You are on page 1of 13

Sex Roles, Vol. 52, Nos.

7/8, April 2005 (


C 2005)

DOI: 10.1007/s11199-005-3712-z

Relationships Between Men’s and Women’s Body Image and


Their Psychological, Social, and Sexual Functioning

Tanya E. Davison1 and Marita P. McCabe1,2

This study was designed to examine the relationships between different aspects of body im-
age and psychological, social, and sexual functioning throughout adulthood. The respondents
were 211 men and 226 women (age range 18–86 years; mean = 42.26). Respondents com-
pleted measures of self-rated attractiveness, body satisfaction, body image importance, body
image behaviors, appearance comparison, social physique anxiety, self-esteem, depression,
anxiety, and social and sexual functioning. Body image was associated with self-esteem for
all groups, but was unrelated to other aspects of psychological, social, and sexual functioning.
There were some exceptions; a disturbance in body image was related to problematic social
and sexual functioning among middle-aged men and to depression and anxiety symptoms in
late adulthood among men and women. Middle-aged men who presented with the type of
body image disturbance typical of women were more likely to have impaired interpersonal
functioning. These results demonstrate that social aspects of body image appear to be impor-
tant in understanding psychological functioning in later life.

KEY WORDS: body image; gender differences; developmental differences; psychological functioning;
social functioning; sexual functioning.

The term body image is typically used to refer suggest that young men are also dissatisfied with their
to perceptions and attitudes individuals hold about bodies (Abell & Richards, 1996; Drewnowski & Yee,
their bodies, although some authors argue that body 1987).
image is a broader term, which encompasses behav- A broad conceptualization of body image may
ioral aspects, such as weight loss attempts, and other prove important in understanding the nature of the
indicators of investment in appearance (Banfield & construct among men, who appear to be less in-
McCabe, 2002). Women are generally considered to clined than women to report holding negative atti-
hold a more negative body image than men (Feingold tudes toward their bodies, but do report a strong
& Mazzella, 1998). As a result, body dissatisfaction motivation to improve the appearance of their bod-
among women has been labeled a “normative discon- ies (Davison, 2002). It may also be helpful to con-
tent” (Rodin, Silberstein, & Striegel-Moore, 1985). sider body image broadly when investigating its role
However, through the use of gender-sensitive instru- throughout adulthood. Although the majority of re-
ments that conceptualize body image concerns in search is limited to college samples, body image con-
terms of a desire to gain muscle, as well as to lose cerns appear to extend into later life (Montepare,
weight, previous beliefs that men are largely resilient 1996), and different age-related changes have been
to concerns about their appearance have been chal- found among both men and women (Halliwell &
lenged, and there is now considerable evidence to Dittmar, 2003; Harmatz, Gronendyke, & Thomas,
1985). However, few researchers have systematically
1 School
explored the development of different aspects of
of Psychology, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia.
2 Towhom correspondence should be addressed at School of Psy-
body image throughout the period of adulthood.
chology, Deakin University, 221 Burwood Highway, Burwood, Although there has been a large body of re-
Victoria 3125, Australia; e-mail: maritam@deakin.edu.au. search on the prevalence of body image concerns and

463 0360-0025/05/0400-0463/0 
C 2005 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
464 Davison and McCabe

potential factors associated with the development of A gap also exists in our knowledge of whether
body image, few researchers have systematically in- a disturbance in body image is relevant to inter-
vestigated the role body image plays in the day-to- personal functioning. In the 1960s and 1970s, so-
day lives of individuals, beyond disturbed eating be- cial psychologists demonstrated the positive impact
haviors. In the present study we addressed this gap of being considered physically attractive by oth-
by exploring the association between body image and ers on desirability as a potential dating or roman-
psychological, social, and sexual functioning among tic partner (Berscheid, Dion, Walster, & Walster,
adult men and women. An innovative aspect of this 1971; Walster, Aronson, & Abrahams, 1966). Less
study is the conceptualization of body image from commonly researched, however, are the social im-
a number of different aspects, making use of multi- plications of an individual’s own rating of his or
ple gender-sensitive instruments, in order to under- her attractiveness or other aspects of body image.
stand the differential roles played by various aspects There are preliminary indications in research with
of body image. In addition, this study extends our un- college students of an association between being con-
derstanding of the role of body image for adult men cerned about one’s appearance and impaired social
and women throughout the community, rather than functioning. College students who perceive them-
focusing only on college students. selves as unattractive have been shown to be more
The associations between a disturbance in body likely to avoid cross-sex interactions (Mitchell &
image and psychological, social, and sexual dysfunc- Orr, 1976), to engage in less intimate social inter-
tion for different populations are currently not well actions with members of the same and other sex
understood. Previous researchers have demonstrated (Nezlek, 1988), and to experience higher levels of
a relationship between body image and self-esteem social anxiety (Feingold, 1992). Negative body im-
among women in early adulthood (Abell & Richards, age may also be related to problematic sexual func-
1996; Monteath & McCabe, 1997) and in later years tioning. Researchers have found that college stu-
(Paxton & Phythian, 1999). This has led some dents with poor views of their bodies are more
authors to conceptualize women’s body image as a likely than others to avoid sexual activities (Faith
component of a multidimensional global self-esteem & Schare, 1993), to perceive themselves as unskilled
(Marsh, 1997; O’Brien & Epstein, 1988). There are sexual partners (Holmes, Chamberlin, & Young,
also preliminary indications that young women who 1994), and to report dissatisfaction with their sex
report dissatisfaction with their physiques are at a lives (Hoyt & Kogan, 2001). However, other re-
greater risk of experiencing symptoms of depression searchers have failed to find a relationship between
or anxiety (Koenig & Wasserman, 1995; Mintz & body image and sexual functioning; Wiederman
Betz, 1986), although this relationship is less well and Hurst (1997), for example, suggested that
understood among older women. There are inconsis- sexuality was related to objective attractiveness
tencies in the literature, however, and it appears that among women, but not to self-ratings of their
results may be dependent on the particular aspect appearance.
of body image measured. For example, self-esteem Remarkably few researchers have made explicit
has been found to be unrelated to weight concerns reference to the social context when investigating
among young women (Silberstein, Striegel-Moore, body image, which has resulted in the impression
Timko, & Rodin, 1986), but strongly related to over- that body image evaluations and behaviors occur in
all physical appearance (Harter, 1999). Researchers social isolation. Recently, however, there is a grow-
have not previously attempted to determine system- ing awareness of the social nature of body image
atically which body image measures are most closely among female college students through their engage-
associated with different facets of psychological ment in comparisons of their own appearance with
functioning. The importance of body image for the that of others; such comparisons appear to be as-
psychological functioning of men is particularly un- sociated with negative evaluations of their bodies
clear, as inconsistent findings among young men stem (Stormer & Thompson, 1996; Thompson, Heinberg,
in part from the use of different instruments, which & Tantleff, 1991). In addition, researchers have
vary in their sensitivity to measure aspects of body found that a concern about others evaluating one’s
image most relevant to the lives of men. Of particular body negatively, a variable termed social physique
concern is the absence of research on the relationship anxiety, is related to low levels of body satisfac-
between body image and self-esteem, depression, tion (Hart, Leary, & Rejeski, 1989). This suggests
and anxiety among men from the general population. that evaluations individuals make of their bodies
Adult Body Image 465

