You are on page 1of 18

DOI: 10.1002/arcp.

1001

RESEARCH REVIEW

Evolution and consumer psychology

Kristina M. Durante1 | Vladas Griskevicius2

1
Rutgers University, Newark, NJ, USA
2
Abstract
University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN,
USA An evolutionary theoretical approach considers the adaptive function of behavior.
Here we discuss what it means to use an evolutionary approach to generate predic-
Correspondence
Kristina Durante, Rutgers Business School— tions about consumer behavior and the value of applying an evolutionary lens to the
Newark and New Brunswick, Rutgers
study of consumer psychology. We begin with a discussion of the core insights of
University, Newark, NJ, USA.
Email: kdurante@business.rutgers.edu evolutionary theory and the common misperceptions associated with an evolutionary
approach to the study of behavior. We then detail how specific evolutionarily in-
formed theories can be applied to four core areas of consumer research: risk prefer-
ence, competition and luxury consumption, self-control and temporal preferences,
and the consumer behavior of women and families. We also discuss the strengths and
limitations of an evolutionarily informed research program.

KEYWORDS
evolutionary psychology, luxury consumption, natural selection, risk, self-control

1  |  INT RO DUCT ION Charles Darwin, she would start with a simple question: What adap-
tive function might behavior serve?
Picture it. The year is 2027. A spacecraft descends upon on a modern This question is the starting point for anyone seeking to under-
shopping mall carrying an alien behavioral scientist sent to collect data stand modern behavior in an evolutionarily informed way. Here we
on Earthlings. The alien enters a store full of a dizzying array of opu- discuss what it means to take an evolutionary approach to understand-
lent trinkets foreign to her eyes. She settles in to record observations ing consumer psychology and highlight the benefits of incorporating
and attempt to make sense of the smells, sights, and peculiar Earthling this approach in consumer research. We discuss relevant theories and
behaviors—from the aroma of heavily scented liquids being sprayed on findings and offer suggestions for easy ways to incorporate an evolu-
passersby to women perched upon elevated chairs while heavy paint tionary perspective into the study of consumer psychology.
is applied to their faces. This alien would likely wonder what (on earth) One important note before we begin: Applying an evolutionarily
is going on. informed lens does not mean throwing away other approaches to
Like our alien visitor, Charles Darwin was similarly puzzled by many consumer research. Rather, an evolutionary lens is a keen theoreti-
of the strange characteristics and behaviors of the beings he encoun- cally driven tool for making novel discoveries within any behavioral
tered in his travels around the world. Darwin became a detective of research area. Thinking about behavior in an evolutionarily informed
behavior and through decades of observations formulated the theory way also does not mean a researcher must re-brand themselves as
of natural selection (Darwin, 1859). Darwin’s theory became the uni- an evolutionary researcher. Instead, an evolutionary perspective can
fying theory of behavior-guiding research in the life sciences. The the- often shed new light on important questions.
ory of natural selection is a meta-theory that helps us understand the
characteristics and behaviors of all living organisms, including humans.
Today this interdisciplinary framework is the bridge between the social 2 | AN EVOLUT IO NARY APP ROACH TO
and the life sciences, regularly incorporated into modern psychology, CO NSUM E R BE HAVIO R
anthropology, and other behavioral sciences.
So how might an alien scientist seek to make sense of the strange An evolutionary explanation of behavior concerns the behavior’s
behaviors in a 21st-century shopping mall? If she was anything like adaptive function. All human behaviors include an evolutionary

4  | © 2017 Society for Consumer Psychology wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/arcp Consum Psychol Rev. 2018;1:4–21.
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS       5|
explanation because, on average, all behaviors result from a neural • Proximate mechanism: What are the triggers (causes) of the behavior?
(brain) and/or physiological (body) mechanism designed to produce a • Development: How does the behavior come about during one’s
particular behavior (output) given a particular stimulus (input). lifetime?
Without prior exposure to evolutionary biology, it is reasonable • Adaptive function: What adaptive problem(s) does the behavior ulti-
to assume that a few human behaviors might be related to evolution mately function to solve?
and that many others are probably unrelated to evolution. But this • Evolutionary history: How did the behavior arise in the species?
assumption is false. In reality, all behaviors include an evolutionary ex-
planation because all behaviors have multiple explanations at different The first two explanations concern processes that occur within the
levels of analysis. lifetime of the individual. The latter two explanations reside within the
Here is a concrete example. Most people love to eat cake. But, deeper realm of evolutionary biology. Table 1 provides a deeper over-
why? One way to answer this question is that cake tastes good. But a view of the four types of explanations.
different way to answer this question is that an attraction to the sight, When seeking to understand a given behavior, a researcher can
smell, and taste of foods rich in sugars and fats helped motivate our begin by starting at any of the four types of explanations. Consumer
ancestors to obtain calorie-dense foods in order to survive in an envi- researchers tend to start and remain focused on the first type of ex-
ronment that was often scarce in calories (Lieberman, 2003). It is also planation—the proximate mechanism. An overwhelming majority of
equally accurate to explain that people love to eat cake because they articles in leading marketing journals focus exclusively on the proxi-
are born with a specific mechanism that activates the pleasure cen- mate mechanism as the sole explanation for behavior (e.g., we eat cake
ters of the brain when tasting sweet foods. And more broadly, people because it tastes good). Focusing on the proximate mechanism makes
love to eat foods rich in sugar and fat because this is typical behavior good sense and provides valuable insight into behavior, but to fully
for all primates and can be traced back to a common ancestor. understand any behavior, it is useful to consider it at more than merely
There are multiple ways to answer the question of why people one level of explanation.
love cake. All of the answers are correct, but each one provides a fun- An evolutionary psychological approach focuses on adaptive
damentally different type of explanation for the behavior. This is be- function. Considering adaptive function—Tinbergen’s third type of
cause all behaviors have multiple explanations. The most widely used explanation—can provide a richer understanding of any behavior
method for categorizing such explanations was developed by Nobel and open the door to untapped research questions. This is because
Prize-winning ethologist Niko Tinbergen (1963). Commonly referred considering adaptive function provides suggestions about previously
to as “the four questions,” Tinbergen grouped explanations into four unknown causal processes at the proximate level, which is where con-
categories, whereby each explanation is associated with a different sumer researchers can find value when generating testable research
type of question: hypotheses.

T A B L E   1   Four types of explanations for behavior

Type of explanation Central question and potential answers Example: Why do people like cake more than carrots?

Proximate mechanism What are the relatively immediate causes of a behavior? People desire cake because sugar and fat tastes good. This
(Causation) Most behaviors have multiple proximate causes, explains the proximate mechanism of the behavior because
How does it work? including environmental cues, social cues, physical state, it pertains to the immediate triggers (causes) of the behavior.
psychological state, hormones, pheromones, genes, and
neurological firing.
Development How does a behavior come about during one’s lifetime? People like to eat cake because they are born with a specific
(Ontogeny) Possibilities include learning, specified developmental neural mechanism that activates the pleasure centers of
How does it come trajectory, imprinting, cognitive maturation, and brain when tasting sweet foods.
about? environmentally contingent expression of genes.
Ultimate function What adaptive problem(s) does a behavior ultimately An attraction to the sight, smell, and taste of foods rich in
(Adaptation) function to solve? sugars and fats helped motivate our ancestors to obtain
Why did it evolve? Some behaviors function to solve one adaptive problem, calorie-dense foods and survive in an environment that was
whereas others can solve multiple problems such as to often scarce in calories (Lieberman, 2003). These are the
obtain food, avoid disease, decrease danger, make ultimate functions of the behavior because they explain how
friends, attain status, attract mates, and care for family. the behavior has enhanced fitness throughout evolutionary
history.
Evolutionary history How did a behavior arise in the species? People like to eat foods rich in sugar and fat because this is
(Phylogeny) Possibilities include that a behavior can be traced to a typical behavior for all primates and can be traced back to a
How did it evolve? common ancestor, behavior evolved independently in common ancestor. This suggests that multiple species share
multiple species, behavior is unique to humans, etc. a similar taste for sweets with a common function that
resulted from the same selection pressure (i.e., need for
sugar and fat to survive).
|
6       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

service of survival and reproduction (think about the distinct problems


2.1 | The adaptive function of behavior
solved by the heart, liver, lungs, etc.), psychological adaptations also
An evolutionary psychological perspective to understanding behavior evolved as solutions to qualitatively distinct adaptive problems.
dates back to Darwin’s (1859) theory of natural selection. Natural se- Adaptive problems are many in number. To successfully enhance
lection is the process by which biologically influenced characteristics fitness, we need to obtain food and water. We must also obtain shel-
become either more or less common in a population depending on ter, avoid disease, and evade physical harm, make friends, attain sta-
how those characteristics affect an individual’s reproductive fitness— tus, find a mate, and care for family. Those humans who became our
the passing of genes onto future generations. Characteristics that ancestors were the ones who were most successful at solving these
enhanced reproductive fitness were passed on to future generations, problems and enhancing their fitness. Those who were less successful
whereas those that impeded it were not. at solving these distinct adaptive problems failed to become anyone’s
Here is an illustration of how the process of natural selection ancestors.
works. Thousands of years ago, one of the most common predators An evolutionary perspective produces five insights into under-
of humans was snakes (Headland & Greene, 2011). Those ancestral standing modern consumer behaviors.
humans who possessed neural mechanisms that facilitated rapid fear
conditioning to snakes ran away from snakes and were less likely to
2.1.1 | Insight #1: Behavior has proximate and
die compared to those who were not afraid. Not dying meant snake-
ultimate explanations
fearing humans were more likely to survive, reach puberty, and have
children of their own. As a result of this advantage, the genes asso- As highlighted in Tinbergen’s four types of explanations, an evolu-
ciated with rapid fear conditioning to snakes were passed on to sub- tionary approach draws an important distinction between proximate
sequent generations and quickly became more prevalent in human reasons for behavior (first type of explanation) and ultimate reasons
populations (i.e., a person who quickly came to fear snakes was more for behavior (third type of explanation). Let us go back to the dessert
likely to survive and has children who also condition quickly to fear example, only this time let us think about brownies. Imagine that your
snakes). This is how natural selection works—particular characteristics colleague just bought a triple-chocolate fudge brownie, and you want
are maintained because they have (or once had) fitness benefits. The to know the reason behind her purchase. So you ask her: “Why did
process of natural selection results in three products: you buy that?” She might simply respond “I was hungry.” If she were
feeling more analytical, she might mention that she loves the taste of
• Adaptations: Characteristics that reliably solved adaptive problems chocolate and could not resist the delectable scent of a warmly baked
better than competing alternatives during evolutionary history (ex- brownie.
ample: fear of poisonous snakes) Your colleague’s explanations all represent proximate reasons.
• By-products: Artifacts without adaptive value that persist because Proximate reasons are important, but they do not address the deeper
they are inherently coupled with adaptations (example: fear of question of why brownies taste good to humans in the first place.
harmless snakes) Understanding these deeper reasons requires an ultimate explanation.
• Noise: Variations in a given characteristic that are due to random Ultimate explanations focus not on the relatively immediate triggers
environmental events or genetic mutations (example: most rare of a behavior, but on its adaptive function. In the brownie case, hu-
types of fears, such as fear of flowers) mans have psychological mechanisms that respond positively to the
sight, smell, and taste of foods rich in sugars and fats. Recall that these
The principles of natural selection have long been applied to the mechanisms exist because an attraction to such calorie-dense foods
study of human anatomy and physiology, as well as the study of animal helped motivate our ancestors to obtain calories and survive in an
behavior (Alcock, 2005). However, there is now widespread recognition environment that was often scarce in calories (Lieberman, 2003). So
that these principles are powerful tools for understanding psychology whereas the proximate reason your friend bought a brownie may be
and behavior in humans (Alcock, 2001; Barkow, Cosmides, & Tooby, because she was hungry for a brownie, the ultimate reason is because
1992; Buss, 2005; Dunbar & Barrett, 2009). a desire for sugary and fatty foods helped solve the critical evolution-
An evolutionary psychological perspective stresses that the human ary challenge of survival.
mind is an integrated assembly of psychological adaptations—a prem- Sometimes the ultimate and proximate reason for a behavior might
ise shared widely by evolutionary biologists in understanding animal be closely connected. In the brownie case, the proximate reason (feel-
behavior (Alcock, 2005; Barash, 1977; Wilson, 2000). Psychological ing hunger) is directly connected to the ultimate function of obtain-
adaptations are information-processing circuits that take in units of ing calories to survive. But most of the time, the connection between
information (from both our external environments and our internal proximate and ultimate reasons will not be that clear. Consider, for
physiological systems) and transform that information into outputs de- example, why birds migrate each year. The proximate reason birds mi-
signed to solve a particular adaptive problem (Barrett, 2012; Barrett & grate is because days get shorter; day length is the immediate cue that
Kurzban, 2006; Cosmides, Barrett, & Tooby, 2010). Psychological ad- triggers the motivation to begin the bird’s journey. But the ultimate
aptations enhanced fitness by solving distinct adaptive problems. Just reason for bird migration has nothing to do with day length. Instead,
like physiological adaptations evolved to solve distinct problems in the the ultimate reason birds migrate is because the locations of the best
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      7

