You are on page 1of 9

The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination

(ACE) in the Differential Diagnosis of Early


Dementias Versus Affective Disorder
Robert B. Dudas, M.D., M.Phil.
German E. Berrios, M.D., D.M., FRCPsych
John R. Hodges, M.D., FRCP

Objective: The authors describe the profile of performance of patients whose cognitive
complaint is due to dementia, affective disorder, or combinations thereof on the Ad-
denbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) test battery. Methods: Authors tested 90
subjects with dementia (63 Alzheimer disease [AD]; 27 fronto-temporal dementia
[FTD]), 60 subjects with “pure” affective disorder (23 major depression [MDD], 37
whose affective symptoms did not meet criteria for major depression [Affective]); 22
patients with symptoms of affective disorder and organic dementia (Mixed); and 127
healthy volunteers (NC). Results: The total ACE scores for the AD, FTD, and Mixed
groups were significantly lower than for the NC group. Likewise, on total score, the
AD and FTD groups scored significantly lower than either of the “pure” affective-dis-
order groups. Within the dementia group, the AD group scored significantly lower
than the fronto-temporal group. Conclusions: The profile of performance on the ACE
of patients with dementia is different from that of patients suffering from affective
illness. Mild impairment in the total ACE score, along with a low score on the memory
domain tasks and letter fluency (in contrast to normal category fluency), are strongly
indicative of an affective, as opposed to organic, pathology. A total score of ⬍88 in
suspected dementia patients with affective symptoms appears strongly predictive of
an underlying organic disorder. (Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2005; 13:218–226)

T he Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination is a


brief, 15–20-minute test battery originally de-
signed to detect and classify different kinds of de-
ory, language, and visuospatial components. The
maximum score is 100, weighed as follows: orienta-
tion (10), attention (8), memory (35), verbal fluency
mentia, particularly Alzheimer disease (AD) and (14), language (28), and visuospatial ability (5). The
fronto-temporal dementia (FTD), without the use of ACE was validated on 115 dementia (AD, FTD, and
specialized test equipment.1 It incorporates the Mini- vascular dementia), and 24 non-dementia patients,
Mental State Exam (MMSE),2 with additional mem- and 127 NC subjects. Of note is the fact that patients

Received March 14, 2003; revised September 15, 2003; July 26, 2004; accepted July 31, 2004. From the Department of Psychiatry, University of
Cambridge (RBD,GEB), and the Department of Neurology, University of Cambridge and the MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit (JRH). Send
correspondence and reprint requests to Robert B. Dudas, M.D., Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Addenbrooke’s Hospital (Box
189), Hills Road, Cambridge, UK. e-mail: rbd21@cam.ac.uk
䉷 2005 American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry

218 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005


Dudas et al.

