You are on page 1of 8

Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602

DOI 10.1007/s12671-016-0635-4

ORIGINAL PAPER

The Differential Moderating Roles of Self-Compassion


and Mindfulness in Self-Stigma and Well-Being Among People
Living with Mental Illness or HIV
Xue Yang 1 & Winnie W. S. Mak 2

Published online: 12 November 2016


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract In addition to endorsing the content of stigmatizing Keywords Self-compassion . Mindfulness . Self-stigma .
thoughts (self-stigma Bcontent^), how frequently and auto- Mental habit . Life satisfaction . People with mental illness .
matically individuals think about these thoughts (self-stigma People living with HIV
Bprocess^) also have implications for their well-being. The
present study examined the roles of self-compassion and
mindfulness in moderating the relationships of self-stigma Introduction
content and process with subjective well-being of people in
recovery of mental illness (PMI) and people living with HIV Highly stigmatized groups, such as people in recovery of men-
(PLHIV). Participants included 169 PMI and 291 PLHIV in tal illness (PMI) and people living with HIV (PLHIV), may
Hong Kong who reported their levels of self-compassion, internalize negative evaluations from the public and subse-
mindfulness, self-stigma content and process, and life satis- quently react with negative self-perceptions and social with-
faction. Path analyses indicated that the proposed model fitted drawal (Corrigan and Watson 2002; Mak et al. 2007a).
the two samples well, χ 2 (10) = 19, p = .04, CFI = .98, However, having self-stigmatizing thoughts (self-stigma
NNFI = .93, and RMSEA = .04. In both groups, self- content) may only partially explain their well-being.
compassion and mindfulness were significantly associated Considering how frequently and automatically they think about
with life satisfaction. Self-compassion moderated the relation- these self-stigmatizing thoughts (self-stigma process) may fur-
ship between self-stigma content and life satisfaction among ther contribute to the understanding of their well-being (Chan
PLHIV, while mindfulness moderated the relationship be- and Mak 2016). Most studies have focused on the content of
tween self-stigma process and life satisfaction among PMI. self-stigma in isolation from the process in which individuals
The differential moderating roles of self-compassion and frequently and automatically think about these self-stigmatizing
mindfulness in buffering the effects of self-stigma content thoughts (Corrigan and Watson 2002; Corrigan et al. 2006).
and process among PMI and PLHIV were identified, and im- The literature on self-stigma process is sparse and has only
plications for stigma reduction and well-being promotion in included PMI samples (Chan and Mak 2015, 2016).
different stigmatized groups were discussed. Mindfulness and self-compassion have been suggested to buff-
er against self-stigma and enhance well-being among people
with various negative experiences (e.g., Keng et al. 2011;
MacBeth and Gumley 2012) through conceptually different
mechanisms (Neff and Dahm 2014). However, empirically lit-
* Winnie W. S. Mak
wwsmak@psy.cuhk.edu.hk
tle is known on the extent to which or the mechanism by which
they work on self-stigmatizing thoughts in affecting well-being.
1
Centre for Health Behaviours Research, JC School of Public Health
Self-stigma content refers to the endorsement of the content
and Primary Care, Faculty of Medicine, The Chinese University of of negative stereotypes and evaluations towards the self
Hong Kong, Shatin, NT, Hong Kong among people with stigmatized identities, and it is the focus
2
Department of Psychology, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, of most studies on self-stigma in the literature (e.g., Mak et al.
Shatin, NT, Hong Kong 2007a, b; Ritsher et al. 2003; Yanos et al. 2008). Typical and
596 Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602

