You are on page 1of 14

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/285988163

Pets in the Family: An Evolutionary Perspective

Article · January 2012


DOI: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195396690.013.0017

CITATIONS READS

12 2,319

2 authors, including:

James A Serpell
University of Pennsylvania
177 PUBLICATIONS   4,731 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

The Domestic Dog, 2nd Edition View project

Equine Behaviour Assessment and Research Questionnaire (E-BARQ) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by James A Serpell on 18 April 2018.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


IN: The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Family Psychology, eds.
Salmon, C. & Shackleford, T.K., pp. 297-309. Oxford: OUP, 2011.
C H APT ER

Pets in the Family: An Evolutionary


17 Perspective

James A. Serpell and Elizabeth S. Paul

Abstract
Pets have become such a common component of modern family life that we tend to take them for
granted. Nevertheless, from an evolutionary standpoint, pets present us with a paradox comparable
to—though even more puzzling than—that posed by the phenomenon of adoption. In the latter case,
one can at least argue that adoptive parents may derive deferred fitness benefits from the future
contribution of adopted children to the family economy (Kramer, 2005). But in the case of adopted
pets, such contributions appear to be minimal at best, whereas the level of investment in their care and
sustenance is sometimes considerable. The paradox further intensifies when one considers that pet
keeping is not confined to modern, affluent societies, but is widespread among subsistence hunters and
horticulturalists whose opportunities to engage in nonfitness enhancing behavior would appear to be
much more constrained. This chapter critically examines theories that purport to explain how pet
keeping evolved and why it continues to persist and flourish in a wide range of cultures. Given the
current state of knowledge, few firm conclusions can be drawn at this time regarding the possible
adaptive consequences of pet keeping. However, it is possible to highlight future areas of research that
may help to illuminate the functional significance (if any) of this intriguing behavior.
Keywords: evolution; mutualism; pets; pet keeping; adoption; human-animal interaction

Introduction because, although it is easy to account for the eco-


Any discussion of the phenomenon of pet keeping, nomic exploitation of animals using classical evolu-
especially from an evolutionary perspective, needs tionary theory, it is far less easy to explain the
to begin with a reasonable working definition of the practice of keeping animals purely as “favorites,” or
word pet. The Oxford English Dictionary (OED) of treating them with “indulgence and fondness.”
defines a pet as: “Any animal that is domesticated or In most modern, industrial societies, pets—in
tamed and kept as a favorite, or treated with indul- the OED sense of the word—have become a ubiq-
gence and fondness.” The OED thus tends to make uitous feature of family life. According to various
a distinction between nonhuman animals (hence- opinion polls and surveys, between 86% and 97%
forth “animals”) kept primarily for social, emo- of pet-owning Americans consider their pets to be
tional, or sentimental reasons (i.e., pets) and those members of the family (American Animal Hospital
that are kept mainly for economic or practical pur- Association, 1996; Associated Press, 2009; Barker
poses (i.e., working animals, livestock, research ani- & Barker, 1988; Cain, 1985; Catanzaro, 1984;
mals). Of course, the two categories frequently Harris Interactive, 2007; Pew Research Center,
overlap in practice, as in the case of working guide 2006), and up to 50% report that their pets are,
dogs or sheepdogs, for example. However, the dis- “just as much a part of the family as any other
tinction remains critical to the present discussion person in the household” (Associated Press, 2009).

297

17-Salmon-17.indd 297 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


Further evidence suggests that pets fulfill primarily Although pet keeping seems to have achieved
childlike roles within families. In one study, 75% of unprecedented levels of popularity in the last few
pet owners surveyed considered their pets akin to decades, the practice of treating particular animals
children, and nearly one-third of participants in with “indulgence and fondness” appears to have
another survey indicated that they felt closer to the ancient roots. Some of the oldest known archaeo-
family dog than to any other member of the family logical remains of domestic wolf-dogs from the
(American Animal Hospital Association, 1996; late Paleolithic of Europe and North America
Barker & Barker, 1988). The familial status of pets (8,000–14,000 years ago) were found buried
is also confirmed by the kinds of things that people together with humans in a manner indicative of
do with their animal companions. For example, strong mutual bonds of attachment (Benecke, 1987;
69%of American pet owners allow their pets to Davis & Valla, 1978; Morey, 1992, 2006). Similarly,
sleep in bed with them, and nearly two-thirds the discovery of nonindigenous cat remains buried
give them holiday and/or birthday gifts (Harris in association with humans on the Mediterranean
Interactive, 2007). island of Cyprus about 9,500 years ago provides evi-
From an evolutionary perspective, the prevalence dence that humans were taking tame wildcats with
and status of pets within families presents an intrigu- them on ocean voyages several thousand years before
ing paradox. Expressed in simple terms, pet keeping these animals became household pets or the objects
entails a significant investment of time, energy, and of religious veneration in ancient Egypt (Malek,
resources on the part of pet owners, and yet appears 1993; Serpell, 2000; Vigne et al., 2004).
to confer no obvious benefits in terms of their sur- The notion that Paleolithic humans may have
vival or genetic fitness. The goal of this chapter is to been in the habit of capturing and taming wild ani-
explore the extent and significance of the use of pets mals and keeping them as pets is consistent with the
as family members in both Western and non-Western observed behavior of more recent hunting and gath-
societies, while also examining different theories ering peoples. According to numerous reports by
that have been proposed to explain the evolutionary explorers and anthropologists, pet keeping among
origins and functional significance of this seemingly subsistence hunting and horticultural peoples is the
paradoxical behavior. norm rather than the exception, and is typically
characterized by intense emotional attachments to
Extent of the Pet Keeping Phenomenon the animals involved, as well as strong moral taboos
Pet keeping is not a rare or unusual behavior. against killing or eating them. This is even the case
According to recent surveys, there are now about 75 when these animals belong to species that are hunted
million pet dogs in America living in roughly 44.8 routinely for food (Erikson, 1987, 2000; Serpell,
million homes, 90.5 million cats in 38.4 million 1989). Furthermore, as with pets in industrialized
homes, 150 million pet fish in 15 million homes, countries, the owner–pet relationship among these
and many millions more ferrets, rabbits, guinea hunting societies is typically understood as analo-
pigs, hamsters, rats, mice, gerbils, and birds of vari- gous to that between parent and child (Basso, 1973;
ous kinds, as well as a wide assortment of pet rep- Crocker, 1977; Fausto, 1999). In some cases, the
tiles and amphibians (American Pet Products connection is fairly explicit. Among the Bororo
Manufacturers’ Association [APPMA], 2008). The people of central Brazil, ownership of pet macaws
figures for the European Union (EU) are more (Ara sp.) is almost entirely limited to women, and
modest but still impressive: 41 million dogs and 47 the standard Bororo explanation for why some
million cats distributed among 55 million house- households keep more of these pets than others is
holds, and some striking differences in per capita that these are the homes of women who have previ-
numbers and proportions of pet animals within the ously lost many children (Crocker, 1977).
different EU countries. Worldwide, the pet popula- Pictorial and documentary evidence further sug-
tion has expanded recently and rapidly. The findings gests that pet keeping has been practiced continu-
of market research suggest that the combined cat ously throughout human history, although its
and dog population of the United States has popularity has waxed and waned somewhat unpre-
increased by a factor of four since the mid-1960s; dictably over time and from place to place (Serpell,
twice the rate of growth of the human population. 1996). In Europe and colonial North America, pet
Pet keeping is now a majority activity in the United keeping did not become widespread until the 18th
States, with 63% of U.S. households owning at least century. Medieval and Renaissance moralists and
one pet, and 45% owning more than one. theologians seem to have regarded most forms of

