You are on page 1of 6

Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August

2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

Towards an empirically based definition of piano touch: Touch/timbre relationship


and key motion measurements
Eleonora Kojucharov,*1 Antonio Rodà,#2
*1
Departments of Musicology, University of Ghent, Belgium, Université Paris 8 Saint-Denis, France
#2
Department of Information Engineering, University of Padova, Italy
1 2
eleonora.kojucharov@ugent.be, roda@dei.unipd.it

resonances, as well as of temporal movement variations,


ABSTRACT that consistently enrich the musical communicative
The concept of touch in piano pedagogy is generally expressed processes. Naturally, one might say that touch parameters
through adjectival descriptors often linked to imagination and are barely constant in a piano execution, since their course
abstraction. Such a metaphorically based reflection determined a and nuances are adjusted according to the specific
rather blurred definition of the term touch, sometimes confused performer's intentions and his/her physiological, cultural
with timbre, even within professional piano performance contexts. and psychological condition at the moment of performing.
Although in last decades the improved MIDI-based data- Nevertheless, such a controlled combination of elements in
collection technologies allowed to specifically measure key- keyboard manipulation, also plays an essential role for
control features, an empirically based definition of touch and its achieving a high degree of expressivity in piano execution
relationship with timbre still need to be further deepened. In this
and phrasing. Moreover, the personal modalities with which
direction, the present research aims at 1) providing elements for
pianists combine touch parameters during performance may
an adequate scientific understanding of touch; (2) clarifying the
relationship between touch and timbre; (3) defining to what extent
also present some recurring features, as if responding to a
the parameters of key-velocity, tempo and articulation may vary as sort of internal coherence: it is this coherence that makes
a function of different types of touch. Through questionnaires 19 any performer's sound and attitude towards the instrument
pianists provided a number of piano-touch quality descriptors; the typical and unique, and that may allow one to distinguish
five most used were proposed as example of touch qualities each pianist's touch and performance style independently
during a recording-session involving only 5 pianists, who played from its correspondence to any physical or aesthetic
four music excerpts one time for each descriptor. The analysis of characteristics of music. However, although easily
pianists' answers in questionnaires allowed an interesting recognizable through an instinctive and qualitative
conceptual classification of timbre/touch into five semantic approach, such a spontaneous combination of elements in
categories: physical-motor, articulatory, sensory, emotional, modulating touch still needs to be deepened and clarified in
aesthetic-stylistic. The analysis of recordings showed significant quantitative terms.
relations in pianists' performance control between key-
Until the first half of the 20th century, in fact, the
velocity/articulation parameters and touch; timing-related aspects
speculation on touch-related aspects of piano performance
seemed to be less influenced by touch changes. As an initial step
of investigation, this research provides practical basis for further
developed in a very qualitative way, mostly resting on
theoretical developments benefiting both a scientific description rather subjective methods of observation and judgement: an
of touch and pedagogical applications, going towards a deeper objective lack of adequate technological systems for
awareness about key-motion control among pianists. measuring musical performance mostly lead researchers and
piano pedagogues to approach the physiological
mechanisms of piano technique from a primarily anatomical
I. INTRODUCTION point of view (Breithaupt 1909, Schultz 1936, Kochevitsky
Belonging to the field of piano performance and 1967, Neuhaus 1973). Indeed, with the exception of very
especially focusing on the keyboard control, the present few studies on piano touch specifically undertaken within a
study aims at objectively describing the piano touch and at scientific framework (Ortmann 1925), the historical piano
shedding light on the means with which it can be modeled, schools of the 19th century used to express the concept of
by: (1) providing elements for an adequate scientific touch mainly through a large amount of imaginative and
understanding of touch; (2) clarifying the relationship abstract metaphorical adjectives. Such a kind of literary
between touch and timbre; (3) defining to what extent the approach, that frequently still exists in the contemporary
parameters of key-velocity, tempo and articulation may piano teaching methods, although fascinating, contributed
vary as a function of different types of touch. to consolidate a rather blurred definition of touch,
Since the 19th Century piano performers and pedagogues sometimes confused with timbre, or even with sound, so
recognised the piano touch as a key element of musical that even professional pianists often show some uncertainty
expression; a very fascinating as much as precious means in discerning between, defining and describing these
by which concert pianists are able to deeply express their concepts.
own musical voice, by modelling sounds through the However, in recent decades, technological developments
production of very subtle timbre nuances, colours and have provided new directions and theoretical approaches to
Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August 2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