common contents of self-stigmatizing thoughts including self- Davis et al. 2007; Teasdale et al. 2000), which is strongly
defeating perceptions of being a burden, having a tainted life, related with the development and relapse of mental disorders.
and having inconvenience in daily life are shared by various Mindfulness-based interventions have been suggested to Bbe
stigmatized groups including PMI and PLHIV (Corrigan et al. uniquely suited to impact distress related to symptoms and
2012; Mak et al. 2007a, b; Ritsher et al. 2003). It is the cog- internalized stigma that are particularly salient for individuals
nitive component of self-stigma and has been shown to be living in the community with severe mental illness who are
associated with low self-worth, self-esteem, and self-efficacy susceptible to experiences of social rejection and interpersonal
(Corrigan et al. 2006; Phillips et al. 2002; Van Brakel 2006). stress^ (Davis and Kurzban 2012, p. 228).
What is missing from this body of self-stigma literature is According to Neff (2003), self-compassion involves posi-
the frequency and degree of automaticity in which individuals tive self-perceptions as well as sensitivity to one’s own expe-
think about these thoughts (Verplanken and Velsvik 2008). rience of suffering. It consists of three components including
The repetition and automaticity of self-stigmatizing thoughts self-kindness (versus self-judgment), common humanity (ver-
is referred to as self-stigma process (Chan and Mak 2016). sus isolation), and mindfulness (versus over-identification).
Specifically, the process includes two features: (1) the repeat- Self-kindness has a soothing quality that offers oneself under-
ed occurrence of negative thoughts about the stigmatized iden- standing and warmth instead of being coldly indifferent, self-
tity and (2) the fact that such thoughts come automatically, critical, or self-blaming in the face of inadequacy or failure.
unconsciously, implicitly, and unintentionally. Frequent and Common humanity provides emotional safety by recognizing
automatic self-stigmatizing thinkers often show a high level one’s suffering does not occur in isolation, but is intimately
of experiential avoidance and a lack of mindfulness in the face interconnected with others and inherent to the nature of life.
of self-stigmatizing thoughts (Chan and Mak 2016). They Finally, the mindfulness facet of self-compassion is similar to
may be entangled in a metacognitive process that involves the abovementioned notion of mindfulness but is narrower in
undue negative evaluations of having such self-stigmatizing scope (Rapgay and Bystrisky 2009). It refers to maintaining a
thoughts, an unwillingness to accept such thoughts balanced perspective in the midst of adversity and difficulties.
(McCracken and Zhao-O’Brien 2010), and deliberate attempts With self-compassion, positive emotions are generated by em-
to eliminate, suppress, or avoid these thoughts (Hayes et al. bracing the negative ones, rather than replacing the negatives
1999). Ironically, striving to control unwanted thoughts may with the positives (Germer and Neff 2013). Given its compre-
paradoxically elevate their frequency and accessibility hensive multi-faceted nature, self-compassion has been found
(Abramowitz et al. 2001; Gross 2002). This vicious circle to be associated with better well-being (e.g., life satisfaction,
can lead to constant mental negotiations within individ- psychological health, and happiness) and less maladaptive
uals, contributing to their experience of self-stigma and perfectionism (Neff 2003; Neff et al. 2005, 2007). People with
chronic distress. depression and anxiety were found to have lower symptom
The cultivation of mindfulness could be an effective ap- severity and higher quality of life when their self-compassion
proach in curtailing the vicious circle in self-stigmatizing is promoted (Van Dam et al. 2011).
thoughts (Chan and Mak 2016). Mindfulness entails being Although both mindfulness and self-compassion may buff-
aware of the present moment experience in a nonjudgmental er negative self-judgment, turn towards painful experiences
manner regardless of whether it is positive, negative, or neu- with an accepting stance, and thereby enhance subjective
tral (Brown and Ryan 2003; Kabat-Zinn 2003). In the face of well-being (e.g., Keng et al. 2011; MacBeth and Gumley
unwanted thoughts and experiences, mindful individuals tend 2012), their distinctions and unique implications for human
to be aware of their body sensations, thoughts, and emotions functioning are worth noting. Mindfulness, as defined by most
with less reactivity and keep a stance of equanimity instead of contemporary literature, has an emphasis on metacognition,
engaging in suppression or excessive fixation. By adding clar- that is, the ability to direct and sustain attention nonjudgmen-
ity and vividness to the experience and by orienting to the tally, to observe our current and internal experience, and to be
present moment with curiosity and openness, mindfulness Bexperientially open^ (Bishop et al. 2004). Therefore, it stands
has shown to improve emotion regulation, distress tolerance, in contrast to the mindless, less Bawake^ states of habitual or
and well-being (Bishop et al. 2004; Hofmann et al. 2010; automatic functioning that may be persistent for many indi-
Ryan and Deci 2000). The nonjudgmental view of one’s viduals. Cultivating mindfulness is the key in disengaging
thoughts and the acceptance-based approach promoted by individuals from automatic thoughts, habits, and unhealthy
mindfulness-based interventions can provide a metacognitive behavior patterns and in buffering the negative effect of rumi-
insight into and acceptance of the illness and its symptoms, nation on well-being (Alleva et al. 2014; Hawley et al. 2014;
alleviating the ruminative patterns of depressive episodes, Ryan and Deci 2000; Verplanken 2012).
stress, and suicidal ideation for individuals with various men- Self-compassion, on the other hand, focuses on cultivating
tal disorders, including anxiety, social phobia, depression, and a positive attitude and state of mind in dealing particularly
schizophrenia (Chadwick et al. 2009; Chien and Lee 2013; with suffering (Germer 2009; Neff and Dahm 2014). In
Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602 597