298 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 298 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


intimacy between people and animals as morally energy consumed by driving a 4 × 4 SUV 6,200 miles
suspect, and generally condemned the practice of a year, including the energy required to build the
keeping animals exclusively for companionship. vehicle. According to another estimate, America’s
Consequently, pet keeping remained chiefly the 75 million pet dogs may consume as many calories
province of the upper classes and ruling élite until as roughly 35 million people. Producing this
the end of the early modern period, when the emer- much food would require the equivalent of approxi-
gence of both Enlightenment attitudes and an urban mately 20,000 square miles of productive farmland
middle class saw the gradual spread of pets into all (Coppinger & Coppinger, 2001).
sectors of Western society (Grier, 2006; Harwood, All of these statistics are derived from relatively
1928; Ritvo, 1988; Salisbury, 1994; Serpell & Paul, affluent, developed countries where people can
1994; Thomas, 1983). afford to invest substantial time and resources in a
variety of superficially non–fitness enhancing activi-
The Costs of Pet Keeping ties, whether visiting casinos or shopping for fash-
In modern, industrialized societies, pets are not only ionable clothes. Unfortunately, there appear to be
tremendously popular and widespread, but also rep- no quantitative studies of pet-related investment
resent a significant cost to those who care for them. among less affluent subsistence hunting or horticul-
The average lifetime monetary costs of dog and cat tural societies in which pet keeping is also common.
ownership in the United States has recently been esti- It is apparent, however, that some individuals in
mated at $13,330 and $8,506,1 respectively, for an such societies expend relatively large amounts of
animal with a 13-year lifespan. By the end of 2010, time and effort on their pets. For instance, Serpell
Americans will be spending around $50 billion (1996, pp. 61–66) cites numerous reports of women
annually on their pets, chiefly on prepared foods breastfeeding pets alongside their own infants, care-
and accessories (APPMA, 2008). Income also fully premasticating fibrous plant foods to hand-
appears to be a limiting factor in the spread of pet feed pet birds and rodents, or spending several hours
keeping. According to some surveys, almost 70% of each day catching tiny fish to feed pet kingfishers.
U.S. adults with annual incomes over $100,000
own pets compared with only 45% of those earning Evolutionary Origins of Pet Keeping
less than $30,000 (Pew Research Center, 2006). In Existing theories of pet keeping usually fail to
addition to their economic impact, pets may also distinguish between the evolutionary origins of this
inflict emotional costs on both their owners and behavior and its possible functional consequences.
others. A significant number of pet owners experi- The distinction is important because the two
ence severe and prolonged grief reactions following approaches involve different levels of analysis.
the deaths of their pets (Archer, 1997), and badly Arguments about origins are concerned with a trait’s
behaved pets can be a source of considerable stress initial appearance and spread within a population,
and conflict within families, and among neighbors whereas accounts based on function refer to the
and friends (Voith, 2009). maintenance of a trait due to its current (or recent)
Pet keeping also imposes costs on society. The effects on individual fitness (Emlen et al., 1991;
U.S. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention Tinbergen, 1963). According to Williams (1966),
estimates that 4.5 million Americans are bitten by a trait or a behavioral predisposition should only be
pet dogs each year, of which nearly 900,000 require considered an adaptation if it has been modified
medical attention (Centers for Disease Control and during its evolutionary history in ways that enhance
Prevention, 2008). Pets are also sources of a variety its effectiveness as measured by fitness consequences
of zoonotic diseases, as well as a common cause of to the performer.
allergies and asthma (Mandhane et al., 2009; Nafstad The evolutionary origins of pet keeping are of
et al., 2001; Robertson & Thompson, 2002). The particular interest because this behavior seems to be
environmental impact of pet keeping on this scale a manifestation of altruism in its purest form. That
may also be considerable. In their recent book, Vale is, it enhances the survival and reproductive success
and Vale (2009) calculate that a medium-size family of biologically unrelated animals while apparently
dog eats around 360 pounds of meat and 210 contributing nothing to the fitness of their human
pounds of cereal annually, roughly equivalent to the owners. Pet keeping therefore appears to violate the
assumptions of both natural and kin selection,
1
http://www.examiner.com/x-9729-Cats-Examiner∼y2009 according to which individuals should seek to maxi-
m8d4-Cats-cost-less-to-own-than-dogs-research-firm-says mize either their own survival and reproductive

serpell, paul 299

17-Salmon-17.indd 299 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


success and/or that of their genetic relatives, to an empathize and cooperate with others are adapta-
extent proportional to the latter’s degree of related- tions that facilitate cooperative breeding (see also
ness and the relative fitness costs thereby incurred Burkart et al., 2009). One could also apply signal
by the donor (Hamilton, 1964). In these respects, detection theory (Green & Swets, 1966) to argue
pet keeping shares much with both alloparenting that overly discriminating systems of kin recogni-
and adoption—two behaviors that are widespread tion might have been disadvantageous to our ances-
among birds and mammals, and which are some- tors, if the costs of negative errors (not offering care
times also directed toward the infants and young of to the offspring of kin) were significantly greater
unrelated parents (Cäsar & Young, 2008; Riedman, than the costs of positive errors (occasionally offer-
1982). Indeed, given the similarities, a reasonable ing care to the infants of nonkin or other species). If
alternative term for “pet keeping” within evolution- this were the case, pet keeping could have evolved
ary psychology might be “cross-species adoption.” and spread as long as it involved relatively minimal
Although cross-species adoption is certainly costs to those who engaged in the behavior.
unusually prevalent in Homo s. sapiens, it is not On a somewhat different tack, Serpell (1996,
without precedent in the animal kingdom. 2003) has suggested that both pet keeping and
Numerous examples of comparable behavior have animal domestication are secondary by-products of
been reported in both captive and free-ranging the evolved human ability to use “reflexive con-
mammals,2 and in some instances, these relation- sciousness” or theory of mind as a model for under-
ships are surprisingly strong and enduring. Izar et al. standing the mental lives of others (Humphrey,
(2006), for example, describe a remarkable case of 1983). This trait is believed to have evolved in the
an infant marmoset (Callithryx sp.) adopted by a context of human social relationships, but due to
group of free-living capuchin monkeys (Cebus sp.) obvious phylogenetic similarities it can also be
in Brazil who was physically carried and cared for by applied to the task of understanding the minds of
two different adoptive “mother” monkeys for a nonhumans. The archaeologist Steven Mithen
period of 14 months before eventually disappearing (1996) refers to this specific application as anthropo-
from the group. morphic thinking, and he argues persuasively that it
The practice of adopting unrelated infants of enabled our Paleolithic ancestors to become more
either one’s own or another species probably results successful hunters by conferring on them the ability
from errors in the evolved mechanisms responsible to “think like” and therefore anticipate the behavior
for mother–infant bonding and care (Maestripieri, of their prey. It may also have had other far-reaching
2001). As Daly and Wilson (1987, p. 93) put it: consequences: By enabling our ancestors to attri-
“Some examples of misdirected ‘parental’ nurture bute humanlike thoughts, feelings, motivations,
surely represent nothing more than rare ‘mistakes’ and beliefs to other animals, it opened the door to
generated by motivational mechanisms that are the incorporation of some animals into human
usually effective in promoting fitness.” If this inter- families and social groups, first as pets and eventu-
pretation is correct, humans are evidently especially ally as domestic dependents (Serpell, 2003). To
prone to these types of errors. Silk (1990, p. 39) some extent, this hypothesis and the previous one
explains this error-proneness in terms of “innate are complementary. The theory of hyperinclusive
psychological predispositions” that promote an parental motivation helps to account for the initial
intense attraction to infants, as well as a lack of human tendency to adopt, socialize, and care for
inborn kin recognition systems or stereotyped young animals, while the concept of anthropomor-
maternal bonding processes. In Silk’s view, humans phic thinking helps to explain why these same
are unusually susceptible because, in the Pleistocene animals might have been tolerated and accepted as
environments in which they evolved, there was little members of human families long after they passed
opportunity for human females to adopt nonkin the infantile stage of dependence.
and therefore few evolutionary pressures to develop Both of the above examples imply that pet keep-
mechanisms to discriminate between related and ing originated as an unavoidable side effect of other
unrelated young. Hrdy (2009) has taken these ideas adaptive traits. Recently, Herzog (2010) has sug-
further by arguing that hypernurturing behavior, gested that this behavior might have emerged and
together with theory of mind and the capacity to spread without any adaptive associations whatso-
ever. He argues that pet keeping may be a meme
2
http://daughterearth.blogspot.com/2008/12/cross-species- (Dawkins, 1976)—a unit of cultural selection—
adoptions.html a relatively inconsequential but highly contagious

