You are on page 1of 8

Eating disorders across two cultures: does the

expression of psychological control vary?

Nerissa Soh, Lois J. Surgenor, Stephen Touyz, Garry Walter

Objective: Because both the expression of eating disorder (ED) symptoms and preferred
psychological control styles may be affected by culture, the purpose of the present study
was to examine whether the expression of psychological control in women with EDs varies
across two cultures.
Method: North European Australian and Chinese Singaporean women (n117) with
anorexia nervosa (n36), bulimia nervosa (n 13) and eating disorders not otherwise
specified (n 3), and without an ED (n 65) recruited in Australia and Singapore
completed a multidimensional inventory assessing sense of control, domains of control,
preferred means by which to gain control, and motivation for control.
Results: Although the normative control profile for each culture differed slightly, control
profiles among those with an ED were very similar across both cultures. However, the
directionality and extent of specific aspects of control pathology associated with the
presence of an ED differed across cultures. North European Australians with an ED were
much more deviant from the cultural norm than their Chinese Singaporean ED
counterparts in relation to overall sense of control, methods of gaining control, and
control in the domain of body. Chinese Singaporean woman with an ED were much more
deviant from the cultural norm than their North European Australian ED counterparts in the
domain of control over impulses.
Conclusions: Having an ED powerfully distorts psychological control irrespective of
culture. However the degree, directionality, and form of the displacement from normal
control styles is also culture dependent. This has implications for treatments that attempt to
redress or correct control issues in people with an ED in other cultures.
Key words: cultural differences, eating disorders, psychological control.

Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 2007; 41:351 358 


Lois Surgenor, Senior Lecturer (Correspondence)
Historically, eating disorders (EDs) were considered
Department of Psychological Medicine, Christchurch School of Medicine rare in non-Western societies. Consequently research
and Health Sciences, University of Otago, PO Box 4345, Christchurch
8140, New Zealand. Email lois.surgenor@chmeds.ac.nz interest about EDs in these societies and information
Nerissa Soh, Research Officer
about potential cross-cultural differences has emerged
Child and Adolescent Mental Health Services, Northern Sydney Central only relatively recently [1]. Being a member of a non-
Coast Area Health Services, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
Western ethnic group was thought to confer some
Stephen Touyz, Professor of Clinical Psychology degree of protection [2,3], although other hypotheses
Psychology Department, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South
Wales, Australia for the reported lower prevalence have included
referral bias [4 6], social stigma in some non-Western
Garry Walter
Professor of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Discipline of Psychological societies [7] and a reluctance to consult Western-based
Medicine, University of Sydney, and Area Clinical Director, Child and physicians over traditionally based practitioners [8,9].
Adolescent Mental Health Services, Northern Sydney Central Coast
Area Health Services, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia The heightened interest in the cultural context of
Received 28 September 2006; accepted 29 November 2006 EDs has included speculation about different risk

# 2007 The Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Psychiatrists


Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016
352 EATING DISORDERS ACROSS TWO CULTURES

