You are on page 1of 9

Health Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association

2009, Vol. 28, No. 5, 579 –587 0278-6133/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016124

Effects of Supportive-Expressive Group Therapy on Pain in Women


With Metastatic Breast Cancer

Lisa D. Butler, Cheryl Koopman, Eric Neri, Janine Giese-Davis, Oxana Palesh, Krista A. Thorne-Yocam,
Sue Dimiceli, Xin-Hua Chen, Patricia Fobair, Helena C. Kraemer, and David Spiegel
Stanford University School of Medicine

Objective: To examine whether a group intervention including hypnosis can reduce cancer pain and trait
hypnotizability would moderate these effects. Design: This randomized clinical trial examined the effects
of group therapy with hypnosis (supportive-expressive group therapy) plus education compared to an
education-only control condition on pain over 12 months among 124 women with metastatic breast
cancer. Main Outcome Measures: Pain and suffering, frequency of pain, and degree of constant pain
were assessed at baseline and 4-month intervals. Those in the treatment group also reported on their
experiences using the hypnosis exercises. Results: Intention-to-treat analyses indicated that the inter-
vention resulted in significantly less increase in the intensity of pain and suffering over time, compared
to the education-only group, but had no significant effects on the frequency of pain episodes or amount
of constant pain, and there was no interaction of the intervention with hypnotizability. Within the
intervention group, highly hypnotizable participants, compared to those less hypnotizable, reported
greater benefits from hypnosis, employed self-hypnosis more often outside of group, and used it to
manage other symptoms in addition to pain. Conclusion: These results augment the growing literature
supporting the use of hypnosis as an adjunctive treatment for medical patients experiencing pain.

Keywords: cancer pain, randomized trial, hypnosis, group therapy, hypnotizability

Pain is a common symptom of cancer that can be mostly deterioration in their condition (D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983b), or as
attributed to tumor involvement and treatment side effects (Foley, a signal of impending death (Ferrell & Dean, 1995) or a recurrence
1975). Studies have found, for example, that about one third of in their cancer (Coward & Wilkie, 2000).
lung and colon cancer respondents had experienced “persistent or The occurrence of pain in cancer may contribute to further
frequent” cancer-related pain in the previous 2 weeks (Portenoy et somatic consequences. Pain is one of the strongest predictors of
al., 1992) and over two thirds of an advanced cancer patient fatigue in breast cancer patients (Haghighat, Akbari, Holakouei,
sample reported pain, with most reporting pain as 5 or higher on a Rahimi, & Montazeri, 2003), and the experience of fatigue and
10-point scale (Yun et al., 2003). Pain can also be episodic; it can pain together have been found to be the most distressing symptoms
flare up even when baseline pain is relatively well controlled. to patients (e.g., in lung cancer; Cooley, Short, & Moriarty, 2003).
Almost two thirds of an international sample of 1,095 cancer Sleep problems are also associated with greater pain (Koopman et
patients reported breakthrough pain (Caraceni et al., 2004). The al., 2002), even after treatment ends (Dow, Ferrell, Leigh, Ly, &
level of pain has been associated with more advanced disease, Gulasekaram, 1996).
which makes pain “one of the most feared consequences of can- Pain also has psychosocial effects. Cancer patients with high
cer” (Foley, 1996, p. 164). It is understood by patients as a sign of pain (vs. low pain) show much higher rates of depression, sug-

Lisa D. Butler, Cheryl Koopman, Eric Neri, Janine Giese-Davis, Oxana Experimental Hypnosis, San Antonio, TX in November 2001. We ac-
Palesh, Krista A. Thorne-Yocam, Sue Dimiceli, Xin-Hua Chen, Patricia knowledge the contributions of Elaine Miller, Jane Benson, Karin Calde,
Fobair, Helena C. Kraemer, and David Spiegel, Department of Psychiatry Leslie Kinder, Elizabeth T. Klinger, Lynne LoPresto, Julie Seplaki, Thai
& Behavioral Sciences, Stanford University School of Medicine. Nguyen, Trina Kurek, Rita Halbach, Christopher Biggs, Catherine Classen,
Lisa D. Butler is now at the School of Social Work, University at Susan Diamond Moore, Susan Weisberg, Frank Stockdale, Amanda
Buffalo, The State University of New York. Janine Giese-Davis is now at Kovattana, Catherine Byers, Bob Carlson, Bernadette Paolini, referring
the University of Calgary Department of Oncology, Division of Psycho- community physicians, the Stanford Oncology Day Care Center, and the
social Oncology, Tom Baker Cancer Centre, Division of Psychosocial patients and their spouses and families who participated in the study. We
Resources. Oxana Palesh is now at the Department of Psychiatry and also thank Jackie Tilston for her much appreciated contributions to the
Radiation Oncology, University of Rochester Medical Center, James P. introduction and Jay Azarow for his feedback on a portion of this paper.
Wilmot Cancer Center. ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT00226928; http://www.clinicaltrials
This study was made possible by National Institute for Mental Health .gov (registered 09/12/05).
Grant 5R01MH047226 with additional funding from the National Cancer Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lisa D.
Institute, the American Cancer Society, the John D. and Catherine T. Butler, who is now at the Buffalo Center for Social Research, University at
MacArthur Foundation, and the Fetzer Institute. Previous analyses of these Buffalo, School of Social Work, 229 Parker Hall, Buffalo, NY 14214-
data were presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Clinical and 8004. E-mail: ldbutler@buffalo.edu

579
580 BUTLER ET AL.