300 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 300 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


mental virus that has spread itself through certain kin, nor even their own species. Central to this issue
human populations by a process of social copying. is the question of whether, at the level of the inclu-
In support of this idea, he cites evidence that pet sive fitness of the humans concerned, the benefits of
keeping, and even the preference for particular types pet keeping outweigh the costs. Below, we consider
of pets, tends to run in families; that different various functional theories and hypotheses that have
nationalities, cultures, and ethnic groups vary widely been proposed to explain this behavior. Our aim is
in their liking for pets; and the fact that the practice to consider current evidence in favor of each pro-
once adopted can sometimes spread very rapidly posal, and to suggest where further studies may be
within societies in which pet keeping was previously needed to distinguish between competing possibili-
rare or unknown. Memes can also spread, at least in ties. Functional consideration of the phenomenon
the short term, even when they are manifestly disad- of pet ownership is particularly interesting because
vantageous to the individuals who adopt them. it highlights the interaction between ultimate and
Although it is hard to refute the meme theory of pet proximate causes of caregiving behavior. Humans,
keeping, it is also difficult to explain why the cul- like other animals, do not always behave as opti-
tural transmission of pet keeping would have per- mally as some theorists might like to expect. Not
sisted over thousands of years of human history in only are we always likely to be at some stage within
the absence of any obvious selective benefit to those the process of adaptation, rather than at any notional
who adopted this superficially costly behavior. endpoint of it, we are also inevitably constrained to
some extent by the mechanisms (e.g., physiological,
Functional Consequences of Pet Keeping neural, behavioral) that natural selection has already
When framed in terms of its function, pet keeping provided us with (McNamara & Houston, 2009).
can be explained using a variety of both adaptive When we care for, love, and trust animals as if they
and nonadaptive arguments (see Table 17.1). At the are kin, are we behaving against our better interests
core of each of these is an attempt to explain the within a system that has evolved for other purposes.
apparent paradox of humans choosing, at not incon- Or, is our interest in pets in fact more fine-tuned,
siderable cost to themselves, to care for and provide less paradoxical, and more advantageous to us than
sustenance to individuals who are neither their own is first apparent?

Table 17.1 Adaptive and Nonadaptive Hypotheses to Account for the Existence of Pet Keeping
Hypothesis Benefits to Pet Owners Fitness Effect (Adaptive) or Mechanism of Maintenance
(Nonadaptive)
Adaptive
Social Buffering Enhanced survival Keeping pets provides a social buffer against the negative health
effects of psychosocial stress.
Parenting Experience Increased reproductive Keeping pets provides experience of parenting/nurturing which
success translates into higher reproductive success when the pet owner
breeds.
Honest Advertisement Enhanced future Keeping pets demonstrates an individual’s parenting/nurturing
(of parenting ability) probability of breeding skills, thereby increasing his/her probability of being chosen as
a mate.
Nonadaptive
Neutral trait None Pet keeping is a selectively neutral trait, maintained by genetic
(and/or memetic) drift.
Social parasitism None Pet keeping is maintained as an unavoidable low-cost by-
product of parental care motivations that are exploited by other
species.
Modified from Emlen, S.T., Reeve, H.K., Sherman, P.W., Wrege, P.H., Ratnieks, F.L.W., & Shellman-Reeve, J. (1991). Adaptive versus
nonadaptive explanations of behavior: The case of alloparental helping. American Naturalist, 138, 259–270, with permission.

serpell, paul 301

17-Salmon-17.indd 301 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


Nonadaptive Consequences however, it is necessary to specify what the mutual
pets as social parasites benefits might be. Certainly, from the perspective
One solution to the evolutionary paradox posed by of the pets, the advantages may seem obvious: By
pet keeping is to argue that pets are essentially social providing them with food, water, shelter, care, and
parasites that manipulate the behavior of their protection from danger, humans have enabled these
owners (hosts) to obtain one-sided fitness benefits species to expand into a new ecological niche in
(Archer, 1997). Pets, according to this view, are which they have become hugely successful in evolu-
comparable to avian brood parasites, such as cuck- tionary terms. But what kinds of benefits might
oos and cowbirds, which rely on their hosts’ inabil- people derive from the company of pets that could
ity to distinguish reliably between its own and the potentially offset the costs of caring for them?
imposter’s eggs or nestlings. To the extent that this
occurs frequently and is costly to the cuckolded Adaptive Consequences
species, an evolutionary “arms race” is likely to ensue pet keeping as nonhuman social
(Dawkins & Krebs, 1979): Although the host support
becomes better at spotting and discriminating Social support is a theoretical construct that
against the parasite’s young, the young will become expresses the degree to which individuals are socially
more similar to the young of the host to avoid detec- embedded and have a sense of belonging, obliga-
tion. These kinds of changes have certainly taken tion, and intimacy with others (Cobb, 1976;
place during the evolution of our most successful Eriksen, 1994; Schwarzer & Knoll, 2007). In prac-
pet species—dogs and cats. And although neoten- tice, it tends to be broken down into different com-
ous morphological features, behavioral dependency, ponents, including (a) emotional support, or the
docility, sociality, playfulness, affection, and tactile sense of being able to turn to others for comfort in
responsiveness have all been enhanced and extended, times of stress, the feeling of being cared for by
often beyond infancy and into the adult animal others; (b) social integration, the feeling of being an
(Coppinger et al., 1987; Dechambre, 1949; Fox, accepted part of an established group or social net-
1978; Lorenz, 1971; Sanefuji et al., 2007; Serpell, work; (c) esteem support, the sense of receiving
2003), it is unclear who is being parasitized by positive, self-affirming feedback from others regard-
whom in this context. For example, to improve their ing one’s value, competence, abilities, or worth;
ability to serve as social companions, the vast major- (d) practical, instrumental, or informational support,
ity of pet dogs and cats in the United States are sur- the knowledge that others will provide financial,
gically sterilized, while eugenicist breeding practices practical, or informational assistance when needed;
leave many of them crippled with genetic disease or and (e) opportunities for nurturance and protection,
anatomical deformities that are detrimental to their the sense of being needed or depended upon by
welfare and biological fitness (Arman, 2007; Serpell, others (Collis & McNicholas, 1998).
2003). It could therefore be argued that these ani- Social support (or a lack of it) is known to have a
mals are in fact the victims of human social parasit- profound impact on human mental and physical
ism rather than vice versa, and that the “cute” health (House et al., 1988; Kiecolt-Glaser &
care-soliciting features of many pets are the prod- Newton, 2001; Lim & Young, 2006; Monroe et al.,
ucts of selection for characteristics that enhance 1986). A growing body of evidence has confirmed a
their ability to satisfy specific human needs. strong positive link between social support and
The assumption that pet keeping confers no fit- enhanced health and survival in humans. For exam-
ness advantages on humans may be false, or it may ple, social support factors have been shown to pro-
be that any parasitism by pets on humans is rela- tect against cardiovascular disease and stroke,
tively weak, and that the costs incurred are far less rheumatoid arthritis, diabetes, nephritis, pneumo-
severe than those inflicted on their hosts by brood nia, and most forms of cancer, as well as depression,
parasites such as cuckoos or cowbirds. Alternatively, schizophrenia, and suicide (see e.g., Eriksen, 1994;
it may be more appropriate to characterize pet keep- Esterling et al., 1994; House, Landis, & Umberson,
ing as a case of mutualism, in which both human 1988; Kikusui et al., 2006; Sherbourne et al., 1992;
and pet derive benefits from the relationship com- Uchino, 2006; Vilhjalmson, 1993). The mecha-
parable to those derived from interactions between, nisms underlying these effects of social support are
say, tropical cleaner-fish (Labroides sp.) and their still the subject of ongoing research, but at least
clients/hosts (Herre et al., 1999; Johnstone & some of the benefits appear to arise from the phe-
Bshary, 2002; Serpell, 2003). If this is the case, nomenon of social buffering: That is, the capacity of