factors in non-Western cultures [10,11], cultural Japanese culture emphasizes external control, while
variation in the expression of symptoms [12,13], and European Americans reported greater internal con-
whether outcomes differ in Western and non-Western trol, as do those in Hispanic cultures [28]. Along the
societies [14]. Generally there has been little evidence same lines, people from East Asian cultures are
for major differences in psychopathology, with stu- perhaps less likely to see matters in isolation;
dies showing no [4,12,13] or few differences accordingly, a sense of personal control is less
[10,15,16]. Lee et al. considered fear of fatness to be important. Ji et al. explained the difference in control
a relatively recent reason for food refusal offered by preferences between East Asian and European socie-
people with anorexia nervosa (AN) in the Western ties in terms of the agricultural heritage of China
world, and used this to explain why non-Western lending itself to a collective society while the hunting
patients may be less likely to present with this core and herding heritage of Europe favours autonomy
symptom [14]. More recently, a typography for and a looser social structure [29]. Chan provides
atypical cases in Chinese patients has been proposed additional empirical evidence of the East Asian
[17]. But with increasing exposure to Western dietary culture valuing and endorsing external control [30].
and beauty values, fear of fatness could become more Further, in terms of forming self-identity, Hong
common. That is, any differences may dissipate. Kong adolescents seem concerned with ability and
Nevertheless at this stage body image dissatisfaction competence rather than other personal control as-
among Hong Kong women, for example, may not be pects of identity [31]. Instead, in that culture there is
as critical a risk factor for EDs [18]. an emphasis on external control, implying that an
Whatever the differences, if they do indeed exist, it individual should work hard on their immediate task
is nonetheless plausible that classic features of EDs and fate would take care of the rest. Levels of
may have a different meaning in different cultural acculturation also affect expression of control, and
contexts and, in turn, different strengths of associa- this provides a powerful argument for studying
tions with ED psychopathology. That is, what is control issues in the primary cultural setting of that
deemed as pathology directly associated with an ED group rather than one in which they have relocated
in Western settings may in fact be due to reasons into. For example, first-generation Japanese students
unrelated to the ED in non-Western settings [19,20]. in the USA scored more highly on external control
while their third- or later generation peer group
Psychological control, culture and eating and body scored more highly on internal control [28].
image disturbances Compounding the Western bias [26] in understand-
ing healthy psychological control styles has been the
It has been proposed that when ED patients in non- traditional reliance on constructs such as locus of
Western countries present without shape and weight control [32]. Invariably, the accompanying assump-
concerns, issues of self-control may be the primary tion is that a lack of active or internal control
motivator instead [21]. A core construct associated orientation in an individual is undesirable.
with EDs is that of psychological control, and there is Bringing these tracts of research together, there is
considerable clinical and empirical evidence to sup- good reason to speculate that any reportedly strong
port this association in general (see [22] for a association between EDs and particular issues of
conceptual review). Control has been argued to be psychological control may be influenced by culture.
important in the aetiology [23,24], maintenance [21], This has been only partially tested in non-Western
and treatment approaches [25] of EDs. The empirical groups and to date has not yet been tested in clinical
evidence consistently supports a profile among those groups in two different countries alongside their non-
with an ED that includes overreliance on particular ED peer group. Kempa and Thomas suggested that
means by which to gain control, and reduced overall ethnic minority individuals may draw a sense of
control [26,27]. control from eating attitudes and behaviours when
Importantly, there is reason to believe that what is facing racism and oppression and the consequential
a normal ‘control profile’ may differ between cultural sense of powerlessness [6]. In Hong Kong, Lee et al.
settings. Marks noted that what has previously been investigated the relationship between psychological
considered normal (and thus leading to a treatment control and outcome in women with AN [33].
bias towards attaining specific control goals) may be Participants classified as having a good outcome
influenced by the Western cultural value of personal had significantly higher overall sense of control,
autonomy [28]. In short, norms for psychological positive sense of control and specific sense of control
control may be culture dependent. For example, compared to patients categorized as having a poor

