You are on page 1of 88

THE EFFECT OF SPECIFIC NONVERBAL CONDUCTING ELEMENTS ON

STUDENT PERCEPTIONS OF TEACHING EFFECTIVENESS IN A CLASSROOM


REHEARSAL ENVIRONMENT

A dissertation submitted to the College of Fine and Professional Arts


of Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the
requirements for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy

by

John P. Roebke

December, 2005

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
UMI Number: 3203449

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy
submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and
photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.
In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript
and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized
copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

UMI
UMI Microform 3203449
Copyright 2006 by ProQuest Information and Learning Company.
All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against
unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest Information and Learning Company


300 North Zeeb Road
P.O. Box 1346
Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f the copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout p erm ission.
Dissertation written by

John P. Roebke

B.M., Hastings College, 1989

M.Mus.Ed., University of Colorado, 1992

Ph.D., Kent State University, 2005

Approved by

ir, Doctoral Dissertation Committee

, Members, Doctoral Dissertation Committee

Accepted by

Director, School of Music

• CWXjjDblk_s. Dean, College of Fine and Professional Arts

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Page

TABLE OF CONTENTS............................................................................................... iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS........................................................................................... iv

DEDICATION.................................................................................................................v

CHAPTER
I. INTRODUCTION..................................................................................... 1
Need for the Study.....................................................................15
Purpose of the Study..................................................................16
Variables.....................................................................................17
Research Questions................................................................... 18
Definition of Terms................................................................... 19
Limitations................................................................................. 20

II. REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE............................................ 21


Studies in Nonverbal Communication.....................................21
Studies in Nonverbal Aspects of Conducting......................... 25
Issues of Teaching Effectiveness............................................. 36

III. METHODOLOGY................................................................................. 42
Development of PTEM.............................................................43
Treatment Tape..........................................................................45
Participants.................................................................................49
Collection of Data..................................................................... 49
Scoring of PTEM.......................................................................52
Analysis of Data........................................................................ 52

IV. RESULTS................................................................................................53
Preliminary Analysis of Data................................................... 54
Presentation of Data.................................................................. 54

V. DISCUSSION........................................................................................ 59
Summary of Findings................................................................60
Discussion and Implications....................................................62
Conclusions................................................................................70
Limitations and Recommendations for Further Research.... 71

APPENDIX
A. Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure...................................74

REFERENCES.................................................................................................................77

iii

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I offer my sincere and grateful appreciation to Daryl Kinney who provided

support and enthusiasm in guiding this project to completion. Thanks also to Wayne

Gorder who was a model of wisdom, civility, and intellectual curiosity, to Janet Meyer

who helped me begin my research, to Dwayne Wasson who offered much needed

support, encouragement, and stability as my project drew to a close, and to John Lee and

Jere Forsythe for guidance along the way.

Thank you to persons who assisted with the collection of data for this project,

especially Laurie Lafferty, Tad Greig, John Venesky, Dan Schmidt, and Angela Adkins.

Many thanks to all who helped me throughout this endeavor, especially the

students, faculty, and staff at Waterloo High School, as well as Betsy Page, Nancy

McCracken, Bob Stadoulis, Fr. John Jerek, and the parishioners of University Parish for

their interest and encouragement.

Most of all I thank my loving wife, Carmen, for keeping me clothed and well fed,

and for always remembering my best qualities while overlooking my flaws.

iv

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
DEDICATION

To my late father, Andrew,

who always was very impressed by the letters P - H - D,

and to my mother, Linda,

who tempered Dad’s exuberance by saying the letters only mean,

“Piled Higher and Deeper,”

both constant advocates for my continuous pursuit of education.

* * *

AMDG

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
1

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Instrumental music teachers encounter a variety of conducting/teaching styles

through the course of their education and teaching experience. Many teachers tend to

teach as they were taught, some with a greater degree of success than others. While the

tendency may exist to emulate a middle or high school teacher, all teachers pass through

a teacher education program before entering the professional realm. One significant

factor affecting teacher preparedness in the classroom is effective teacher education.

The purpose of a teacher education program is to provide pre-service teachers

with information, skills, and experiences they need to become successful classroom

teachers. At the broadest point of dissemination, the information content may include

learning theory and basic school law. Technology skills may encompass topics from film

projectors to cutting-edge computer-assisted instruction. Pre-service teachers accumulate

educational experiences both in and beyond the halls of the university. But, in each area

of educational expertise, there are field-specific elements of teacher preparation.

Teacher education programs in colleges and universities address the development

of content-specific teaching skills. These are the skills unique to any academic

discipline. The more thoroughly prepared teachers are to educate students in their

classrooms, the more significant the results of those students’ learning experiences will

be.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
Educators of pre-service music teachers need to be aware of the perceived

effectiveness of skills and professional practices imparted to their students. In this

endeavor music educators provide the pre-service music teacher with information and

skills specific to the discipline of music. One such skill specific to music education is

conducting.

The skill of conducting includes the development of specific gestures. This set of

conducting gestures is part of the broader field of nonverbal communication. Knowledge

of effective use of nonverbal components in conducting is one component of music

teaching that may contribute to effective music instruction.

Conducting

In the investigation of nonverbal components of conducting, it is necessary first to

review information from three main areas. The first area is the practice of conducting. A

second area is the issue o f gesture in conducting. Finally, the area of research in

conducting is addressed.

Conducting, as a practice, is defined in a variety of ways. In consulting an Italian

dictionary one finds that maestro is defined as “a master teacher or instructor” (Melzi,

1976, p. 210). Webster’s (1997) defined a conductor as “a person who directs an

orchestra or chorus, communicating to the performers by motions of a baton or the hands

his or her interpretation of the music” (p. 426). In practice, the conductor’s responsibility

is one of communication and interpretation. “Like all semiotic activities, conducting is

construed in terms of a hierarchy of meanings which complement each other: time-

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
3

beating movements - the signs of conducting - exist only as basic aspects of authority

over features of performance” (Galkin, 1988, p. 767). These definitions fail to address

the reality that in the United States most conductors are school music teachers.

A significant aspect of conducting is the gesture, or nonverbal behavior.

“Nonverbal behavior is not a single, unified phenomenon with but one type of usage, one

origin and one form of coding. Instead, facial and body behavior involve a number of

quite different behaviors” (Ekman & Friesen, 1969, p. 62-63). The development of

effective nonverbal behaviors can be a significant, if not imperative, element in the

training of a teacher/conductor. Ostling (1977) went so far as to suggest that it is possible

that aspects of conducting may be learned as acquired skills in nonverbal patterns of

behavior.

A variety of research has focused on identification and categorization of the

elements of conducting. In 1983, Berz conducted a study for the purpose of developing

an instrument designed to classify specific nonverbal conducting techniques. Berz

indicated in his introduction, “Since nonverbal communication is very important to a

conductor, it is also important to determine how a conductor uses nonverbal behaviors to

communicate” (Berz, 1983, p. 2). Hausmann (1984) conducted a similar study of coded

gestural analysis, which took into account and controlled the music used in the study. He

found in his results that gestures determined by and appropriate to a specific musical style

made a difference in ensemble interpretation. In his summary of research in music

teacher effectiveness, Brand (1985) commented, “Frequent eye contact, use of physical

gestures, and variation of facial expressions and speaking voice are characteristics of

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
4

many effective directors.” A study by Dickey (1988) compared verbal and nonverbal

instruction techniques. Through his study, involving four middle school band classes

receiving all verbal or all modeling instruction, he found that all-nonverbal instruction

was a more efficient means of teaching than was all-verbal instruction.

Different gestures convey different meanings. Green (1987) reduced expressive

conducting gestures into two types: “the active gestures, requiring a response from the

players, and the passive gestures, which ask only for silence, no sound, from the members

of the ensemble. The active gestures are accompanied by much impulse of will on the

conductor’s part; the passive gestures show an apparent lack of this factor” (p. 47). If a

gesture conveys no meaning, it should not be incorporated into conducting. McElheran

(1966) emphasized the importance of cleanliness in conducting technique. “Be sure that

everything you do has a purpose, and that it has a specific effect on the music. Otherwise

eliminate it. The players see your arms flapping around enough as it is, without having

the scene cluttered with meaningless frills” (p. 63).

As several researchers have worked to codify and categorize nonverbal

conducting gestures, it should be noted that gestures commonly taught in conducting

classes and used by conductors sometimes fail to convey intended meaning to

performers. This idea is found in the orchestra pedagogy book, Training the High School

Orchestra. “The motions necessary to communicate clearly with an orchestra are more

varied than the diagrams in conducting manuals would lead us to believe” (Rothrock,

1971, p. 178). In his dissertation, Sousa (1988) investigated the relationship between

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
5

conducting gesture and player response. His study sample included musicians ranging

from 7th grade to university graduate student. In his conclusion he states:

It can no longer be assumed that the conducting gestures commonly taught


in the college classroom and recommended in conducting textbooks have
universal interpretation by instrumental performers. Even the college
population did not recognize all of the gestures thought to be in common
usage. It should also be noted that only seven of the fifty-five
instrumental conducting gestures had 100% recognition by the college
students (p. 90).

Without understood meaning, the value of the communication that takes place between

conductor and ensemble is limited.

In research linking the practice of instrumental conducting with nonverbal

communication, few researchers have referred to primary literature of nonverbal

communication research. For instance, Berz (1983), Francisco (1994), Grechesky

(1985), and Sousa (1988), each suggested the need for research beyond their dissertations

combining the areas of conducting and nonverbal communication. Buell (1990) was

specific in his recommendation for future research in asking, “Are there elements of

gestural communication relating to expression in music which can and should be taught?”

(p. 164).

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
6

Nonverbal Communication

Nonverbal communication on the part of the conductor and reception by the

ensemble is the essence of conducting. A definitive element of conducting is the

nonverbal gesture. As a well-developed area of social science, the field of

communication is replete with information germane to this issue.

Ekman and Friesen (1972) describe emblems as nonverbal acts that have a direct

verbal translation. Emblems are used to convey specific messages to other people. A

linguistic translation of an emblem is usually a word or a short phrase. By definition, the

message of an emblem could be verbalized without significantly altering its meaning.

Emblems most often occur when verbal communication is prevented by external

circumstance (e.g., between pilot and landing crew), by distance (e.g., between hunters

spaced apart from each other in the field), by agreement (e.g., while playing charades), or

by organic impairment. Emblems can be either arbitrarily or iconically coded. This

means that one might use an emblem that looks something like the message it conveys

(e.g., Churchill’s “V for victory” sign), or meaning may be agreed upon for an emblem of

an arbitrary nature (e.g., a shrug of the shoulders for “I don’t know”). Emblems can

involve actions in any part of the body, although typically they involve the hands, head

orientation, facial muscular movement, and/or posture.

Emblems are developed primarily through enculturation. That is, the meaningful

translation of an emblem is understood primarily by members of a specific group or

culture (Johnson, Ekman & Friesen,1975). Thus, by definition, emblems require some

sort of explanation in order to be correctly interpreted by the recipient. This may explain

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
the conclusions of Sousa (1988) that student musicians correctly interpreted few

conducting gestures. Cofer (1998) suggests that research be pursued which examines the

effect of conducting-gesture instruction on student learning in a rehearsal situation. In

practice, this would meet the definition of emblem by creating meaningful nonverbal

gestures understood by both the sender and the recipient.

In addition to emblems, illustrators are movements or gestures that are directly

related on a moment-to-moment basis with the elements of speech, and they may

augment what is being said verbally (e.g. a gesture tracing a line of thought). Illustrators

are similar to emblems in that they are used with awareness and intent, although the use

of illustrators is usually in peripheral, not focal, awareness. Illustrators differ from

emblems in a number of ways. Many illustrators do not have as precise a verbal

definition as emblems. For some illustrators there is no obvious or agreed upon verbal

translation of the act. Also, illustrators do not occur without conversation, while

emblems typically occur when the communicants cannot or choose not to converse.

Illustrators help the speaker explain and the listener understand what is said.

They also serve a self-priming function, helping a speaker through hesitations in his or

her speech or thought, and accelerating the flow of ideas (Ekman & Friesen, 1972).

Illustrators are socially learned. It is hypothesized that children learn illustrators during

language acquisition. Their chief function is probably to get the attention of adults, and

to help the child with a minimal vocabulary explain concepts for which he or she does not

yet have words (Ekman & Friesen, 1972). While children use illustrators for attracting

attention, adults may use illustrators advantageously to teach children. A study involving

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
the educational use of illustrators (Goldin-Meadow, Kim & Singer, 1999) showed that

students learn better when instruction is accompanied by matching gestures than they do

with gesture-less instruction.

