You are on page 1of 7

sumer.

Therefore, it is important to
The Influence of Harvest Time on Sensory understand the impact of harvest time
Properties and Consumer Acceptance of and fruit maturity on fruit quality
attributes and the sensory acceptance
Sweet Cherries of the fruit.
Product quality is assessed by
various means, but ultimately is
Maite A. Chauvin1, Matthew Whiting2, and Carolyn F. Ross1,3 judged by the consumer. Consumer
assessments of cherry fruit quality are
ADDITIONAL INDEX WORDS. fruit maturity, quality, Prunus avium based on the fruit’s external and
internal characteristics. Visual cues
SUMMARY. The objective of this study was to identify the influence of harvest time of sweet cherry quality include the
on sensory attributes of sweet cherries (Prunus avium) as evaluated by a trained presence/absence of defects such as
and consumer panel. Over three separate panel days, trained and consumer
pitting and bruising, fruit shape, size,
panelists evaluated ‘Sweetheart’ cherries that were harvested 3 days before
commercial maturity (early harvest), at commercial maturity (midharvest), and 3 pedicel color and thickness, and exo-
days postcommercial maturity (late harvest). Fruit attributes from each harvest time carp coloration. Previous research has
were characterized empirically by quantifying soluble solids concentration, exocarp underscored the importance of exo-
color, and firmness. A sensory panel (n = 12), trained to recognize and evaluate carp color, with consumers preferring
the attributes of cherry appearance (color intensity), texture (flesh firmness and a dark red or mahogany to lighter and
juiciness), and flavor/taste (sweetness, sourness, and cherry flavor intensity), darker-colored fruit (Long et al.,
evaluated the fruit. Fruit were then evaluated by a consumer panel for purchase 2007). Purchase decisions are based
intent, overall acceptance, appearance, flavor, and texture. From the trained panel upon visual assessment (i.e., per-
results, late-harvest cherries were higher in color and flavor intensity. Midharvest ceived quality) and price. Important
cherries were highest in firmness, while early-harvest cherries were lowest in
sweet cherry quality traits include
color intensity and sweetness. Consumer panel results indicated midharvest and
late-harvest cherries had the highest overall acceptance, and midharvest cherries had fruit size, fruit color, firmness, and
the highest acceptance for appearance. Overall acceptance was strongly correlated to sweetness (Sloulin, 1990). Consumer
flavor acceptance (r = 0.94). These results indicated that cherries harvested at acceptance of sweet cherries appears
midharvest were the most preferred of the harvest times despite not having the to be most related to sweetness, flavor
highest intensities of color, sweetness, or flavor. This indicated the importance of intensity, and skin color (Cliff et al.,
color, sweetness, and flavor of cherries on the overall acceptance and the possible 1996; Crisosto et al., 2003). For
interaction of these attributes in consumer acceptance. Furthermore, the results other stone fruit, the most important
suggest that standard harvest maturity indicator (i.e., red coloration of exocarp) was factors that influence the decision to
appropriate for optimum consumer acceptance of ‘Sweetheart’. More broadly, these purchase peaches (Prunus persica),
results suggest that there is some flexibility for the grower with regards to
plums (Prunus domestica), and nec-
‘Sweetheart’ cherry harvest and acceptable sensory properties.
tarines (P. persica) were: safety of the
fruit, healthy, sweet, good appear-

S
weet cherry production has exocarp, with a dark red/mahogany ance, good value, firmness, and ripe-
increased worldwide over the thought of as ideal for the industry ness (Wolf et al., 2003).
past 10 years due to high con- standard cultivar, Bing (Kappel et al., Cherries have a relatively short
sumer demand and good returns 1996). We could find no published fresh market season due to high fruit
to growers. This is particularly true reports that assess attributes of new perishability (Dever et al., 1996).
in the Pacific northwestern United sweet cherry cultivars in relation to Furthermore, the optimum ‘‘harvest
States where production has doubled eating quality and consumer prefer- window’’ (i.e., period of time in
in 8 years. Increased production ele- ence. The development of important which fruit are at optimum harvest
vates competition among growers for quality attributes is not consistent maturity and storability) is perceived
the limited supply of skilled harvest among sweet cherry cultivars (M. to be very short for sweet cherries.
laborers. As a result, growers may Whiting, unpublished data) and there- This underscores the importance of
begin harvest before the fruit achieves fore, a dark red skin color may not be harvesting fruit at the ideal time.
ideal harvest maturity to secure a the ideal harvest maturity for new From a cherry growers’ perspective,
labor force. Furthermore, it is not cultivars. In the competitive sweet the examination of the adjustment of
unusual for fruit to be harvested past cherry market, it is desirable to deliver harvest date may allow more flexibil-
optimum maturity due to an insuffi- an acceptable product to the con- ity with regard to harvest time and
cient labor force.
Optimum commercial maturity
of sweet cherry fruit is generally deter-
mined by red coloration of fruit Units
To convert U.S. to SI, To convert SI to U.S.,
1
School of Food Science, Washington State Univer- multiply by U.S. unit SI unit multiply by
sity, Pullman, WA 99164 10 % gL–1 0.1
2
Department of Horticulture and Landscape Archi- 29.5735 fl oz mL 0.0338
tecture, Irrigated Agriculture Research and Extension 2.54 inch(es) cm 0.3937
Center, Washington State University, Prosser, WA 0.4536 lb kg 2.2046
99350 1.1161 oz/inch gmm–1 0.8960
3
Corresponding author. E-mail: cfross@wsu.edu. (F – 32) O 1.8 F C (1.8 · C) + 32