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


N. SOH, L. J. SURGENOR, S. TOUYZ, G. WALTER 353

outcome. In general, the control profile of Hong Method


Kong women with AN was consistent with other
research in that greater eating pathology is associated Participant and recruitment method
with a greater desire for control and greater use of
more negative forms of gaining control. Lee et al. Participants (n117) were women aged 14  38 years recruited in
acknowledged that the study could not ascertain if the 2 years ending June 2004 across two countries (Australia and
these control issues were peculiar to EDs because the Singapore) as part of a larger study investigating cross-cultural
differences in body composition, body image, and EDs. The study
study design did not include a non-ED sample [33],
was approved by the relevant Ethics Committee and Authority at
and thus it is not clear what the psychological control
each recruitment site in each country (Human Research Ethics
norms would be in the Hong Kong population. Committee, University of Sydney; Ethics Review Committee,
Nevertheless, the authors hypothesized that while Central Sydney Area Health Board; Human Research Ethics
fear of fatness may not be present in some women Committee, Western Sydney Areas Health Board; Ethics Commit-
with AN in Hong Kong, the general similarity of tee, Singapore General Hospital). When participants were deemed
control profiles between typical and atypical (non-fat minors (under 16 years of age in Australia; under 21 years of age in
phobic) patients may mean that psychological control Singapore), written informed consent was obtained from the
is a more useful culturally impervious diagnostic participant’s parents.
The ED participants were recruited from four teaching hospitals
criterion. Because information regarding this is
affiliated with University of Sydney and from two teaching
almost absent, an equally plausible hypothesis is hospitals in Singapore. All were undergoing treatment for an ED
that a typography using phenomenon of psychologi- (AN, bulimia nervosa (BN), or eating disorder not otherwise
cal control (rather than fat phobia as proposed by specified (EDNOS)) as classified by DSM-IV criteria [34].
[17]) may be equally worthwhile exploring. That is, Healthy Australian participants (of North European ethnicity)
can variability between cultures, or typical and who had never been diagnosed with an ED (as per clinical interview
atypical cases within a culture, be captured by a self-report) were recruited via advertising flyers at University of
typography based on psychological control profiles? Sydney campus and website, and by word of mouth. Participants
In summary, a particular control profile in itself needed to have been born in Australia, or migrated there from
North European settings before the age of 12 years. Healthy
may not be pathological; instead, the degree to which
Singaporean participants (Chinese ethnicity) were recruited
it deviates from the relevant population norm may be through the nursing department at Singapore General Hospital.
of greater clinical importance in understanding its In this instance, potential participants were made aware of the
association with EDs. To the best of our knowledge, study by verbal announcements through the nursing management
this hypothesis has not been previously investigated structure and each staff shift handover. Exclusion criteria were
despite the existing implication that control issues non-fluency in spoken or written English, self-report of current or
may be the psychopathology that permeates all past ED, and current pregnancy or lactating.
cultures. This leaves the intriguing and entirely
underresearched question as to whether any pre-
Data collection and measures
sumed pathological control styles, expressions and
ambitions in EDs are culturally-bound. In all cases, face-to-face appointments were made to obtain
The aim of the present study was to investigate written consent, undertake the physical assessments (weight, height
psychological control profiles in North European etc.), and complete demographics sheets recording ethnicity/
Australian and Singaporean Chinese women with cultural group and age. For the ED participants, formal diagnosis
and without an ED in their own cultural setting. was made by the treating psychiatrists using DSM-IV criteria, and
Based on the limited studies to date, it is hypothesized collected from their current clinical medical records in the clinic
that (i) certain expression of psychological control setting where they were receiving treatment. All participants then
may be culture dependent and, in particular, Singa- completed the Shapiro Control Inventory (SCI) [35] in their own
time as part of a larger questionnaire booklet examining cultural
porean Chinese participants will prefer less assertive
differences in ED psychopathology and sociocultural factors. This
and more passive means by which to experience was returned immediately or shortly thereafter.
control, and have less motivation for control overall
than their North European Australian counterparts; Shapiro Control Inventory
(ii) the previously observed strong association be-
tween psychological control and EDs will permeate The SCI is a 187-item Likert-style questionnaire assessing an
both cultures; and (iii) the specific psychological individual’s multi-dimensional psychological control profile. The
control pathology associated with an ED may be duration for completion is 20  30 min. The SCI has been used to
culture dependent. investigate issues across a wide range of psychiatric and medical