302 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 302 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


supportive social relationships to buffer or amelio- Two studies have demonstrated significant increases
rate the deleterious health consequences of psycho- in plasma oxytocin levels in human subjects follow-
social stress. It is well established that prolonged ing interactions with their own (but not with unfa-
psychosocial stress results in chronically elevated miliar) dogs (Odendaal & Meintjes, 2003; Miller
levels of circulating glucocorticoid (stress) hor- et al., 2009), whereas another study detected sig-
mones, and that these in turn can have a damaging nificantly elevated levels of urinary oxytocin among
impact on the immune system (Ader, Cohen, & dog owners who received greater amounts of visual
Felten, 1995; Kikusui et al., 2006; Uchino, 2006). attention (gaze) from their dogs in an experimental
At least some of these effects of social support appear trial. When questioned, these owners also professed
to be mediated by the neuropeptide hormones, oxy- stronger attachments for their more attentive dogs
tocin and vasopressin, which also play critical roles (Nagasawa et al., 2009).
in the modulation of attachment behavior and social The social buffering idea may also go some way
bonding in mammals (Donaldson & Young, 2008; toward explaining the relatively recent and ongoing
Lim & Young, 2006). Furthermore, the elevation in explosion in the popularity of pets among industri-
oxytocin associated with pleasurable social interac- alized nations within the last 30–40 years. In the
tions also has a down-regulating effect on the hypo- United States, for instance, the results of a variety of
thalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis that regulates social and public health surveys have documented
the stress response (Heinrichs et al., 2003: Kikusui the gradual collapse or fragmentation of traditional
et al., 2006). social support systems, particularly since the 1960s.
The findings of research on the role of pets in Such trends have been marked by a substantial rise
people’s lives seem to fit this social support/social in the number of people living alone, especially in
buffering paradigm. Pet owners, for example, have urban areas; escalating divorce rates and an increase
been shown to possess fewer physiological risk fac- in the number of couples choosing to have fewer
tors (high blood pressure; high serum triglycerides children or none at all (Morgan & Taylor, 2006);
and cholesterol levels) for cardiovascular disease people spending increasingly less time socializing
than nonowners, as well as exhibiting improved sur- with their friends, or getting involved in their local
vival and longevity following heart attack (Anderson communities; and families dispersing geographi-
et al., 1992; Friedmann, Thomas, & Eddy, 2000; cally, so that fewer close relatives now live within
Garrity & Stallones, 1998). They also appear to be easy reach (Putnam, 2000). It seems plausible to
more resilient in the face of stressful life events, argue in light of these trends that the recent growth
resulting in fewer health problems and fewer visits of the pet population at least partly reflects people’s
to doctors for medical care (Heady, 1998; Siegel, attempts to augment their traditional support sys-
1990). The acquisition of a new pet has been found tems using pets (see also Archer, 1997).
to be associated with improvements in an owner’s Why humans should apparently be so dependent
mental and physical health, and with sustained on the support of others to maintain their health
reductions in his or her tendency to overreact to and well-being in the face of psychosocial stress
stressful situations and stimuli (Allen et al., 1991, remains an important evolutionary question of its
2001; Serpell, 1991). Also, pet owners who report own. One can surmise that, ancestrally, the survival
being very attached to their pets tend to report more and reproductive advantages of being affiliated with
benefits than those who are less attached, and dog closely knit social groups selected for individuals
owners tend to do better than cat owners, perhaps who experienced psychological distress when socially
because the attachment to dogs, on average, is stron- isolated and who were therefore highly motivated to
ger (Freidmann & Thomas, 1995; Ory & Goldberg, seek out and maintain social attachments.
1983; Serpell, 1991).
These apparent links between pet keeping and pets and the acquisition of
human health are consistent with the view of pets parenting skills
serving as sources of social support (Collis & Among K-selected animals, such as humans, in
McNicholas, 1998; Garrity & Stallones, 1998; which females are only able to produce a limited
Serpell, 1996; Virués-Ortega & Buela-Casal, 2006). number of offspring in a lifetime, and in which the
Additional recent findings suggest that the mecha- young require a prolonged and intensive period of
nisms underlying these effects of pet ownership may parental investment, parenting abilities are likely
be similar to those thought to be responsible for to have a disproportionate impact on the survival
the social buffering effect in human relationships. and reproductive success of each offspring. In such