748 • October–December 2009 19(4)


perhaps an early or late delivery of Prosser, WA (lat. 4617#N), on 6, 9, endpoint of 8.1 and was expressed as
acceptable cherries to the market for and 14 July. Trees were 5 years old milliliters of NaOH added to reach
purchase. and were grown following standard pH 8.1.
Several previous studies have commercial practices. Row orienta- TRAINED PANEL. The sensory
examined the relationship between tion was north-south and trees were panel was composed of 12 panelists
analytical assessment of cherry quality trained to a Y-trellised architecture. (five men and seven women), all
and sensory evaluation. A trained On each harvest day, 30 kg of fruit between the ages of 25 and 69, with
panel was employed to evaluate sen- were picked between 0600 and 0700 an average age of 41. Panelists were
sory properties of cherries and to HR. Random samples of about 100 to recruited using advertising from the
compare trained panel performance 200 fruit were collected in triplicate Washington State University/Pull-
to analytical assessments (Cliff et al., for analytical assessment of quality man community based on their avail-
1996). Results showed a moderate attributes and the remainder was ability and their consumption of fresh
correlation (r = 0.78) between per- transported to Pullman, WA, for use cherries at least once per week during
ceived sweetness and the soluble sol- in panel tests. Fruit were transported local cherry season (e.g., June and
ids concentration (SSC) to titratable under refrigeration at 4 C. Over 3 d, July). Panelists were screened for
acidity (TA) ratio (SAR). A high trained and consumer panelists eval- known dental problems and other
correlation was found between sour- uated ‘Sweetheart’ cherries that were conditions that may have affected
ness and TA (r = 0.82) and sourness harvested 3 d before commercial their performance in this panel. A
and SAR (r = –0.93). However, fla- maturity (early harvest), at commer- minimum amount of information on
vor/texture liking was unrelated to cial maturity (midharvest), and 5 d the nature of the study was provided
any of the analytical variables. Another postcommercial maturity (late to reduce potential bias; panelists
study used consumer evaluations and harvest). were informed that they were assess-
analytical determinations to evaluate ANALYTICAL ASSESSMENT OF ing the sensory properties of cherries.
sweet cherries, but correlations were FRUIT QUALITY. Fruit quality was The panelists were trained using
not determined between the two eval- assessed empirically on three random techniques described by Meilgaard
uations (Crisosto et al., 2003). subsamples of about 100 fruit. Fruit et al. (1999). The cherry attributes
The objective of this study was were weighed individually and were were selected using previous litera-
to determine fruit quality differences then scored visually for skin color on a ture (Cliff et al., 1996). Panelists
and consumer preference for ‘Sweet- 1 to 7 scale [Center Technique Inter- received about 10 h of training during
heart’ sweet cherries harvested at dif- professionnel des Légumes (CTIFL), which they learned each attribute
ferent times: early harvest, midharvest Paris] with 1 being very light red and definition and evaluation technique.
and late harvest. This study used 7 being very dark mahogany. Fruit Panelists were trained to recognize
trained sensory panelists to provide a firmness was determined using the the sensory attributes of cherry ap-
description of the sensory properties industry standard equipment (Firm- pearance (color intensity), texture
of the cherries, and consumer panel- Tech 2; Bioworks, Wamego, KS). (flesh firmness and juiciness), and
ists to determine sensory acceptance SSC was determined on juice ex- flavor/taste (sweetness, sourness,
of the cherries. pressed from five fruit samples using and cherry flavor intensity). Attribute
a pocket refractometer (PAL-1; definitions and reference standards
Materials and methods Atago, Bellevue, WA). TA was deter- are presented in Table 1.
CHERRY SAMPLES. ‘Sweetheart’ mined by titrating 10 mL of juice and In Session One, panelists com-
sweet cherries were harvested in 10 mL of distilled water with 0.1 N pleted a demographic questionnaire
2008 from an orchard just north of sodium hydroxide (NaOH) to a pH and were introduced to the 15-cm