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


354 EATING DISORDERS ACROSS TWO CULTURES

populations, including in ED populations in New Zealand [27] and ment. Components of desire include the perceived importance of
Hong Kong [33]. It uses language comprehensible to individuals being in control, efforts to achieve control, and fear of losing
with equivalence of 8 years formal education, and has demon- control.
strated good internal consistency across clinical populations,
including Hong Kong Chinese women [33,35  37]. Nine scales Statistical analyses
examine three different facets of control, which can be summarized
as follows. All analyses were conducted with SPSS Version 13.0 for
(1) As the first and most general component, Sense of Control Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Each SCI scale and the
‘‘measures a person’s view that s/he has control, as well as the belief seven domain-specific subscales of scale 4 were analysed using the
that s/he can gain control if desired’’ (p. 7, [35]) The Overall Sense general linear model (GLM) with cultural group (North European
of Control score (scale 1) gives the broadest view of the Australian and Chinese  Singaporean) and caseness (ED or no ED)
respondent’s sense of control, and is further analysed with respect as independent variables. Because body mass index (BMI) was
to its constituent parts: Positive Sense of Control (scale 2), which associated with cultural group (t(115)1.9, p0.056) and caseness
assesses belief in the ability to attain future control, ability to utilize (t(115)6.7, p0.000), and both BMI and age were significantly
positive modes of control, and current level of self-control; and associated with aspects of the dependent measure (SCI scales), BMI
Negative Sense of Control (scale 3), which assesses the sense of loss and age were included as covariables in all linear regression
of control in areas previously experienced as controlled, aspects of analyses.
inadequate of self-control or environmental control, and feelings of
helplessness and passivity. Sense of Control is also examined with
respect to domains in which such control is experienced, either as
Results
an overall Domain Sense of Control score (scale 4) or by each of
seven specific domain scores (body, mind, relationships, self,
career, environment, or impulse control) imbedded in the scale.
Characteristics of the sample
In this way, to what extent loss of control in one domain is also
experienced in another can be examined. Participants from both cultures included young women in the age
(2) Mode of Control assesses the means by which an individual range typical of those who present with EDs both in Western and
attempts to attain and maintain a sense of control. Distinctions are Asian settings (Table 1) [33,38]. Among the ED participants, 69%
made across the two dimensions of assertive  yielding and positive  had a primary diagnosis of AN, 25% a primary diagnosis of BN,
negative, thus yielding four scales. Positive  assertive (scale 5) and the remaining 3% were diagnosed with EDNOS.
measures the perceived ability to use an active, altering mode of
control, and includes descriptors such as ‘decisive’, ‘leading’ Relationship between sense of control, culture and
and ‘communicating needs’. Positive-yielding (scale 6) measures caseness
sense of control through means of letting go of active control.
Descriptors include ‘patient’ and ‘accepting’. Negative-assertive Culture was significantly associated with three aspects of
(scale 7) measures too much active control (‘manipulating’, psychological control (Table 2), all of which related to the means
‘dogmatic’) while Negative-yielding (scale 8) measures aspects by which people prefer to gain control. North European Australian
of too little control, with descriptors such as ‘indecisive’ and participants preferred to gain control through positive assertive
‘manipulated’. methods (SCI scale 5; F(1,110)17.4, p B0.001) and positive
(3) Motivation (Desire) for Control (scale 9) measures the desire yielding methods (SCI scale 6; F(1,110)6.4, p B0.05), and were
for psychological control, over oneself, others, and the environ- significantly less likely to use negative yielding methods (SCI scale

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of sample (mean 9 SD)

North European Australians Chinese Singaporeans


No ED ED No ED ED Total
n 32 34$ 33 18 117
Height (m) 1.68 9 0.59 1.65 9 0.50 1.59 9 0.47 1.59 9 0.54 1.63 9 6.4
Weight (kg) 60.4 9 7.6 47.4 9 8.5 57.4 9 13.0 44.9 9 11.8 53.4 9 11.9
BMI (kg m  2) 21.4 9 2.5 17.4 9 2.7 22.6 9 4.6 17.7 9 4.6 20.0 9 4.2
Age (years) 21.5 9 2.9 20.8 9 3.8 22.2 9 3.0 22.5 9 5.2 21.6 9 3.6

ED, eating disorder.; $One participant was excluded from this anthropometric analysis due to a history of weight-altering in situ silicone
implants, but was included in all other subsequent analyses.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


N. SOH, L. J. SURGENOR, S. TOUYZ, G. WALTER 355

Table 2. Psychological control (SCI) scores by culture and caseness (mean 9 SD)