Another element of nonverbal behavior is that of facial expression. “Although

nonverbal acts such as facial displays are often acknowledged to have communicative

value, the information and process by which we use nonverbal acts (including facial

displays) to convey information has been considered distinct from that underlying spoken

language” (Chovil, 1991, p. 142). While researchers have demonstrated similarity in

interpretation of facial displays of emotion across literate cultures, some nonverbal

communication researchers would argue that facial expressions of emotion are

specifically learned within a given culture. There is, however, “unambiguous evidence of

universality ... for the expressions of happiness, anger, disgust, sadness, and combined

fear/surprise” (Fridlund, Ekman & Oster, 1987, p. 159).

Facial expressions of emotion can provide accurate information about the

occurrence and intensity of pleasant, or positive, as compared to unpleasant, or negative,

emotional states. Observers of facial expressions also can distinguish among several

specific negative and positive emotions (Ekman & Oster, 1982). In Motley’s research

(1993) he showed that receivers in conversational situations attribute the sender’s

emotions more on the basis of the emotion assumed to be resulting from the shared

context than on the sole basis of the sender’s facial expression. It follows then that

interpretation of facial expression in conversation is heavily dependent upon the verbal

context (Motley, 1993).

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
9

Music Teacher Education

In order to meet the needs of students in our schools pre-service teachers must be

taught skills specific to their particular cognate area. The typical musical experience of

middle and high school students in public schools is in the area of large ensemble

participation. A resulting expectation is that music educators will be skilled conductors.

The National Association of Schools of Music handbook recommends that, “The

prospective music teacher must be a competent conductor, able to create accurate and

musically expressive performances with various types of performing groups and in

general classroom situations” (p. 84). Areas of instruction specified by NASM include

score reading and analysis, performance practices, and baton technique. Teachers of pre­

service music educators in colleges and universities are faced with the task of imparting

these conducting skills within three to five semesters of instruction (Johnson,

Fredrickson, Achey & Gentry, 2003).

Differences in the development of conducting/teaching skills among music

teacher training programs results in a wide variety of conducting/teaching methods

implemented by teachers in the classroom. Some educators feel that the rehearsal of an

instrumental ensemble is best facilitated through frequent and consistent verbal

explanation (Byo & Austin, 1994). Other teachers believe that instrumental music

students learn best in a rehearsal situation involving nonverbal communication with

minimal discussion (Willard, 1986). Regardless of the method employed, clarity in

communication to the ensemble is the goal of the conductor’s technique.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
10

Students involved in a dynamic classroom environment encounter previously

unknown ideas every day. Nonverbal conducting technique is not enough to explain

every situation in a satisfactory manner. Verbal communication on the part of the teacher

and consequent interaction between the teacher and learners are necessary components to

a productive educational process. “To teach is to communicate. To teach effectively

requires that positive human relationships be established between the teacher and his

students. Possession of superior musicianship and knowledge of methodology is not

enough” (Kohut, 1973, p. 8).

Nonverbal behaviors are a part of the pedagogical repertoire in which teachers

should be trained. Hunnicutt (1999) demonstrated that pre-service teachers trained in the

use of selected nonverbal behaviors had fewer student disruptions in their classes than the

untrained control group had. Yarbrough (1975) found that student ensembles rehearsed

and performed better with high magnitude teacher behavior than with low magnitude

teacher behavior. High magnitude behavior includes such nonverbal characteristics as

demonstrative facial gestures, expressive use of emblems in conducting, and vocalic pitch

variance implying enthusiasm on the part of the teacher.

Further, Grechesky (1985) found that one of the conductor behaviors related to

more expressive musical performances was a combination of verbal explanation and

verbal imagery. “It was shown that some verbal explanation is necessary in rehearsal, but

that verbal imagery has a much stronger affect on ranking. The use of metaphor,

analogy, simile, and other imagery can help clarify the musical conception for the group”

(Grechesky, 1985, p. 151).

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
11

The identification and evaluation of effective teaching techniques may provide

useful information to those who prepare future teachers. Students are the primary

audience of both effective and ineffective teachers. In fact, research has demonstrated

that high school age students know the difference between effective and ineffective

teachers, at least by perception if not through an understanding of pedagogical technique

(Aubrecht, Hanna & Hoyt, 1986). This perception of effective teaching may provide a

means toward categorizing specific behaviors contributing to teaching effectiveness. “An

objective observer can provide perceptions that are informative and useful for instructors

and leaders who are caught up in their own subjective interpretations of day-to-day

activities” (Acheson & Gall, 1992, p. 47).

Educational researchers have codified many components of effective teaching.

Yuzdepski and Elliott (1985) collected 19 components of effective teaching which they

found to have significant research documentation including:

1) Plans, Organization, and Implementation


- Arrangements and procedures worked out in advance of instruction by
the teacher and the extent to which these are appropriate and used.
2) Academic Learning Time
- The time a student is engaged with instructional materials or activities
that allow a high level of success for that student.
3) Teacher’s Expectations
- The appropriateness of the teacher’s predictions of the individual’s or
group’s achievements in a particular content area.
4) Teacher’s Awareness
- Alertness to pupil behavior at all times; “withitness,” having “eyes in the
back of one’s head.”
5) Concurrent Tasks Management
- The efficiency and ease with which a teacher handles various classroom
activities at the same time.
6) Pace o f Instruction

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
12

- The rate at which learning experiences are provided for pupils by the
teacher.
7) Classroom Management
- The teacher’s facility to anticipate, prevent and deal satisfactorily with
pupils’ interpersonal and academic difficulties. Success in appropriately
disciplining pupils.
8) Feedback to Pupils
- Verbal and non-verbal responses to students.
9) Time on Task
- Engaged time; the time a student is attending to instruction in a
particular content area.
10) Enthusiasm
- Teacher’s display of eagerness and zeal.
11) Clarity of Instruction
- The communication of the teacher is such that the students are totally
informed about what they should be doing, where to do it, and for how
long, in order to ensure that students do not lose time because they are
confused or because they are waiting for decisions to be made.
12) Task Oriented / Businesslike Orientation
- The degree of academic focus and the teacher’s directness in providing
academically focused instruction.
13) Variability of Instruction
- The appropriate use of variety in providing learning experiences for
pupils.
14) Flexibility
- The ease with which the teacher adapts and adjusts satisfactorily to
varying factors in the classroom setting.
15) Understanding of Instructional Goals
- The pupils’ comprehension about what they should be doing, where to
do it, and for how long.
16) Teacher Centred / Direct Instruction
- An instructional strategy where time spent on instruction is sufficient
and continuous, content covered is extensive, goals are clear to students,
materials are structured and sequential, questions are low level to produce
many correct student responses, student progress is monitored, feedback is
immediate and correct responses are reinforced. With direct instruction,
the teacher sets the goals, plans the activities, chooses the material;
questions are convergent, not divergent; interaction is structured, not
authoritarian; tasks are cognitive oriented, not humanistic.
17) Praise and Encouragement / Criticism
- The appropriate use of positive (e.g., praise and encouragement) and
negative (e.g., criticism) reinforcement methods.
18) Individualization of Instruction

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
13

- Teaching adapted to the background, ability, and needs of individual


learners.
19) Questioning Techniques
- Matching the types of questions asked of the student to the
characteristics of the student and to the instructional objectives.

They were cautionary in commenting that, “The nineteen criteria [are not] intended to be

utilized as an exhaustive checklist for evaluating teacher performance. The intent is

solely to indicate ... those teaching behaviors which have been empirically shown to

correlate with student achievement” (p. 51).

Saunders and Worthington (1990) made several comments relevant to teacher

effectiveness in the music classroom. “Research findings concerning the academic

classroom are not easy to apply to the music performance classroom due to a number of

characteristics unique to the group-rehearsal setting” (Saunders & Worthington, 1990, p.

26). They identified strategies that may promote teaching success in the performance

classroom by comparing the role of teacher in rehearsal situations to that of the teacher in

an academic classroom. One of the primary factors in this comparison was

communication. “No matter how well a lesson is planned or how skillful the teacher

might be at making decisions, the link that transfers ideas from the teacher to the student

is communication. .. .Directors who can communicate their ideas quickly and efficiently

will make the best use of limited rehearsal time, allowing more time for actual

performance” (p. 27-28).

Good teachers tend to have successful music programs, while the quality of music

programs facilitated by poor teachers tends to be lower. In seeking an answer to the

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
question of the wide variety in quality of school music programs, Baker (1982) stated,

“The answer is simple. To a great extent it is the difference between good teaching and

incompetent teaching.... A music program has little chance of success if poor teaching is

not recognized and corrected” (p. 5-6). Baker also provided descriptions of several

teacher-evaluation methods. Her descriptions included evaluative judgments as to their

accuracy and appropriateness to the music classroom. These teacher-evaluation formats

included: traditional rating systems - subjective in nature, the observer must make

subjective judgments within limited criteria for evaluation; checklists - consisting of a

series of categorized behaviors to be checked; selective verbatim - word-for-word

transcript of classroom dialogue; interaction analysis - measuring the number and type of

teacher-student interactions occurring during a class period. Baker stated that, “A

detailed search of the literature, correspondence with individual school districts, and a

nationwide mail search to Departments of Education revealed that only a minimal amount

of research has been conducted in the field of music teacher evaluation” (p. 80).

In his research into teacher evaluation Berz (1983) found that, “Numerous

attempts have been made to obtain objective measurements of teacher behaviors in the

classroom. However, a great many of these systems have concentrated solely on verbal

behaviors” (p. 34). Brophy (1993), in a review of relevant research, proposed several

broad areas in which music teachers might be appropriately evaluated. One of these

areas was the effective use of nonverbal strategies.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
15

Need for the Study

An important factor bearing upon teacher preparedness in the classroom is the

acquisition of information by the pre-service teacher during the course of his or her

teacher education program. The purpose of any teacher education program is to provide

pre-service teachers with the information, skills, and experiences they need to become

successful classroom teachers. The more thoroughly prepared teachers are to educate

students in their classrooms, the more significant the results of those students’ learning

experiences will be.

In order to serve the needs of their constituency, educators of pre-service teachers

must be knowledgeable regarding the perceived effectiveness of the skills and methods

imparted to their students after these students become professional teachers. In this

endeavor, music educators provide the pre-service music teacher with information and

skills specific to the discipline of music.

Conducting, as it is taught to most music education students, includes the

development of specific gestures. These conducting gestures, being elements of

nonverbal communication, warrant some understanding of nonverbal communication.

Effective communication leads to effective teaching. Knowledge regarding effective use

of nonverbal components in conducting should result in effective music instruction. The

increase of knowledge in this area requires examination of the associated factors of

conducting, nonverbal communication, and pre-service music teacher preparation.

Teacher education programs intend to educate students by teaching them skills to

become effective teachers. Not all skills leading to effective teaching have been

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
16

identified. Research should be done to identify the effect of non-verbal symbols on

music teaching. Music teachers need information regarding teaching effectiveness which

can be used to make them better teachers. If the use of defined emblems in music

teaching situations increases the effectiveness of the classroom teacher then these skills

should be addressed in teacher training programs.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of this research was to examine and analyze the effect of specific

nonverbal behaviors (i.e., conducting emblems) on music education majors’ perceptions

of teaching effectiveness in the classroom environment of the instrumental music

ensemble (band) rehearsal. The nonverbal behavior of interest was the emblem,

specifically housed within the paradigm of conducting gestures. The current research

focused on a comparison of the presence or absence of defined emblems in

teaching/conducting situations.

College music education majors were engaged as subjects for this study for

several reasons. First, because of their background and experience as music students

themselves, music education majors have the ability to discriminate between good and

bad teaching (Aubrecht, Hanna & Hoyt, 1986; Murray, 1994). Second, they are being

trained to be teachers, therefore they should be thinking about what constitutes good

teaching. Third, they have experience in ensembles and should be able to identify and

evaluate conducting gestures (Sousa, 1988). Fourth, they may benefit directly from this

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
17

line of research. Finally, they may hold a personal interest in participating in this type of

study.

Variables

The independent variable in this research was the defined emblem as a specific

nonverbal behavior. This variable was manipulated at two levels: nonverbal exhibitor

and nonverbal inhibitor. The nonverbal exhibitor defined emblems as conducting

gestures. The nonverbal inhibitor used the same gestures as the nonverbal exhibitor, but

gave only verbal explanation without the concurrent gesture.

Ekman and Friesen (1972) described emblems as nonverbal acts that have a direct

verbal translation, which are used to convey specific messages to other people.

Meaningful translation of an emblem is understood primarily by members of a specific

group or culture (Johnson, Ekman & Friesen,1975). Because emblems by definition

require some sort of explanation in order to be correctly interpreted by the recipient the

lack of explanation negated the emblematic value.

The defined emblem is the key component of this study. Students may fail to

interpret conducting gestures correctly because their teacher/conductor did not define the

gesture adequately. If indeed this is the case the teacher may not be as effective as he/she

could be, and students may not be learning as well as they might.

A summary o f terms and definitions specific to this project appears later in this

chapter. The dependent variable is the numerical score derived from the use of the

Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure (described later).

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
Research Questions

This study was designed to answer the following research questions:

1. Do music education majors perceive a difference in overall teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

2. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to teaching

behaviors?

3. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to musical

competence?

4. Do areas of agreement exist between composite evaluations of the nonverbal

exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

In order to answer these research questions two rehearsal situations were scripted

by the researcher. The first situation included the use of a nonverbal exhibitor. For

example, the teacher/conductor demonstrated the emblem concurrently with a verbal

definition. The second rehearsal situation involved a nonverbal inhibitor. In this case the

teacher/conductor presented a verbal explanation without a corresponding gesture. An

adjudication instrument developed by the researcher for the purpose of assessing

perceived teaching effectiveness, which includes items related to teaching behaviors and

musical competence, was used in assessing each rehearsal situation.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
19

Music education students from four mid-western colleges and universities

comprised the adjudicator sample for this study. Participants received instruction for

using the adjudication instrument. These participants watched one of the video samples,

the distribution of which was randomized. Following the viewing of the video sample

participants completed the adjudication instrument. Upon completion of the adjudication

process, the participants received an explanation of the research.

Data were assessed using appropriate descriptive and inferential statistical

measures. The independent variable was the presence or absence of the defined emblem.

Significance levels were set atp < .05.

Definition of Terms

Nonverbal communication: “Those attributes or actions of humans, other than the use of

words themselves, which have socially shared meaning, are intentionally sent or

interpreted as intentional, are consciously sent or received, and have the potential

for feedback from the receiver” (Burgoon & Saine, 1978, p. 9-10).

Emblems: nonverbal gestures intended by the sender to communicate meaning, the

meaning of which has been translated verbally by the sender to the receiver

(Johnson, Ekman & Friesen, 1975).

Nonverbal Exhibitor model: predominantly exhibited defined emblems within the context

of the demonstration video.

Nonverbal Inhibitor model: predominantly inhibited the use of defined emblems within

the context of the demonstration video.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
20

Teaching effectiveness: a judgment based on a comparison between the observation of

two teaching samples. Although the classic interpretation of effective teaching

relies on a measurement of desired change in pupil behavior attributed to teacher

influence (Aubrecht, Hanna & Hoyt, 1986), for the purposes of this study

teaching effectiveness will be based on measurement of process rather than

product.

Limitations

One limitation of this study was the selection of convenient samples of

adjudicators. Random sampling of adjudicators was beyond the scope of this research

project. Because of this, it is understood that results of this research may not necessarily

be generalized to other situations. While the adjudicator sample may not be randomized,

it is representative of the described population. Generalization of results should be made

with caution.

A second limitation of this study is the definition of teaching effectiveness. True

teaching effectiveness could be, and oftentimes is, measured through student achievement

or behavioral outcomes as a result of the teaching technique. Because this study did not

focus on this particular way of measuring teacher effectiveness, conclusions with regard

to the effectiveness of teaching behavior is relegated to perceptions of collegiate music

education majors, which may or may not translate to student outcomes. Further research

which investigates the use of emblems in regard to student outcomes would be a logical

follow up to the present investigation.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
21

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE

This review of related literature addresses the topics of nonverbal communication,

nonverbal aspects of conducting, and issues related to teacher effectiveness. It is

pertinent to this current research project to demonstrate that students are able to recognize

a teacher’s use of appropriate nonverbal teaching gestures. Nonverbal gestures specific

to conducting have been identified. These gestures may be taught and learned by

conducting practitioners. In order for a conducting gesture to be an effective

communication emblem, the intended meaning conveyed by the conductor must be

understood similarly by the student. The issue of evaluating music teacher effectiveness

is complex, but it is possible to isolate the components of such given an appropriate

instrument. That is, students, working within certain given evaluative parameters, can

identify gradated differences in teaching effectiveness

Studies in Nonverbal Communication

Ekman and Friesen (1969) state that the relationship of the nonverbal act to verbal

behavior is an aspect of usage. This includes awareness, intentionality, and external

feedback. According to these researchers, interactive nonverbal behaviors are acts by one

person that influence or modify the behavior of another person or a group of persons.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
22

They also comment that many socially interactive nonverbal behaviors may be formally

taught or imitated. These ideas are significant to both teaching and conducting.

Ekman and Friesen (1972) define emblems as those nonverbal acts which have a

direct verbal translation usually consisting of a word or two, or a phrase, for which the

precise meaning is known by most or all members of a group, class, subculture or culture.

Emblems are most often deliberately used with the conscious intent to send a particular

message to other persons for which the persons who see the emblem usually not only

know the emblem’s message, but also know that it was deliberately sent to him or her.

Significant in this present research project is the premise that students are aware

of the presence or absence of nonverbal behaviors demonstrated by a teacher. A study

involving the educational use of illustrators (Goldin-Meadow, Kim & Singer, 1999)

showed that students learn better when instruction is accompanied by matching gestures

than they do with gesture-less instruction. Eight teachers were asked to instruct 49

children individually on mathematical equivalence as it applied to addition. Each of the

participating teachers used gesture to convey problem-solving strategies to the students.

Gestured strategies either reinforced or differed from strategies conveyed in the context

of instructional speech. These researchers found that students were more likely to

correctly interpret and demonstrate understanding of teacher speech when it was

accompanied by matching gestures than by no gestures at all. Students were less likely to

reiterate teacher instruction when it was accompanied by mismatching gestures than by

instruction accompanied by no gesture. Children were able to comprehend problem­

solving strategies from the teachers’ gestures and incorporate them into their own speech.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
23

This research demonstrates that students notice when teachers use gestures that express

task-relevant instructional information.

Hunnicutt (1999) published a study demonstrating the effective use of nonverbal

techniques in a teaching setting. This study, which included both experimental and

control groups of pre-service teachers in their senior year, was designed to provide

insight into the use of selected nonverbal behaviors as a behavior management tool in the

elementary and middle school setting. Participants in the experimental group were

trained in the use of selected nonverbal behaviors including dress, haptics, kinesics, and

proximity. The control group received no training. The researcher designed an

observation instrument and trained a group of pre-service teachers to use it. These

observers were unaware that any training had been provided to any of the participants.

Participants were observed three times. While no significant difference was reported,

Hunnicutt noted that the experimental group maintained a lower mean number of

disruptions throughout the three observations. This research helps support the idea that

teachers trained in the use of content specific nonverbal techniques may be more

effective classroom managers.

A study by Thompson (1997) investigated whether nonverbal communication

skill influenced opinion. This study examined the question of whether a person's level of

skill in nonverbal communication is linked to leadership effectiveness as defined by the

ability to influence opinion. Participants were 105 teachers and future teachers enrolled

in education courses in a large, southern university. The research treatment was a three-

step process. Subjects first completed a one-question opinionnaire to establish initial

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
24

opinion on a given topic. Subjects then were exposed to a videotaped presentation of the

opposite opinion. Finally, subjects were given the same one-question opinionnaire to

indicate any change in opinion because of treatment. Independent variables were high or

low levels of nonverbal skill as viewed on the videotapes. The dependent variables were

change or no change of opinion. Results suggested that there was no statistically

significant relationship between nonverbal communication skill and the ability to

influence teacher opinion.

Thompson described three major findings pertinent to this study. The first was

that high-level nonverbal communication might not be significantly related to an ability

to change opinions, especially strongly held opinions. Secondly, in a presentation, the

use of high-level nonverbal communication skill is likely to take a 25% longer length of

time than using low-level nonverbal communication skill for presenting the same

material. Finally, supporting the findings of related literature, Thompson indicated that

attitude shift seemed to be related to more than one variable, such as the topic, the

speaker, the setting, and other variables. A high level of nonverbal communication in

and of itself may not affect opinions.

Nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor models in this research project were

designed based on the work of Ekman and Friesen (1969, 1972) who described

interactive nonverbal behaviors as acts by one person that can modify the behavior of a

group. They defined emblems as nonverbal acts having simple but specific verbal

translations for which the precise meaning is understood by members of a group. This

project relied on the assumption that observers notice the use of nonverbal

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
25

communication. Goldin-Meadow, Kim & Singer, (1999) demonstrated that students

notice when teachers use gestures that express instructional information. The focus of

this project was a comparison of the use or non-use of the emblem. Opinions related to

other aspects of teaching were not sought. Thompson’s (1997) study shows that

nonverbal communication in and of itself may not affect opinions. Finally, with regard to

the potential value of incorporating effective nonverbal communication skills into

teaching behaviors, Hunnicutt’s (1999) research helps support the idea that teachers

trained in the use of content specific nonverbal techniques may be more effective

classroom managers.

Studies in Nonverbal Aspects of Conducting

Significant to this project is the connection between nonverbal behavior and

conducting. Ekman & Friesen (1969) described nonverbal behavior as having multiple

developments, conventional understandings, and uses. Ostling (1977) suggested that

aspects of conducting may be learned as acquired skills in nonverbal patterns of behavior.

The development of effective nonverbal behaviors may be an essential element in the

training of a teacher/conductor.

Berz (1983) conducted a study for the purpose of developing an instrument

designed to classify specific nonverbal conducting techniques. Rehearsals of 15

conductors representing a wide range of performance settings were videotaped. These

tapes were analyzed through repeated viewings, and detected behaviors were recorded on

cards. The cards then were grouped according to similarity of observed behavior and

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
26

compared with previously established behavior lists. A two-part instrument was

developed from these behavior groupings. The first part was a multi-category, time-

sampling system accounting for seven categories of non-static behavior: gaze, facial

expression, posture, proxemic (space usage), right hand, left hand, and vocalic

(communicative function of the voice separate from verbal meaning). The second part

was a checklist system accounting for eight categories of static behavior: type and size of

ensemble, room conditions, lighting conditions, physical background, podium factors

(usage, dimensions, construction, condition), music stand, dress and personal appearance,

baton (usage, length, color), and chronemic (time spent in performance activities/time

spent in nonperformance activites). It was concluded that the instrument developed was a

viable means to the classification of nonverbal conducting behaviors (Berz, 1983).

A 1994 study by Byo and Austin set out to devise and field test a method by

which nonverbal conducting behaviors could be documented as demonstrated in a

rehearsal setting. They also wanted to compare the nonverbal repertoire of novice

conductors (n = 6) to that of accomplished university band conductors (n = 6). Novices

were pre-service teachers in instrumental music. Experts were experienced conductors

who were nationally recognized for consistently outstanding levels of performance.

A nonverbal script consisting of four categories - right arm/hand gestures, eye

contact, facial expression, and body movement - was developed from videotape

recordings of 15-minute rehearsal segments for each expert and novice conductor. The

script format provided a means of recording chronological sequences of nonverbal

behaviors and time spent in each behavior. Gestures of the left arm/hand and cuing were

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
27

documented by counting occurrences in some cases and obtaining cumulative timings in

others.

Data in this research describe the nonverbal behaviors exhibited by conductors

and suggest no one standard of excellence. As was evidenced by the large standard

deviations, the expert conductors when compared to each other were as dissimilar as they

were similar in their nonverbal behavior. Despite this nonverbal variety, the experts as a

group were significantly different from the novices in several categories. Results served

to expose general nonverbal characteristics that separate experts from novices and

suggest aspects of a general sort of standards which novices might be encouraged to

pursue. Additionally, the act of scripting may function as an alternative means of

inducing beginning conductors to see more clearly what they do on the podium.

Hausmann (1984) produced a similar study of coded gestural analysis that took

into account and controlled the music used in the study. The purpose of this study was to

develop a procedural model for the choral conductor that presented in sequential fashion

the various components contributing to choral artistry. Results of this study offered

solutions for transferring analytical information into verbal and nonverbal

communication.

With his study Hausmann provided a systematic plan for score study by posing

two questions related to transference. First, how will the musical style affect your

conducting gestures? Secondly, what rehearsal and performance procedures can be used

to reinforce the unique style characteristics of the music? The author went on to present a

coded gestural analysis system that isolated and defined gestural possibilities. This

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
28

gestural shorthand system was to be used to select and record gestures that were

appropriate to the stylistic dictates of the score. In addition, a rehearsal sequencing

model was presented which was based on the ideal that the rehearsal of choral

components should relate directly to the musical characteristics of the score in order to

have an ensemble perform with appropriate technique and style. Hausmann concluded

that the procedural model presented in this study could provide pedagogical tools for the

training of choral conductors. This procedural model might become an effective and

comprehensive approach to the problem of transferring analytical insights to a chorus.

Finally, Hausmann thought that this model could provide a systematic and

comprehensive design for the study of choral music.

Cofer (1998) investigated the effects of short-term conducting gesture instruction

on seventh-grade band students' recognition of and performance response to conducting

gestures. It also examined the proportion of students successfully recognizing gestures to

determine which gestures could be considered musical conducting emblems. Finally, it

investigated the influence of instruction and test format on gestures achieving musical

conducting emblem status. Subjects of Cofer’s study were 60 seventh-grade wind

instrumentalists from a state university town. Treatment group subjects (n = 30) received

instruction designed to improve their recognition and response to common conducting

gestures for five consecutive days. The control group (n —30) participated in a warm-up

designed to review concepts of musical expression without the use of conducting

gestures. Two dependent measures of conducting gesture recognition were employed in

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
29

this study: (1) a multiple-choice paper-and-pencil measure and (2) an individual musical

performance measure.