gesting that pain may have a causal role in such depression (e.g., A number of reviews looking at nonpharmacologic treatments
D. Spiegel, Sands, & Koopman, 1994). One study reported that concluded that there is much support for using hypnosis for
50% of the variance in pain was accounted for by mood distur- cancer-related pain management (Mundy, DuHamel, & Montgom-
bance, patient’s belief that the pain indicated a worsening of the ery, 2003; Sellick & Zaza, 1998; D. Spiegel & Classen, 2000; H.
illness, and the use of analgesic medication (D. Spiegel & Bloom, Spiegel & Spiegel, 2004; Trijsburg, van Knippenber, & Rijpma,
1983b). “A cycle is established whereby the pain is a reminder of 1992). Moreover, one recent study (Montgomery et al., 2007)
the presence of the illness and of the helplessness of the patient to found that a brief presurgery hypnosis session reduced pain inten-
master it, and this in turn only reinforces the pain experienced” sity and unpleasantness, nausea, fatigue, discomfort, and emo-
(D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983b, p. 341). This cycle can also lead to tional upset in breast cancer patients in the postsurgical period,
reduced activity levels (Ahles, Blanchard, & Ruckdeschel, 1983), highlighting a range of possible target symptoms for hypnosis in
anxiety (Dalton & Feuerstein, 1989), and fear and anger (Reynolds the psychosocial care of cancer patients.
& Olsen, 1996). In addition, pain and distress can increase signif- Previous research also indicated that there is typically a corre-
icantly prior to death in metastatic breast cancer (MBC) patients, lation between degree of hypnotizability and extent of pain relief
although pain alone does not account for the increase in distress (DeBenedittis, Panerai, & Villamira, 1989; Hilgard & Hilgard,
(Butler et al., 2003). 1994) suggesting that hypnotic interventions for pain may be most
Poor adaptation to pain has led to the development of psycho- effective for those higher in hypnotic responsiveness, though scant
social therapy protocols to teach coping skills and reduce the research has examined this question in metastatic cancer patients.
emotional impact of pain. Given the evidence pertaining to the Consequently, we sought to examine whether highly hypnotizable
health benefits of social support (e.g., Levy et al., 1990; Turner- women in the treatment condition would report significantly lower
Cobb, Sephton, Koopman, Blake-Mortimer, & Spiegel, 2000; increases in pain over time than would less hypnotizable women in
Weis, 2003), support groups are now widely employed for chronic the same condition or women randomized to the control group. In
illness sufferers. A number of randomized trials have found pos- addition, we expected that highly hypnotizable women would find
itive benefits for a range of group interventions for cancer patients hypnosis to be more beneficial than their less hypnotizable coun-
(e.g., Antoni et al., 2006; Kissane et al., 2003; Lane & Viney, terparts in the same group.
The aim of this study was to examine the effect of supportive-
2005; Levine, Eckhardt, & Targ, 2005), although some studies
expressive group therapy employing hypnosis on pain experience
have found little or limited psychological effects (e.g., Edelman,
(degree of pain and suffering, pain frequency, constant pain) in
Bell, & Kidman, 1999; Edmonds, Lockwood, & Cunningham,
MBC patients, and to explore the interaction between hypnotiz-
1999). Supportive-expressive group therapy (SET)—the interven-
ability and pain experience in women trained to use hypnosis. We
tion examined in the present study—also has demonstrated psy-
hypothesized that: (a) women randomized to receive treatment
chosocial effects, including reductions in mood disturbance, de-
would report a smaller increase in pain over the first 12 months in
pression, trauma symptoms, emotional control, and maladaptive
the study than women randomized to the control condition; (b)
coping (Classen et al., 2001; Giese-Davis et al., 2002; Goodwin et
those in the treatment group who were higher in hypnotizability
al., 2001; Kissane et al., 2007; D. Spiegel, Bloom, & Yalom, 1981;
would report the smallest increase in pain over the 12 months,
reviewed in Boutin, 2007), among others. SET also teaches self-
when compared to those lower in hypnotizability in that group or
hypnosis for pain management and has been effective in reducing those not trained in hypnosis (i.e., the control group); and (c) the
pain among cancer patients (Goodwin et al., 2001; D. Spiegel & high hypnotizable women in the treatment group would report
Bloom, 1983a). A comparison of SET with and without hypnosis more benefit from the hypnosis exercises than their less hypnotiz-
being taught (D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a), so that the effect of that able counterparts.
element could be tested, found that the treatment group with
hypnosis reported less increase in pain sensation experience at
follow-up when compared to the therapy group without hypnosis. Method
One of the oldest uses of hypnosis is for medical analgesia
(Esdaile, 1846), and current research shows that it is a highly Participants
effective adjunct to treatment for pain and related anxiety (Lang et One hundred and twenty-five women with confirmed metastatic
al., 2000; Patterson & Jensen, 2003; D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a) or locally recurrent breast cancer were randomized into the parent
across a range of medical patients. Within the context of the study between 1991 and 1996. All women but 3 had MBC; the
conceptualization of pain as a combination of sensory and affec- remaining 3 women had breast cancer recurrences after breast
tive aspects, hypnosis for pain aims to help the cancer patient filter conserving therapy as their only site of recurrent/metastatic disease
out the emotional or “hurt” (D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a, p. 335) but were judged by our expert oncological consultant as having
component of the experience by reinterpreting the pain as a dif- equivalent prognoses. For ease of presentation, we refer to all
ferent sensation (H. Spiegel & Spiegel, 2004). This effect may also participants in the present report as having MBC. Women were
be seen as a function of focused attention relegating pain signals to recruited through the Oncology Day Care Center at Stanford
the periphery. There is also growing evidence that hypnosis alters University Medical Center, letters to community oncologists, bro-
the perception of pain, reducing the brain response to nociceptive chures distributed in the community, and notices in local newspa-
stimuli, not merely the reaction to it (De Pascalis, 1999; Kosslyn, pers and breast cancer newsletters (for details on recruitment, see
Thompson, Costantini-Ferrando, Alpert, & Spiegel, 2000; Rain- Classen et al., 2001). A total of 155 women initially entered the
ville, Hofbauer, Bushnell, Duncan, & Price, 2002; D. Spiegel, study; 30 of these women were not randomized: 12 due to disease
Bierre, & Rootenberg, 1989). progression that precluded participation; 7 were found to be inel-
GROUP THERAPY REDUCES CANCER PAIN 581

