You are on page 1of 25

Personal Relationships, 16 (2009), 263–283. Printed in the United States of America.

Copyright 2009 IARR. 1350-4126/09

Positive illusions about a partner’s physical


attractiveness and relationship quality

DICK P. H. BARELDS AND PIETERNEL DIJKSTRA


University of Groningen

Abstract
The present research examined the existence of positive illusions about a partner’s physical attractiveness and its
relations to relationship quality. Positive illusions were assumed to exist when individuals rated their partner as more
attractive than their partner rated him or herself. In two Dutch community samples of 117 and 203 married or
cohabiting heterosexual couples, both partners rated their own and their partner’s facial and bodily attractiveness. In
Study 2, both partners completed three measures of relationship quality. Both studies found evidence for the existence
of positive illusions about a partner’s physical attractiveness. Moreover, Study 2 found clear positive illusion
actor effects for relationship quality. This association differed by age of couples.

During their relationship, partners will d.p.h.barelds@rug.nl.


frequently uncover sources of negativity and
conflict that may threaten their feelings of
security by raising the fear that one’s partner
really is not the “right” person after all (e.g.,
Murray, 1999). Such doubts are troublesome
because negativity typically surfaces when
partners have already invested their hopes
in the relationship (e.g., Miller, Niehuis, &
Huston, 2006). In order to reach some sort of
cognitive resolution between their hopes and
doubts and to sustain a sense of felt security,
partners often weave an elaborate story (or
fiction) that both embellishes a partner’s
virtues and minimizes his or her faults (e.g.,
McNulty, O’Mara, & Karney, 2008; Murray,
Holmes, & Griffin, 1996a). Several studies
have, for instance, found that individuals
often rate their partner overly positive on
charac- teristics such as “kind” and
“intelligent,” a phenomenon that has been
called positive