with affective pathology were excluded from this sion [Ham-D] and other standard instruments for
study. When the lower cut-off score of 83 was used, affective change). We were particularly interested in
the sensitivity of the ACE to detect dementia was the ability of the ACE to detect progressive degen-
79%, one-third more than the MMSE using the con- erative dementias in patients presenting with diffi-
ventional cut-off score of 24. The ACE appeared to be cult, mixed symptoms.
especially sensitive in FTD, where it nearly doubled
the rate of detection, compared with the MMSE. At a
cut-off score of 88, in a memory clinic with a dementia
METHODS
prevalence rate of 40%, a high proportion (68%) of
those who screen positive will have dementia, and
Subjects
94% of those above the cut-off score will not. Age,
number of years of education, or gender did not in- A total of 299 patients participated: major depres-
fluence predictive outcome during the initial valida- sive disorder (MDD; N⳱23), Affective (N⳱37), AD
tion. (N⳱63), FTD (N⳱27), mixed: possible progressive
Patients presenting with combinations of cognitive, degenerative dementing disorder with an affective
affective, and behavioral problems pose a clinical co- component (N⳱22), and healthy volunteers (NC;
nundrum. In some cases, it is difficult to establish N⳱127). The research protocol was approved by the
whether the cognitive impairment is secondary to an Local Research Ethics Committee. Data for the pa-
affective disorder, or a more sinister, organic de- tients with dementia were collected from the Cam-
menting process—most often AD. Depressive symp- bridge Memory Clinic, and for those with MDD from
toms are common in the elderly population without the Medical Research Council Link Study between
dementia.3,4 A small proportion of depressed individ- June 1996 and April 2001. Patients were excluded if
uals present with very significant cognitive under- they had 1) any concurrent psychiatric illness other
functioning, formerly known as depressive pseudo- than those on the affective spectrum (excluding bi-
dementia, also termed “functional (as opposed to polar disorder according to ICD-10), such as schizo-
organic) dementia,” “memory disorder in the context phrenia, or obsessive-compulsive disorder, or 2)
of depressive illness,” or “the dementia syndrome of causes of cognitive impairment other than primary
depression.” In clinical practice, this group remains degenerative or affective pathology (e.g., vascular de-
difficult to identify. Approximately 10% of the pa- mentia, closed head injury, or alcoholism). The
tients with depression are misdiagnosed as having healthy volunteers for the NC group were ascer-
dementia,5,6 leading to a misdiagnosis of depression tained from a previous study.1 Exclusion criteria were
rather than early dementia. One of the major con- history of head injury, drug abuse, alcoholism, and
founding factors is that many patients with organic cognitive complaints of neurological or psychiatric
dementia also have affective symptoms. Patients with origin.
AD often contact their general practitioner or the The diagnosis of AD was in accordance with the
memory clinic with complaints of depressive symp- National Institute of Neurological and Communica-
toms. tive Disorders and Stroke–AD and Related Disorders
Several screening and diagnostic tests for dementia Association (NINCDS-ADA) criteria.10 The diagnosis
(such as the CAMDEX7 or the Mattis Dementia Rat- of FTD was used in all cases with focal lobar degen-
ing Scale8) are available, but their complexity and eration, including patients with the core feature of
need for specialized test equipment put them beyond personality and behavioral changes (frontal-variant
routine bedside use.9 FTD), non-fluent spontaneous speech (progressive
The aim of this study was to investigate the ability non-fluent aphasia), and fluent empty spontaneous
of the ACE to discriminate between cognitive defi- speech with semantic breakdown (semantic demen-
cits resulting from dementia versus those from af- tia).11,12 The severity of dementia was assessed with
fective disorder. Correlations were sought between the Clinical Dementia Rating scale.13,14 Patients with
the cognitive deficits (reflected by the cognitive do- MDD met the DSM-IV criteria for major depression.
main scores of ACE) and affective symptoms (as Patients in the affective group (Affective) had signifi-
captured by the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depres- cant memory complaints and depressive symptom-

