You are on page 1of 8

Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

The brain in your pocket: Evidence that Smartphones


are used to supplant thinking q
Nathaniel Barr 1, Gordon Pennycook ⇑,1, Jennifer A. Stolz, Jonathan A. Fugelsang
Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Canada

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: With the advent of Smartphone technology, access to the internet and its associated knowledge base is at
one’s fingertips. What consequences does this have for human cognition? We frame Smartphone use as
an instantiation of the extended mind—the notion that our cognition goes beyond our brains—and in so
Keywords: doing, characterize a modern form of cognitive miserliness. Specifically, that people typically forego
Smartphone use effortful analytic thinking in lieu of fast and easy intuition suggests that individuals may allow their
Internet use Smartphones to do their thinking for them. Our account predicts that individuals who are relatively less
Cognitive miserliness
willing and/or able to engage effortful reasoning processes may compensate by relying on the internet
Dual-process theories
Extended mind
through their Smartphones. Across three studies, we find that those who think more intuitively and less
Situated cognition analytically when given reasoning problems were more likely to rely on their Smartphones (i.e., extended
mind) for information in their everyday lives. There was no such association with the amount of time
using the Smartphone for social media and entertainment purposes, nor did boredom proneness qualify
any of our results. These findings demonstrate that people may offload thinking to technology, which in
turn demands that psychological science understand the meshing of mind and media to adequately char-
acterize human experience and cognition in the modern era.
Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

‘‘...the medium is the message... the personal and social conse- bounded by wires, next by proximity to a router, the advent and
quences of any medium - that is, of any extension of ourselves - rise of the Smartphone means that the internet and the massive
result from the new scale that is introduced into our affairs by each knowledge base it contains now knows no bounds for billions.
extension of ourselves, or by any new technology.’’ Soon the number of Smartphones in use will be in the billions
[McLuhan (1964), p. 8] and the anticipated power of such devices is expected to continue
to grow at a rate reminiscent of science fiction rather than science
fact (Miller, 2012). Smartphones have undeniably become a medi-
1. Introduction um with a very important message – a message that has yet to be
deciphered.
McLuhan (1964) turned the attention of the world to the impact
that technological change can impart by famously proclaiming
1.1. The extended mind
‘‘the medium is the message.’’ The internet, although now ubiqui-
tous, is a recent addition to our society that has afforded unprece-
The extended mind – the notion that the mind goes beyond
dented access to information of nearly every possible kind. This
grey matter – is an idea that allows an understanding of human
easily accessible online knowledge base has largely supplanted
cognition as acting in a coupled system with the environment
other media in terms of acquiring knowledge for daily life. As such,
(Clark & Chalmers, 1998; Clark, 2008). In the seminal work on this
few, if any, media have been as monumental as the internet. First
topic, Clark and Chalmers (1998) define an extended cognitive sys-
tem as an external object that serves to accomplish a function that
q
Funding for this study was provided by the Natural Sciences and Engineering would otherwise be attained via the action of internal cognitive
Research Council of Canada.
⇑ Corresponding author at: Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, processes. A simple example is the jotting down of a number on
200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, ON N2L 3G1, Canada. paper, rather than maintaining that same information in memory.
E-mail address: gpennyco@uwaterloo.ca (G. Pennycook). The technological properties of Smartphones provide an exciting
1
These authors contributed equally to this work. and interesting new means of externalization. Such devices go