addition to not over-identifying with negative experiences, Department of Health. PMI were recruited from four non-
self-compassionate persons also actively soothe and comfort governmental organizations (NGOs) in Hong Kong that pro-
oneself when painful experiences arise and remember that vided community mental health services to people with men-
such experiences are part of being human. When the painful tal illness in Hong Kong. PLHIV were recruited at the only
experience involves thoughts of inadequacy (e.g., self- community-based outpatient clinic specialized in treating
stigmatizing judgments), extra mental effort may be needed HIV-related illnesses in Hong Kong. The study was first in-
to curb self-criticism and to maintain a gentle kindness and troduced to PMI by social workers at the service units or to
compassion towards the self (Neff 2003). For example, al- PLHIV by nurses at the outpatient clinic. For those individuals
though PLHIV might have applied the HIV-related critical who expressed initial interest, the staff referred them to our
judgments and inadequacy about the self, by being self-com- trained research assistants, who explained the study purpose to
passionate, they are able to appreciate their imperfection that them for their informed consent. All participants were in-
is shared with other human beings and be mindfully aware of formed that their decision to participate is unrelated to the
their stigmatizing experiences, instead of denying themselves services received. Research assistants collected data when cli-
as a whole, which mitigated the effect self-stigma content has ents visited the organizations and were waiting for service. All
on their well-being. participants provided written informed consent prior to com-
The present study aimed to address the relationships of pleting the battery of questionnaires. Upon completion of the
self-stigma content and process with subjective well-being questionnaires, each participant was given HK$50
and to identify the potential moderating roles of mindfulness (∼US$6.45) as compensation for their time spent on the study.
and self-compassion on these relationships in two highly stig-
matized groups. We hypothesized that self-stigma content and Measures
process would be negatively related to subjective well-being.
In addition, mindfulness would be especially useful in weak- Mindfulness
ening the association between self-stigma process and subjec-
tive well-being, while self-compassion would be a powerful The tendency to be mindful in daily life was measured by the
buffer against the association between self-stigma content and validated Chinese version of the 20-item Five Facet
subjective well-being. Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Hou et al. 2014). It in-
cludes five dimensions: observing, describing, acting with
awareness, nonreactivity, and nonjudging. The participants
Method rated each item on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
(never) to 5 (always). Higher scores indicated higher levels
Participants of mindfulness. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas were
.75 in PLHIV and .90 in PMI.
Sample 1 consisted of 169 PMI (52.6% males) with an aver-
age age of 42.7 years (SD = 10.2). Most of them (68%, Self-Compassion
n = 116) were single. Participants included people with
schizophrenia (67.2%, n = 114), depression (13.6%, n = 23), The Chinese version of the Self-Compassion Scale-Short
bipolar disorder (11.2%, n = 19), or other mental disorders Form (SCS-SF; Raes et al. 2011) was used to measure the
(8%, n = 14). Most of them (97.6%, n = 165) reported using level of self-compassion. The 12 items were rated on a 5-
psychotropic medication. Sample 2 consisted of 291 PLHIV point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never experience the
(94.8% men) with an average age of 41.8 years (SD = 11.1). feeling) to 5 (experience the feeling continuously or almost
Most of them (67%, n = 195) were single. Around half of them continuously). Higher scores indicated higher levels of self-
identified themselves as homosexual (48.3%, n = 140), compassion. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas were .70
while the rest reported being bisexual (24.1%, n = 70) or in PLHIV and .82 in PMI.
heterosexual (23.4%, n = 68). Over half of them reported that
their CD4 levels were lower than 500 cells/uL (55.2%, Self-Stigma Content
n = 137), and 43.2% reported that their CD4 levels were
between 500 and 1000 cells/uL. Most of them (87.2%, The cognitive subscale of the Self-Stigma Scale (SSS; Mak
n = 254) were receiving anti-retroviral therapy. and Cheung 2010), which was originally developed in
Chinese and validated in Hong Kong, was used to measure
Procedure the degree of endorsement of self-stigmatizing cognitive con-
tent. Participants rated the extent to which they endorse each
Ethics approval was obtained from the Clinical Research item on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6
Ethics Committee of the authors’ institution and the (strongly agree). The target identity in the items was changed
598 Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602

