You are on page 1of 9

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/243966350

What (or who) causes health inequalities: Theories, evidence and


implications?

Article  in  Health Policy · June 2013


DOI: 10.1016/j.healthpol.2013.05.021 · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

49 2,617

3 authors:

Gerry McCartney Chik Collins


NHS Health Scotland University of the West of Scotland
202 PUBLICATIONS   1,210 CITATIONS    42 PUBLICATIONS   433 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Mhairi Mackenzie
University of Glasgow
43 PUBLICATIONS   962 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Evaluation of the Links Worker Programme in ‘Deep End’ general practices in Glasgow View project

Research Associate View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Gerry McCartney on 22 December 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached
copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research
and education use, including for instruction at the authors institution
and sharing with colleagues.
Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or
licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party
websites are prohibited.
In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the
article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or
institutional repository. Authors requiring further information
regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are
encouraged to visit:
http://www.elsevier.com/authorsrights
Author's personal copy

Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Health Policy
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/healthpol

What (or who) causes health inequalities: Theories, evidence


and implications?
Gerry McCartney a,∗ , Chik Collins b , Mhairi Mackenzie c
a
Public Health Observatory Division, NHS Health Scotland, Elphinstone House, 65 West Regent Street, Glasgow G2 2AF, Scotland, United
Kingdom
b
Politics & Sociology Group, School of Social Sciences, University of West of Scotland, Paisley PA1 2BE, Scotland, United Kingdom
c
Public Policy, Urban Studies, School of Social & Political Sciences/Institute of Health & Wellbeing, University of Glasgow, 27 Bute
Gardens, Glasgow G12 8RS, Scotland, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Health inequalities are the unjust differences in health between groups of people occupying
Received 6 February 2012 different positions in society. Since the Black Report of 1980 there has been considerable
Received in revised form 29 April 2013
effort to understand what causes them, so as to be able to identify actions to reduce them.
Accepted 29 May 2013
This paper revisits and updates the proposed theories, evaluates the evidence in light of
subsequent epidemiological research, and underlines the political and policy ramifications.
Keywords:
The Black Report suggested four theories (artefact, selection, behavioural/cultural and
Health inequalities
Health inequities structural) as to the root causes of health inequalities and suggested that structural the-
Social class ory provided the best explanation. These theories have since been elaborated to include
Theory intelligence and meritocracy as part of selection theory. However, the epidemiological evi-
dence relating to the proposed causal pathways does not support these newer elaborations.
They may provide partial explanations or insights into the mechanisms between cause and
effect, but structural theory remains the best explanation as to the fundamental causes of
health inequalities.
The paper draws out the vitally important political and policy implications of this assess-
ment. Health inequalities cannot be expected to reduce substantially as a result of policy
aimed at changing health behaviours, particularly in the face of wider public policy that
militates against reducing underlying social inequalities. Furthermore, political rhetoric
about the need for ‘cultural change’, without the required changes in the distribution of
power, income, wealth, or in the regulatory frameworks in society, is likely to divert from
necessary action.
© 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction [1]. They occur across a range of social dimensions includ-


ing income, social class, deprivation,caste, ethnicity and
Health inequalities1 are the, “systematic differences in geography. Mere variations in health outcomes within a
the health of people occupying unequal positions in society” population do not necessarily represent inequalities; they
do soonly if those variations are patterned by some char-
acteristic of the population which renders the variations
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 0141 354 2928. unfair. Populations which have large health inequalities
E-mail addresses: gmccartney@nhs.net (G. McCartney), affecting the majority of the population are likely to have
chik.collins@uws.ac.uk (C. Collins), mhairi.mackenzie@glasgow.ac.uk
(M. Mackenzie).
1
Some authors use inequalities to denote differences between groups
and inequities to denote unjust differences between groups, but this commonly used term “inequality” has been adopted throughout this arti-
distinction is not consistently applied across the literature. The more cle to describe unjust differences.

0168-8510/$ – see front matter © 2013 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.healthpol.2013.05.021
Author's personal copy