food sites and the best mating sites change with the seasons (Cocker four types of explanations against each other. All four types of ex-
& Mabey, 2005; Lincoln, 1952). planations are complementary, and all four are needed to fully un-
Like other animals, human beings do not need to consciously know derstand any behavior. An evolutionary psychological perspective
the connections between the proximate triggers of their behavior focuses on Tinbergen’s third type of explanation, asking about the
and the ultimate reasons behind those behaviors. In fact, people are adaptive function of behavior. It focuses on how a given behavioral or
especially poor at recognizing the ultimate reasons for their actions psychological tendency would have helped our ancestors solve some
(Barrett & Kurzban, 2006; Kenrick, Griskevicius, Neuberg, & Schaller, adaptive problem. Sometimes the answer is obvious and people are
2010; Saad, 2017; Tooby & Cosmides, 2005). But an important insight consciously aware of how a given tendency helps solve an adaptive
is that behavior has both proximate and ultimate causes. Behaviors are problem. But many times the answer is not obvious because a given
often driven by multiple causes, even if people are not always aware behavior can be concurrently driven by very different proximate rea-
of the ultimate reasons for their choices. For example, a person can sons and ultimate reasons.
be consciously motivated to buy a sporty luxury car because its ex-
pensive leather interior and peppy acceleration make him feel good
2.1.3 | Insight #3: Ultimate reasons beget predictions
(a proximate reason). At the same time, the person can be subcon-
about previously unknown causal processes at the
sciously motivated to buy that luxury car because owning such a car
proximate level
can increase his desirability as a potential mate and thereby enhances
his reproductive fitness (an ultimate reason) (Griskevicius et al., 2007; To take an evolutionarily informed approach to consumer research
Sundie et al., 2011). means using ultimate level explanations to generate predictions about
proximate level psychology. Evolutionary scholars use ultimate expla-
nations to develop hypotheses about causal mechanisms at the psy-
2.1.2 | Insight #2: Evolution is not the
chological level.
opposite of learning
To illustrate, let us revisit why men might be motivated to buy a
Just because behaviors are rooted in psychological adaptations sporty luxury car. While one proximate (cognitive) reason men buy
does not mean that the environment plays no role in the behavior. such cars is because they like the feel of the leather interior, one ul-
Consider again the fear of snakes. Fear of snakes is an adaptation, timate (adaptive) reason is because owning such products helps at-
as a programmatic series of studies has shown (Mineka & Öhman, tract mates (Griskevicius et al., 2007; Sundie et al., 2011). The ultimate
2002; Nesse, 1990; Öhman & Mineka, 2003). Poisonous snakes have reason was formulated by considering what the adaptive function of
persistently posed a threat throughout evolutionary history, lead- purchasing a sporty luxury car might be. Ancestral humans who were
ing humans to possess adaptations designed to solve this adaptive motivated to signal wealth (as luxury cars do) attracted more sexual
problem. partners and produced more offspring.
However, this does not mean that people are born with a hard- Once an ultimate explanation has been formulated by considering
wired fear of snakes. Instead, humans and other primates have spe- adaptive function, it can then be used to predict and test conditions
cialized learning mechanisms that prepare them to learn this particular that can enhance or dampen people’s desire for expensive sports cars,
association after they are born. As alluded to earlier, individuals rapidly including the evolutionarily informed psychological process that un-
condition to fear snake-like objects, often acquiring an intense fear in derlies it (e.g., ease of attracting a partner). If attracting mates is an
only one trial. For example, witnessing another person responds to a ultimate function of sports car purchasing, then men should be less
snake with fear just once can instill an intense phobia of snakes. The likely to purchase a sports car depending on whether mates are or
fear of snakes is extremely difficult to extinguish (it is easy to learn but are not easy to come by in their neighborhood (Durante, Griskevicius,
difficult to unlearn), and it can be traced to specialized neural circuitry Simpson, Cantú, & Tybur, 2012; Griskevicius, Cantú, & Vugt, 2012;
(Öhman & Mineka, 2001). Griskevicius, Tybur, et al., 2012). Note that ease of attracting a partner
The important takeaway is that evolution is not the opposite of and liking the feel of the leather interior are distinct but related psy-
learning, socialization, or culture. An evolutionary approach dissolves chological processes (i.e., they are not competing), with each providing
false dichotomies such as “nature versus nurture,” “innate versus valuable information to marketers.
learned,” and “biological versus cultural.” For instance, it does not
make sense to ask whether fear of snakes or love of cake is “evolved”
2.1.4 | Insight #4: Human psychology is adapted to
or “learned” or due to “nature” or “nurture.” Most psychological ad-
ancestral environments, not modern environments
aptations require some sort of environmental input for their activa-
tion. Because all behaviors are produced by the brain, and because All human ancestors were nomadic hunter-gatherers who lived in
the brain evolved via natural selection, all behaviors are biologically roving small bands of about 50–100 individuals, many of whom were
influenced and have some evolutionary component. members of a few kin groups (Dunbar, 1993, 1998). For many thou-
In summary, all human behaviors include an evolutionary explana- sands of years, human environments were fairly stable and natural
tion. It does not make sense to ask whether a behavior is evolutionary selection shaped human psychology and behavior to solve adaptive
or not. Asking such a question is the equivalent of pitting Tinbergen’s problems specifically in this kind of environment. After the dawn of
|
8       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

agriculture, human environments began to change rapidly and modern ancestral women, modern women take advantage of tools available
environments now change at lightning speed. This creates a problem in the modern environment that can serve to make them more at-
because brain evolution takes many, many thousands of years. The tractive. Most consumer products and services are tools that people
result is that people today interact with their present-day world using use to serve adaptive ends.
brains that evolved to confront ancestral problems (Nesse & Williams,
1994; Ornstein & Ehrlich, 1989).
2.1.5 | Insight #5: Evolutionary theory is a
Although our Stone Age brains are designed to produce adap-
meta-theory, not one single theory
tive behaviors in the ancestral environment, this does not mean
that they will always produce adaptive behaviors today. Some of Although the unifying tenet of evolutionary theory is the process of
the behaviors, especially consumer behaviors, we observe today evolution—the passing of genes that aid survival and reproduction on
are by-products of psychological mechanisms that were adaptive in to future generations—a common misconception about an evolution-
the ancestral environment. Consider modern eating behavior. We ary perspective is that it relies on one single theory. This is false. In
evolved in a world where calories and nutrients—particularly those the same way that consumer researchers do not base all predictions
foods high in the sugar and fat necessary for survival—were scarce. on a single theory called “social science theory” or “consumer theory,”
This produced a strong desire and taste for foods high in sugar and an evolutionary approach is not based on a single theory called “evo-
fat. But whereas this desire was adaptive in our past environment lutionary theory.” Instead, natural selection is a meta-theory that en-
because it motivated our ancestors to work to procure such foods, compasses hundreds of different theories. Table 2 presents a small
this proclivity can have downsides in the modern world of afford- sampling of evolutionarily informed theories about various domains
able caloric abundance. Put simply, the desire to eat more cookies (for more details, see Saad, 2007, 2017).
than carrots is an adaptation. But, instead of producing adaptive In the section that follows, we examine how an evolutionary lens
outcomes, it can instead lead to obesity and type 2 diabetes in our has been applied to several areas of marketing research, leading to
modern environment (Hu et al., 2001; Meyer et al., 2000; Srinivasan, novel predictions and a deeper understanding of consumer psychol-
Viswanad, Asrat, Kaul, & Ramarao, 2005). ogy. The research areas below are not intended to be an exhaustive
There are other adaptive malfunctions created by a mismatch be- list, but rather examples of how an evolutionary approach has been
tween ancestral and current environments. For example, our ancestors used as a prescriptive tool to generate testable predictions about con-
did not have television or the internet. If they saw a person come into sumer behavior.
their living space every day, this person was likely a friend, mate, or
family member. In the modern world, however, people come daily into
3 | CORE E VOLUT IO NARY THEOR IE S
our living space via television and social media. Although we obviously
APPLIE D TO ESTABLISHE D ARE AS OF
“know” that such people are not people who are physically present or
CO NSUM ER RESEARCH
have much impact in our lives, at some level our brain continues to
classify these people as “friends” or “family” (Barkow, 1989, 1992; De
3.1 | Risk preference
Backer, Nelissen, Vyncke, Braeckman, & McAndrew, 2007; Kanazawa,
2002). We then react to these people in much the same way we would
3.1.1 | Established knowledge
a close friend or kin. We end up wanting to know gossip about these
“friends,” even though we are unlikely to ever meet them, and we can Risk is an inherent part of the decision process. Landmark theo-
even come to care deeply about people that do not have much in- ries of risk suggest that risk perception—the extent to which
fluence on our daily lives. Just ask the producers of the tabloid pro- people are more or less willing to take on risk—is subject to bias
gram TMZ or the publishers of People—a celebrity and human-interest (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979; Read, Loewenstein, Rabin, Keren,
magazine that has the largest audience of any magazine in the United & Laibson, 1999; Thaler & Johnson, 1990). One consistent find-
States. ing in the decision literature is that people are loss averse, with
For a researcher using an evolutionary lens, it is therefore use- losses looming much larger psychologically than equivalent gains
ful to think about the kind of environment in which our ancestors (Ariely, Huber, & Wertenbroch, 2005; Camerer, 2005; Novemsky
evolved and for which our psychology is geared. As in the case of & Kahneman, 2005; Zhang & Fishbach, 2005). Consumer research
celebrity gossip, our ancestral psychology can sometimes be “hi- has explored how risk aversion strengthens and weakens depend-
jacked” by modern contraptions. This is especially true when con- ent upon factors such as prior wins and losses, interpreting biases
sidering the ultimate reasons people buy consumer products—which in risk preference as irrational (Gneezy & Potters, 1997; Liu, Wang,
did not exist in ancestral environments. Sometimes the reason is & Zhao, 2010; Russell & Thaler, 1985; Shiv, Loewenstein, Bechara,
straightforward as is the case for food. But, take lipstick for example. Damasio, & Damasio, 2005). An evolutionary perspective, however,
Many women desire, purchase, and wear lipstick. Of course lipstick suggests that loss aversion may be an adaptive bias that helped hu-
was not available to our human ancestors who lived thousands of mans solve survival-related ancestral challenges. In the following
years ago. But, the desire to enhance one’s attractiveness—a trait sections, we consider how a particular theory of selection can aid in
that is universally valued in mates and friends—is adaptive. Just like understanding when people are likely to tolerate more or less risk.
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      9