An independent /-test revealed statistically significant differences in favor of the

treatment group for the multiple-choice paper-and-pencil test (p < .001). Additional

analysis by MANOVA revealed statistically significant differences in favor of the

treatment group for the individual musical performance test (p < .001). The two

dependent measures were compared to determine whether they measured conducting

gesture recognition. Results indicated that a high, positive relationship existed between

the two measures (r = .82). Test comparability also was analyzed. Results of this

analysis showed that the treatment group performed as well on the multiple-choice test as

the performance test. Sixteen gestures reached musical conducting emblem status for the

treatment group on both dependent measures. While eleven gestures reached musical

conducting emblem status for the control group on the multiple- choice paper-and-pencil

test, only three reached this status on the performance test. Results of this study indicated

that short-term conducting gesture instruction wa’s effective in improving the recognition

and performance of musical conducting emblems for seventh-grade band students.

A study by Dickey (1988) compared verbal and nonverbal instruction techniques.

This study concerned the comparative effects of verbal instruction and modeling

instruction on instructional effectiveness in instrumental music classrooms. The primary

hypothesis maintained that modeling was more effective than verbal instruction. Four

middle school band classes, two taught for ten weeks with verbal instruction and two

with modeling instruction, were pre- and post-tested for ear-to-hand skills, kinesthetic

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
30

response skills, and music discrimination skills. Videotapes of classes were analyzed to

compare the efficiency of the two methods and to verify the absence of researcher and

teacher bias. The two classes receiving modeling instruction demonstrated significantly

greater ear-to-hand skills and kinesthetic response skills, although not general music

discrimination skills, compared to the two classes receiving verbal instruction. These

findings suggest that the use of modeling strategies and devices such as melodic echoes

and rhythmic movement to music could lead to increased ear-to-hand skills and

kinesthetic response skills. Dickey believed this finding implied that modeling strategies

and devices should play a more prominent role in instrumental music pedagogy and

should be addressed in pre- and inservice instrumental music teacher training.

Mayne (1992) went so far as to isolate facial expression as nonverbal gesture in

conducting. The purpose of this study was to investigate the use of facial expression by

the conductor on the performer's ability to interpret common musical conducting

gestures. Instrumental performers compared their interpretation of conducting gestures

without facial expression, to the same gestures with facial expression. The author

examined three specific questions: Does the use of facial expression by conductors affect

the ability of instrumental performers to interpret musical conducting gestures? Does the

interpretation of conducting gestures across three grade/experience levels vary between

gestures without facial expression to the same gestures with facial expression? And, are

specific conducting gestures affected more than others by the use of facial expression?

Two videotapes were used for administration of this study. Both tapes used the

same conductor demonstrating the same 53 gestures in matching order. The only

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
31

difference between the two tapes was the conductor's added use of facial expression on

Tape 2. Junior high, high school, and college instrumentalists were randomly assigned to

view Tape 1 or Tape 2. Using a multiple-choice paper and pencil instrument, subjects

were asked to select the answer which best described each conducting gesture example.

Results indicated that with all grade levels combined, or when comparing like grade

levels, the use of facial expression did not significantly increase the instrumental

performer's ability to interpret the 53 common conducting gestures used in the study.

The author emphasized that a generalization should not be made that facial expression

does not affect the art of conducting, since conducting involves much more than the 53

isolated conducting gestures used in the study. The study produced results that indicated

significant differences resulted when different grade levels were compared against each

other (junior high to high school to college). With and without facial expression, the

percentage of correct responses increased as grade level increased, suggesting the need to

continue to teach specific conducting gestures and to visually sensitize the instrumental

performers to these common conducting gestures.

Sousa (1988) investigated the relationship between conducting gesture and player

response. Sousa’s review of the literature in the area of instrumental conducting revealed

that instrumental conductors communicate specific musical ideas nonverbally through the

use of a specific and common gestural system. The purpose of this study was to

investigate the common nonverbal gestures taught and used by instrumental conductors,

and the interpretation of these gestures by instrumental performers. In order to realize

this purpose, three procedural steps were followed. A list of 55 specific nonverbal

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
32

gestures commonly used by instrumental conductors to communicate musical concepts

was established. A videotape was prepared utilizing one conductor demonstrating,

without sound, each of the 55 gestures. The videotape was shown to a sample of junior

high school, high school, and college instrumental performers to measure the

effectiveness of the gestures to communicate specific musical ideas.

Nineteen commonly used instrumental conducting gestures were successfully

identified by the total population of instrumental performers as well as for each of the

individual population groups. The total population of instrumental performers

successfully identified nineteen additional gestures. Two gestures were not recognized

by the high school students. Nineteen were not recognized by the junior high school

group. Seventeen commonly used conducting gestures were not successfully recognized

by the total population.

Significant differences were found among the groups in their abilities to

understand the gestures that are commonly used by instrumental conductors. A

significant increase in the ability to recognize these gestures appeared to have taken place

between the junior high school level of experience and high school, as well as between

the high school and college level. Each of these increases was accompanied by decreases

in the variability of the scores. According to Sousa, ensemble experience not only had a

significant effect on a student's ability to recognize these gestures, but also on a group's

homogeneity in terms of understanding nonverbal conducting gestures.

Grechesky (1985) found that one of the conductor behaviors related to more

musical performances was a combination of verbal explanation and verbal imagery. This

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
33

study examined the conducting behaviors exhibited by the conductors of randomly

selected high school bands in central Indiana. Verbal and nonverbal behaviors were

observed, categorized, and analyzed to determine how they affected band performance.

An expert panel of judges evaluated audiotapes that were musically representative of

each band. The bands with the highest scores were designated as “musical” and those

with the lowest scores were designated as “less musical.” The eleven bands in the sample

were videotaped in a rehearsal and performance of the first and second movements of

Brevities by Robert Keyes Clark. Each band had the same amount of rehearsal and

performance time and had not seen the music prior to the experimental session.

Additionally, the same panel of judges evaluated audiotapes made of the performance

portion for each band.

Conducting behaviors were observed and coded from the videotapes. These

observed behaviors were the independent variables; rank order as a determinant of

performance quality was the dependent variable. Through correlation and regression

analysis, eleven variables were identified as having an effect on rank. Grechesky (1985)

indicated six findings. First was that some verbal explanation was necessary in rehearsal,

but verbal imagery had a much stronger impact on ranking. Second was that minimal

time should be spent on talk or instructions about nonmusical matters. The third was that

the conductors of the more musical groups displayed significantly more body movement.

Fourth, approving facial expressions had a positive effect on performance, whereas

disapproving facial expressions had a negative effect. Fifth, conductors who

demonstrated more use of the left hand, and coordination of right and left hands had

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
34

better results. And sixth, emblems and illustrators had the most powerful effect of any of

the variables. Grechesky felt that the findings indicated that conductors who sharpened

their nonverbal communication skills could have a positive effect on their groups'

musical performance.

A study of verbal behaviors of teacher/conductors (Carpenter, 1986) contained

evidence that the quality and frequency of initiating behaviors and feedback behaviors

had an effect on the quality of band rehearsals. The purpose of the study was to describe

both qualitative and quantitative aspects of junior high and senior high band teacher-

conductors' behaviors, and to determine if there were specific factors in the verbal

behaviors of teacher-conductors that are predictive of overall ratings of rehearsals. Data

were collected through the use of two researcher-constructed forms. A panel of experts

used the first form to qualitatively rate teacher/conductor personal qualities,

procedures/organization, pedagogy, and error detection skills during total rehearsals. The

second form was used by two other judges to categorize specific verbal behaviors of

teachers, including specific or general approval and disapproval feedback to social and

musical behavior, musical element attended to, and techniques used to initiate behavior.

Results indicated that teacher/conductors were more disapproving than approving and

more likely to attend to musical behavior than social behavior. Feedback behaviors were

better predictors o f overall rehearsal ratings than initiating behaviors. The frequency of

attending to various musical elements was not predictive of a highly rated rehearsal,

although the type of musical element was related to ratings. Differences between junior

and senior high rehearsals were evident throughout the study.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
The purpose of a study by Francisco (1994) was to determine the relative effects

of verbal communication, visual communication, and modeling, used individually and in

combinations, on performance improvement of high school bands. Twenty-five

conductors and bands at 15 summer music camps were audio- and videotaped in

rehearsal. Following the rehearsals, examples of conductor communication behavior

were identified and categorized from the videotapes by expert judges. The corresponding

pre-communication and post-communication performances were then identified from the

audio tapes and edited to a set of evaluation tapes in a series of paired performances, with

the pre-communication performance presented first. Expert judges reviewed the

evaluation audiotapes. Using the pre-communication performance as a reference, the

judges rated each post-communication performance as (a) no improvement, (b)

improvement, (c) inaudible. All audible examples were subjected to statistical analysis

except those in the categories of modeling and visual/modeling, which had occurred with

insufficient frequency to evaluate. Francisco used a series of multiple regression

analyses on ensemble improvement scores as the dependent variable to determine the

relative effects of the various types of communication on performance improvement.

Other issues addressed included the relative difficulty of correcting performance

problems in the categories of tone, intonation, rhythm, technique, interpretation, and

balance, and the significance of judge differences and interaction between judges and

types of communication.

According to Francisco, results of the study demonstrated that the type and

purpose of communication had a significant effect upon ensemble improvement scores.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
36

Visual communication, when used alone, was significantly less likely to improve

ensemble performance than verbal/modeling/visual, verbal communication, and

verbal/modeling. The combination of verbal/modeling/visual was significantly more

likely to improve ensemble performance than visual communication and verbal/visual.

Combinations of two types of communication were not necessarily more likely to

improve ensemble performance than was the use of either of the component types of

communication by itself.

Researchers concluded that effective use of nonverbal communication by

conductors improved classroom achievement (Carpenter, 1986) and ensemble

performance (Cofer, 1998; Francisco, 1994; Grechesky, 1985). Researchers also

concluded that these behaviors were identifiable and teachable (Austin, 1994; Berz, 1983;

Byo & Dickey, 1988; Hausmann, 1984). These ideas along with Sousa’s (1988) finding

that many commonly used conducting gestures were not recognized correctly by middle

school, high school or college instrumental music students and Mayne’s (1992) research

suggesting the need to visually sensitize instrumental music students to specific

conducting gestures led to the formulation of this project.

Issues of Teaching Effectiveness

As may be deduced from preceding material, elements of nonverbal

communication along with aspects of conducting are issues pertinent to music teacher

effectiveness. The issue o f evaluating music teacher effectiveness is in itself a complex

issue.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
37

A study by Buell (1990) identified and examined factors contributing to success

in effective teaching/conducting practice. The investigation focused on the symphonic

band and conducted wind ensemble settings. Research questions centered on three broad

theoretical areas: (a) the role of conducting, gesture, and verbal and nonverbal

communication in effective teaching/conducting; (b) teaching to the simultaneous goals

of conceptual understanding, technical development, and quality performance; (c) the

development of student understanding, retention, and transfer of learning in the

instrumental ensemble experience. The study examined the relationship between

successful teaching/conducting practice and elements of selected educational theory.

Data from analysis of videotaped instruction, subject interviews, ensemble-member

interviews, and researcher observations were triangulated to corroborate factors that

appeared to contribute to the subject's teaching/conducting effectiveness. The purpose of

the subject interviews was to determine why the instruction took the form that it did and

how the subject's background and beliefs guided observed instructional behaviors. The

purpose of the student interviews was to determine if, and to what degree, instruction was

efficient. The nature of the student's learning, and its transferability, were also examined.

Effective instruction in this study was characterized by: (a) a varied, creative use of

speaking, singing, and movement in different sensory-modal combinations; (b) a linking

of specific teaching strategies to specific instructional goals; (c) an abundant use of multi­

modal instruction.

Multi-modal instruction was a factor in generating instructional efficiency.

Students learned quickly, retained what they had learned, and were able to transfer their

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
38

learning to other musical settings. Buell concluded that effectiveness in

teaching/conducting was not the result of any single factor such as exceptional

musicianship, conducting technique, or personality. It involved the establishment of

positive learning environments, the appropriate linking of teaching strategies to

instructional goals, and the use of instructional behavior sympathetic to individual

differences in students. Continuous development of personal musicianship and thorough

score-study were indicated to be requisites of effectiveness in teaching/conducting.

In a study by Aubrecht, Hanna and Hoyt (1986) two classes of each of 400 high

school teachers were rated for teaching effectiveness by the teachers and by their students

using the same instrument. This instrument, the IDEA Form H from the University of

Kansas Center for Faculty Evaluation and Development, was developed to help high

school faculty improve their instructional effectiveness. This instrument is comprised of

nine general instructional goal items and 24 teacher behavior items, all rated with a five-

point Likert-type response. Separate factor analyses yielded highly similar underlying

dimensions. Convergent validity coefficients were significant for all factors. These

findings provide support for the validity of high school student ratings of instruction.