222 G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227

large health variations. In contrast, if a health inequal- 2. Health inequalities theory I: the artefact theory
ity affects only a small proportion of the population (e.g.
amongst stigmatised benefit recipients or a minority eth- The artefact view proposes that the association between
nic or migrant population) the variation across the whole markers of social status and health outcomes is a statistical
population may be small, even when the gap between the artefact relating to the way in which social status has been
two groups is large. classified over time [5,14].
As with poverty measures, inequalities in health can be The theory is gravely undermined by the ubiquitous
considered in absolute or relative terms. This can be impor- demonstration of inequalities in health outcomes [21],
tant when there are secular trends in the population health even where different statistical measures of social status
mean (e.g. a downward trend in the mean can increase rel- are used (including income, area deprivation, education,
ative inequalities even whilst absolute differences remain social class and occupational group). In light of this, it is
stable). Methods of enumerating health inequalities con- very difficult to sustain a theory that such outcomes are
sequently vary depending on which inequality is of most unrelated to social status. Consequently, this theory can
interest [2,3]. confidently be discarded – as indeed it has been since at
Health inequalities are persistent through time and least the time of the Black Report. However, this is not
have been found in most countries where they have been to suggest that improved measures of social status, or,
investigated [4]. Yet they represent the starkest and most perhaps better, of the social realities of people’s ‘lived expe-
profound inequalities: the right to life itself is at stake. In rience’, could not be found.
the UK, inequality in health and its causes were investi-
gated in detail in 1980 [5] and have been the explicit focus
of policy since at least 1997. Despite this attention, there is 3. Health inequalities theory II: selection theory
little or no evidence that these inequalities have narrowed
[6,7]. 3.1. ‘Health selection’
Theories of health inequalities matter, for the obvious
reason that the successful identification of causes of any The possibility that a health selection effect might
problem is crucial to the elaboration of appropriate meas- explain inequalities was examined, and quite decisively
ures to address the problem. The Black Report, published rejected, in the Black report. The theory is essentially
in 1980, identified four key theories for understanding that of reverse causation: that poor health causes a social
how health inequalities arise [5]. These were: artefact; selection (a ‘social slide’) which leads to the observed
selection (including natural and social selection); struc- association between ill health and low social status
tural factors; and behaviours (including culture). Since [5,22].
then, there have been elaborations of these underlying This ‘health selection’ theory can be tested using lon-
theories [6,8–11] (including that of MacIntyre which dis- gitudinal studies which measure pre-morbid social status
tinguishes between ‘hard’ and ‘soft’ versions of each) and test for an association with subsequent morbidity and
[12]; different approaches to categorising the under- mortality. A large number of such longitudinal studies
lying causal mechanisms [13], and numerous UK and have subsequently demonstrated that the vast majority
international reviews of health inequalities tasked with (although not all) of the concentration of ill-health in
recommending policy measures to bring about their reduc- lower social groups is explained by pre-morbid social status
tion [14–18]. rather than any subsequent social slide. Such evidence indi-
In light of empirical developments, the manifest failure cates that this view fails to account for health inequalities
of policy, and a global financial crisis with a near ubiquitous [23–25].
response of inequality-heightening public expenditure,
and particularly welfare cuts, it seems necessary to revisit 3.2. Intelligence
and critically appraise the main theories on how health
inequalities arise, so as to aid clarity in thinking about how Despite the rejection of health selection as a major
best to address them. explanation of health inequalities, an attempt has been
In reviewing these theories we utilise the broad cat- made to reinvigorate selection hypothesis more recently –
egorisation employed in the Black Report – which has in particular by those proposing a role for intelligence [26].
had common currency both internationally and over time. Intelligence and health are closely associated, but this
Where some recent contributors have identified more than could be due to:
the four key theories identified by Black, we have treated
these additional theories as sub-categories within Black’s
four-way categorisation [19,20]. 1. Chance (which can be discounted on the basis of the
We seek to evaluate the extant theories as to how cumulative, statistically significant evidence of associ-
and why health inequalities arise, including the most ation) [27,28];
recent elaborations, using basic epidemiological reason- 2. Reverse causation where the differences in intelligence
ing relating to association, causality and confounding. are caused by differences in health (a possibility for
In doing this, we help clarify which retain validity, and measures of intelligence gathered in later life which may
in what respects they may do so, and we briefly draw be affected by stroke disease or similar, but which can be
out the vital political and policy ramifications which discounted given the association between pre-morbid
emerge. intelligence and later health outcomes) [27,28];
Author's personal copy