T A B L E   2   Sampling of evolutionarily informed theories

Theory Domain Sample insight Key references

Kin selection Interactions between People help family because of shared genes Hamilton (1963)
family members
Trivers-Willard How parents treat Harsh conditions lead parents to favor daughters Trivers and Willard (1973)
hypothesis sons and daughters over sons
Parent-offspring Interactions between Children seek to extract more resources than Maestripieri (2002), Trivers (1974)
conflict parents and children parents are willing to give
Reciprocal altruism Interactions between People help nonfamily to get rewards from them Hamilton (1964), Trivers (1971)
nonrelatives later
Indirect reciprocity Interactions between People help nonfamily to build a good reputation Alexander (1987), Nowak and Sigmund
nonrelatives (1998)
Sexual selection Gender differences Men and women differ in competitiveness Andersson (1994), Emlen and Oring (1977),
Trivers (1972)
Parental investment Gender differences Women are choosier than men in sex partners Trivers (1972)
theory
Tend and befriend Gender differences Men and women behave differently under stress Taylor et al. (2000)
Paternity uncertainty Gender differences Fathers are more likely to mistreat their children Møller (2000), Westneat and Sherman
than mothers (1993)
Sexual strategies Mating and Men and women differ in what they seek in a Buss and Schmitt (1993)
theory relationships mate
Strategic pluralism Mating and People can follow very different types of mating Gangestad and Simpson (2000)
relationships strategies
Attachment theory Mating and Bond between mother and infant influences Bowlby (1969), Simpson and Belsky (2008)
relationships bonds with others later in life
Costly signaling theory Costly show-off People spend a lot of resources on signals to Miller (2000), Zahavi and Zahavi (1997)
behavior ensure that signals are honest
Fundamental motives Context effects on People’s evolutionary goals change depending Griskevicius and Kenrick (2013), Kenrick,
behavior on situation Griskevicius, et al. (2010), Kenrick,
Neuberg, et al. (2010)
Environmental Differences between Some tendencies adaptive in ancestral environ- Nesse and Williams (1994), Ornstein and
mismatch ancestral and current ments are maladaptive today Ehrlich (1989)
environment
Life history theory Individual differences Childhood environment calibrates people to Chisholm (1993), Kaplan and Gangestad
differ in adaptive ways (2005), Roff (2002), Stearns (1992)
Error management Errors in judgment and People make errors in adaptive ways Haselton and Buss (2000), Haselton and
theory decision making Nettle (2006)
Darwinian gastronomy Food preferences People prefer spicier foods in hotter climates Billings and Sherman (1999)
Adaptive memory Memory People have better memory for survival-related Nairne and Pandeirada (2008)
theory content
Ovulatory shift Hormones Women’s behavior changes during the ovulatory Gangestad and Thornhill (1998)
hypothesis phase of the cycle

The fundamental motives framework maintains that the specific


3.1.2 | Theory of selection to extend knowledge
ancestral social challenges faced by humans map onto fundamental
The fundamental motives framework (Table 2) proposes that there motivational systems that function to help solve each challenge. A
are a number of distinct evolutionary challenges that had to be sur- fundamental motive can be activated or primed by external or internal
mounted to achieve reproductive success. Like all other animals, at cues indicating threats or opportunities related to a specific evolu-
a base level our ancestors needed nourishment and shelter. But, our tionary challenge (Kenrick, Griskevicius, et al., 2010). For example, the
ancestors also faced recurrent social challenges (Ackerman & Kenrick, mate acquisition system can be activated by interacting with a desir-
2008; Griskevicius & Kenrick, 2013; Kenrick, Griskevicius, et al., 2010). able member of the opposite sex, or by being in the same room with
These fundamental ancestral challenges included: (1) evading physical such a person, being exposed to an image involving such a person,
harm, (2) avoiding disease, (3) making friends, (4) attaining status, (5) or merely imagining a desirable romantic encounter. The system can
acquiring a mate, (6) keeping a mate, and (7) caring for family. also be activated when a person is confronted with a decision that
|
10       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

concerns potential mates, as opposed to a decision that pertains to Women find risk-taking desirable in men because it signals traits
family, status, disease, affiliation, or danger. When a fundamental mo- such as social dominance, confidence, and ambition, all of which can
tivational system has been activated, it produces a specific set of con- lead to benefits such as more money, status, and protection (Buss,
sequences for attention, memory, cognition, and preferences (Kenrick, 1989; Li, Bailey, Kenrick, & Linsenmeier, 2002). Over the course of
Neuberg, Griskevicius, Becker, & Schaller, 2010; Neuberg, Kenrick, & evolutionary history, some degree of risk-taking helped increase men’s
Schaller, 2011). This coordinated cascade of responses functions to chances of attracting women (Wilson & Daly, 1985). This suggests
help solve the ultimate problem associated with the currently active that the mate acquisition system—facing the challenge of acquiring a
system. The activation of the mate acquisition system, for example, mate—may be the Achilles’ heel of risk aversion. Consistent with this
leads a person to prefer and seek products that facilitate achieving idea, research shows that activating the mate acquisition system can
the ultimate need of acquiring a mate. Alternatively, the presence of increase risk-taking and cause loss aversion to vanish (Baker & Maner,
a threat to personal safety activates the self-protection system and 2009; Li et al., 2012). For example, leading men to believe they will
leads a person to prefer and seek products that facilitate achieving the soon meet a single, attractive woman increases their risk preference,
ultimate need of self-preservation. Because it is difficult to attract a resulting in the decision to gamble for a larger payoff (Baker & Maner,
mate while protecting one’s self from harm, the fundamental motiva- 2009). And, for men, triggering the motive to attract a mate leads loss
tional systems are often activated in isolation and lead to very differ- aversion to reverse itself, leading gains to loom larger than losses (Li
ent behavioral outcomes. et al., 2012).
Research finds that mating motivations increase men’s risk-taking,
but does not influence women’s risk-taking. However, applying an
3.1.3 | Example of research application
evolutionary lens in a slightly new direction reveals a distinct, but
Several recent studies have applied the fundamental motives frame- parallel story for women. To determine how the mate acquisition sys-
work to study risk preferences (Baker & Maner, 2008, 2009; Hill & tem might influence women’s risk-taking, it is important to consider
Durante, 2011; Li, Kenrick, Griskevicius, & Neuberg, 2012), demon- what men find desirable when choosing a partner. When it comes to
strating that people’s risk preferences are dependent on which par- mate selection, whereas women across cultures place a high value on
ticular social challenge is at the fore. Think about the kind of world economic resources and status, men across cultures value youth and
our ancestors lived in thousands of years ago. This was a world physical attractiveness (Buss, 1989; Li et al., 2002; Symons, 1979).
full of unpredictability. The environment of our ancestors did not Therefore, when it comes to risk-taking, mating motivations should
contain a Doppler radar to predict weather, farmers (much less gro- enhance women’s risk-taking within the realm of what men find de-
cery stores!), medicine, or adequate shelter. It did contain multiple sirable—physical appearance. A series of studies have found that acti-
predators, war-faring enemies, and disease. In this kind of world, it vating the mate acquisition system leads women, but not men, to take
becomes difficult to predict whether there will be access to food, increased risks to enhance their appearance (e.g., tanning, diet pills;
shelter, and other resources tomorrow. Our ancestors who were, on Hill & Durante, 2011). This effect occurs because, when faced with
average, highly sensitive to losses were more likely to survive be- the challenge of attracting a mate, women experience decreased per-
cause they tended to be prudent with the resources they did have, ceptions of vulnerability to the negative effects of risky attractiveness-
protect them at all cost, and value certainty versus a gamble. Thus, enhancing behavior.
a bias toward risk aversion is likely an adaptation. In fact, recent re- This work provides a previously unknown understanding of deci-
search has found that activating the self-protection system makes sion biases and offers a blueprint for how marketers, managers, and
people particularly loss averse (Li et al., 2012). When people are led others can alter consumers’ decisions. The central implication of the
to believe that an enemy is afoot, the motivation to protect oneself fundamental motives framework is that a person’s preferences can
from danger enhances loss aversion. change quite dramatically depending on which motivational system is
However, other fundamental challenges faced by our ancestors currently active. This is because what constitutes adaptive behavior to
may have a very different effect on risk. Consider the following. In further one ultimate need may be very different from—and sometimes
1986, an action film called Top Gun quickly became one of the highest even completely opposing to—what is adaptive to further another. The
grossing movies of all time. In the film, Tom Cruise plays Maverick, a fundamental motives framework highlights the adaptive social prob-
young and reckless pilot. The film depicts Maverick (and his equally lems that the mind is geared to solve. It then shows how, why, and
reckless nemesis, Ice Man) rebuffing authority and taking unsanc- when people’s preferences and behavior change depending on which
tioned risks. Maverick’s risky aerial dogfights ultimately lead to his adaptive problem they are currently seeking to solve.
winning the best pilot spot at Top Gun and the heart of his leading lady
(not to mention the hearts of tens of millions of young women across
3.2 | Competition and luxury consumption
the globe). If Maverick were not a pilot, but instead an accountant who
saved prudently, was home for dinner at 6 p.m. every night, drove a
3.2.1 | Established knowledge
sedan, and avoided confrontation, it is fairly safe to say that nary a
movie poster would have graced a young girl’s wall. This is because People care about hierarchy and their relative position compared
when it comes to mate attraction, a taste for risk becomes an asset. with others (Frank, 1985, 1999; Veblen, 1899). When people make a
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      11