Dick P. H. Barelds and Pieternel Dijkstra, Department


of Psychology, University of Groningen, Groningen,
Netherlands.
Correspondence should be addressed to Dick P. H.
Barelds Department of Psychology, Grote Kruisstraat
2/1, 9712 TS, Groningen, Netherlands, e-mail:
illusions (e.g., Murray & Holmes, 1997; (Murray
Murray et al., 1996a). By means of & Holmes, 1997; Murray et al., 1996a).
positive illusions, partners enhance their
sense of secu- rity, overstate the case of
commitment, and derogate alternative Physical attractiveness in romantic
partners, thus stabilizing their long-term relationships
bond (Murray, 1999). Several studies In general, physical attractiveness is a
indeed found positive illusions about a potent factor fostering sexual attraction. Men
partner to predict greater satisfaction, love, and women both highly value a potential
and trust, and less conflict and ambivalence partner’s physical attractiveness (e.g., Buss,
in both dating and marital relationships 1989; Feingold, 1990). As a consequence,
(Murray physically attractive individuals are more
& Holmes, 1997). In addition, longitudinal successful in dating and more popular among
studies show that the stronger individuals’ members of the opposite sex (e.g., Green,
initial illusions about their partners, the Buchanan, & Heuer, 1984). Several theories
more likely their relationships are to persist
263
26 D. P. H. Barelds and P. Dijkstra
4
provide explanations for the importance of about their partner’s physical attractiveness.
physical attractiveness. According to evolu- Positive illusions about a partner’s physical
tionary psychologists, physical attractiveness attractiveness may seem trivial in nature
is such a valuable characteristic because it but are certainly not. As argued above, in
reflects fertility (in women), dominance (in general, people consider physical attractive-
men), and health (in both sexes) attributes that ness an important characteristic in a mate
contribute to individuals’ chances of survival (Buss, Larsen, Westen, & Semmelroth,
and reproduction (e.g., Buss, 1994). A social- 1992; Dijkstra & Buunk, 1998): Feeling
cultural explanation proposes that physical that one partner is very attractive will
attractiveness affects attraction through therefore enhance one’s satisfaction with
a positive stereotype. When someone is one’s relationship. Partners may feel they
beautiful, individuals automatically attribute are lucky to have such an attractive partner.
many other positive characteristics to him or In addition, individuals often compare their
her that make him or her more likable (the partner’s physical attractiveness to those of
beautiful-is-good effect; Dion, Berscheid, & others. The media continually confronts indi-
Walster, 1972). People, for instance, perceive viduals with images of extremely attractive
attractive individuals as sexually warmer, opposite-sex targets (e.g., Englis, Solomon,
more sociable, more assertive, happier, and & Ashmore, 1994). Exposure to these images
emotionally more stable than unattractive can make people feel dissatisfied with their
individuals (Eagly, Ashmore, Makhijani, & partner and cause them to fall out of love
Longo, 1991). In addition, people perceive with their partner (Kenrick, Gutierres, &
physically attractive individuals to be more Goldberg, 1989). Positive illusions about a
familiar than physically unattractive individu- partner’s physical attractiveness may prevent
als, regardless of prior exposure (the so-called partners from becoming dissatisfied with their
beautiful-is-familiar effect ; Monin, 2003). partner’s looks in response to these images
Because, in general, higher familiarity leads and, in so doing, protect the relationship with
to higher liking (e.g., Lewandowski, Aron, the partner. Despite all of this, researchers
& Gee, 2007; Zajonc, 2001), people perceive have hardly examined positive illusions about
attractive individuals as more likable and as a partner’s physical attractiveness. Murray,
eliciting “a warmer glow,” at least more than Holmes, and Dolderman (2000) examined
unattractive individuals do (Corneille, Monin, self-perceptions of physical attractiveness
& Pleyers, 2005). Studies also have shown as part of the Self-Attributes Questionnaire
physical attractiveness to be an important (SAQ; contains one physical attractiveness
attribute once partners are already involved item) and reported that intimates in satisfying
in an intimate relationship. Sangrador and relationships perceived more virtues in
Yela (2000; same sample, Yela & Sangrador, general (total SAQ scores) in each other than
2001), for instance, found perceptions of partners perceived themselves. These authors
a partner’s physical attractiveness to be did not specifically report whether and to
positively related to levels of commitment, what extent they found a positive illusion for
passion, intimacy, and satisfaction. McNulty, their physical attractiveness item.
Neff, and Karney (2008) found observers’
ratings of partners’ physical attractiveness
to be related to relationship quality, finding, Defining positive illusions
for instance, that both spouses behaved more Murray and colleagues (2000) view self-
positively in relationships in which wives perceptions as a “reality” benchmark. As
were more attractive than their husbands a consequence, positive illusions occur
were. when individuals perceive their partners
to be physically more attractive than their
Positive illusions and physical attractiveness
partners perceive themselves. One might
Barelds-Dijkstra and Barelds (2008) found argue, however, that self-perceptions are
that individuals also hold positive illusions rarely unbiased. In general, people’s ratings
of self tend to be positively biased (e.g., of couples indeed perceive their partner to be
Alicke, 1985; Brown, 1985; Taylor & Brown, physically more attractive than their partners
1988); that is, people rate their own qualities perceive themselves (Hypothesis 1).
more favorably than the qualities of the
typical person. Thus, rather than serving as a
“reality” benchmark, according to Miller and Positive illusions and relationship quality
colleagues (2006) self-perceptions may serve Despite the potentially important role positive
as a conservative standard against which illusions about a partner’s physical attractive-
to evaluate a partner’s perception of these ness may play in the relationship, researchers
qualities for signs of a positive bias. have not related positive illusions about a
Recently, Barelds-Dijkstra and Barelds part- ner’s physical attractiveness to
(2008) showed that partners indeed hold relationship quality. Both Barelds-Dijkstra
positive illusions about each other’s physical and Barelds (2008) and Swami, Furnham,
attractiveness, in terms of the definition of and colleagues (2007) merely speculated
Murray and colleagues (2000) and Miller about the rela- tionship between positive
and colleagues (2006). These authors asked illusions about a partner’s physical
couples to fill in the Body Esteem Scale attractiveness and relation- ship quality,
(BES; Franzoi & Herzog, 1986; Franzoi without examining the relation- ship
& Shields, 1984) for both themselves and between these two constructs. A recent
their partners and showed that individuals study by Swami and colleagues (2009) made
perceived their partner to be physically progress in the right direction: These authors
more attractive than their partners perceived found a positive relation between relation-
themselves. Recently, Swami, Furnham, ship quality and the “love is blind bias” indi-
Georgiades, and Pang (2007) found that indi- cating that individuals who view their part-
viduals rate their partner as more physically ner as more attractive than they view them-
attractive than they rate themselves, a finding selves have qualitatively better relationships.
they refer to as “the love is blind bias” (see As noted before, however, according to the
also Swami, Steiger, Haubner, Voracek, & definition of Murray and colleagues (2000)
Furnham, 2009). According to the definition and Miller and colleagues (2006), the “love
of Murray and colleagues (2000) and Miller is blind bias” is not a positive illusion. Thus,
and colleagues (2006), however, “the love is although presumed, the existence of a
blind bias” is not a positive illusion: Swami relation- ship between relationship quality
and colleagues used only within subject and positive illusions about a partner’s
comparisons of self and partner physical physical attractive- ness remains unstudied.
attractiveness ratings. Their study included The present study set out to fill this gap by
no external standard to which it was possible examining the relation- ship between
to compare individuals’ perceptions. As a positive illusions about a part- ner’s
result, in theory, it might be possible that physical attractiveness and relationship
individuals who perceive their partner as quality. In addition, whereas previous stud-
more physically attractive than themselves ies examined positive illusions about a part-
have partners who indeed are more physically ner’s general attractiveness, the present study
attractive, in which case there would disentangled the concept of general attractive-
obviously be no positive illusion. To establish ness into its two most important components
the existence of positive illusions adequately, (Alicke, Smith, & Klotz, 1986), that is, pos-
one needs to compare ratings of a partner’s itive illusions about a partner’s bodily attrac-
physical attractiveness with external physical tiveness and a partner’s facial attractiveness.
attractiveness ratings, such as provided by the In line with the theory on positive illusions
partner himself or herself (cf. Murray et al., (e.g., Murray, 1999; Murray & Holmes,
2000). The first aim of the present study was 1997), we expected positive illusions about
to replicate Barelds-Dijkstra and Barelds’s a part- ner’s physical attractiveness— both
(2008) findings by showing that both partners bodily and facial — to be positively related to
relationship quality. That is, as individuals
hold stronger positive illusions about their
partner’s physical
attractiveness, they will report higher relation- extend Barelds-Dijkstra and Barelds’s (2008)
ship quality (Hypothesis 2). findings by showing that partners hold posi-
tive illusions about both their partner’s bodily
Positive illusions, relationship quality, and and facial attractiveness. Next, Study 2 exam-
age ined the relationship between positive illu-
Finally, the present research examined age sions about a partner’s bodily and facial
dif- ferences in the relation between attrac- tiveness and relationship quality
relationship quality and positive illusions (Hypothesis
about a partner’s physical attractiveness. 2) and examined age differences in this rela-
Those who study pos- itive illusions have tionship (Hypothesis 3).
neglected the role of age: Previous studies
examined couples in their 20s and 30s only
Study 1
(e.g., Murray & Holmes, 1997; Murray et
al., 1996a; Murray, Holmes, Study 1 set out to replicate and extend
& Griffin, 1996b; Swami, Furnham, et al., Barelds-Dijkstra and Barelds’s (2008) find-
2007). We therefore examined positive illu-
ings by showing that partners hold positive
sions about a partner’s physical attractiveness
illusions about each other’s facial and bod-
in two samples that were highly
ily attractiveness. In addition to perceptions
heterogeneous with regard to age. We
of physical attractiveness, Study 1 assessed an
expected positive illu- sions about a partner’s
objective index of physical attractiveness, that
physical attractiveness to be less strongly
is, individuals’ body mass index (BMI; i.e.,
related to relationship qual- ity among older
a subject’s weight scaled for height). BMI is
couples than among younger couples
one of the most important objective indexes
(Hypothesis 3). In general, as peo- ple age,
of physical attractiveness for both men and
they become less physically attractive (e.g.,
women (e.g., Swami, Antonakopoulos, Tove
Teuscher & Teuscher, 2007). As a result, it
´e,
becomes more difficult to uphold positive
& Furnham, 2006; Swami & Tove´e, 2005)
illusions about a partner’s physical attractive-
ness. Although this may imply that people and research has found it to explain up to
become less satisfied with their relationship 80% of the variance of attractiveness ratings
as they age, several studies suggest that this is (e.g., Swami & Tove´e, 2006). In general,
not the case. With age, people come to value individ- uals find women with a relatively
different characteristics in a mate. Research, low BMI (ranging between 19 and 21;
for instance, found that, with age, individu- Swami, Neto, Tove´e, & Furnham, 2007),
als more strongly value a (potential) mate’s indicating slender- ness, to be most attractive,
education (Buunk, Dijkstra, Fletchenhauer, at least in contexts of high socioeconomic
& Kenrick, 2002) and socioeconomic sta- development (e.g., Fal- lon & Rozin, 1985;
tus (Gil-Burmann, Pela´ez, & Sa´nchez, Swami et al., 2006). For men, a curvilinear
2002). Although a partner’s physical relationship exists between BMI and ratings
attractiveness remains an important attribute of attractiveness: Individuals find both obese
throughout marriage (e.g., Sangrador & and thin men to be less attrac- tive than men
Yela, 2000), it becomes of less relative with intermediate BMI (e.g., Swami &
importance. As a result, to remain satisfied Tove´e, 2005). One could argue that as
with their relation- ship, older individuals no perceptions of a partner’s attractiveness and
longer need to bias their partner’s physical his or her actual BMI are less related, individ-
attractiveness in a pos- itive way to the extent uals’ perceptions of their partner’s physical
they had to when they were younger. attractiveness are more illusionary in nature.
By showing that perceptions of a partner’s
physical attractiveness are not simply reflec-
The present research
tions of a partner’s BMI (or objective
We conducted two studies to examine posi- physical attractiveness), Study 1 aims to
tive illusions and their relationship to demonstrate that perceptions of a partner’s
relation- ship quality. Study 1 aimed to physical attrac- tiveness are, indeed,
replicate and illusionary in nature, at least to a large
degree.
Method we selected only those items from the
Participants and procedure BES that explicitly refer to a person’s
physical appearance (22 items in total). We
We recruited participants through a postal divided these items over two scales (see
mail survey, as part of a large-scale study also Alicke et al., 1986), one for facial
on intimate relationships. We randomly attractiveness (7 items: nose, lips, ears, chin,
selected names from telephone directories eyes, cheeks/cheekbones, and face), and one
of all middle-sized cities throughout the for bodily attractiveness (15 items: waist,
Netherlands. The selected individuals (1,700 thighs, biceps, body build, buttocks, width
in total) received a cover letter by mail asking of shoulders, arms, chest or breasts, hips,
for their participation in the study. Criterion legs, figure, feet, sex organs, appearance of
for participation was that the selected indi- stomach, body hair). Advantages of adopting
vidual was either married or cohabiting at the this procedure are that these scales may
time of the study. Those willing to participate serve as operationalizations of physical
could return a preaddressed response card, on attractiveness rather than body esteem, that
which they could also indicate if their partner we were able to distinguish two distinct parts
was willing to participate. To all willing to of an individual’s physical attractiveness
participate, we send a set of questionnaires (facial and bodily attractiveness), and that we
by mail. Upon completion, participants were able to use the same scales for both
returned the questionnaires anonymously in men and women.
an enclosed preaddressed return envelope. To examine the extent to which the fac-
We removed participants from our study tor structure of the 22 selected BES items
whose partner did not participate and who in the present study corresponds to the two
were homosexual (3 couples). As a result, a priori defined scales for facial and bodily
our sample consisted of 234 participants, attractiveness, we conducted a principal com-
or 117 heterosexual couples. Mean age ponents analysis (PCA) with varimax rotation
was 49.2 years (SD = 13.1, range = 21 – on the self-ratings on these 22 items. The
83), mean relationship length was 23.5 PCA yielded a solution with two components
years (SD = 13.5, range = 6 months to 56.8 (explaining 47.8% of the variance) that we
years), and mean educational level (scored identified as facial attractiveness (21.7% of
on a 5-point scale; 1 = primary school the variance) and bodily attractiveness
level, 5 = higher educational level ) (26.0% of the variance; all items had a high
corresponded to a higher grade elementary loading on the expected component). In
school level (M = 3.4, SD = 1.2). addition, we cal- culated congruence
coefficients between the two-component
Materials solution found in the present data, and the
expected (a priori) structure of the items
Participants filled in a questionnaire (simple structure, using only 1s and 0s for
consisting of the following variables. factor loadings; e.g., in the a priori structure
we assigned the item “waist” a factor loading
BES. We used the BES (Franzoi & 1 for bodily attractiveness, and a fac- tor
Shields, 1984; see also Franzoi & Herzog, loading 0 for facial attractiveness). In order to
1986) as a measure for physical attractive- optimize the opportunity for correspond-
ness. The BES lists 35 aspects of physical ing factors to be identified as equivalents, we
appearance and bodily functioning. Partici- orthogonally rotated the two-factor solutions,
pants had to rate their attractiveness on each using the a priori solution as the target struc-
of these 35 aspects (1 = not at all attractive, ture (see Kiers & Groenen, 1996). The
5 = very attractive). Because not all BES congru- ence coefficients after rotation were
items refer to physical attractiveness per 0.93 for bodily attractiveness and 0.91 for
se, but rather to body esteem (e.g., sex facial attrac- tiveness. These coefficients
drive, physical stamina, reflexes, agility), indicate a high level of correspondence
between factors (e.g., Haven & Ten Berge,
1977) and support the
use of separate scales for facial and bodily illusions about his wife’s facial attractive-
attractiveness. ness are equal to B – C = 32 – 25 = 7, and
All participants provided both self-ratings positive illusions about the husband’s facial
and partner ratings for facial attractiveness attractiveness held by his wife are equal to
and bodily attractiveness. Cronbach’s alpha D – A = 34 – 21 = 13.
coefficients (as an indication of reliability) for We examined the magnitude of the posi-
facial attractiveness were .83 for self-ratings tive illusions by computing the mean facial
and .81 for partner ratings, whereas the alpha and bodily attractiveness scores for men and
coefficients for bodily attractiveness were .90 women, both for self-ratings and partner rat-
for self-ratings and .91 for partner ratings. ings. For positive illusions about a partner’s
Correlations between facial attractiveness and physical attractiveness to occur, we had to
bodily attractiveness were r = .55 (p < .001) find that individuals’ ratings of their partner’s
for self-ratings and r = .62 (p < .001) for physical attractiveness are substantially higher
partner ratings.
than their partner’s self-ratings of physical
attractiveness. Table 2 shows the results, sep-
BMI. We asked participants to report arately for men and women.
their body weight (in kilograms) and body In support of Hypothesis 1, Table 2 shows
height (in centimeters) to compute their BMI, that both men and women rated their partners
expressed as weight in kilograms divided by as clearly more attractive than their partners
height in meters squared (kg/m2; e.g., Bray, did themselves (rowwise comparisons), repli-
1998). Table 1 shows the mean height, cating previous findings of Barelds-Dijkstra
weight, and BMI for men and women in the and Barelds (2008). In addition, we compared
present study. The numbers in Table 1 individuals’ self-ratings and partner ratings of
correspond closely to the numbers found for physical attractiveness separately for men and
the gen- eral Dutch population (Central
women (within sex comparisons; i.e., diag-
Bureau voor de Statistiek, 2007).
onal comparisons of the means in Table 2)
and found that both men and women rated
Results themselves as significantly less attractive than
they rated their partners (facial attractiveness:
Positive illusions men, t = −9.76, p < .001, and women, t =
Because of the complex nature of the data −2.47, p < .01; bodily attractiveness: men,
(self-ratings and partner ratings for facial and t = −6.95, p < .001, and women, t = −9.10,
bodily attractiveness are available for both p < .001). Moreover, we found both the
partners, which means a total of eight physi- mean self-ratings for facial attractiveness and
cal attractiveness scores per couple), we will the positive illusions about a partner’s facial
graphically illustrate positive physical attrac- attractiveness to differ significantly between
tiveness illusions, as defined in the present men and women (columnwise comparisons in
study, by means of Figure 1. Table 2; facial attractiveness, t = −4.46, p <
To clarify Figure 1, we will briefly discuss .001; facial attractiveness illusion, t = −3.16,
a numeric example. When the facial attrac- p < .01), with men reporting lower self-
tiveness scores that the husband provides are ratings of facial attractiveness and stronger
21 for the self-rating (A) and 32 for the rat- positive illusions about a partner’s facial
ing of his partner’s facial attractiveness (i.e., attractiveness. Finally, we computed correla-
how he judges his wife; B). The facial attrac- tions for men and women separately, between
tiveness scores that his wife provides are 25 BMI and self-ratings of physical attractive-
for the self-rating (i.e., the self-rating of facial ness, ratings of a partner’s physical attractive-
attractiveness the partner makes; C) and 34 ness that are made by the partner, positive
for the rating of her partner’s facial attrac- illu- sions about a partner’s physical
tiveness (i.e., how she judges her husband; attractiveness, and positive illusions about a
D). From the husband’s perspective, positive partner’s physi- cal attractiveness that are
held by the partner.
Table 1. Description of participants in terms of body height, body weight, and body mass index
(BMI)