Table 1. Attribute definitions and standards used by the trained panel for the evaluation of cherries using a 15-cm
unstructured line scale (1 cm = 0.3937 inch).
Appearance Definition
Color intensity The degree of color intensity from light to dark. A seven-point color comparator [Center Technique
Interprofessionnel des Légumes (CTIFL), Paris] used for anchors at 0, 2.5, 5, 7.5, 10, 12.5,
and 15 cm, respectively
Texture
Flesh firmness The relative degree of firmness from soft to firm. ‘Bing’ cherries presorted and classified as soft, intermediate, and
firm were used as anchors at 1, 7.5, and 15 cm, respectively.
Juiciness The relative degree of juiciness of the cherries from low to high. Cucumber (Cucumis sativus), honeydew melon
(Cucumis melo), and strawberry (Fragaria ·ananassa) were used as anchors at 1, 7.5, and 15 cm, respectively.
Flavor/taste
Sweetness The relative degree of sweetness from low to high. Grams of sucrose per liter of water (0, 30, and 60, 1 gL–1 =
0.1%) used as anchors at 0, 7.5, and 15 cm, respectively.
Sourness The relative degree of acidity from low to high; Grams of citric acid per liter of water (0, 2, and 4) used as
anchors at 0, 7.5, and 15 cm, respectively.
Cherry flavor The relative intensity of cherry flavor from low to high. Cherry powder extract (Carmi Flavors, Commerce, CA),
unsweetened pie cherries (Safeway Stores, Pleasanton, CA), and red cherry preserve (Safeway) used as anchors
at 0, 7.5, and 15 cm, respectively.