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.029
0747-5632/Ó 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
474 N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480

beyond assisting memory through simple maintenance, as has long 2. Study 1


been done with various media, and in fact accomplish much in the
way of the storage and retrieval of memory as well. As an initial test of the proposed association between Smart-
Given the significance of the central tenet of this idea, as well as phone (SP) use and heuristic thinking, we gave participants a set
the staggering efficiency with which ‘cognitive’ functions can now of ‘‘heuristics and biases’’ problems that have been used extensive-
operate externally, it is surprising how little work has directly ly in previous research. Each problem is designed to cue an incor-
explored Smartphones within such a framework. Empirical support rect intuitive response that is difficult to override and is therefore
for the notion that the internet, an integral component of the considered at least partially reflective of analytic cognitive style
Smartphone’s utility, acts as an extension of the mind comes from (otherwise referred to as thinking disposition, see Stanovich,
research demonstrating that people think of, and use, the internet 2009). Participants were also asked to indicate if they own a Smart-
as a transactive, or external, memory source (Sparrow, Liu, & phone and, if so, how much time they spend on it generally and
Wegner, 2011). One can easily probe the depths of the internet, using search engines in particular.
as opposed to one’s own mind to retrieve information, thus limit-
ing the use of effortful cognitive processes. 2.1. Method

1.2. Cognitive miserliness and dual-process theories 2.1.1. Participants


Participants in Study 1 were Americans recruited through
Parallel to this work, a long tradition of reasoning research has Mechanical Turk™, an online marketplace where ‘workers’ can sign
demonstrated that humans are ‘‘cognitive misers’’ (Kahneman, up for paid studies (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011). Par-
2011; Stanovich, 2004), a term adopted from early information ticipants who failed an attention check question were excluded
processing frameworks (Dawes, 1976; Taylor, 1981). Empirical evi- from analysis. For this, participants were shown a list of activities
dence shows that people tend to eschew costly analytic thought in and asked to write ‘‘I read the instructions’’ in the ‘‘other’’ box if
favor of comparatively effortless intuitive processing (for reviews, they were, in fact, reading the instructions. The final sample
see Baron, 1998; Evans, 1989; Evans & Over, 1996; Kahneman, included 190 individuals (94 females, Mage = 35).
2003; Kahneman, Slovic, & Tversky, 1982; Shafir & Tversky,
1995; Stanovich & West, 2000). Consider the following example 2.1.2. Measures
from the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT; Frederick, 2005): These data were collected as part of a larger set of studies on
A bat and a ball cost $1.10 in total. The bat costs $1.00 more political ideology, religious beliefs, and moral values. As such, addi-
than the ball. How much does the ball cost? _____ cents. tional measures were included in the study session that are not of
interest here, and are thus not reported.
The bat and ball problem cues an intuitive response (i.e., 10
cents) that can be shown to be obviously incorrect using a very 2.1.2.1. Cognitive. Cognitive measures for Study 1 included 4 syllo-
simple mathematical operation (.10 + 1.00 + .10 = 1.20). However, gisms (De Neys & Franssens, 2009), 4 base-rate problems (De Neys
college students and participants recruited online nonetheless tend & Glumicic, 2008), and a 14 item ‘‘heuristics and biases’’ battery
to have a great deal of difficulty correctly solving the problem – (Toplak, West, & Stanovich, 2011). Individual items for the cogni-
with typical scores falling around 33% correct (Campitelli & tive measures can be found in Supplementary materials. An overall
Gerrans, 2014; Frederick, 2005). cognitive style score was computed by taking the mean of the
The difficulty that people have with problems like the above three measures.
example is easily understood under a dual-process perspective of
cognitive functioning (e.g., Evans & Stanovich, 2013). Dual-process 2.1.2.2. Smartphone use. After answering a set of demographic
theories distinguish between autonomously cued intuitive or questions, participants were asked to indicate whether they own
‘‘Type 1’’ processes that require few cognitive resources on the a Smartphone (SP). Those who responded affirmatively were asked
one hand and more deliberative ‘‘Type 2’’ processes that require to indicate roughly how many minutes they spend per day (a) on
working memory capacity on the other (Evans & Stanovich, their SP and (b) on their SP specifically using search engines.
2013; see also Evans, 2009; Sloman, 1996; Stanovich, 2009;
Thompson, Prowse Turner, & Pennycook, 2011). Under this expla-
2.2. Results
nation, performance on the bat and ball problem is relatively low
because the features of the problem cause Type 1 processing to
Performance did not significantly differ between SP owners
rapidly output a response that requires resource demanding Type
(N = 131) and non-owners (N = 47) on any of the cognitive mea-
2 processes to override. Thus, in other words, the miserly nature
sures, t’s < 1.5, p’s > .18. To assess the predicted association
of human cognition lends itself to an overreliance on simple
between performance and SP use, SP owners were broken up into
heuristics and mental shortcuts (Kahneman, 2011; Kahneman
three roughly equivalent usage groups (low, medium, and high).
et al., 1982).
This approach was taken in lieu of correlational analyses because
it is not influenced by outliers and as a consequence does not
1.3. Hypothesis require case exclusion. We computed an overall performance score
on the cognitive style measures and compared the three usage
One potential consequence of the accessibility of Smartphone groups using one-way ANOVA’s (see Table 1). There were main
technology is that the general disinclination and/or inability to effects of SP usage group, F(2, 128) = 9.61, MSE = .03, p < .001,
engage analytic thinking may now be applicable not only to reli- gp2 = .13, and SP search engine usage group, F(2, 128) = 10.76,
ance on intuitive and heuristic thinking, but also to no thinking MSE = .03, p < .001, gp2 = .14 (see Table 1). Follow-up t-tests
at all. A straightforward prediction follows from this line of reason- revealed that there was no difference between low and medium
ing: There should be a relation between these two forms of cogni- usage groups for general SP use, t < 1. However, all three search
tive miserliness, such that those more prone to rely on intuitive engine use groups differed, all t’s > 2.68, p’s < .01.
cognitive heuristics should be more prone to heavy Smartphones The main effect of SP usage group held for each of the individual
use. We tested this prediction in three studies. cognitive style measures: Syllogisms, F(2, 128) = 3.65, MSE = .134,
N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480 475