are related to the evaluations that they expect oth- METHOD


ers may make. However, the relative importance
of social aspects of body image compared to indi- Participants
vidual aspects of body image evaluations and re-
lated behaviors has not been examined. It is cur- The participants were 211 men and 226 women,
rently unclear whether being dissatisfied with one’s who ranged in age from 18 to 86 years (M = 42.26
physique, considering oneself unattractive, rating years, SD = 17.11). This age range was divided into
one’s appearance as important, applying effort to im- three groups, and each participant was assigned to
prove or conceal one’s body, appearance compar- one of the following age groups: young adulthood,
isons, or social physique anxiety are of greatest rel- 18–29 years (n = 129), middle adulthood, 30–49
evance to people’s psychological, social, and sexual years (n = 153), and late adulthood, 50–86 years
functioning. (n = 145). This division was carried out to create
There are a number of other limitations in the equal groups to meet the requirements of parametric
literature. Few researchers have examined a range of statistical analyses. Reported occupations and postal
body image constructs in order to understand which addresses suggest that participants represented a
aspects of body image are most relevant to par- wide range of socioeconomic backgrounds from
ticular psychological, social, and sexual functioning metropolitan and rural areas. Over 80% of partici-
variables. The diversity of different evaluative and pants indicated they were originally from Australia;
behavioral body image constructs may account for the remainder were predominantly from Western
some of the inconsistent research findings. Past re- European countries. Nearly all (95.78%) participants
search has also primarily focused on college students, identified themselves as heterosexual, and over 70%
typically women; very few studies have included par- were in current relationships. The weight and height
ticipants from the general community. As a conse- of the sample corresponded well with national Aus-
quence, conclusions about the role of body image in tralian data for men and women (Australian Bureau
the lives of men and women cannot be made. The of Statistics, 1998). These data are documented for
relevance of body image may vary with age and gen- men and women, and each age group separately in
der, although researchers have previously failed to Table I.
address this question.
The present study was designed to investigate
systematically the role of body image in the lives Materials
of men and women throughout adulthood. A cross-
sectional design was employed, due to the practicali- Body Image Measures
ties of obtaining a sample large enough to consider
body image separately among men and women of Participants completed two subscales from
different age groups. The lack of previous research the Body Image and Body Change Questionnaire
in this area supports the contribution made by ex- (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2001) that are related to
ploratory designs of this kind. Multiple measures of Body Image Satisfaction and Body Image Impor-
body image, including evaluative, investment, and tance. Each scale contained 10 items. An example
social aspects, were compared, in order to deter- item of body image satisfaction is “How satisfied are
mine which aspects of body image were most strongly you with your weight?,” and an example item of
predictive of psychological (i.e., self-esteem, depres- body image importance is “How important to you
sion, anxiety), social (i.e., relations with members is the shape of your body, compared to other things
of the same and other sex, social anxiety), and sex- in your life?” Responses were on a 5-point Likert
ual (i.e., sexual optimism, sexual self-efficacy, sex- scale from 1 = extremely dissatisfied/unimportant to
ual satisfaction) functioning. It was hypothesized that 5 = extremely satisfied/important. Scores on each
negative body image would be associated with poor scale ranged from 10 to 50; a high score represents
functioning in these areas. Stronger relationships be- a high level of satisfaction with the body or a rat-
tween body image and psychological, social, and sex- ing of appearance as highly important. These scales
ual functioning were expected for women, and for emerged from both exploratory and confirmatory
younger participants, given the emphasis in the lit- factor analysis, and they have demonstrated high
erature on the importance of body image for these levels of internal consistency, satisfactory test-retest
groups. reliability, and concurrent and discriminant validity
466 Davison and McCabe

Table I. Height, Weight, and BMI by Gender and Age Group


Height (cm) Weight (kg) Body mass index (BMI)
Age group M SD M SD M SD
Women
18–29 165.24 7.26 63.08 12.01 23.24 4.35
30–49 164.28 7.42 71.32 17.28 26.43 6.23
50–86 162.54 7.56 69.72 13.29 26.46 5.22
Overall 164.00 7.44 68.63 15.13 25.60 5.63
Men
18–29 179.68 8.03 74.87 12.48 23.18 3.55
30–49 180.15 7.22 83.72 14.14 25.75 3.84
50–86 175.69 7.66 82.26 12.66 26.75 3.30
Overall 178.34 7.87 79.84 13.56 25.13 3.86