purchase, for example, they often place importance on getting some- an ovum or egg) and estrogen—the sex hormone active in women—is
thing better than what one’s peers have (Han, Nunes, & Drèze, 2010; high (Jones, 1997; Lipson & Ellison, 1996). And, unlike men, women’s
Ordabayeva & Chandon, 2010). This desire for relative advantage is sexual behavior can result in pregnancy only near ovulation (days 8–14
played out in the luxury goods market. Billions of dollars are spent of the cycle). It is perhaps then no surprise that women think about sex
each year on lavish goods such as luxury handbags and jewelry, with a little more during this time. Men, on the other hand, maintain high
women accounting for more than half (55%) of this luxury spending levels of their sex hormone—testosterone—because they are always
(D’Arpizio, Levato, Zito, & de Montgolfier, 2016; Mintel Report, 2011). producing sex cells that enable reproduction. Put another way, men
A large body of consumer research has examined the factors that mo- are always ovulating.
tivate people’s desire for such products. This research shows that Because over evolutionary history women could only reproduce
people desire luxury goods because it enhances self-esteem and well- when they ovulate, women’s motivations and behavior evolved
being (Belk, 1985, 1988; Berger & Heath, 2007; Douglas & Isherwood, to adaptively shift specifically at this time (Gangestad & Thornhill,
1978; Holt, 1998; Richins, 1987; Sivanathan & Pettit, 2010; Solomon, 1998; Gangestad, Thornhill, & Garver, 2002; Thornhill & Gangestad,
1983) and this sense of well-being occurs because luxury goods signal 2008). Supporting the ovulatory shift hypothesis, research has found
wealth or prestige to others (Berger & Ward, 2010; Han et al., 2010; that women experience an increase in sexual desire near ovulation
Rucker, Galinsky, & Dubois, 2012; Wernerfelt, 1990; Wilcox, Kim, & (Bullivant et al., 2004; Gangestad et al., 2002). Specifically, ovula-
Sen, 2009). This research is important because it provides an under- tion increases women’s sexual attraction toward men who possess
standing of the proximate reasons why people are willing to spend indicators of genetic fitness such as facial symmetry and attractive-
money on luxury products. But, it provides little insight into the un- ness, masculinity, and social dominance (Cantú et al., 2013; Durante,
derlying factors that guide these proximate level cognitions in the first Griskevicius, Simpson, Cantú, & Li, 2012; Durante, Griskevicius,
place. Simpson, Cantú, & Tybur, 2012; Gangestad, Garver-Apgar, Simpson,
Some research has used an evolutionary lens to examine the ul- & Cousins, 2007; Gangestad, Simpson, Cousins, Garver-Apgar, &
timate reasons why people desire luxury goods, finding that men are Christensen, 2004; Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Gangestad et al.,
motivated to acquire luxury products as a means to attract women 2005; Penton-Voak et al., 1999).
(Griskevicius et al., 2007; Sundie et al., 2011). However, this research
finds that women are not similarly motivated to acquire luxury prod-
3.2.3 | Example of research application
ucts to attract men. Yet, women account for more than half of the
spending on luxury goods. What, then, is the ultimate function under- The ovulatory shift hypothesis suggests that mating goals are particu-
lying women’s desire for luxury goods? Some research suggests that larly salient for women near ovulation. For example, when it comes to
women desire luxury goods because it signals her partner’s level of fashion, research shows that ovulation has a large effect on women’s
investment in the relationship and can deter other women from try- desire to look more attractive and dress in sexier outfits (Durante,
ing to steal a partner (Wang & Griskevicius, 2014). But, the answer to Griskevicius, Hill, Perilloux, & Li, 2011; Durante, Li, & Haselton, 2008;
this question is still far from clear. One way to examine the underlying Saad & Stenstrom, 2012). The desire to dress sexy at ovulation was
function of women’s luxury consumption is to turn to an evolutionary found to be related specifically to outcompeting other women for ac-
theory that focuses on women. cess to the best men available. For example, ovulation has the strong-
est effect on women’s desire for sexier clothing when women know
that there are many other attractive women in their local environment
3.2.2 | Theory of selection to extend knowledge
(Durante et al., 2011), that is, when there is lots of competition for
The ovulatory shift hypothesis (Table 2) posits that women should ex- mates.
perience a shift in mating-related motivation and behavior near ovu- Thus, if status competition and luxury consumption are related to
lation—the time during each ovulatory cycle when estrogen is high mating, the ovulatory shift hypothesis may shed light on women’s de-
and women are most fertile (Gangestad & Thornhill, 1998; Gangestad, sire for luxury products. Recent research drawing on this hypothesis
Thornhill, & Garver-Apgar, 2005). Most women’s ovulatory cycles proposed the idea that ovulation should not only enhance women’s
span about 28 days. Here is a thought experiment that helps explain mating motivations, but also women’s desire to compete with other
the ovulatory shift hypothesis. women for status (Durante, Griskevicius, Cantú, & Simpson, 2014).
Imagine a random man and woman. Now imagine 28 days. How This idea is consistent with research in nonhuman primates. For exam-
often do you think the desire to have sex will cross each of their ple, female rhesus monkeys become more aggressive and competitive
minds? It might seem logical to assume a main effect whereby the with other females (not males) during the ovulatory phase (Walker,
man thinks about sex a lot and the woman thinks about sex much Wilson, & Gordon, 1983; Wallen, 2000).
less for each of the 28 days. For the man, this would be roughly ac- Putting this lens to women’s desire for luxury goods, it was found
curate. But, not for the woman. The woman would likely think about that, near ovulation, women desire more expensive products and this
sex a little during days 1–7, a lot on days 8–14, and very little on days effect is related to increased competitive tendencies (Durante et al.,
15–28. Why would women think about sex more on days 8–14? This 2014; Kim, Durante, Griskevicius, & Nikiforidis, 2017). For example, in
is the time when women ovulate (produce a single sex cell known as one study, ovulating and nonovulating women made product choices
|
12       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

that could either maximize absolute gains or maximize gains relative to & Poynor, 2008; Hofmann et al., 2012; Laran & Janiszewski, 2010;
other women (Durante et al., 2014). The findings showed that ovula- Poynor & Haws, 2008; Redden & Haws, 2012; Righetti & Finkenauer,
tion made women more competitive with regard to other women. Near 2011; Wilcox, Kramer, & Sen, 2010). For instance, research has shown
ovulation, women were willing to accept lesser versions of a product (a that people often give in to temptation if desire exceeds willpower
$5,000 diamond ring in lieu of a $7,000 diamond ring) as long as they (Hoch & Loewenstein, 1991) and that willpower can be enhanced by
had better products than other women. And, ovulating women kept lowering cognitive load (Shiv & Fedorikhin, 1999), using mental budg-
more money for themselves in the Dictator Game rather than give it ets to designate number of indulgences (Krishnamurthy & Prokopec,
to another woman. Ovulating women also prefer luxury (compared to 2009), changing how the behavior is construed (Haws & Poynor, 2008;
nonluxury) products and conspicuous goods as a direct reflection of a Laran & Janiszewski, 2010), and weighting the potential negative con-
desire to outcompete rival women for status and access to mates (Kim sequences of temptations (Zhang, Huang, & Broniarczyk, 2009).
et al., 2017). When it comes to wanting multiple versions of products, While all people struggle with self-control efforts at some level,
ovulation enhances this, too (Durante & Arsena, 2014). there are individual differences in self-control (Haws, Davis, &
What about hormones and men’s luxury consumer behavior? Dholakia, 2016; Hofmann et al., 2012). People with high trait self-
Recall that men, especially men in their prime reproductive years, control are better able to control their thoughts and regulate behav-
think about sex fairly consistently because testosterone levels do ior (Muraven, Collins, Shiffman, & Paty, 2005), and research suggests
not rise and fall dramatically on a fixed, internal cycle as they do in that it may be possible for some people to increase their ability to
women. But, this does not mean that testosterone levels never vary exert self-control (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000; Muraven, Shmueli,
in men. Men’s testosterone levels can vary with age and time of day & Burkley, 2006). This research is important because a deeper under-
(van Anders, Goldey, & Bell, 2014; Dabbs, 1990), and also in response standing of self-control can uncover workarounds for impulsive be-
to external contexts such as winning or losing a competition (Apicella, haviors like over-eating and over-spending that can have vast negative
Dreber, & Mollerstrom, 2014; Booth, Shelley, Mazur, Tharp, & Kittok, consequences for both the individual and society.
1989), both of which can have implications for men’s luxury consump-
tion. For example, it has been found that men’s testosterone levels
3.3.2 | Theory of selection to extend knowledge
increase after driving a luxury sports car versus a family sedan, and
when seeing another man displays his wealth through products (Saad One theory of selection that can illuminate our understanding of self-
& Vongas, 2009). This is consistent with men’s testosterone respond- control is life history theory (Chisholm, 1993; Kaplan & Gangestad,
ing to status competition signaled via consumer luxury goods. 2005; Roff, 2002; Stearns, 1992; see Table 2). Life history theory is
In addition to hormones regulating mating, there are various other a theory of individual differences. An evolutionary perspective high-
hormones that influence people’s behavior in other domains. For ex- lights that many important individual differences between people are
ample, cortisol drives our responses to stressful situations (Dickerson linked to a person’s life history strategy (Ellis, Figueredo, Brumbach, &
& Kemeny, 2004), which can influence consumer behavior (Durante & Schlomer, 2009; Griskevicius et al., 2013). Across species, life history
Laran, 2016). Oxytocin and vasopressin drive our desire to bond with strategies vary on a fast–slow continuum. Some individuals follow
family and friends (Young & Insel, 2002). Each of these hormones is faster strategies, and others follow slower strategies. A fast life his-
likely to influence consumer behavior in important ways. Hormones tory strategy is characterized by accelerated maturation and very little
are the puppeteers of our social motivations and behaviors, with each long-term planning. A slow life history strategy, on the other hand, is
hormone responsible for a particular motive (e.g., mating, protection, characterized by delayed maturation and a focus on long-term plan-
bonding; Nelson, 2005). They are internal drivers of our cognitive op- ning. Consequently, life history strategies are related to important dif-
erating systems. Thus, understanding how hormones influence behav- ferences in mating. Fast strategists start puberty at earlier ages, have
ior provides a unique window into the psychological underpinnings of sex at earlier ages, and have more sexual partners. By contrast, slow
consumer behavior. strategists tend to start puberty at later ages, have sex later in life, and
have fewer sexual partners, preferring monogamous relationships.
Research shows that the nature of one’s environment early in life is
3.3 | Self-control and temporal preferences
a critical determinant of adult life history strategy (Griskevicius, Tybur,
Delton, & Robertson, 2011; Griskevicius et al., 2013). Similar to how
3.3.1 | Established knowledge
a person’s childhood environment is a critical period for acquiring a
Self-control is defined as foregoing immediate impulses and de- language, a person’s childhood environment is a critical period for
sire to serve long-term self-regulatory goals (e.g., dieting, saving calibrating their life history strategy (Belsky, Schlomer, & Ellis, 2012;
money; Baumeister, Vohs, & Tice, 2007; Hoch & Loewenstein, 1991; Brumbach, Figueredo, & Ellis, 2009; Simpson, Griskevicius, Kuo, Sung,
Hofmann, Baumeister, Förster, & Vohs, 2012; Hofmann & Van Dillen, & Collins, 2012; Simpson, Griskevicius, & Rothman, 2012). Resource-
2012). A significant body of consumer research has examined how abundant environments such as higher-SES tend to be safer and more
self-control efforts impact consumption, especially when it comes stable, making a slow strategy adaptive because such environments
to eating and spending (Fennis, Janssen, & Vohs, 2008; Fitzsimons, are relatively predictable and are not expected to change dramatically
Nunes, & Williams, 2007; Galak, Kruger, & Loewenstein, 2012; Haws over time. By contrast, resource-deprived environments such as lower
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      13

SES calibrate a faster strategy (Ellis et al., 2009; Mittal & Griskevicius, & Furstenberg, 2013; Lino, Kuczynski, Rodriguez, & Schap, 2017). In
2014). Resource-deprived environments tend to be harsh and unpre- 1960, the average cost of raising a child to age 17 was $25,229 for a
dictable, making a fast strategy adaptive because in such constantly married couple in the middle of the income distribution, whereas by
fluctuating environments, it is difficult to know what tomorrow will 2015 that cost had risen to $233,610 (Lino et al., 2017).
bring or if tomorrow will come at all. Some consumer research has investigated how women’s con-
sumer behavior differs from men’s (Chiger, 2001; Meyers-Levy &
Loken, 2015; Mitchell & Walsh, 2004; Nikolova & Lamberton, 2016;
3.3.3 | Example of research application
Rich & Jain, 1968; Seock & Bailey, 2008; Young Kim & Kim, 2004),
Research stemming from life history theory has examined how peo- and considerable consumer research has investigated how advertising,
ple’s childhood environment influences their self-control in adult family, and peers influence children’s preferences (Boland, Connell,
eating behavior. For example, people who grew up in poor versus & Erickson, 2012; Carlson & Grossbart, 1988; Childers & Rao, 1992;
wealthy environments were given food to snack on, while research- Churchill & Moschis, 1979; Goldberg & Gorn, 1978; John, 1999;
ers observed how much food people ate (Hill, Prokosch, DelPriore, John & Whitney, 1986; Moschis & Churchill, 1978; Peracchio, 1992;
Griskevicius, & Kramer, 2016). For people who grew up in wealthy Robertson & Rossiter, 1974). Less research has focused specifically on
environments, snack consumption depended on whether they were understanding women’s consumer motivations and how parents make
currently hungry: People who were hungry ate more snacks while spending decisions on behalf of their children. Because family and
people who were not hungry ate fewer snacks. By contrast, current women’s role in resource sharing have long been a part of human life,
levels of hunger played almost no role in snack consumption for peo- an evolutionary perspective can be useful for understanding behavior
ple who grew up in economically deprived environments: If food was in these domains. Table 2 highlights many evolutionarily informed the-
presented to them, they ate it regardless of whether they were hungry ories central to the domains of women, kin, and family.
or not. For people who grow up in unpredictable and resource-scarce
environments, it is generally adaptive to not pass up the opportunity
3.4.2 | Theory of selection to extend knowledge
to consume as much as possible. After all, such environments made it
uncertain when another opportunity will arise. There are several evolutionary theories that can inform our under-
Research has also found that fast and slow strategies are associ- standing of the consumer behavior of women and families. Two theo-
ated with vastly different psychologies and orientations to decision ries that have been particularly illuminating in this domain are parental
making, such as an individual’s propensity to experience enhanced investment theory (Trivers, 1972) and the Trivers–Willard hypothesis
desire for immediate versus delayed rewards. Whereas fast strategists (Trivers & Willard, 1973).
tend to be short-term opportunists and take immediate benefits with Parental investment theory proposes that because female mam-
little regard for long-term consequences, slow strategists tend to be mals invest more heavily in reproduction and the care of offspring
long-term planners who delay immediate gratification to increase fu- than males do (i.e., an obligatory period of 9 months gestation and
ture payoffs (Griskevicius et al., 2011, 2013). Although the ability to years of nursing at a minimum), this differential parental investment
delay gratification is associated with many positive outcomes, such as will lead to specific sex differences in behavior (Trivers, 1972). These
educational attainment and lifetime income (Mischel, 2014), not de- differences are born out in sex differences in mating strategies and be-
laying gratification may be an adaptive strategy in environments that havior. Unlike females, males’ reproductive success is not contingent
are unpredictable and in which payoffs are uncertain (Ellis et al., 2009; on a period of obligatory parental investment, but has been primarily
Griskevicius et al., 2011). In a predictable environment, it often advan- dependent on their ability to gain sexual access to fertile females. On
tageous to wait for a larger outcome that is available later. However, the other hand, because of their higher obligatory parental investment
when the environment is unpredictable, options that are available now in offspring, females’ reproductive success has been strongly depen-
may not be available in the future. dent on their ability to secure a partner able to invest resources in
themselves and their offspring (Buss, 1994; Symons, 1979; Trivers,
1972). In other words, across human history, women of reproductive
3.4 | The consumer behavior of women and families
age were busy with pregnancies and nursing children, and thus bene-
fited greatly from securing a partner who could help find food, shelter,
3.4.1 | Established knowledge
and other resources for her and her children. Accordingly, research-
Women and parents of young children are powerful consumers. ers have predicted and found that men are more promiscuous than
Women currently handle the bulk of purchase decisions for consumer women and value qualities related to fertility when choosing mates
goods, including the purchase of big-ticket items such as homes and (e.g., physical attractiveness), and women are less promiscuous and
cars (Silverstein & Sayre, 2009). It is estimated that women will control value qualities related to a potential mate’s provisioning ability (e.g.,
two-thirds of consumer wealth in the United States within the next status and money; Buss, 1989; Li et al., 2002; Li & Kenrick, 2006).
10 years (Nielson, 2013). Parents are another consumer group whose Another theory, the Trivers–Willard hypothesis, also predicts a sex
spending continues to grow. Parents across all social strata today are difference, but instead focuses on how both men and women will bias
spending more on their children than in previous generations (Kornrich investment in their children depending on the biological sex of the
|
14       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