according to the participants. A sample item is BMy identity as Results


a person with mental illness/living with HIV is a burden to
me.^ Higher scores indicated greater endorsement of self- Significant differences in the means of variables between the
stigma content. In the present study, Cronbach’s alphas of two samples were found (see Table 1). Compared to PMI,
the scale were .82 in PLHIV and .86 in PMI. PLHIV were significantly higher on their levels of mindful-
ness, self-compassion, self-stigma content, and self-stigma
process (ps < .01). Life satisfaction was not significantly dif-
Self-Stigma Process ferent between the two samples. In both samples, all variables
had significant correlations with each other (see Table 1).
Five items in the Self-stigmatizing Thinking’s Automaticity Mindfulness and self-compassion were positively associated
and Repetition Scale that was originally developed and vali- with life satisfaction, while self-stigma content and process
dated among Chinese in Hong Kong (STARS; Chan and Mak were negatively associated with life satisfaction (ps < .05).
2016) were employed. Participants rated the extent to which Path analyses showed that the proposed model (Fig. 1)
they endorse each item on a 6-point Likert scale from 1 fitted the data in each group well, χ2(5) = 10.55, p = .06,
(strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). A sample item is CFI = .98, NNFI = .93, and RMSEA = .06 in PLHIV and
BThinking negatively about my MI/HIV identity is something χ 2 ( 5 ) = 8 . 4 5 , p = . 0 4 , C F I = . 9 8 , N N F I = . 9 3, a n d
I do every day.^ Higher scores indicated more frequent and RMSEA = .06 in PMI. Multi-sample path analysis showed
automatic self-stigmatizing thoughts. Cronbach’s alphas were that PLHIV and PMI in the present study shared the same
.80 in PLHIV and .89 in PMI. model structure (Hu and Bentler 1999; Satorra and Bentler
2001), χ2(10) = 19.00, p = .04, CFI = .98, NNFI = .93, and
RMSEA = .04.
Life Satisfaction For PLHIV, self-compassion (β = .27, p < .001), mindful-
ness (β = .20, p < .01), and self-stigma content (β = −.24,
The 5-item Chinese version of the Satisfaction with Life Scale p < .001) were significantly related to life satisfaction, where-
(Diener et al. 1985; Wu and Yao 2006) was used to measure as no significant relationship between self-stigma process and
subjective well-being. Items are rated on a 7-point Likert scale life satisfaction was found (β = .08, p > .05). For PMI, self-
(1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree), with higher compassion (β = .22, p < .01) and mindfulness (β = .23,
scores indicating high life satisfaction. Cronbach’s alphas p < .01) were significantly related to life satisfaction, whereas
were .88 in PLHIV and .89 in PMI. self-stigma content (β = −.09, p > .05) and process (β = −.12,
p > .05) were not significantly related to life satisfaction after
controlling for self-compassion and mindfulness.
Data Analyses For PLHIV, the interaction between self-compassion and
self-stigma content was significantly related to life satisfaction
Independent t tests were conducted by SPSS 22.0 to identify (β = .11, p < .05), whereas the interaction between mindfulness
the group difference in the means of variables. Path analyses and self-stigma process was not significant (β = .04, p > .05).
and multi-sample path analyses were conducted by AMOS 19 Simple slope analysis showed that when self-compassion was
to test the proposed model, moderation effects, and whether low, self-stigma content was significantly and negatively as-
the two groups shared the same model structure. All tests were sociated with life satisfaction (t = −4.22, p < .05). When self-
two-tailed with a significance level of p < .05. compassion was high, the association was not significant

Table 1 Correlations and


independent t tests of M (SD) in M (SD) in t 1 2 3 4 5
psychological variables PLHIV PMI

1. Mindfulness 3.23 (0.40) 3.02 (0.39) 5.70** – .44** −.15* −.48** .38**
2. Self-compassion 3.41 (0.52) 3.12 (0.53) 5.69** .53** – −.32** −.41** .34**
3. Self-stigma content 4.20 (1.30) 3.48 (1.31) 5.74** −.25** −.31** – .38** −.21**
4. Self-stigma process 3.35 (0.69) 3.18 (0.89) 2.15** −.27** −.26** .51** – −.35**
5. Life satisfaction 4.60 (1.22) 4.35 (1.43) 1.95 .37** .41** −.31** −.15* –

Correlation coefficients below lines for PLHIV and above lines for PMI
*
p < .05; ** p < .01
Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602 599

the heterogeneity in the illness experience, social identity, and


Self-compassion
the consequences that they have to bear in the society between
PMI and PLHIV.
.11*/.06 For PMI, mindfulness buffered against the negative asso-
.27**/.22**
ciation between self-stigma process and well-being. This find-
Self-stigma content ing added further understanding on the role of self-stigma