North European Chinese Caseness


Australians Singaporean Culture Caseness  Culture
SCI scale No ED ED No ED ED (p) (p) (p value)
Scale 1: Overall 5.2 9 0.65 3.8 9 0.98 4.8 9 0.72 4.0 9 0.81 0.306 0.000 .046
Sense of Control
Scale 2: Positive 5.3 9 0.69 3.9 9 1.0 4.6 9 0.88 4.2 9 0.83 0.316 0.000 .004
Sense of Control
Scale 3: Negative 2.9 9 0.84 4.4 9 1.1 2.9 9 0.98 4.6 9 1.2 0.489 0.000 .573
Sense of Control
Scale 4: Overall Domain 4.8 9 0.54 4.0 9 0.54 4.8 9 0.55 3.9 9 0.77 0.873 0.000 .785
Sense of Control
Body 4.7 9 0.82 3.4 9 0.92 4.1 9 0.87 3.6 9 1.2 0.372 0.000 .043
Mind 4.5 9 0.84 2.9 9 1.2 4.7 9 0.84 2.9 9 1.2 0.703 0.000 .665
Relationships 4.7 9 0.55 4.0 9 1.2 5.0 9 0.72 4.0 9 0.92 0.564 0.000 .500
Self 4.9 9 1.2 2.3 9 1.5 4.8 9 1.1 3.2 9 1.8 0.096 0.000 .063
Career 4.5 9 0.90 4.1 9 1.1 4.6 9 0.64 3.8 9 1.2 0.628 0.002 .354
Environment 4.6 9 1.2 4.5 9 1.6 4.9 9 0.72 3.9 9 2.0 0.690 0.053 .124
Impulse 5.4 9 0.58 5.5 9 0.52 5.6 9 0.31 5.3 9 0.66 0.527 0.006 .005
Scale 5: Positive 2.8 9 0.44 2.3 9 0.51 2.2 9 0.35 2.2 9 0.41 0.000 0.042 .005
Assertive Mode
Scale 6: Positive 2.7 9 0.46 2.2 9 0.49 2.3 9 0.37 2.1 9 0.42 0.013 0.005 .188
Yielding Mode
Scale 7: Negative 1.8 9 0.48 2.1 9 0.56 1.8 9 0.45 2.4 9 0.67 0.166 0.000 .094
Assertive Mode
Scale 8: Negative 1.7 9 0.43 2.1 9 0.66 2.0 9 0.49 2.2 9 0.68 0.047 0.004 .640
Yielding Mode
Scale 9: Motivation 4.3 9 0.85 5.3 9 0.82 4.3 9 0.76 5.2 9 1.1 0.841 0.000 .843
for Control

ED, eating disorder; SCI, Shapiro Control Inventory.

8; F(1,110)4.0, p B0.05) to attain control than their Chinese counterparts, Chinese Singaporeans with an ED deviated signifi-
Singaporean counterparts. cantly more from their cultural norm on aspects control in the
Caseness was significantly associated with all major SCI scales. domain of impulsivity (SCI subscale 4 Impulsivity; F(1,109)8.1,
That is, ED participants reported a reduced overall sense of control pB0.01).
(SCI scale 1; F(1,111)35.2, p B0.001) including the positive and
negative components of this (SCI scales 2,3; F(1,111)18.3,
pB0.001 and F(1,111)48.3, p B0.001 respectively), along with Discussion
reduced control in all domains (SCI scale 4; F(1,103)51.8,
pB0.001) of functioning (excepting control over environment), Psychological control is a construct of sustained
reduced use of positive means of gaining control (SCI scales 5,6; interest in the area of EDs, but one that requires
F(1,110)4.2, pB0.05 and F(1,110)8.1, pB0.01, respectively), further explication to reconcile the contradictory
increased use of negative means (SCI scales 7,8; F(1,106)17.4, literature within cultures and add to the limited
pB0.001 and F(1,110)8.4, p B0.01, respectively), and signifi- knowledge base across cultures. Theories and some
cantly elevated desire for control (SCI scale 9; F(1,111)23.4,
empirical evidence suggest that expressions of control
pB0.001).
may be, in part, affected by culture. Existing argu-
A significant interaction for culture and caseness was found for
ments are also made that the expression of ED
specific aspects of psychological control. In this respect, compared
with their Chinese Singaporean ED counterparts, eating disordered
symptoms may be influenced by culture. This is the
North European Australians deviated significantly more from their first study to examine multidimensional aspects of
cultural norm on aspects of overall sense of control (SCI scale 1; psychological control in ED subjects and their non-
F(1,111)4.1, pB0.05), positive sense of control (SCI scale 2; ED peer group concurrently in two cultures.
F(1,111)8.9, p B0.01), positive assertive methods of attaining The study does suffer from a number of limitations,
control (SCI scale 5; F(1,110)8.3, pB0.01) and control in many of which are associated with obtaining cross-
the domain of body (SCI 4 subscale Body; F(1,108)4.2, p B cultural samples, and a larger sample overall would
0.05). Compared with their North European Australian ED have enabled more subtle and detailed results beyond