child. The hypothesis predicts that parents should invest more in one recessions increased women’s desire for products that enhance attrac-
gender over the other depending on whether conditions are currently tiveness (Hill, Rodeheffer, Griskevicius, Durante, & White, 2012)—the
good or bad (Trivers & Willard, 1973). When times are bad and ac- first experimental demonstration of the lipstick effect. The lipstick ef-
cess to food and other resources is limited, parents should invest more fect was found to be driven by women’s desire to attract mates with
heavily in female offspring. The logic for this idea stems from mamma- resources and depended on the perceived mate attraction function
lian sex differences in reproduction, which we discuss next. served by these products—two important pieces of knowledge about
Males and females are expected to produce the same number women’s consumer motivations that would not have been uncovered
of offspring on average, a fact that must be mathematically true in a through any other lens.
sexually reproducing species. But despite producing the same average Another recent line of research has drawn on the Trivers–Willard
number of offspring, males and females have different variances in off- hypothesis to examine the consumer behavior of parents. Recall that
spring production. Among mammals, including humans, females have this hypothesis predicts that investment in children should differ de-
lower variance than males in the number of offspring they produce pending on the child’s reproductive value, which is the child’s ability
(e.g., recall women’s obligatory investment of gestation and nursing to convert parental resources into reproductive success by having
that prohibit the production of another child within her body; Clutton- children of their own (Daly & Wilson, 1988). The Trivers–Willard hy-
Brock & Albon, 1982; Leimar, 1996). Whereas nearly all females will pothesis suggests that parents should invest more in one sex over the
find a partner and have a moderate number of offspring, there are other depending on whether resources are scarce or abundant (Trivers
some males who will attract many partners (think Mick Jagger who is & Willard, 1973). In one of the first experimental demonstrations of
still producing children with new partners at 72—a feat biologically im- the Trivers–Willard hypothesis, a series of studies using both real
possible for all women) and some males who will attract zero. So, when and experimentally manipulated perceptions of the economy showed
times are tough, it will be more difficult for both males and females to that poor economic conditions lead people to spend more money
find mates and reproduce, but it will have a much more catastrophic on daughters compared to sons (Durante, Griskevicius, Redden, &
effect on males—whose reproductive success is more strongly tied to Edward White, 2015). Consistent with the Trivers–Willard hypothe-
their status (as Mick Jagger’s continued popularity with young women sis, the effect was driven by beliefs about which child will have more
attests). Thus, investing more resources in female offspring in tough children. This research is just the tip of the iceberg in the examination
times is an evolutionary risk management strategy to ensure that there of how parents make spending decisions on behalf of their children
are some grandchildren in future generations. Put more simply, if Mick (Nikiforidis, Durante, Redden, & Griskevicius, 2018). Future research is
Jagger’s fame and wealth were to disappear, so too would the women poised to examine the many ways the adaptations forged during our
who are attracted to him. When a woman’s fame and wealth disap- ancestral history shape modern family spending.
pear, she is still able to find men to mate with.

4 | STRE NGTHS, LIMITAT IO NS,


3.4.3 | Example of research application AND CO NCLUSION
An evolutionary perspective drawing on parental investment theory
4.1 | Strengths
predicts that there are fundamentally different goals in a relationship
for men and women. Conditions that alter the quality and availabil- Incorporating an evolutionary perspective provides several benefits.
ity of mates should shift the value of certain consumer products that First, it provides consumer researchers with a key to unlock hypoth-
relationship partners use to attract a mate (Durante, Griskevicius, eses about behavior that might have never been generated. Indeed,
Simpson, Cantú, & Tybur, 2012). For men, this could include products many of the findings discussed in this chapter were generated be-
that signal their commitment and willingness and ability to invest re- cause an evolutionary perspective provided ideas about new effects
sources in a partner (e.g., jewelry, exotic vacations, designer clothes; or new moderators. For example, an evolutionary perspective on
Griskevicius, Cantú, et al., 2012; Griskevicius, Tybur, et al., 2012). For emotions highlights that different emotions serve different adaptive
women, this could include products that signal youth and fertility (e.g., functions (Griskevicius et al., 2009; Neufeld & Griskevicius, 2014).
botox, cosmetics, diet plans; Hill & Durante, 2011). Using the theorized function of each emotion as a starting point, it is
One condition that alters the quality of partners women have to possible to derive novel hypotheses regarding how and why specific
choose from is an economic recession. Good men are just harder to find emotions should influence psychology and behavior (Griskevicius,
when jobs and money are scarce. In fact, for many years, there existed a Shiota, & Neufeld, 2010). For instance, previous research shows that
phenomena known as the lipstick effect—recessions appear to increase positive emotions produce a “rose-colored glasses” effect by making
women’s spending on cosmetics (Nelson, 2001). But, why? It was as- products more attractive. However, different positive emotions have
sumed that women were spending more on trinkets like lipstick to make unique effects and specific positive emotions can actually decrease
themselves feel better in difficult times. But parental investment theory the desirability of some products (Griskevicius, Shiota, & Nowlis,
suggested that there might be more going on than meets the eye. 2010). Consider the emotion of pride. The evolutionary function of
A series of studies examined both historical and experimental data pride is to motivate public displays to draw positive attention to one-
that manipulated people’s perceptions of the economy, and found that self. Consistent with this function of pride, feelings of pride enhance
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      15

the attractiveness of products useful for public positive differentia- sunlight; lived in a biotic environment with predators; were predated
tion. However, pride does not enhance the attractiveness of prod- on; bled when wounded; were incapacitated from injuries; were vul-
ucts used primarily around the house—and sometimes even decreases nerable to a large variety of parasites and pathogens; had deleterious
their desirability. Thus, rather than positive emotions always produc- recessives rendering them subject to inbreeding depression if they
ing a rose-colored glasses effect, different positive emotions produce mated with siblings” (Tooby & Cosmides, 2005, pp. 23–24).
an emotion-specific and evolutionarily functional pattern of percep- Scientists also know that fertilization occurred internally within
tions and evaluations. females, not within males; that females, not males, bore the meta-
Second, an evolutionary perspective can help explain and build bolic costs of breastfeeding; that our ancestors engaged in hunting
on well-established behavioral phenomena. Take, for example, clas- for at least the past one million years; that our ancestors lived in
sic findings on conformity. People are found to be heavily influenced small groups, ranging in size from a few dozen to 150; and that our
by the actions and beliefs of others, and often pattern their own ancestors made and used tools for hunting, gathering, cooking, and
behavior and choices after those around them (Asch, 1956; Cialdini, warfare. We also know that bipedal locomotion, extended childhood,
Reno, & Kallgren, 1990; Sherif, 1936). When considering conformity long-term pair bonds, biparental investment, and relatively concealed
through the lens of the fundamental motives framework (Table 2), ovulation distinguish our ancestors from their closest primate relative,
it was found that conformity effects are even stronger when peo- the chimpanzee.
ple are motivated to protect themselves from danger (Griskevicius, Evolutionary psychologists also use evidence from anthropology,
Goldstein, Mortensen, Cialdini, & Kenrick, 2006). However, activat- archeology, primatology, comparative biology, and ethology to eluci-
ing mating motives led to a reversal of classic conformity effects, date some aspects of an otherwise scientifically uncertain ancestral
leading men to go against the group when they were motivated to past. For example, the paleontological evidence is rife with ancient
attract a mate. caches of skulls and skeletons showing patterned lethal injuries, cor-
An evolutionary perspective also helps avoid isolated islands of responding in size and shape to ancient weapons discovered in the
research by helping scholars think about the bigger scientific picture. vicinity. When combined with cave art depictions of fighting and many
For example, researchers working only at one level of analysis (such other sources of evidence, the cumulative findings yield reasonable
as by focusing only on Tinbergen’s first question) can end up building inferences that human warfare was a potent hostile force of nature
isolated islands of research that do not have much connection to other for human ancestors, that males were far more often perpetrators and
research. However, by thinking about multiple levels of analysis—and victims of homicide than females, and that the majority of ancestral
by realizing that all findings have to logically fit together across differ- attackers were right handed (Duntley & Buss, 2012).
ent types of explanations—researchers are more likely to do research In summary, although convergent evidence from independent data
that better connects to the work of other scholars within and outside sources yields especially reasonable inferences about some past selec-
their field. Evolutionarily informed theories provide an underlying logic tion pressures, evolutionary psychology, and indeed the entire field of
that helps connect all the pieces of the puzzle of human behavior. psychology, will always be limited by incomplete knowledge of past
selection pressures.