G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227 223

a) Socioeconomic status 3.3. Meritocracy


and early years nurture
(effect modifiers) Another relatively recent iteration of selection theory
Intelligence has been the social mobility – or meritocracy – theorem
Health
derived from [40]. This has arisen in light of findings showing greater
genotype
relative health inequalities in Scandinavia, as compared
to southern Europe [4]. The theory is that in Scandina-
vian countries, which are seen to have been more social
b) democratic and meritocratic than most other countries,
Socioeconomic Health ‘more able’ individuals, born into lower socio-economic
status and
early years groups (characterised as those with higher ‘intelligence’),
nurture Intelligence rise to a higher socio-economic group in adulthood. On
the other hand, ‘less able’ individuals, born into higher
Fig. 1. Logical explanations for the association between intelligence, socio-economic groups, experience a socio-economic slide.
health and socioeconomic status.
Consequently, there is an accumulation of those at high-
est risk of ill health by adulthood in the lower groups, and
3. Intelligence being due to genetic endowment and this an accumulation of those at lowest risk of ill health in the
determining subsequent health (and other correlates higher groups. In less ‘meritocratic’ societies, by contrast,
such as social status, education, etc.); those with the highest risk of poor health would be spread
4. Intelligence and health both being caused by another between socio-economic groups – since groups at the bot-
factor (i.e. a confounder such as socioeconomic status tom of the socio-economic gradient would tend less to ‘lose’
or early life experiences). their ‘more able’ (and, therefore, healthier) constituents
through upward social mobility, whilst those groups at the
top would tend to be more able to protect their least able
Only 3 and 4 remain plausible (Fig. 1). If intelligence (and least healthy) members from a social slide. This would,
is a function of genetics, the most common markers of it is argued, inhibit any health inequality gap.
socioeconomic status (including social class, deprivation The meritocracy theorem is therefore similar to intel-
and income) would only be ‘effect modifiers’ in the rela- ligence theory, with the added insight that nations
tionship between intelligence and health (i.e. if people of characterised as ‘meritocratic’ (those in which ‘intelli-
equal social class, deprivation or income were examined, gence’, ‘ability’, or the confounders that it might actually be
the variation in intelligence within that group would be measuring, such as early years’ experience, class or depri-
associated with health outcomes) (Fig. 1a). vation, result in marked social selection and stratification)
When the relationship between intelligence and would be likely to have greater apparent inequalities in
health is adjusted for markers of socioeconomic sta- health.
tus, the association with mortality weakens in some What does this mean for social mobility as an explana-
cohorts [29–31] and disappears completely in oth- tory theory for health inequalities? Similar to the case for
ers [32]. In general, residual confounding is common the role of intelligence, it assumes that variation in ‘abil-
where only single markers of socioeconomic status ity’ across the population is unrelated to socio-economic
are used in examining associations [23,33–35]. Fur- position, early years nurture and the wider social context
thermore, in a study measuring intelligence in early in which people grow up. If ‘ability’ is actually a parameter
life, before the full effects of social factors have of this context, then the links are similar to that shown in
become embedded, the independent impact of intelli- Fig. 1b. A further problem with the theory is its somewhat
gence on subsequent self-reported health and negative partial understanding of the Scandinavian welfare systems.
health behaviours was very small [27,36]. This sug- Whilst it is true that social democratic countries are likely
gests that the earlier intelligence is measured in life, to manifest greater social mobility, they also have stronger
the less the independent effect on health outcomes, universal welfare systems, more progressive taxation and
a finding which would support the view of intelli- higher levels of social solidarity, all of which would mitigate
gence as a consequence of socio-structural influences against the deleterious effects of any ‘social slide’ [41,42].
(Fig. 1b).
There are two additional issues which challenge the 4. Health inequalities theory III: cultural and
role of intelligence as the fundamental cause of health behavioural
inequalities. The first is the ‘Flynn Effect’ – the secular
increase in measures of intelligence witnessed in various 4.1. Health behaviours as the cause
populations [37,38]. The second is that the differences in
intelligence between populations have varied over time Cultural and behavioural theories suggest that differ-
[39]. Both these features make it less likely that intelligence ences in the prevalence of behaviours such as smoking,
is principally genetically determined – since genotype can- alcohol consumption, illicit drug-taking, diet and physi-
not evolve over a single generation. Rather, socio-economic cal activity between groups, or differences in the dominant
and contextual features are more likely to explain the cultures between groups, are fundamental causes of health
changing population variation in the measures of intelli- inequalities. For example, many analyses suggest that risk
gence. factors, such as smoking, explain a large proportion of the
Author's personal copy