igible after medical record review; and 11 decided that they did not Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
want to continue in the study.
Data for all participants are included in the present analyses All participants gave written informed consent for participation
except for 1 participant in the treatment group at the Stanford site in a protocol approved by the Stanford University School of
who refused to have her hypnotizability assessed at baseline. Medicine Institutional Review Board. Women were eligible for the
Intention-to-treat analyses on this sample of 124 were conducted study if they had documented metastatic or recurrent breast cancer,
using slopes computed over the first 12 months of treatment. were proficient enough in English to be able to complete ques-
Slopes were calculated for 94 participants for whom we had at tionnaires and participate in a support group, were living in the
Greater San Francisco Bay Area, and had a Karnofsky score of at
least one usable follow-up assessment. A zero slope was assumed
least 70 (Karnofsky & Burchenal, 1949). A patient with a Karnof-
for the remaining 30 participants. Regarding these 30 participants,
sky score of 70 is able to care for herself, but unable to carry on
20 of them did not complete any postbaseline assessments (13
normal activity or do active work. We did not include women with
were too ill to complete questionnaires or had died [4 treatment
positive supraclavicular lymph nodes as the only metastatic lesion
and 9 control]; 1 was too busy [control]; 4 withdrew from the study
at the time of initial diagnosis; active cancers within the past 10
because they were not assigned to a support group; 1 woman
years other than breast cancer, basal cell or squamous cell carci-
withdrew because she did not like the support group; and 1 woman
nomas of the skin, in situ cancer of the cervix (severe cervical
assigned to the treatment condition withdrew for no stated reason).
intraepithelial neoplasia or squamous intraepithelial lesion II); or
With respect to the other 10 women (6 treatment and 4 control),
melanoma with a Breslow depth below 0.76 mm. We also ex-
each had completed only one postbaseline assessment, but it was cluded those with other concurrent medical conditions likely to
proximal to death. In these cases the final assessment point was influence short-term survival.
removed from analyses and, consequently, the slope was assumed
to be zero. This step was taken because we have previously
identified a “spike” in pain (and distress) that occurs proximal to Procedure
death independent of receiving group therapy (Butler et al., 2003)
Baseline Assessments and Randomization
and that can obscure treatment effects examined by slopes (e.g.,
Classen et al., 2001). Seventeen other women (9 treatment and 8 Baseline assessments were conducted at our three study sites
control) who had postbaseline assessment data in addition to an (Stanford, San Francisco, and San Jose, California) and included
assessment just prior to death were included in the analyses, measures of pain, distress, coping, social support, and physical
though the assessment point proximal to death was excluded in the activity as well as evaluations of immune and endocrine function.
creation of their slopes. Demographic and medical status charac- The present analyses focus on changes in four pain outcomes
teristics are summarized in Tables l and 2. (described below). On completion of baseline testing, participants

Table 1
Demographic Characteristics of the Sample Analyzed by Treatment Condition

Education onlya Group therapy plus educationb

Age, M ⫾ SD (range) 53.1 ⫾ 10.8 (30 to 80) 52.7 ⫾ 10.5 (33 to 73)
Education in years, M ⫾ SD (range) 15.9 ⫾ 2.4 (12 to 20) 16.2 ⫾ 2.7 (12 to 26)
Ethnicity, n (%)
Asian 7 (11.5) 1 (1.6)
African American/Black 1 (1.6) 0 (0)
Hispanic/Latina 2 (3.3) 1 (1.6)
American Indian 1 (1.6) 1 (1.6)
White 50 (82.0) 58 (92.1)
Other 0 (0.0) 2 (3.2)
Marital status, n (%)
Married 31 (50.8) 39 (61.9)
Never married 8 (13.1) 3 (4.8)
Separated or divorced 18 (29.5) 17 (27.0)
Widowed 3 (4.9) 4 (6.3)
Other 1 (1.6) 0 (0.0)
Household income, n (%)
⬍$20,000 10 (16.4) 7 (11.1)
$20,000 to 39,999 11 (18.0) 7 (11.1)
$40,000 to 59,999 16 (26.2) 16 (25.4)
$60,000 to 79,999 6 (9.8) 8 (12.7)
$80,000 to 99,999 5 (8.2) 10 (15.9)
⬎$100,000 13 (21.3) 14 (22.2)
Not reported 0 (0.0) 1 (1.6)

Note. Some percentages do not sum to 100% due to rounding. No differences between the conditions were statistically significant.
a
n ⫽ 61. b n ⫽ 63.
582 BUTLER ET AL.

Table 2
Summary of Medical Status Characteristics Analyzed by Treatment Condition

Education onlya Group therapy plus educationb

Age at initial diagnosis in years, M ⫾ SD (range) 48.0 ⫾ 10.3 (24.9 to 72.1) 47.8 ⫾ 10.2 (28.2 to 67.3)
Age at metastatic diagnosis in years, M ⫾ SD (range) 51.6 ⫾ 10.2 (30.5 to 73.5) 51.8 ⫾ 10.6 (30.8 to 73.2)
Disease-free interval in months, M ⫾ SD (range) 44.5 ⫾ 34.7 (0.0 to 146.1) 48.0 ⫾ 36.6 (0.0 to 162.3)
Time from metastatic diagnosis to study entry in months, M ⫾ SD (range) 28.7 ⫾ 48.2 (1.1 to 244.7) 21.9 ⫾ 27.3 (1.0 to 138.5)
Estrogen receptor negative, n (%) 12 (19.7) 13 (20.6)
Treatment for metastatic disease as of study entry, n (%)
Chemotherapy within the previous 2 months 29 (47.5) 26 (41.3)
Radiation within the previous 2 months 30 (49.2) 24 (38.1)
Hormonal therapy was used for metastasis 47 (77.0) 49 (77.8)
Dominant site of metastasis at study entry, n (%)
Chest wall 20 (32.8) 18 (28.6)
Bone 22 (36.1) 27 (42.9)
Viscera 19 (31.1) 18 (28.6)

Note. Some percentages do not sum to 100% due to rounding. No differences between the conditions were statistically significant.
a
n ⫽ 61. b n ⫽ 63.