in previous studies with adolescents (Ricciardelli & the body improvement scale ranged from 3 to 18; a
McCabe, 2001). In the present sample, internal re- high score indicates a high engagement in attempts to
liability (Cronbach’s alpha) for each scale was high improve the body. Internal reliability for each scale
among both women and men (α > .90). was high among both men and women (α > .80).
Participants rated their physical attractiveness Concern about others evaluating one’s body was
using a scale specifically designed for this study, the assessed using the Social Physique Anxiety Scale
Physical Attractiveness Scale, which measures how (Hart et al., 1989). This scale contains 12 items, an
attractive they perceived themselves, for example, example of which is “In the presence of others, I
in terms of general appearance, facial attractive- feel apprehensive about my physique/figure.” Fol-
ness, and sexual attractiveness. This scale contains six lowing the recommendation of Eklund, Kelley, and
items, an example of which is “Compared to other Wilson (1997), item 2 was modified (to improve per-
men, I am . . . ” Participants responded on a 5-point formance) to “I worry about wearing clothes that
Likert scale from 1 = extremely unattractive to 5 = might make me look too thin or overweight.” Par-
extremely attractive. Scores ranged from 6 to 30; a ticipants rated how true each of the items were using
high score indicates a high self-rating of attractive- a 5-point Likert scale, from 1 = not at all true to 5 =
ness. Internal reliability was high among both men extremely true. Scores ranged from 12 to 60; a high
and women (α > .90). score indicates a high level of concern about others
Two body image behaviors, body concealment evaluating one’s body (the responses to some items
(the tendency to conceal one’s body from the gaze of were reverse scored). Internal and test-retest relia-
others and to avoid discussion about body size and bility have been found to be adequate with a number
shape) and body improvement (engagement in at- of adult samples (Hart et al., 1989; Martin, Rejeski,
tempts to improve one’s body), were assessed using Leary, McAuley, & Bane, 1997; Motl & Conroy,
an instrument constructed for this study, the Body 2000; Petrie, Diehl, Rogers, & Johnson, 1996). Inter-
Image Behavior Scales. Items were derived in part nal reliability was high among both men and women
from two extant instruments, the Body Image Avoid- in the present sample (α > .80).
ance Questionnaire (Rosen, Srebnik, Saltzberg, & Participants indicated their level of appearance
Wendt, 1991) and the Attention to Body Shape comparison by completing the Physical Appearance
Scale (Beebe, 1995), which were selected through ex- Comparison Scale (Thompson et al., 1991). This scale
ploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. The Body contains five items, an example of which is “At par-
Concealment Scale consists of five items, an example ties or other social events, I compare my physical ap-
item of which is “I avoid wearing ‘revealing’ clothes, pearance to the physical appearance of others.” Re-
like shorts or bathing suits.” The Body Improvement sponses were made on a 5-point Likert scale, from
Scale consists of three items, an example of which is 1 = never to 5 = always. Scores ranged from 5 to
“I exercise in order to get a better body.” Participants 25; a high score indicates a strong tendency to com-
responded on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 = never pare one’s own appearance with that of others. Al-
to 6 = always. Scores on the body concealment scale though psychometric characteristics were found to be
ranged from 5 to 30; a high score indicates a high en- adequate with a university sample (Thompson et al.,
gagement in attempts to conceal the body. Scores on 1991), item 4 correlated with others at a low level
Adult Body Image 467

in the present community sample (squared multiple example of which is “It takes me time to get over
correlation <.05). Therefore, this item was excluded my shyness in new situations.” Responses were made
from the composite variable, which resulted in an on a 4-point Likert scale, from 1 = not at all like me
acceptable level of internal reliability among men to 4 = a lot like me. Scores ranged from 6 to 24; a
(α > .70) and women (α > .80). high score represents a high level of social anxiety
(the responses to one item were reverse scored). The
revised Self-Consciousness Scale has demonstrated
Psychological Functioning Measures
good psychometric properties with samples from the
general population (Scheier & Carver, 1985). Inter-
Participants completed the Rosenberg Self-
nal reliability was moderate among men (α > .70)
Esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1965). This scale contains
and high among women (α > .80) in the present
10 items, an example of which is “I feel that I have
study.
a number of good qualities.” Responses were made
Social functioning was also assessed by the
on a 4-point Likert scale, from 1 = strongly disagree
Same-Sex Relations and Opposite-Sex Relations
to 4 = strongly agree. Scores ranged from 4 to 40; a
subscales of the Self-Description Questionnaire III
high score indicates high self-esteem (the responses
(Marsh, 1989). Each subscale contains 10 items. An
to some items were reverse scored). This instrument
example of same-sex relations is “I have few friends
has been widely used in research, and has demon-
of the same sex that I can really count on,” and an
strated good psychometric properties (Rosenberg,
example of opposite-sex relations is “I make friends
1979). Internal reliability was high among both men
easily with members of the opposite sex.” Responses
and women in the present sample (α > .80).
to each subscale were made on an 8-point Likert
Participants also completed two subscales from
scale, from 1 = definitely false to 8 = definitely true.
the Depression Anxiety Stress Sub Scales (Lovibond
Scores ranged from 10 to 80; a high score indicates
& Lovibond, 1995). The Depression Scale contains
positive same-sex or opposite-sex relations (the re-
14 items related to symptoms of depression, an ex-
sponses to some items were reverse scored). These
ample of which is “I felt downhearted and blue.” The
subscales have been found to have adequate inter-
Anxiety Scale contains 14 items related to symptoms
nal consistency and reliability in previous studies
of anxiety, an example of which is “I felt I was close
(Marsh, 1989), and internal reliability for each scale
to panic.” Participants were asked to indicate the
was high among both men and women in the present
extent to which they had experienced each symptom
study (α > .80).
over the previous week. Responses were made on
a 4-point Likert scale from 0 = did not apply to me
to 3 = applied to me very much or most of the time.
Sexual Functioning Measures
Scores on each scale ranged from 0 to 42; a high
score indicates a high level of depression or anxiety.
Sexual functioning was measured with three
These subscales are reliable measures of negative af-
subscales from the Multidimensional Sexual Self-
fective states among nonclinical college populations
Concept Questionnaire (Snell, 1995). The Sexual
(Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). Minor modifications
Self-Efficacy Scale contains five items, an example of
were made to four items to improve comprehension
which is “I have the ability to take care of any sexual
in a community sample, with the aim of retaining
needs and desires that I may have.” The Sexual Opti-
the original meaning of items. To illustrate, the item
mism Scale contains five items, an example of which
“I found it difficult to work up the initiative to do
is “I expect that the sexual aspects of my life will
things” was modified to “I found it difficult to work
be positive and rewarding in the future.” The Sexual
up the energy to do things.” Internal reliability for
Satisfaction Scale contains five items, an example of
each scale was high among both men and women
which is “I am satisfied with the way my sexual needs
(α > .90) in the present study.
are currently being met.” Responses to items on each
scale were made on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 =
Social Functioning Measures not at all true to 5 = very true. Scores on each scale
ranged from 5 to 25; a high score represents a high
Participants completed the social anxiety factor level of the construct—high sexual self-efficacy, high
of the revised Self-Consciousness Scale (Scheier & sexual optimism, and high sexual satisfaction (the re-
Carver, 1985). This subscale contains six items, an sponses to some items were reverse scored). Internal
468 Davison and McCabe