224 G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227

Socio-economic factors Lewis [53], and more recently ‘underclass’/‘dependency


Differences in the (effect modifiers)
prevalence of culture’ theory, associated in particular with Charles Mur-
Health inequalies
negave health ray [54,55]. The latter two theories are of primary interest
behaviours here. Each contends that certain poor populations tend to
develop aberrant cultural patterns which have destructive
Fig. 2. The relation between health behaviours, socio-economic factors
and negative implications for social and health out-
and health inequalities in the behavioural theory.
comes. For Lewis, such a ‘culture of poverty’ tended to be
self-perpetuating – even when the broader structural envi-
inequality in health outcomes [43]. However, for health ronment which gave rise to it changed, allowing for better
behaviours to be the cause of health inequalities, socio- outcomes. For Murray, on the other hand, the rootedness
economic factors would have to be only an effect modifier of the ‘culture of dependency’ in the structures of gov-
in the relationship as shown in Fig. 2. ernment welfare required a new ‘responsibilization’ of the
It is clear that a whole plethora of unhealthy behaviours poor – linked to a substantial withdrawal of state provision.
are more prevalent in lower socio-economic groups [23]. This is reflected in the current UK Government narrative
However, a simple focus on behaviours as a fundamen- of ‘welfare reform’ (and to a lesser degree also in that
tal theory of health inequalities has two problems. First, of its ‘New Labour’ predecessor). The implication of both
when populations in different social groups, but with equal theories is that behaviours reflect cultural patterns which
exposure to behavioural risk factors, are compared, mor- become inter-generational, entrenched and rather resis-
tality remains higher in the lower socio-economic groups tant to remediation. Culturally generated and sustained
[44]. Second, and more importantly, a simple focus on patterns occasion problems for which individuals and com-
behaviours ignores how and why individuals in particu- munities themselves must to a substantial degree carry, or
lar social groups adopt unhealthy behaviours [45]. When be made to carry, the primary responsibility.
the patterning of health behaviours has been examined However, the existence of a separate sub-culture within
in adulthood and related to socio-economic exposures countries such as the United Kingdom (UK) is disputed
throughout the life-course, the patterning of adult health [56,57] and Small, Harding and Lamont cite substantial evi-
behaviours is explained by the earlier socio-economic dence showing that among the poor in the United States
exposures [46]. Furthermore, the link between negative of America, mainstream values are widely shared and
health behaviours and lower social status has almost disap- adopted [58]. Even if it were to be accepted that a dif-
peared over time in some populations without a weakening ferent, or somehow more extreme, culture exists in some
of the link between lower social status and mortality [47]. contexts, there is no evidence to suggest that such a cul-
Changes over time in the cause-specific mortalities ture would somehow precede, or operate without strong
affecting lower socio-economic groups suggest that remov- links to, underlying socioeconomic inequalities [58–60].
ing one particular exposure (e.g. alcohol abuse) changes Indeed, the definition of culture as “the collective adap-
one high cause-specific mortality rate for another [8,48,49]. tation to everyday working and living conditions” makes
For example, during the 19th Century in Western Europe such links explicit [61]. It is therefore more likely that struc-
mortality from infectious disease was more common in tural influences are in fact key. Culture may provide some
lower social groups. During the 20th Century this mech- helpful explanations as to the mechanisms linking socio-
anism of death was superseded as a major driver of economic circumstances with health outcomes, allowing
inequalities by ischaemic heart disease, cancer and respi- for greater complexity, feedback loops and emergent prop-
ratory disease. By the late 20th Century the mechanisms erties to be conceptualised. However, the candidacy of
had again evolved – with violence, drug/alcohol-related cultural theory as an explanation for the fundamental
and suicide mortality becoming more important [50]. It cause of inequalities, independent of socioeconomic cir-
seems, therefore, that while inequalities in mortality have cumstances, remains weak.
persisted, the particular mechanisms driving them have
changed. Thus, if a behavioural thesis is to provide a 5. Health inequalities theory IV: structural
valid fundamental explanation, it needs to be able to
explain, without reference to the prevailing circumstances Structural theory has provided the dominant frame for
of lower socio-economic groups, how a whole range of analysis in all of the independent reviews of health inequal-
persistently damaging, but specifically different, health ities in the UK [5,16–18]. The theory is that differences in
behaviours have arisen in these groups, consistently and the socioeconomic circumstances of social groups (includ-
over a very long period. Furthermore, the differentials in ing differences in income, wealth, power, environment
power, income and social circumstances between groups and access), at all stages of the life-course, cause differ-
would have to be explained away as an incidental finding. ences in health outcomes [62]. Structural theorists of health
All of this seems highly implausible. inequalities see competing explanations as subordinate:
that is, behaviours, culture and intelligence may theorise
4.2. Cultural theories potential mechanisms linking structural determinants and
health outcomes, but do not identify the causal roots of
Closely related to behavioural theory is culturally ori- health inequalities [12].
entated theory. There have been various expressions of Supporting the above view is evidence that health
this over time, including: Durkheim’s theory of ‘anomie’ inequalities have reduced in periods when structural
[51,52], the ‘culture of poverty’ thesis associated with Oscar inequalities have diminished, and have risen when such
Author's personal copy