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005 219


Dementia Versus Depression

atology (e.g., low mood, anhedonia, lack of motiva- detection of early AD. The language component com-
tion, lack of energy, etc., alone or in combination) of prises naming 12 line-drawings of medium and low
various degrees, but did not meet the DSM-IV criteria familiarity, comprehension of sentences, repeating
for MDD. Importantly, they were also not felt to suffer words and phrases, reading regular and irregular
from a progressive degenerative dementing condi- words, and writing a sentence. Naming has been
tion. In contrast, the Mixed group consisted of pa- found to be dependent upon concept frequency and
tients with a mixture of cognitive and affective symp- age of acquisition. Alterations made to the non-
toms; and, in this group, it was difficult to arrive at a semantic aspects of language evaluation in the ACE
formal diagnosis with high certainty and to establish are intended for the early detection of FTD, especially
whether or not the cognitive impairment was due to the progressive aphasic variants. Frontal executive
a degenerative disease, and, if so, to what extent. A function is tested by verbal fluency in two tasks: letter
large proportion of these patients developed frank fluency (generating words beginning with the letter
progressive degenerative dementia over an average P in 1 minute), and category fluency (generating
follow-up period of 2 years. The NC consisted of 127 names of animals in 1 minute). Letter fluency relies
age- and education-matched neuropsychiatrically upon phonologic processing, and category fluency on
healthy volunteers who were attendees at an ortho- semantic memory in addition to other executive pro-
pedic (N⳱36) or gynecologic (N⳱28) clinic, spouses cesses. The former is a particularly sensitive indicator
of the before-mentioned (N⳱26), or members of the of FTD, whereas the latter is a marker of semantic
Medical Research Council subject panel (N⳱37). memory impairment. Visuospatial testing includes
All patients were assessed by a senior neurologist copying overlapping pentagons (from the MMSE)
(JRH) and psychiatrist (GEB), using standardized as- and a wire cube, and drawing a clock face. Adding
sessments described elsewhere.15 In brief, a care- the three-dimensional wire cube copying and the
giver/relative is also always interviewed. Patients clock face-drawing test provide a greater scope for
undergo a neuropsychological evaluation, including detecting impaired constructional abilities. Raw
the Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised,16 the Rey Com- scores are used for each item except the two fluency
plex Figure Test,17 the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning, tasks, where scaled scoring systems derived from the
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS),18 the Trail- Gaussian distributions of the raw scores from NC
Making Test,19 Stroop Test, Verbal Fluency,20 Wiscon- subjects are used instead. The calculated six domain
sin Card-Sorting Test, Graded Naming Test,21 Na- scores add up to the composite score on the ACE.
tional Adult Reading Test,22 Visual Object and Space We chose the 17-question version of the Hamilton
Perception battery,23 and the Warrington Recognition Rating Scale for Depression (Ham-D) as an observer-
Memory Test.24 The team met at the end of the day, rated instrument to assess the actual affective status
and reviewed evidence from all clinical and test re- of our patients.25
sults. All patients have structural brain scans (CT or The statistical analysis was carried out with the
MRI), and are followed up in the clinic after 6 months. SPSS for Windows package. One-way ANOVAs and
Those with progressive degenerative dementias or suitable post-hoc multiple comparisons (Scheffé test
ambiguous diagnoses are followed thereafter at 6–12- when equal variances were assumed and Games-
month intervals. Howell test when the Levene test was significant)
were carried out to compare the relevant group
Procedures means. As in our original sample, ascertained
through the natural occurrence and presentation of
The ACE was completed in the memory clinic as patients in the memory clinic, the groups presenting
part of the routine assessment. The orientation and with diagnoses other than a progressive degenerative
attention components of the ACE are identical to the dementia tended to be younger than those with a pro-
ones in the MMSE. The memory component evalu- gressive degenerative process, we carried out analy-
ates episodic and semantic memory. In addition to the ses of covariance for age with confidence-interval ad-
recall of three items from the MMSE, there is a “name justment, using the Bonferroni method for each
and address learning and delayed recall test,” which variable. As a check, but not reported, the appropri-
appears to be a remarkably sensitive measure in the ate nonparametric tests were also carried out, and

220 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005


Dudas et al.

they were consistent in each case. Logistic-regression 9.4; [SD: 5.0]) had equivalent levels of depression,
analysis was used to predict group membership, and and, although significantly less depressed than the
we applied a receiver operating characteristic curve MDD group (Games-Howell p⳱0.0000), they were
(ROC) analysis to examine the sensitivity and speci- still significantly more depressed than either of the
ficity of our measures. progressive-degenerative dementia groups (p⳱0.0000
and p⳱0.0003, respectively, for AD and FTD). The
Ham-D mean scores indicated minimal affective
symptoms in the AD (1.8; SD: 2.0) and FTD (2.8; [3.7])
RESULTS groups. In an ANCOVA, age was not a significant
covariate of the Ham-D score.
Clinical and demographic characteristics of the pa-
tient and NC groups are summarized in Table 1. ACE Scores
Comparison of the age of the subject groups revealed
an overall significant difference (one-way ANOVA: The mean scores on the ACE of the various groups
F[5, 291]⳱14.31; p⳱0.0000). The AD patients, although are shown in Table 2. An ANCOVA showed a signifi-
not significantly different from the subjects in the NC cant between-group difference (F[5, 290]⳱73.73; p⳱
and patients in the Mixed group, were older than 0.0000). It is of note that there was a lack of correlation
those in the other groups, and, at the other extreme, between age and the ACE total score. To explore this
the Affective group was significantly younger than further, we carried out post-hoc pairwise compari-
all the other groups except the MDD and the FTD. sons. The AD, FTD, and Mixed groups showed sig-
The NC group matched each patient group for age, nificant impairment relative to the NC group (Bon-
except the Affective group who were younger. There ferroni; p⳱0.0000), and, importantly, they were also
were no significant differences in education level be- more impaired than any of the affective groups
tween the groups (one-way ANOVA: F[5, 242]⳱1.79; (p⳱0.0000). Also, the AD group scored significantly
p⳱0.115). lower than the FTD group (p⳱0.0004). The Mixed
The MMSE scores reflected the fact that the AD group was indistinguishable from either of the pro-
group (mean score: 20.4; standard deviation [SD]: 5.6) gressive-degenerative dementia groups. Compared
consisted of mild cases, and patients in the FTD with the NC group, the small decrements in the over-
group had very mild overall cognitive impairment all performance of the MDD and Affective groups
(mean score: 26.0; SD: 3.7). The Clinical Dementia failed to reach significance. In view of the relatively
Rating Scale (CDR)26 scores, likewise, indicated rela- low number of cases in the MDD and Affective
tively mild disease. groups, we repeated the analysis with a single affec-
As expected from the entry criteria, the MDD tive (MDD Ⳮ Affective) group. Again, an ANCOVA
group scored high on the Ham-D (mean: 24.7; [SD: demonstrated a significant difference between the
4.7]), and had significantly more signs and symptoms progressive-degenerative dementia groups and the
of depression than any other group (one-way AN- Mixed group versus the collapsed affective group,
OVA: F[4, 133]⳱134.28; p⳱0.0000). The Affective and but it could not distinguish the collapsed affective
the Mixed groups (Ham-D scores: 10; [SD: 4.4] versus group from the NC group.