Table 1 miserliness. Cognitive style refers to the disposition to engage


Mean accuracy on cognitive style measures as a function of number of minutes using analytic thinking whereas cognitive ability refers to the computa-
Smartphone.
tional power and knowledge that is available for use. Both factors
Low Medium High ANOVA undermine elaborative Type 2 processing and, as a consequence,
Overall .54 .51 .39 F = 9.61*** they both could theoretically contribute to a ‘‘Smartphone heuris-
Search engine .57 .47 .41 F = 10.76*** tic’’. This more encompassing approach to rationality was pio-
***
p 6 .001.
neered by Stanovich and West (e.g., Stanovich & West, 1997,
1998, 2000) is becoming more common in the literature (e.g.,
Barr, Pennycook, Stolz, & Fugelsang, 2014; Pennycook, Cheyne,
p = .029, Base-rate problems, F(2, 128) = 4.52, MSE = .141, p = .013, Barr, Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2014; Pennycook, Cheyne, Seli,
and the Heuristics and biases battery, F(2, 128) = 7.62, MSE = .03, Koehler, & Fugelsang, 2012; Toplak et al., 2011). It is important
p = .001. The main effect of SP search engine usage group also held to keep in mind, however, that using performance-based measures
for each of the individual cognitive style measures: Syllogisms, to assess cognitive style means that all cognitive style and ability
F(2, 128) = 3.26, MSE = .135, p = .042, Base-rate problems, measures necessarily require some degree of ability and cognitive
F(2, 128) = 3.94, MSE = .142, p = .022, and the Heuristics and biases motivation. The measures should therefore be viewed as more
battery, F(2, 128) = 10.31, MSE = .03, p < .001. reflective of one over the other, but not as purely one or the other
(for a similar discussion, see Pennycook et al. (2014)).
2.3. Discussion
3.1. Method
In Study 1 we found that participants who reported a relatively
3.1.1. Participants
high level of SP usage scored lower on each of the cognitive style
Participants were Americans recruited through Mechanical
measures employed. In contrast, there were no differences
Turk™. As in Study 1, participants who failed an attention check
between SP owners and non-owners. This null result supports
question were excluded from analysis. The final sample consisted
the causal explanation that we have hypothesized. Namely, if SP
of 208 participants (84 females, Mage = 34.5).
use over time leads to an increase in cognitive miserliness (as
opposed to the converse), one would expect SP owners to score
3.1.2. Measures
lower than non-owners. Thus, our results supported the hypothesis
As with Study 1, these data were collected as part of a larger set
that cognitive misers offload analytic thinking to their SP.
of studies on political ideology, religious beliefs, and moral values.