G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227 225

inequalities have increased [63,64]; that the health of com- the distribution of power, income, wealth, or in the regula-
munities has improved when they have been given more tory frameworks in society, are likely to be ineffective – and
resources by chance [65]; and, most convincingly, that the also a diversion from the necessary focus and appropriate
people with the most resources within any society are action. The evidence base to support structural interven-
always the healthiest, regardless of their behaviours [15]. tions in one particular area over another remains patchy
Even where a health condition is clearly attributable to a [77], but those which have the widest implications (such as
genetic mutation (such as cystic fibrosis), inequalities in those which narrow income differentials and those which
mortality by social class are wide and vary depending on democratise power) seem most likely to be effective at
changing contextual factors [66]. reducing inequalities [75,78].
Common to all the dimensions of inequality are power
imbalances [67]. This raises important questions about
7. Conclusion
which systems most perpetrate inequalities, who gains
from inequalities, and what can be done to reduce them
Health inequalities have been, and continue to be, best
[68]. This, in turn, has led to a growing recognition of the
explained from a structural theoretical perspective. The-
importance of the political dimension of broader structural
ories focusing primarily on behaviour and culture can
processes in shaping health outcomes. Some have argued,
provide some insights around the mechanisms through
convincingly, that the proliferation of health inequalities
which such inequalities are generated, but they cannot
is linked to the diminution of broader democratic controls
provide sufficient explanation as to their principal causes.
over the preferred priorities of the rich and powerful. Such
The broader, politically determined social inequali-
growing inequalities of power and health has marked much
ties, which in turn determine health inequalities, are not
of the past 30 years in many nations of the world, but is not,
inevitable: only 40 years ago the gap between rich and poor
as experience in previous decades and in other parts of the
in the UK and in the USA was much narrower than today.
world has shown, inevitable [67,69–71].
Health inequalities have grown in synchrony with income
and power inequalities and are highly likely to diminish if
6. Why the theory of inequalities matters
income and power is redistributed [63,79]. Structural the-
ory indicates that the ‘problem’ we are dealing with here is
Distinguishing between the underlying causes and
not caused by the poor, but is caused more fundamentally
ensuing mechanisms is vitally important if we are to take
by the actions of the rich and powerful [80]. In this light,
appropriate action to eradicate what are, by their very def-
the political dimension is increasingly recognised as vital.
inition, mortal injustices. Policy rhetoric in the UK and
Consequently, in common with other challenges to public
around the world recognises that the contrasting socio-
health, the structural causes of health inequalities become
economic circumstances of people’s lives provide the best
the focus of ‘denialism’. Such denialism entails the selective
explanation as to how health inequalities arise among them
deployment of a range of characteristic tactics and tech-
[15,72]. There is, however, less understanding as to how
niques to undermine the case for interventions which are
these contrasting socioeconomic circumstances are shaped
backed by a legitimate scientific consensus, but which are
and determined by political processes and choices [68,73].
opposed by vested interests [81,82]. There is evidence that
It is clear that health inequalities will persist unless
this has been the approach of various governments over
their actual causes (socioeconomic circumstances and the
time, not least as exemplified by the paradox of rhetorical
political processes and choices which determine them)
commitments to tackling health inequalities alongside the
become the key focus of action. Continued focus on inter-
development of policies that manifestly operate to exacer-
vening mechanisms (whether they be health behaviours or
bate them [83,84].
cultures) can at best contribute to overall (mean) health
As a matter of scientific clarity and intellectual honesty,
improvement. But such a focus has limited potential to
there should be no pretence or illusion that health inequal-
actually reduce inequalities, because, while broader inequal-
ities can be eliminated, or even meaningfully reduced,
ities persist or intensify, alternative mechanisms for the
without a primary focus on structural factors. In the
production of health inequalities are very likely to sub-
absence of this focus, other explanations are insufficient
stitute for those which may successfully be addressed
to understand how inequalities arise, and so the policy foci
[8,48,49]. The overwhelming balance of evidence is that
which emerge from them will in the future, as they have in
interventions which focus on individual behaviours are
the past, lead to wasted effort, a lack of appropriate focus
least likely to prove effective; interventions which utilise
and, most importantly, continuing sustained and unjust
taxation, legislation, regulation and changes in the broader
premature mortality in society.
distribution of income and power in society are much more
likely to do so [11,15,74–76]. Health inequalities are first
and foremost an emergent effect of political decisions, and References
the collective processes and actions within societies which
shape those decisions. [1] Graham H. The challenge of health inequalities. In: Graham H, editor.
The policy implications flowing from all of the above Understanding health inequalities. Maidenhead: Open University
Press; 2009.
are important. Health inequalities cannot be expected to [2] Regidor E. Measures of health inequalities: part 2. JECH
reduce substantially as a result of policy aimed at changing 2004;58:900–3, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2004.023036.
health behaviours. Furthermore, political rhetoric about [3] Regidor E. Measures of health inequalities: part 1. JECH
2004;58:858–61, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2003.015347.
the need for ‘cultural change’, in isolation from changes in
Author's personal copy

