original article

The relationship between impulsivity and Internet addiction


in a sample of Chinese adolescents
Fenglin Cao a,b, Linyan Su a,*, TieQiao Liu a, Xueping Gao a
a
The 2nd Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, No. 139 Renmin Road, Changsha, Hunan 410011, P.R. China
b
School of Nursing, Shandong University, No. 44 Wenhua Xi Road, Jinan, Shandong 250012, P.R. China
Received 21 March 2007; received in revised form 27 May 2007; accepted 30 May 2007
Available online 31 August 2007

Abstract

Objective. e Previous studies regarding Internet addiction have investigated associated psychological variables such as shyness, loneliness,
self-consciousness, anxiety, depression and interpersonal relations. Few studies about the relationship between Internet Addiction and impulsiv-
ity have been done. This study aimed to assess whether Internet addiction is related to impulsivity among Chinese adolescents.
Method. e This study was performed in two stages. We screened for the presence of Internet Addiction among 2620 high school students(age
ranging from 12 years to 18 years) from four high schools of Changsha City using Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addiction (YDQ). Ac-
cording to the modified YDQ criteria by Beard, 64 students were diagnosed as Internet addiction. Excluding current psychiatric comorbidity, 50
students who were diagnosed as Internet Addiction (mean age, 14.8  1.4 years) and 50 normal students in Internet usage(mean age,
14.5  1.8 years) were included in a case control study. The two groups were assessed using Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 11 (BIS-11) and be-
havioral measure of impulsivity (GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm).
Results. e Sixty-four students met the modified YDQ criteria by Beard, of whom 14 students suffered from comorbid psychiatric disorders,
especially comorbid ADHD. The Internet Addiction group had significantly higher scores on the BIS-11 subscales of Attentional key, Motor key,
and Total scores than the control group (P < 0.05). The Internet Addiction group scored higher than the control group on the failure to inhibit
responses of GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm (P < 0.05). There was a significant positive correlation between YDQ scores and BIS-11subscales
and the number of failure to inhibit responses of GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm.
Conclusion. e This study suggests that adolescents with Internet addiction exhibit more impulsivity than controls and have various comorbid
psychiatric disorders, which could be associated with the psychopathology of Internet addiction.
Ó 2007 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Internet; Disorders of environmental origin; Behavior; Addiction; Impulsivity; Adolescents

1. Introduction 18 years old. With this soaring number of Internet users, the
problem of Internet addiction has attracted high attention
The use of the Internet has increased considerably over the from psychiatrists, educators, and the public. Internet addic-
last few years. Data from China Internet Network Information tion is currently becoming a serious mental health problem
Center (CNNIC), as of June 30, 2006, showed that 123 million among Chinese adolescents. Chou and Hsiao reported that
people had gone online, of which 14.9% were teenagers below the incidence rate of Internet Addiction among Taiwan college
students was 5.9% [7]. Wu and Zhu identified 10.6% of Chi-
nese college students as Internet addiction [20].
Abbreviations: YDQ, Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addiction; Internet addiction, also described as pathological Internet
BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 11; GoStop, GoStop Impulsivity
Paradigm. use, is defined as an individual’s inability to control his or
* Corresponding author. Tel./fax: 86 731 5531781. her use of the Internet, which eventually causes psychological,
E-mail address: caofenglin@gmail.com (L. Su). social, school, and/or work difficulties in a person’s life [8,22].

0924-9338/$ - see front matter Ó 2007 Elsevier Masson SAS. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.05.004
F. Cao et al. / European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471 467