reflection on music performance. The paradigm of II. METHOD


embodied music cognition (EMC) (Leman 2008), for
This study has been conducted in Italy and included an
example, and other studies on the performers' movements,
experimental part which was realised at the Centro di
opened up novel theoretical perspectives on gestural
Sonologia Computazionale (Centre of Computational
aspects of expressive performance: differently from the
Sonology, C.S.C.) of the University of Padova. The
classical disembodied view, that considers physical action
research was articulated on a double analytical path, in
and perception as separate entities, the EMC paradigm
order to approach on one side the touch-timbre relationship
particularly underlines the complementarity of action and
as it is theoretically conceived by professional pianists, and
perception in musical communication, focusing on the
provide on the other side a more practical view on what
human body’s central role in influencing these processes,
pianists effectively do at keyboard while performing.
by bringing together matter and mind. Also, studies have
Precisely, the procedure consisted in six steps: (1)
been carried out on expressive gestures in instrumental
acquisition of information on concert pianists’ conception
performance (Camurri et al. 2001, Canazza et al. 2004,
of touch and timbre, through a questionnaire; (2) analysis of
Maes et al. 2013), in which the touch has been objectively
questionnaires and selection of touch/timbre descriptors; (3)
investigated as the first step of a more complex analysis of
selection of piano pieces to be recorded; (4) recording of
piano skills, involving the entire body’s movements. Such
expressive performances by five pianists; (5) analysis of
an innovative view leads us to reconsider the habitual
questionnaires; (6) extraction and analysis of the audio data.
modalities of musical analysis and to consider the point of
A quantitative approach was used for classifying touch
contact between the performer's body, the instrument and
descriptors and for midi-data analysis, and a qualitative
the properties of sound to be a central aspect of any
method for interpreting questionnaires.
investigation on the processes involved in sound
production. A. Participants
In parallel, from a highly practical point of view as well, Nineteen concert pianists (four women and fifteen men,
MIDI-based technologies have recently allowed the control aged between 28-62), were asked to complete a
of touch on the keyboard to be measured and analysed more questionnaire on piano touch and timbre. They were
objectively and in greater detail (Lee 1989, 1990, Salmon selected among professional musicians coming from and
and Newmark 1989, Dalla Bella and Palmer 2004), by working in different areas of Italy, having an experience
detecting subtle features of movement (total kinetic energy, both as performers within international contexts, and as
position, velocity, acceleration, finger height, dynamic pedagogues at music schools and conservatories. After
control, finger-joint force). Other studies have provided having answered the questionnaire, five of them were also
insights on timbre and touch, shedding light on the role of asked to take part in the recording session, by playing some
hammer velocity and intensity in sound production, and on musical excerpts.
factors involving gesture and key-control used by pianists
to produce different nuances in timbre (Goebl et al. 2004, B. Questionnaire
Bernays and Traube 2013), identifying specific patterns in The questionnaire consisted in two parts. The first one
terms of dynamics, attack, key-depression depth, pedalling included a sets of questions aiming at gathering participants'
and articulation. professional information, such as education, concerts
Nevertheless, despite the big progresses made in experience, recordings, teaching activity, etc.. The second
measuring elements of musical performance, finally part consisted in five questions asking for a number of
allowing the overcome of any literary based description of definitions of piano touch and timbre, and a list of
touch parameters, an unequivocal definition of touch adjectival descriptors. Questions were presented as openly
specifically focusing on the relationship between touch and as possible, in order to leave the participants free to express
timbre still needs to be put forward. With this purpose, the their ideas.
present research intends to provide further elements for a
scientific approach to the topic. In fact, by taking a distance C. Piano touch/timbre descriptors
from subjective, metaphorical and literary descriptions of The questionnaires supplied a list of 209 adjectival
touch, as passed on through historical piano schools, one touch/timbre descriptors, which have been later evaluated
particular aim is here to determine the extent to which key- by semantic proximity and then divided into five categories
control parameters (key-velocity, articulation and tempo) (physical-motor, articulatory, sensory, emotional, aesthetic-
can be influenced by the pianist's ability to specifically stylistic). The most recurring adjectives identified for
focus on different qualities of touch. especially defining touch were legato, staccato, light, heavy
So, in what exactly does piano touch consist, and which and soft. These five touch descriptors have been also used
features determine its difference from timbre? Moreover, as a basis of the experimental part of this study, including a
how can we recognize pianists' touch only according to the piano performance recording.
aural features of their sound?
Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August 2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