TABLE 1. Clinical and Demographic Characteristics of the Patient and Control Groups
Diagnostic Group Age, years Sex (M/F) Education, years MMSE CDR Ham-D
Major depression (MDD) (N⳱23) 59.1 (7.9) 11/12 11.2 (1.7) 27.6 (2.4) 24.7 (4.7)
Affective (N⳱37) 54.4 (10.2)* 21/16 12.4 (3.1) 28.3 (2.5) 10.0 (4.4)
Alzheimer disease (AD) (N⳱63) 68.9 (8.0) 27/36 10.6 (2.3) 20.4 (5.6) 1.1 (0.6) 1.8 (2.0)
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) (N⳱27) 61.3 (7.3) 18/9 11.3 (2.7) 26.0 (3.7) 0.6 (0.3) 2.8 (3.7)
Mixed (N⳱22) 64.1 (8.1) 12/10 11.1 (2.9) 23.9 (4.7) 0.8 (0.3) 9.4 (5.0)
Normal-control (NC) (N⳱127) 64.4 (9.4) 63/64 11.3 (2.6) 29.1 (0.9)
Note: Values are mean (standard deviation). MMSE: Mini-Mental State Exam; CDR: Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; Ham-D: Hamilton Rating
Scale for Depression.
*Scheffé: p⳱0.0000 relative to NC group.

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005 221


Dementia Versus Depression

To examine further the usefulness of the ACE total and NC subjects scoring less than 83 and 88 (the
score as a quick guide to assist the clinician in the recommended cut-off scores on the ACE for research
detection of progressive-degenerative dementias in a and for screening, respectively) are shown in Table
patient population with cognitive deficits of various 3. These proportions indicated a need for further
etiologies, we carried out a logistic-regression anal- indices in the domain scores to enable us to better
ysis with only two target variables: progressive-de- separate the affective patients from those with
generative dementia patients (AD and FTD) versus progressive-degenerative dementia, especially MDD/
subjects with no progressive-degenerative dementia Affective patients from FTD patients, on an individual
(MDD, Affective, and NC). The patients originally basis.
classified into the Mixed group for whom we had fol- A series of ANCOVAs revealed significant mean
low-up diagnoses were allocated into the appropriate differences (p⳱0.0000) between the groups for all do-
group according to their follow-up diagnoses; that is, main scores (Table 2). Age only seemed to correlate
those who developed a progressive-degenerative de-
mentia were put into the “progressive degenerative” FIGURE 1. Receiver Operating Characteristics (ROC) of the
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) as a
group; those who still suffered from an affective ill-
Test for Progressive Degenerative Dementia
ness without significant cognitive deterioration, into
the “no organic dementia” group; and those with still ROC Curve
1.00
mixed affective Ⳮ cognitive symptomatology or no
follow-up information were excluded from this anal-
ysis. The total ACE score correctly classified 88.5% of
the cases. The trade-off between sensitivity (true posi- 0.75
tive rate) and 1–specificity (false positive rate) of the
ACE in diagnosing progressive dementia in a patient
population with and without a later confirmed pro-
Sensitivity

gressive-degenerative pathology is shown in the ROC 0.50


curve in Figure 1. The area under the ROC curve is
0.95, which suggests that the ACE has a high specific-
ity for a large range of sensitivities. At 88, the previ-
ously recommended cut-off score for clinical use in the 0.25
detection of dementia,1 the ACE showed a sensitivity
of 93%, and a specificity of 82% for progressive-
degenerative pathology in our study.
A scatterplot showing the individual scores of our
0.00 0.25 0.50 0.75 1.00
subjects on the ACE is displayed in Figure 2, and the 1-Specificity
proportions of MDD, Affective, AD, FTD, Mixed,