The description of Internet addiction has been based on the explained, at least in part, by abnormal functioning of the
definition for substance dependence or pathological gambling. orbitofrontal cortex [6].
It shares characteristics like preoccupation, mood modifica- Research into impulsivity found that pathological gambling,
tion, tolerance, withdrawal, and functional impairment drug addiction, and alcohol abuse have similarities in neuropsy-
[10,12]. chology and personality characteristic. If Internet addiction is
Noticeably, Internet addiction is also a problem that has been related to impulsivity, research is likely to demonstrate that
observed in different cultures. Yoo et al. conducted a study in neuropsychological characteristics may be similar to other
which 535 Korean children were assessed. This study found sig- disorders.
nificance associations between attention deficit hyperactivity Previous studies regarding adolescent Internet addiction
disorder (ADHD) symptoms and the severity of Internet addic- have investigated associated psychological variables such as
tion [21]. A survey carried out by Morahan-Martin and Schu- shyness, loneliness, self-consciousness, anxiety, depression
macher reported that among 277 U.S. college students and interpersonal relations. However, few studies about the re-
pathological Internet users were more likely to be males and lationship between Internet Addiction and impulsivity have
to use online games as well as technologically sophisticated been done.
sites [16]. With a Chinese Internet-related addictive behavior The objective of this study was to assess whether Internet
Inventory version II and Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet addiction is related to impulsivity.
Addiction (YDQ), Chou and Hsiao explored Internet addiction
in 910 Taiwanese college students and identified the high com- 2. Methods
munication pleasure score as a high predictor for Internet
dependence [7]. This study was performed in two stages. First we screened
Since the initial recognition of Internet addiction, several for the presence of Internet addiction within a nonclinical
assessment instruments have been developed. Brenner devel- group using the Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addic-
oped an Internet Related Addictive Behavior Inventory tion (YDQ) that has been commonly used. According to the
(IRABI) [4], which has 32 trueefalse questions that assess modified YDQ criteria by Beard and structured clinical inter-
users’ Internet experiences. Morahan Martin and Schumacker view (excluding current psychiatric comorbidity), 50 students
introduced their scale PIUS (Pathological Internet Use Scale) who were diagnosed as Internet addicted (mean age,
[16], with 13 questions to assess whether heavy Internet use 14.8  1.4 years) and 50 students on normal Internet usage
negatively affects academic and other work, interpersonal re- (mean age, 14.5  1.8 years) were included in a caseecontrol
lations, individual stress levels, social withdrawal, and mood study. The two groups were assessed using Barratt Impulsive-
alteration. In 2002, Caplan published the Generalized Prob- ness Scale-11 (BIS-11) and behavioral measure of impulsivity
lematic Internet Use Scale (GPIUS) [5]. In addition, (GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm).
a widely-used eight-item Internet Addiction Diagnostic Ques-
tionnaire (YDQ) was developed by Young [23], partly adapted 2.1. Participants
from DSM-IV criteria for pathological gambling. Young also
created a 20-item questionnaire, called the Internet Addiction Four high schools were selected randomly in Changsha
Test (IAT) [3]. Up to now, there exists yet no Internet Addic- City (the capital of Hunan, in South-central China, a city
tion diagnosis in the DSM system. with a population of over 6 million). Over all four schools,
By using the DSM-IV criteria, some authors suggest Internet we then adopted a 2-stage sampling method to select 3 classes
addiction is an impulse disorder or at least related to impulse respectively from four grades, namely, 1st grade (mean age,
control [2,23]. Several studies have revealed a correlation be- 13.1 years) and 2nd grade (mean age, 14.3 years) in junior
tween impulsivity and pathological gambling, substance abuse, high schools and 1st grade (mean age, 16.5 years) and 2nd
and alcohol abuse. Barnes et al. found that impulsivity was a sig- grade (mean age: 17.1 years) in senior high schools. From
nificant predictor of alcohol misuse for females and delinquency the selected classes, all students (a total of 2787 students) par-
for males [1]. Vitaro et al. used a prospective-longitudinal de- ticipated in this study and completed the self-report question-
sign to investigate whether impulsivity measured in 12e14- naires in class after the researchers had explained the
year-olds could predict problem gambling in late adolescence. procedures and requirements. Questionnaires were collected
They validated ‘‘a self-report measure of impulsiveness and immediately after they were completed. A final 2620 eligible
a card-sorting task that significantly predicted problem gam- questionnaires remained. The response rate was 94%.
bling (even after controlling for socio-demographic variables), Among the 2620 students, 100% were aged between 12 and
early gambling behavior and other personality variables such 18 years, with the average age being 15.2  3.5 years. A sum-
as aggressiveness and anxiety’’ [19]. Moelle et al. found that im- mary of the participant characteristics is given in Table 1.
pulsivity is a significant predictor of cocaine use and treatment School approval and parental consent were obtained before
retention [15]. Cavedini et al. also explored the relationship be- participation in the study. Investigators visited schools, ex-
tween the ventromedial orbitofrontal circuits and pathological plained the purpose of the study to students and teachers,
gambling. The results suggest the existence of a link between and also informed the parents the objective of the study, a guar-
pathological gambling and drug addiction, all having dimin- antee of confidentiality, and a contact telephone number of the
ished ability to consider future consequences, which may be prime investigator for any questions and concerns by sending
468 F. Cao et al. / European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471