serpell, paul 303

17-Salmon-17.indd 303 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


species, natural selection will therefore tend to favor among men and boys, and that interest in pets
the evolution of traits or behaviors that improve should be strongest among preadolescent and ado-
parenting skills (Hrdy, 2009; Silk, 1990). Among lescent females, since these groups stand to gain
primates living in naturalistic social groups, for most from the acquisition of nurturing experience
example, first-time mothers are more likely than (Maestripieri & Pelka, 2002; Maestripieri & Roney,
multiparous mothers to neglect and abandon their 2006). The evidence from research partially sup-
infants or handle them clumsily. Conversely, the ports these predictions. Most studies suggest that
probability of infant abandonment is greatly reduced enthusiasm for pets tends to be highest among girls
as females give birth to successive infants, and the in the 8- to 12-year age range, especially in those
quality of their maternal care typically improves as without access to younger siblings, and that overall
well. The experience of interacting with younger interest in pet keeping tends to decline thereafter
siblings or other females’ infants during the juvenile (Melson et al., 1997; Paul & Serpell, 1992). On the
period can also improve the quality of maternal care other hand, it is also clear that pet keeping is popu-
and the probability of infant survival of first-time lar with both sexes throughout the human lifespan,
mothers. In vervet monkeys (Cercopithecus aethiops), even among postreproductive adults for whom
first-time mothers who had greater infant handling additional parenting experience would, presumably,
experience as juveniles were more likely to be be nonfunctional.
competent mothers, and their infants had a higher
probability of survival than did females with less pets as advertising
juvenile experience with infants (Fairbanks, 1990; Although the possibility of pets acting as signals has
Maestripieri & Roney, 2006). not been extensively explored, evidence from a
Based on evidence of this kind, several authors number of studies suggest that the presence of a pet
have proposed that the widespread and intense can influence other people’s perceptions of the
interest in infants shown by juvenile female pri- owner or carer in ways that might influence fitness-
mates (including humans), as well as phenomena relevant behaviors toward them. The most basic
such as alloparenting and the adoption of some- hypothesis that can be proposed in this respect is
times unrelated infants, represent adaptations for that pet keeping is (or was until relatively recently) a
facilitating the acquisition of parenting skills (Hrdy, form of conspicuous consumption that advertises
2005, 2009; Maestripieri & Roney, 2006; Riedman, the owner’s surplus wealth and ability to accrue
1982). Identical arguments can also be applied to resources. Although such effects may not be readily
instances of cross-species adoption (pet keeping), if apparent in contemporary developed societies,
it is assumed that the experience gained from nur- where food (especially the protein rich foods required
turing and caring for a young nonhuman animal by pet dogs and cats) is relatively affordable, it may
can enhance an individual’s subsequent efforts at still apply to other species. In Britain, for example,
child rearing (Hrdy, 2009). Regrettably, the possible where grazing land is particularly expensive, the
relationship between pet keeping and reproductive keeping of some pet-like animals such as horses does
success has never been investigated in any human frequently seem to carry with it connotations of
population, although there is certainly abundant wealth or elevated social rank. And, in the past,
evidence of cross-species adoption of orphaned when food would have been scarcer, it was primarily
young animals among hunting and gathering peo- the wealthy nobles and aristocracy who indulged
ples, and numerous reported instances of women in passions for lap dogs and other intimates (Ritvo,
these societies caring for and even breastfeeding a 1988; Serpell, 1996; Thomas, 1983). Although the
wide variety of species (Erikson, 1987, 2000; Serpell, causes and fitness consequences of these kinds of
1989). Evidence from a number of retrospective displays may be hard to determine or quantify, it
studies also suggests that people exposed to pet must at least be conceded that the keeping of certain
ownership in childhood may develop traits, such as types of pets, in certain societies or environments,
enhanced empathy, that could contribute to may confer benefits to the owner by virtue of the
improved parenting behavior (Bierer, 2001; Daly & high status thereby attributed to them by others.
Morton, 2009; Paul & Serpell, 1993; Poresky & Of course, being seen to be keeping and caring
Hendrix, 1990; Visek-Vidovic et al., 1999, 2001). for a pet may transmit more information about a
Two important predictions of the “parenting person than mere wealth. As a social activity, in par-
experience” hypothesis are that pet keeping should ticular, it might be expected to convey details of
be more popular among women and girls than a person’s overall temperament or willingness to

304 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 304 12/21/2010 10:38:18 AM


display altruistic behavior. In the early years of attribute human mental characteristics to these ani-
human–animal research, a number of studies dem- mals long after they were passed the care-soliciting
onstrated that people tend to be perceived differ- stage of infantile dependence. Various adaptive and
ently and more positively in the presence of pet nonadaptive theories have been proposed to explain
animals than when unaccompanied (Lockwood, the ongoing, although sporadic, popularity of pets
1983; Serpell, 1996). A seminal study by Messent among human cultures ever since. The least flatter-
(1984) found that passers-by made more friendly ing of these “functional” hypotheses regards pet
social acknowledgments to a person walking with a keeping as a nonadaptive or maladaptive conse-
dog through a public park than when they were quence of hyperinclusive nurturing tendencies that
alone. In a series of subsequent studies, similar renders our species vulnerable to exploitation by
results were obtained. Both adults and children nonhuman social parasites in the form of dogs, cats,
using wheelchairs were acknowledged, spoken to, rabbits, gerbils, hamsters, and so on. Most other
and smiled at more often by passers-by when in the theories take the view that human–pet relationships
presence of a dog (Eddy et al., 1988; Mader et al., are essentially mutualistic, and that both partici-
1989), although in more recent research, this appar- pants gain fitness benefits by associating with the
ent “social facilitation” of approachability or like- other. According to one such theory, pet keeping
ability was found to depend on the type of animal from the human perspective is the social equivalent
involved. Experimenters with puppies were acknowl- of wearing warm clothes—i.e., adaptive behavior
edged more than those with adult dogs, and a person that can protect or buffer people from the “chill” of
was acknowledged less in the presence of a Rottweiler psychosocial stress. Another views pet keeping, at
than a Labrador Retriever (Fridlund & MacDonald, least ancestrally, as a means by which young humans
1998; Wells, 2004). Even more interesting from a acquire nurturing or empathic skills that enable
fitness point of view was the finding of another them to become more successful at rearing their
recent study conducted in France in which an attrac- own offspring later in life. Still another sees this
tive male experimenter loitered in a pedestrian area behavior as a potential way of signaling or advertis-
and attempted to persuade young female passers-by ing to other humans that one is capable of support-
to give him their phone numbers. He was three ing and caring for a dependent, thereby enhancing
times as successful when he had a pet dog with him one’s social status—and perhaps increasing one’s
than when he did not (Guéguen & Ciccotti, 2008). chances of being chosen as a mate. Based on current
Further studies are needed to dissect the precise pro- evidence, it is difficult to choose among these vari-
cesses by which the presence of a dog could have ous competing hypotheses, and the task is further
such effects, and additional research is needed to complicated by the fact that the functional value of
investigate the possible effect that more private types pet keeping in modern Europe or North America
of pet keeping (e.g., cat ownership) might have. So (i.e., its current utility) may be quite different from
far, however, the evidence is both intriguing and its adaptive value in the past. It is also possible that
compelling: Being seen to be with, and perhaps pet keeping serves, or has served, all of these differ-
caring for, a pet appears to have significant effects ent functions at different times and in different cul-
on the perception others have of us, and on the tural contexts during its long history. Additional
behavior they show toward us. Perhaps these effects research into the history and prehistory of pet keep-
are similar to those that accrue from being seen to ing, as well as its current functions, is needed to
engage in human-directed caregiving; indicating explore these various alternatives. To this end, a
that, as a willing alloparent, an individual is hon- number of potential research questions can be posed
estly advertising his or her potential skills as a high- that may help to clarify our understanding of the
investing parent. significance of family pets, both in terms of current
functions and possible evolutionary significance.
Conclusion
On the basis of relatively limited evidence, it appears
Future Directions
most likely that pet keeping, or “cross-species adop-
tion,” originated among Paleolithic hunting peoples 1. If pet keeping is genuinely adaptive, why isn’t
as a consequence of misdirected parental behavior. it universal? Conversely, if pet keeping confers no
The human capacity for anthropomorphic thinking measurable adaptive benefits, why does its
may have helped the process of assimilating these popularity continue to grow? What individual and
early pets into human families by enabling people to cultural factors, apart from disposable income,

