You are on page 1of 2

Clinical Review & Education

Neuroscience and Psychiatry

Mood Instability and Reward Dysregulation—


A Neurocomputational Model of Bipolar Disorder
Liam Mason, PhD, DClinPsy; Eran Eldar, PhD; Robb B. Rutledge, PhD

Classically, bipolar disorder is seen as a relapsing-remitting condi- bipolar disorder, the result is recursive cycles that cause mood, ex-
tion with episodes of dizzying and capricious highs (manic epi- pectations, and behavior to escalate to extremes (Figure, A).6 Im-
sodes) that are clearly separated from melancholic lows and inertia portantly, the same elevated mood bias parameter leads to hyper-
(depressive episodes) and interspersed with remission. Yet, in the sensitivity to rewards when mood is high and hyposensitivity to
clinical setting, the picture is often far more complicated. Bipolar rewards when mood is low, reconciling previously contradictory ac-
disorder lies on a spectrum with subtypes that are defined by arbi- counts of bipolar disorder.3,4 Empirical support for the model comes
trary and often unfulfilled criteria for the severity and duration of from the finding that trait differences in propensity toward el-
mood episodes, which leads to high rates of “unspecified” diagno- evated moods were explained by the degree to which mood induc-
ses. “Mixed affective” episodes, in which depressive and manic tion biased reward valuation, as inferred from changes in striatal re-
symptoms co-occur, are the norm rather than the exception. Mood sponses and preference for options encountered when mood was
instability also persists out of episode, causing as much impairment elevated (Figure, B).6
as within-episode symptoms1 and muddying the classically episodic Our neurocomputational model makes several predictions about
presentation. how mood changes will escalate, resolve, and reverse in bipolar dis-
These complexities challenge existing models of bipolar disorder2 order. We predict that a stronger mood bias parameter that ampli-
and broader psychobiological frameworks that describe the relation- fies normal mood fluctuations in response to rewards will be asso-
ship between mood and behavior. A prominent theory proposes that ciated with less time in remission and more severe mood episodes
bipolar disorder arises from the dysregulation of a ‘“behavioral acti- (Figure, C). We also predict that high mood bias parameters are a
vation system” that drives positive affect, confidence, and the ap- risk factor for developing bipolar disorder, consistent with findings
proach of rewarding stimuli,2 and temporary increases and de- that this parameter correlates with trait measures of bipolar disor-
creases in this system result in mania and depression, respectively. der vulnerability (Figure, B).6 The mood bias parameter, being stable
However, a plausible neurobiological mechanism for these tempo- for a given individual, should also be similar across euthymic, de-
rary changes is lacking. There is fundamental disagreement about the pressed, and manic phases. Through amplification of normal mood
core pathophysiology of bipolar disorder. Some research supports fluctuations, the model also accounts for the mood instability that
a “reward hypersensitivity” account,3 but other research suggests that occurs both during episodes and residually between mood epi-
reward sensitivity is fundamentally blunted.4 In contrast to existing sodes. For example, as mood episodes progress, expectations be-
models that do not make quantitative predictions, we argue that a come increasingly discrepant with the objective value of out-
new computational approach, based on a dynamic understanding of comes. Surprises that run counter to the valence of the episode
the 2-way relationship between mood and reward sensitivity, can pro- become increasingly likely, causing mood and behavior to fluctu-
vide insight into the mechanisms by which patients with bipolar dis- ate (Figure, C). This greater variability in prediction errors might also
order transition between relapse and recovery, as well as explain account for mood lability in mixed affective states, which could be
mixed affective states and pervasive mood instability. readily tested using computational modeling.
The application of computational approaches to the study of Another parameter within the model is the learning rate, which
emotion and decision making provides new ways to relate momen- determines how quickly expectations are updated based on re-
tary changes in mood and behavior to well-defined neural circuits. ward prediction errors. Learning rate asymmetries may explain the
Recent studies have found that mood fluctuations are highly con- dominance of a positive or negative mood (eg, in patients with uni-
tingent on reward prediction error signals that are represented by polar mania and bipolar II) (Figure, C). Further, an increasing learn-
activity in the ventral striatum, a region that is involved in goal- ing rate may explain why episode frequency can increase across the
directed behavior.5 These signals encode how rewarding an out- course of the disorder, at least for some patients (“kindling”). In a
come was compared with what was expected. For example, posi- changeable environment, it is advantageous to increase the rate of
tive surprises (eg, winning £100 [$132] on a scratch card) elicit striatal learning to adapt behavior.5 The volatility that results from re-
activity and a positive mood state. This then biases how subse- peated relapses and their sequelae could strengthen the belief that
quent rewards are perceived, increasing their perceived value when further changes are likely, warranting an increased learning rate to
mood is elevated (and conversely decreasing perceived value when adapt to these changes. However, an increased learning rate would
mood is low). Expectations that guide future decisions are up- also increase spontaneous mood instability, increasing the likeli-
dated based on these mood-biased reward prediction errors.6,7 A hood of further mood episodes. Alternatively, a gradual increase in
moderate mood bias helps individuals adapt quickly to an environ- mood bias parameters could also lead to kindling. Testing which pa-
ment that is changing, either for better or for worse. However, if rameter provides a better explanation (eg, by comparing model para-
mood strongly biases reward perception, as we propose occurs in meters among patients with bipolar disorder in early and later stages)

jamapsychiatry.com (Reprinted) JAMA Psychiatry Published online October 11, 2017 E1