D. Musical fragments mechanisms of touch control. However, a recurring


Four fragments were selected from various romantic and definition of piano touch emerged, conceived as the result
modern piano works of a medium level of technical of the relationship between three elements: (1) the
difficulty: namely Grieg's Wächterlied from Lyric pieces expression of the performer's musical thinking, imagination
op. 12, Mendelssohn's Volkslied from Lieder ohne Worte and ability to communicate feelings and ideas; (2) his/her
op. 53 and Takemitsu's Clouds. Tempo indications were bodily awareness and capability to manipulate sounds, in
conserved on the scores, while all agogic and dynamics order to acoustically achieve a musical idea, through
signs were removed, in order not to influence pianists' gestures, movements and attitudes; (3) the influence that the
interpretative intentions. physical properties of the instrument, as well as the
environment acoustics, may have on sound control and
E. Equipment production.
A Yamaha Disklavier C3 grand piano was used for the From a global view on questionnaires, the idea of touch
recording session, equipped with optical sensors that and timbre seems to go well beyond a mere description of
enabled subtle keys movements to be measured acoustical and physical-motor functions, as it also includes
(onset/offset time of keys, and the key-velocity of each references to the sensory sphere, as well as to
note). The performances were recorded both as audio historical/aesthetic/stylistic categories. The pianists
signals (microphone AKG C 414) and MIDI-data provided, in fact, an imaginative list of 209 descriptors, a
(Disklavier's optical sensors and control unit), and also large number of which was used to define touch (122), and
video-recorded with a video camera (Canon MVX330i), a smaller number for timbre (87); in addition, some
capturing a lateral view of the performer's torso, arms and adjectives were used for representing both touch and timbre
fingers. (26), as a demonstration of the blurred frames that usually
enclose these concepts. According to the participants'
F. Procedure answers and comments, it can be generally assumed that
By answering a questionnaire, the nineteen piano timbre and touch in piano performance are intended as
performers were requested to give a general definition of strictly related to each other, although not perfectly
keyboard touch and timbre, and to provide a list of their coinciding, and timbre seems to be conceived as “deriving”
respective qualitative adjectival descriptors. The already from touch.
mentioned five most often recurring touch adjectives The analysis continued with the semantic classification
(legato, staccato, light, heavy, soft) were then selected, and of the descriptors: five main categories have been here
in parallel four musical excerpts were chosen and prepared identified, providing a multi-layered view on the subject,
for a recording session, which practically involved only five that seems to very well reflect the articulated conception of
performers. The selected adjectives were so proposed to the piano touch and timbre, and the different levels of
pianists, each of them to be performed as an example of abstraction normally occurring in their appraisal. Going
different touch qualities. The recording procedure consisted into detail of each category, the physical-motor one
in six total executions of each musical fragment, initially includes the adjectives describing body parts, actions and
played in a personal version (ad libitum), and afterwards movements; the articulatory makes a more direct reference
repeated five more times, each time taking one of the five to elements of musical phrasing, such as the length of
descriptors into account. It was strictly prohibited to use the sound/key-depression, the key-velocity attack and release,
pedals, so as to let performers concentrate on their fingers' dynamic variations, etc.; to the sensory category belong
activity. adjectives related to the human senses, as well as to
chromatic/thermal synaesthesias, and to qualities,
III. RESULTS & DISCUSSION dimensions and spatial features of matter; all evocative
images and adjectives expressing feelings and sensations
G. Analysis of questionnaires: a view on the touch- were listed in the emotional category; last, the aesthetic-
timbre relationship stylistic one included formal and structural definitions
The participant pianists' written contributions on piano linked to musical styles, traditional practices, authors,
touch and timbre were very illuminating and allowed a genres and instruments. The following table (Tab. 1) shows
rather realistic understanding of the complex conception of some examples from the list of descriptors used by pianists,
touch and timbre that can be generally found among divided by category.
professional piano performers, as well as the rather low
degree of consciousness that they may show in the practical
application of such a theoretical reflections to their piano
performances. Globally speaking, in fact, although a
number of pianists participating in this study provided very
articulated, detailed and interesting reflections on the topic,
most of them on the contrary gave brief and simple
answers, expressing a rather generic knowledge about
Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August 2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