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


356 EATING DISORDERS ACROSS TWO CULTURES

those obtained, including analysis by ED subtype. choice of methods by which day-to-day control is
A further limitation is that the sample was recruited achieved. Longstanding cultural traditions in both
under different circumstances and with different settings could account for this. East Asian societies
strategies  in part due to the cultural and have been described as highly conforming and
infrastructural difference between Australia and Sin- supportive of a social norm that promotes a sense
gapore. Although care was taken to minimize such of fatalism [29,31]. Likewise, a western cultural
differences, they may have, nevertheless, impacted on conditioning to value self-responsibility and decisive-
the findings. Also, the ED participants recruited in ness is likely to colour which methods of gaining
the treatment centres may not be representative of the control are selected. This style is one that also
wider ED population in each setting, especially given permeates psychological interventions [39]. Although
the referral bias that may be operating in non- we hypothesized that Singaporean Chinese women
Western clinic settings. would have less motivation for control overall, the
The overall pattern revealed in this series of present study did not find this. A potential reason is
analyses was that women with EDs in these two that level of acculturation was not taken into
cultures exhibit control profiles that cannot be account; and westernization in Singapore in view of
differentiated by culture. That is, ED women, irre- its prominent use of the English language, and access
spective of culture, can be marked out from their to English language media, may have served to
non-ED peer group by their reduced overall sense of distort (increase) desire for control in this more recent
control, greater negative sense of control, reduced sample.
control in the domains of mind, relationships, self By far the most intriguing findings, especially in
and career, and marginally lower levels of control in terms of understanding the directionality and extend
the domain of environment. ED women across both of control psychopathology, relate to the interactions
cultures also reported significant differences in the between cultural group and caseness. In many
methods by which they attain psychological control. respects, there was some evidence of convergence at
Having an ED seems to mute use of active assertive the stage of a clinical ED, yet on specific aspects of
methods. It means that these people are less likely to psychological control North European women with
experience control through trusting others, and more an ED deviated much more from their cultural peers
likely to resort to dogmatic methods at the same time than their Chinese Singaporean ED counterparts.
as feeling manipulated by others. Clearly, this occurs That is, the extent of specific aspects of control
while experiencing elevated motivation for control. pathology in those with an ED is culture dependent,
The ED control profile across these two cultures is with North European Australian women more likely
very similar in these respects, and it is a profile that to exhibit much more severe deviations from their
concurs with existing recent studies within cultures non ED peers in the direction of reduced overall
[27,33,39]. In particular, Lee et al. observed that AN control, reduced control in the domain of body, and
patients in Hong Kong used less assertive means of more aberration in their ability to utilize positive
achieving control and were more likely to view assertive methods by which to gain control. Whereas
themselves as being controlled by others when having an ED in Singapore may lead to greater
compared to normal or recovered individuals [33]. deviancy from one’s Chinese Singaporean peer group
Although the aforementioned similarities initially in the domain of impulse control than one’s Western
suggest that control psychopathology may be imper- ED counterparts. It is not clear why this latter finding
vious to culture, this is not the case for all aspects of emerged but it may reflect the higher proportion of
control, and some important differences exist. In BN patients in the Singaporean Chinese ED sample
terms of cultural norms, in the present study the compared to the North European Australian clinical
means by which individuals prefer to attain and sample.
maintain control is significantly associated with The study of the role of psychological control in
culture, with Chinese Singaporean women being less EDs in non-Western groups has only recently com-
likely to use methods involving assertiveness and self- menced and further studies are clearly necessary
initiation, whereas North European Australians were before more conclusions can be drawn. Fear of
less likely to use strategies that risk a sense of fatness has been previously proposed to be a
manipulation or indecision. Put another way, culture-bound feature of EDs [40], and in many
although no more motivated to achieve control than respects the present study suggests that, in general,
Chinese Singaporean women, the Australian norm is psychological control issues may be the more uni-
one of less timidity and more assertiveness in the versal and less culture-bound feature of EDs. That is,