process and echoed previous research on mindfulness
-.24**/-.09
Life satisfaction (Verplanken and Fisher 2014). Different from the findings of
Chan and Mak (2016), self-stigma process imposed no signif-
Self-stigma process icant main effect on well-being. However, given that self-
.08/-.12
.20**/.23** stigma process was correlated with life satisfaction bivariately,
04/.19** it is plausible that the linkage between self-stigma process and
well-being was not robust in the current sample and its effect
Mindfulness was accounted when controlling for other variables in the path
analysis. Given the limited literature on self-stigma process
Fig. 1 Model testing between samples with standardized direct effects and its impact, more studies with empirical evidence are war-
(PLHIV/PMI) ranted. Relative to PLHIV, PMI are often described in the
mass media with derogatory linguistic references (e.g., mad,
(t = −1.13, p > .05). For PMI, the interaction effect between nuts, crazy, psycho). The frequency of the negative thoughts
mindfulness and self-stigma process was significantly related of their mental illness identity may play a major role in affect-
to life satisfaction (β = .19, p < .05), whereas the interaction ing their subjective well-being, and their mental illness iden-
effect between self-compassion and self-stigma content was tity becomes more salient than other social identities (Quinn
not significant (β = .06, p > .05). Simple slope analysis and Chaudoir 2009; Quinn and Earnshaw 2011). Thus, when
showed that when mindfulness was low, self-stigma process they lack mindfulness, PMI may judge themselves as inferior
was significantly and negatively associated with life satisfac- to the public frequently and without conscious awareness,
tion (t = −3.16, p < .05). When mindfulness was high, the as- which dispose them to chronic distress and poor well-being.
sociation was not significant (t = 0.08, p > .05). The findings shed light on the importance of mindfulness for
The alternative model which included interaction effects the well-being of PMI, especially for those who have automat-
not only between self-compassion and self-stigma content ic and frequent self-stigmatizing thoughts. Consistent with the
and between mindfulness and self-stigma process but also effect of mindfulness, although individuals frequently think
between self-compassion and self-stigma process as well as about themselves negatively, as long as they allow unwanted
between mindfulness and self-stigma content was tested. The thoughts to come and go without automatic reactions, their
results showed significantly poorer model fit compared with subjective well-being may be intact (Verplanken and Fisher
our proposed model, Δχ2(22) = 124.54, p < .05. In addition, 2014). Furthermore, the significant relationships among mind-
the interaction effects between self-compassion and self- fulness, self-stigma process, and well-being among PMI may
stigma process (for PLHIV, β = .07, p > .05; for PMI, be due to the fact that self-stigma process is closely related to
β = .14, p > .05) and between mindfulness and self-stigma rumination which has been found to be particularly harmful to
content (for PLHIV, β = −.02, p > .05; for PMI, β = −.09, the well-being and symptoms (e.g., increased depression and
p > .05) were not significant in the model. It indicated that psychopathologies) of PMI (Nolen-Hoeksema et al. 2008).
the proposed model was more parsimonious and preferred. Future studies should test the role of rumination in affecting
the current model among PMI and the relationship between
rumination and self-stigma process.
For PLHIV, self-stigma content was significantly related to
Discussion subjective well-being and self-compassion moderated this re-
lationship. Although both self-stigma content and process
The present study investigated the moderating roles of self- were higher among PLHIV than PMI, what they thought
compassion and mindfulness in the relationships between self- about their PLHIV status had more impact on their well-
stigma content and process with subjective well-being among being than how often they negatively thought about them-
PMI and PLHIV. Findings suggested that both mindfulness selves. The reason may be that given the most prominent
and self-compassion were positively associated with well- way of HIV transmission in Hong Kong is through unprotect-
being for the two groups while their moderating effects were ed sexual intercourse, with much higher percentage of HIV
group-specific. It is plausible that the specific moderating ef- transmission being observed among men who have sex with
fects of self-compassion and mindfulness can be explained by men (Mak et al. 2006; Virtual AIDS Office of Hong Kong
600 Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602