Murray (1994) reviewed research on whether or not student evaluations could

provide reliable and valid information on the quality of college teaching. He concluded

that both validity and reliability could be assumed. Murray demonstrated that student

ratings were found to show acceptable levels of intra-tester and inter-tester reliability. He

also found a moderate positive correlation between student ratings of teaching and

objective measures o f student achievement.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
39

A study by Baker (1981) identified the music teaching competencies

characteristic of effective music teaching. In this study five major processes were used to

construct a music teacher evaluation survey. These included evaluating responses to a

questionnaire sent to 119 music educators and general administrators, the synthesis of

689 recommended competencies into 60 major items, deriving the 30 most crucial

competencies to be used on each area-specific form, constructing the pilot test form, and

revising the form. Analysis of data from the survey indicated that participants considered

six criterion categories as essential to the identification of important music teacher

competencies: instructional skills, interest in work and in pupils, classroom management,

musical scholarship and musicianship, personality qualities, and quality of concert

performances. This survey also showed that music teachers as a whole consider the most

crucial competencies to be: enthusiasm for teaching coupled with caring for students,

maintaining strong yet fair discipline, and observing student enjoyment and interest in

music. The conclusions of this research were that the evaluation of music teaching

should be regarded as different and separate from teacher assessment in general. General

teaching forms do not sufficiently reflect the specific teaching behaviors, characteristics,

and qualities essential to excellence in music teaching.

Brophy’s (1993) research questioned whether it is appropriate to evaluate music

educators exclusively on general competencies, or whether effective music teaching

involves certain teaching behaviors, characteristics, and attributes that are so significantly

unique that they demand their own set of evaluative criteria. Brophy reviewed selected

relevant research from 1989 to 1993, discussed pertinent practical issues involved in the

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
40

evaluation of public school music teachers, and synthesized eight general areas for

further development as potential criteria for music teacher evaluation, including personal

characteristics, musical competence, and the effective use of nonverbal strategies.

Research conducted by Baker (1981), Brophy (1993), and Buell (1990)

demonstrated some of the difficulties in measuring and evaluating effective teaching. As

Buell (1990) concluded that teaching effectiveness was not the result of any single factor,

this project examines only one aspect of music teaching. The current research separated

components of musical competence and teaching behaviors as suggested by Baker (1981)

and Brophy (1993). Aubrecht, Hanna and Hoyt (1986) and Murray (1994) surmised the

ability of students to reliably measure behaviors related to teaching effectiveness, which

was a factor in this project.

Restatement of Purpose

The purpose of this research was to examine and analyze the effect of specific

nonverbal behaviors (i.e., conducting emblems) on music education majors’ perceptions

of teaching effectiveness in the classroom environment of the instrumental music

ensemble (band) rehearsal. The nonverbal behavior of interest was the emblem,

specifically housed within the paradigm of conducting gestures. The current research

focused on a comparison of the presence or absence of defined emblems in

teaching/conducting situations. Specific research questions were as follows:

1. Do music education majors perceive a difference in overall teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
41

2. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to teaching

behaviors?

3. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to musical

competence?

4. Do areas of agreement exist between composite evaluations of the nonverbal

exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
42

CHAPTER III

METHODOLOGY

Purpose

The purpose of this research was to examine and analyze the effect o f specific

nonverbal behaviors (i.e., conducting emblems) on music education majors’ perceptions

of teaching effectiveness in the classroom environment of the instrumental music

ensemble (band) rehearsal. The nonverbal behavior of interest was the emblem,

specifically housed within the paradigm of conducting gestures. The current research

focused on a comparison of the presence or absence of defined emblems in

teaching/conducting situations. Specific research questions were as follows:

1. Do music education majors perceive a difference in overall teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

2. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to teaching

behaviors?

3. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to musical

competence?

4. Do areas of agreement exist between composite evaluations of the nonverbal

exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
43

Issues of the selection of subjects, the construction of a measurement instrument, the

development of a treatment device, and assessment of the data will be discussed below.

Development of the Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure

The instrument for assessing subjects’ perception of conductor/teacher

effectiveness was the Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure (PTEM). This

instrument, designed by the researcher, was an 18-item Likert-type response assessment

survey. The researcher chose to use six levels of response to avoid the neutral response

of “no difference” or “no opinion.” Format recommendation for this instrument was

taken from Patten (1998). As per the recommendations for assessment items, item

concepts for PTEM were modified from various sources including Acheson and Gall

(1992), Aubrecht, Hanna and Hoyt (1986), Baker (1981), Brophy (1993), and Yuzdepski

and Elliott (1985). Although Patten (1988) recommended an instrument containing 25

items, the two most frequently used item counts were 10 and 15 (Acheson & Gall, 1992;

Aubrecht, Hanna & Hoyt, 1986; Baker, 1981; Brophy, 1993; and Yuzdepski & Elliott,

1985).

Items used by the researcher in the construction of this instrument were written to

reflect two broad areas of assessment: teaching behaviors and musical competence.

Statements related to these areas follow:

Musical Competence:
This teacher/conductor seemed musically creative.
This teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
44

This teacher/conductor was skillful in communicating musical ideas.


This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble to play expressively.
This teacher/conductor was helpful in guiding students to develop musical skills
for future learning.
This teacher/conductor encouraged the band to play with good technique.
This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play with a good sound.
This teacher/conductor was knowledgeable about music.

Teaching Behaviors:
This teacher/conductor used a variety of teaching methods.
This teacher/conductor cared about student learning.
This teacher/conductor had good teaching skills.
This teacher/conductor gave clear instructions.
This teacher/conductor had a firm grasp of the material he/she presented.
This teacher/conductor provided students with appropriate feedback.
This teacher/conductor maintained good eye contact with the students.
This teacher/conductor communicated information clearly to the students.
This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play better.
This teacher/conductor made certain that students understood his/her presentation
of the material.

Content validity was established through the examination of previous researchers’

teaching assessment instruments. In the construction of an instrument for the evaluation

of instrumental music teachers, Baker (1981) used items reflecting teacher behaviors and

musical competence. Similarly, Brophy (1993) discussed the use of broad areas of music

teacher evaluation including personal characteristics and musical competence. Brophy

provided no actual evaluation instrument, just criteria for the development of such an

instrument. Aubrecht, Hanna and Hoyt (1986) used an instrument that included student

self-evaluation items in instructional effectiveness as well as teacher behavior items.

Yuzdepski and Elliott (1985) described nineteen areas of assessment including clarity of

instruction, feedback to pupils, concurrent tasks management, and other areas related to

teacher behaviors and subject competence.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
In order to establish the validity of the instrument, three university professors in

music education and conducting critiqued these statements. Revisions as per their

recommendations included clarification of wording and broadening of items evaluated.

Redundant items were removed and replaced with distinct items. Some examples

include: one item changed from “This conductor seemed creative” to “This

teacher/conductor seemed musically creative;” another changed from “This conductor

was not enthusiastic” to “This teacher/conductor gave clear instructions;” and a third

changed from “This teacher was helpful in guiding students to develop skills for future

learning” to “This teacher/conductor made certain that students understood his/her

presentation of the material.”

After revising items according to expert feedback, another version of the

instrument was evaluated by experts. Being satisfied with the content of the instrument, a

pilot test was conducted to ascertain the reliability of the PTEM. The PTEM was

administered to eight high school instrumental music teachers; four viewed each

treatment type. Cronbach’s alpha, which measures how well a set of items measures a

single unidimensional construct, indicated that the instrument was sufficiently reliable (a

= .90).

Treatment Videos

The development of the treatment videos for this study involved scripting two

rehearsal situations. The first script contained a majority of nonverbal exhibitors, while

the second script contained a majority of nonverbal inhibitors. Because “pure” scripts

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
46

isolating one type of emblem over another would appear contrived in video

representations, it was decided that each example would be composed primarily of

nonverbal exhibitors or nonverbal inhibitors, but neither video would be comprised

exclusively o f one or the other.

The musical selection chosen for this project was Grainger’s Ye Banks and Braes

O ’Bonnie Doon. The researcher chose this piece because of its lack of technical

difficulties for young players and its short duration (approximately two minutes). Each

teaching episode lasted approximately eight minutes. The researcher chose to script ten

items for comment in each piece because this number was manageable and reasonable

within the context of a rehearsal situation.

Script 1 was labeled as Nonverbal Exhibitor. In this situation the

teacher/conductor defined 80% of the emblems used as conducting gestures. The

emblems were defined by verbal description of the desired musical responses with

simultaneous demonstration of the nonverbal gestures. For example, prior to beginning

the piece the conductor described the meter verbally while demonstrating the motion of

the conducting pattern. Gestures in bold were demonstrated concurrently with verbal

description.

BEGINNING: “Ye Banks and Braes in 6/8—the pattern looks like this.”
(Conductor shows a 6 pattern.)

“Stretch the first note.”


(Conductor shows a lifting gesture with the baton following.)

MEASURE 3: “Place a slight accent in the third bar on the fourth beat.”

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
47

(Conductor shows a cross-motion accent appropriate to beat 4.)

MEASURE 12: “In measure 12 we need to crescendo.”


(Conductor enlarges pattern by pulling hands apart.)

MEASURE 13: “In measure 13 we need to decrescendo.”


(Conductor decreases pattern by bringing hands together.)

MEASURE 17: “In measure 17, if you have a tie make sure you’re making the
connection.”

MEASURE 18: “At measure 18, listen for balance between the melody and
the descending eighth-notes; we need to hear the melody.”

MEASURE 25: “In measure 25, put a slight break between beats five
and six; everyone take a breath there.”
(Conductor emphasizes the luftpause at the appropriate place.)

MEASURES 30: “In measure 30, we need to hear the accents on the eighth-notes.”
(Conductor shows emphasis for each accented beat.)

MEASURE 33: “On the final release, the sound should evaporate with the tuba
being the last sound heard.”
(The conductor gradually closes the fingers on his left hand.)

Script 2 was labeled as Nonverbal Inhibitor. In this situation the

teacher/conductor did not define most emblems. In 80% of the cases, only a verbal

description of the desired musical response was presented by the teacher/conductor to the

students. Gestures in bold were demonstrated concurrently with verbal descriptions.

BEGINNING: “Ye Banks and Braes in 6.”

“Stretch the first note.”


(Conductor shows a lifting gesture with the baton following.)

MEASURE 3: “In the third bar, put a slight accent on the sixteenth-note on the
fourth beat.”

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
48

MEASURE 12: “In measure 12, make sure we hear a crescendo.”

MEASURE 13: “Then decrescendo in measure 13.”

MEASURE 17: “If you have a tie in measure 17, be sure you’re connecting the
notes.”

MEASURE 18: “At measure 18, listen for balance between the melody and the
descending eighth-notes; we need to hear the melody.”

MEASURE 25: “In measure 25, put a slight break between beats five and six;
everyone take a breath there.”
(Conductor emphasizes the luftpause at the appropriate place.)

MEASURES 30: “In measure 30, make more out of the accented eighth-notes—
make them lead to the dotted quarter in the second half of the bar.’

MEASURE 33: “On the final release, watch. Pick a finger for your release,
with the tuba being last.”
(The conductor gradually closes the fingers on his left hand.)

The researcher acted as teacher/conductor model in the scripted situations. A

high school ensemble was convened and rehearsed in the musical selection. The

presentation of each script was recorded on videotape, with the help of a music teaching

colleague, using a portable VHS camcorder (Magnavox CVK332AV01). The video

camera was aimed so as to capture the face and upper body of the conductor. None of the

high school instrumentalists were identifiable on the demonstration videos. Each

teaching episode lasted approximately eight minutes.

To control for any possible effects owed to ensemble performance, the ensemble

was instructed to play consistently regardless of the gestures given in recording the test

videos. Several rehearsals were given to practicing consistency in performance

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
49

regardless of gesture. Because both video samples used in administration of the

measuring instrument were recorded during the same rehearsal period, samples were

similar. The video camera operator and another music educator verified similarity of

performance samples. Furthermore, college students evaluating the video samples saw

only one version; they could not compare the two different versions before responding to

the evaluation instrument.

Participants

Participants (N= 101) in this study were a volunteer sample of instrumental music

education majors from four Midwestern university undergraduate music programs.

Schools from which subjects volunteered to participate were selected because of the

willingness of the respective instructors to participate in this research. Two of the

schools were medium-sized state universities, and two were small private colleges.