4.2 | Limitations
4.3 | Conclusion
Although an evolutionary approach provides multiple benefits, it is
also important to highlight its limitations. One limitation is that an Evolutionary psychology is not a field or an area of research. Instead,
evolutionary perspective has difficulty explaining certain phenomena. an evolutionary perspective is a broad way of thinking about behavior
For example, consider homosexual orientation, which is a Darwinian in any domain. Because all human behaviors include an evolutionary
paradox. Exclusive homosexual orientation seems to defy evolution- explanation, an evolutionary perspective can be useful for generating
ary logic as it seems to fail to increase an individual’s reproductive suc- theories and ideas for any area of consumer behavior (Saad, 2007,
cess. Although several evolutionary hypotheses have been proposed 2013, 2017). For example, an evolutionary perspective is useful for
for homosexuality, none has received empirical support thus far (e.g., helping make sense of the many errors and biases people make, in-
Bobrow & Bailey, 2001). cluding helping predict when and why people will make specific types
Another limitation is that we lack detailed knowledge of many se- of errors (Griskevicius & Kenrick, 2013). This approach is also highly
lection pressures that humans faced over the millions of years of their relevant for understanding environmental behavior and conservation.
evolution. We do not possess a time machine or a video of deep time By better understanding how our evolved tendencies contribute to
that would reveal in precise detail all of the selective events over mil- modern-day problems, we can create better interventions to solve
lions of years that have led to the current design of the human body these problems (Griskevicius, Cantú, et al., 2012). Whatever the topic,
and mind. Nonetheless, this limitation is not total. There is a surpris- an evolutionary perspective can help researchers think more broadly
ingly abundant amount of information about the human ancestral about that topic and generate novel ideas.
environment that we do know to a reasonable degree of certainty On university campuses, there is a long-standing division be-
(Confer et al., 2010). For example, ancestral humans “had two sexes; tween the social and the natural sciences. Generations of research-
chose mates; had color vision calibrated to the spectral properties of ers studying ecology and biology have been physically divided from
|
16       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

those studying psychology and economics. Although scholars on both Barrett, H. C. (2012). A hierarchical model of the evolution of human brain
sides are studying behavior, this wall has led people on one side to be specializations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the
United States of America, 109(Supplement 1), 10733–10740. https://
generally unaware of the theory and research on the other side. This
doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201898109
division has spawned continued isolation, leading various fields to be- Barrett, H. C., & Kurzban, R. (2006). Modularity in cognition: Framing
come ever more insulated. the debate. Psychological Review, 113(3), 628. https://doi.org/
It is time to tear down this wall. Charles Darwin wrote that, “In 10.1037/0033-295X.113.3.628
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of
the distant future, I see open fields for more important researches.
self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351–355.
Psychology will be based on a new foundation” (Darwin, 1859). It has https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00534.x
been over 100 years, but Darwin’s vision is beginning to be realized Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects of living in the material
by scholars across the behavioral sciences. The current generation of world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12(3), 265–280. https://doi.
org/10.1086/208515
scholars is poised to tear down this wall by incorporating evolution-
Belk, R. W. (1988). Possessions and the extended self. Journal of Consumer
arily informed thinking into their research. As the bridge between the Research, 15(2), 139–168. https://doi.org/10.1086/209154
natural and social sciences becomes stronger, future generations of Belsky, J., Schlomer, G. L., & Ellis, B. J. (2012). Beyond cumulative risk:
researchers are poised to build a truly interdisciplinary science of con- Distinguishing harshness and unpredictability as determinants of par-
enting and early life history strategy. Developmental Psychology, 48(3),
sumer behavior.
662. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024454
Berger, J., & Heath, C. (2007). Where consumers diverge from others:
Identity signaling and product domains. Journal of Consumer Research,
REFERENCES
34(2), 121–134. https://doi.org/10.1086/519142
Ackerman, J. M., & Kenrick, D. T. (2008). The costs of benefits: Help-refusals Berger, J., & Ward, M. (2010). Subtle signals of inconspicuous consump-
highlight key trade-offs of social life. Personality and Social Psychology tion. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(4), 555–569. https://doi.
Review, 12, 118–140. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308315700 org/10.1086/655445
Alcock, J. (2001). The triumph of sociobiology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Billings, J., & Sherman, P. W. (1999). Darwinian gastronomy: Why we use
Press. spices. BioScience, 49, 453–463.
Alcock, J. (2005). Animal behavior: An evolutionary approach. Sunderland, Bobrow, D., & Bailey, J. M. (2001). Is male homosexuality maintained via kin
MA: Sinauer Associates. selection? Evolution and Human Behavior, 22(5), 361–368. https://doi.
Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. Piscataway, NJ: org/10.1016/S1090-5138(01)00074-5
Transaction Publishers. Boland, W. A., Connell, P. M., & Erickson, L. M. (2012). Children’s response
van Anders, S. M., Goldey, K. L., & Bell, S. N. (2014). Measurement of tes- to sales promotions and their impact on purchase behavior. Journal
tosterone in human sexuality research: Methodological considerations. of Consumer Psychology, 22(2), 272–279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 43(2), 231–250. https://doi.org/10.1007/ jcps.2011.04.003
s10508-013-0123-z Booth, A., Shelley, G., Mazur, A., Tharp, G., & Kittok, R. (1989). Testosterone,
Andersson, M. B. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton, NJ: Princeton and winning and losing in human competition. Hormones and Behavior,
University Press. 23(4), 556–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/0018-506X(89)90042-1
Apicella, C. L., Dreber, A., & Mollerstrom, J. (2014). Salivary testosterone Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment. New York, NY:
change following monetary wins and losses predicts future finan- Basic Books.
cial risk-taking. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 39, 58–64. https://doi. Brumbach, B. H., Figueredo, A. J., & Ellis, B. J. (2009). Effects of harsh and
org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.09.025 unpredictable environments in adolescence on development of life his-
Ariely, D., Huber, J., & Wertenbroch, K. (2005). When do losses loom larger tory strategies. Human Nature, 20(1), 25–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/
than gains? Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 134–138. https://doi.org/ s12110-009-9059-3
10.1509/jmkr.42.2.134.62283 Bullivant, S. B., Sellergren, S. A., Stern, K., Spencer, N. A., Jacob, S., Mennella,
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: A minority of J. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2004). Women’s sexual experience during
one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General the menstrual cycle: Identification of the sexual phase by noninvasive
and Applied, 70(9), 1–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093718 measurement of luteinizing hormone. The Journal of Sex Research, 41,
Baker, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2008). Risk-taking as a situationally sensitive 82–93. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490409552216
male mating strategy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(6), 391–395. Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2008.06.001 hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(1),
Baker, M. D., & Maner, J. K. (2009). Male risk-taking as a context-sensitive 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00023992
signaling device. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(5), 1136– Buss, D. M. (1994). The strategies of human mating. American Scientist,
1139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.06.006 82(3), 238–249.
Barash, D. P. (1977). Sociobiology and behavior. New York, NY: Elsevier Buss, D. M. (Ed.) (2005). The handbook of evolutionary psychology. Hoboken,
North-Holland. NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Barkow, J. (1989). Darwin, sex, and status: Biological approaches to mind Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: An evolu-
and culture. Toronto, ON: University of Toronto Press. https://doi. tionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Review, 100, 204.
org/10.3138/9781442673724 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.204
Barkow, J. (1992). Beneath new culture is old psychology: Gossip and so- Camerer, C. (2005). Three cheers—psychological, theoretical, empirical—for
cial stratification. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The loss aversion. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 129–133. https://
adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.42.2.129.62286
627–637). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Cantú, S. M., Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Weisberg, Y. J., Durante, K. M.,
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. E., & Tooby, J. E. (1992). The adapted mind: & Beal, D. J. (2013). Fertile and selectively flirty: Women’s behavior
Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. New York, NY: toward men changes across the ovulatory cycle. Psychological Science,
Oxford University Press. 25, 431–438.
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      17

Carlson, L., & Grossbart, S. (1988). Parental style and consumer socializa- Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S. E., Perilloux, C., & Li, N. P. (2011).
tion of children. Journal of Consumer Research, 15(1), 77–94. https://doi. Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: Hormonal influ-
org/10.1086/209147 ences on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 921–
Chiger, S. (2001). Consumer shopping survey. Catalog Age, 18(11), 47–49. 934. https://doi.org/10.1086/656575
Childers, T. L., & Rao, A. R. (1992). The influence of familial and peer-based Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Redden, J. P., & Edward White, A. (2015).
reference groups on consumer decisions. Journal of Consumer Research, Spending on daughters versus sons in economic recessions. Journal
19(2), 198–211. https://doi.org/10.1086/209296 of Consumer Research, 42(3), 435–457. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/
Chisholm, J. S. (1993). Death, hope, and sex: Life-history theory and the de- ucv023
velopment of reproductive strategies. Current Anthropology, 34, 1–24. Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Cantú, S. M., & Li, N. P.
https://doi.org/10.1086/204131 (2012). Ovulation leads women to perceive sexy cads as good dads.
Churchill, G. A. Jr, & Moschis, G. P. (1979). Television and interpersonal Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 292. https://doi.
influences on adolescent consumer learning. Journal of Consumer org/10.1037/a0028498
Research, 6(1), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1086/208745 Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Simpson, J. A., Cantú, S. M., & Tybur, J. M.
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. (1990). A focus theory of norma- (2012). Sex ratio and women’s career choice: Does a scarcity of men
tive conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in pub- lead women to choose briefcase over baby? Journal of Personality and
lic places. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 1015. https:// Social Psychology, 103, 121. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027949
doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015 Durante, K. M., & Laran, J. (2016). The effect of stress on consumer saving
Clutton-Brock, T. H., & Albon, S. A. (1982). Parental investment in male and spending. Journal of Marketing Research, 53, 814–828. https://doi.
and female offspring in mammals. In King’s College Sociobiology Group org/10.1509/jmr.15.0319
(Ed.), Current problems in sociobiology (pp. 223–247). Cambridge, UK: Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women’s
Cambridge University Press. choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory
Cocker, M., & Mabey, R. (2005). Birds britannica (pp. 418–425). London, UK: task-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,
Chatto & Windus. 1451–1460. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208323103
Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., Goetz, C., Lewis, D. L., Perilloux, Ellis, B. J., Figueredo, A. J., Brumbach, B. H., & Schlomer, G. L. (2009).
C., & Buss, D. M. (2010). Evolutionary psychology: Questions, pros- Fundamental dimensions of environmental risk. Human Nature, 20,
pects, and limitations. American Psychologist, 65, 110–126. 204–268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9063-7
Cosmides, L., Barrett, H. C., & Tooby, J. (2010). Adaptive specializations, so- Emlen, S. T., & Oring, L. W. (1977). Ecology, sexual selection, and the
cial exchange, and the evolution of human intelligence. Proceedings of the evolution of mating systems. Science, 197, 215–223. https://doi.
National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(Supplement org/10.1126/science.327542
2), 9007–9014. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0914623107 Fennis, B. M., Janssen, L., & Vohs, K. D. (2008). Acts of benevolence: A
Dabbs, J. M. (1990). Salivary testosterone measurements: Reliability across limited-resource account of compliance with charitable requests.
hours, days, and weeks. Physiology & Behavior, 48(1), 83–86. https:// Journal of Consumer Research, 35(6), 906–924.
doi.org/10.1016/0031-9384(90)90265-6 Fitzsimons, G. J., Nunes, J. C., & Williams, P. (2007). License to sin: The
Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Evolutionary social psychology and fam- liberating role of reporting expectations. Journal of Consumer Research,
ily homicide. Science, 242, 519–524. https://doi.org/10.1126/ 34(1), 22–31. https://doi.org/10.1086/513043
science.3175672 Frank, R. H. (1985). Choosing the right pond: Human behavior and the quest
D’Arpizio, C., Levato, F., Zito, D., & de Montgolfier, J. (2016). Luxury goods for status. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
worldwide market study. Retrieved from http://www.bain.com/pub- Frank, R. H. (1999). Luxury fever: Money and happiness in an era of excess.
lications/articles/luxury-goods-worldwide-market-study-fall-win- Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
ter-2016.aspx Galak, J., Kruger, J., & Loewenstein, G. (2012). Slow down! Insensitivity to
Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species. London, UK: Murray. rate of consumption leads to avoidable satiation. Journal of Consumer
De Backer, C. J., Nelissen, M., Vyncke, P., Braeckman, J., & McAndrew, F. T. Research, 39(5), 993–1009.
(2007). Celebrities: From teachers to friends. Human Nature, 18, 334– Gangestad, S. W., Garver-Apgar, C. E., Simpson, J. A., & Cousins, A. J.
354. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9023-z (2007). Changes in women’s mate preferences across the ovulatory
Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and corti- cycle. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 151–163. https://
sol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of labora- doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.151
tory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 355–391. https://doi. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating:
org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355 Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(04),
Douglas, M., & Isherwood, B. (1978). The world of goods: Towards an anthro- 573–587. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0000337X
pology of goods. London, UK: Allen Lane. Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., &
Dunbar, R. I. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and lan- Christensen, P. N. (2004). Women’s preferences for male behavioral
guage in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–694. https:// displays across the menstrual cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203–
doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325 207. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.01503010.x
Dunbar, R. I. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Brain, 9, 178–190. Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in wom-
Dunbar, R., & Barrett, L. (2009). Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology. en’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927–
Duntley, J. D., & Buss, D. M. (2012). The evolution of stalking. Sex Roles, 66, 933. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.1998.0380
311–327. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9832-0 Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver, C. E. (2002). Changes in women’s
Durante, K. M., & Arsena, A. R. (2014). Playing the field: The effect of fer- sexual interests and their partners’ mate retention tactics across the
tility on women’s desire for variety. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), menstrual cycle: Evidence for shifting conflicts of interest. Proceedings
1372–1391. of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 269, 975–
Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Cantú, S. M., & Simpson, J. A. (2014). 982. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2001.1952
Money, status, and the ovulatory cycle. Journal of Marketing Research, Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Garver-Apgar, C. E. (2005). Women’s sex-
51, 27–39. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.11.0327 ual interest across the ovulatory cycle depending on primary partner
|
18       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