2015), the public has low acceptance towards homosexuality versus process. A more integrative perspective is highlighted
in Hong Kong, and such stigma is morally based and unduly that both self-compassion and mindfulness need to be consid-
harsh (Equal Opportunities Commission 2016). As such, the ered in self-stigma reduction programs since they target at
extent to which PLHIV who have internalized the prevalent different self-stigma facets. Although interventions, such as
negative stereotypes towards PLHIV in the society has a sig- mindfulness-based stress reduction (Kabat-Zinn 2003),
nificant impact on their well-being, and this overrides how mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (Teasdale et al. 2000),
much they think about such thoughts. It is consistent with and mindful self-compassion training (Germer and Neff
the finding that when PLHIV perceive that the stigma against 2013), have consistently shown to be effective in promoting
them implies a sexual or moral transgression, they are more subjective well-being for people living with psychiatric symp-
likely to endorse others’ negative judgments, socially with- toms (e.g., Mayhew and Gilbert 2008; Teasdale et al. 2000) or
draw, and suffer physical and mental health problems HIV (e.g., Brion et al. 2014; Brion and Menke 2008), few of
(Young et al. 2007). Furthermore, self-compassion could buff- them have specifically examined their relationships with self-
er against the negative relationship between self-stigma con- stigma. Our findings provide an alternative approach and en-
tent and well-being in PLHIV, which is consistent with the courage future self-compassion and mindfulness-based inter-
argument that the understanding and warmth to oneself of- ventions to target at self-stigma content and process. Through
fered by self-compassion serves as a useful antidote to the these interventions, stigmatized individuals can decenter self-
harsh negative self-thoughts (Neff 2003). stigmatizing thoughts from themselves, develop kindness and
The present study demonstrated the group-specific moder- warmth towards their self-stigmatizing thoughts, recognize
ating roles of self-compassion and mindfulness in the relation- their adversities are shared human experience, and mindfully
ships between self-stigma content and process with well-being extend their self-thoughts beyond their stigmatized status by
among PMI and PLHIV. Although both of them originate embracing both negative and positive aspects of themselves
from Buddhism, they take different paths to promote well- (Lillis et al. 2009; Yadavaia and Hayes 2012).
being. According to our findings, while the ameliorative ef-
fects of self-compassion are specific to self-stigma content, the
salutary effects of mindfulness are specific to self-stigma pro- Limitations and Future Directions
cess. In addition, although prior research has established the
detrimental effects of self-stigma content on stigmatized indi- Several limitations are worth being noted when applying these
viduals, it has neglected the role of the process by which findings in future research. First, we only adopted self-report
individuals think about the self-stigma content. The present measures. Future research should adopt different (e.g., implic-
study found that whether the frequency and automaticity of it, physical, behavioral) measuring methods to validate the
the negative thoughts are related to well-being was contingent findings. Also, the cross-sectional data restricted its ability in
on their mindfulness levels, especially for people in recovery making causal inferences. Experimental studies that induce
of mental illness. Meanwhile, whether the content of their self-compassion and/or mindfulness states in different stigma-
stigmatizing thoughts is detrimental to their well-being was tized groups are warranted to draw causality. To examine the
contingent on their level of self-compassion, especially among possible utility of long-term training in the diminution of the
PLHIV. Given that the specific moderation effects of self- negative impact of self-stigma, future work should also utilize
compassion and mindfulness in PLHIV and PMI might be established training methods to cultivate self-compassion and
group-specific, it highlights the need to be careful when trying mindfulness (e.g., Germer and Neff 2013; Kabat-Zinn 2003;
to generalize the self-stigma models and interventions to dif- Teasdale et al. 2000). Regarding the sample of PMI, the pres-
ferent stigmatized groups in future work. Nevertheless, future ent study has considered individuals with different mental
studies are encouraged to test the moderating roles of mind- disorders as a collective group with mental illness. Although
fulness and self-compassion on self-stigma content and pro- a recent study found no significant difference in the relation-
cess in different samples of PLHIVand PMI as well as in other ship between self-stigma and recovery outcomes across sam-
stigmatized populations for replication of the findings. ples of individuals with psychotic disorders, mood disorders,
Practically, the findings shed light on several previously and substance use disorders (Mak et al. 2016), future studies
under-examined perspectives when considering interventions should consider potential differences on the impact of self-
of self-compassion and mindfulness to reduce stigma and pro- stigma content and process on well-being of individuals
mote well-being. First, the model differences in the two sam- across different mental disorders by examining their effects
ples cautioned the generalization of interventions and the need separately. Moreover, although a recent meta-analysis focus-
to account for the heterogeneity of stigmatized groups (e.g., ing on 13 studies examining the effects of mindfulness-based
illness experience, stigmatizing attributes, stereotype content, interventions (which includes mindfulness-, compassion-, and
and attributions of being stigmatized). Interventions also need acceptance-based interventions) for psychosis found a medi-
to differentiate between the effects of self-stigma content um effect size (Khoury et al. 2013), and another study also
Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602 601