Men Women
Variable M SD Range M SD Range
Body height 181.1 7.2 164 – 197 170.8 6.8 150 – 193
Body weight 82.1 10.7 54 – 125 70.5 10.7 52 – 102
BMI 25.0 2.6 18.0 – 24.2 3.3 16.8 –
32.9 33.4

Person rated
Ratings made by Self Partner

Self A B

Partner C D

Figure 1. Definition of self and partner positive illusions within couples.


Note: A = self-ratings of physical attractiveness; B = ratings of a partner’s physical attractive-
ness; C = self-ratings of physical attractiveness made by the partner; D = ratings of a
partner’s physical attractiveness made by the partner. Positive illusions about a partner’s
physical attractiveness are now defined as (B – C; solid line), and positive illusions about
a partner’s physical attractiveness held by the partner as (D – A; dotted line).

BMI addition, both men and women rated them-


There were no significant relations between selves as significantly less attractive than they
BMI and positive physical attractiveness illu- rated their partners. This finding is consistent
sions. In addition, both self-ratings of facial with previous studies by, for instance, Swami
attractiveness and ratings of a partner’s facial and colleagues (2006). The present study
attractiveness that were made by the partner hardly showed significant relations between
actual body build (reflected in a person’s
were not significantly related to BMI. Only
BMI) on the one hand and positive physical
for women, there were significant negative
attractiveness illusions, self-ratings of physi-
rela- tions between BMI and self-ratings of
cal attractiveness, and ratings of a partner’s
bodily
physical attractiveness made by the partner
attractiveness (r = −.37, p < .01) and ratings on the other hand. Only for women, we
of a partner’s bodily attractiveness that were
found BMI to be related significantly to bod-
made by their partners (r = −.26, p < .01):
ily attractiveness (both self-ratings and part-
As women had a higher BMI, both women
and ner ratings by their partners). These results
suggest that both self-perceptions and per-
their male partners rated the woman’s body as
ceptions of a partners’ physical attractiveness
less attractive. Additional regression analyses
are, to a large extent, illusionary in nature.
using BMI and BMI2 as predictors showed no
Naturally, one might challenge the validity
curvilinear relations between BMI and physi-
of BMI as an objective indicator of physi-
cal attractiveness ratings or positive illusions.
cal attractiveness as far as facial attractive-
ness is concerned. BMI by definition refers
Discussion primarily to bodily attractiveness (expressed
as the presence or absence of overweight),
Both men and women rated their partners as and might therefore not be a valid indicator
clearly more attractive than their partners did of a person’s facial attractiveness. Hume and
themselves, thus supporting Hypothesis 1 (see
also Barelds-Dijkstra & Barelds, 2008). In
27
0

Table 2. Mean self-rated and partner-rated facial and bodily attractiveness scores for men and women (standard deviations in parentheses)

Facial attractiveness Bodily attractiveness


Participant Ratings by Positive Ratings by Positive
sex Self-ratings partner illusion t p Self-ratings partner illusion t p
Men 24.0 (4.0) 27.4 (3.9) 3.3 (4.7) −7.75 < .001 50.5 (8.6) 57.9 (8.8) 7.4 (10.1) −7.87 < .001
Women 26.4 (4.2) 27.7 (3.7) 1.3 (5.2) −2.70 < .001 49.8 (9.5) 56.9 (8.3) 7.1 (11.1) −6.88 < .001
D.
P.
H.
B
ar
el
ds
an
d
P.
Di
jk
272 D. P. H. Barelds and P. Dijkstra