TABLE 2. Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) Cognitive Domain and Total Scores
MDD Affective AD FTD Mixed NC ANCOVA: F[5]
Orientation (max: 10) 9.7 (0.6) 9.7 (0.8) 6.7 (2.6) 9.4 (0.9) 8.6 (1.7) 9.9 (0.4) 46.62
Attention (max: 8) 6.9 (1.4) 7.5 (1.4) 5.6 (2.2) 7.3 (1.2) 6.2 (1.9) 7.9 (0.4) 46.27
Memory (max: 35) 30.4 (5.0) 29.1 (5.9) 15.7 (8.3) 24.4 (7.9) 20.0 (8.7) 32.7 (2.0) 79.99
Verbal fluency (max: 14) 9.8 (2.7) 9.9 (3.5) 6.6 (3.1) 6.0 (3.4) 7.3 (3.1) 11.0 (2.2) 30.15
Naming (max: 12) 12.0 (0.0) 11.9 (0.3) 10.3 (2.6) 9.2 (3.8) 11.0 (1.8) 12.0 (0.3) 16.72
Language (max: 16) 15.4 (0.7) 15.1 (2.2) 14.1 (2.5) 13.8 (2.1) 14.7 (1.9) 15.9 (0.4) 13.64
Visuospatial (max: 5) 4.5 (0.5) 4.6 (0.9) 2.9 (1.8) 4.0 (1.3) 3.8 (1.5) 4.6 (0.6) 20.58
ACE Total (max: 100) 89.2 (9.2) 89.0 (9.2) 61.9 (18.3) 74.2 (15.4) 71.0 (16.6) 93.9 (3.5) 73.73*
Note: Values are mean (standard deviation). MDD: major depression; AD: Alzheimer disease; FTD: frontotemporal dementia; NC: normal-
control subjects.
*p⳱0.0000; AD, FTD, and Mixed groups showed significant impairment relative to the NC group (Bonferroni; p⳱0.0000), and were also
more impaired than any of the affective groups (p⳱0.0000). Also, the AD group scored significantly lower than the FTD group (p⳱0.0004).

222 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005


Dudas et al.

FIGURE 2. Scatterplot of the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) Total Scores in the Diagnostic Groups

100

75
ACE Score

50

25

AD FTD MD Affective Mixed NC

Diagnostic Group

Note: Reference line is a score of 88, the recommended cut-off score on the ACE for screening purposes.

with verbal fluency but none of the other domain with the Affective group, the AD group was, again,
scores. The Bonferroni test was used for pairwise impaired in each domain except language, and the
comparisons. As would be predicted, the AD group FTD group was impaired on verbal fluency, and nam-
showed significant deficits in all domains, relative to ing. In summary, the AD group performed more
the NC group. In contrast, the FTD group was not poorly in all domains (except language) than either
impaired in the orientation, attention, and visuospa- of the affective disorder groups, and, not surprisingly,
tial domains, but showed deficits in all the other do- it was the lower verbal fluency and naming scores
mains (p⳱0.0000). The MDD and Affective groups that could clearly differentiate the FTD group from
were again found to be statistically indistinguishable both affective disorders groups. From the non–
on the basis of the domain scores. Compared with the progressive-degenerative groups, only the Affective
MDD group, the AD group was impaired on all mea-
group was impaired in memory relative to the NCs
sures except language, and the FTD group on mem-
(p⳱0.0033). The combined AffectiveⳭMDD group,
ory, verbal fluency, naming, and language. Compared
again, showed significant impairment in memory
and verbal fluency (p⳱0.0062 and p⳱0.0377, respec-
TABLE 3. Proportion (in percent) of Subjects Scoring Below
the Recommended Cut-off Scores on the
tively) relative to the NC group.
Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination (ACE) The Mixed group was not as impaired as the AD
Cut-Off Score group in the orientation (p⳱0.0000) and visuospatial
⬍83 ⬍88 domains (p⳱0.0421), and, relative to the FTD group,
Major depression (MDD) 17 39 performed better on the naming (p⳱0.0097), and
Affective 19 35 slightly worse on the attention (p⳱0.0429) tasks.
Alzheimer disease (AD) 87 97
Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) 63 81
In view of our findings with the verbal fluency
Mixed 73 77 scores and previous reports of selective impairment
Normal-control (NC) 0 4 of category fluency in AD, we analyzed the perfor-
Note: 83 and 88 are the recommended cut-off scores for mance of the groups on the two measures of fluency:
research and screening, respectively.
initial letter (P) and category (animals; see Figure 3).