• October–December 2009 19(4) 749


RESEARCH REPORTS

scale. In Sessions Two, Three, and of variance (ANOVA) using panelist, time; all evaluations were made using
Four, panelists were trained to recog- replicate, and cherry as main effects a seven-point scale (1 = dislike very
nize visual attributes, texture, and with appropriate interactions. Tukey’s much, 7 = like very much) (Meilgaard
flavor/taste attributes, respectively. honestly significant difference (HSD) et al., 1999). For each sample, panel-
All of the remaining training sessions was calculated to interpret the differ- ists were then asked their willingness
involved practicing evaluation of the ences in attributes between harvest to pay $2.99/lb for cherries using a
attributes using cherries varying in times. Principal component analysis five-point scale (1 = definitely buy, 5 =
appearance, flavor/taste, and texture (PCA) was applied to sensory attrib- definitely not buy). During the eval-
intensities. During these training ses- utes as a function of attribute means uation, panelists were instructed to
sions, a 15-cm line scale was used for for replicate evaluations. SAS (version cleanse their palates between samples
each attribute, anchored by the terms 9.1; SAS Institute, Cary, NC) and with saltine crackers and water. Re-
‘‘extremely low’’ (1 cm) and ‘‘ex- XLSTAT (version 7.5.3; Addinsoft, sults were collected using Compusen-
tremely high’’ (14 cm). Paris) were used for all statistical seÒfive software.
Panelist performance was as- analyses. Significance for all analyses DATA ANALYSIS. The sensory data
sessed using PanelCheck (version was defined as P £ 0.05. were subjected to two-way ANOVA
1.3.1; Danmarks Tekniske Universi- CONSUMER PANEL. The con- using panelist and cherry as main
tet, Copenhagen, Denmark). Panelist sumer sensory panel was conducted effects. Tukey’s HSD was calculated
validity was assessed through devia- over 3 d, a different day for each to interpret the differences in sensory
tion from group mean and reliability harvest time. All panelists were be- attributes between harvest times
was assessed through duplicate evalu- tween the ages of 18 and 75. For using SAS and XLSTAT. Correlation
ations. Panelists were provided with the early-harvest cherries, 72 con- analysis was conducted to identify
feedback following each training sumers performed evaluations. For correlations between attributes. PCA
session, with additional training pro- midharvest and late-harvest cherries, was applied to sensory attributes as a
vided if necessary. Three formal prac- 100 consumers performed evalua- function of replicate mean using
tice evaluations were conducted in tions. Panelists were recruited using XLSTAT. Significance was estab-
individual sensory booths equipped advertising in the Washington State lished at P £ 0.05.
with laptop computers to familiarize University /Pullman community. Pan-
panelist with the computer program elists were awarded a small nonmonet- Results and discussion
(CompusenseÒfive, release 4.6; ary incentive for their participation. ANALYTICAL EVALUATIONS. Har-
Compusense, Guelph, ON, Canada) Evaluations took place in indi- vest date had an impact on key fruit
that was used during data collection. vidual sensory booths equipped with quality attributes. Fruit skin color was
TRAINED SENSORY PANEL laptop computers for recording data. similar for early-harvest and midharv-
EVALUATIONS. Following analytical The cherries were presented at room est fruit, but darker red for the late-
evaluations (Prosser, WA), cherry temperature under natural lighting harvested fruit (Table 2). The 2.7
samples were presented to each pan- conditions. Cherry selections were CTIFL skin color rating of the early-
elist for evaluation. For each harvest identified using three-digit codes and midharvest fruit was lighter than
time, panelists were presented with a and presented one at a time to panel- the ideal skin color of CTIFL 5
total of four samples per session (one ists. Panelists were presented with reported by Kappel et al. (1996) from
cherry per sample), with each sample two cherries per sample plate, with work on unidentified sweet cherry
being presented one at a time. Thus, three flights per session. Thus, each cultivars and selections. Sweet cherry
the fruit from each harvest time were panelist evaluated each cherry harvest skin color darkens as fruit ripen and
evaluated four times on the same day time in triplicate. Panelists were first consumers use color to determine
by each panelist. Evaluations took asked to complete a demographics freshness (Kappel et al., 1996; Pro-
place in individual sensory booths questionnaire, followed by evalua- ebsting and Mills, 1981); however,
equipped with laptop computers for tions for overall acceptance and the there is no relationship between skin
recording data and under natural acceptability of color, texture, and color and other key quality attributes
light conditions and at room temper- flavor/taste for each cherry harvest when comparing different cultivars
ature. The cherry samples were iden-
tified using a three-digit code.
Panelists first evaluated the intensity
of the cherry appearance (color inten- Table 2. Separation of three harvest times of ‘Sweetheart’ sweet cherries based on
sity). The panelists then tasted the soluble solids concentration (SSC), degree of color intensity, and firmness. Mean
values of SSC, color, and firmness are presented (n = 10).
sample and evaluated the texture
(flesh firmness and juiciness) and the Color intensity Firmness
flavor/taste (sweetness, sourness, and Harvest timez SSC (%) (1–7 scale)y (gmm–1)x
cherry flavor intensity) of each cherry. Early harvest 19.28 aw 2.65 a 303.80 a
Panelists were presented with water, Midharvest 19.43 a 2.73 a 281.95 b
saltine crackers, and expectoration Late harvest 19.49 a 3.51 b 264.61 c
cups to cleanse the palate between z
Early harvest = 3 d before commercial harvest, midharvest = commercial harvest, late harvest = 5 d after
samples. Results were collected using commercial harvest.
y
CompusenseÒfive software. CTIFL (Centre Technique Interprofessionnel des Légumes, Paris) color scale where 1 = light and 7 = dark.
x
Determined by the FirmTech 2 (Bioworks, Wamego, KS); 1 gmm–1 = 9.8100 Nm–1.
DATA ANALYSIS. The sensory data w
Within each analytical measurement (column), a different letter indicates a significant difference via Tukey’s
were subjected to three-way analysis honestly significant difference (HSD) test at P £ 0.05.

750 • October–December 2009 19(4)