found Chinese with schizophrenia in Hong Kong who partic- Chien, W. T., & Lee, I. Y. (2013). The mindfulness-based
psychoeducation program for Chinese patients with schizophrenia.
ipated in a mindfulness-based psychoeducation to have signif-
Psychiatric Services, 64, 376–379. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.002092012.
icant improvements in symptoms, functioning, and number Corrigan, P. W., & Watson, A. C. (2002). The paradox of self-stigma and
and length of rehospitalization, compared with usual care mental illness. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 9, 35–53.
(Chien and Lee 2013), given the number of controlled studies doi:10.1093/clipsy.9.1.35.
Corrigan, P. W., Watson, A. C., & Barr, L. (2006). The self-stigma of
on mindfulness-based interventions for individuals with psy-
mental illness: implications for self-esteem and self-efficacy.
chotic disorders was small and the mindfulness-based inter- Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25, 875–884.
ventions used were diverse, heterogeneity in outcomes was doi:10.1521/jscp.2006.25.8.875.
large. As such, caution should be made when applying Corrigan, P. W., Morris, S. B., Michaels, P. J., Rafacz, J. D., & Rüsch, N.
(2012). Challenging the public stigma of mental illness: a meta-
mindfulness-based interventions for individuals with psychot-
analysis of outcome studies. Psychiatric Services, 63, 963–973.
ic disorders in future studies. Finally, the study should be doi:10.1176/appi.ps.201100529.
replicated with other stigmatized groups to investigate the Davis, L., & Kurzban, S. (2012). Mindfulness-based treatment for
moderating effects of mindfulness and self-compassion on people with severe mental illness: a literature review.
American Journal of Psychiatric Rehabilitation, 15, 202–
self-stigma content and process.
232. doi:10.1080/15487768.2012.679578.
Davis, L. W., Strasburger, A. M., & Brown, L. F. (2007). Mindfulness: an
intervention for anxiety in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychosocial
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Nursing and Mental Health Services, 45, 23–29.
Diener, E. D., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The
Funding This study was funded by the University Direct Grant of the satisfaction with life scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49,
Chinese University of Hong Kong (Ref No. 2021054 and 2021091) and 71–75. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4901_13.
the Social Welfare Development Fund (Ref. No R03). Equal Opportunities Commission (2016). Study on Legislation against
Discrimination on the Grounds of Sexual Orientation, Gender
Conflict of Interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of Identity and Intersex Status. Report commissioned by the Equal
interest. Opportunities Commission and conducted by Gender Research
Centre of the Hong Kong Institute of Asia-Pacific Studies of The
Chinese University of Hong Kong. Retrieved from http://www.eoc.
org.hk/eoc/upload/ResearchReport/20161251750293418312.pdf.
References Germer, C. K. (2009). The mindful path to self-compassion: freeing your-
self from destructive thoughts and emotions. New York: Guilford.
Abramowitz, J. S., Tolin, D. F., & Street, G. P. (2001). Paradoxical effects Germer, C. K., & Neff, K. D. (2013). Self-compassion in clinical
of thought suppression: a meta-analysis of controlled studies. practice. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69, 856–867.
Clinical Psychology Review, 21, 683–703. doi:10.1016/S02727358 doi:10.1002/jclp.22021.
(00)00057X. Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and
Alleva, J., Roelofs, J., Voncken, M., Meevissen, Y., & Alberts, H. (2014). social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39, 281–291.
On the relation between mindfulness and depressive symptoms: doi:10.1017/S0048577201393198.
rumination as a possible mediator. Mindfulness, 5, 72–79. Hawley, L. L., Schwartz, D., Bieling, P. J., Irving, J., Corcoran, K., Farb,
doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0153-y,0.01,0.01,0. N. A., et al. (2014). Mindfulness practice, rumination and clinical
outcome in mindfulness-based treatment. Cognitive Therapy and
Bishop, S. J., Duncan, J., & Lawrence, A. D. (2004). State anx-
iety modulation of the amygdala response to unattended Research, 38, 1–9. doi:10.1007/s10608-013-9586-4.
threat-related stimuli. The Journal of Neuroscience, 24, Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and
10364–10368. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2550-04.2004. commitment therapy: an experiential approach to behavior change.
New York: Guilford.
Brion, J. M., & Menke, E. M. (2008). Perspectives regarding adherence to
Hofmann, S. G., Sawyer, A. T., Witt, A. A., & Oh, D. (2010). The effect
prescribed treatment in highly adherent HIV-infected gay men.
of mindfulness-based therapy on anxiety and depression: a meta-
Journal of the Association of Nurses in AIDS Care, 19, 181–191.
analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
doi:10.1016/j.jana.2007.11.006.
78, 169–183. doi:10.1037/a0018555.
Brion, J. M., Leary, M. R., & Drabkin, A. S. (2014). Self-compassion and Hou, J., Wong, S. Y. S., Lo, H. H. M., Mak, W. W. S., & Ma, H. S. W.
reactions to serious illness: the case of HIV. Journal of Health (2014). Validation of a Chinese version of the five facet mindfulness
Psychology, 19, 218–229. doi:10.1177/1359105312467391. questionnaire in Hong Kong and development of a short form.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: Assessment, 21, 363–371. doi:10.1177/1073191113485121.
mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in
Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848. covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alter-
Chadwick, P., Hughes, S., Russell, D., Russell, I., & Dagnan, D. (2009). natives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal,
Mindfulness groups for distressing voices and paranoia: a replica- 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118.
tion and randomized feasibility trial. Behavioural and Cognitive Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past,
Psychotherapy, 37, 403–412. doi:10.1017/S1352465809990166. present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10,
Chan, K. S. K., & Mak, W. W. S. (2015). Attentional bias associated with 144–156. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg016.
habitual self-stigma in people with mental illness. PloS One, 10(7), Keng, S. L., Smoski, M. J., & Robins, C. J. (2011). Effects of mindfulness
e0125545. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0125545. on psychological health: a review of empirical studies. Clinical
Chan, K. S. K., & Mak, W. W. S. (2016). The content and process of self- Psychology Review, 31, 1041–1056. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2011.04.006.
stigma in people with mental illness. American Journal of Khoury, B., Lecomte, T., Gaudiano, B. A., & Paquin, K. (2013).
Orthopsychiatry. Mindfulness interventions for psychosis: a meta-analysis.
602 Mindfulness (2017) 8:595–602