Montgomery (2001), using objective ratings in Study 1). We sent letters to approxi-
of physical attractiveness, found a signifi- mately 3,000 individuals. If only one part-
cant negative relation between BMI and facial ner participated in the study, we removed
attractiveness for women but not for men. him or her from the sample, as we did
The fact that in the present study, at least with a few homosexual participants (four
among men, BMI was unrelated to self- couples in total). As a result, our sample
perceptions of both facial and bodily consisted of 406 participants, or 203 cou-
attractiveness under- lines the importance of ples, with a mean age of 47.4 years (SD =
viewing self-ratings of physical attractiveness
13.2, range = 20 – 81). In total, 87.4%
not as “reality” bench- marks, as Murray and
were married and 12.6% were cohabiting.
colleagues (2000) sug- gested, but rather as Mean
conservative standards against which to length of the relationship was 22.7 years
evaluate a partner’s percep- tion of these (SD = 13.5 years), with a minimum of 1 year
qualities for signs of a positive bias (Miller and a maximum of 55 years. Mean edu-
et al., 2006). cational level (1 = primary school level, 5 =
higher educational level ) corresponded to a
Study 2 higher grade elementary school level (M =
3.6, SD = 1.2).
We conducted Study 2 to investigate the rela-
tionship between positive illusions about a
partner’s physical attractiveness and relation- Materials
ship quality. We expected that positive illu-
sions about a partner’s physical attractive- BES. As in Study 1, we used the BES as
ness would be positively related to relation- a measure for physical attractiveness. Con-
ship quality (Hypothesis 2). In addition, sistent with Study 1, we constructed scales
Study from the BES for facial attractiveness (7
2 examined age differences in the relation items) and bodily attractiveness (15 items; see
between relationship quality and positive illu- Study 1). To examine the validity of the a
sions about a partner’s physical attractiveness. priori defined scales, we conducted a PCA
We expected that among older couples posi- with varimax rotation on the self-ratings on
tive illusions about a partner’s physical the 22 selected BES items (see also Study 1).
attrac- tiveness would be more strongly The PCA yielded a solution with two compo-
related to relationship quality than among nents (explaining 47.2% of the variance) that
younger cou- ples (Hypothesis 3). we identified as facial attractiveness (18.6%
With regard to a long-term partner, several of the variance) and bodily attractiveness
studies have shown that men find a partner’s (28.6% of the variance). Congruence coeffi-
physical attractiveness more important than cients between the two-factor solution found
women do (e.g., Buss et al., 1992; Buunk, in the present study, and the expected (a pri-
Dijkstra, Kenrick, & Warntjes, 2001). In addi- ori) structure of the items after target rotation
tion, research has found one’s own and one’s (see Study 1) were 0.96 for bodily attractive-
partner’s physical attractiveness to be related ness and 0.90 for facial attractiveness. These
differently to relationship quality for men and coefficients are comparable to Study 1 and
women (e.g., McNulty, Neff, et al., 2008). again support the use of separate scales for
Therefore, in addition to examining the rela- facial and bodily attractiveness.
tions between positive illusions and relation- All participants provided self-ratings and
ship quality in the entire sample, we also
ratings of a partner’s facial and bodily attrac-
examined potential sex differences.
tiveness (partner ratings). Cronbach’s alpha
coefficients for facial attractiveness in the
Method present study were .77 for self-ratings and
Participants and procedure .84 for ratings of a partner’s facial attrac-
tiveness, and the alpha coefficients for bodily
We recruited Study 2’s participants through attractiveness were .90 for self-ratings and .92
a postal mail survey (same procedure as
for ratings of a partner’s bodily attractive- (1 = n thought of separation ever, 5 = on brink,
ness. Correlations between facial attractive- of separation or divorce) and “Does life with
ness and bodily attractiveness in the present your partner bring you satisfaction (leaving
study were r = .46 (p < .001) for self-ratings the sexual side apart)?” (1 = completely satis-
and r = .60 (p < .001) for ratings of a part- factory, 5 = totally unsatisfactory). Alpha in
ner’s attractiveness. the present study was .90, and the correlation
with the DRQ total score was r = .81
Dutch Relationship Questionnaire (DRQ). (p < .001).
The DRQ (Barelds, Luteijn, & Arrindell,
2003) is a measure for relationship qual- IPSI. The IPSI (Lange, 1983) is a 17-
ity with the intention of measuring the most item questionnaire describing couples’ behav-
important aspects of marital quality. The iors in marital problem solving (e.g., “We
DRQ consists of 80 true– false items have little trouble in choosing a solution for
dis- tributed over five scales: independence, a given problem” and “Our quarrels often
close- ness, identity, conflict resolution, and end up in discussions about who is right and
sexu- ality. These scales are summed to who is wrong”). Participants rated the items
obtain a total relationship quality score.
on a 5-point scale (1 = totally disagree, 5 =
Examples of items are “My partner tends to totally agree). Higher scores are indicative of
control me,” “I often tell my partner that I a couple’s greater ability to solve their prob-
love him/her,” “When we have an argument, lems adequately. The IPSI has good psycho-
we start yelling at each other,” and “I am metric properties (e.g., Hageman, Lange, van
content with our sex life.” For brevity Dyck, Hoogduin, & Meijer, 1990), and, in the
reasons, the present study will only use the present study, an alpha of .90. The correlation
total DRQ score. Cronbach’s alpha for the between the IPSI and the DRQ total score was
total DRQ in the present study was .94,
r = .74 (p < .001) and between the IPSI and
which is comparable to previous stud- ies MMQ – MA was r = .73 (p < .001). These
(e.g., Barelds, 2005; Barelds & Barelds- results are consistent with previous research
Dijkstra, 2006, 2007). (Barelds et al., 2003).
Because Study 1 found BMI to be largely
Relationship adjustment. In addition, the unrelated to both positive illusions about a
present study used the Marital Adjustment partner’s physical attractiveness and self and
scale of the Dutch language version of the partner perceptions of physical attractiveness,
Maudsley Marital Questionnaire (MMQ; Study 2 did not assess participants’ BMI.
Arrindell, Boelens, & Lambert, 1983; see
also Arrindell & Schaap, 1985). The Dutch
MMQ is a 20-item questionnaire that consists Results
of three subscales: Marital Adjustment (10 Positive illusions
items), Sexual Adjustment (5 items), and
General Life Adjustment (5 items). Because First, and consistent with Study 1, we estab-
previous studies have generally reported lished the magnitude of the positive illusions
better psychometric properties for the Marital about a partner’s physical attractiveness by
Adjustment scale, as compared with the computing the mean facial and bodily attrac-
other two scales (mainly as a result of tiveness scores for men and women (self-
differences in scale length), we decided to ratings and ratings of a partner’s facial and
use only the 10 MMQ items that make up bodily attractiveness). For positive illusions
the Marital Adjustment Scale (MMQ – to occur, we had to find that individuals rate
MA). Because the present study also included their partners as more attractive than their
cohabiting nonmarried couples, we replaced partners rate themselves (see also Figure 1,
the term marriage in some items with Study 1). Table 3 shows the results.
relationship. Participants rated these items Table 3 again supports the existence of
on 5-point scales. Examples of items are positive illusions about a partner’s physical
“How committed are you to this
relationship?”
P
os
iti
ve
ill
us
io
ns
an
d
ph
ys
ic
al
at
tr
Table 3. Mean self-rated and partner-rated facial and bodily attractiveness scores for men and women (standard deviations in parentheses) ac

Facial attractiveness Bodily attractiveness


Participant Ratings by Positive Ratings by Positive
sex Self-ratings partner illusion t p Self-ratings partner illusion t p
Men 24.7 (3.5) 26.7 (4.1) 1.9 (4.9) −5.56 < .001 51.3 (8.3) 56.5 (9.5) 5.3 (10.6) −7.05 < .001
Women 25.6 (3.7) 27.5 (3.5) 1.9 (4.7) −5.67 < .001 47.3 (10.0) 55.2 (10.1) 7.8 (10.5) −10.59 < .001