(M. Whiting, unpublished data). Table 3. Separation of ‘Sweetheart’ cherries at early harvest, midharvest, and late
‘Bing’ for example, is a cultivar that harvest based on evaluation of sensory attributes by the trained panel (n = 12)
has low SSC (e.g., <18%) at CTIFL using a 15-cm unstructured line scale. Mean values for color intensity, firmness,
skin color rating of 2 to 3. In contrast, juiciness, sweetness, sourness, and flavor intensity are presented (n = 48).
‘Sweetheart’ appears to accumulate Harvest timez
high SSC at relatively light skin color Early harvest Midharvest Late harvest
(i.e., apparently immature). Indeed, Cherry attribute Mean value (cm)y
SSC was high and not different
among the harvest timings (Table 2) Color intensity 6.04 ax 7.55 b 8.69 c
despite fruit having different skin Firmness 7.00 a 8.50 b 7.35 a
color and being harvested up to 8 d Juiciness 10.79 a 10.62 a 10.52 a
apart. Fruit firmness was different for Sweetness 8.07 a 9.31 b 10.00 b
all harvest times, with early-harvest Sourness 9.04 a 8.56 a 8.31 a
cherries having the highest firmness Flavor intensity 8.90 a 9.41 a 10.49 b
z
and late-harvest cherries being the Early harvest = 3 d before commercial harvest, midharvest = commercial harvest, late harvest = 5 d after
commercial harvest.
softest (Table 2). This trend reflected y
Trained panel scores were evaluated along a 15-cm unstructured line scale anchored by the terms ‘‘extremely
the softening of fruit flesh during high’’ (14 cm) and ‘‘extremely low’’ (1 cm); 1 cm = 0.3937 inch.
x
Within each harvest time (column), a different letter indicates a significant difference via Tukey’s honestly
ripening and has been reported pre- significant difference (HSD) test at P £ 0.05.
viously for ‘Bing’ (Proebsting and
Mills, 1981).
Based strictly upon empirical firmness decreased, juiciness and harvest cherries were defined by high-
assessment of fruit quality at the dif- sourness did not change, and sweet- intensity ratings for sourness and low
ferent harvest dates, the early-harvest ness and cherry flavor increased. This ratings for sweetness, flavor inten-
‘Sweetheart’ cherries would likely be study also reported that a first harvest sity, firmness, and juiciness. Cherries
rated as the best quality. This is due of ‘Bing’ cherries was higher in sour- picked at midharvest were described
predominantly to their having similar ness compared with a second ‘Bing’ by their high intensity of flavor and
SSC and color, but higher firmness harvest. color intensity or high juiciness, while
than fruit picked later. Instrumental assessments of fruit late-harvest cherries had high inten-
SENSORY EVALUATION. Trained SSC showed no differences between sities of sweetness, color, and flavor.
panel assessments of fruit quality the three harvest times (Table 2). In Following the analytical and
provided a useful comparison with contrast, trained panelists perceived trained panel evaluation of the cher-
instrument-based assessments (Table an increase in sweetness with each ries, a consumer panel was used as the
3). No differences between panelists harvest date, with the mid- and late- third approach to evaluate the quality
or replicate sessions were observed harvested fruit being rated sweeter of the cherries. Overall acceptance
based on ANOVA. Differences among than early-harvested fruit (P £ 0.05). was higher in the midharvest and
the early-harvest, midharvest, and These trained panel results are sup- late-harvest cherries compared with
late-harvest cherries were observed ported by the sugar-to-acid ratios of the early-harvest cherries (Table 4).
for all sensory attributes, including the cherries. The early-harvest cherry Consumers had greater acceptance of
those where instrument-based analy- was found to have the lowest sugar- the midharvest cherries compared
ses revealed no differences. Color to-acid ratio (13:1), while the late- with the early or late-harvest cherries.
intensity and sweetness were highest harvest cherry had the highest ratio In terms of texture, the midharvest
in the late-harvest cherries and lowest (16:1). Cliff et al. (1996) reported a cherries were higher in acceptance
in the early-harvest fruit. Cherries har- weak correlation between SSC and compared with the late-harvest cher-
vested at midseason were perceived as perceived sweetness. In the present ries, with the early-harvest cherries in
the most firm and the early- and late- study, together with this increase in between. Consumers rated their
harvest fruit were similar. Flavor inten- sweetness, cherry color intensity also acceptance of flavor of early-harvest
sity was greater in the late-harvest increased. Crisosto et al. (2003) cherries lower than the other two
cherries compared with the other found that as cherry skin color turned harvest times, which were similar.
two. No differences in juiciness or from light to dark, there was a corre- Overall, these results indicated that
sourness were observed between the sponding increase in SSC. The for all attributes, including overall
three harvest times. Sweetness was present study found similar results acceptance, cherries harvested at mid-
lowest in the early-harvest cherries. with the trained panel evaluations. season maturity were the most ac-
Color, sweetness, and flavor The position of the sensory cepted of the three harvest times.
intensity for late-harvest maturity attributes of ‘Sweetheart’ cherries on However, this apparent preference
cherries showed high-intensity means. the PCA plot as evaluated by the for midharvest fruit was not reflected
These results corroborated those trained sensory panel is presented in in the consumers’ willingness to pay
from a study by Dever et al. (1996), Fig. 1. PC1 described 58.2% of the $2.99/lb cherries. No differences
who compared ‘Bing’ cherries from a variation and was primarily described were found between the three harvest
third harvest (characterized as a mid- by flavor and color intensity. PC2 times. These results indicate that on
season harvest) to ‘Bing’ cherries described 22.2% of the variation and the whole, consumers accepted all
from a fifth harvest (late harvest). In was primarily defined by and juiciness cherries, with the lowest score for
comparing these two harvest times, firmness. A clear separation among any attribute being 5.5 on a seven-
the authors found that cherry flesh harvest timings was observed. Early- point scale.
• October–December 2009 19(4) 751
RESEARCH REPORTS