Collection of Data

For each college or university instructor who indicated a willingness to

participate, the researcher assembled a package of materials including measurement

instruments (PTEMs), a videotape, and instructions for the proctor. Measurement

instruments were color coded to correspond with the two different video samples. This

color-coding scheme served the need of the researcher and had no bearing on the

subjects.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
50

At the beginning of the experimental session, each subject received a blank copy

of the Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure (PTEM) [Appendix A], Following

distribution of the PTEM, the proctor read aloud the following instructions to

participants:

In music education, as in any academic or scientific field, a process of


experimental research improves practice. This study is part of a research
project intended to improve instrumental music teacher preparation. If
you choose to participate in this project you will be asked to view a
videotape showing a portion of a band rehearsal. Following the viewing
of this rehearsal sample, you will be asked to complete a questionnaire
related to the video example. Throughout the process your anonymity will
be maintained. Your participation in this study is completely voluntary
and not connected with your grade in any way. You may choose not to
participate or discontinue participation at any time without penalty or
prejudice.

(Allow time for any students not wishing to participate to leave at this
time.)

Thank you for agreeing to participate in this research project. Your


participation in this study poses no risks to your safety beyond those
normally encountered in everyday life. Your privacy is protected by
anonymity. No identifying information or marks on the response form are
necessary. All individual responses will be held in confidence by the
researcher.

The purpose of this research is to explore possibilities for the


improvement of instrumental music teacher training. Please read through
the Perception o f Teaching Effectiveness Measure (PTEM) so you are
familiar with its contents. (Allow a moment for perusal.) After you have
watched the videotape, please complete the PTEM by circling the
numbered response closest to your position regarding each statement.

(At this time show the video sample in its entirety. At its conclusion,
please repeat the following instruction:)

Please complete the PTEM by circling the numbered response closest to


your position regarding each statement.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
51

Upon completion of the verbal instructions, subjects viewed the treatment

videotape. Each teaching episode contained a rehearsal of Percy Grainger’s Ye Banks

and Braes O ’ Bonnie Doon utilizing one of the emblem schemes described above. After

viewing the video sample, the students completed the PTEM.

Once all subjects completed their questionnaires, the researcher provided them

with a more specific explanation of the research project by reading the following:

Thank you for completing the assessment measure. The specific focus of
this research is to determine the significance of emblem definition in
nonverbal rehearsal communication. An emblem is a nonverbal gesture
that holds identical meaning for the sender as well as the receiver. If a
conductor uses a gesture in a rehearsal, he or she assumes that the players
understand the meaning of the gesture.

If your PTEM sheets were blue, you observed a majority of gestures


simultaneously demonstrated and defined by the teacher/conductor. If
your PTEM sheets were yellow, a majority of the gestures you observed
were undefined.

The researcher intends to compare the results to determine if observers


perceive a significant difference in the effectiveness of one method over
the other.

If you have any questions about this research project, please address them
to your instructor who will relay them to the researcher.

Once again, thank you for your participation.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
Scoring of Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure

As research materials packages were returned, the researcher examined PTEMs

for accurate completion. Of 101 completed instruments returned, four were discarded as

unusable because respondents circled multiple Likert numbers for individual response

items (e.g., the subject drew an ellipse around the three and the four in response to a

single statement). Remaining instruments (N = 97) were numbered and responses were

entered into a statistics program for analysis.

Analysis of Data

The main variable of interest in this study was the construct of nonverbal

emblems used in conducting/teaching. This variable was comprised of two levels. In

Level 1 (nonverbal exhibitor) the majority of the emblems exhibited by the

teacher/conductor were defined. In Level 2 (nonverbal inhibitor) the majority of the

emblems used by the teacher/conductor were undefined. The dependent variables were

means of scores collected from the measurement instruments. Because the intent was to

determine whether means of two groups were statistically different from each other a t-

test was used. The a priori alpha level for this study was set at .05. Results of the study,

including descriptive and inferential statistics are presented in Chapter IV.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
53

CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

The purpose of this research was to examine and analyze the effect of specific

nonverbal behaviors (i.e., conducting emblems) on music education majors’ perceptions

of teaching effectiveness in the classroom environment of the instrumental music

ensemble (band) rehearsal. The nonverbal behavior of interest was the emblem,

specifically housed within the paradigm of conducting gestures. The current research

focused on a comparison of the presence or absence of defined emblems in

teaching/conducting situations.

Collegiate level subjects from four universities observed one of two different

videotaped teaching episodes. In one episode the teacher used a preponderance of

nonverbal exhibitors, while in the other episode, the teacher used a preponderance of

nonverbal inhibitors. Subjects (music education majors) were asked to rate the

effectiveness of the teacher using the Perception of Teacher Effectiveness Measure

(PTEM) developed by the researcher.

As research materials packages were returned, the researcher examined PTEMs

for accurate completion. O f 101 completed instruments returned, four were discarded as

unusable because respondents circled multiple Likert numbers for individual response

items (e.g., the subject drew an ellipse around the 3 and the 4 in response to a single

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
54

statement). Remaining instruments (N= 97) were numbered and responses were entered

into a statistics program for analysis.

Preliminary Analysis of Data

In order to determine that differences in subject responses were not significantly

related to subjects’ institutions, an independent samples Mest was employed to compare

scores by institution. A comparison was made between institutions one (n = 23) and two

(,n = 29), which evaluated the nonverbal exhibitor tapes (M= 76.65, SD = 8.00 to M=

72.19, SD = 13.04, respectively). An independent samples Mest confirmed no significant

differences between groups [7(50) = 1.40,/? = .17]. Similarly, a comparison was drawn

between institutions three (n = 17) and four (n = 28), which evaluated the nonverbal

inhibitor tapes (M= 67.76, SD = 15.50 to M = 61.11, SD = 14.18, respectively). Again,

an independent samples Mest confirmed no significant difference [7(43) = 1.47,p = .15].

Consequently, further analyses were able to combine institutions, and focus on the main

variables of interest.

Presentation of Data

Research Question 1: Do music education majors perceive a difference in

overall teaching effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

The first step in answering this question was to calculate means and standard

deviations for a composite score taking into account all questions on the PTEM. For each

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
55

group means were calculated by adding total scores reported by individual subjects then

dividing by the number of subjects.

Subjects (n = 52) assessing the nonverbal exhibitor condition perceived teaching

effectiveness to be higher than those subjects (n = 45) viewing the nonverbal inhibitor

condition (M= 74.42, SD = 13.60 to M = 63.62, SD = 14.88, respectively). To explicate

further the nature of this finding, an independent samples t-test was employed to test

whether the nonverbal exhibitor mean was significantly higher than the nonverbal

inhibitor mean. The Mest confirmed a significant difference [/(95) = 3.74, p < .001],

Research Question 2: Do music education majors perceive a difference in

teaching effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect

to teaching behaviors?

In order to determine subjects’ perceptions of teaching behavior, items on the

PTEM pertaining to the actual practice/implementation of instruction were examined in

isolation. These items consisted of:

This teacher/conductor used a variety of teaching methods.


This teacher/conductor cared about student learning.
This teacher/conductor had good teaching skills.
This teacher/conductor gave clear instructions.
This teacher/conductor had a firm grasp on the material he/she presented.
This teacher/conductor provided students with appropriate feedback.
This teacher/conductor maintained good eye contact with the students.
This teacher/conductor communicated information clearly to the students.
This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play better.
This teacher/conductor made certain that students understood his/her presentation
of the material.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
56

Examining perceptions of teaching effectiveness exclusively revealed that

subjects {n = 52) who viewed the nonverbal exhibitor condition perceived teaching

effectiveness to be higher than those subjects (n = 45) who viewed the nonverbal

inhibitor condition (M = 38.23, SD = 6.22 to M = 33.69, SD = 7.07, respectively). To

explicate further the nature of this finding, an independent samples Mest was employed

to test whether the nonverbal exhibitor mean was significantly higher than the nonverbal

inhibitor mean. The Mest confirmed a significant difference [7(95) = 3.67,p < .001],

Research Question 3: Do music education majors perceive a difference in

teaching effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect

to musical competence?

In order to determine subjects’ perceptions of musical competence, items on the

PTEM pertaining to the musical competence of the director were examined in isolation.

These items consisted of:

This teacher/conductor seemed musically creative.


This teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students.
This teacher/conductor was skillful in communicating musical ideas.
This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble to play expressively.
This teacher/conductor was helpful in guiding students to develop musical skills
for future learning.
This teacher/conductor encouraged the band to play with good technique.
This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play with a good sound.
This teacher/conductor was knowledgeable about music.

Examining perceptions of musical competence exclusively revealed that subjects

(n = 52) who viewed the nonverbal exhibitor condition perceived teaching effectiveness

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
to be higher than those subjects (n = 45) who viewed the nonverbal inhibitor condition

(M = 36.19, SD = 7.84 to M = 29.93, SD = 8.34, respectively). To explicate further the

nature of this finding, an independent samples /-test was employed to test whether the

nonverbal exhibitor mean was significantly higher than the nonverbal inhibitor mean.

The /-test confirmed a significant difference [/(95) = 3.80,/? < .001].

Research Question 4: Do areas of agreement exist between composite

evaluations of the nonverbal exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

As the above research questions revealed composite significant differences with

respect to overall means, teaching effectiveness means and musical competence means, it

was decided to analyze each item on the PTEM individually to determine if occurrences

of agreement were evidenced on particular items. Occurrences of agreement were

defined as those individual questions not manifesting a significant difference between the

groups. Examining the individual items from the PTEM, revealed that responses to four

items were not statistically different. Specifically, there were no significant differences

ip > .05) in responses between subjects (n = 52) viewing the nonverbal exhibitor

condition and subjects (n = 45) viewing the nonverbal inhibitor condition for the

following:

Item 1: This teacher/conductor seemed musically creative (M = 3.79, SD = .96 to

M = 3.40, SD = 1.30, respectively);

Item 3: This teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students (M

= 4.04, SD = .93 to M = 3.87, SD = 1.22, respectively);

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
Item 9: This teacher/conductor gave clear instructions (M = 4.83, SD = 1.10 to M

= 4.84, SD = 1.0, respectively); and

Item 15: This teacher/conductor communicated information clearly to the students

(M = 4.48, SD = 1.13 to M = 4.31, SD = 1.06, respectively).

Mean scores for these individual items for each group ranged from 3.40 to 4.84

with close standard deviations. This indicates that participants generally agreed with

these statements regardless of the nonverbal manipulation employed by the researcher.

For every other PTEM question, a significant difference was evidenced with the verbal

exhibitor condition being rated significantly higher than the verbal inhibitor condition.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
59

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

Restatement of the Purpose and Research Questions

The purpose of this research was to examine and analyze the effect of specific

nonverbal behaviors (i.e., conducting emblems) on music education majors’ perceptions

of teaching effectiveness in the classroom environment of the instrumental music

ensemble (band) rehearsal. The nonverbal behavior of interest was the emblem,

specifically housed within the paradigm of conducting gestures. The current research

focused on a comparison of the presence or absence of defined emblems in

teaching/conducting situations. Specific research questions were as follows:

1. Do music education majors perceive a difference in overall teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

2. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to teaching

behaviors?

3. Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness

between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to musical

competence?

4. Do areas of agreement exist between composite evaluations of the nonverbal

exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
60

Summary of Findings

Research Question 1: Will music education majors perceive a difference in

overall teaching effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor?

To evaluate this question total PTEM scores were calculated. The highest

possible score for the PTEM was 108. Subjects assessing the nonverbal exhibitor

reported a mean score of 74.42 (SD = 13.60), which was significantly (p < .001) higher

with regard to teaching effectiveness than the mean score of 63.62 (SD = 14.88) reported

by subjects assessing the inhibitor condition. This finding indicates that music education

students do indeed perceive a difference in teaching effectiveness based on emblematic

nonverbal behavior, and that predominant use of emblems rated a higher score than did

nonuse.

Research Question 2: Do music education majors perceive a difference in

teaching effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect

to teaching behaviors?

For this question mean scores from PTEM items regarding teaching behaviors

were calculated. The highest possible score for these nine items was 54. The mean score

reported by those music education students who observed the nonverbal exhibitor

condition was 38.23 (SD = 6.22). This was significantly higher (p < .001) than the mean

score of 33.69 (SD = 7.07) reported by those who observed the nonverbal inhibitor

condition. This implies that the perception of teaching behaviors by music education

students is positively related to emblematic nonverbal behavior.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
61

Question 3: Do music education majors perceive a difference in teaching

effectiveness between nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor with respect to

musical competence?

Evaluation of this question required the calculation of mean scores for PTEM

items related to musical competence. The highest possible score for these nine items was

54. The mean score reported for subjects assessing the nonverbal exhibitor condition was

36.19 (SD = 7.84). This was significantly (p < .001) higher than the mean score of 29.93

(SD = 8.34) for those assessing the nonverbal inhibitor condition. This result suggests

that use of emblematic nonverbal behavior is regarded positively by music education

students with regard to musical competence.

Question 4: Do areas of agreement exist between composite evaluations of the

nonverbal exhibitor model and the nonverbal inhibitor model?

While the above research questions revealed differences with respect to overall

means, an examination of individual items from the PTEM revealed similarities between

the two groups of respondents. Specifically, there were no significant differences (p >

.05) in responses between subjects viewing the nonverbal exhibitor condition and

subjects viewing the nonverbal inhibitor condition for four items.