Table 1 intended? (6) Have you jeopardized or risked the loss of sig-
Age and gender distribution of subjects nificant relationship, job, educational or career opportunity be-
Grade Age Male students Female students Total cause of the Internet? (7) Have you lied to family members,
(years) n (%) n (%) n (%) therapist, or others to conceal the extent of involvement with
Grade 1 of junior 12e14 376 (54.1) 319 (45.9) 695 (26.5) the Internet? (8) Do you use the Internet as a way of escaping
high school from problems or of relieving a dysphoric mood (e.g., feelings
Grade 2 of junior 13e15 325 (51.3) 308 (48.7) 633 (24.2) of helplessness, guilt, anxiety, or depression)? Young asserted
high school that five or more yes responses to the eight questions indicate
Grade 1 of senior 15e17 296 (45.5) 355 (54.5) 651 (24.8)
high school
a dependent user. Beard modified the YDQ criteria. Respon-
Grade 2 of senior 16e18 319 (49.8) 322 (50.2) 641 (24.5) dents who answered ‘‘yes’’ to questions 1 through 5 and at
high school least any one of the remaining three questions were classified
Total 1316 (50.2) 1304 (49.8) 2620 (100.0) as suffering from Internet addiction [2]. Beard stated that the
modification may help strengthen Young’s proposed criteria.
In previous publications about YDQ, the split-half reliability
a letter. All parents were assured that they were free to refuse was 0.729 and the Cronbach a was 0.713 [11]. In our study,
if they did not agree with the objective of the study. The re- the calculation of a SpearmaneBrown coefficient resulted in
search project has been approved by Ethics Committee of a split-half reliability of 0.719.The consistency of the YDQ
the Second Xiangya Hospital. was tested with Cronbach’s a 0.722. Thus, the YDQ has
a good reliability and consistency.
2.2. Group selection

Internet addicted individuals who met the modified YDQ 2.3.3. Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 11 (BIS-11)
criteria were administered a structured clinical interview (K- BIS-11 is a questionnaire on which participants rate their
SADS) for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disor- frequency of several common impulsive or nonimpulsive be-
ders, Fourth Edition. Participants who evidenced any DSM-IV haviors/traits on a scale from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost al-
axis I disorder were not included. The diagnosis was made on ways/always). The 11th version of the BIS consists of 30 items
the basis of clinical observation and history provided by the and can be divided into three subscales including attentional
parents and the patients themselves. Finally, 50 students who key, motor key, non-planning key, to determine overall impul-
were diagnosed as Internet addicted were included in the siveness scores, all items were summed, with higher scores in-
case group. Fifty normal students were matched by gender, dicating greater impulsivity [17]. We used the translated
age and educational levels from among the lists of students version by Li et al.; its split-half reliability was 0.752, the
with the same age, gender, level of education and class. Inde- testeretest reliability was 0.825, and the Cronbach a coeffi-
pendent t-tests showed that age and educational levels did not cient was 0.794 [13].
differ between the Internet Addiction group and the control
group (P > 0.05). Chi-square test indicated no differences be- 2.3.4. GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm (GoStop)
tween the groups in distribution of gender (P > 0.05). GoStop is a response disinhibition procedure for assessing
the capacity to inhibit an already initiated response. For the
2.3. Measures GoStop, subjects are shown a series of 5-digit numbers in
black on a computer screen. The randomly generated 5-digit
2.3.1. Basic Information Questionnaire numbers appear for 500 ms, once every 2 s (500 ms on,
We used the Basic Information Questionnaire to collect de- 1500 ms off). Participants are told to respond when the num-
mographic information such as age, gender, educational levels, ber they see is identical to the previous number; this is a tar-
and Internet experience. get trial. Half of all target trials feature a target-stop trial,
when the color of the matching target’s numerals changes
2.3.2. Diagnostic Questionnaire for from black to red at 50, 150, 250, or 350 ms after its presen-
Internet Addiction (YDQ) tation. Participants are instructed to respond to the identically
YDQ was adapted from DSM-IV criteria for pathological matching numbers before the number disappears from the
gambling by Young [23]. YDQ consisting of eight ‘‘yes’’ or screen, but not to respond to a number that turns red. Target
‘‘no’’ questions was translated into Chinese. It includes the and target-stop trials each occur 25% of the time. The re-
following questions: (1) Do you feel preoccupied with the In- maining 50% of the trials consist of numbers that differ ran-
ternet (think about previous online activity or anticipate next domly from the previous number. Within the target-stop
online session)? (2) Do you feel the need to use the Internet trials, the interval the target remains black (go) before turn-
with increasing amounts of time in order to achieve satisfac- ing red (stop) has an equal probability of a 50, 150, 250, or
tion? (3) Have you repeatedly made unsuccessful efforts to 350 ms duration [9].
control, cut back, or stop Internet use? (4) Do you feel restless, The failure to inhibit responses was used as an indicator of
moody, depressed, or irritable when attempting to cut down or ability to withhold responses when presented with a stop-signal.
stop Internet use? (5) Do you stay online longer than originally The number of target-stop stimuli that the participant did not
F. Cao et al. / European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471 469