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


N. SOH, L. J. SURGENOR, S. TOUYZ, G. WALTER 357

an ED powerfully distorts sense of control irrespec- Handbook of gender, culture and health , Mahwah, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2000:227  253.
tive of culture. That said, the present study provides 5. Tchanturia K, Katzman M, Troop NA, Treasure J. An
preliminary evidence that it is western culture that exploration of eating disorders in a Georgian sample. Int J
may experience the more pathological shifts once an Soc Psychiatry 2002; 48:220  230.
ED takes hold. Although these findings cannot 6. Kempa ML, Thomas AJ. Culturally sensitive assessment and
treatment of eating disorders. Eat Disord 2000; 8:17  30.
evaluate the hypothesis that issues of self-control 7. Nakamura K, Yamamoto M, Yamazaki O et al . Prevalence
are the more primary motivator in non-Western EDs of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa in a geographically
[21], they nonetheless suggest that the ambition to defined area in Japan. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 28:173  180.
8. Tsai G. Eating disorders in the Far East. Eat Weight Disord
achieve self-control is no more intense than that seen 2000; 5:183  197.
in Western ED cases. 9. Pate JE, Pumariega AJ, Hester C, Garner DM. Cross-cultural
Clearly, cultural differences with respect to aspects patterns in eating disorders: a review. J Am Acad Child
of a normal control profile need to be taken into Adolesc Psychiatry 1992; 31:802  809.
10. Shaw H, Ramirez L, Trost A, Randall P, Stice E. Body image
account when understanding the meaning or signifi- and eating disturbances across ethnic groups: more
cance of any assumed control psychopathology similarities than differences. Psychol Addict Behav 2004;
associated with an ED. In short, deviation from a 18:12  18.
11. Humphry TA, Ricciardelli LA. The development of eating
cultural norm may be just as important in under- pathology in Chinese-Australian women: acculturation versus
standing control psychopathology in each culture, culture clash. Int J Eat Disord 2004; 35:579  588.
and universal conclusions cannot be drawn simply 12. le Grange D, Stone AA, Brownell KD. Eating disturbances in
about absolute levels of control. In specific areas, white and minority female dieters. Int J Eat Disord 1998;
24:395  403.
deviation from a cultural norm on issues of psycho- 13. le Grange D, Telch CF, Agras WS. Eating and general
logical does not occur to the same degree in psychopathology in a sample of Caucasian and ethnic
Singaporean ED patients. It could be that future minority subjects. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21:285  293.
14. Lee S, Ho TP, Hsu LK. Fat phobic and non-fat phobic
shifts towards westernization (and thus more wester- anorexia nervosa: a comparative study of 70 Chinese patients
nized control norms) could see ED women in Asian in Hong Kong. Psychol Med 1993; 23:999  1017.
settings become more deviant in their control profile, 15. Ryu HR, Lyle RM, McCabe GP. Factors associated with
and for this reason future research should consider weight concerns and unhealthy eating patterns among young
Korean females. Eat Disord 2003; 11:129  141.
the effect of adherence to Western or traditional 16. Lee HY, Lee EL, Pathy P, Chan YH. Anorexia nervosa in
cultures. If deviance from a cultural norm is some- Singapore: an eight-year retrospective study. Singapore Med J
thing that matters, then this may not be static. These 2005; 46:275  281.
17. Ngai ES, Lee S, Lee AM. The variability of phenomenology
are all important hypotheses requiring further exam- in anorexia nervosa. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2000; 102:314 
ination before control-based approaches to treatment 317.
[e.g. 21] can be confidently applied. 18. Lake AJ, Staiger PK, Glowinski H. Effect of Western culture
on women’s attitudes to eating and perceptions of body
shape. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 27:83  89.
19. Jennings PS, Forbes D, McDermott B, Juniper S, Hulse G.
Acknowledgement Acculturation and eating disorders in Asian and Caucasian
Australian adolescent girls. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2005;
59:56  61.
This study was partly supported by the Edith Mary 20. le Grange D, Louw J, Breen A, Katzman MA. The meaning
Rose Travelling Scholarship awarded to the first of ‘self-starvation’ in impoverished black adolescents in South
Africa. Cult Med Psychiatry 2004; 28:439  461.
author. 21. Fairburn CG, Shafran R, Cooper Z. A cognitive behavioural
theory of anorexia nervosa. Behav Res Ther 1998; 37:1  13.
22. Surgenor LJ, Horn J, Plumridge EW, Hudson SM. Anorexia
nervosa and psychological control: a reexamination of
References selected theoretical accounts. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2002;
10:85  101.
23. Bruch H. Eating disorders: obesity, anorexia nervosa, and the
1. Soh N, Touyz S, Surgenor LJ. Eating and body image
person within , New York: Basic Books, 1973.
disturbances across cultures. Eur Eat Disord Rev 2006; 14:54 
24. Polivy J, Herman CP. Causes of eating disorders. Annu Rev
65.
2. Wildes JE, Emery RE, Simons AD. The roles of ethnicity and Psychol 2002; 53:187  213.
culture in the development of eating disturbance and body 25. Jarman M, Smith JA, Walsh S. The psychological battle for
dissatisfaction: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2001; control: a qualitative study of health professionals’
21:521  551. understandings of the treatment of anorexia nervosa. J
3. Nasser M. The emergence of eating disorders in other Community Appl Soc Psychol 1997; 7:137  152.
cultures/societies. In: Nasser M, ed. Culture and weight 26. Shapiro DH, Blinder BJ, Hagman J, Pituck S. A
consciousness , London: Routledge, 1997:24  60. psychological ‘‘sense-of-control’’ profile of patients with
4. Streigel-Moore RH, Smolak L. The influence of ethnicity on anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. Psychol Rep 1993;
eating disorders in women. In: Eisler RM, Hersen M, eds. 73:531  541.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016