consistency of the scales has previously been found tance, body concealment, body improvement, so-
to be high, and research has produced reasonable ev- cial physique anxiety, and appearance comparison.
idence for their validity (Snell, 2001). Internal relia- Body image was found to be significantly different
bility for each scale was high among both men and for men and women, F (7, 368) = 22.48, p < .001, and
women (α > .80) in the present study. for different age groups, F (14, 738) = 6.00, p < .001.
There was no significant interaction effect. The uni-
variate F-tests for each dependent variable were ex-
Procedure amined in order to determine which body image
variables contributed to the significant multivariate
Participants were recruited from the general effects.
community; they were selected at random from the Women reported a lower level of body im-
White Pages telephone directory of metropolitan age satisfaction, F (1, 381) = 35.92, p < .001, and a
Melbourne and a variety of rural areas in Victoria, higher level of social physique anxiety, F (1, 381) =
Australia. Questionnaires were distributed by mail 64.87, p < .001, than men did (see Table II). Women
to individuals who agreed to participate, and were also reported concealing their bodies more fre-
completed at home and returned via mail to the re- quently than men did, F (1, 381) = 130.38, p < .001,
searchers. A total of 157 individuals indicated they and they were more likely than men to engage in ap-
did not want to participate in the study and received pearance comparisons, F (1, 381) = 25.61, p < .001.
no further contact from the researchers. Of the 720 However, there were no differences between men
questionnaires distributed, 437 were returned, which and women in their ratings of physical attractiveness,
resulted in a response rate of 60.69% among those body image importance, or level of engagement in ef-
who agreed to receive a questionnaire, and an over- forts to improve their bodies.
all response rate of 49.83% among those contacted. After we controlled for the effects of BMI, we
There was no incentive provided for individuals to found significant differences between age groups in
participate in the study, and responses were anony- body image satisfaction, F (2, 381) = 11.74, p < .001,
mous. Completion of the questionnaire took approx- and body concealment, F (2, 381) = 5.52, p < .01;
imately 20–30 min. men and women in their 30s and 40s reported lower
satisfaction with their bodies, and more frequent
attempts to conceal their bodies, than did other
RESULTS participants (see Table II). Social physique anxiety
scores also differed significantly between age groups,
In order to address the hypotheses outlined F (2, 381) = 18.97, p < .001; individuals in late
earlier, multivariate analyses of variance were con- adulthood reported a lower level of concern about
ducted to determine the nature of sex and age differ- others evaluating their bodies than did the younger
ences in body image. Regression analyses were then participants. In addition, level of engagement in ap-
conducted to determine which aspects of body image pearance comparison differed significantly between
(if any) predicted the psychological, social, and sex- age groups, F (2, 381) = 12.34, p < .001; individuals
ual functioning of both men and women in each age in late adulthood were less likely than others to
group. Because of the number of analyses being con- make appearance comparisons. Ratings of physical
ducted p < .01 was used to define significant results attractiveness, body image importance, and body
(Coakes & Steed, 1999). improvement did not differ significantly between
participants of different age groups.

Gender and Age Differences in Body Image Relationships Between Body Image and
Psychological, Social, and Sexual Functioning
Differences in body image between men and
women and among the different age groups were Hierarchical multiple regression analyses were
examined using a 2-way MANOVA, after control- conducted in order to determine which aspects of
ling for the effects of Body Mass Index (BMI). In- body image most strongly predicted each psychologi-
dependent variables were gender and age group, cal (i.e., self-esteem, depression, anxiety), social (i.e.,
and dependent variables were physical attractive- same-sex relations, opposite-sex relations, social anx-
ness, body image satisfaction, body image impor- iety), and sexual functioning (i.e., sexual self-efficacy,
Adult Body Image

Table II. Body Image Scores by Gender and Age Group


Physical attractiveness Body image satisfaction Body image importance Body concealment Body improvement Social physique anxiety Appearance comparison

Group M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD
Gender
Women 18.57 3.51 31.23 8.56 32.00 7.44 15.40 5.72 9.22 3.74 34.50 9.46 10.98 3.29
Men 18.51 3.57 35.46 7.21 30.94 7.61 9.77 4.73 8.70 3.70 27.68 8.11 9.17 3.35
Age
18–29 19.80 3.68 33.41 7.56 30.83 7.93 12.12 5.12 9.40 3.41 32.47 9.27 11.00 3.48
30–49 18.11 3.49 31.73 8.82 31.60 7.03 14.07 6.39 9.05 3.38 32.92 9.95 10.64 3.25
50–86 18.00 3.24 34.80 7.95 31.93 7.77 11.91 6.07 8.62 3.45 28.25 8.45 8.76 3.18
469
470 Davison and McCabe

Table III. Unique Body Image Predictors From Regression Equations That Significantly Increased the
Prediction of Psychological Functioning at Step 2 Among Men and Women of Different Age Groups
Unique body
Outcome variable Group image predictors sr2
Self-esteem Women 18–29 years None —
Women 30–49 years Social physique anxiety .06
Body image importance .05
Women 50–86 years Physical attractiveness .13
Men 18–29 years Physical attractiveness .07
Body image importance .07
Men 30–49 years Body concealment .10
Men 50–86 years Appearance comparison .08
Body image satisfaction .07
Depression Women 50–86 years Social physique anxiety .08
Anxiety Women 50–86 years None —
Men 50–86 years Appearance comparison .11