226 G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227

[4] Mackenbach JP, Stirbu I, Roskam AJR, Schaap MM, Menvielle G, Lein- [31] Batty GD, Shipley MJ, Dundas R, Macintyre S, Der G, Mortensen LH,
salu M, et al. Socioeconomic inequalities in health in 22 European et al. Does IQ explain socio-economic differentials in total and cardio-
countries. New England Journal Medicine 2008;358:2468–81. vascular disease mortality? Comparison with the explanatory power
[5] Black D, Morris JN, Smith C, Townsend P. The Black report. In: Inequal- of traditional cardiovascular disease risk factors in the Vietnam Expe-
ities in health. London: Penguin; 1980. rience Study. European Heart Journal 2009;30:1903–9.
[6] Graham H. Social determinants and their unequal distribu- [32] Singh-Manoux A, Ferrie JE, Lynch JW, Marmot M. The role of cog-
tion: clarifying policy understandings. The Millbank Quarterly nitive ability (intelligence) in explaining the association between
2004;82(1):101–24. socioeconomic position and health: evidence from the Whitehall
[7] Mackenbach JP. Has the English strategy to reduce health inequalities II prospective cohort study. American Journal of Epidemiology
failed? Social Science & Medicine 2010;71:1249e1253. 2005;161(9):831–9.
[8] Link BG, Phelan J. Social conditions as fundamental causes of disease. [33] Smith GD, Hart C, Hole D, MacKinnon P, Gillis C, Watt G, et al. Edu-
Journal of Health and Social Behaviour 1995:80–94. cation and occupational social class: which is the more important
[9] Batty GD, Deary IJ, Gottfredson LS. Premorbid (early life) IQ and indicator of mortality risk? Journal of Epidemiology & Community
later mortality risk: systematic review. Annals of Epidemiology Health 1998;52:153–60.
2007;17:278–88. [34] Huisman M, Kunst AE, Mackenbach JP. Intelligence and socioecono-
[10] Dibben C, Popham F. Are socio-economic groupings the most appro- mic inequalities in health. Lancet 2005;366:807–8.
priate method for judging health equity between countries? Journal [35] Vagero D. Commentary: intelligence in youth and all-cause mortal-
of Epidemiology & Community Health 2011;65(1):4–5. ity: some problems in a recent meta-analysis. International Journal
[11] Krieger N. Epidemiology and the people’s health: theory and context. of Epidemiology 2011;40:644–6.
New York: Oxford University Press; 2011. [36] Lawlor DA, Batty GD, Morton SMB, Deary IJ, Macintyre S,
[12] Macintyre S. The Black report and beyond what are the issues? Social Ronalds G, et al. Early life predictors of childhood intelligence:
Science and Medicine 1997;44(6):723–45. evidence from the Aberdeen children of the 1950s study. Jour-
[13] Bambra C. Health inequalities and welfare state regimes: theoreti- nal of Epidemiology and Community Health 2005;59:656–63,
cal insights on a public health ‘puzzle’. Journal of Epidemiology and http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2004.030205.
Community Health 2011;65:740–5. [37] Flynn JR. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations. Pychological Bulletin
[14] Whitehead M. The health divide. In: Inequalities in health. London: 1987;101:171–91.
Penguin; 1988. [38] Flynn JR. The mean IQ of Americans: massive gains 1932 to 1978.
[15] Commission on Social Determinants of Health. Closing the gap in a Psychological Bulletin 1984;95:29–51.
generation: Health equity through action on the social determinants [39] Tuddenham RD. Soldier intelligence in World Wars I and II. American
of health. Final Report of the Commission on Social Determinants of Psychologist 1948;3:54–6.
Health. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2008. [40] Boyle PJ, Norman P, Popham F. Social mobility: evidence that
[16] Scottish Government. Equally well: report of the ministerial task- it can widen health inequalities. Social Science and Medicine
force on health inequalities. Edinburgh: Scottish Government; 2008. 2009;68:1835–42.
[17] Acheson D. Independent inquiry into inequalities in health, (the [41] Andersen TM. Challenges to the Scandinavian welfare model. Euro-
Acheson Report). London: TSO; 1998. pean Journal of Political Economy 2004;20:743–54.
[18] Marmot M, Atkinson T, Bell J, Black C, Broadfoot P, Cumberlege J, et al. [42] Kim I-H, Muntaner C, Shahidi FV, Vives A, Vanroelen C, Benach J.
Fair society, healthy lives. London: The Marmot Review; 2010. Welfare states, flexible employment, and health: a critical review.
[19] Mackenbach JP. The persistence of health inequalities in modern wel- Health Policy 2012;104:99–127.
fare states: the explanation of a paradox. Social Science and Medicine [43] Gruer L, Hart CL, Gordon DS, Watt GCM. Effect of tobacco smoking