For the PTEM items “this teacher/conductor seemed musically creative,” “this

teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students,” “this

teacher/conductor gave clear instructions,” and “this teacher/conductor communicated

information clearly to the students” mean scores were similar. Mean scores for these

individual items by group ranged from 3.40 (SD = 1.30) to 4.84 (SD = 1.00). This

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
62

suggests that respondents generally agreed with these statements regardless of the

nonverbal manipulation.

Discussion and Implications

Results of this research indicate that music education majors generally rate a

nonverbal exhibitor model as better than a nonverbal inhibitor model when evaluating

teaching practice. Similarities, however, may be found among certain aspects of teaching

practice such as oral communication and musical creativity. Along with implications for

improving professional training, there also are questions requiring further exploration.

Similarities (i.e., no significant statistical difference) between teaching models

were found between four pairs of PTEM item responses. Respondents viewing both the

nonverbal exhibitor and the nonverbal inhibitor shared common opinions regarding two

statements dealing with musical competence: “This teacher/conductor seemed musically

creative,” and “This teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students.”

Responses for these items were between the “somewhat agree” and “somewhat disagree”

captions on the Likert scale. Because of the even number of Likert responses there was

no neutral choice for those whose preferred response might have been “no opinion.” A

lack of musical creativity may have been perceived by respondents because of the tightly

scripted nature of the rehearsal videotapes (see Chapter III).

In the same way, both groups of respondents generally agreed with two

statements aimed toward teaching behaviors: “This teacher/conductor gave clear

instructions,” and “This teacher/conductor communicated information clearly to

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
63

students.” Mean responses for these items fell between the “somewhat agree” and

“agree” captions on the Likert scale. In this situation it seems the perception of

instructional clarity was unaffected by nonverbal behavior.

All other pairs of responses yielded significant differences between the nonverbal

exhibitor and the nonverbal inhibitor. In each case the nonverbal exhibitor rated higher

mean scores than the nonverbal inhibitor. Elaboration on these differences follows.

The four smallest statistically significant mean differences (A = 0.60 or smaller)

between teaching models were found in statements related to teaching behaviors. For the

statements: “This teacher/conductor cared about student learning,” and “This

teacher/conductor had a firm grasp of the material he/she presented,” both groups of

respondents indicated agreement with the nonverbal exhibitor group rating a higher level

of agreement than the nonverbal inhibitor group. For the statements: “This

teacher/conductor used a variety of teaching methods,” and “This teacher/conductor

provided students with appropriate feedback,” respondents generally answered between

“somewhat agree” and “somewhat disagree” with the nonverbal exhibitor group aligned

with the side of agreement and the nonverbal inhibitor group aligned with the side of

disagreement.

The four greatest mean differences (A = 0.80 or greater) between teaching models

involved three statements related to musical behavior: “This teacher/conductor helped the

ensemble to play expressively;” “This teacher/conductor was helpful in guiding students

to develop musical skills for future learning;” and “This teacher/conductor helped the

ensemble play better;” and one statement related to teaching behavior: “This

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
64

teacher/conductor made certain that students understood his/her presentation of the

material.” In each of these cases, respondents to the nonverbal exhibitor tended to agree

with the statements, while respondents to the nonverbal inhibitor tended to disagree.

Connecting a gesture with verbal instruction seemed to increase the perception

that the activities of the teacher/conductor were contributing to better ensemble

musicianship. This may be related to the effect described in Yarbrough’s (1975) research

linking high magnitude teacher/conductor behaviors, which included expressive use of

emblems in conducting, to positive student ensemble performance. Similarly, the

dynamic teaching/conducting behavior may have contributed to the perception that the

teacher/conductor model made certain that students understood the material presented.

The six remaining statements with mean differences between 0.61 and 0.79

yielded varying responses. For the statements: “This teacher/conductor was skillful in

communicating musical ideas;” “This teacher/conductor had good teaching skills;” and

“This teacher/conductor encouraged the band to play with good technique;” nonverbal

exhibitor respondents agreed while nonverbal inhibitor respondents disagreed. These

results continue the general pattern of higher scores for the nonverbal exhibitor.

Both groups, however, agreed with the statements: “This teacher/conductor

maintained good eye contact with the students,” and “This teacher/conductor was

knowledgeable about music.” Because the scripted sequences for the video samples were

memorized by the teacher/conductor model, a high level of eye contact could have been

observed in both situations. The perception of both sample groups that the

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
65

teacher/conductor was knowledgeable about music may have been determined by the

similarity of scripted verbal information conveyed to students in the video samples.

Conversely, both sample groups disagreed with the statement: “This

teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play with a good sound.” The scripted material

did not include comments relating to ensemble sound. The video samples contain no

instructional suggestions to the students about improving their blend, balance, or tone

quality. Thus, the music majors evaluating the video samples perceived this statement

negatively.

Results of this research reinforce findings of previous research demonstrating the

importance of linking appropriate nonverbal behavior with the educational process.

Participants perceived the nonverbal exhibitor model to be more effective, overall as well

as with regard to teaching behaviors and musical competence, than the nonverbal

inhibitor model. Along with Francisco (1994), who found that visual communication,

when used alone, was significantly less likely to improve ensemble performance than

verbal/modeling/visual, this research demonstrated the importance of emblemizing

conducting gestures by linking the verbal definition with the nonverbal gesture.

Likewise, Cofer (1988) found that gesture instruction was effective in improving the

recognition and performance of conducting emblems. As this research seemed to

indicate that the teacher/conductor’s use of emblem had a positive effect on the

perception of teaching effectiveness, Grechesky’s (1985) study demonstrated that

emblems and illustrators had a more powerful effect than any of the teaching/conducting

variables. And, as this research connected specific nonverbal behaviors with the

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
66

perception o f teaching effectiveness, Hunnicutt’s (1999) research supported the idea that

teachers trained in the use of content specific nonverbal techniques may be more

effective classroom managers.

Questions remain. Buell (1990) concluded that teaching effectiveness was not the

result of any single factor. This research project seems to bear that out. Because

agreement was found between sample groups’ perceptions of instructional clarity (“This

teacher/conductor gave clear instructions,” and “This teacher/conductor communicated

information clearly to students”), some element of oral communication may transcend the

strength of the nonverbal emblem. Interestingly, respondents had one of their greatest

differences based upon understanding of material (This teacher/conductor made certain

that students understood his/her presentation of the material). This may be due to the fact

that respondents to the nonverbal exhibitor observed the teacher/conductor repeatedly

connecting a gesture with a verbal definition whereas respondents to the nonverbal

inhibitor observed mostly verbal instruction. Further investigation may be warranted

regarding the development, implementation and effectiveness of verbal instructional

behaviors used by experienced teachers.

Another area for further exploration may involve the differentiation of

instructional gesture and meaningful gesture—the difference between traffic-control

gestures and purely expressive gestures. The current research examines gesture as a

mode of instruction. Teacher/conductors may use meaningful gestures to facilitate

instruction. Within a musical context, however, a gesture may be meaningful without

being instructional.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
67

Meyer (1956) discussed the idea that the human experience of musical stimuli is

inextricably linked to experiences of nonmusical stimuli.

Both music and life are experienced as dynamic processes of growth and decay,
activity and rest, tension and release. These processes are differentiated, not only
by the course and shape of the motions involved in them, but also by the quality
of the motion (p. 261).

A teacher/conductor may represent “growth and decay” or “tension and release” with

gestures that have no direct verbal definition. These types of gestures may communicate

meaningful information to musicians without also conveying instructional information.

Furthermore, Langer (1957) posits the symbolization of human reality facilitated

by music. In her view, signs are different from symbols. Signs contain something of

what they signify, whereas symbols convey abstractions of concepts. She discusses the

idea that music is neither the cause nor the result of feeling, but rather the symbolic

expression of feeling. This being the case, gestures used by a conductor to guide a

musical performance may likewise be signs or symbols. While signal gestures may

convey verbally definable information, symbolic gestures may be meaningful but less

directly definable.

Results of this current research support the idea that nonverbal behaviors are

effective in aiding classroom instruction. Findings from research questions one, two and

three bear out the notion that students perceive a teacher who uses defined emblems as

more effective than a teacher who does not. This may be because the teacher/conductor’s

use of defined emblems contributes to an increased perception of teaching intensity.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
68

Nonverbal conducting behaviors are necessary elements pertaining to the

instrumental music ensemble rehearsal classroom. Data from this study reinforce the

notion that coupling conducting emblems with verbal descriptions of their meaning lead

to more effective teaching practice as perceived by music education students. Such

nonverbal gestures specific to conducting may be taught and learned by conducting

practitioners. With his study, Hausmann (1984) developed a systematic plan for score

study including a coded gestural analysis system that isolated and defined gestural

possibilities for teacher/conductors. Byo & Austin (1994) found general nonverbal

characteristics that separated expert conductors from novice conductors and suggested

standards which novices might be encouraged to pursue.

Instrumental music teachers encounter a variety of conducting/teaching styles

through the course of their education and teaching experience. One significant factor

affecting teacher preparedness in the classroom is effective teacher education. The more

thoroughly prepared teachers are to educate the students in their classrooms, the more

significant the results of those students’ learning experiences will be. Educators of pre­

service music teachers need to be aware of the perceived effectiveness of the skills and

professional practices imparted to their students. Results of this research have

implications for educators of pre-service teachers. Specifically, empirical evidence

showing the benefit of the incorporation of nonverbal behaviors such as defined

emblems, illustrators, and posed facial gestures within the context of instrumental

classroom teaching may help pedagogues improve the content of their instruction.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
69

Educators of pre-service teachers should incorporate methods to enhance and refine these

skills in their curricula.

As conducting students are encouraged to develop a vocabulary of gestures, they

should be taught to think about what the desired effects of the gestures are and how these

effects might be described to students in various developmental stages.

Teacher/conductors in the classroom should be aware of the effectiveness of their

rehearsal communication. If a gesture does not elicit an appropriate response, then verbal

explanation of the gesture concurrent with the physical demonstration of the gesture may

be helpful. As evidenced in these results, a definition coupled with an emblem appears to

have a positive impact on teacher effectiveness, both in musical and general teaching

competencies.

Results from this research project indicate that music education students perceive

a teacher/conductor who incorporates defined emblems into his/her rehearsal as more

skillful in communicating musical ideas than the teacher who does not use defined

emblems. This research also suggests that teacher/conductors who use defined emblems

are perceived to help ensembles play better than those who do not. Other researchers

including Carpenter (1986), Cofer (1988), Francisco (1994), and Greschesky (1985)

reported similar findings.

In many cases instrumental ensembles in school settings include students new to

the ensemble each year (e.g., incoming freshmen, transfer students, newly auditioned

members, etc.). This means that the teacher/conductor’s repertoire of gestures is new,

and possibly foreign, to some of the learners. The teacher/conductor should be aware of

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
70

this and should review the intent of various conducting gestures so that the gestures

become emblematic to everyone in the ensemble.

Results of this research suggest that the teacher/conductor who explains the

meaning of most of his/her gestures as they are given, thereby defining emblems, is

perceived as being more helpful in guiding students to develop musical skills for future

learning than the teacher/conductor who did not define most gestures. These findings

bear out previous research showing that even commonly employed conducting gestures

must be defined for greatest rehearsal effectiveness (Sousa, 1988), and that students must

also be sensitized to the use of these gestures (Mayne, 1992).

Conclusions

The following conclusions are drawn from the analysis of the data:

1. Music education majors perceive a nonverbal exhibitor model to be more

effective in teaching than a nonverbal inhibitor model.

2. Music education majors perceive a nonverbal exhibitor model to be more

effective in teaching than a nonverbal inhibitor model with respect to teaching behaviors.

3. Music education majors perceive a nonverbal exhibitor model to be more

effective in teaching than a nonverbal inhibitor model with respect to musical

competence.

4. Areas o f agreement exist among the perceptions of music education students as

they evaluate nonverbal exhibitor and nonverbal inhibitor models specifically in terms of

instructional clarity. Evaluators of both teaching models believed that the

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
71

teacher/conductor gave clear instructions, and that the teacher/conductor communicated

information clearly to students.

Limitations and Recommendations for Future Research

This study was limited to music education majors from four Midwestern colleges.

As with any research involving limited respondents the researcher cannot assume that

results are generalizable to the greater population. Since, however, the goal of the

research was to determine the relationship between verbal exhibitors and nonverbal

exhibitors, generalizability was not a primary concern of this study. Future research may

replicate this study with a larger population to determine whether results are applicable to

a general population of music majors.

A second limiting factor was the possible effect posed by the nonverbal element

of facial expression. Other researchers have found that facial expression can affect

emotional and informative aspects of communication (Chovil, 1991; Ekman & Oster,

1982). The researcher controlled this factor by modeling for each experimental video

segment so that facial expressions were as consistent as possible between the different

scripted segments. Future research may be designed so as to further isolate use of

emblematic gesture by eliminating facial expression.