Table 1. Semantic partition of adjectives. to be strongly stressed: in fact, the 26 descriptors commonly
Semantic Examples (touch Touch Timbre used by pianists to represent both timbre and touch were all
Description placed in the sensory category, in between physical and
category and timbre adj.) adj. adj.
by arm, by finger tip, cultural levels, as an ideal connection between the human
by curved finger, by senses and various aspects of matter (dimensional, spatial,
shoulder, from a chromatic and thermal).
PHYSICAL- body parts /
MOTOR
distance, from the 35 4
movements
key, percussive,
quick, slow, vertical
70
60
legato, tenuto,
musical 50 PHYSICAL-MOTOR
ARTICULAT marked, pizzicato,
ORY
12 2 40 ARTICULATORY
phrasing portato, staccato
30 SENSORY
bright, clear, deep, 20 EMOTIONAL
definite, delicate, AESTHETIC-
10
dense, electric, fluid, STYLISTIC
0
human hard, heavy, inactive,
Timbre
senses / intense, light, misty,
SENSORY 38 64 Touch Touch/Timbre
features of pearly, penetrating,
matter plain, rigid,
smoothed, soft, sweet,
white Figure 1. Adjectival semantic partition.
Although the most recent studies conducted on this
aggressive, brutal, subject tend to focus on physical parameters of touch (such
dark, deep, energetic, as the key-velocity and the articulation control), however
ethereal, evocative, the complex outlook here expressed by pianists seems thus
harmonious, magic,
images / to suggest that an exhaustive definition of piano touch may
EMOTIONAL nervous, open, 17 14
feelings not exclusively be based on measuring physical properties,
persuasive, powerful,
relaxed, tender, but that it conversely needs to consider a multi-layered
violent taxonomy of descriptors, based on various levels of
abstraction: a lower level linked to physical properties (cat.
approximate, 1-2), a medium and more abstract level (cat. 3-4), and a
Bachian, baroque, very highly refined level (cat. 5) that rests on conceptual
"beautiful", judgements derived from specific cultural backgrounds. As
cantabile, classical, such, the present study wishes to underline the need for
styles /
controlled, elegant, integrating various perspectives while attempting to give a
AESTHETIC- traditions / 25 12
STYLISTIC expressive, jazz, complete definition of touch, thus including both scientific-
genres
modern, Mozartian,
theoretical and artistic-practical reflections.
imitating other
instruments, H. Musical data analysis
romantic, "ugly"
As regards the second investigative path of the present
research, which considers an empirical view on piano
Interestingly, according to this classification – as the
keyboard control, an evaluation of the musical recordings
following graph shows (Fig. 1) – the touch descriptors are
was made. Here some precise touch parameters and
almost equally distributed among the five semantic
nuances were extracted and quantified by measuring the
categories, with a dominance of physical-motor and sensory
onset/offset time between one key pressure and the
ones; whereas on the other side timbre descriptors
subsequent one. Sound variations were captured in terms
exclusively figure into the sensory, emotional, aesthetic-
of: key velocity (KV), from a minimum value of 0 to a
stylistic categories. Going in greater detail, the five most
maximum of 127; temporal duration in beats per minute
recurring adjectives identified for especially defining touch
(BPM); the articulation interval between notes (A), where
mainly belong to the articulatory (legato, staccato) and
the value =1 expresses a standard legato (minimum onset
sensory categories (light, soft, heavy). Such a partition
interval in between notes), >1 a more legato effect
seems, thus, to suggest however a stricter connection
(overlapping of subsequent notes), and <1 a less legato
between the idea of touch and the physical aspects of
effect.
musical performance, as well as between the idea of timbre
As the data clearly show, pianists are able to precisely
and the sensory, emotional and aesthetic spheres.
control performance parameters in relation to the 5 different
Moreover, the central role of the sensory sphere seems here
touch types, by translating definitions of touch into different
Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August 2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