were randomized to treatment or control conditions by the project Hypnosis training. Each session ended with a hypnosis exer-
director, a research nurse, using adaptive randomization biased cise to help patients with stress management and to deal with pain.
coin-design method to ensure comparability of medical status in Patients were encouraged to use this exercise at home. Hypnosis is
treatment and control conditions (for details on the randomization a state of highly focused attention with a suspension of peripheral
and stratification procedure, see Classen et al., 2001). Sixty-four awareness and heightened responsiveness to social cues (H. Spie-
women were randomized to the intervention arm of the study, and gel & Spiegel, 2004). The hypnosis exercises involved a rapid
61 women to the control arm. Because this was a psychotherapy entry into the hypnotic state by counting from one to three while
trial, blinding of condition was not possible. Postbaseline assess- looking up and slowly closing the eyes, and reducing muscle
ments were conducted every 4 months for the first year and every tension by imagining floating in a “bath, lake, a hot tub, or just
6 months thereafter. floating in space.” Then participants were instructed to imagine
changing the sensation in the part of their body that hurt by
Intervention Condition imagining that it was “warmer, cooler, tingling, or numb.” Various
images were utilized involving snow or ice, a hot pack, and so
Three treatment groups were conducted, one at each geographic forth. Then participants were told, “Concentrate on what this feels
site, and they met weekly for 90-min sessions. The size of the like to you in a private sense, and then bring yourself out of the
groups varied over time due to participants dying and to rolling state of hypnosis by counting backward from three to one. On
recruitment, with the group size ranging from 3 to 15 participants three, get ready; on two, roll up your eyes; and on one, let your
in any given group. The intended duration of treatment was 1 year, eyes open.” They were instructed to practice this exercise several
and participants were encouraged to remain in the group for that times a day, and any time the pain became noticeable or started to
time. Most women continued participating for as long as their increase.
health permitted.
The therapy sessions were co-led by two psychotherapists;
among these were a psychiatrist, psychologists, and social work- Control Condition
ers. The supportive-expressive therapy model involved the cre- To ensure full participation and cooperation, we offered a self-
ation of a supportive environment in which participants were directed education intervention to women randomized to the con-
encouraged to confront their problems, strengthen their relation- trol condition, and the same materials were also offered to the
ships, and find enhanced meaning in their lives. The intervention women randomized to treatment. Thus, all participants were given
was semistructured, with therapists trained to enhance the creation a list of materials to select from or take home on loan, including a
of social bonds, identify and encourage the expression of emotion, selection of books, pamphlets, video- and audiotapes covering a
and facilitate discussion of the themes as the material emerged and range of topics related to breast cancer (for details, see Classen et
in an emotionally expressive rather than didactic format. The al., 2001) and also a 1-year membership to a consumer health
themes included: (a) fears of dying and death, including dealing library in their community.
with the deaths of group members; (b) reordering life priorities; (c)
improving support from and communication with family and
friends; (d) integrating a changed self and body image; and (e) Measures
improving communication with physicians (D. Spiegel & Classen, Demographic Characteristics and Medical Status
2000). Through sharing their experiences, group members also
became role models for one another, teaching each other coping Demographic characteristics and medical status information
strategies that they found to be effective in managing the illness. were collected at baseline.
GROUP THERAPY REDUCES CANCER PAIN 583

Pain Level vival). Results indicated that there was better than 90% power to
detect effects of the treatment condition on this primary outcome.
The Pain Rating Scale (D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a, 1983b) was
Slopes analyses were employed to test our present hypotheses
used to query self-reported pain levels. This measure was used to
(Gibbons, Hedeker, Waternaux, Kraemer, & Greenhouse, 1993).
assess pain in an earlier sample of MBC patients in our laboratory
Each participant with a prerandomization baseline measure had a
(D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a, 1983b) and in previous analyses with
slope constructed across all available assessments regressed on
the present sample (Butler et al., 2003). The pain items include
time using months as the unit of time. To conduct an intention-
single Likert-type items assessing “pain in this moment” using a
to-treat analysis, a zero slope was assumed for each of the partic-
scale ranging from 1 (not noticeable) to 10 (excruciating—worst
ipants who did not have at least one postbaseline assessment point.
ever) and “suffering in this moment” using a scale ranging from 1
As mentioned previously, data from the final assessment point
(easily bearable) to 10 (agonizing— unbearable). The constancy
were excluded from the slope for a given participant if that
and frequency of pain were also assessed. “Constant pain” was
assessment was proximal to death (i.e., it was collected at the
coded (as “1”) if the current episode of pain had lasted for at least
assessment closest to her death and that assessment fell in the 4-
6 months and was experienced as constant or persistent during that
[or 6-] month follow-up assessment window prior to her death).
time (otherwise it was coded as “0”). “Pain frequency” was coded
Previous research has demonstrated that there is a significant rise
as the number of days (1 to 7) that were affected by pain in a given
in reported distress and pain prior to death in women with MBC
week of a typical episode since the last assessment (also with
(Butler et al., 2003), which adds variance at the final assessment
values of 0 ⫽ no pain episode since the last assessment, and 7 ⫽
that may obscure earlier treatment effects examined by slopes, as
7 or more days affected in a typical episode). At baseline, 48% of
it did in a previous report (Classen et al., 2001) and in preliminary
the sample reported some degree of current pain (M ⫽ 2.2, SD ⫽
analyses with the present data. In slopes analysis the endpoints
1.7, range ⫽ 1 to 9). Preliminary analysis showed that “pain in this
have a greater influence on the slope relative to other assessment
moment” and “suffering in this moment” were highly correlated
points. Consequently, the effect of proximity to death on the slope
(r ⫽ .71, 95% confidence interval [CI] ⫽ 0.65, 0.76); therefore the
was removed in the primary analyses for testing the hypotheses.
average of these two scales was computed for use in the analyses.
Secondary analyses in which these endpoints were included in
computing the slopes are also presented.
Hypnotizability These outcome slopes became the dependent variables in mod-
The Hypnotic Induction Profile (HIP; H. Spiegel & Spiegel, els that were analyzed by analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) in a
2004), a structured interview assessment of hypnotizability, was 2 (treatment vs. control) ⫻ 3 (geographic sites) ⫻ 2 (low vs. high
conducted with each participant at baseline by a trained inter- hypnotizability level) design, testing also for the two-way interac-
viewer. Hypnotizability refers to the robust finding of valid and tion of treatment condition with hypnotizability. Covariates in this
reliable individual differences in response to hypnotic suggestions analysis included the overall model intercept and the respective
(Cardeña, Maldonado, van der Hart, & Spiegel, 2000). Possible pain variable intercept that was computed along with the pain
scores range from 0 to 10. For the present analyses, participants variable slope. Because change in pain is typically associated with
were divided into low (scores of 0 to 5) and high (scores ⬎5) initial levels, each ANCOVA included this pain variable intercept
hypnotizability groups. HIP scores were widely distributed with a as a covariate, rather using than the baseline value itself because
mean of 5.3 (SD ⫽ 2.8, range ⫽ 0 to 9.5). Forty-six women with the intercept is a better estimate of the true baseline value. Separate
HIP scores at or below 5 were classified as low in hypnotizability analyses were conducted on scores for pain and suffering in this
(27 treatment, 19 control); 78 women whose HIP scores were moment, pain frequency, and constant pain as dependent variables
greater than 5 were classified as high in hypnotizability (36 treat- based on the slopes computed for each participant’s assessments
ment, 42 control). over the first year.
All hypothesized treatment versus control relationships were
Group Feedback tested with two-tailed tests with an alpha level of .05. For all
outcomes, effect sizes were calculated using Cohen’s d (Cohen,
A Group Feedback Questionnaire was created for the present
1977), which is based on the standardized mean difference com-
study to assess group members’ reactions to and feelings about
paring the group therapy condition to the education-only control
participating in the group, the therapists, and using hypnosis. In
condition. A positive effect size indicates that the group therapy
addition to questions regarding therapist characteristics, respon-
condition had better results than the education-only control con-
dents rated the extent to which they felt the hypnosis exercises had
dition, and a negative effect size indicates the reverse. Analyses of
been beneficial to them, using a single item, 10-point scale using
demographic and medical status characteristics with independent
a scale ranging from 1 (not at all beneficial) to 10 (extremely
sample t tests (on continuous data) and chi-square tests (on cate-
beneficial), how often they used the exercises outside of group (8
gorical data) found no statistically significant differences between
category checklist, ranging from “never” to “several times per
participants assigned to the experimental and control treatment
day”), and what they used hypnosis for (e.g., pain control, con-
conditions.
trolling nausea and vomiting, stress management, feeling comfort-
able, considering problems, etc.).
Results
Analyses
Table 3 presents the descriptive statistics at baseline, slopes of
The size of the sample in the overall study was based on the change associated with each condition, F test value and probabil-
results of a power analysis related to the primary outcome (sur- ity, and the treatment effect size for each outcome.
584 BUTLER ET AL.