acceptance of cherries by consumers


was flavor/taste (r = 0.94) (Table 5).
For the consumer evaluations, sweet-
ness, sourness, and flavor intensity
were not evaluated separately as they
were for the trained panel. Therefore,
the definition of ‘‘flavor’’ as evalu-
ated by the consumer panel included
many attributes. A previous study also
reported a strong correlation between
liking and flavor, with a slightly
weaker correlation between liking
and sweetness (Dever et al., 1996).
Flavor and sweetness intensities have
been reported to be the best predic-
tors of flavor/texture liking of cher-
ries (Cliff et al., 1996). Cliff et al.
(1996) also showed cherry flavor
intensity to be highly correlated with
the sum of sweetness and sourness.
Another study found that high con-
sumer acceptance of ‘Brooks’ and
‘Bing’ cherries was highly dependent
upon SSC, with results indicating that
Fig. 1. Position of the sensory attributes of early harvest (EH), midharvest (MH), SSC needed to be at least 16% to
and late harvest (LH) ‘Sweetheart’ cherries on the principal components analysis satisfy the majority of American con-
(PCA) plot as evaluated by the trained sensory panel. Separation was along principal
sumers (Crisosto et al., 2003).
component 1 [PC1 (58.19%)] and PC2 (22.23%)]. EH-1, EH-2, EH-3, and EH-4
represent the mean of 12 evaluations for four replicate cherry samples harvested 3 d Another study (Kappel et al., 1996)
before commercial harvest. MH-1, MH-2, MH-3, and MH-4 represent the mean of found a linear relationship between
12 evaluations for four replicate cherry samples at commercial harvest. LH-1, LH-2, SSC and favorable cherry rating, sug-
LH-3, and LH-4 represent the mean of 12 evaluations for four replicate cherry gesting a minimum SSC for optimal
samples harvested 5 d after commercial harvest. acceptance of sweet cherries to be
15%. As SSC increased as fruit
matured, the highest SSC or sweet-
Table 4. Separation of ‘Sweetheart’ cherries at different harvest times based on ness value was expected in the late-
overall acceptance and acceptance of appearance, texture, and flavor/taste
attributes as evaluated by the consumer panel (n = 72 for early harvest, n = 100
harvest fruit as was observed in the
for mid- and late harvest). Mean values for attribute intensity are presented trained panel results (Table 3). Even
(n = 216 for early harvest and n = 300 for mid and late harvest). though the late-harvest fruit was the
sweetest, it did not have the highest
Harvest timez overall acceptance, indicating the
Early harvest Midharvest Late harvest importance of other attributes, such
Cherry attribute Hedonic scores (1–7 scale)y as sourness, flavor, and texture on
Overall acceptance 5.71 ax 5.98 b 5.90 ab sweet cherry acceptance.
Appearance 6.14 a 6.32 b 6.14 a Appearance had a positive corre-
Texture 6.06 ab 6.18 b 5.91 a lation with overall acceptance (r =
Flavor/taste 5.49 a 5.75 b 5.77 b 0.73) in this study. These findings
z
Early harvest = 3 d before commercial harvest, midharvest = commercial harvest, late harvest = 5 d after agreed with a study conducted by
commercial harvest. Crisosto et al. (2003), who reported
y
Hedonic scores ranged from 1 to 7 where 1 = dislike very much and 7 = like very much. a strong influence of appearance on
x
Within each harvest time (column), a different letter indicates a significant difference via Tukey’s honestly
significant difference (HSD) test at P £ 0.05. cherry purchase, with the highest
percentage (64%–80%) of consumers
deciding to purchase ‘Bing’ and
Separation of the cherries by were characterized by lower overall ‘Brooks’ cherries based upon their
consumers harvested at the three har- and flavor acceptance. Cherries har- dark skin color. Another study (Cliff
vest times is shown in Fig. 2. PC1 vested at midseason were generally et al., 1996) also found that color had
described 73.4% of variation and had rated positively for all attributes. an impact on consumer acceptance of
heavy positive loadings for appear- Late-harvest cherries were character- cherries, with consumers preferring a
ance and overall acceptance. PC2 ized by lower acceptance of texture uniform, darker cherry. In the present
described 21.5% of the variation in and appearance compared with the study, the consumers evaluated over-
the data with a positive loading for other two timings. all appearance and not specifically
texture. Clear separation between the Pearson correlations among color. Thus, ‘‘appearance,’’ as eval-
cherries from the three harvest dates cherry harvest times indicated that uated by the consumers, may have
was observed. Early-harvest cherries one of the main drivers of overall included other parameters such as
752 • October–December 2009 19(4)