27
3
27 D. P. H. Barelds and P. Dijkstra
4
attractiveness: Both men and women rated We examined the relations between posi-
their partners as significantly more attractive tive illusions concerning the partner’s facial
than their partners rated themselves. In addi- and bodily attractiveness (actor and partner
tion, as in Study 1, both men and women effects) and relationship quality by means of
rated themselves as significantly less attrac- multilevel modeling (linear mixed models in
tive than they rated their partner (within-sex SPSS 14.0; cf. Kenny, 2004). Actor effects
comparisons, i.e., diagonal comparisons of refer to the effects of individuals’ own predic-
the means in Table 3; facial attractiveness: tor scores on the dependent variables (e.g., the
men, t = −10.90, p < .001, and women,
effect of A’s positive illusions on A’s
t = −3.67, p < .001; bodily attractiveness:
relation- ship), and partner effects refer to the
men, t = −5.45, p < .001, and women, t =
effects of the partner’s predictor scores on the
−12.11, p < .001). In addition, we found dependent variables (e.g., the effect of B’s
the mean self-ratings for bodily attractive- positive illu- sions on A’s relationship). We
ness to differ significantly between men and entered dyads as subjects (distinguishable)
women (columnwise comparisons in Table 3;
and subject sex as a repeated measures
t = 4.37, p < .001), with men rating their variable (covariance type compound
own bodily attractiveness significantly higher symmetry heterogeneous; cf. Kenny, 2004).
than women rated their own bodily attractive- We used both partners’ ratings of a
ness. Neither the other attractiveness ratings, partner’s facial and bodily attractive- ness,
nor the positive illusions differed significantly controlled for the other partner’s self-
i
between men and women (absolute t s < ratings (e.g., Murray et al., 1996b) to estimate
i
2.44, p s > .015). actor and partner positive illusion effects. We
included the relationship variables as depen-
Positive illusions and relationship quality
dent variables. Table 4 summarizes the results.
Next, we examined the relations between pos- We found consistent actor positive
itive physical attractiveness illusions and rela- illusion effects: Positive illusions about a
tionship quality using the actor partner inter- partner’s facial and bodily attractiveness
dependence model (APIM; e.g., Campbell & were related positively and significantly
Kashy, 2002; Kashy & Kenny, 2000) as a to all three relationship variables. That is,
con- ceptual framework. The APIM treats supporting Hypothesis 2, as individuals held
dyads (here intimate couples) as the unit more positive illusions about their partner’s
of anal- ysis (e.g., Kashy, Campbell, & physical attractiveness, they reported higher
Harris, 2006) and takes into account that relationship quality and adjustment, and
scores that intimate partners provide are better problem-solving skills. There were
interdependent. To have a rough indication only two marginally significant partner effects
of the degree of interde- pendence with for positive facial attractiveness illusions. To
regard to the relationship vari- ables that we examine potential sex differences, we com-
used in our study, we computed within- puted four interaction terms by multiplying
couple correlations between both part- ners’ the predictor variables with the gender of
relationship quality scores. The resulting either the actor (for the two actor variables)
correlations were, as expected, all substan- or the partner (for the two partner variables;
i
tial and significant (all p s < .001), indicating cf. Campbell & Kashy, 2002). We centered
a high degree of interdependence: DRQ r = the variables before computing the interaction
.61, MMQ– MA r = .63, and IPSI r = . variables (e.g., Campbell & Kashy, 2002). We
51. added the four interaction terms to the model
These correlations differed only marginally separately, but the analyses showed no signif-
(absolute differences ≤ .02) from the within- icant Gender × Positive Illusion interactions
couple correlations that we controlled for the (absolute t ’s between 0.04 and 1.75,
predictor variables in the following analyses range p’s = .081 –.965), indicating that the
(cf. Kenny, 2004) and are comparable to relations between the positive physical
those attractiveness
found in previous studies (e.g., Barelds, 2005; illusions and relationship quality did not
see also Karney, Bradbury, Fincham, & Sul- differ significantly for men and women.
livan, 1994).
Table 4. Actor and partner effects for the relations between positive illusions about a partner’s
physical attractiveness and relationship quality

MMQ– MA DRQ IPSI


Effect b t p b t p b t p
Actor effect
Facial attractiveness illusion .309 3.35 .001 .556 3.15 .002 .531 2.67 .008
Bodily attractiveness illusion .172 4.38 .000 .269 3.56 .000 .277 3.31 .001
Partner effect
Facial attractiveness illusion .218 2.36 .019 .371 2.11 .036 .167 0.84 .402
Bodily attractiveness illusion .041 1.02 .307 .085 1.13 .258 .146 1.72 .087

Note. MMQ –MA = Maudsley Marital Questionnaire –Marital Adjustment Scale; DRQ = Dutch Relationship
Questionnaire; IPSI = Interactional Problem Solving Inventory.

Table 5. Actor and partner effects for the relations between ratings of facial and bodily
attractiveness (self-ratings and partner ratings) and relationship variables

MMQ– MA DRQ IPSI


Effect b t p b t p b t p
Actor effect
Self-ratings of facial −.007 −0.07 .942 .093 0.54 .589 .536 2.71 .007
attractiveness Self-
ratings of bodily −.031 −0.78 .436 −.117 −1.63 .104 −.172 −2.07 .039
attractiveness
Ratings of a partner’s facial .292 2.94 .004 .496 2.66 .008 .350 1.65 .100
attractiveness
Ratings of a partner’s bodily .183 4.52 .000 .326 4.24 .000 .297 3.47 .001
attractiveness
Partner effect
Self-ratings of facial .064 0.71 .481 .436 2.51 .013 .099 0.51 .612
attractiveness
Self-ratings of bodily −.118 −3.10 .002 −.124 −1.83 .069 −.176 −2.18 .030
attractiveness
Ratings of a partner’s facial .196 1.98 .049 .152 0.81 .417 .071 0.34 .737
attractiveness
Ratings of a partner’s bodily .049 1.19 .234 .086 1.13 .261 .197 2.27 .024
attractiveness

MMQ –MA = Maudsley Marital Questionnaire –Marital Adjustment Scale; DRQ = Dutch Relationship Questionnaire;
IPSI = Interactional Problem Solving Inventory.

Next, we conducted analyses to examine For this purpose, we used all physical attrac-
which of the physical attractiveness ratings of tiveness ratings per couple as predictors in the
both partners related most strongly to analyses: four actor variables (self-ratings of
relation- ship quality. Whereas the previous facial and bodily attractiveness and ratings of
analyses examined the effect of positive a partner’s facial and bodily attractiveness),
physical attrac- tiveness illusions, these and four partner variables (the same variables,
additional analyses might show which part but rated by the partner; see Table 5).
of the positive phys- ical attractiveness We found consistent significant actor
illusion (i.e., the ratings of a partner’s effects between ratings of a partner’s bodily
physical attractiveness, or a part- ner’s self- and (to a lesser extent) facial attractiveness
ratings of physical attractiveness) is related and relationship variables: Individuals
most strongly to relationship quality.
evaluated their relationships more positively We did not find a significant relation between
when they judge their partner to be more age and self-ratings of bodily attractiveness.
physically attractive. Table 5 also shows a Moreover, the ratings of a partner’s facial and
tendency toward a negative partner effect of bodily attractiveness did not correlate signifi-
bodily attractiveness, that is, as their partners i
cantly with age (both r s = −.07, > p). These
rated themselves as more bodily attractive, results suggest a potential interaction between
participants reported lower relationship positive physical attractiveness illusions and
quality. These results, however, did not participant age, particularly with regard to
consistently reach significance. In addition, facial attractiveness: As people grow older,
we found an isolated significant positive actor self-ratings of facial attractiveness get signif-
effect for self-ratings of facial attractiveness: icantly lower, but ratings of a partner’s facial
The more attractive participants rated their attractiveness remain virtually the same. This
own face, the more skilled they were at would suggest stronger positive facial attrac-
problem solving (IPSI). The results in tiveness illusions for older couples, whereas
Table 5 clearly indicate that particularly the older couples also report lower levels of rela-
ratings of a partner’s physical attractiveness tionship quality.
relate positively to relationship quality. To examine potential interaction effects
Self-rated physical attractiveness of both between positive facial attractiveness illusions
partners, however, related only weakly to and age with regard to relationship quality,
relationship quality. An additional analysis we computed the interactions between age
including only self-ratings of both partners and positive facial attractiveness illusions
showed (marginally) significant positive (actor and partner effects, i.e., the controlled
Actor effects for facial attractiveness for ratings of a partner’s facial attractiveness;
the DRQ (b = .431, t = 2.65, p < .01), see pre- vious APIM analyses) after we
the IPSI (b = .738, t = 4.07, p < .001), centered the variables around their mean
and the MMQ – MA scale (b = . (e.g., Campbell
197, t = 2.23, p = .027). In addition, we & Kashy, 2002). We added these interaction
found one significant positive partner effects together with age and the facial attrac-
effect, for tiveness illusions (actor and partner effects)
facial attractiveness (b = .439, t = 2.70, to the model as predictors for the relationship
p < .01). Together, these results indicate variables.
that
We found significant Actor Age × Positive
primarily the ratings of a partner’s physical Facial Attractiveness Illusion Effects for the
attractiveness, not the partner’s self-ratings MMQ – MA scale (b = .855, t = 3.69, p
of physical attractiveness, determine the <
effect of positive illusions about a partner’s .001), the DRQ (b = 1.137, t = 2.57, p <
physical attractiveness on relationship quality .01), and the IPSI (b = 1.357, t = 2.61, p <
(see Table 4). .01). In addition, and in accordance with the
correlational analyses, we found significant
Age differences main effects for age for the IPSI (b =
−2.388, t = −3.36, p < .01) and the
To examine potential age differences with DRQ (b =
regard to physical attractiveness and mea-
−2.104, t = −3.12, p < .01), indicating that
sures of relationship quality, we first calcu- older people reported lower relationship qual-
lated correlations between age and the rela- ity in terms of the DRQ and the IPSI. We did
tionship and physical attractiveness variables. not find significant partner interaction effects.
We found significant inverse relations To illustrate the three actor interaction effects,
between we computed simple slopes for low or high
age and the DRQ (r = −.17, p < .001), the levels of the positive facial attractiveness illu-
IPSI (r = −.18, p < .001), and self-ratings sions, and low or high age. Figures 2 – 4
of facial attractiveness (r = −.17, p < .001): show the mean DRQ, MMQ – MA, and IPSI
With age, people reported lower relationship scores. In addition to showing the main
quality in terms of the DRQ and the IPSI effects of age for the IPSI and DRQ,
and also reported lower facial attractiveness. Figures 2 – 4 also indicate that there is
hardly any relation
Figure 2. Interaction between self positive facial attractiveness illusions and age for the Dutch
Relationship Questionnaire (DRQ).