358 EATING DISORDERS ACROSS TWO CULTURES

27. Surgenor LJ, Horn J, Hudson SM. Empirical scrutiny of a 34. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical
familiar narrative: sense of control in anorexia nervosa. Eur manual of mental disorders , 4th ed. Washington, DC:
Eat Disord Rev 2003; 11:291  305. American Psychiatric Association, 1994.
28. Marks LI. Deconstructing locus of control: implications for 35. Shapiro DH. Manual for the Shapiro Control Inventory , Palo
practitioners. J Counsel Dev 1998; 76:251  260. Alto, CA: Behaviordata, 1994.
29. Ji LJ, Peng K, Nisbett RE. Culture, control, and perception of 36. Surgenor LJ, Horn J, Hudson SM, Lunt H, Tennent J.
relationships in the environment. J Pers Soc Psychol 2000; Metabolic control and psychological sense of control in
78:943  955. women with diabetes mellitus: alternative considerations of
30. Chan DW. Dimensionality and adjustment correlates of locus the relationship. J Psychosom Res 2000; 49:267  273.
of control among Hong Kong Chinese. J Pers Assess 1989; 37. Surgenor LJ, Horn J, Hudson SM, Adamson SJ, Robertson
53:145  160. P. Alcohol dependence and psychological sense of control:
31. Leung PWL, Salili F, Baber FM. Common adolescent refining the links. N Z J Psychol 2007; 35:146  152.
problems in Hong Kong: their relationship with self-esteem, 38. Makino M, Tsuboi K, Dennerstein L. Prevalence of eating
locus of control, intelligence and family environment.
disorders: a comparison of Western and non-Western
Psychologia Int J Psychol Orient 1986; 29:91  101.
32. Rotter J. Generalised expectancies for internal versus external countries. Medscape Gen Med 2004; 6:49.
39. Shapiro DH, Astin JA. Control therapy: an integrated
locus of control of reinforcement. Psychol Monogr 1966;
80:489  493. approach to psychotherapy, health and healing , New York:
33. Lee S, Chan YY, Kwok K, Hsu LK. Relationship between John Wiley and Sons, 1998.
control and the intermediate term outcome of anorexia 40. Keel PK, Klump KL. Are eating disorders culture-bound
nervosa in Hong Kong. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2005; 39: syndromes? Implications for conceptualizing their etiology.
141  145. Psychol Bull 2003; 129:747  769.

Downloaded from anp.sagepub.com at PENNSYLVANIA STATE UNIV on February 20, 2016

You might also like