sound nuances through a very subtle manipulation of the


keys. Nevertheless, such an ability to obtain a detailed and 25,00

expressive piano performance seems to be rather 20,00

instinctive, as pianists frequently appeared not to be overly 15,00

precise in verbally explaining touch mechanisms in terms of 10,00

KV and A control. 5,00 Tot av. – differences in KV


0,00 Tot st. dev. – differences in KV
A comparison of the pianists' recordings allowed a high
-5,00
degree of agreement to emerge with respect to the influence
-10,00
that the 5 touch qualities have on key-parameters:
-15,00
significant relations were found, in particular, between the
-20,00
touch descriptors and changes in intensity (KV legato light soft heavy staccato
manipulation) and articulation (A), thus allowing KV and A
to emerge as highly characteristic aspects of touch.
Figure 2. KV-comparison between db/al performances for all
Conversely, the lesser degree of concordance between pianists (total average and standard deviation).
speed and touch descriptors suggests that metronome
variation (BPM) is not as representative of touch as KV and
As regards the second parameter here considered, that is
A. The table below (Tab. 2) gives a global view of the
the articulation (A) (Tab. 2), the data show that in order to
results of the five pianists' executions.
obtain a legato pianists homogeneously used the greatest
Table 2. Total average and standard deviation of KV, A, BPM number of overlapping notes (average of 1,04), whereas the
(KV: 0/127 scale; A: 1 = standard legato >1 = overlapping staccato executions logically present the lowest A value
notes, <1 = separated notes; BPM = beats per minute). (0,31). Also, again by comparing the db versions with the
ad libitum ones (Fig. 3), the biggest divergence in A evenly
Recorded version KV A BPM appears for all the pianists in the staccato versions,
followed by the light, legato and soft, whereas any
av st_d av st_d av st_d difference seems to emerge between the heavy and ad
libitum ones.
legato 52 11 1,04 0,3 95 20

light 43 6 0,74 0,25 101 18 0,40

soft 49 8 0,95 0,2 94 18 0,20

0,00
heavy 77 5 0,92 0,2 96 18
Tot av. – differences in A
-0,20
Tot st. dev.– differences in A
staccato 63 11 0,31 0,17 111 12
-0,40
ad libitum 58 10 0,92 0,32 95 18
-0,60

As the KV values provided here indicate, changes in -0,80


legato light soft heavy staccato
intensity are apparently related to an idea of 'weight': the
highest KV corresponds in fact to the heavy versions, and
the lowest KV to the light versions. Also, another
interesting perspective may be taken into account by Figure 3. A-comparison between db/al performances for all
comparing the descriptor-based performances (db) with the pianists (total average and standard deviation).
ad libitum ones (al), represented by the level “0” in the
Lastly, looking at the temporal variations of the
following graph (Fig. 2): here the greater difference in KV
performances, as mentioned earlier a lower degree of
manipulation occurs in the heavy and light performances.
concordance can be identified among pianists: any evident
connection between touch-descriptors and tempo (BPM)
seems to emerge, thus suggesting that touch variations have
a rather small influence on BPM, and that BPM may be not
a very representative aspect of touch. However, from a
comparison with the ad libitum version (Fig. 4), pianists
seem to associate a fast tempo with the staccato/light
descriptors, and a slow tempo with the heavy/soft/legato
descriptors.
Proceedings of the Ninth Triennial Conference of the European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music, 17-22 August 2015, Manchester, UK
Ginsborg, J., Lamont, A., Phillips, M., Bramley, S. (Editors)