Schizophrenia Research, 150, 176–184. doi:10.1016/j. Quinn, D. M., & Earnshaw, V. A. (2011). Understanding concealable
schres.2013.07.055. stigmatized identities: the role of identity in psychological, physical,
Lillis, J., Hayes, S. C., Bunting, K., & Masuda, A. (2009). Teaching and behavioral outcomes. Social Issues and Policy Review, 5, 160–
acceptance and mindfulness to improve the lives of the obese: a 190. doi:10.1037/a0015815.
preliminary test of a theoretical model. Annals of Behavioral Raes, F., Pommier, E., Neff, K. D., & Van Gucht, D. (2011).
Medicine, 37, 58–69. doi:10.1007/s12160-009-9083-x. Construction and factorial validation of a short form of the
MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: a meta- self-compassion scale. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy,
analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopa- 18(3), 250–255. doi:10.1002/cpp.702.
thology. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 545–552. doi:10.1016/j. Rapgay, L., & Bystrisky, A. (2009). Classical mindfulness. Annals
cpr.2012.06.003. of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1172, 148–162.
Mak, W. W. S., & Cheung, R. Y. M. (2010). Self-stigma among conceal- doi:10.1002/cpp.702.
able minorities in Hong Kong: conceptualization and unified mea- Ritsher, J. B., Otilingam, P. G., & Grajales, M. (2003). Internalized stigma
surement. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 80, 267–281. of mental illness: psychometric properties of a new measure.
doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01030.x. P s y c h i a t r y R e s e a rc h , 1 2 1 , 3 1 – 4 9 . d o i : 1 0 . 1 0 1 6 / j .
Mak, W. W. S., Mo, P. K. H., Cheung, R. Y. W., Woo, J., Cheung, F. M., psychres.2003.08.008.
& Lee, D. (2006). Comparative stigma of HIV/AIDS, SARS, and Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the
tuberculosis in Hong Kong. Social Science & Medicine, 63, 1912– facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-
1922. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.016. being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. doi:10.1037/0003-066
Mak, W. W. S., Cheung, R. Y. M., Law, R. W., Woo, J., Li, P. C. K., & X.55.1.68.
Chung, R. W. Y. (2007a). Examining attribution model of self- Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M. (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test
stigma on social support and psychological well-being among peo- statistic for moment structure analysis. Psychometrika, 66, 507–514.
ple with HIV+/AIDS. Social Science & Medicine, 64, 1549–1559. doi:10.1007/BF02296192.
doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.12.003. Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M. G., Ridgeway, V. A.,
Mak, W. W. S., Poon, C. Y. M., Pun, L. Y. K., & Cheung, S. F. (2007b). Soulsby, J. M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/
Meta-analysis of stigma and mental health. Social Science & recurrence in major depression by mindfulness-based cognitive ther-
Medicine, 65, 245–261. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.03.015. apy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615–623.
Mak, W. W. S., Chan, R. C. H., Wong, S. Y. S., Lau, J. T. F., Tang, W. K., doi:10.1037//0022-006X.68.4.615.
Tang, A. K. L., et al. (2016). A cross-diagnostic investigation of the
Van Brakel, W. H. (2006). Measuring health-related stigma—a
differential impact of discrimination on clinical and personal recov-
literature review. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 11, 307–
ery. Psychiatric Services.
334. doi:10.1080/13548500600595160.
Mayhew, S. L., & Gilbert, P. (2008). Compassionate mind training with
Van Dam, N. T., Sheppard, S. C., Forsyth, J. P., & Earleywine, M. (2011).
people who hear malevolent voices: a case series report. Clinical
Self-compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom
Psychology & Psychotherapy, 15, 113–138. doi:10.1002/cpp.566.
severity and quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. Journal
McCracken, L. M., & Zhao-O’Brien, J. (2010). General psychological
of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 123–130.
acceptance and chronic pain: there is more to accept than the pain
Verplanken, B. (2012). When bittersweet turns sour: adverse effects of
itself. European Journal of Pain, 14, 170–175. doi:10.1016/j.
nostalgia on habitual worriers. European Journal of Social
ejpain.2009.03.004.
Psychology, 42, 285–289. doi:10.1002/ejsp.1852.
Neff, K. D. (2003). The development and validation of a scale to
measure self-compassion. Self and Identity, 2, 223–250. Verplanken, B., & Fisher, N. (2014). Habitual worrying and benefits of
doi:10.1080/15298860309027. mindfulness. Mindfulness, 5, 566–573. doi:10.1007/s12671-013-
Neff, K. D., & Dahm, K. A. (2014). Self-compassion: what it is, what it 0211-0.
does, and how it relates to mindfulness. In M. Robinson, B. Meier, Verplanken, B., & Velsvik, R. (2008). Habitual negative body image
& B. Ostafin (Eds.), Handbook of mindfulness and self-regulation thinking as psychological risk factor in adolescents. Body Image,
(pp. 121–137). New York: Springer. 5, 133–140. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.11.001.
Neff, K. D., Hsieh, Y., & Dejitthirat, K. (2005). Self-compassion, Virtual AIDS Office of Hong Kong (2015, August 8). Retrieved from
achievement goals, and coping with academic failure. Self and http://www.info.gov.hk/aids/english/surveillance/latest_stat.htm.
Identity, 4, 263–287. doi:10.1080/13576500444000317. Wu, C. H., & Yao, G. (2006). Analysis of factorial invariance across
Neff, K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion gender in the Taiwan version of the satisfaction with life scale.
and adaptive psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality and Individual Differences, 40, 1259–1268.
Personality, 41, 139–154. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.11.001.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. E., & Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Yadavaia, J. E., & Hayes, S. C. (2012). Acceptance and commitment
Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, therapy for self-stigma around sexual orientation: a multiple baseline
400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x. evaluation. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 19, 545–559.
Phillips, M. R., Pearson, V., Li, F., Xu, M., & Yang, L. (2002). Stigma and doi:10.1016/j.cbpra.2011.09.002.
expressed emotion: a study of people with schizophrenia and their Yanos, P. T., Roe, D., Markus, K., & Lysaker, P. H. (2008). Pathways
family members in China. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, between internalized stigma and outcomes related to recovery in
488–493. doi:10.1192/bjp.181.6.488. schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatric Services, 59, 1437–
Quinn, D. M., & Chaudoir, S. R. (2009). Living with a concealable 1442. doi:10.1176/appi.ps.59.12.1437.
stigmatized identity: the impact of anticipated stigma, centrality, Young, S. D., Nussbaum, A. D., & Monin, B. (2007). Potential moral
salience, and cultural stigma on psychological distress and health. stigma and reactions to sexually transmitted diseases: evidence for a
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 634–651. disjunction fallacy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
doi:10.1037/a0015815. 789–799. doi:10.1177/0146167207301027.

You might also like