Table 3
Baseline Pain Measure Scores, Slopes of Change, and Treatment Effect Sizes

Education only Group therapy plus education

n Baseline score M ⫾ SD Slope n Baseline score M ⫾ SD Slope F test value p d

Current pain and suffering intensity 61 1.9 ⫾ 1.4 0.54 63 2.0 ⫾ 1.5 ⫺0.002 4.61 .034 0.31
Frequency of pain 61 4.2 ⫾ 3.1 ⫺0.14 63 4.2 ⫾ 3.2 0.32 0.12 .734 ⫺0.13
Constant pain 61 0.3 ⫾ 0.5 0.02 63 0.3 ⫾ 0.5 0.06 0.03 .863 ⫺0.07

Note. F test values are presented for the tests of treatment effects in the analysis of covariance models.

Pain and Suffering in This Moment Pain Frequency


The ANCOVA on changes in intensity of current pain and In the analysis of changes in the frequency of pain, the overall
suffering showed that the overall model was statistically signifi- model was significant, F(6, 117) ⫽ 4.95, p ⫽ .001, and accounted
cant, F(6, 117) ⫽ 3.86, p ⫽ .001, accounting for 17% of the for 20% of the variance. Women who reported the most frequent
variance. Women who received group therapy plus education showed pain at baseline tended to show a decrease in their frequency of
a significantly smaller increase in the intensity of current pain and pain, whereas women who reported the least frequent pain at
suffering compared to women who received the educational interven- baseline tended to show an increase in the frequency of pain over
tion alone, F(1, 117) ⫽ 4.61, p ⫽ .034 (see Figure 1). Women who the course of the study, F(1, 117) ⫽ 26.83, p ⫽ .001. Changes in
reported greater intensity of current pain/suffering at baseline pain frequency were not significantly related to treatment condi-
tended to experience greater decreases in its intensity, F(1, 117) ⫽ tion, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.12, p ⫽ .734; site, F(2, 117) ⫽ 1.57, p ⫽ .213;
14.44, p ⫽ .001, compared to women who reported less pain/ hypnotizability level; F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.01, p ⫽ .926; or the interac-
suffering at baseline, who tended to experience increases in its tion of condition with hypnotizability; F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.02, p ⫽ .889.
intensity. No significant relationships were found for changes in There were no differences in the significant findings when this
current pain/suffering intensity across the recruitment sites, F(2, analysis was repeated with the death-proximal data points in-
117) ⫽ 1.77, p ⫽ .175, or associated with hypnotizability, F(1, cluded.
117) ⫽ 0.39, p ⫽ .532; and hypnotizability did not show a
significant interaction with treatment, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.07, p ⫽ .792. Constant Pain
When this analysis was repeated without removing the assess-
ments proximal to death from the slopes, the overall model ac- The overall model was significantly related to the changes in
counted for less of the variance (R2 ⫽ 0.10). The treatment effect experiencing constant pain, F(6, 117) ⫽ 4.44, p ⫽ .001, account-
was no longer significant, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.95, p ⫽ .331, but the ing for 19% of the variance. Women who reported constant pain at
baseline level of pain/suffering continued to significantly predict baseline showed the greatest reduction in constant pain, in contrast
greater decreases in pain, F(1, 117) ⫽ 7.22, p ⫽ .008. to women who did not report constant pain at baseline, who tended
to show an increase in constant pain, F(1, 117) ⫽ 21.54, p ⫽ .001.
No significant effects on constant pain were found for treatment
condition, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.03, p ⫽ .863; site, F(2, 117) ⫽ 2.55, p ⫽
Control Slope
3.5 .082; hypnotizability level, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.60, p ⫽ .441; or the
interaction of condition with hypnotizability, F(1, 117) ⫽ 0.01,
Treatment Slope
p ⫽ .921. There were no differences in the significant findings
3.0 when this analysis was repeated with the death-proximal data
points included.
2.5
Uses and Perceived Benefit of Hypnosis Within the
Treatment Group
2.0
Table 4 presents the descriptive statistics and effect sizes for the
uses and perceived benefit of the self-hypnosis exercises analyzed
1.5 by low versus highly hypnotizable women within the treatment
group. Highly hypnotizable women reported greater use of hyp-
nosis exercises for controlling nausea and vomiting (Cohen’s d ⫽
1.0 .73), to manage mental stress/anxiety (Cohen’s d ⫽ .66), and to
0 4 8 12 feel comfortable (Cohen’s d ⫽ .51) than did low hypnotizable
Time (Months) women. Highly hypnotizable women also reported using the self-
hypnosis exercises more often (Cohen’s d ⫽ .55) and, as hypoth-
Figure 1. Slopes and mean scores for pain and suffering over the first 12 esized, they perceived significantly more benefit from them
months analyzed separately for education only (control) and group therapy (Cohen’s d ⫽ .62; p ⫽ .04) when compared to those lower in
plus education (treatment) conditions. hypnotizability.
GROUP THERAPY REDUCES CANCER PAIN 585

Table 4
Exploratory Comparison of Uses and Perceived Benefit of Hypnosis (Mean Across Three Follow-Up Assessments) Analyzed by Low
Versus Highly Hypnotizable Women in Group Therapy Condition