sexual optimism, sexual satisfaction) variable. Sepa- Table III). Body image variables also significantly in-
rate analyses were conducted for men and women in creased the prediction of self-esteem among women
each age group, as it was considered likely that the in early adulthood, F change (3, 50) = 4.60, p < .01,
relationships would vary with both gender and age. middle adulthood, F change (6, 84) = 5.41, p < .001,
In order to reduce the large number of independent and late adulthood, F change (3, 56) = 4.37, p < .01.
body image variables for inclusion in each analysis, Although there were no unique body image predic-
only those variables that significantly correlated with tors of self-esteem for women in early adulthood, low
the dependent variable for each group were entered social physique anxiety and a low rating of body im-
into the analysis. It was decided to control for the ef- age importance predicted self-esteem among women
fects of self-esteem, depression, anxiety, and BMI, if in middle adulthood, and positive ratings of physi-
they correlated significantly with the dependent vari- cal attractiveness predicted high self-esteem among
able. In addition, perceived relations with the other women in late adulthood.
sex were considered as a potential control variable in Inclusion of body image variables failed signif-
analyses to predict sexual functioning. Control vari- icantly to increase the prediction of depression or
ables were entered as independent variables on the anxiety beyond the effect of control variables among
first step of each analysis, and body image variables most groups. However, body image variables entered
were included as additional independent variables on at the second step significantly increased the predic-
the second step. The level of significance is typically tion of depression among women in late adulthood,
corrected when there are a high number of contrasts. F change (4, 46) = 4.57, p < .01; high social physique
However, given the exploratory nature of these anal- anxiety acted as a unique body image predictor (see
yses, it was decided to consider effects significant at Table III). Body image variables entered at the sec-
an alpha less than .05. ond step significantly increased the prediction of anx-
Results indicated that inclusion of body image iety among men in late adulthood, F change (2, 62) =
variables at the second step significantly increased 6.65, p < .01; a high level of appearance comparison
the prediction of self-esteem beyond that predicted acted as a unique body image predictor. For the pre-
by control variables among men in early adulthood, dictor of anxiety among women in late adulthood, F
F change (5, 55) = 2.88, p < .05, middle adulthood, change (4, 56) = 4.16, p < .01, although no specific
F change (4, 50) = 5.36, p < .001, and late adult- body image predictor was found to explain unique
hood, F change (4, 59) = 4.66, p < .01. The unique variance.
body image predictors of high self-esteem were pos- Body image variables significantly increased the
itive ratings of physical attractiveness and a low rat- prediction of social anxiety at the second step, be-
ing of body image importance among men in early yond the effect of control variables, among men in
adulthood, a low level of body concealment among middle adulthood, F change (2, 52) = 4.54, p < .05;
men in middle adulthood, and a low tendency to com- the unique body image predictor was a high level of
pare their appearance with others and high body im- appearance comparison (see Table IV). Inclusion of
age satisfaction among men in late adulthood (see body image variables did not significantly increase
Adult Body Image 471

Table IV. Unique Body Image Predictors From Regression Equations That Significantly Increased the Pre-
diction of Social and Sexual Functioning at Step 2 Among Men and Women of Different Age Groups
Unique body
Outcome variable Group image predictors sr2
Social anxiety Women 50–86 years Social physique anxiety .08
Body improvement .07
Men 30–49 years Appearance comparison .08
Same-sex relations Men 30–49 years Physical attractiveness .13
Opposite-sex relations Men 18–29 years Body concealment .09
Sexual self-efficacy Men 30–49 years Body satisfaction .09
Sexual satisfaction Men 30–49 years Appearance comparison .12
Body concealment .10
Body satisfaction .08
Sexual optimism Women 30–49 years None —
Men 30–49 years Social physique anxiety .18

the prediction of social anxiety among men in early high body image satisfaction acted as the unique
or late adulthood, beyond the effect of control vari- body image variable in both instances (see Table IV).
ables. Among women, inclusion of body image vari- A low tendency to compare their appearance to that
ables significantly increased the prediction of social of others and a low level of body concealment also
anxiety during late adulthood, F change (6, 51) = predicted sexual satisfaction.
3.63, p < .01, but not at other ages. The unique body The group of body image variables, entered at
image predictors of social anxiety among women in the second step, did not significantly increase the pre-
late adulthood were high social physique anxiety and diction of sexual optimism among men or women in
a high level of body improvement. early or late adulthood beyond the effect of control
Inclusion of body image variables, entered as variables. Inclusion of body image variables signif-
a group at the second step, did not significantly in- icantly increased the prediction of sexual optimism
crease the prediction of same-sex relations among among men in middle adulthood, however, F change
men in early or late adulthood, or among women (4, 48) = 6.69, p < .001; low social physique anx-
of any age group, beyond the effect of control vari- iety acted as a unique body image predictor (see
ables. However, a significant increase in the predic- Table IV). Although body image variables increased
tion of same-sex relations was found among men in the prediction of sexual optimism as a group among
middle adulthood, F change (5, 49) = 2.61, p < .05. women in middle adulthood, F change (6, 81) = 2.72,
Positive same-sex relations were uniquely predicted p < .05, there were no unique body image predictors.
by positive ratings of physical attractiveness among
this group (see Table IV). Inclusion of body image
variables at this step significantly increased the pre- DISCUSSION
diction of positive cross-sex relations among men in
young adulthood, F change (2, 57) = 4.17, p < .05; a In the present study we considered a number
low level of body concealment acted as a unique body of aspects of body image among men and women
image predictor, but did not increase the prediction across different stages of adulthood. Body image
of cross-sex relations beyond the effect of control concerns were generally found to be more prevalent
variables among any other group. among women than men; women reported lower
Inclusion of body image variables, entered as satisfaction with their bodies and a greater tendency
a group at the second step, did not significantly in- to conceal their bodies. Women appeared to be more
crease the prediction of sexual self-efficacy or sex- focused on the social aspects of body image; they
ual satisfaction among women in any age group, or compared their appearance to that of others more
among men in early or late adulthood, beyond the frequently than men did, and they reported higher
effect of control variables. Among men in middle levels of social physique anxiety, which indicates that
adulthood, however, inclusion of body image vari- they were more concerned about others evaluating
ables significantly increased the prediction of sexual their appearance negatively. However, there were
self-efficacy, F change (5, 46) = 3.69, p < .01, and no gender differences in ratings of physical attrac-
sexual satisfaction, F change (4, 49) = 6.27, p < .001; tiveness or the perceived importance of appearance
472 Davison and McCabe