serpell, paul 305

17-Salmon-17.indd 305 12/21/2010 10:38:19 AM


limit or encourage the spread of pet keeping within ability, and on the number, quality, and survival of a
and between human societies? person’s offspring. Again, it would be essential to
2. What are the cumulative effects of pet control for other potentially confounding variables
keeping throughout the lifespan on human health that might affect the results, such as parental
and longevity? Most studies relating pet ownership nurturance, subcultural covariations in pet keeping
to human health have focused on current pet and other cultural traditions, duration and intensity
ownership versus nonownership, and the results in of childhood attachments to pets, and access to
some cases have been equivocal (Cutt et al., 2007; alternative nurturing objects (dolls, soft toys, infants,
Wells, 2009). But perhaps the beneficial effects of younger siblings, etc.). Such studies may also be
pets are cumulative over the lifespan and depend more revealing in societies in which offspring
on a person’s lifetime exposure to these kinds of mortality is higher and, consequently, the effects of
relationships. Investigating such effects might be parenting variation can be more readily observed.
possible with large, retrospective cohort studies of 6. How does pet ownership and/or the presence
seniors in which pet ownership history is compared of a pet affect the way persons are perceived and
with long-term medical records. Such studies evaluated by others, and how do these perceptions
might also include a prospective followup vary across different cultural groups?
component to explore the relationship between
Exploring the possible historical and adaptive
lifetime pet ownership and longevity.
significance of pet keeping will help us to further
3. Similarly, how, if at all, are the putative
understand this apparently paradoxical and long-
health benefits of pet ownership related to the
standing cultural phenomenon. It may also allow us
quality (as opposed to the quantity) of these
to appreciate some of the more subtle, dynamic pro-
relationships? If pets are indeed serving as sources
cesses that go on in multispecies interactions, when
of nonhuman social support, then one would
ongoing relationships influence the life courses of
predict that their impact would depend on how
all the partners involved. Investigation of the cur-
effective these relationships are at satisfying each
rent functions of human–pet relationships, on the
person’s individual social support needs. Future
other hand, will have value in informing contempo-
studies could therefore consider the extent to
rary discussions about the future role for resource-
which particular pets meet or conflict with their
expensive domestic animals in a world suffering
owner’s expectations and perceived social
from exponential human population growth and
requirements, and how this in turn affects the
ever-increasing pressures to reduce carbon dioxide
person’s health. More focused studies could explore
production. This is likely to prove an emotive aca-
the possibility of an interaction between the health
demic and political discourse that may bring more
effects of pet keeping and the social support needs
attention to the fascinating and puzzling relation-
of the individuals concerned.
ship between people and pets than ever before.
4. Does pet keeping in childhood/early
adolescence produce measurable and significant
changes in social and affective skills that are known References
to be important in mothering/parenting—such as Ader, R.L., Cohen, N., & Felten, D. (1995). Psychoneuro-
empathy, social perspective taking, and nurturing? immunology: Interactions between the nervous system and
If the theory that pet keeping fosters nurturing/ the immune system. The Lancet, 345, 99–103.
Allen, K.M., Shykoff, B.E., & Izzo, J.L. (2001). Pet ownership,
parenting skills is correct, it would predict but not ACE inhibitor therapy, blunts home blood pressure
enhanced skills in these areas among pet owning responses to mental stress. Hypertension, 38, 815–820.
versus nonowning children/adolescents. Such Allen, K.M., Blascovich, J., Tomaka, J., & Kelsey, R.M. (1991).
research would need to be carefully designed to Presence of human friends and pet dogs as moderators of
control for potentially confounding factors, such autonomic responses to stress in women. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 61, 582–589.
the levels of empathy and nurturing that are shown Anderson, W.P., Reid, C.M., & Jennings, G.L. (1992). Pet own-
by the parents and other caregivers of children and ership and risk factors for cardiovascular disease. Medical
adolescents who do and do not keep pets. Journal of Australia, 157, 298–301.
5. Is pet keeping in childhood/adolescence related American Animal Hospital Association (1996). National Survey
in any way to reproductive success? Again, if it is true of People and Pet Relationships. Denver, CO: AAHA.
American Pet Products Manufacturers’ Association (APPMA).
that pet keeping fosters nurturing/parenting skills, (2008). APPMA National Pet Owners Survey: 2007–2008.
one would predict a positive association between Greenwich, CT: American Pet Products Manufacturers
early exposure to pet care and subsequent parenting Association Inc.

306 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 306 12/21/2010 10:38:19 AM


Archer, J. (1997). Why do people love their pets? Evolution and on motivation, 1987 (Vol. 35, pp. 91–144). Lincoln:
Human Behavior, 18, 237–259. University of Nebraska Press.
Arman, K. (2007). A new direction for kennel club regula- Davis, S.J.M., & Valla, F. (1978). Evidence for domestication of
tions and breed standards. Canadian Veterinary Journal, 48, the dog 12,000 years ago in the Natufian of Israel. Nature,
953–965. 276, 608–610.
Associated Press (2009). The AP-Petside.com Poll. Retrieved Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University
March 2, 2010 from http://surveys.ap.org/. Press.
Barker, S.B., & Barker, R.T. (1988). The human-canine bond: Dawkins, R., & Krebs, J.R. (1979). Arms races between and
Closer than family ties? Journal of Mental Health Counseling, within species. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London,
10, 46–56. Series B, 205, 489–511.
Basso, C.B. (1973). The Kalapalo Indians of Central Brazil. Dechambre, E. (1949). La Theorie de foetalization et la
New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. formationdes races chiens et de porc. Mammalia, 13,
Benecke, N. (1987). Studies on early dog remains from northern 129–137.
Europe. Journal of Archaeological Science, 14, 31–39. Donaldson, Z.R., & Young, L.J. (2008). Oxytocin, vasopressin,
Bierer, R.E. (2001). The Relationship between pet bonding, self- and the neurogenetics of sociality. Science, 322, 900–904.
esteem and empathy in pre-adolescents. Unpublished PhD dis- Eddy, T.J., Hart, L.A., & Boltz, R.P. (1988). The effects of service
sertation, University of New Mexico. Dissertation Abstracts dogs on social acknowledgements of people in wheelchairs.
International: Section B: The Sciences & Engineering. Journal of Psychology, 122, 39–45.
Vol. 61(11-B), June 2001. Emlen, S.T., Reeve, H.K., Sherman, P.W., Wrege, P.H.,
Burkart, J.M., Hrdy, S.B. & van Schaik, C.P. (2009). Cooperative Ratnieks, F.L.W., & Shellman-Reeve, J. (1991). Adaptive
breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary versus nonadaptive explanations of behavior: The case of
Anthropology, 18, 175–186. alloparental helping. American Naturalist, 138, 259–270.
Cain, A.O. (1985). Pets as family members. Marriage and Family Eriksen, W. (1994). The role of social support in the pathogene-
Review, 8, 5–11. sis of coronary heart disease: A literature review. Family
Cäsar, C., & Young, R.J. (2008). A case of adoption in a wild Practice, 11, 201–209.
group of black-fronted titi monkeys (Callicebus nigrifrons). Erikson, P. (1987). De L’apprivoisement à l’approvisionnement:
Primates, 49, 146–148. Chasse, alliance et familiarisation en Amazonie Amérindienne.
Catanzaro, T.E. (1984). The human-animal bond in military Techniques et Cultures 9, 105–140.
communities. In R.K. Anderson, B.L. Hart, & L.A. Hart Erikson, P. (2000). The social significance of pet-keeping
(Eds.), The pet connection: Its influence on our health and qual- among Amazonian Indians. In A.L. Podberscek, E.S. Paul, &
ity of life (pp. 341–347). Minneapolis: Center to Study J.A. Serpell (Eds.), Companion animals & us: Exploring the
Human-Animal Relationships and Environments, University relationships between people & pets (pp. 7–26). Cambridge:
of Minnesota. Cambridge University Press.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2008). Esterling, B.A., Kiecolt-Glaser, J., Bodnar, J.C., & Glaser, R.
Dog Bite Prevention. Retrieved March 12, 2009 from http:// (1994). Chronic stress, social support, and persistent altera-
www.cdc.gov/HomeandRecreationalSafety/Dog-Bites/ tions in the natural killer cell response to cytokines in older
biteprevention.html. adults. Health Psychology, 13, 291–128.
Cobb, S. (1976). Social support as a moderator of life stress. Fairbanks, L. A. (1990). Reciprocal benefits of allomothering for
Psychosomatic Medicine, 38, 300–314. female vervet monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 40, 553–562.
Collis, G.M., & McNicholas, J. (1998). A theoretical basis for Fausto, C. (1999). Of enemies and pets: Warfare and shamanism
health benefits of pet ownership. In C.C. Wilson & in Amazonia. American Ethnologist, 2, 933–956.
D.C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in human health Fox, M.W. (1978). The dog: Its domestication and behaviour.
(pp. 105–222). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. New York: Garland STPM Press.
Coppinger, R., & Coppinger, L. (2001). Dogs: A startling new Fridlund, A.J., & MacDonald, M. (1988). Approaches to Goldie:
understanding of canine origin, behavior & evolution. New A field study of human response to canine juvenescence.
York: Scribner. Anthrozoös, 11, 95–100.
Coppinger, R.P., Glendinning, J., Torop, E., Matthay, C., Freidmann, E., & Thomas, S.A. (1995). Pet ownership, social
Sutherland, M., & Smith, C. (1987). Degree of behav- support, and one-year survival after acute myocardial infarc-
ioral neoteny differentiates canid polymorphs. Ethology, 75, tion in the Cardiac Arrhythmia Suppression Trial (CAST).
89–108. American Journal of Cardiology, 76, 1213–1217.
Crocker, J.C. (1977). My brother the parrot. In J.D. Sapir & Friedmann, E., Thomas, S.A., & Eddy, T.J. (2000). Companion
J.C. Crocker (Eds.), The social uses of metaphor: Essays on the animals and human health: Physical and cardiovascular influ-
anthropology of rhetoric (pp. 164–192). Philadelphia: ences. In A.L. Podberscek, E. Paul, & J.A. Serpell (Eds.),
University of Pennsylvania Press. Companion animal and us (pp. 125–142). Cambridge,
Cutt, H., Giles-Corti, B., Knuiman, M., & Burke, V. (2007). Cambridge University Press.
Dog ownership, health and physical activity: A critical review Garrity, T.F., & Stallones, L. (1998). Effects of pet contact on
of the literature. Health and Place, 13, 261–272. human well-being: Review of recent research. In C.C. Wilson
Daly, B., & Morton, L.L. (2009). Empathic differences in adults & D.C. Turner (Eds.), Companion animals in human health
as a function of childhood and adult pet ownership and pet (pp. 3–22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
type. Anthrozoös, 22, 371–382. Green, D.M., & Swets, J.W. (1966). Signal detection theory and
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1987). The Darwinian psychology of psychophysics. New York: Wiley.
discriminative parental solicitude. In D.W. Leger (Ed.), Grier, K.C. (2006). Pets in America: A history. Chapel Hill:
Comparative perspectives in modern psychology: Nebraska symposium University of North Carolina Press.