the early results from Table 5 showing
the correlation between overall ac-
ceptance and flavor. Cherry texture
provoked fewer comments, suggest-
ing that these properties were not as
important to the consumers when
making comments about the cherries.
No comments about appearance were
noted. The frequency of comments
about the ‘‘good firmness’’ of the
midharvest cherries supported the re-
sults from Table 4 showing the higher
acceptance of midharvest cherries and
the lower acceptance of late-harvest
cherries.
In this study, three techniques
were employed to evaluate cherry
quality: analytical evaluations, trained
panel, and consumer panel evalua-
tions. The analytical evaluations
showed no differences in SSC be-
tween the three harvest times, while
the trained sensory panel did find
differences in sweetness. The con-
Fig. 2. Position of the sensory attributes of early (EH), midharvest (MH), and late sumer panel did not explicitly evalu-
harvest (LH) ‘Sweetheart’ cherries on the principal components analysis (PCA) plot ate ‘‘sweetness’’ acceptance, but
as evaluated by the consumer sensory panel. Separation was along principal
flavor/taste acceptance did show the
component [PC1 (73.40%)] and PC2 (21.47%)]. EH-1, EH-2, and EH-3 represent
the mean of 72 evaluations for three replicate cherry samples harvested 3 d before same trends as observed in the trained
commercial harvest. MH-1, MH-2, and MH-3 represent the mean of 100 panel results. Based on a previous
evaluations for three replicate cherry samples at commercial harvest. LH-1, LH-2, study (Cliff et al., 1996), an increase
and LH-3 represent the mean of 100 evaluations for three replicate cherry samples in SSC would increase cherry accept-
harvested 5 d after commercial harvest. PC = principal component. ance. These results indicated that the
analytical evaluation was not as sensi-
tive as the sensory evaluations of
Table 5. Pearson correlations between overall acceptance and acceptance of sweetness or that other factors such
appearance, texture, and flavor/taste of ‘Sweetheart’ cherries harvested at three as the concentration, temperature,
harvest times as evaluated by the consumer panel (n = 272). No linear chemical composition of panelist sal-
relationship is indicated by 0. A perfect positive linear relationship is indicated by
+1. This means that following an exact linear rule, as one variable increases in its
iva, the presence of other tastants, or
value, the other variable also increases in its value. A perfect negative linear the incidence of ageusia may have
relationship is indicated by –1: as one variable increases in its value, the other influenced the panelists’ evaluation
variable decreases via an exact linear rule. of sweetness (Meilgaard et al., 1999).
Analytical color evaluation
Attributes Overall acceptance Appearance Texture Flavor
showed the late-harvest cherries to
Overall acceptance 1.00 0.73 0.23 0.94 have the darkest color, with early-
Appearance 0.73 1.00 0.83 0.44 harvest and midharvest cherries as
Texture 0.23 0.83 1.00 –0.13 lighter. Similar results were observed
Flavor/taste 0.94 0.44 –0.13 1.00 with the trained panel. Firmness was
different when evaluated analytically,
with the late-harvest cherries being
speckles and stem length, which were observed with the trained panel the least firm and the early-harvest
not as useful as color uniformity and (Table 3) or the consumer panel cherries being the most firm. The
size on predicting visual liking (Cliff (Table 4). These results reflected a trained panel results did not show
et al., 1996). lack of panelist sensitivity to changes the same clear separation. Identifica-
Texture showed the weakest cor- in texture or a large range in texture tion of subtle textural changes in
relation to overall acceptance (r = liking. foods has been shown to be difficult
0.227). In a study of the attributes Table 6 shows the frequency of for panelists, as these textural changes
of an ideal sweet cherry, panelists consumer comments about the cher- are often accompanied by differences
indicated there was an ideal firmness ries at the different harvest times. or changes in other sensory properties
value associated with a cherry (Kappel Comments about the sweetness and (Corradini et al., 2001).
et al., 1996). In the present study, the sourness of the cherries had the high- In summary, for determination
analytical evaluations indicated a clear est frequency, indicating the impor- of taste/flavor and color quality, sen-
separation of cherries based on firm- tance of these attributes to the sory evaluation techniques appeared
ness; however, this separation was not consumers. These results support to be the most appropriate, while for
• October–December 2009 19(4) 753
RESEARCH REPORTS