3. Study 2 3.1.2.1. Cognitive. Cognitive style measures included 3 base-rate


problems (as above) and the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT;
A simple alternative explanation for the association between SP Frederick, 2005). Cognitive ability measures included a 3 item
usage and reasoning performance would be that the participants numeracy test (Schwartz, Woloshin, Black, & Welch, 1997) and
who fell in the high SP usage group are simply worse at estimating the Wordsum verbal intelligence test (Huang & Hauser, 1998).
how much time they spend on things, leading to overestimation Individual items for the cognitive measures can be found in Sup-
much of the time. Presumably, if this were the case, the relatively plementary materials.
more heuristic thinkers in the sample would be more prone to
overestimation. To rule out this possibility, we asked participants 3.1.2.2. Smartphone and computer use. Participants who reported
to estimate how much time they spend on computers, both online owning a SP were asked to estimate how much time they spent
and specifically using search engines. For the participants who online and specifically using search engines on their SP. We also
have a SP, there should be no relation between computer usage included two additional questions asking participants to estimate
and heuristic thinking because, presumably, access to a SP would the amount of time they spent on their SPs using social media
undermine the potential function of the computer as an extended and for general entertainment. Finally, participants were asked to
mind source. Certainly, a lack of association for computer usage indicate how many minutes per day they spent online using a com-
would suggest that the lower accuracy on the heuristics and biased puter (generally and specifically using search engines, social
measures for the high SP usage group is not simply due to media, and for general entertainment).
overestimation.
A second alternative explanation relates to the multiple poten- 3.2. Results
tial online uses of the SP. We have hypothesized a direct link
between cognitive miserliness and use of the SP as an information Performance did not significantly differ between SP owners
source. However, in Study 1 we found that use of search engines on (N = 98) and non-owners (N = 106) on any set of cognitive mea-
the SP and general SP use were both associated with lower perfor- sures, t’s < 1.1, p’s > .25. As in Study 1, participants were broken
mance on the reasoning tasks. Thus, to investigate the possible up into three roughly equivalent usage groups for each SP and
association between cognitive miserliness and alternative online computer use item (low, medium, and high; see Table 2). There
uses of the SP, we also asked participants to estimate how much were main effects of SP online usage group for both cognitive style,
time per day they spend using their SP for general entertainment F(2, 92) = 3.75, MSE = .07, p = .027, gp2 = .08, and ability, F(2, 92) =
purposes (e.g., YouTube, reddit, tumblr) and on social media (e.g., 8.23, MSE = .04, p = .001, gp2 = .15 (see Table 2) composite scores.
Facebook, Twitter). These questions were also extended to com- There were also main effects of SP search engine usage group for
puter use. both cognitive style, F(2, 92) = 3.87, MSE = .07, p = .024, gp2 = .08,
Finally, we also extended our battery of cognitive tasks to and ability, F(2, 92) = 6.14, MSE = .04, p = .003, gp2 = .12 (see
include cognitive ability measures. In contrast to cognitive style Table 2). There were no differences between low and medium
measures, cognitive ability measures do not cue an intuitive usage groups for both general and specifically search engine relat-
response and therefore primarily assess the participants’ ability ed SP use on either measure, t’s < 1.
to apply analytic thinking to a problem. As a consequence, cogni- The main effect of SP usage group held for each of the individual
tive style and ability assess different dimensions of cognitive cognitive style and ability measures, with the exception of base-
476 N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480

Table 2
Mean accuracy on cognitive style and ability measures as a function of number of minutes spend online using Smartphones (SP) and computers.