2012;75:761–9. on survival of men and women by social position: a 28 year cohort
[20] Bambra C. Health inequalities and welfare state regimes: theoreti- study. BMJ 2009;338:b480, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b480.
cal insights on a public health ‘puzzle’. Journal of Epidemiology & [44] Hart CL, Gruer L, Watt GCM. Cause specific mortality, social position,
Community Health 2011;65:740–5. and obesity among women who had never smoked: 28 year cohort
[21] Beckfield J, Krieger N. Epi+demos+cracy: linking political systems and study. BMJ 2011;342:d3785.
priorities to the magnitude of health inequities—evidence, gaps, and [45] Nettle D. Social class through the evolutionary lens. The Psychologist
a research agenda. Epidemiologic Reviews 2009;31:152–77. 2009;22(11):934–7.
[22] Canning D, Bowser D. Investing in health to improve the well- [46] Lynch JW, Kaplan GA, Salonen JT. Why do poor people behave poorly?
being of the disadvantaged: reversing the argument of fair society, Variation in adult health behaviours and psychosocial characteris-
healthy lives (The Marmot Review). Social Science & Medicine tics by stages of the socioeconomic lifecourse. Social Science and
2010;71:1223–6. Medicine 1997;44(6):809–19.
[23] Smith GD, Hart C, Watt DG, Hole D, Hawthorne V. Individual social [47] Stringhini S, Dugravot A, Shipley M, Goldberg M, Zins M, Kivima
class, area-based deprivation, cardiovascular disease risk factors, and M, et al. Health behaviours, socioeconomic status, and mortal-
mortality: the Renfrew and Paisley study. Journal of Epidemiology & ity: further analyses of the British Whitehall II and the French
Community Health 1998;52:399–402. GAZEL prospective cohorts. PLoS Medicine 2011;8(2):e1000419,
[24] Power C, Matthews S. Origins of health inequalities in a national http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000419.
population sample. Lancet 1997;350(9091):1584–9.
[48] Link BG, Phelan J. McKeown and the idea that social conditions are
[25] Brimblecombe N, Dorling D, Shaw M. Migration and geographi-
fundamental causes of disease. American Journal of Public Health
cal inequalities in health in Britain. Social Science and Medicine
2002;92(5):730–2.
2000;50:861–78.
[49] Mackenbach JP. What would happen to health inequalities if smoking
[26] Gottfredson LS. Intelligence: is it the epidemiologists’ elusive “fun-
were eliminated? BMJ 2011;342:d3460.
damental cause” of social class inequalities in health? Journal of
[50] Hanlon P, Carlisle S, Hannah M, Reilly D, Lyon A. Making the case for
Personality and Social Psychology 2004;86(1):174–99.
a ‘fifth wave’ in public health. Public Health 2001;125:30e36.
[27] Batty GD, Deary IJ, Macintyre S. Childhood IQ in relation to
[51] Durkheim E. The division of labour in society. New York: The Free
risk factors for premature mortality in middle-aged per-
Press; 1984.
sons: the Aberdeen children of the 1950s study. Journal
[52] Durkheim E. Suicide. New York: The Free Press; 1951.
of Epidemiology and Community Health 2007;61:241–7,
[53] Lewis O. The culture of poverty. Scientific American 1966;215:19–25.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/jech.2006.048215.
[54] Buckingham A. Is there an underclass in Britain? The British Journal
[28] Calvin CM, Deary IJ, Fenton C, Roberts BA, Der G, Leckenby N, of Sociology 1999;50:49–75.
et al. Intelligence in youth and all-cause mortality: systematic [55] Dean M. Christmas, the poor, and the development of a UK under-
review with meta-analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology class. Lancet 1989;334:1536–7.
2011;40:626–44. [56] Lee P. Housing and spatial deprivation: relocating the underclass and
[29] Batty GD, Der G, Macintyre S, Deary IJ. Does IQ explain socioecono- the new urban poor. Urban Studies 1994;31(7):1191–209.
mic inequalities in health? Evidence from a population based cohort [57] Gallie D. Are the unemployed an underclass? Some evidence from the
study in the west of Scotland. BMJ 2006;332:580–4. Social Change in Economic Life Initiative. Sociology 1994;28:737–57.
[30] Batty GD, Shipley MJ, Mortensen LH, Boyle SH, Barefoot J, Gron- [58] Small ML, Harding DJ, Lamont M. Reconsidering culture and poverty.
baek M, et al. IQ in late adolescence/early adulthood, risk factors Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science
in middle age and later all-cause mortality in men: the Vietnam 2010;629:6.
Experience Study. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health [59] Hills J, Brewer M, Jenkins S, Lister R, Lupton R, Machin S, et al.
2008;62:522–31. Anatomy of economic inequality in the UK: report of the National
Author's personal copy