respond to was used to calculate the number of failure to inhibit attentional key, motor key, and total scores for the Internet
responses from the total presentations for each delay condition. Addiction group (P < 0.05).
A computer with a 14-inch monitor was used to administer As can be seen in Table 3, there was significant difference
GoStop. Participants were asked to use their dominant hand between the groups’ ability to withhold responses. The Inter-
when responding on the task. They received standardized net Addiction group demonstrated more difficulty withholding
task instructions before the computer tasks. All subjects com- responses than the control group at the 50, 150, 250, and 350
pleted one session of GoStop task. stop signal delays.

2.4. Statistical analysis 3.4. Relationship between Internet


addiction and impulsivity
Group differences in demographic variables were computed
using independent t-test and chi-square test. The differences in Pearson correlations were used to examine the relationship
BIS-11 and GoStop scores between Internet addiction group between YDQ scores and impulsivity level. Table 4 shows
and the control group were analyzed using independent t- that, YDQ scores correlated positively with BIS-11 attentional
test. The correlation between Internet addiction and BIS-11 key, motor key, and non-planning key scores. There was a sig-
and GoStop scores was assessed using Pearson’s correlations nificant positive correlation between YDQ scores and the
analysis. Statistical significances were defined at the 0.05 number of failures to inhibit responses of GoStop. There
level, two-tailed. was also a significant positive correlation between BIS-11
and GoStop scores. All correlations demonstrated statistical
3. Results significance (P < 0.05).

3.1. Ongoing main Internet activities


4. Discussion
According to the students’ self-report, the Internet was used
This study primarily focused on examining the relationship
mainly for virtual games (78.1%), chat room activity (14.2%),
between impulsivity and Internet addiction among Chinese ad-
school-related activities (4.1%), and other (3.6%). There was olescents. As predicted, Internet addiction subjects were more
no significant difference between the Internet Addiction group impulsive than controls as measured by both the BIS-11 and
and the control group (P > 0.05). GoStop. The Pearson correlation analysis indicated there was
a moderate positive relationship between YDQ scores and
3.2. Psychiatric comorbidity BIS-11 scores, and the number of failures to inhibit responses.
This study supports the concept that Internet addiction should
Of the 2620 participants who fully completed the survey, 64 be considered as an impulse control disorder [23]. Similar re-
participants met the criteria for Internet addiction. Of the 64 search in other countries has also pointed out this relationship.
participants with Internet addiction, 1 participant fulfilled For example, a study by Treuer et al. reported that Internet ad-
DSM-IV criteria for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), 2 diction is a new subtype of impulse control disorder [18].
for conduct disorder (CD), 2 for oppositional defiant disorder Since the BIS-11 is considered more of a trait measure of
(ODD), 1 for depressive disorder, and 8 for attention deficit impulsivity, this supports the hypothesis that impulsivity is
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). a risk factor for the development of Internet addiction. How-
ever, since subjects were not abstinent from Internet overuse,
3.3. Comparison of impulsivity between Internet it cannot be ruled out that at least part of the increase in impul-
Addiction group and control group sivity in Internet Addiction subjects was due to effects of In-
ternet addiction. Further research is needed to conclusively
A comparison of average BIS-11 scores in the Internet Ad- determine whether impulsivity per se is a significant risk factor
diction group and control group is shown in Table 2. Compared for development of Internet addiction, or a result of Internet
to the control group, we observed significantly higher addiction, or both.