serpell, paul 307

17-Salmon-17.indd 307 12/21/2010 10:38:19 AM


Guéguen, N., & Ciccotti, S. (2008). Domestic dogs as facilita- Maestripieri, D. (2001). Biological bases of maternal attach-
tors in social interaction: An evaluation of helping and court- ment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 79–83.
ship behaviors. Anthrozoös, 21, 339–349. Maestripieri, D., & Pelka, S. (2002). Sex differences in interest in
Hamilton, W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behav- infants across the lifespan: A biological adaptation for par-
ior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–32. enting? Human Nature, 13, 327–344.
Harris Interactive. (2007). Pets are “members of the family” and Maestripieri, D., & Roney, J.R. (2006). Evolutionary developmen-
two-thirds of pet owners buy their pets holiday presents. Retrieved tal psychology: Contributions from comparative research with
March 2, 2010 from http://www.harrisinteractive.com/ nonhuman primates. Developmental Review, 26, 120–137.
harris_poll/index.asp?PID=840. Malek, J. (1993). The Cat in Ancient Egypt. London: British
Harwood, D. (1928). Love for animals and how it developed in Great Museum Press.
Britain. Lampeter: Edwin Mellen Press (republished 2002). Mandhane, P.J., Sears, M.R., Poulton, R., Green, J.M.,
Headey, B. (1998). Health benefits and health cost savings due to Lou, W.Y.W., Taylor, D.R., & Hancox, R.J. (2009). Cats and
pets: Preliminary estimates from an Australian national dogs and the risk of atopy in childhood and adulthood.
survey. Social Indicators Research, 47, 233–243. Journal of Allergy and Clinical Immunology, 124, 745–750.
Heinrichs, M., Baumgartner, T., Kirshbaum, C., & Ehlert, U. McNamara, J.M., & Houston, A.I. (2009). Integrating func-
(2003). Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cor- tion and mechanisms. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 24,
tisol and subjective responses to stress. Biological Psychiatry, 670–675.
54, 1389–1398. Melson, G.F., Schwarz, R.L., & Beck, A.M. (1997). Importance
Herre, E.A., Knowlton, N. Mueller, U.G., & Rehner, S.A. of companion animals in children’s lives–Implications for
(1999). The evolution of mutualisms: Exploring the paths Veterinary Practice. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical
between conflict and cooperation. Trends in Ecology and Association, 211, 1512–1518.
Evolution, 14, 49–53. Messent, P.R. (1984). Correlates and effects of pet ownership.
Herzog, H. (2010). Some we love, some we hate, some we eat: Why In R.K. Anderson, B.L. Hart, & L.A. Hart (Eds.), The pet
it’s so hard to think straight about animals. New York: Harper connection: Its influence of our health and quality of life
Collins. (pp. 331–340). Minneapolis: Centre to Study Human-Animal
House, J.S., Landis, K.R., & Umberson, D. (1988). Social rela- Relationships and Environments, University of Minnesota.
tionships and health. Science, 241(4865), 540–545. Miller, S.C., Kennedy, C., DeVoe, D., Hickey, M., Nelson, T., &
Hrdy, S.B. (2005). Mother Nature: A history of mothers, infants, Kogan, L. (2009). An examination of changes in oxytocin
and natural selection. New York: Pantheon Books. levels before and after interactions with a bonded dog.
Hrdy, S.B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of Anthrozoös, 22, 31–42.
mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Mithen, S. (1996). The prehistory of the mind: A search for the
Press. origins of art, religion and science. London: Thames &
Humphrey, N.J. (1983). Consciousness regained. Oxford: Oxford Hudson.
University Press. Monroe, S.M., Bromet, E.J., Connell, M.M., & Steiner, S.C.
Izar, P., Verderane, M.P., Visalberghi, E., Ottoni, E.B., Gomes de (1986). Social support, life events, and depressive symptoms:
Oliveira, M., Shirley, J., & Fragaszy, D. (2006). Cross-genus A 1-year prospective study. Journal of Consulting & Clinical
adoption of a marmoset by wild capuchin monkeys Psychology, 54 (4), 424–431.
(Callithryx jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 68, Morey, D.F. (1992). Size, shape and development in the evolu-
692–700. tion of the domestic dog. Journal of Archaeological Science,
Johnstone, R.A., & Bshary, R. (2002). From parasitism to mutu- 19, 181–204.
alism: Partner control in asymmetric interactions. Ecology Morey, D.F. (2006). Burying key evidence: The social bond
Letters, 5, 634–639. between dogs and people. Journal of Archaeological Science,
Kikusui, T., Winslow, J.T., & Mori, Y. (2006). Social buffering: 33, 158–175.
Relief from stress and anxiety. Philosophical Transactions of the Morgan, S.P., & Taylor, M.G. (2006). Low fertility at the turn
Royal Society, Series B. 361, 2215–2228. of the twenty first century. Annual Review of Sociology, 32,
Kiecolt-Glaser, J.K., & Newton, T.L. (2001). Marriage and 375–399.
health: Jis and hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127 (4), 472–503. Nafstad P., Magnus P., Gaarder P.I., & Jaakkola J.J. (2001).
Kramer, K.L. (2005). Children’s help and the pace of reproduc- Exposure to pets and atopy-related diseases in the first 4 years
tion: Cooperative breeding in humans. Evolutionary of life. Allergy, 56, 307–312.
Anthropology, 14, 224–237. Nagasawa, M., Kikusui, T., Onaka, T., & Ohta, M. (2009). Dog’s
Lim, M.M., & Young, L.J. (2006). Neuropeptide regulation of gaze at its owner increases owner’s urinary oxytocin during
affiliative behavior and social bonding in animals. Hormones social interaction. Hormones & Behavior, 55, 434–441.
and Behavior, 50, 506–517. Odendaal, J., & Meintjes, R. (2003). Neurophysiological
Lockwood, R. (1983). The influence of animals on social percep- correlates of affiliative behavior between humans and dogs.
tion. In A. H. Katcher & A. M. Beck (Eds.), New perspectives The Veterinary Journal, 165, 296–301.
on our lives with companion animals (pp. 64–71). Philadelphia: Ory, M.G., & Goldberg, E.L. (1983). Pet possession and life
University of Pennsylvania Press. satisfaction in elderly women. In A.H. Katcher & A.M. Beck
Lorenz, K. (1971). Studies in animal and human behaviour (Eds.), New perspectives on our lives with companion animals
(vol. 2). (Translation by R. Martin; First published 1950). (pp. 303–317). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Press.
Mader, B., Hart, L.A., & Bergin, B. (1989). Social acknowledge- Paul, E.S., & Serpell, J.A. (1992). Why children keep pets: The
ments for children with disabilities: Effects of service dogs. influence of child and family characteristics. Anthrozoös, 5,
Child Development, 60, 1529–1534. 231–244.