G. McCartney et al. / Health Policy 113 (2013) 221–227 227

Equality Panel. London: Government Equalities Office and Centre for [72] Shaw M, Smith GD, Dorling D. Health inequalities and New
Analysis of Social Exclusion at the London School of Economics and Labour: how the promises compare with real progress. BMJ
Political Science; 2010. 2005;330:1016–21.
[60] Julius Wilson W. When work disappears. London: Alfred A. Knoff [73] Collins C, McCartney G. The impact of neoliberal ‘political attack’ on
publishing; 1996. health: the case of the Scottish Effect. International Journal of Health
[61] Damer S. Glasgow going for a song. London: Lawrence &Wishart; Services 2011;41(3):501–23.
1990. [74] Nuffield Council on Bioethics. Public health: ethical issues. London:
[62] Krieger N. Theories for social epidemiology in the 21st Century: Nuffield Council on Bioethics; 2007.
an ecosocial perspective. International Journal of Epidemiology [75] Macintyre S. Inequalities in health in Scotland: what are they and
2001;30:668–77. what can we do about them? Glasgow: MRC Social and Public Health
[63] Krieger N, Rehkopf DH, Chen JT, Waterman PD, Marcelli E, et al. The Sciences Unit; 2007.
fall and rise of US inequities in premature mortality: 1960–2002. [76] Capewell S, Graham H. Will cardiovascular disease prevention
PLoS Medicine/Public Library of Science 2008;5:e46. widen health inequalities? PloS Medicine 2011;7(8):e1000320,
[64] Thomas B, Dorling D, Smith GD. Inequalities in premature mor- http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1000320.
tality in Britain: observational study from 1921 to 2007. BMJ [77] Bambra C, Gibson M, Sowden A, Wright K, Whitehead M, Petticrew M.
2010;341:c3639, http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.c3639. Tackling the wider social determinants of health and health inequal-
[65] Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angoid A. Relationships between ities: evidence from systematic reviews. Journal of Epidemiology &
poverty and psychopathology. JAMA 2003;290:2023–9. Community Health 2010;64:284–91.
[66] Barr HL, Britton J, Smyth AR, Fogarty AW. Association between [78] Navarro V, Muntaner C, Borrell C, Benach J, Quiroga Á, Rodríguez-
socioeconomic status, sex, and age at death from cystic fibrosis Sanz M, et al. Politics and health outcomes. Lancet 2006;368:1033–7.
in England and Wales (1959 to 2008): cross sectional study. BMJ [79] Singh GK, Siahpush M. Widening socioeconomic inequalities in US
2011;343:d4662. life expectancy, 1980–2000. International Journal of Epidemiology
[67] Hofrichter R. Tackling health inequities: a framework for public 2007;35:969–79, http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyl083.
health practice. In: Hofrichter R, Bhatia R, editors. Tackling health [80] Stewart-Brown S. What causes social inequalities: why is this ques-
inequities through public health practice: theory to action. New tion taboo? Critical Public Health 2000;10(2):233–42.
York: Oxford University Press; 2010. [81] McKee M, Diethelm P. How the growth of denialism undermines
[68] Krieger N. Epidemiology and the web of causation: has anyone seen public health. BMJ 2010;341:1309–11.
the spider? Social Science and Medicine 1994;39:887–903. [82] McKee M, Diethelm P. Denialism: what it is and how should scientists
[69] Navarro V, Muntaner C, Borrell C, Benach J, Quiroga A, Rodríguez- respond? European Journal of Public Health 2009;19(1):2–4.
Sanz M, et al. Politics and health outcomes. Lancet 2006;368:1033–7. [83] Townsend P, Davidson N, Whitehead M. Introduction to inequalities
[70] Navarro V. Social class, political power and the state and their impli- in health. In: Inequalities in health. London: Penguin; 1998. p. 3–11.
cations in medicine. Social Science and Medicine 1976;10:437–57. [84] Shaw M, Dorling D, Mitchell R, Smith GD. Labour’s
[71] Dorling D. Injustice: why social inequality persists. Bristol: Policy “Black report” moment? BMJ 2005;331:575,
Press; 2010. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.331.7516.575.

View publication stats

You might also like