Cognitive Style Cognitive Ability


Low Medium High ANOVA Low Medium High ANOVA
Smartphone Overall .44 .38 .25 F = 3.75* .69 .75 .53 F = 8.23***
Search engine .43 .40 .25 F = 3.87* .69 .74 .56 F = 6.14**
Social media .42 .30 .36 F = 1.43 .73 .62 .64 F = 2.20
Entertainment .38 .45 .30 F = 1.79 .69 .75 .61 F = 2.42
Computer (SP owners) Overall .38 .37 .37 F = .02 .65 .71 .69 F = .64
Search engine .41 .40 .30 F = 1.62 .71 .69 .64 F = .85
Social media .37 .43 .33 F = 1.08 .71 .67 .67 F = .31
Entertainment .36 .38 .38 F = .04 .67 .67 .75 F = 1.17
Computer (SP non-owners) Overall .35 .41 .34 F = .69 .69 .75 .71 F = .71
Search engine .44 .38 .19 F = 6.10** .71 .75 .66 F = 1.83
Social media .36 .37 .36 F = .01 .78 .68 .66 F = 4.41*
Entertainment .31 .40 .41 F = 1.24 .70 .72 .75 F = .45
*
p 6 .05.
**
p 6 .01.
***
p 6 .001.

rate problems: Cognitive Reflection Test, F(2, 92) = 5.39, MSE = 3.3. Discussion
.139, p = .006, Base-rate problems, F(2, 92) = 1.16, MSE = .09,
p = .318, Numeracy, F(2, 92) = 8.07, MSE = .097, p = .001, and Word- Study 2 replicated and extended the results of Study 1. As in
sum, F(2, 92) = 3.26, MSE = .03, p = .043. The same pattern of results Study 1, participants who reported relatively high levels of general
occurred for SP search engine usage: Cognitive Reflection Test, online SP use and SP search engine use scored lower on measures
F(2, 92) = 4.84, MSE = .141, p = .010, Base-rate problems, F(2, 92) = of analytic cognitive style. This finding was extended to cognitive
1.50, MSE = .09, p = .230, Numeracy, F(2, 92) = 3.99, MSE = .105, ability measures as well, indicating a role for both dispositional
p = .022, and Wordsum, F(2, 92) = 5.59, MSE = .02, p = .005. and computational sources of cognitive miserliness in SP use.
In contrast, there were no main effects of SP social media use for A number of informative null results also emerged from Study
the overall cognitive style or ability scores (see Table 2) or any 2. The amount of estimated time using computers for SP owners
individual measure. There was a small trend for the numeracy did not differ for either general use or any of the specific computer
measure, F(2, 92) = 2.29, MSE = .108, p = .108, but none of the other use items. This indicates that the association between reasoning
measures approached significance, all other F’s < 1.3, p’s > .29. Use performance and SP use cannot be explained by a mere overesti-
of the SP for entertainment purposes was also not associated with mation error among high users. Rather, the evidence suggests that
the overall cognitive style or ability scores (see Table 2), although high SP users are genuinely lower in cognitive ability and have a
there were small trends among all individual measures except for more intuitive cognitive style. In addition, there was limited evi-
the CRT: Cognitive Reflection Test, F(2,92) = .85, MSE = .153, dence for an association between cognitive miserliness and com-
p = .431, Base-rate problems, F(2, 92) = 1.88, MSE = .09, p = .159, puter use among the participants who did not own a SP. Those
Numeracy, F(2, 92) = 1.73, MSE = .110, p = .184, and Wordsum, who used search engines on computers (and who did not own a
F(2, 92) = 2.28, MSE = .03, p = .108. SP) scored lower on the cognitive style, but not ability measures.
Further, there were no main effects in terms of online com- Thus, although it appears that – as predicted – the use of SPs as
puter usage for any cognitive style or ability measure when informational sources has much to do with proximity and ease of
the analysis was isolated to SP owners, all F’s < 1. The same pat- use, offloading thinking by relying on external information sources
tern held for search engine use, all F’s < 1.71, all p’s > .18, social may extend past SPs as well. This is an important question for
media use, all F’s < 1.36, all p’s > .26, and entertainment, all future research.
F’s < 1.76, all p’s > .17. For SP non-owners, there was no main Finally, the use of SPs and computers for entertainment and
effect of general online computer usage for any cognitive style social media did not strongly associate with our cognitive style
or ability measure, all F’s < 1.32, all p’s > .27. However, cognitive and ability measures. The only exception was lower performance
style performance did differ as a function of computer search on cognitive ability measures among SP non-owners who frequent
engine use for SP non-owners (albeit marginally for the CRT): social media sites on the computer. Future research is required to
Overall, F(2, 103) = 6.10, MSE = .07, p = .003, gp2 = .11, Cognitive further probe this observation to determine if this is a stable and
Reflection Test, F(2, 103) = 2.78, MSE = .149, p = .066, and Base- replicable observation or an anomaly. Regardless, this exception
rate problems, F(2, 92) = 4.60, MSE = .10, p = .012. This pattern does not undermine the hypothesis that cognitive misers use SPs
did not hold for cognitive ability measures, all F’s < 1.91, all as an external mind.
p’s > .15. There was also a main effect of cognitive ability for
social media use on the computer among SP non-owners: Over- 4. Study 3
all, F(2, 103) = 4.41, MSE = .04, p = .015, gp2 = .08. This difference
was marginal for Numeracy, F(2, 103) = 2.69, MSE = .09, p = .073, Recent research on cell phone use that is relevant to our
and significant for Wordsum, F(2, 103) = 4.74, MSE = .03, hypothesis has come from a rather different perspective from that
p = .011. However, there were no significant differences for the offered here. Specifically, research demonstrating a negative asso-
cognitive style measures, all F’s < 1. Finally, the amount of time ciation between cell phone use and academic performance (e.g.,
using the computer for entertainment online among SP Hong, Chiu, & Hong, 2012; Jacobsen & Forste, 2011; Lepp,
non-owners was not associated with any cognitive ability or Barkley, & Karpinski, 2014; Sanchez-Martinez & Otero, 2009; Yen
style measures, although there was a trend for the Cognitive et al., 2009) has focused on the cell phone as a source of distraction
Reflection Test, F(2, 103) = 2.26, MSE = .15, p = .109, all other in educational settings. For example, Lepp et al. (2014) found a
F’s < 1. negative correlation between grade point average (GPA) and both
N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480 477