Figure 3. Interaction between self positive facial attractiveness illusions and age for Maudsley
Marital Questionnaire– Marital Adjustment Scale (MMQ– MA).

between positive facial attractiveness illusions facial attractiveness were not or hardly related
and relationship quality for younger people, to relationship quality, in older couples pos-
whereas particularly older people with low itive illusions about a partner’s facial attrac-
positive facial attractiveness illusions report tiveness were related positively to relationship
lower relationship quality. Simple slopes tests quality. We also examined potential interac-
confirm this: For younger people, there is tion effects between positive bodily attractive-
no significant effect of positive facial attrac- ness illusions and age on relationship qual-
i ity. We only found one marginally significant
tiveness illusions (t s < 2.01, > p), whereas
for older people we found significant effects interaction effect, for the MMQ – MA scale
(b = .442, t = 2.07, p = .039).
for all three dependent variables (DRQ t =
2.57, p < .01; MMQ– MA t = 4.31, p
< Discussion
.001; IPSI t = 3.36, p < .01). Thus, contra- Study 2 found positive illusions about a part-
dicting Hypothesis 3, whereas for younger ner’s facial and bodily attractiveness to be
couples positive illusions about a partner’s
Figure 4. Interaction between self positive facial attractiveness illusions and age for the
Interactional Problem Solving Inventory (IPSI).

positively related to relationship quality vari- held stronger positive illusions about their
ables, particularly as a result of strong rela- partner’s physical attractiveness, they reported
tions between ratings of a partner’s facial and higher relationship quality and relationship
bodily attractiveness and relationship quality. adjustment and dealt more constructively with
In addition, we found an interaction between relationship problems. Positive illusions about
facial attractiveness illusions and couple age: one’s partner’s physical attractiveness may
particularly for older couples there is a posi- enhance relationship quality because they
tive relation between positive facial attractive- may increase levels of sexual satisfaction,
ness illusions and relationship quality. Con- facilitate relationship-enhancing attributions,
sistent with studies that have shown men and and help partners accept and overcome their
women respond similarly to a (potential) part-
weaknesses, dissimilarities, and doubts (e.g.,
ner’s physical attractiveness (e.g., Eastwick
McNulty & Karney, 2004; Murray & Holmes,
& Finkel, 2008), the present study did not
1997; Murray et al., 1996b). We would like to
find participant’s sex to moderate the
note, however, that the opposite might be true
relation- ship between relationship quality
as well. That is, high relationship quality may
and positive illusions.
strengthen, facilitate, or even evoke positive
illusions about a partner’s physical attractive-
General Discussion ness. Because our data were cross-sectional,
we could not test causal relationships between
Consistent with a previous study by Barelds- our study’s variables. Nonetheless, previous
Dijkstra and Barelds (2008), both of our longitudinal studies, by showing that positive
studies found evidence for the existence of illusions have predictive power with regard
positive illusions about a partner’s bodily to relationship quality, have shown positive
and facial attractiveness. That is, we found illusions to be an important and positive shap-
perceptions of a partner’s physical attrac- ing factor of feelings of satisfaction with the
tiveness to be more positive than a part-
relationship (e.g., Miller et al., 2006; Murray
ner’s self-perceptions of physical attractive-
et al., 1996a). Although positive illusions are
ness. In addition, the present research is the
related positively to relationship quality, we
first to present convincing evidence for a
would like to note that, according to Swami
positive relationship between positive illu-
and Furnham (2008), positive illusions may
sions about a partner’s physical attractive-
also have negative consequences for the rela-
ness and relationship quality. As individuals
tionship. People who perceive their partner as
highly attractive may, for instance, become only those older individuals who are highly
unreasonably jealous. Particularly men, who motivated to maintain the quality of their
highly value a mate’s attractiveness, may relationship will be able to adopt or uphold
become overly vigilant in response to positive illusions about their partner’s facial
potential rivals for their, in their perception, attractiveness. Alternatively, it is also possible
extremely attractive partner (Dijkstra & that, in contrast to what we argued earlier,
Buunk, 1998). as people age, facial attractiveness becomes
a more important characteristic in a mate,
Positive illusions, relationship quality, for instance, because it becomes a stronger
and age marker of physical health and condition. It is,
however, hard to find support for a growing
Of much interest is also our finding that the importance of facial attractiveness with age:
relationship between positive illusions about There are no studies on facial attractiveness
a partner’s physical attractiveness and that show that older individuals value facial
relation- ship quality depends on the couples’ attractiveness more than younger individuals.
age. Our results refute the expectation that Finally, it is also possible that our findings
positive illu- sions about a partner’s physical are due to a restriction of range; that is,
attractiveness would be more strongly related among young individuals the variability in
to relationship quality among younger than ratings of relationship quality was much
among older cou- ples. We found no age smaller than among older couples. Regardless
differences in the rela- tionship between of the most valid explanation, our study raises
positive bodily attractiveness illusions and an interesting question concerning the results
relationship quality. This is quite intriguing of previous studies that studied relatively
given the fact that, with age, the body young individuals. If the relation between
becomes less attractive and a partner’s relationship quality and positive illusions
physical attractiveness, relative to other part- about a mate’s facial attractiveness differs for
ner characteristics, less important. A possi- people of different ages, might this not also
ble explanation is that bodily attractiveness be the case with regard to positive illusions
means something different to young people about other mate characteristics, such as those
than to older people. Whereas young peo- Murray and colleagues (1996a, 1996b; e.g.,
ple may emphasize the beauty side or esthet- kindness, dominance, patience, etc.) studied?
ics of bodily attractiveness, such as having
a tight waist or large breasts, older individ-
uals may define bodily attractiveness more in Positive illusions or partner perceptions?
terms of health and physical condition. As a One of the most important findings of the
result, bodily attractiveness does not become present studies, the one with probably the
less important with age nor will positive illu- most implications, is that the positive rela-
sions about a partner’s physical attractiveness. tion between positive illusions and relation-
Contrary to our expectations, we even ship quality can be attributed primarily to the
found that, with age, positive facial attrac- evaluations individuals make of their part-
tiveness illusions were more rather than ners’ physical attractiveness, not to the eval-
less strongly tied to relationship quality. uations partners make of their own physical
Especially older couples who held low attractiveness. This finding strongly indicates
positive facial illusions reported lower that individuals’ perceptions of their partner
relationship quality. A possible explanation are the driving force behind the potentially
for the intriguing finding that positive facial relationship-enhancing effect individuals may
attractiveness illusions are, with age, more derive from positive illusions. Our finding
strongly tied to relationship quality may lie in is consistent with previous studies on part-
the fact that biasing one’s perception of a rel- ner idealization (Knee, Nanayakkara, Vietor,
atively old (i.e., facially unattractive) partner Neighbors, & Patrick, 2001; Miller, Caughlin,
requires more cognitive effort than biasing & Huston, 2003; Miller et al., 2006). Miller
one’s perception of a relatively young (i.e.,
facially attractive) partner. As a consequence,
Positive illusions and physical attractiveness 281