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005 223


Dementia Versus Depression

ANCOVAs indicated significant mean differences be- target groups with and without a progressive-degen-
tween the groups on both scores, and age covaried erative dementia indicated a satisfactorily high pro-
with category (p⳱0.0017), but not with letter fluency; portion, 89.5%, of the observed cases correctly pre-
for letter fluency, F[5, 290]⳱14.68; p⳱0.0000; for cate- dicted, and the naming, orientation, attention,
gory fluency, F[5, 290]⳱37.28; p⳱0.0000. Pairwise com- category fluency, memory, and attention scores being
parisons revealed that both the AD group and the the discriminating variables in this order of size of
FTD group were impaired on both category and letter effect; and age had a negligible effect.
fluency, compared with the NC group (p⳱0.0000).
Relative to the MDD and Affective groups, they were Follow-Up of the Mixed Group
also similarly impaired on category fluency (p⳱
0.0000), whereas on letter fluency only the FTD group The most interesting group, from the point of dif-
showed a statistically demonstrable difference. Of ferential diagnosis, was the Mixed group. At presen-
note was the finding that neither the MDD nor the tation, 8 out of a total of 22 patients in this group were
Affective group was statistically different from the suspected to have a progressive-degenerative demen-
AD group on the letter fluency task. tia with concomitant affective disorder, and the other
The performance of the Mixed group was also sig- 14 were diagnosed as suffering primarily from an af-
nificantly poorer on both the letter and category flu- fective disorder with a probable progressive-degen-
ency tasks compared with the NC group (p⳱0.0001 erative component. After an average 2 years of fol-
and p⳱0.0000, respectively), and was not distin- low-up, there was a marked shift to organic
guishable from that of the AD and FTD groups on diagnoses: of the 18 subjects for whom data were
either measure. available, 15 had clinically confirmed progressive-de-
A logistic-regression analysis using the domain generative dementias (8 still with concomitant affec-
scores of the ACE again to predict membership in the tive symptoms). The organic etiologies were the fol-
lowing: AD: 10; FTD: 2; Lewy-body dementia: 1;
vascular dementia: 1; and dementia of other origin:
FIGURE 3. Dot-line Chart of Letter Fluency and Category 1. Three patients suffered from affective disorder
Fluency (Derived Scores) Means in the Subject
Groups only. Of particular note is the fact that all the subjects
who eventually developed a progressive-degenera-
Letter fluency
tive dementia scored below the recommended cut-off
point (88) for screening on the ACE (except one who
Category fluency
scored just at the cut-off point), and 15 out of the 16
5 patients scoring lower than 88 had a confirmed pro-
gressive-degenerative dementia diagnosis within 2
years.
Fluency Mean Scores

4
DISCUSSION

The earlier study by Mathuranath et al. established


that the Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination is
sensitive to both AD and FTD, and is able to distin-
3
guish between these two disorders.1 We have ex-
tended this work by showing that patients with af-
fective disorders are clearly separable from those
MD Affective AD FTD Mixed NC
with a progressive degenerative dementia whether
Diagnostic Group
considered as a combined Affective-MDD group or
Note: Maximum attainable score is 7 on both tests of fluency. as separate subgroups. There was a small difference
This scoring system was previously derived by means of a Gaussian between the affective groups and the normal-com-
distribution of the raw scores from normal-control subjects.
parison group that failed to reach statistical signifi-

224 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005


Dudas et al.