overall cell phone use (i.e., including all uses apart from listening to 4.1.2.2. Smartphone use. The SP use items were identical to Study 1.
music) and text messaging among university students. Lepp et al.’s
explanation for this association was that cell phone use – at least at
4.1.2.3. Boredom proneness. Participants completed a self-report
high levels – distracts students from school work (see also
boredom proneness measure in an earlier session as part of depart-
Jacobsen & Forste, 2011). This hypothesis was supported by anec-
mental mass testing. The Boredom Proneness Scale (Farmer &
dotal evidence from students who report feeling obligated to con-
Sundberg, 1986) includes 28 items such as ‘‘I am not good at wait-
stantly be in touch via texting and/or social media (Lepp et al.,
ing patiently’’ and ‘‘time always seems to pass slowly’’. Participants
2014). Moreover, Jacobsen and Forste (2011) found that more than
responded on a 7-point likert scale from strongly disagree to
two-thirds of their sample of university students reported multi-
strongly agree. Responses were summed to create the overall bore-
tasking with electronic media (including but not limited to cell
dom proneness score. The scale had good internal consistency
phone use) while in class, studying, or doing homework (see also
(Cronbach’s Alpha = .92).
Junco & Cotton, 2011, 2012).
These studies differ from our own in two important respects.
First, we are interested in a very specific form of cell phone use. 4.1.2.4. Grade point average. A self-report estimate of GPA was
Clearly, those who spend a lot of time online on their SPs will also included in a prescreen study at the beginning of the semester. Par-
spend more time on their cell phones in general. However, at least ticipants selected from the following options: (1) below 60%, (2)
in the case of search engine use, this likely only represents a frac- 60–64.9%, (3) 65–69.9%, (4) 70–74.9%, (5) 75–79.9%, (6) 80–
tion of the time spent using cell phones. Second, although aca- 84.9%, (7) 85–89.9%, (8) 90–94.9%, (9) 95–100%.
demic performance is surely facilitated by cognitive
reflectiveness and ability, there are obviously multiple additional
factors that determine success. Thus, it is quite possible that (a) 4.2. Results
academic performance is decreased by general cell phone use
(including internet use in particular) and (b) those who use their Overall cognitive style or ability performance, GPA, and bore-
cell phone as an extended mind (during class and otherwise) are dom proneness did not significantly differ between SP owners
less analytic in disposition and computational power and, as a con- (N = 227) and non-owners (N = 35), t’s < 1.5, p’s > .14. There were,
sequence, have lower academic success. In Study 3, we investigat- however, two marginal differences among the individual measures
ed this possibility by including a measure of self-report GPA in a reflecting better reasoning performance for SP non-owners:
university sample. This also had the added benefit of allowing us Heuristics and biases battery, t(42.2) = 1.84, SE = .04, p = .0742,
to replicate our findings with a different sample. and Wordsum, t(260) = 2.01, MSE = .03, p = .045. The other two mea-
The research discussed above focuses on the cell phone as an sures did not differ: Cognitive Reflection Test, t(260) = .95, MSE = .07,
ultimate source of distraction: students who might normally do p = .342, and Numeracy, t(260) = .47, MSE = .05, p = .637.
well in an academic setting feel obligated or compelled to use their As in Study 1 and 2, SP owners were broken up into three