As a result of this study the researcher suggests that music teacher preparation

programs consider dedicating appropriate instructional time toward the teaching of

emblematic nonverbal behaviors, especially in the area of conducting.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
72

Research may be conducted in which teachers described by their colleagues as

effective teacher/conductors are videotaped in real rehearsal situations. These video

samples could be evaluated to find the proportion of defined and undefined gestures to

determine whether a pattern exists.

While much research seems to support a positive correlation between expressive

gesture and teaching effectiveness, Price & Chang (2001, 2005) found no such

correlation. Thus, additional research is needed in the use of gesture by music teachers

and its relationship to instruction in the music classroom.

Effectiveness of oral instruction in the music rehearsal classroom is another area

for additional research. One such study might measure the effectiveness of oral

instruction. Longitudinal research might be done to learn whether teachers get better at

giving instruction as they gain experience. Another project might involve determining

whether effective oral instruction compensates for ineffective nonverbal information.

Furthermore, an investigation combining the results of this study with the added

dimension of teaching intensity as a variable may determine the role teaching intensity

plays compared to use of emblematic gestures as students perceive teaching

effectiveness.

Because personalities of individuals vary, as do classroom environments,

examples of successful teaching may be found in widely different packages. “Within

each teacher’s experience, even within as brief a time scale as a single lesson or

rehearsal, there exists a range of differences with regard to each aspect of the teacher’s

performance” (Duke, 1999, p. 22). Finding common paths to dynamic, effective

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
73

instruction seems to be the initial goal of much educational research including this paper.

Whether we teach K -12 or in a university, whether our students are developing musicians

or those who want to teach developing musicians, when we find a practice that enhances

student learning we should consider adopting and adapting it to our own teaching and

sharing it with teachers for the future.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
74

APPENDIX A

Perception of Teaching Effectiveness Measure


Circle the response closest to your position regarding each statement.

1. This teacher/conductor seemed musically creative.

Strongly Agree A gree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

2. This teacher/conductor used a variety of teaching methods.

Strongly Agree A gree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

3. This teacher/conductor provided good musical information to students.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

4. This teacher/conductor cared about student learning.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

5. This teacher/conductor was skillful in communicating musical ideas.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

6. This teacher/conductor had good teaching skills.

Strongly Agree Agree S om ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
75

7. This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble to play expressively.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

8. This teacher/conductor was helpful in guiding students to develop musical skills


for future learning.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

9. This teacher/conductor gave clear instructions.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Somewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

10. This teacher/conductor had a firm grasp of the material he/she presented.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

11. This teacher/conductor encouraged the band to play with good technique.

Strongly Agree Agree S om ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

12. This teacher/conductor provided students with appropriate feedback.

Strongly Agree A gree S om ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
76

13. This teacher/conductor maintained good eye contact with the students.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

14. This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play with a good sound.

Strongly Agree A gree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

15. This teacher/conductor communicated information clearly to the students.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

16. This teacher/conductor was knowledgeable about music.

Strongly Agree A gree Som ew hat Agree Som ewhat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

17. This teacher/conductor helped the ensemble play better.

Strongly Agree A gree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

18. This teacher/conductor made certain that students understood his/her presentation
of the material.

Strongly Agree Agree Som ew hat Agree Som ew hat Disagree Disagree Strongly Disagree

6 5 4 3 2 1

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
77

REFERENCES

Acheson, K. A. & Gall, M. D. (1992). Techniques in the clinical supervision o f teachers.


New York: Longman.

Aubrecht, J. D., Hanna, G. S. & Hoyt, D. P. (1986). A comparison of high school student
ratings of teaching effectiveness with teacher self-ratings: Factor analytic and
multitrait-multimethod analyses. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
46(1), 223-231.

Baker, P. J. (1981). The development of music teacher checklists for use by


administrators, music supervisors, and teachers in evaluating music teaching
effectiveness. (UMI No. 8201803)

Berz, W. L. (1983). The development of an observation instrument designed to classify


specific nonverbal communication techniques employed by conductors of musical
ensembles. (UMI No. 8400532)

Brand, M. (1985). Research in music teacher effectiveness. Update, 3 (2), 13 - 16.

Brophy, T. (1993). Evaluation of music educators: Toward defining an appropriate


instrument. (ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 375 029).

Buell, D. S. (1990). Effective rehearsing with the instrumental music ensemble: A case
study. (UMI No. 9025698)

Burgoon, J. K. & Saine, T. (1978). The unspoken dialogue: An introduction to nonverbal


communication. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

Byo, J. L. & Austin, K. R. (1994). Comparison of expert and novice conductors: An


approach to the analysis of nonverbal behaviors. Journal o f Band Research, 30
(1), 1 1 -3 4 .

Carpenter, R. A. (1986). A descriptive analysis of relationships between verbal behaviors


of teacher-conductors and ratings of selected junior high and senior high school
band rehearsals. (UMI No. 8618756)

Chovil, N. (1991). Social determinants of facial displays. Journal o f Nonverbal


Behavior, 15 (3), 1 41-154.

Cofer, R. S. (1998). Effects of conducting-gesture instruction on seventh-grade band


students’ performance response to conducting emblems. Journal o f Research in
Music Education, 46(3), 360 - 373.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
78

Colwell, R. & Goolsby, T. (1992). The teaching o f instrumental music (2nd ed.).
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Corporon, E. (1997). The quantum conductor. In R. Miles (Ed.), Teaching music


through performance in band (pp. 11-26). Chicago: GIA.

Cutietta, Robert A. (1990). Adapt your teaching style to your students. Music
Educators Journal, 76. (6), 31-36.

Dickey, M. R. (1988). A comparison of the effects of verbal instruction and nonverbal


teacher-student modeling on instructional effectiveness in instrumental music
ensembles. (UMI No. 8907022)

Duke, R. A. (1999). Measures of instructional effectiveness in music research. Bulletin


fo r the Council o f Research in Music Education, 143, 1-48.

Duke, R. A. & Henninger, J. C. (2002). Teachers’ verbal corrections and observers’


perceptions of teaching and learning. Journal o f Research in Music Education, 50
(1), 75-87.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories,


origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49 - 98.

Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1972). Hand movements. The Journal o f Communication,


22, 353-374.

Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1982). Review of research, 1970 - 1980. In: P. Ekman (Ed.),
Emotion in the human face (2nd ed.) (pp. 147 - 173). Cambridge: Cambridge
University Press.

Elliott, D. J. (1995). Music matters. New York: Oxford University Press.

Erbes, R. (1982). Music teacher education: Toward the twenty-first century. In R.


Colwell (Ed.). Symposium in music education. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois
Press.

Francisco, J. M. (1994). Conductor communication in the ensemble rehearsal: The


relative effects of verbal communication, visual communication, and modeling on
performance. (UMI No. 9542638)

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
79

Fridlund, A. J., Ekman, P., & Oster, H. (1987). Facial expression of emotion. In: A. W.
Siegman and S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (2nd
ed.) (pp. 143 - 224). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fry, W. E. (1990). Books and dissertations on the technique of instrumental conducting:


A select bibliography. Journal o f Band Research 26 (1), 30-43.

Galkin, E. W. (1988). A history o f orchestral conducting: In theory and practice. New


York: Pendragon.

Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., & Singer, M. (1999). What the teacher’s hands tell the
student’s mind about math. Journal o f Educational Psychology, 91(4), 720 - 730.

Grainger, P. A. (1949). Ye Banks and Braes O ’ Bonnie Doon. New York: G. Schirmer.

Grant, J. W. & Drafall, L. E. (1991). Teacher effectiveness Research: A review and


comparison. Bulletin o f the Council for Research in Music Education, 108, 31-
48.

Grechesky, R. N. (1985). An analysis of nonverbal and verbal conducting behaviors and


their relationship to expressive musical performance. (UMI No. 8513459)

Green, E. A. H. (1987). The modern conductor (4th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
Hall.

Hamann, D. L., Baker, D. S., McAllister, P. A., Bauer, W. I. (2002). Factors affecting
university music students’ perceptions of lesson quality and teaching
effectiveness. Journal o f Research in Music Education, 48 (2), 102-113.

Hausmann, C. S. (1984). A procedural model for the transference of analytical insights


into verbal and nonverbal communication in choral music. (UMI No. 8404796)

Huck, S. W. (2000). Reading statistics and research (3rd ed.). New York: Addison
Wesley Longman.

Hunnicutt, V. A. W. (1999). Training in the use of selected nonverbal behaviors to reduce


student disruptions in the classroom. (UMI No. 9909068)

Hunsberger, D. & Ernst, R. (1983). The art o f conducting. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

Johnson, C. M., Fredrickson, W. E., Achey, C. E., & Gentry, G. R. (2003). The effect of
nonverbal elements of conducting on the overall evaluation of student and
professional conductors. Journal o f Band Research, 38 (2), 64-79.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
80

Johnson, H. G., Ekman, P, & Friesen, W. V. (1975). Communicative body movements:


American emblems. Semiotica, 15 (4), 335-353.

Kohut, D. L. (1973). Instrumental music pedagogy. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-


Hall.

Langer, S. K. (1957). Philosophy in a new key: A study in the symbolism o f reason, rite,
and art (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Leinsdorf, E. (1981). The composer’s advocate: A radical orthodoxy fo r musicians.


New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Madsen, C. K., Standley, J. M., & Cassidy, J. W. (1989). Demonstration and recognition
of high and low contrasts in teacher intensity. Journal o f Research in Music
Education, 37 (2), 85-92.

Madsen, K. (2003). The effect of accuracy of instruction, teacher delivery, and student
attentiveness on musicians’ evaluation of teacher effectiveness. Journal o f
Research in Music Education, 51 (1), 38-50.

Mayne, R. G. (1992). An investigation of the use of facial expression in conjunction with


musical conducting gestures and their interpretation by instrumental performers.
(UMI No. 9238229)

McElheran, B. (1966). Conducting technique for beginners and professionals. New


York: Oxford University Press.

Melzi, R. C. (1976). The Bantam new college Italian and English dictionary. New York:
Bantam.

Meyer, L. B. (1956). Emotion and meaning in music. Chicago: University of Chicago


Press.

Miller, P. W. (2000). Nonverbal communication in the classroom. Munster, IN: Miller &
Associates.

Motley, M. T. (1993). Facial affect and verbal context in conversation: Facial expression
as interjection. Human Communication Research, 20 (1), 3 - 40.

Murray, H. G. (1994). The impact of formative and summative evaluation of teaching


in North American Universities, Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education,
9(2), 117-132.

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
81

National Association of Schools of Music (2005). National Association o f Schools o f


Music Handbook 2005-2006. Reston, VA: NASM.

Ostling, A. (1977). Research on nonverbal communication with implications for


conductors. Journal o f Band Research, 12 (2), 2 9 -4 3 .

Patten, M. L. (1998). Questionnaire research: A practical Guide. Los Angeles: Pyrczak


Publishing.

Price, H. E. & Chang, E. C. (2005). Conductor and ensemble performance expressivity


and state festival ratings. Journal o f Research in Music Education 53 (1), 66-11.

Price, H. E. & Chang, E. C. (2001). Conductor expressivity and ensemble performance:


An exploratory investigation. Contributions to Music Education 28 (2), 9-20.

Price, H. E. & Winter, S. (1990). Effect of strict and expressive conducting on


performances and opinions of eight-grade students. Journal o f Band Research, 27
(1), 30-43.

Random House Webster's unabridged dictionary (2nd ed.) (1997). New York: Random
House.

Rothrock, C. (1971). Training the high school orchestra. West Nyack: Parker
Publishing.

Rudolph, M.(1994). The grammar o f conducting (3 rd ed.). New York: Schirmer.

Saunders, T. C. & Worthington, J. L. (1990). Teacher effectiveness in the performance


classroom. Update: Applications o f Research in Music Education, 8 (2), 26-29.

Sousa, G. D. (1988). Musical conducting emblems: an investigation of the use of specific


conducting gestures by instrumental conductors and their interpretation by
instrumental performers. (UMI No. 8820356)

SPSS version 13.0 fo r Windows. (2004). Chicago: SPSS, Inc.

Swanwick, K. (1999). Teaching music musically. New York: Routledge.

Thompson, J. B. (1997). The relationship between nonverbal communication skill and


effectiveness in influencing opinion. (UMI No. 9822848)

Willard, R. (1986). Speak less, communicate more. The Instrumentalist, 40(6), 3 8 - 4 1 .

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.
82

Yarbrough, C. (1975). Effect of magnitude of conductor behavior on students in selected


mixed choruses. Journal o f Research in Music Education, 23 (2), 134-146.

Yuzdepski, I & Elliott, L. (1985). Planning for an evaluation o f teaching performance.


Alberta Department of Education, Planning Services Branch. (ERIC Document
Reproduction Service No. ED 270 399).

R ep ro d u ced with p erm ission o f th e copyright ow ner. Further reproduction prohibited w ithout perm ission.

You might also like