Low hypnotizable Highly hypnotizable

n M SD n M SD d

Controlling nausea and vomiting 15 1.1 0.3 26 2.1 2.4 0.73


Pain management 17 3.5 3.1 26 3.6 3.0 0.46
Physical stress 17 3.1 2.9 26 3.8 3.1 0.24
Mental stress/anxiety 16 3.8 2.9 26 5.7 2.7 0.66
Considering problems 15 2.5 2.3 26 3.3 2.8 0.32
Discovering solutions to problems 15 2.5 2.3 26 3.3 2.8 0.03
Undergoing treatment 15 1.9 2.1 26 3.0 2.7 0.40
Imagining the cancer going away 16 3.3 3.2 26 3.7 3.3 0.14
Feeling comfortable 16 3.5 2.8 26 5.0 2.7 0.51
Dealing with strong emotions 16 2.7 2.5 26 3.3 2.4 0.26
Total number of times used self-hypnosis exercises 15 25.5 18.9 26 36.2 19.1 0.55
Perceived benefit derived from the self-hypnosis exercises 20 4.5 2.9 27 6.3 3.0 0.62

Discussion period. In that study, the induction included instructions to use


hypnosis for many of these symptoms. In the present study, how-
The results of this randomized trial indicate that supportive ever, participants’ use of hypnosis for purposes beyond pain man-
expressive group therapy that includes hypnosis can significantly
agement was more self-initiated, yet it appeared to result in ben-
reduce the experience of pain and suffering for women with MBC.
efits nonetheless, especially for those more hypnotizable.
The primary analyses indicate that participants in the treatment
Unfortunately, our study design does not allow us to disentangle
condition plus education compared to the education-only condition
the effects of the hypnosis from those associated with being in
reported significantly less increase in pain and suffering over the
group therapy generally. Our findings indicate that these combined
first 12 months of treatment. The magnitude of this treatment
aspects were associated with less increase in pain over time, and
effect was in the small to medium range. Participants did not,
these beneficial pain results were not limited to those higher in
however, differ significantly in their reports of pain frequency or
hypnotic responsiveness. The lack of interaction between condi-
constancy. These findings of a reduction in aspects of pain, as well
tion and hypnotizability is consistent with the suggestion that this
as the lack of an effect on pain frequency and constancy, are in line
intervention may be useful for those of different levels of hypnotic
with previous research (Goodwin et al., 2001; D. Spiegel &
susceptibility. Although previous research has found a correlation
Bloom, 1983a).
The present study also examined whether trait differences in between degree of hypnotizability and extent of pain relief (e.g.,
hypnotic susceptibility would affect pain experiences and other Hilgard & Hilgard, 1994), some researchers emphasized the im-
outcomes. Contrary to one of our hypotheses, higher hypnotizable portance of the patient’s motivation and other personality factors
participants in the treatment condition did not report a significantly in the prediction of the hypnotic response (Frischholz, Spiegel, &
smaller increase in pain over time, when compared to those lower Spiegel, 1981). A review by Holroyd (1996) noted several possible
in hypnotizability in the same group or those who did not receive explanations for how hypnosis can benefit low hypnotizables,
the intervention. However, among those who received the inter- including that pain thresholds tend to increase with repeated hyp-
vention, highly hypnotizable participants reported significantly notic analgesia suggestions, hypnotic control of pain can improve
greater benefit from the hypnosis exercises than did those who with practice, and low hypnotizables can increase their responsive-
were less hypnotizable, suggesting that greater hypnotizability ness to hypnotic suggestions with practice. Consequently, it is
may facilitate other beneficial effects of the intervention. Explor- possible that less hypnotizable participants improved in hypnotic
atory analyses also indicated that these highly hypnotizable pain control with the training they received.
women did not differ from the less hypnotizable women in the Such training and improvements may also lead to higher self-
frequency with which they used hypnosis for pain control, but they efficacy and feelings of pain-management mastery, which are both
did use self-hypnosis more often outside of the group setting associated with reductions in subjective levels of pain (Bandura,
overall and applied the exercises to address other symptoms be- O’Leary, Taylor, Gauthier, & Gossard, 1987; Litt, 1988; Manning
sides pain, including anxiety/stress, discomfort, and nausea/ & Wright, 1983). Moreover, D. Spiegel and Bloom (1983a) sug-
vomiting. These findings imply that teaching self-hypnosis to gested that hypnosis, as well as general participation in supportive-
cancer patients may provide them with a tool they can apply to expressive group therapy, can reduce distress and may thereby
purposes beyond simply control of pain. have an indirect impact on pain, and treatment effects on distress
A recent report echoes this finding of multifaceted benefits of have been found in this sample (Classen et al., 2001). In addition,
hypnosis for cancer patients. Montgomery et al. (2007) found that those in the treatment group may have practiced hypnosis to
postsurgical patients who had undergone a brief presurgery hyp- increase feelings of relaxation, and thereby lowered their pain
nosis session reported less pain intensity and unpleasantness, nau- experience through reductions in muscle tension and stress
sea, fatigue, discomfort, and emotional upset in the postsurgical (De Pascalis, Magurano, & Bellusci, 1999; Gay, Philippot, &
586 BUTLER ET AL.