in the lives of men and women, and men were just sions of other researchers (e.g., Tiggemann, 1994)
as likely as women to report engaging in efforts to and the findings from a recent review (Powell &
improve their bodies. Hendricks, 1999). In the present study, although men
Body image concerns were relatively consis- at all stages of adulthood were less likely than women
tent throughout adulthood, which supports previous to hold a global negative body image, once devel-
indications of the high prevalence of body image oped, a poor body image was as strongly related to
concerns among individuals beyond their college- the general self-concept of men as it was of women.
aged years (Allaz, Bernstein, Rouget, Archinard, & However, the particular aspect of body image most
Morabia, 1998; Ben-Tovim & Walker, 1994; Pliner, relevant to self-esteem differed according to age and
Chaiken, & Flett, 1990). There were some develop- gender. For example, physical attractiveness played
mental trends, however, as men and women in their an important role among men in early adulthood, but
30s and 40s were more vulnerable than other groups was more relevant to women’s self-esteem in later
to dissatisfaction with their bodies and engaged in years. Gender differences in the types of body image
more attempts to conceal their bodies, for example, variables relevant to self-esteem may explain some
with nonrevealing clothing. This highlights the im- of the inconsistencies in the literature, given that pre-
portance of attending to body image among adults vious researchers exploring the relationship between
beyond early adulthood, which is typically consid- body image and self-esteem have typically employed
ered the most vulnerable period for body image dis- a single measure of body image.
turbance. A developmental shift was also apparent in The absence of relationships between body im-
later years, most particularly in relation to the social age and other aspects of psychological, social, and
aspects of body image. Although men and women sexual functioning among most groups in this study
over 50 years of age tended to make evaluations appears to be best explained by shared relationships
of their own appearance that were just as negative with self-esteem. To illustrate, although depression
as those of younger participants, and did not per- and body image variables were generally correlated,
ceive their appearance to be any less important than consistent with earlier research (Denniston, Roth,
younger participants did, they reported less concern & Gilroy, 1992; Mable, Balance, & Galgan, 1986;
about others evaluating their bodies, and they were Sarwer, Wadden, & Foster, 1998), associations were
less likely to compare their appearance with that of no longer present among most groups when we con-
others. trolled for self-esteem. This is a surprising finding,
This exploratory study was designed to examine given the attention paid by researchers to the im-
the relationships between different aspects of body portance of body image in understanding depression
image and psychological, social, and sexual function- among women. In contrast to conceptualizations of
ing, rather than simply to document the existence body dissatisfaction as either a symptom or source
or prevalence of body image concerns. Previous re- of depression (Boggiano & Barrett, 1991; Koenig &
search, based on correlational analyses, has tended Wasserman, 1995; McCarthy, 1990), it may be better
to conclude that a negative body image is associ- understood in this context as an aspect of self-esteem
ated with impaired psychological and interpersonal (Allgood-Merten, Lewinsohn, & Hops, 1990). Thus,
functioning. However, we used hierarchical regres- although men and women with a negative body im-
sion analyses that controlled for the effects of pos- age were more likely than others to report nega-
sible moderator variables (self-esteem, depression, tive social and sexual functioning and to experience
anxiety, BMI, and cross-sex relations), and found symptoms of depression and anxiety, this appeared
that body image variables did not contribute to a to be due to the presence of a negative general self-
unique understanding of psychological, social, and concept.
sexual functioning among most groups. This conclusion is made tentatively, given that
An exception was found for self-esteem as a de- it is contrary to much of the literature, and may be
pendent variable. Self-esteem was predicted by body considered a preliminary finding. However, with the
image variables among all groups. There were few exception of depression, the relationships between
gender differences in the overall strength of the asso- body image and psychological, social, and sexual
ciation between body image and self-esteem, a find- functioning have received little previous empirical in-
ing that supports a number of previous studies of col- vestigation, even among samples of young women. In
lege students (e.g., Abell & Richards, 1996; Stowers the limited research available authors failed to con-
& Durm, 1996), but is inconsistent with the conclu- sider the role of self-esteem, with the exception of
Adult Body Image 473

Allgood-Merten et al. (1990) whose conclusions sup- disturbance more typically observed among women,
port those of the present study. The current method- such as low satisfaction with their appearance, high
ology does not allow for a direct evaluation of the social physique anxiety, attempts to conceal their
relationships for men and women of different age bodies from others, and a tendency to compare their
groups, due to limitations in sample sizes. Replica- appearance to others, are more likely to experience
tion of the findings is recommended, particularly us- significant difficulties in their interpersonal func-
ing methods of analyses that allow for modeling of re- tioning, most noticeably in the sexual arena. Social
lationships, with particular attention paid to the role aspects of body image played a particularly im-
of self-esteem. For example, self-esteem may act as portant role in middle-aged men’s interpersonal
an important mediating factor between body image functioning. To illustrate, high social physique anxi-
and day-to-day functioning. ety was a particularly strong predictor of low sexual
Of interest in this study is the finding that body optimism, which suggests that middle-aged men who
image played a role in psychological functioning were concerned about others evaluating their bodies
among men and women over 50 years of age, in were likely to expect unrewarding future sexual
contrast to other adults. This was the only group interactions.
for whom body image contributed to a unique un- In contrast to the findings with men, women
derstanding of depression and anxiety, beyond the who expressed dissatisfaction with their bodies, and
shared association with self-esteem. Social aspects of women who worried about how they “shaped up” in
body image were most relevant, as men in late adult- comparison with others and how others may perceive
hood who engaged in a high level of appearance com- their bodies, experienced relatively few problems in
parison reported higher levels of anxiety and self- their psychological, social, or sexual functioning be-
esteem than did men who were not concerned about yond poor general self-esteem. The well-established,
how they looked in comparison to others. In addi- normative nature of women’s views of their bod-
tion, women in late adulthood who were highly con- ies may result in their body image concerns having
cerned about how others may evaluate their appear- only a limited negative association with other aspects
ance were more likely than other women their age of women’s lives. This point has been made previ-
to report symptoms of depression and social anxi- ously in relation to women’s views of their sexual-
ety. Thus, although in general older men and women ity (Wiederman & Hurst, 1997), but can be extended
were less concerned about the social aspect of body to include more general psychological and social
image than younger individuals, the minority who did functioning.
hold such concerns experienced symptoms of nega- This research has demonstrated the importance
tive psychological adjustment. of considering multiple measures of body image,
Although body image was found to play a less given that different measures were associated with
important role in social and sexual functioning than different aspects of psychological, social, and sex-
previously proposed, it did appear to have particular ual functioning. Social aspects of body image, par-
relevance to the social and sexual functioning of ticularly concerns about how others may evaluate
men during middle adulthood, that is, men be- one’s body, are a particular area that requires fur-
tween the ages of 30 and 50 years. Men undergo ther research. The results of the present research
a number of changes at this stage of their lives, also demonstrated the importance of investigating
in their interpersonal relationships, their roles at the effects of body image separately for men and
work, their families, and also in their physiques. It is women and for different age groups. This is the first
during this developmental period when the negative study to demonstrate that body image may play dif-
physical effects of aging tend to become particularly ferent roles in the lives of different adult popula-
apparent; men continue to gain body fat up until the tions. Replication of these findings is required, par-
age of 50 years, particularly around the abdomen ticularly in longitudinal research, in order to explore
area (Bemben, Massey, Bemben, Boileau, & Misner, potential underlying mechanisms to explain the role
1998). Men do not typically express concerns about of body image in the psychological, social, and sex-
these changes directly, and they report a more pos- ual functioning of men and women at different stages
itive body image than similarly aged women, both of adult development. The current sample was di-
in this study and in previous research (Feingold & vided into three broad age categories, on the ba-
Mazzella, 1998). However, it appears that a minority sis of sample size. Future researchers exploring the
of men, who present with the type of body image development of body image in adulthood should
474 Davison and McCabe