cance. Overall, therefore, the affective groups showed of letter- and category-based fluency in the demen-
very little impairment in the total ACE score, and this tias,30–33 relatively little attention has been paid to
could be attributed almost entirely to mild deficits in verbal fluency in affective disorders. Some research-
memory and verbal fluency. ers have found no significant difference between de-
Considering first memory, it was surprising that pressed and normal-comparison subjects,31 whereas
the memory impairment (as reflected by the ACE others have reported impairment.32,33 A more recent
Memory Domain score) reached statistical signifi- study, in accordance with our findings, reported im-
cance in the Affective (but not the MDD) group. Nev- paired letter but unimpaired semantic fluency in
ertheless, it is of note that this impairment was also mildly depressed elderly subjects.34 Impaired verbal
demonstrable in the combined Affective-MDD group. fluency in severely depressed patients has also been
This finding was unexpected for two reasons: First, found to normalize after successful treatment of de-
the memory deficit in depression has long been de- pression.35 A previous study also found impairment
scribed in the literature,27 but we found a significant on letter fluency in patients with AD and depression,
deficit in the generally less depressed Affective group and a trend toward poorer performance than in pa-
only. Second, our sample consisted of depressed pa- tients with AD with no depression.36 More recently,
tients with significant complaints of poor memory. however, Berger et al.37 found that AD and “AD-
Other researchers reported a lack of relationship be- with-depression” patients were indistinguishable on
tween the severity of memory complaints and mem- category and letter fluency tasks. Our finding of a
ory performance28 and alternatively that the linkage correlation between age and category but not letter
between depression and memory impairment was fluency is consistent with the findings of others.37
present only in a subset of the depressed subjects, The Mixed group, in whom we suspected under-
rather than in all depressed individuals.29 It should lying dementia but were unable to reach a clear di-
also be borne in mind that the complaint of “poor agnosis at presentation because of the prominence of
memory” is a general catch-phrase used by subjects mood-related symptoms, represents a clinically im-
in a folk-psychology sense to describe a broad range portant but neglected group. Of the 18 patients for
of cognitive deficits, including poor attention, work- whom follow-up data were available, 15 developed a
ing, episodic, or semantic memory (see Berrios and clear-cut dementia within 2 years (seven pure pro-
Hodges15). The memory complaints in patients with gressive-degenerative [four AD, one FTD, one Lewy-
affective disorders are likely to reflect, therefore, de- body dementia, and one vascular dementia], and
fective attentional processing. eight still mixed, but clearly diagnosed progressive
Moving to the verbal fluency findings, although degenerative dementia). On the ACE, the Mixed
the total Verbal Fluency Domain scores were not sig- group was not separable from the AD and FTD
nificantly impaired in either of the groups without a groups in terms of total scores. Our finding was con-
progressive degenerative dementia diagnosis relative sistent with that of Lopez et al.38 in patients with AD
to the normal-comparison group, a finer-grained and concomitant depression. The ACE appears to be
analysis of the two components uncovered some in- particularly valuable in this challenging group. Pa-
teresting findings. Whereas the MDD and Affective tients scoring above 88 on the ACE are unlikely to
group showed unimpaired category fluency, their develop a progressive degenerative dementia,
performance on letter fluency was the same as that of whereas, for those scoring below 88, the likelihood is
the AD group. In comparison to the huge literature very high.

References
1. Mathuranath PS, Nestor PJ, Berrios GE, et al: A brief cognitive of the elderly in New York and London using the computer di-
test battery to differentiate Alzheimer’s disease and frontotem- agnosis AGECAT. Br J Psychiatry 1987; 151:466–473
poral dementia. Neurology 2000; 55:1613–1620 4. Blazer D, Williams CD: Epidemiology of dysphoria and depres-
2. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: “Mini-Mental State:” a prac- sion in an elderly population. Am J Psychiatry 1980; 137:439–
tical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the 444
clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12:189–198 5. DesRosiers G: Primary or depressive dementia: hedonic tone
3. Copeland JR, Gurland BJ, Dewey ME, et al: Is there more demen- and memory. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Cambridge, UK,
tia, depression, and neurosis in New York? a comparative study 1994

Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005 225


Dementia Versus Depression

6. Garcia CA, Reding MJ, Blass JP: Overdiagnosis of dementia. J Am Battery. Bury St. Edmunds, UK, Thames Valley Test Company,
Geriatr Soc 1981; 29:407–410 1991
7. Roth M, Huppert FA, Tym E, et al: CAMDEX: The Cambridge 24. Warrington EK: Recognition Memory Test. Windsor, Berks, UK,
Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly. Cambridge, UK, Nelson, 1984
Cambridge University Press, 1988 25. Berrios GE, Bulbena-Villarasa A: The Hamilton Depression Rating
8. Mattis S: Dementia Rating Scale. Odessa, FL, Psychological As- Scale and the Numerical Description of the Symptoms of De-
sessment Resources Inc., 1988 pression, in The Hamilton Scales: Psychopharmacology Series 9.
9. Gifford DR, Cummings JL: Evaluating dementia screening tests: Edited by Bech P, Coppen A. Berlin, Heidelberg, Springer-Verlag,
1990, pp 80–92
methodologic standards to rate their performance. Neurology
26. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger W, et al: A new clinical scale for the
1999; 52:224–227
staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 1982; 140:566–572
10. McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al: Clinical diagnosis of
27. DesRosiers G: Depressive pseudodementia, in Memory Disorders
Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group in Psychiatric Practice. Edited by Berrios GE, Hodges JR. Cam-
Under the Auspices of the Department of Health and Human bridge, UK, Cambridge University Press, 2000, pp 268–290
Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1984; 28. Derouesne C, Labomblez L, Thibault S, et al: Memory complaints
34:939–944 in young and elderly subjects. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1999;
11. Neary D, Snowden JS, Mann DM: Classification and description 14:291–301
of frontotemporal dementias. Ann NY Acad Sci 2000; 920:46–51 29. Burt DB, Zembar MJ, Niederehe G: Depression and memory im-
12. Hodges JR, Miller B: The classification, genetics, and neuropa- pairment: a meta-analysis of the association, its pattern, and spec-
thology of frontotemporal dementia: introduction to the special ificity. Psychol Bull 1995; 117:285–305
topic papers, part I: Neurocase 2001; 7:31–35 30. Monsch AU, Bondi MW, Butters N, et al: Comparisons of verbal
13. Hughes CP, Berg L, Danziger WL, et al: A new clinical scale for fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type.
the staging of dementia. Br J Psychiatry 1982; 140:566–572 Arch Neurol 1992; 49:1253–1258
14. Morris JC: The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): current version 31. Fossati P, Deweer B, Raoux N, et al: Deficits in memory retrieval:
and scoring rules. Neurology 1993; 43:2412–2414 an argument in favour of frontal subcortical dysfunction in de-
15. Hodges JR, Berrios GE, Breen K: The multidisciplinary approach, pression. Encephale 1995; 21:295–305
in Memory Disorders in Psychiatric Practice. Edited by Berrios 32. Geffen G, Bate A, Wright M, et al: A comparison of cognitive
impairments in dementia of the Alzheimer type and depression
GE, Hodges JR. Cambridge, UK, Cambridge University Press,
in the elderly. Dementia 1993; 4:294–300
2000, pp 101–121
33. Kuzis G, Sabe L, Tiberti C, et al: Cognitive functions in major
16. Wechsler DA: Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised Manual. San An- depression and Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 1997; 54:982–
tonio, TX, The Psychological Corporation, 1987 986
17. Meyers J, Meyers K: Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition 34. Ravdin LD, Katzen HL, Agrawal P, et al: Letter and semantic flu-
Trial, Professional Manual. Odessa, FL, Psychological Assessment ency in older adults: effects of mild depressive symptoms and
Resources, Inc., 1995 age-stratified normative data. Clin Neuropsychol 2003; 17:195–
18. Wechsler D: WAIS–R Manual. San Antonio, TX, The Psychological 202
Corporation, 1981 35. Trichard C, Martinot JL, Alagille M, et al: Time course of prefron-
19. Reitan RM, Wolfson D: The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological tal lobe dysfunction in severely depressed inpatients: a longitu-
Test Battery, 2nd Ed., Tucson, AZ, Neuropsychology Press, 1993, dinal neuropsychological study. Psychol Med 1995; 25:79–85
pp 279–288 36. Wefel JS, Hoyt BD, Massma PJ: Neuropsychological functioning
in depressed versus nondepressed participants with Alzheimer’s
20. Benton AL, Hamsher K: Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa
disease. Clin Neuropsychol 1999; 13:249–257
City, IA, University of Iowa Press, 1976
37. Berger AK, Fahlander K, Wahlin A, et al: Negligible effects of
21. McKenna P, Warrington EK: The Graded Naming Test. Windsor, depression on verbal and spatial performance in Alzheimer’s dis-
Berks, UK, Nelson, 1983 ease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2002; 13:1–7
22. Nelson HE: The National Adult Reading Test (NART). Windsor, 38. Lopez OL, Boller F, Becker J, et al: Alzheimer’s disease and de-
Berks, UK, NFER-Nelson, 1982 pression: neuropsychological impairment and progression of the
23. Warrington EK, James M: The Visual Object and Space Perception illness. Am J Psychiatry 1990; 147:855–860

226 Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 13:3, March 2005

You might also like