Luminet, 2002). Finally, participating in a group in which expe- Together these findings suggest that there may be a number of
riences of pain are shared, along with pain management strategies benefits to the use of hypnosis in cancer care including, but not
and encouragement, may have contributed to the present treatment necessarily limited to, its more traditional application for pain
effect on pain. The relationship between responsiveness to hypno- control.
sis and pain experience in this population warrants further inves-
tigation. Indeed, studies are needed to clarify the benefits of
References
training/practice to hypnotic responsiveness and pain control, pos-
sible pathways to pain reduction that hypnosis and/or support Ahles, T. A., Blanchard, E. B., & Ruckdeschel, J. C. (1983). Multidimen-
group participation may contribute to (e.g., changes in stress level sional nature of cancer pain. Pain, 17, 277–288.
or perceived social support), as well as the role of individual Antoni, M. H., Wimberly, S. R., Lechner, S. C., Kazi, A., Sifre, R. Urcuyo,
differences in hypnotic responsiveness in determining pain and K. R., et al. (2006). Reduction of cancer-specific thought intrusions and
other outcomes, so that future clinical interventions that include anxiety symptoms with a stress management intervention among women
hypnosis for cancer pain (and perhaps other objectives) may be undergoing treatment for breast cancer. American Journal of Psychiatry,
163, 1791–1797.
better targeted.
Bandura, A., O’Leary, A., Taylor, C. B., Gauthier, J., & Gossard, D.
There are a number of important limitations to this study. The (1987). Perceived self-efficacy and pain control: Opioid and nonopioid
study’s generalizability may be limited to predominantly White, mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 563–
middle to upper class women with MBC. It is not known whether 571.
SET would be as equally effective in reducing pain and suffering Boutin, D. L. (2007). Effectiveness of cognitive behavioral and supportive-
for people who come from a wider range of ethnic and socioeco- expressive group therapy for women diagnosed with breast cancer: A
nomic status backgrounds or who experience other types of cancer review of the literature. The Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 32,
or pain (Edwards, Fillingim, & Keefe, 2001). Moreover, the bur- 267–284.
den of completing extensive assessments at 4-month intervals and Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., Cordova, M. J., Garlan, R. W., DiMiceli, S.,
participating in weekly group sessions may have precluded the & Spiegel, D. (2003). Psychological distress and pain significantly
increase prior to death in metastatic breast cancer patients. Psychoso-
participation of many potential participants who felt that they were
matic Medicine, 65, 416 – 426.
unable to meet these requirements due to the debilitating effects of Caraceni, A., Martini, C., Zecca, E., Portenoy, R. K., Ashby. M. A.,
their illness. Hawson, G., et al. (2004). Breakthrough pain characteristics and syn-
Because we did not include a group that received the interven- dromes in patients with cancer pain. An international survey. Palliative
tion without hypnosis, we are unable to separate the positive Medicine, 19, 177–183.
contributions of the SET intervention from those that may be due Cardeña, E., Maldonado, J., van der Hart, O., & Spiegel, D. (2000).
to the hypnosis exercises in the sessions (and use of self-hypnosis Hypnosis. In E. Foa, T. Keane, & M. Friedman (Eds.), Effective treat-
outside of the group). However, a previous study found that ments for PTSD (pp. 407– 440). New York: Guilford.
members of SET groups that employed hypnosis reported signif- Classen, C., Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., Miller, E., DiMiceli, S., Giese-
icantly less pain than those receiving the same intervention but Davis, J., et al. (2001). Supportive-expressive group therapy and distress
in patients with metastatic breast cancer: A randomized clinical inter-
without hypnosis (D. Spiegel & Bloom, 1983a).
vention trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 494 –501.
There are also a number of methodological limitations of the Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
study including the use of self-reported measures of pain, each of Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
which was composed of a single item. This may, therefore, have Cooley, M. E., Short, T. H., & Moriarty, H. J. (2003). Symptom preva-
resulted in less reliable assessments of aspects of the pain experi- lence, distress, and change over time in adults receiving treatment for
ence and could thereby have limited our ability to detect changes lung cancer. Psycho-Oncology, 12, 694 –708.
over time. In addition, the study may have been underpowered to Coward, D. D., & Wilkie, D. J. (2000). Metastatic bone pain: Meanings
detect interactions, such as the one between hypnotizability and associated with self-report and self-management decision making. Can-
treatment condition. Applying intention-to-treat analyses to data cer Nursing, 23, 101–108.
from this population that had so many incomplete assessments Dalton, J. A., & Feuerstein, M. (1989). Fear, alexithymia and cancer pain.
Pain, 38, 159 –170.
(due to illness and death) may also limit the ability to identify
DeBenedittis, G., Panerai, A. A., & Villamira, M. A. (1989). Effects of
positive effects for those able to participate throughout the inter- hypnotic analgesia and hypnotizability on experimental ischemic pain.
vention. International Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 37, 55– 69.
In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that the experi- De Pascalis, V., Magurano, M. R., & Bellusci, A. (1999). Pain perception,
ence of pain and suffering for patients with MBC can be success- somatosensory event-related potentials and skin conductance responses
fully reduced with an intervention that includes hypnosis in a to painful stimuli in high, mid, and low hypnotizable subjects: Effects of
group therapy setting. These results augment the growing literature differential pain reduction strategies. Pain, 83, 499 –508.
supporting the use of group therapy with hypnosis as an effective De Pascalis, V. (1999). Psychophysiological correlates of hypnosis and
adjunctive treatment for medical patients experiencing pain. Our hypnotic susceptibility. International Journal of Clinical & Experimen-
additional findings that participants higher in hypnotizability who tal Hypnosis, 47, 117–143.
Dow, K. H., Ferrell, B. R., Leigh, S., Ly, J., & Gulasekaram, P. (1996). An
received the intervention, compared to those lower in hypnotiz-
evaluation of the quality of life among long-term survivors of breast
ability in the same group, reported greater benefits from using cancer. Breast Cancer Research and Treatment, 39, 261–273.
hypnosis, used it more often overall, and in some cases employed Edelman, S., Bell, D. R., & Kidman, A. D. (1999). A group cognitive
it to address other symptoms, suggest that although hypnosis is not behaviour therapy programme with metastatic breast cancer patients.
at present standard practice for treating a wide range of symptoms Psycho-Oncology, 8, 295–305.
that trouble cancer patients, it is worth examining this potential. Edmonds, C. V. I., Lockwood, G. A., & Cunningham, A. J. (1999).
GROUP THERAPY REDUCES CANCER PAIN 587