Table 6. Frequency analysis of consumer comments regarding ‘Sweetheart’ prediction of sweet cherry liking. Food
cherries harvested at early harvest, midharvest, and late harvest by 272 Res. Int. 28:583–589.
consumers.
Corradini, M.G., R. Engel, and M. Peleg.
Harvest timez 2001. Sensory thresholds of consistency
Attribute Comment Early harvest Midharvest Late harvest Total of semiliquid foods: Evaluation by
squeezing flow viscometry. J. Texture
Taste/flavor attributes Consumer comments (no.) Stud. 32:143–154.
Sour ‘‘Too sour’’ 57 58 48 163
Sweet ‘‘Acceptable sweetness’’ 16 18 16 50 Crisosto, C.H., G.M. Crisosto, and P.
Bitter ‘‘Too bitter’’ 1 1 0 2 Metheney. 2003. Consumer acceptance
of ‘Brooks’ and ‘Bing’ cherries is mainly
Texture attributes Consumer comments (no.)
dependent on fruit SSC and visual skin
Juicy ‘‘Good juiciness’’ 8 1 2 11 color. Postharvest Biol. Technol. 28:
Firm ‘‘Good firmness’’ 6 16 6 28 159–167.
Soft ‘‘Too soft’’ 7 14 19 40
z
Early harvest = 3 d before commercial harvest, midharvest = commercial harvest, late harvest = 5 d after
Dever, M.C., R.A. MacDonald, M.A.
commercial harvest. Cliff, and W.D. Lane. 1996. Sensory
evaluation of sweet cherry cultivars.
HortScience 31:150–153.
firmness evaluation, analytical deter- effective for taste/flavor and color Kappel, F., B. Fisher-Fleming, and E.
minations provided good separation. attributes, while analytical measure- Hogue. 1996. Fruit characteristics and
ments were the most effective for sensory attributes of an ideal sweet cherry.
Conclusions firmness determinations. HortScience 31:443–446.
Results from this study show the In a broader sense, these results
influence of harvest time on the sen- suggest that cherries harvested at Long, L.E., M.D. Whiting, and A.B.
Marin. 2007. Consumers show preference
sory properties and consumer accept- commercial maturity, while not hav- for key cherry attributes. Good Fruit
ance of sweet cherries. The trained ing the highest intensities of color, Grower 58:15–17.
panel profile of the cherries found the sweetness, or flavor, were still the
midharvest cherries higher in firm- most accepted of the harvest times Meilgaard, M., G.V. Civille, and B.T.
ness, the late-harvest cherries were compared. This indicates the impor- Carr. 1999. Descriptive analysis techni-
highest in color and flavor intensity, tance of a number of sensory attrib- ques, p. 161–172. In: M. Meilgaard, G.V.
Civille, and B.T. Carr (eds.). Sensory
and the early-harvest cherries were utes of cherries on the overall evaluation techniques. 3rd ed. CRC Press,
lowest in sweetness (P £ 0.05). Con- acceptance and the possible interac- New York.
sumer panel results indicated mid- tion of these attributes in consumer
harvest cherries (harvested at acceptance. In addition, the results Proebsting, E.L. and H.H. Mills. 1981.
commercial maturity) had the highest indicate that while sensory differences Effects of season and crop load on ma-
overall acceptance, and highest ac- between the cherries harvested at the turity characteristics of ‘Bing’ cherry.
J. Amer. Soc. Hort. Sci. 106:144–146.
ceptance for appearance. Late-harvest three times are apparent, the cherries
cherries had the lowest acceptance for are still accepted by the consumers. Sloulin, W. 1990. Cherry quality survey:
texture, while early-harvest cherries This suggests that for ‘Sweetheart’ Status report. Proc. Washington State
had the lowest acceptance for fla- cherries, growers do have flexibility Hort. Assn. 86:226–227.
vor/taste (P £ 0.05). Overall accept- in regards to harvest date. Wolf, M.M., A.J. Martin, and T. Cagianut.
ance was strongly correlated to flavor 2003. An analysis of the importance of
acceptance (r = 0.82). For cherry ripeness to consumers in the United States
quality determinations, the most Literature cited when making a purchase decision for
effective technique depended upon Cliff, M.A., M.C. Dever, J.W. Hall, and B. peaches, plums, and nectarines. Acta Hort.
the attribute of interest. Sensory eval- Girard. 1996. Development and evalua- 604:61–67.
uation techniques were the most tion of multiple regression models for

754 • October–December 2009 19(4)

You might also like