SP while in class or studying. However, it is possible that, in addi- roughly equivalent usage groups (low, medium, and high; see
tion to this mechanism, there are ultimate psychological factors Table 3). There were main effects of general SP usage group for
that lead to compulsive cell phone use. For example, boredom both cognitive style, F(2, 224) = 7.34, MSE = .05, p = .001, gp2 = .06,
proneness is associated with lower academic performance in uni- and ability, F(2, 224) = 9.44, MSE = .03, p < .001, gp2 = .08, compos-
versity samples (Mann & Robinson, 2009) and internet addiction ite scores (see Table 3). There were also main effects of SP search
in a non-clinical sample (Nichols & Nicki, 2004). Moreover, high engine usage group for cognitive style, F(2, 224) = 4.96, MSE = .05,
cell phone users may be susceptible to boredom during leisure p = .008, gp2 = .04, and, marginally, ability, F(2, 224) = 2.95,
time (Lepp, Li, Barkley, & Salehi-Esfahani, 2015). Thus, it may be MSE = .04, p = .054, gp2 = .03 (see Table 3). There were no differ-
that boredom proneness mediates the association between SP ences between low and medium usage groups for both general
use and reasoning performance. Specifically, boredom proneness and specifically search engine related SP use, t’s < 1.5, p’s > .13.
may lead people to (a) do poorly on reasoning tasks due to a lack While the main effect of search engine use for cognitive ability
of attention and (b) compulsively use their SP. These same people was moderate (p = .054), the high usage group differed from the
would presumably also do poorly in educational settings. To inves- combined low and medium usage groups, t(225) = 2.37, SE = .03,
tigate this possibility we also assessed boredom proneness. p = .019.
The main effect of SP usage group held for each of the individual
cognitive style and ability measures: Cognitive Reflection Test,
4.1. Method
F(2, 224) = 7.51, MSE = .12, p = .001, Heuristics and biases battery,
F(2, 224) = 3.06, MSE = .03, p = .049, Numeracy, F(2, 224) = 6.75,
4.1.1. Participants
MSE = .08, p = .001, and Wordsum, F(2, 224) = 5.75, MSE = .03,
For Study 3, participants were Canadian undergraduate stu-
p = .004. The main effect of SP search engine usage group held for
dents. As above, participants who failed an attention check ques-
both of the individual cognitive style measures: Cognitive Reflec-
tion were excluded from analysis. The final sample included 262
tion Test, F(2, 224) = 4.37, MSE = .12, p = .014, and the Heuristics
participants (193 females, Mage = 20.3).
and biases battery, F(2, 224) = 3.07, MSE = .03, p = .048. Perfor-
mance on the individual cognitive ability measures did not differ
4.1.2. Measures despite the overall main effect: Numeracy, F(2, 224) = 2.55,
These data were collected as part of a larger set of studies on MSE = .08, p = .080, and Wordsum, F(2, 224) = 1.39, MSE = .03,
political ideology, religious beliefs, and moral values. As such, addi- p = .251.
tional measures were included that are not of interest here, and are In contrast to previous work (e.g., Lepp et al., 2014), GPA did not
thus not reported. differ as a function of either general SP use or SP search engine use,
both F’s < 1. Further, boredom proneness also did not differ as a
4.1.2.1. Cognitive. Cognitive style measures included the Cognitive function of either general SP use or SP search engine use, both
Reflection Test and the heuristics and biases battery. Cognitive F’s < 1.
ability measures included the numeracy test and the Wordsum
verbal intelligence test. 2
Equal variances not assumed due to significant Levene’s test, F = 6.2, p = .013.
478 N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480