consider theoretically developed stages of adult de- Ben-Tovim, D. I., & Walker, M. K. (1994). The influence of age
velopment when selecting appropriate age categories and weight on women’s body attitudes as measured by the
Body Attitudes Questionnaire (BAQ). Journal of Psychoso-
to investigate. For example, body image may play a matic Research, 38, 477–481.
different role in the lives of adults 50–65 years than Berscheid, E., Dion, K., Walster, E., & Walster, G. W. (1971).
for adults in later years. Smaller, more homogeneous Physical attractiveness and dating choice: A test of the match-
ing hypothesis. Journal of Experimental and Social Psychol-
groups may demonstrate differences in the develop- ogy, 7, 173–189.
ment of body image and highlight specific associa- Boggiano, A. K., & Barrett, M. (1991). Gender differences in de-
tions of body image and day-to-day functioning at pression in college students. Sex Roles, 25, 595–605.
Coakes, S. J., & Steed, L. G. (1999). SPSS: Analysis without an-
different ages. guish: Versions 7.0, 7.5, 8.0 for Windows. Brisbane, Australia:
This study was limited by the use of correlational Jacaranda Wiley.
data. Small sample sizes in each group precluded the Davison, T. E. (2002). Body image and psychological, social,
and sexual functioning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation,
use of more sophisticated techniques, such as struc- Deakin University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia.
tural equation modeling, which may be employed Denniston, C., Roth, D., & Gilroy, F. (1992). Dysphoria and body
in future research with larger samples to model re- image among college women. International Journal of Eating
Disorders, 12, 449–452.
lationships between body image and psychological, Drewnowski, A., & Yee, D. K. (1987). Men and body image:
social, and sexual functioning variables. An investi- Are males satisfied with their body weight? Psychosomatic
gation of these relationships was beyond the scope Medicine, 49, 626–634.
Eklund, R. C., Kelley, B., & Wilson, P. (1997). The Social Physique
of this article, and they were not accounted for in Anxiety Scale: Men, women, and the effects of modifying item
the present analysis, which was focused on under- 2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 188–196.
standing which specific aspects of body image were Faith, M. S., & Schare, M. L. (1993). The role of body image in
sexually avoidant behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22,
of most relevance to particular aspects of day-to- 345–356.
day functioning. Future researchers may gainfully Feingold, A. (1992). Good-looking people are not what we think.
model the nature of the relationships between dif- Psychological Bulletin, 111, 304–341.
Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender differences in body
ferent aspects of body image for different popula- image are increasing. Psychological Science, 9, 190–195.
tions. It is hoped that increased acknowledgement of Halliwell, E., & Dittmar, H. (2003). A qualitative investigation of
the complexity of the body image construct, partic- women’s and men’s body image concerns and their attitudes
toward aging. Sex Roles, 49, 675–684.
ularly in relation to the varied roles it plays in the Harmatz, M. G., Gronendyke, J., & Thomas, T. (1985). The under-
lives of adult men and women, will stimulate fur- weight male: The unrecognized problem group of body image
ther theoretical and empirical development in this research. Journal of Obesity and Weight Regulation, 4, 258–
267.
area. Hart, E. A., Leary, M. R., & Rejeski, W. J. (1989). The measure-
ment of social physique anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exercise
Psychology, 11, 94–104.
REFERENCES Harter, S. H. (1999). The construction of the self: A development
perspective. New York: Guilford.
Holmes, T., Chamberlin, P., & Young, M. (1994). Relations of ex-
Abell, S., & Richards, M. (1996). The relationship between body ercise to body image and sexual desirability among a sample
shape dissatisfaction and self-esteem: An investigation of gen- of university students. Psychological Reports, 74, 920–922.
der and class differences. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, Hoyt, W. D., & Kogan, L. R. (2001). Satisfaction with body im-
25, 691–703. age and peer relationships for males and females in a college
Allaz, A. F., Bernstein, M., Rouget, P., Archinard, M., & Morabia, environment. Sex Roles, 45, 199–215.
A. (1998). Body weight preoccupation in middle-age and age- Koenig, L. J., & Wasserman, E. L. (1995). Body image and dieting
ing women: A general population survey. International Jour- failure in college men and women: Examining links between
nal of Eating Disorders, 23, 287–294. depression and eating problems. Sex Roles, 32, 225–249.
Allgood-Merten, B., Lewinsohn, P. M., & Hops, H. (1990). Sex Lovibond, P. F., & Lovibond, S. H. (1995). The structure of nega-
differences and adolescent depression. Journal of Abnormal tive emotional states: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety
Psychology, 99, 55–63. Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety
Australian Bureau of Statistics. (1998). How Australians measure Inventories. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33, 335–343.
up. Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics. Mable, H. M., Balance, W. D. G., & Galgan, R. J. (1986). Body-
Banfield, S. S., & McCabe, M. P. (2002). An evaluation and clinical image distortion and dissatisfaction in university students.
implications of the construct of body image. Adolescence, 37, Perception and Motor Skills, 63, 907–911.
373–394. Marsh, H. W. (1997). The measurement of physical self-concept: A
Beebe, D. W. (1995). The Attention to Body Shape Scale: A new construct validation approach. In K. R. Fox (Ed.), The phys-
measure of body focus. Journal of Personality and Assess- ical self: From motivation to well-being (pp. 27–58). Cham-
ment, 65, 486–501. paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Bemben, M. G., Massey, B. H., Bemben, D. A., Boileau, R. A., Marsh, H. W. (1989). Age and sex effects in multiple dimensions
& Misner, J. E. (1998). Age-related variability in body com- of self-concept: Preadolescence to early adulthood. Journal of
position methods for assessment of percent fat and fat-free Educational Psychology, 81, 417–430.
mass in men aged 20–74 years. Age and Ageing, 27, 147– Martin, K. A., Rejeski, J. W., Leary, M. R., McAuley, E., &
153. Bane, S. (1997). Is the Social Physique Anxiety Scale really
Adult Body Image 475