© 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: by a UNIVERSITY OF SYDNEY LIBRARY User on 11/03/2017


Clinical Review & Education Neuroscience and Psychiatry

Figure. Neurocomputational Model of Mood Instability

A Positive feedback cycles underlying mood instability B Effects of mood on reward valuation

Expectations Positive mood induction


become Manic Cycle Negative mood induction
overly low
Biased 1.0
perception Higher 9
mood 0.6 A, Increases in mood follow from

Increase in Reward Response


Positive r=
0.5 unexpected rewards, which cause
surprises
Negative subsequent rewards to be perceived
Lower surprises 0 as better than they are, increasing
mood Biased future expectations and further
perception
−0.5 escalating mood (manic cycle). Over
Expectations
become overly high r= time, expectations become
Depressive Cycle −0.
45 increasingly discrepant with actual
−1.0
outcomes, with the resulting
negative surprises setting in motion a
−1.5
1.5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 depressive cycle. B, Vulnerability to
C Computer simulations for different mood bias parameters
Trait Mood Instability bipolar disorder is associated with a
Normal mood greater influence of the current mood
variation Cyclothymia Bipolar IIa Bipolar I on striatal response to rewards.6
Expectation C, Stronger mood bias parameters
Prediction error inflate reward prediction errors,
Mania Mood causing expectations and mood to
hypomania oscillate, akin to mood episodes
(shaded area).
Depression
a
A negative mood predominates if
the learning rate is higher for
Increased mood bias negative than positive reward
predictions errors.

is important because these explanations imply different underly- proposed model accounts for how positive and negative life events
ing neural circuitry and interventions. increase the likelihood of mood episodes, the etiology of individual
Model parameters provide a way of quantifying mood distur- differences in this parameter remains unaddressed. In addition to
bances in a mechanistic way. By quantifying mood and behavior in illuminating multiple poorly understood features of bipolar disor-
relation to well-defined neural circuits, this computational ap- der, this computational approach provides an inroad for improving
proach strongly accords with the dimensional approach that is ad- the diagnosis and treatment of mood instability across psychiatric
vocated by the Research Domain Criteria framework. Although the disorders.

ARTICLE INFORMATION London and the Max Planck Society. No other 3. Mason L, O’Sullivan N, Montaldi D, Bentall RP,
Author Affiliations: Max Planck University College disclosures were reported. El-Deredy W. Decision-making and trait impulsivity
London Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding in bipolar disorder are associated with reduced
Ageing Research, University College London, London, organizations had no role in the preparation, prefrontal regulation of striatal reward valuation.
England (Mason, Eldar, Rutledge); Wellcome Trust review, or approval of the manuscript or decision to Brain. 2014;137(pt 8):2346-2355.
Centre for Neuroimaging, University College submit the manuscript for publication. 4. Schreiter S, Spengler S, Willert A, et al. Neural
London, London, England (Eldar, Rutledge). Additional Contributions: We thank Peter Dayan, alterations of fronto-striatal circuitry during reward
Corresponding Author: Liam Mason, PhD, PhD, Gatsby Computational Neuroscience Unit, anticipation in euthymic bipolar disorder. Psychol
DClinPsy, Max Planck University College London University College London, for helpful comments Med. 2016;46(15):3187-3198. doi:10.1017
Centre for Computational Psychiatry and Ageing and discussions arising from an earlier version of /S0033291716001963
Research, University College London, London, this paper. He was not compensated for his 5. Eldar E, Rutledge RB, Dolan RJ, Niv Y. Mood as
England (l.mason@ucl.ac.uk). contribution. representation of momentum. Trends Cogn Sci.
Published Online: October 11, 2017. 2016;20(1):15-24. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.07.010
doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3163 REFERENCES 6. Eldar E, Niv Y. Interaction between emotional
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported. 1. Broome MR, Saunders KEA, Harrison PJ, state and learning underlies mood instability. Nat
Marwaha S. Mood instability: significance, Commun. 2015;6:6149. doi:10.1038/ncomms7149
Funding/Support: Dr Rutledge receives research definition and measurement. Br J Psychiatry. 2015;
support from a Career Development Award from 7. Rutledge RB, Moutoussis M, Smittenaar P, et al.
207(4):283-285. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.114.158543 Association of neural and emotional impacts of
the Medical Research Council. Dr Eldar receives
research support from grant 095844/Z/11/Z from 2. Urosević S, Abramson LY, Harmon-Jones E, Alloy reward prediction errors with major depression.
the Wellcome Trust Cambridge–University College LB. Dysregulation of the behavioral approach JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74(8):790-797.
London Mental Health and Neurosciences Network. system (BAS) in bipolar spectrum disorders: review
The Max Planck University College London Centre is of theory and evidence. Clin Psychol Rev. 2008;28
a joint initiative supported by University College (7):1188-1205. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.04.004

E2 JAMA Psychiatry Published online October 11, 2017 (Reprinted) jamapsychiatry.com

© 2017 American Medical Association. All rights reserved.

Downloaded From: by a UNIVERSITY OF SYDNEY LIBRARY User on 11/03/2017

You might also like