Psychological response to long term group therapy: A randomized trial Berbaum, K. S., et al. (2000). Adjunctive non-pharmacological analgesia
with metastatic breast cancer patients. Psycho-Oncology, 8, 74 –91. for invasive medical procedures: A randomised trial. The Lancet, 355,
Edwards, C. L., Fillingim, R. B., & Keefe, F. (2001). Race, ethnicity and 1486 –1490.
pain. Pain, 94, 133–134. Levine, E. G., Eckhardt, J., & Targ, E. (2005). Change in post-traumatic
Esdaile, J. (1846). Hypnosis in medicine and surgery. New York: Julian. stress symptoms following psychosocial treatment for breast cancer.
Ferrell, B. R., & Dean, G. (1995). The meaning of cancer pain. Seminars Psycho-Oncology, 14, 618 – 635.
in Oncology Nursing, 11, 17–22. Levy, S. M., Herberman, R. B., Whiteside, T., Sanzo, K., Lee, J., &
Foley, K. M. (1975). Pain syndromes in patients with cancer. In J. J. Kirkwood, J. (1990). Perceived social support and tumor estrogen/
Bonioca, V. Ventafridda, R. B. Fink, L. E. Jones, & J. D. Loeser (Eds.), progesterone receptor status as predictors of natural killer cell activity in
Advances in pain research and therapy (Vol. 2, pp. 59 –75). New York: breast cancer patients. Psychosomatic Medicine, 52, 73– 85.
Raven. Litt, M. D. (1988). Self-efficacy and perceived control: Cognitive media-
Foley, K. M. (1996). Controlling the pain of cancer. Scientific American, tors of pain tolerance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,
275, 164 –165. 154 –160.
Frischholz, E. J., Spiegel, H., & Spiegel, D. (1981). Hypnosis and the Manning, M. M., & Wright, T. L. (1983). Self-efficacy expectancies,
unhypnotizable: A reply to Barber. American Journal of Clinical Hyp- outcome expectancies, and the persistence of pain control in childbirth.
nosis, 24, 55–58. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 421– 431.
Gay, M. C., Philippot, P., & Luminet, O. (2002). Differential effectiveness Montgomery, G. H., Bovbjerg, D. H., Schnur, J. B., David, D., Goldfarb,
of psychological interventions for reducing osteoarthritis pain: A com- A., Weltz, C. R., et al. (2007). A randomized clinical trial of a brief
parison of Erikson hypnosis and Jacobson relaxation. European Journal hypnosis intervention to control side effects in breast surgery patients.
of Pain, 6, 1–16. Journal of the National Cancer Institute, 99, 1280 –1281.
Gibbons, R. D., Hedeker, D., Waternaux, C., Kraemer, H. C., & Green- Mundy, E. A., DuHamel, K. N., & Montgomery, G. H. (2003). The
house, J. B. (1993). Some conceptual and statistical issues in the analysis efficacy of behavioral interventions for cancer treatment-related side
of longitudinal psychiatric data. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, effects. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 8, 253–275.
730 –750. Patterson, D. R., & Jensen, M. P. (2003). Hypnosis and clinical pain.
Giese-Davis, J., Koopman, C., Butler, L. D., Classen, C., Cordova, M., Psychological Bulletin, 129, 495–521.
Fobair, P., et al. (2002). Change in emotion-regulation strategy for
Portenoy, R. K., Miransky, J., Thaler, H. T., Hornung, J., Bianchi, C.,
women with metastatic breast cancer following supportive-expressive
Cibas-Kong, I., et al. (1992). Pain in ambulatory patients with lung or
group therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 70,
colon cancer. Cancer, 70, 1616 –1624.
916 –925.
Rainville, P., Hofbauer, R. K., Bushnell, M. C., Duncan, G. H., & Price,
Goodwin, P. J., Leszcz, M., Ennis, M., Koopmans, J., Vincent, L., Guther,
D. D. (2002). Hypnosis modulates activity in brain structures involved in
H., et al. (2001). The effect of group psychosocial support on survival in
the regulation of consciousness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14,
metastatic breast cancer. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 1719 –
887–901.
1726.
Reynolds, P. T., & Olsen, E. J. (1996). Psychiatric aspects of cancer pain.
Haghighat, S., Akbari, M. E., Holakouei, K., Rahimi, A., & Montazeri, A.
Journal of the Florida Medical Association, 83, 705–707.
(2003). Factors predicting fatigue in breast cancer patients. Supportive
Sellick, S. M., & Zaza, C. (1998). Critical review of 5 nonpharmacologic strategies
Care in Cancer, 11, 533–538.
for managing cancer pain. Cancer Prevention and Control, 2, 7–14.
Hilgard, E. R., & Hilgard, J. R. (1975). Hypnosis in the relief of pain. Los
Spiegel, D., Bierre, P., & Rootenberg, J. (1989). Hypnotic alteration of so-
Altos, CA: Kauffman.
Holroyd, J. (1996). Hypnosis treatment in clinical pain: Understanding why matosensory perception. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 749 –754.
hypnosis is useful. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Spiegel, D., & Bloom, J. R. (1983a). Group therapy and hypnosis reduce
Hypnosis, 44, 33–51. metastatic breast carcinoma pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 45, 333–339.
Karnofsky, D. A., & Burchenal, J. H. (1949). The clinical evaluation of Spiegel, D., & Bloom, J. R. (1983b). Pain in metastatic breast cancer.
chemotherapeutic agents in cancer. In C. M. MacLead (Ed.), Evaluation Cancer, 52, 341–345.
of chemotherapeutic agents (pp. 191–205). New York: Columbia Uni- Spiegel, D., Bloom, J. R., & Yalom, I. D. (1981). Group support for
versity Press. patients with metastatic cancer: A randomized outcome study. Archives
Kissane, D. W., Bloch, S., Smith, G. C., Miach, P., Clarke, D. M., Ikin, J., of General Psychiatry, 38, 527–533.
et al. (2003). Cognitive-existential group psychotherapy for women with Spiegel, D., & Classen, C. (2000). Group therapy for cancer patients: A
primary breast cancer: A randomized controlled trial. Psycho-Oncology, research-based handbook of psychosocial care. New York: Basic Books.
12, 532–546. Spiegel, D., Sands, S., & Koopman, C. (1994). Pain and depression in
Kissane, D. W., Grabsch, B., Clarke, D. M., Smith, G. C., Love, A. W., patients with cancer. Cancer, 74, 2571–2578.
Bloch, D., et al. (2007). Supportive-expressive group therapy for women Spiegel, H., & Spiegel, D. (2004). Trance and treatment: Clinical uses of
with metastatic breast cancer: Survival and psychosocial outcome from hypnosis. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
a randomized controlled trial. Psycho-Oncology, 16, 277–286. Trijsburg, R. W., van Knippenberg, F. C., & Rijpma, S. E. (1992). Effects
Koopman, C., Nouriani, B., Erickson, V., Anupindi, R., Butler, L. D., of psychological treatment on cancer patients: A critical review. Psy-
Bachmann, M. H., et al. (2002). Sleep disturbances in women with chosomatic Medicine, 54, 489 –517.
metastatic breast cancer. Breast Journal, 8, 362–370. Turner-Cobb, J. M., Sephton, S. E., Koopman, C., Blake-Mortimer, J., &
Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Costantini-Ferrando, M. F., Alpert, Spiegel, D. (2000). Social support and salivary cortisol in women with
N. M., & Spiegel, D. (2000). Hypnotic visual illusion alters color metastatic breast cancer. Psychosomatic Medicine, 62, 337–345.
processing in the brain. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1279 – Weis, J. (2003). Support groups for cancer patients. Supportive Care in
1284. Cancer, 11, 763–768.
Lane, L. G., & Viney, L. L. (2005). The effects of personal construct group Yun, Y. H., Heo, D. S., Lee, I. G., Jeong, H. S., Kim, H. J., Kim, S. Y., et
therapy on breast cancer survivors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical al. (2003). Multicenter study of pain and its management in patients with
Psychology, 73, 284 –292. advanced cancer in Korea. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management,
Lang, E. V., Benotsch, E. G., Fick, L. J., Lutgendorf, S., Berbaum, M. L., 25, 430 – 437.

You might also like