308 pets in the family

17-Salmon-17.indd 308 12/21/2010 10:38:19 AM


Paul, E.S., & Serpell, J.A. (1993). Childhood pet keeping and (Eds.), Animals & human society: Changing perspectives
humane attitudes in young adulthood. Animal Welfare, 2, (pp. 127–144). London: Routledge.
321–337. Sherbourne, C.D., Meredith, L.S., Rogers, W., & Ware, J.E.
Pew Research Center. (2006). Gauging family intimacy: Dogs edge (1992). Social support and stressful life events: Age differ-
cats (dads trail both). A Social Trends Report. Washington, ences in their effects on health-related quality of life among
DC: Pew Research Center. the chronically ill. Quality of Life Research, 1, 235–246.
Poresky, R.H., & Hendrix, C. (1990). Differential effects of pet Siegel, J.M. (1990). Stressful life events and use of physician
presence and pet bonding on young children. Psychological services among the elderly: The moderating role of pet
Reports, 67, 51–54. ownership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
Putnam, R.D. (2000). Bowling alone. New York: Simon & 1081–1086.
Schuster. Silk, J.B. (1990). Human adoption in evolutionary perspective.
Riedman, M.L. (1982). The evolution of alloparental care and Human Nature, 1, 25–52.
adoption in mammals and birds. Quarterly Review of Biology, Thomas, K. (1983). Man and the natural world: Changing atti-
57, 405–435. tudes in England, 1500–1800. London: Allen Lane.
Ritvo, H. (1988). The emergence of modern pet-keeping. In Tinbergen, N. (1963). On the aims and methods of ethology.
A.N. Rowan (Ed.), Animals and people sharing the world Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.
(pp. 13–31). Hanover, NH: University Press of New Uchino, B.N. (2006). Social support and health: A review of
England. physiological processes potentially underlying links to disease
Robertson, I.D., & Thompson, R.C. (2002). Enteric parasitic outcome. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 29, 377–387.
zoonoses of domesticated dogs and cats. Microbes and Vale, B., & Vale, R.J.D. (2009). Time to eat the dog? The real guide
Infection, 4, 867–873. to sustainable living. London: Thames and Hudson.
Salisbury, J.E. (1994). The beast within: Animals in the Middle Vilhjalmson, R. (1993). Life stress, social support and clinical
Ages. London & New York: Routledge. depression: A reanalysis of the literature. Social Science
Sanefuji, W., Ohgami, H., & Hashiya, K. (2007). Development Medicine, 37, 331–342.
of preference for baby faces across species in humans (Homo Vigne, J.D., Guilaine, J., Debue, K., Haye, L., & Gérard, P.
sapiens). Journal of Ethology, 25, 249–254. (2004). Early taming of the cat in Cyprus. Science,
Schwarzer, R., & Knoll, N. (2007). Functional roles of social 304, 259.
support within the stress and coping process: A theoretical Virués-Ortega, J., & Buela-Casal, G. (2006). Psychophysiological
and empirical overview. International Journal of Psychology, effects of human-animal interaction: Theoretical issues and
42, 243–252. long-term interaction effects. The Journal of Nervous and
Serpell, J.A. (1989). Pet keeping and animal domestication: Mental Disease, 194, 52–57.
A reappraisal. In J. Clutton-Brock (Ed.), The walking larder: Visek-Vidovic, V., Stetic, V.V., & Bratko, D. (1999). Pet owner-
Patterns of animal domestication, pastoralism & predation ship, type of pet and socio-emotional development of school
(pp. 10–21). London: Unwin Hyman. children. Anthrozoös, 12, 211–217.
Serpell, J.A. (1991). Beneficial effects of pet ownership on some Visek-Vidovic, V., Arambasic, L., Kerestes, G., Kuterovac-
aspects of human health and behavior. Journal of the Royal Jagodic, G., & Vlahovic-Stetic, V. (2001). Pet ownership in
Society of Medicine, 84, 717–720. childhood and socio-emotional characteristics, work values
Serpell, J.A. (1996). In the company of animals: A study of human- and professional choices in early adulthood. Anthrozoös, 14,
animal relationships. Cambridge: Cambridge University 224–231.
Press. Voith, V.L. (2009). The impact of companion animal problems
Serpell, J.A. (2000). Domestication and history of the cat. In on society and the role of veterinarians. Veterinary Clinics of
D.C. Turner & P.P.G. Bateson (Eds.), The domestic cat: The North America–Small Animal Practice, 39, 327.
biology of its behavior (pp. 180–192). Cambridge: Cambridge Wells, D.L. (2004). The facilitation of social interactions by
University Press. domestic dogs. Anthrozoös, 17, 340–352.
Serpell, J.A. (2003). Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic Wells, D.L. (2009). The effects of animals on human health and
selection–Beyond the “cute response.” Society and Animals, well-being. Journal of Social Issues, 65, 523–543.
11, 83–99. Williams, G.C. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A cri-
Serpell, J.A., & Paul, E.S. (1994). Pets and the development of tique of current evolutionary thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
positive attitudes to animals. In A. Manning & J.A. Serpell University Press.

serpell, paul 309

17-Salmon-17.indd 309 12/21/2010 10:38:19 AM

View publication stats

You might also like