and colleagues (2006), for instance, found phenomenon, suggests that especially older
that, as newlyweds idealized each other more, couples struggling with relationship problems
over the next 13 years, they reported less may benefit from interventions that help them
decline in love. These studies, however, did adopt or maintain a positive image of their
not include self-perceptions and thus were not partner. Psychologists may, for instance, help
able to detect to what extent self-perceptions older couples to learn to focus on their part-
matter or not. The present study did and can ner’s physically attractive features, especially
therefore confidently state that partner percep- facial ones, rather than on his or her physical
tions drive partner idealization and positive shortcomings. Therapists may, for instance,
illusions, not self-perceptions nor the differ- ask partners to write down the physical fea-
ence between self- and partner perceptions. tures of their partner they find attractive,
It would be interesting to examine the remember what about their partner’s appear-
existence of positive illusions about a part- ance initially attracted them, and help partners
ner’s physical attractiveness in other cultures openly talk about their wishes with regard
than the Dutch culture. Are people even more to each other’s physical appearance. If part-
inclined to bias their partner’s physical attrac- ners communicate wishes that are not
tiveness in a positive way, for instance, in realistic, therapists may help them accept
more primitive cultures where people are not their partner’s physical shortcomings and
confronted on a daily basis with innumer- look at those short- comings more mildly.
able images of highly attractive opposite-sex Our study suffers from several limitations.
individuals? Or, instead, do they care less First the present study explained the existence
about their partner’s looks? Another intrigu- of positive illusions about a partner’s physi-
ing question is whether positive illusions dif- cal attractiveness by referring to individuals’
fer between individualistic and collectivistic need for cognitive resolution between their
cultures. Compared with people in individual- relationship hopes and doubts (e.g., McNulty,
istic cultures, such as the Netherlands, people O’Mara, et al., 2008; Murray et al., 1996a).
in collectivistic cultures are more altruistic That is, by adopting positive illusions about
and more concerned about satisfying others’ their partner’s physical attractiveness individ-
wishes (e.g., Markus & Kitayama, 1991). As uals may strengthen their commitment to the
a result, partners in collectivistic cultures may relationship and their sense of security (Mur-
be more concerned by what they can con- ray, 1999). An entirely other possibility, how-
tribute to the relationship and what they can ever, is that people are merely more gener-
offer their partner (such as a physically attrac- ous when they rate someone else’s body than
tive body or face) than what they may take or when they rate their own body. To check for
demand from their partner (such as a partner’s this possibility, we should have asked partici-
physically attractive body or face). This may pants to rate, in addition to their own and
result in a qualitatively different type of posi- their partner’s physical attractiveness, the
tive illusion with regard to physical attractive- physi- cal attractiveness of others (e.g.,
ness, that is, one in which self-perceptions of friends or strangers). Neither did the present
physical attractiveness play a more important study assess other people’s perceptions of a
role than perceptions of a partner’s physical partner’s phys- ical attractiveness, such as
attractiveness. As a result, it is possible that, those of friends or strangers. Murray and
in collectivistic cultures, relationship quality colleagues (2000) have shown that ratings of
is more strongly tied to self-perceptions of friends are relatively objective. Because self-
physical attractiveness than to perceptions of perceptions are often positively biased (e.g.,
a partner’s physical attractiveness. Alicke, 1985; Brown, 1985; Taylor &
Brown, 1988), using percep- tions of friends
Implications, limitations, and conclusion or strangers might result in a less
conservative and more objective indica- tion
Our finding that, among older couples, posi- of positive illusions regarding a partner’s
tive illusions constitute a positive relationship physical attractiveness. An interesting topic
for future research would therefore be to
study
the relationship between relationship quality Barelds-Dijkstra, P., & Barelds, D. P. H. (2008). Positive
and positive illusions about a partner’s physi- illusions about one’s partner’s physical attractiveness.
cal attractiveness as rated by both partners and Body Image, 5 , 99–108.
Bray, G. A. (1998). Contemporary diagnosis and man-
objective observers. In addition, the present
agement of obesity. Newton, PA: Handbooks in
study was cross-sectional in nature. We do Health Care.
not know whether positive illusions lead to Brown, J. D. (1985). Evaluations of self and others:
higher relationship quality, or vice versa, or Self-enhancement biases in social judgments. Social
whether a third variable is responsible for the Cognition, 4 , 353–376.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate
relationship between positive illusions about preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37
a partner’s physical attractiveness and cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12 , 1–49.
relation- ship quality. It would therefore be Buss, D., Larsen, R., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, J.
interesting for future research to follow (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, phys-
iology, and psychology. Psychological Science, 3 ,
couples longitudi- nally and examine
251–255.
whether positive illusions about a partner’s Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Strategies of
physical attractiveness are able to predict human mating. New York: Basic Books.
future relationship quality and stability. Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Fletchenhauer, D., & Ken-
Nonetheless, by using large and het- rick, D. T. (2002). Age and gender differences in
mate selection criteria for various involvement levels.
erogeneous community samples, the present Personal Relationships , 9 , 271–278.
research showed that positive illusions about Buunk, B. P., Dijkstra, P., Kenrick, D. T., & Warnt-
a partner’s physical attractiveness are a posi- jes, A. (2001). Age preferences for mates as related
tive and constructive relationship to gender, own age, and involvement level. Evolution
and Human Behavior , 22 , 241–250.
phenomenon that should we should cherish.
Campbell, L., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Estimating actor,
partner, and interaction effects for dyadic data using
References PROC MIXED and HLM: A user-friendly guide.
Personal Relationships , 9 , 327–342.
Central Bureau voor de Statistiek (CBS). (2007). Zelfger-
Alicke, M. D. (1985). Global self-evaluation as deter-
apporteerde medische consumptie, gezondheid en
mined by the desirability and controllability of trait
leefstijl [self reported medical consumption, health
adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social
and lifestyle]. Retrieved August 18, 2008, from
Psychol- ogy , 49 , 1621–1630.
http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb.
Alicke, M. D., Smith, R. H., & Klotz, M. L. (1986).
Corneille, O., Monin, B., & Pleyers, G. (2005). Is pos-
Judgments of physical attractiveness: The role of
itivity a cue or a response option? Warm glow vs
faces and bodies. Personality and Social Psychology
evaluative matching in the familiarity for attractive
Bulletin, 12 , 381–389.
and not-so-attractive face. Journal of Experimental
Arrindell, W. A., Boelens, W., & Lambert, H. (1983). Social Psychology , 41 , 431–437.
On the psychometric properties of the Maudsley Mar- Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (1998). Jealousy as a func-
ital Questionnaire (MMQ): Evaluation of self-ratings tion of rival characteristics: An evolutionary perspec-
in distressed and “normal” volunteer couples based tive. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24 ,
on the Dutch version. Personality and Individual Dif- 1158–1166.
ferences, 4 , 293–306. Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is
Arrindell, W. A., & Schaap, C. (1985). The Maudsley beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social
Marital Questionnaire (MMQ): An extension of its Psychology , 24 , 285–290.
construct validity. British Journal of Psychiatry , 147 Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., &
, 295–299. Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good,
Barelds, D. P. H. (2005). Self and partner personality in but. . .: A meta-analytic review of research on the
intimate relationships. European Journal of Personal- physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bul-
ity , 19 , 501–518. letin, 110 , 109–128.
Barelds, D. P. H., & Barelds-Dijkstra, P. (2006). Partner Eastwick, P. W., & Finkel, E. J. (2008). Sex differences
personality in distressed relationships. Clinical Psy- in mate preferences revisited: Do people know what
chology and Psychotherapy , 13 , 392–396. they initially desire in a romantic partner? Journal of
Barelds, D. P. H., & Barelds-Dijkstra, P. (2007). Rela- Personality and Social Psychology , 94 , 245–264.
tionship onset and personality similarity. Journal of Englis, B. G., Solomon, M. R., & Ashmore, R. D.
Social and Personal Relationships , 24 , 479–496. (1994). Beauty before the eyes of the beholders: The
Barelds, D. P. H., Luteijn, F., & Arrindell, W. A. (2003). cultural encoding of beauty types in magazine adver-
Nederlandse Relatievragenlijst (NRV): Handleiding tising and music television. Journal of Advertising,
[Dutch Relationship Questionnaire (DRQ): Manual]. 23 , 49–64.
Amsterdam: Harcourt.
Positive illusions and physical attractiveness 283

Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in per- strangers and mates. Journal of Experimental Social
ceptions of desirable body shape. Journal of Abnor- Psychology , 25 , 159–167.
mal Psychology , 94 , 102–105. Kiers, H. A. L., & Groenen, P. (1996). A monotonically
Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of convergent algorithm for orthogonal congruence rota-
physical attractiveness on romanticattraction: A com- tion. Psychometrika, 61 , 375–389.
parison across five research paradigms. Journal of
Knee, C. R., Nanayakkara, A., Vietor, N. A., Neigh-
Personality and Social Psychology , 59 , 981–993.
bors, C., & Patrick, H. (2001). Implicit theories of
Franzoi, S. L., & Herzog, M. E. (1986). The Body
relationships: Who cares if romantic partners are less
Esteem Scale: A convergent and discriminant validity
than ideal? Personality and Social Psychology Bul-
study. Journal of Personality Assessment , 50 , 24–31.
letin, 27 , 808–819.
Franzoi, S. L., & Shields, S. A. (1984). The Body
Lange, A. (1983). De Interactionele Probleem Oplossings
Esteem Scale: Multidimensional structure and sex dif-
Vragenlijst [The Interactional Problem Solving Inven-
ferences in a college population. Journal of Person-
tory]. Deventer, Netherlands: Van Loghum Slaterus.
ality Assessment , 48 , 173–178.
Lewandowski, G., Aron, A., & Gee, J. (2007). Personal-
Gil-Burmann, C., Pela´ez, F., & Sa´nchez, S. (2002). ity goes a long way: The malleability of opposite-sex
Mate choice differences according to sex and age: physical attractiveness. Personal Relationships , 14 ,
An analysis of personal advertisements in Spanish 571–585.
newspapers. Human Nature, 13 , 493–508. Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:
Green, S. K., Buchanan, D. R., & Heuer, S. K. (1984). Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation.
Winners, losers, and choosers: A field investigation Psychological Review , 98 , 224–253.
of dating initiation. Personality and Social McNulty, J. K., & Karney, B. R. (2004). Positive expec-
Psychology Bulletin, 10 , 502–511. tations in the early years of marriage: Should couples
Hageman, P., Lange, A., van Dyck, R., Hoogduin, K., expect the best or brace for the worst? Journal of
& Meijer, A. M. (1990). Het voorspellend vermogen Personality and Social Psychology , 86 , 729–743.
van de Interactionele Probleem Oplossings Vragen- McNulty, J. K., Neff, L. A., & Karney, B. R. (2008).
lijst; een onderzoek bij agorafobici, dwangneurotici, Beyond initial attraction: Physical attractiveness in
ouders van astmapatie¨nten en drugsverslaafden newlywed marriage. Journal of Family Psychology ,
[The prognostic abilities of the Interactional Problem 22 , 135–143.
Solv- ing Inventory: A study of agoraphobics, McNulty, J. K., O’Mara, E. M., & Karney, B. R. (2008).
obsessive- compulsives, and parents of asthma Benevolent cognitions as a strategy of relationship
patients and drug addicts]. Nederlands Tijdschrift maintenance: “Don’t sweat the small stuff”. . . . But
voor de Psychologie en haar Grensgebieden, 45 , it is not all small stuff. Journal of Personality and
335–344. Social Psychology , 94 , 631–646.
Haven, S., & Ten Berge, J. M. F. (1977). Tucker’s coef- Miller, P. J. E., Caughlin, J. P., & Huston, T. L. (2003).
ficient of congruence as a measure of factorial invari- Trait expressiveness and marital satisfaction: The role
ance: An empirical study . Unpublished manuscript, of idealization processes. Journal of Marriage and
University of Groningen, Netherlands. the Family , 65 , 978–995.
Hume, D. K., & Montgomery, R. (2001). Facial attrac- Miller, P. J. E., Niehuis, S., & Huston, T. L. (2006). Pos-
tiveness signals different aspects of “quality”’ in itive illusions in marital relationships: A 13-year lon-
women and men. Evolution and Human Behavior , 22 gitudinal study. Personality and Social Psychology
, 93–112. Bulletin, 32 , 1579–1594.
Karney, B. R., Bradbury, T. N., Fincham, F. D., & Sul- Monin, B. (2003). The warm glow heuristic: When liking
livan, K. T. (1994). The role of negative affectivity in leads to familiarity. Journal of Personality and Social
the association between attributions and marital satis- Psychology , 85 , 1035–1048.
faction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Murray, S. L. (1999). The quest for conviction: Moti-
, 66 , 413–424. vated cognition in romantic relationships. Psycholog-
Kashy, D. A., Campbell, L., & Harris, D. W. (2006). ical Inquiry , 10 , 23–34.
Advances in data analytic approachesfor relationships Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1997). A leap of faith?
research: The broad utility of hierarchical linear Positive illusions in romantic relationships. Personal-
modelling. In A. L. Vangelisti & D. Perlman (Eds.), ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23 , 586–604.
The Cambridge handbook of personal relationships Murray, S. L., Holmes, J., & Dolderman, D. (2000).
(pp. 73–89). New York: Cambridge University Press. What the motivated mind sees: Comparing friends’
Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2000). The analysis perspectives to married partners’ views of each
of data from dyads and groups. In H. T. Reis & other. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology , 36
C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods , 600–620.
in social and personality psychology (pp. 451–477). Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996a).
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. The self-fulfilling nature of positive illusions in
Kenny, D. A. (2004). Dyadic analysis. Retrieved Febru- romantic relationships: Love is not blind, but pre-
ary 22, 2007, from http://davidakenny.net/dyad.htm. scient. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology ,
Kenrick, D. T., Gutierres, S. E., & Goldberg, L. L. 71 , 1155–1180.
(1989). Influence of popular erotica on judgments of
Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (1996b). Individual and relationship correlates of the love- is-
The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization and blind bias. Journal of Individual Differences, 30 ,
the construction of satisfaction in close relationships. 35–43.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology , 70 , Swami, V., & Tove´e, M. J. (2005). Male physical
79–98. attrac- tiveness in Britain and Malaysia: A cross-
Sangrador, J. L., & Yela, C. (2000). “What is beautiful is cultural study. Body Image, 2 , 383–393.
loved”: Physical attractiveness in love relationships in Swami, V., & Tove´e, M. J. (2006). The influence of
a representative sample. Social Behavior and Person- body mass index on the physical attractiveness
ality , 28 , 207–218. preferences of feminist and nonfeminist
Swami, V., Antonakopoulos, N., Tove´e, M. J., & heterosexual women and lesbians. Psychology of
Furn- ham, A. (2006). A critical test of the waist-to- Women Quarterly , 30 , 252–257.
hip ratio hypothesis of female physical attractiveness Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-
in Britain and Greece. Sex Roles, 54 , 201–211. being: A social psychological perspective on mental
Swami, V., & Furnham, A. (2008). Is love really so health. Psychological Bulletin, 103 , 193–210.
blind? The Psychologist , 21 , 108–111. Teuscher, U., & Teuscher, C. (2007). Reconsidering
Swami, V., Furnham, A., Georgiades, C., & Pang, L. the double standard of aging: Effects of gender
(2007). Evaluating self and partner physical attrac- and sexual orientation on facial attractiveness rat-
tiveness. Body Image, 4 , 97–101. ings. Personality and Individual Differences, 42 ,
Swami, V., Neto, F., Tove´e, M. J., & Furnham, A. 631–639.
(2007). Preferences for female body weight and shape Yela, C., & Sangrador, J. L. (2001). Perception of phys-
in three European countries. European Psychologist , ical attractiveness throughout loving relationships.
12 , 220–228. Current Research in Social Psychology, 6 , 57–75.
Swami, V., Stieger, S., Haubner, T., Voracek, M., & Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to
Furnham, A. (2009). Evaluating the physical attrac- the subliminal. Current Directions in Psychological
tiveness of oneself and one’s romantic partner: Science, 10 , 224–228.
Copy r ight of Personal Relationshi ps is the property of Blackwell Pu bl ishing Li mi ted and its
content may not be copied or emai led to mul ti ple sites or posted to a l istserv wi thout the copy r
ight
holder 's express wr itten permission. However. users may pr int. download. or emai l articles for
ind ividual use.

You might also like