Table 2 Table 3
Difference between Internet Addiction group and the control on BIS-11 Difference between Internet Addiction group and the control on the number of
(mean  SD) failure to inhibit responses (GoStop) (mean  SD)
Variable Internet Addiction group Control group t P Trial type Trials Internet Addiction group Control group t P
(n ¼ 50) (n ¼ 50) (n ¼ 50) (n ¼ 50)
Attentional key 31.47  4.20 28.88  4.31 3.03 <0.05 50 ms Stop 20 6.96  2.44 3.66  1.70 7.85 <0.05
Motor key 24.33  3.94 22.20  4.20 2.58 <0.05 150 ms Stop 20 11.76  2.14 7.50  3.51 7.32 <0.05
Non-planning key 20.57  2.71 19.50  3.06 1.85 >0.05 250 ms Stop 20 15.10  2.35 10.86  2.88 8.07 <0.05
Total scores 76.37  8.56 70.69  9.77 3.06 <0.05 350 ms Stop 20 17.66  1.60 14.00  2.22 9.45 <0.05
BIS-11, Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11. GoStop, GoStop Impulsivity Paradigm.
470 F. Cao et al. / European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471

Table 4
Correlation matrix between psychometric variables
YDQ Attentional key Motor key Non-planning key 50 ms Stop 150 ms Stop 250 ms Stop 350 ms Stop
YDQ e 0.389** 0.409** 0.228* 0.461** 0.475** 0.460** 0.508**
Attentional key e 0.495** 0.607** 0.297** 0.148 0.186 0.213*
Motor key e 0.489** 0.225* 0.239* 0.274* 0.275*
Non-planning key e 0.251* 0.157 0.096 0.220*
50 ms Stop e 0.516** 0.445** 0.445**
150 ms Stop e 0.627** 0.526**
250 ms Stop e 0.633**
350 ms Stop e
*P < 0.05; **P < 0.01. YDQ, Diagnostic Questionnaire for Internet Addiction.