multidimensional? Conceptual and statistical arguments for a tivation, 1984 (pp. 277–307). Lincoln, NE: University of Ne-
unidimensional model. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychol- braska Press.
ogy, 19, 359–367. Rosen, J. C., Srebnik, D., Saltzberg, E., & Wendt, S. (1991). De-
McCarthy, M. (1990). The thin ideal, depression, and eating dis- velopment of a Body Image Avoidance Questionnaire. Psy-
orders in women. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 28, 205– chological Assessment, 3, 32–37.
225. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and adolescent self-image. Prince-
Mintz, L. B., & Betz, N. E. (1986). Sex differences in the na- ton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
ture, realism, and correlates of body image. Sex Roles, 15, 85– Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self. New York: Basic
98. Books.
Mitchell, K. R., & Orr, F. E. (1976). Heterosexual social Sarwer, D. B., Wadden, T. A., & Foster, G. D. (1998). Assess-
competence, anxiety, avoidance, and self-judged physi- ment of body image dissatisfaction in obese women: Speci-
cal attractiveness. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 43, 553– ficity, severity, and clinical significance. Journal of Consulting
554. and Clinical Psychology, 66, 651–654.
Monteath, S. A., & McCabe, M. P. (1997). The influence of societal Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1985). The Self-Consciousness
factors on female body image. Journal of Social Psychology, Scale: A revised version for use with general populations.
137, 708–727. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 15, 687–699.
Montepare, J. M. (1996). An assessment of adults’ perceptions of Silberstein, L. R., Striegel-Moore, R. H., Timko, C., & Rodin, J.
their psychological, physical, and social ages. Journal of Clin- (1986). Behavioral and psychological implications of body dis-
ical Geropsychology, 2, 117–128. satisfaction: Do men and women differ? Sex Roles, 19, 219–
Motl, R. W., & Conroy, D. E. (2000). Validity and factorial in- 232.
variance of the Social Physique Anxiety Scale. Medicine and Snell, W. E., Jr. (1995). The Multidimensional Sexual Self-Concept
Science in Sports and Exercise, 32, 1007–1017. Questionnaire. In C. M. Davis, W. L. Yarber, R. Bauseman,
Nezlek, J. B. (1988). Body image and day-to-day social interaction. G. Schree, & S. L. Davis (Eds.), Handbook of sexuality-related
Journal of Personality, 67, 793–817. measures (pp. 521–524). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
O’Brien, E. J., & Epstein, S. (1988). MSEI: The Multidimensional Snell, W. E., Jr. (2001). Measuring multiple aspects of the
Self-Esteem Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment sexual self-concept: The Multidimensional Sexual Self-
Resources. Concept Questionnaire. In W. E. Snell, Jr. (Ed.), New
Paxton, S. J., & Phythian, K. (1999). Body image, self-esteem, and directions in the psychology of human sexuality: Re-
health status in middle and later adulthood. Australian Psy- search and theory. Cape Girardeau, MO: Snell. Elec-
chologist, 34, 116–121. tronic book retrieved December 2004 from: http://cstl-cla.
Petrie, T. A., Diehl, N., Rogers, R. L., & Johnson, C. L. (1996). semo.edu/snell/books/sexuality/sexualtity.htm
The Social Physique Anxiety Scale: Reliability and construct Stormer, S. M., & Thompson, J. K. (1996). Explanations of body
validity. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 420– image disturbance: A test of maturational status, negative ver-
425. bal commentary, social comparison, and sociocultural hypoth-
Pliner, P., Chaiken, S., & Flett, G. L. (1990).Gender differences in esis. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 19, 193–202.
concern with body weight and physical appearance over the Stowers, D. A., & Durm, M. W. (1996). Does self-concept depend
life span. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 263– on body image? A gender analysis. Psychological Reports, 78,
273. 643–646.
Powell, M. R., & Hendricks, B. (1999). Body schema, gen- Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L., & Tantleff, S. (1991). The Physical
der, and other correlates in nonclinical populations. Genet- Appearance Comparison Scale (PACS). Behavior Therapy,
ics, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 125, 333– 14, 174.
412. Tiggemann, M. (1994). Gender differences in the interrelation-
Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). Self-esteem and neg- ships between weight dissatisfaction, restraint, and self-
ative affect as moderators of sociocultural influences on body esteem. Sex Roles, 30, 319–330.
dissatisfaction, strategies to decrease weight, and strategies to Walster, E., Aronson, V., & Abrahams, D. (1966). Importance of
increase muscles among adolescent boys and girls. Sex Roles, physical attractiveness in dating behavior. Journal of Person-
44, 189–207. ality and Social Psychology, 4, 508–516.
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1985). Women Wiederman, M. W., & Hurst, S. R. (1997). Physical attractiveness,
and weight: A normative discontent. In T. B. Sonderegger body image, and women’s sexual self-schema. Psychology for
(Ed.), Psychology and gender: Nebraska symposium on mo- Women Quarterly, 21, 567–80

You might also like