fluctuating asymmetry. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series Haselton, M. G., & Nettle, D. (2006). The paranoid optimist: An integrative
B: Biological Sciences, 272, 2023–2027. https://doi.org/10.1098/ evolutionary model of cognitive biases. Personality and Social Psychology
rspb.2005.3112 Review, 10, 47–66. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_3
Gneezy, U., & Potters, J. (1997). An experiment on risk taking and eval- Haws, K. L., Davis, S. W., & Dholakia, U. M. (2016). Control over what?
uation periods. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 112(2), 631–645. Individual differences in general versus eating and spending self-
https://doi.org/10.1162/003355397555217 control. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 35(1), 37–57. https://doi.
Goldberg, M. E., & Gorn, G. J. (1978). Some unintended consequences of org/10.1509/jppm.14.149
TV advertising to children. Journal of Consumer Research, 5(1), 22–29. Haws, K. L., & Poynor, C. (2008). Seize the day! Encouraging indulgence for
https://doi.org/10.1086/208710 the hyperopic consumer. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(4), 680–691.
Griskevicius, V., Ackerman, J. A., Cantu, S. M., Delton, A. W., Robertson, https://doi.org/10.1086/592129
T. E., Simpson, J. A., … Tybur, J. M. (2013). When the economy falters Headland, T. N., & Greene, H. W. (2011). Hunter–gatherers and other pri-
do people spend or save? Responses to resource scarcity depend on mates as prey, predators, and competitors of snakes. Proceedings of the
childhood environments. Psychological Science, 24, 197–205. https:// National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(52),
doi.org/10.1177/0956797612451471 E1470–E1474. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1115116108
Griskevicius, V., Cantú, S. M., & Vugt, M. V. (2012). The evolutionary bases Hill, S. E., & Durante, K. M. (2011). Courtship, competition, and the pursuit
for sustainable behavior: Implications for marketing, policy, and social of attractiveness: Mating goals facilitate health-related risk taking and
entrepreneurship. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing, 31, 115–128. strategic risk suppression in women. Personality and Social Psychology
https://doi.org/10.1509/jppm.11.040 Bulletin, 37(3), 383–394. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167210395603
Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Hill, S. E., Prokosch, M. L., DelPriore, D. J., Griskevicius, V., & Kramer, A.
Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Going along versus going alone: When fun- (2016). Low childhood socioeconomic status promotes eating in the
damental motives facilitate strategic (non)conformity. Journal absence of energy need. Psychological Science, 27(3), 354–364. https://
of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 281–294. https://doi. doi.org/10.1177/0956797615621901
org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.2.281 Hill, S. E., Rodeheffer, C. D., Griskevicius, V., Durante, K., & White, A. E.
Griskevicius, V., Goldstein, N. J., Mortensen, C. R., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, (2012). Boosting beauty in an economic decline: Mating, spending, and
R. B., & Kenrick, D. T. (2009). Fear and loving in Las Vegas: Evolution, the lipstick effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(2),
emotion, and persuasion. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 384–395. 275. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028657
https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.46.3.384 Hoch, S. J., & Loewenstein, G. F. (1991). Time-inconsistent preferences and
Griskevicius, V., & Kenrick, D. T. (2013). Fundamental motives for why we consumer self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 17(4), 492–507.
buy: How evolutionary needs influence consumer behavior. Journal https://doi.org/10.1086/208573
of Consumer Psychology, 23, 372–386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Hofmann, W., Baumeister, R. F., Förster, G., & Vohs, K. D. (2012). Everyday
jcps.2013.03.003 temptations: An experience sampling study of desire, conflict, and
Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., & Neufeld, S. (2010). Influence of different self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(6), 1318.
positive emotions on persuasion processing: A functional evolutionary https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026545
approach. Emotion, 10, 190–206. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018421 Hofmann, W., & Van Dillen, L. (2012). Desire: The new hot spot in self-
Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., & Nowlis, S. M. (2010). The many shades of control research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 317–
rose-colored glasses: An evolutionary approach to the influence of dif- 322. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412453587
ferent positive emotions. Journal of Consumer Research, 37, 238–250. Holt, D. B. (1998). Does cultural capital structure American con-
https://doi.org/10.1086/651442 sumption? Journal of Consumer Research, 25(1), 1–25. https://doi.
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Ackerman, J. M., Delton, A. W., Robertson, org/10.1086/209523
T. E., & White, A. E. (2012). The financial consequences of too many Hu, F. B., Manson, J. E., Stampfer, M. J., Colditz, G., Liu, S., Solomon, C. G., &
men: Sex ratio effects on saving, borrowing, and spending. Journal of Willett, W. C. (2001). Diet, lifestyle, and the risk of type 2 diabetes mel-
Personality and Social Psychology, 102, 69. https://doi.org/10.1037/ litus in women. New England Journal of Medicine, 345, 790–797. https://
a0024761 doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa010492
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W., & Robertson, T. E. (2011). The John, D. R. (1999). Consumer socialization of children: A retrospective look
influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk and delayed at twenty-five years of research. Journal of Consumer Research, 26(3),
rewards: A life history theory approach. Journal of Personality and Social 183–213. https://doi.org/10.1086/209559
Psychology, 100, 1015–1026. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022403 John, D. R., & Whitney, J. C. Jr (1986). The development of consumer
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Sundie, J. M., Cialdini, R. B., Miller, G. F., & knowledge in children: A cognitive structure approach. Journal of
Kenrick, D. T. (2007). Blatant benevolence and conspicuous con- Consumer Research, 12(4), 406–417. https://doi.org/10.1086/208526
sumption: When romantic motives elicit strategic costly signals. Jones, R. E. (1997). Human reproductive biology (2nd ed.) New York, NY:
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 85. https://doi. Academic Press.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.85 Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of de-
Hamilton, W. D. (1963). The evolution of altruistic behavior. American cision under risk. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 47,
Naturalist, 97, 354–356. https://doi.org/10.1086/497114 263–291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. Journal Kanazawa, S. (2002). Bowling with our imaginary friends. Evolution and
of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5193 Human Behavior, 23(3), 167–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-
(64)90038-4 5138(01)00098-8
Han, Y. J., Nunes, J. C., & Drèze, X. (2010). Signaling status with luxury Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2005). Life history theory and evolu-
goods: The role of brand prominence. Journal of Marketing, 74(4), 15– tionary psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary
30. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkg.74.4.15 psychology (pp. 68–95). New York, NY: Wiley.
Haselton, M. G., & Buss, D. M. (2000). Error management theory: Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Neuberg, S. L., & Schaller, M. (2010).
A new perspective on biases in cross-sex mind reading. Journal Renovating the pyramid of needs contemporary extensions built upon
of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 81–91. https://doi. ancient foundations. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 292–314.
org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.1.81 https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610369469
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      19

Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., Griskevicius, V., Becker, D. V., & Schaller, Mitchell, V., & Walsh, G. (2004). Gender differences in German consumer
M. (2010). Goal-driven cognition and functional behavior: The decision-making styles. Journal Consumer Behaviour, 3(4), 331–346.
fundamental-motives framework. Current Directions in Psychological Mittal, C., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Sense of control under uncertainty
Science, 19, 63–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359281 depends on people’s childhood environment: A life history theory ap-
Kim, A., Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., & Nikiforidis, L. (2017). “The effect proach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107(4), 621. https://
of fertility on women’s luxury spending”, working paper. doi.org/10.1037/a0037398
Kornrich, S., & Furstenberg, F. (2013). Investing in children: Changes in Møller, A. P. (2000). Male parental care, female reproductive success,
parental spending on children, 1972–2007. Demography, 50(1), 1–23. and extrapair paternity. Behavioral Ecology, 11, 161–168. https://doi.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s13524-012-0146-4 org/10.1093/beheco/11.2.161
Krishnamurthy, P., & Prokopec, S. (2009). Resisting that triple-chocolate Moschis, G. P., & Churchill, G. A. Jr (1978). Consumer socialization: A the-
cake: Mental budgets and self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, oretical and empirical analysis. Journal of Marketing Research, 15, 599–
37(1), 68–79. 609. https://doi.org/10.2307/3150629
Laran, J., & Janiszewski, C. (2010). Work or fun? How task construal and Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and depletion of
completion influence regulatory behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, limited resources: Does self-control resemble a muscle? Psychological
37(6), 967–983. Bulletin, 126(2), 247. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.247
Leimar, O. (1996). Life-history analysis of the Trivers and Willard sex-ratio Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., Shiffman, S., & Paty, J. A. (2005). Daily fluctua-
problem. Behavioral Ecology, 7(3), 316–325. https://doi.org/10.1093/ tions in self-control demands and alcohol intake. Psychology of Addictive
beheco/7.3.316 Behaviors, 19(2), 140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.140
Li, N. P., Bailey, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., & Linsenmeier, J. A. (2002). The ne- Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving self-control
cessities and luxuries of mate preferences: Testing the tradeoffs. strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 524. https://
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 947–955. https://doi. doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.524
org/10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.947 Nairne, J. S., & Pandeirada, J. N. S. (2008). Adaptive memory: Remembering
Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences with a stone-age brain. Current Directions in Psychology, 17, 239–243.
for short-term mates: What, whether, and why. Journal of Personality and https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00582.x
Social Psychology, 90(3), 468. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.90.3.468 Nelson, E. (2001, November 26). Rising lipstick sales may mean pouting
Li, Y. J., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., & Neuberg, S. L. (2012). Economic economy. The Wall Street Journal, B1.
decision biases and fundamental motivations: How mating and Nelson, R. J. (2005). An introduction to behavioral endocrinology. Sunderland,
self-protection alter loss aversion. Journal of Personality and Social MA: Sinauer Associates.
Psychology, 102, 550. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025844 Nesse, R. M. (1990). Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature,
Lieberman, L. S. (2003). Dietary, evolutionary, and modernizing influences 1, 261–289. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02733986
on the prevalence of type 2 diabetes. Annual Review of Nutrition, 23, Nesse, R. M., & Williams, G. C. (1994). Why we get sick: The new science of
345–377. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.nutr.23.011702.073212 Darwinian medicine. New York, NY: Times Books.
Lincoln, F. C. (1952). Migration of birds. New York, NY: Doubleday and Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., & Schaller, M. (2011). Human threat man-
Company, Garden City. agement systems: Self-protection and disease avoidance. Neuroscience
Lino, M., Kuczynski, K., Rodriguez, N., & Schap, T. (2017). Expenditures & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1042–1051. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
on Children by Families, 2015 (Miscellaneous Publication No. 1528– neubiorev.2010.08.011
2015). United States Department of Agriculture. Center for Nutrition Neufeld, S., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Positive emotions, marketing, and social
Policy and Promotion. Retrieved from https://www.cnpp.usda.gov/sites/ influence. In M. M. Tugade, M. N. Shiota, & L. D. Kirby (Eds.), Handbook of
default/files/crc2015_March2017_0.pdf positive emotions (pp. 463–478). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Lipson, S. F., & Ellison, P. T. (1996). Endocrinology comparison of salivary Nielson (2013). U.S. women control the purse strings. Retrieved from http://
steroid profiles in naturally occurring conception and non-conception www.nielsen.com/us/en/insights/news/2013/u-s-women-control-
cycles. Human Reproduction, 11, 2090–2096. https://doi.org/10.1093/ the-purse-strings.html
oxfordjournals.humrep.a019055 Nikiforidis, L., Durante, K. M., Redden, J. P., & Griskevicius, V. (2018). Do
Liu, Y. J., Wang, M. C., & Zhao, L. (2010). Narrow framing: Professions, so- mothers spend more on daughters while fathers spend more on sons?
phistication, and experience. Journal of Futures Markets, 30(3), 203–229. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28, 149–156. https://doi.org/10.1002/
Maestripieri, D. (2002). Parent–offspring conflict in primates. International jcpy.1004
Journal of Primatology, 23, 923–951. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015 Nikolova, H., & Lamberton, C. (2016). Men and the middle: Gender dif-
537201184 ferences in dyadic compromise effects. Journal of Consumer Research,
Meyer, K. A., Kushi, L. H., Jacobs, D. R., Slavin, J., Sellers, T. A., & Folsom, A. 43(3), 355–371. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucw035
R. (2000). Carbohydrates, dietary fiber, and incident type 2 diabetes in Novemsky, N., & Kahneman, D. (2005). The boundaries of loss aversion.
older women. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71, 921–930. Journal of Marketing Research, 42(2), 119–128. https://doi.org/10.1509/
Meyers-Levy, J., & Loken, B. (2015). Revisiting gender differences: What jmkr.42.2.119.62292
we know and what lies ahead. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(1), Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (1998). The dynamics of indirect reciprocity.
129–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2014.06.003 Journal of Theoretical Biology, 194, 561–574. https://doi.org/10.1006/
Miller, G. F. (2000). The mating mind: How sexual selection shaped the evolu- jtbi.1998.0775
tion of human nature. New York, NY: Doubleday. Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward
Mineka, S., & Öhman, A. (2002). Phobias and preparedness: The selective, an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108,
automatic, and encapsulated nature of fear. Biological Psychiatry, 52, 483–522.
927–937. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01669-4 Öhman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious serpent snakes as a pro-
Mintel Report (2011). Consumer attitudes toward luxury goods. Retrieved from totypical stimulus for an evolved module of fear. Current Directions in
http://academic.mintel.com.ezp2.lib.umn.edu/sinatra/oxygen_academic/ Psychological Science, 12, 5–9.
search_results/show&/display/id=543136 Ordabayeva, N., & Chandon, P. (2010). Getting ahead of the Joneses: When
Mischel, W. (2014). The marshmallow test: Understanding self-control and equality increases conspicuous consumption among bottom-tier con-
how to master it. New York, NY: Random House. sumers. Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 27–41.
|
20       DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS

Ornstein, R. E., & Ehrlich, P. R. (1989). New world, new mind: Changing the Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern
way we think to save our future. London, UK: Methuen. evolutionary framework. In P. R. Shaver, & J. Cassidy (Eds.), Handbook
Penton-Voak, I. S., Perrett, D. I., Castles, D. L., Kobayashi, T., Burt, D. M., of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp.
Murray, L. K., & Minamisawa, R. (1999). Menstrual cycle alters face 131–157). New York, NY: Guilford.
preference. Nature, 399, 741–742. https://doi.org/10.1038/21557 Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., Kuo, S. I., Sung, S., & Collins, W. A. (2012).
Peracchio, L. A. (1992). How do young children learn to be consumers? A Evolution, stress, and sensitive periods: The influence of unpredictability
script-processing approach. Journal of Consumer Research, 18(4), 425– in early versus late childhood on sex and risky behavior. Developmental
440. https://doi.org/10.1086/209271 Psychology, 48(3), 674. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027293
Poynor, C., & Haws, K. L. (2008). Lines in the sand: The role of motivated Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V., & Rothman, A. J. (2012). Consumer deci-
categorization in the pursuit of self-control goals. Journal of Consumer sions in relationships. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 304–314.
Research, 35(5), 772–787. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.09.007
Read, D., Loewenstein, G., Rabin, M., Keren, G., & Laibson, D. (1999). Choice Sivanathan, N., & Pettit, N. C. (2010). Protecting the self through consump-
bracketing. In B. Fischhoff & C. F. Manski (Eds.), Elicitation of preferences tion: Status goods as affirmational commodities. Journal of Experimental
(pp. 171–202). Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer Netherlands. Social Psychology, 46(3), 564–570. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.
Redden, J. P., & Haws, K. L. (2012). Healthy satiation: The role of decreas- 01.006
ing desire in effective self-control. Journal of Consumer Research, 39(5), Solomon, M. R. (1983). The role of products as social stimuli: A symbolic
1100–1114. interactionism perspective. Journal of Consumer Research, 10(3), 319–
Rich, S. U., & Jain, S. C. (1968). Social class and life cycle as predictors of 329. https://doi.org/10.1086/208971
shopping behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 5, 41–49. https://doi. Srinivasan, K., Viswanad, B., Asrat, L., Kaul, C. L., & Ramarao, P. (2005).
org/10.2307/3149792 Combination of high-fat diet-fed and low-dose streptozotocin-treated rat:
Richins, M. L. (1987). Media, materialism, and human happiness. Advances A model for type 2 diabetes and pharmacological screening. Pharmacological
in Consumer Research, 14(1), 352–356. Research, 52, 313–320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.phrs.2005.05.004
Righetti, F., & Finkenauer, C. (2011). If you are able to control yourself, I Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories, Vol. 249. Oxford, UK:
will trust you: The role of perceived self-control in interpersonal trust. Oxford University Press.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 874. https://doi. Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal,
org/10.1037/a0021827 D. J. (2011). Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen: Conspicuous
Robertson, T. S., & Rossiter, J. R. (1974). Children and commercial persua- consumption as a sexual signaling system. Journal of Personality and
sion: An attribution theory analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 1(1), Social Psychology, 100, 664. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021669
13–20. https://doi.org/10.1086/208577 Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York, NY: Oxford
Roff, D. A. (2002). Life history evolution, Vol. 7. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer University Press.
Associates. Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Gurung, R. A. R., &
Rucker, D. D., Galinsky, A. D., & Dubois, D. (2012). Power and consumer Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral responses to stress in females:
behavior: How power shapes who and what consumers value. Journal Tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–
of Consumer Psychology, 22(3), 352–368. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 429. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.411
jcps.2011.06.001 Thaler, R. H., & Johnson, E. J. (1990). Gambling with the house money and trying
Russell, T., & Thaler, R. (1985). The relevance of quasi rationality in competi- to break even: The effects of prior outcomes on risky choice. Management
tive markets. The American Economic Review, 75(5), 1071–1082. Science, 36(6), 643–660. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.36.6.643
Saad, G. (2007). The evolutionary bases of consumption. Mahwah, NJ: Thornhill, R., & Gangestad, S. W. (2008). The evolutionary biology of human
Psychology Press. female sexuality. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Saad, G. (2013). Evolutionary consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, Tinbergen, N. (1963). On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für
23, 351–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2013.03.002 Tierpsychologie, 20, 410–433.
Saad, G. (2017). On the method of evolutionary psychology and its ap- Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2005). Conceptual foundations of evolutionary
plicability to consumer research. Journal of Marketing Research, 54(3), psychology. In David M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psy-
464–477. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmr.14.0645 chology (pp. 5–67). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
Saad, G., & Stenstrom, E. (2012). Calories, beauty, and ovulation: The Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review
effects of the menstrual cycle on food and appearance-related con- of Biology, 46, 35–57. https://doi.org/10.1086/406755
sumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22, 102–113. https://doi. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In B. G.
org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.10.001 Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the decent of man, 1871–1971 (pp.
Saad, G., & Vongas, J. G. (2009). The effect of conspicuous consump- 136–179). Chicago, IL: Aldine Publishing Company.
tion on men’s testosterone levels. Organizational Behavior and Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent-offspring conflict. American Zoologist, 14, 249–
Human Decision Processes, 110(2), 80–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. 264. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/14.1.249
obhdp.2009.06.001 Trivers, R. L., & Willard, D. E. (1973). Natural selection of parental ability
Seock, Y. K., & Bailey, L. R. (2008). The influence of college students’ shop- to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science, 179, 90–92. https://doi.
ping orientations and gender differences on online information searches org/10.1126/science.179.4068.90
and purchase behaviours. International Journal of Consumer Studies, Veblen, T. (1899). The theory of the leisure class: An economic study in the
32(2), 113–121. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2007.00647.x evolution of institutions. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. New York, NY: Harper. Walker, M. L., Wilson, M. E., & Gordon, T. P. (1983). Female rhesus mon-
Shiv, B., & Fedorikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay of key aggression during the menstrual cycle. Animal Behavior, 31, 1047–
affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer 1054. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(83)80011-6
Research, 26(3), 278–292. https://doi.org/10.1086/209563 Wallen, K. (2000). Risky business: Social context and hormonal modula-
Shiv, B., Loewenstein, G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). tion of primate sexual desire. In K. Wallen, & J. E. Schneider (Eds.),
Investment behavior and the negative side of emotion. Psychological Reproduction in context (pp. 289–323). Cambridge, UK: The MIT Press.
Science, 16(6), 435–439. Wang, Y., & Griskevicius, V. (2014). Conspicuous consumption, relationships,
Silverstein, M. J., & Sayre, K. (2009). The female economy. Harvard Business and rivals: Women’s luxury products as signals to other women. Journal
Review, 87(9), 46–53. of Consumer Research, 40, 834–854. https://doi.org/10.1086/673256
DURANTE AND GRISKEVICIUS |
      21

Wernerfelt, B. (1990). Advertising content when brand choice is a signal. Young Kim, E., & Kim, Y. K. (2004). Predicting online purchase intentions
Journal of Business, 63, 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1086/296485 for clothing products. European Journal of Marketing, 38(7), 883–897.
Westneat, D. F., & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Parentage and the evolu- https://doi.org/10.1108/03090560410539302
tion of parental behavior. Behavioral Ecology, 4, 66–77. https://doi. Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of
org/10.1093/beheco/4.1.66 Darwin’s puzzle. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Wilcox, K., Kim, H. M., & Sen, S. (2009). Why do consumers buy counterfeit Zhang, Y., & Fishbach, A. (2005). The role of anticipated emotions in the
luxury brands? Journal of Marketing Research, 46(2), 247–259. https:// endowment effect. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 15(4), 316–324.
doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.46.2.247 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327663jcp1504_6
Wilcox, K., Kramer, T., & Sen, S. (2010). Indulgence or self-control: A dual Zhang, Y., Huang, S. C., & Broniarczyk, S. M. (2009). Counteractive con-
process model of the effect of incidental pride on indulgent choice. strual in consumer goal pursuit. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(1),
Journal of Consumer Research, 38(1), 151–163. 129–142.
Wilson, E. O. (2000). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Wilson, M., & Daly, M. (1985). Competitiveness, risk taking, and violence: How to cite this article: Durante KM, Griskevicius V. Evolution
The young male syndrome. Ethology and Sociobiology, 6(1), 59–73. and consumer psychology. Consum Psychol Rev. 2018;1:4–21.
https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(85)90041-X
https://doi.org/10.1002/arcp.1001
Young, L. J., & Insel, T. R. (2002). Hormones and parental behavior. In J. B.
Becker, M. S. Breedlove, D. Crews, & M. M. McCarthy (Eds.), Behavioral
endocrinology, 2nd ed. (pp. 331–369). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

You might also like