We also found that the number of failures to inhibit re- Acknowledgements


sponses on the GoStop was correlated with BIS-11 personality
factors, suggesting that this task may measure a similar con- This work was supported by grants from National Natural
struct of impulsiveness with BIS-11 inventories. Science Foundation of China (No. 30370521) and National Nat-
This study differs from previous studies showing increased ural Science Foundation of China (No. 30570659). The authors
impulsivity in that it included a variety of different measures do have not any competing interest in relation to this paper.
of impulsiveness, including behavioral and personality mea-
sures. In previous studies a single measure of impulsivity References
was administered to Internet Addiction subjects. For example,
Lin and Tsai examined excessive Internet use of Taiwanese ad- [1] Barnes GM, Welte JW, Hoffman JH, Dintcheff BA. Shared predictors of
olescents and sensation seeking by using the Sensation Seeking youthful gambling, substance use, and delinquency. Psychol Addict
Scale Taiwan version. They found Internet-dependent adoles- Behav 2005;19:165e74.
[2] Beard KW, Wolf EM. Modification in the proposed diagnostic criteria for
cents scored significantly higher in terms of overall sensation Internet addiction. Cyberpsychol Behav 2001;4:377e83.
seeking and disinhibition than the Internet non-dependents, [3] Beard KW. Internet addiction: a review of current assessment techniques
disinhibition being the most important predictor of Internet and potential assessment questions. Cyberpsychol Behav 2005;8:7e14.
dependence [14]. [4] Brenner V. Psychology of computer use: XLVII. parameters of Internet
In addition, our results indicate that some adolescents use use, abuse and addiction: the first 90 days of the Internet usage survey.
Psycho Rep 1997;80:879e82.
the Internet excessively, and a total of 2.4% could be described [5] Caplan SE. Problematic Internet use and psychosocial well-being: devel-
to suffer from ‘‘Internet addiction’’. Owing to the different opment of a theory based cognitive-behavioral measurement instrument.
sample, and different social and cultural contexts, and also Comput Human Behav 2002;18:553e75.
the rigid modified YDQ criteria by Beard which we used, it [6] Cavedini P, Riboldi G, Keller R, D’Annucci A, Bellodi L. Frontal lobe
is difficult to compare the findings effectively. Nonetheless, dysfunction in pathological gambling patients. Biol Psychiatry 2002;
51:334e41.
the Internet Addiction percentage varied considerably. [7] Chou C, Hsiao MC. Internet addiction, usage, gratification, and pleasure ex-
The students with Internet addiction suffered from comorbid perience: the Taiwan college students’ case. Comput Educ 2000;35:65e80.
psychiatric disorders, especially comorbid ADHD. The result is [8] Davis RA. A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use.
similar to another study which found significant associations Comput Human Behav 2001;17:187e95.
between the level of ADHD symptoms and the severity of Inter- [9] Dougherty DM, Mathias CW, Marsh DM. Laboratory behavioral mea-
sures of impulsivity. Behav Res Methods 2005;37:82e90.
net Addiction in children [21]. Though we cannot conclude that [10] Hall AS, Parsons J. Internet addiction: college student case study using
Internet addiction is a cause or consequence of these disorders, best practices in cognitive behavior therapy. J Mental Health Counseling
clinicians must consider the comorbid psychiatric disorder in 2001;23:312e27.
cases of Internet addiction. [11] Johansson A, Götestam KG. Internet addiction: characteristics of a ques-
This study used YDQ scores of higher than 6 as an indicator tionnaire and prevalence in Norwegian youth (12e18 years). Scand J
Psychol 2004;45:223e9.
of Internet addiction. Although this questionnaire is a frequently [12] Leung L. Net-Generation attributes and seductive properties of the Inter-
used instrument for assessing Internet addiction, its validity as net as predictors of online activities and Internet addiction. Cyberpsychol
a diagnostic instrument has been questioned [3]. Future studies Behav 2004;7:333e48.
may utilize other measures of assessing diagnostic criteria for [13] Li F, Su Ly, Geng YG. Reliability and validity of the Barratt Impulsive-
Internet addiction or severity of Internet addiction problems ness Scale in Chinese children. Chin J Clin Psychol 2006;14:115e7.
[14] Lin SSJ, Tsai CC. Sensation seeking and Internet dependence of Taiwa-
to assess the relationship between impulsivity and Internet nese high school adolescents. Comput Human Behav 2002;18:411e26.
addiction problems. [15] Moeller FG, Dougherty DM, Barratt ES, Schmitz JM, Swann AC,
In conclusion, the results of this study show a specific rela- Grabowski J. The impact of impulsivity on cocaine use and retention
tionship between impulsivity and Internet addiction and sup- in treatment. J Subst Abuse Treat 2001;21:193e8.
port the need for further research on impulsivity as a risk [16] Morahan-Martin J, Schumacher P. Incidence and correlates of patholog-
ical Internet use among college students. Comput Human Behav
factor for the development of Internet addiction. Subsequent 2000;16:13e29.
studies may investigate further the origin of impulsivity and [17] Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impul-
its relationship to Internet addiction. siveness scale. J Clin Psychol 1995;51:768e74.
F. Cao et al. / European Psychiatry 22 (2007) 466e471 471

[18] Treuer T, Fabian Z, Furedi J. Internet addiction associated with features [21] Yoo HJ, Cho SC, Ha J, Yune CK, Kim SJ, Hwang J, et al. Attention
of impulse control disorder: is it a real psychiatric disorder? J Affect deficit hyperactivity symptoms and Internet addiction. Psychiatry Clin
Disord 2001;66:283. Neurosci 2004;58:487e94.
[19] Vitaro F, Arseneault L, Tremblay RE. Impulsivity predicts problem gam- [22] Young KS, Rogers RC. The relationship between depression and Internet
bling in low SES adolescent males. Addiction 1999;94:565e75. addiction. Cyberpsychol Behav 1998;1:25e8.
[20] Wu HR, Zhu KJ. Path analysis on related factors causing Internet [23] Young KS. Internet addiction: The emergence of a new clinical disorder
addiction disorder in college students. Chin J Public Health 2004;20: [OnLine]. 1996. Available at: <http://www.netaddiction.com/articles/
1363e4. newdisorder.html>.

You might also like