ACKNOWLEDGMENT
18,00
16,00 A very special thanks is due to Mario Baroni for his
14,00 support to this study.
12,00
10,00
Tot av. – differences in BPM
REFERENCES
8,00
6,00
Tot st. dev. – differences in BPM Bernays, M., & Traube, C. (2013). Expressive production of
4,00
piano timbre: Touch and playing techniques for timbre
2,00
control in piano performance. In Proceedings of the Sound
0,00 and Music Computing Conference 2013, SMC2013.
-2,00 Stockholm, Sweden.
legato light soft heavy staccato Breithaupt, R. M. (1909). Natural piano technic. Trad. John
Bernhoff. Leipzig: CF Kahnt Nachfolger.
Canazza, S., De Poli, G., Drioli, C., Rodà, A., Vidolin, A. (2004).
Figure 4. BPM-comparison between db/al performances for Modeling and control of expressiveness in music
all pianists (total average and standard deviation). performance. In Proceedings of the IEEE, 92(4) (pp. 686-
701).
Camurri, A., De Poli, G., Leman, M., Volpe, G. (2001). A multi-
IV. CONCLUSION layered conceptual framework for expressive gestures
applications. In Proc. intl MOSART workshop, Barcelona.
With a view to avoiding metaphorically based
Dalla Bella, S., & Palmer, C. (2004). Tempo and dynamics in
reflections, this research allows plausible and encouraging piano performance: The role of movement amplitude. In
assumptions to emerge which may enable further theoretical Proceedings of the International Conference on Music
developments. Especially by confirming the validity of the Perception and Cognition (pp. 256-257).
analytical and technological systems of calculation, this Goebl, W., Bresin, R., Galembo, A. (2004). Once again: The
study can help stimulate pianists' reflection on touch-related perception of piano touch and tone. Can touch audibly change
technical aspects of piano performance, thus leading to a piano sound independently of intensity. In Proceedings of the
more fluent transmission even in pedagogical terms of International Symposium on Musical Acoustics (pp. 332-
specific knowledge concerning key control. Moreover, on 335).
Kochevitsky, G. (1967). The art of piano playing: A scientific
the basis of the collected data, a preliminary definition of
approach. Secaucus, NJ: Summy-Birchard.
touch may be hypothesized as a result of a more or less Lee, S. H. (1989). Using the personal computer to analyze piano
homogeneous interaction between three aspects of key performance. Psychomusicology, 8, special issue on
control: KV, A, BPM. So, it could be assumed that an microcomputers in psychomusicology research, 143-149.
individuation and measurement, in terms of relationship Lee, S. H. (1990). Pianists' hand ergonomics and touch control.
between KV, A and BPM, of that kind of 'internal Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 5(2), 72-78.
coherence' which makes pianists' personal musical Leman, M. (2008). Embodied music cognition and mediation
expression unique and recognisable, could provide a key technology. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Mit Press.
Maes, P. J., Leman, M., Palmer, C., Wanderley, M. M. (2013).
towards a scientific understanding of touch, thus suggesting
Action-based effects on music perception. Frontiers in
the development of a hypothetical paradigm for a its psychology, 4.
definition, allowing for an exact description of the complex Neuhaus, H. (1973). The art of piano playing. (Eng. trans. by
interaction between acoustic, physical and emotional K.A. Leibovitch). London: Barrie and Jenkins.
elements. However, although generally illuminating, these Ortmann, O. (1925). The physical basis of piano touch and tone.
results can only constitute the initial step of a longer London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., Ltd..
investigative path, and still need to be integrated with more Salmon, P. & Newmark, J. (1989). Clinical applications of MIDI
data. Next research may include, for example, a deeper technology. Medical Problems of Performing Artists, 4(1),
exploration of the specific touch/BPM relationship features, 25-31.
Schultz, A. (1936). The riddle of the pianist's finger and its
as well as further experiments involving a larger number of
relationship to a touch-scheme. New York: Carl Fischer Inc..
pianists, and a novel analysis of the video-recordings
through a visually-based assessment of performers' bodily
movements.
Finally, this study offered an interesting view on the
topic from the practical perspective of piano performers',
whose verbal contributions actually showed a much broader
and more articulated conception of touch – related not only
to physical and motor descriptions, but also to references to
emotional and cultural-aesthetic spheres – thus providing
significant insights that may however integrate any attempt
to approach a scientific definition of touch, giving a more
complete view on the subject.

You might also like