Table 3
Mean accuracy on cognitive style, ability, and boredom proneness measures as a function of number of minutes using Smartphone.

Cognitive Style Cognitive Ability Boredom Proneness


Low Medium High ANOVA Low Medium High ANOVA Low Medium High ANOVA
Overall .49 .45 .35 F = 7.34*** .70 .68 .58 F = 9.44*** 84.7 85.3 83.3 F = .14
Search engine .49 .44 .36 F = 4.96** .66 .68 .61 F = 2.95  83.7 85.8 83.2 F = .26

p 6 .05.
**
p 6 .01.
***
p 6 .001.
 
p = .054.

4.3. Discussion however, that higher order reasoning is difficult to alter even given
direct interventions (for a short review see, Lilienfeld, Ammirati, &
In Study 3, we replicated the association between high levels of Landfield, 2009). While it seems unlikely that Smartphone use
SP use and cognitive miserliness in a sample of Canadian university would be so influential, future research is required. We advocate
students. However, in contrast to what would be predicted based for longitudinal approaches to this issue (i.e., an analysis of cogni-
on prior research (e.g., Jacobsen & Forste, 2011; Lepp et al., tive changes that accompany adoption or changes in Smartphone
2014), there were no associations between SP use and either GPA use). However, given the rising ubiquity of use, there seems to be
or boredom proneness. Although this may be because our GPA a shrinking temporal window in which to find people who are
measure was obtained via self-report, it is likely that more direct not already reliant to some degree upon such technologies.
questions about SP use during class-time and studying would Reasoning performance also did not generally relate to comput-
reveal differences. Moreover, Jacobsen and Forste (2011) linked a er use of any type or Smartphone use that was specific to social
wide range of electronic media, not just SP use, with diminished media (e.g., Facebook) and online entertainment (e.g., YouTube).
academic performance. Thus, the lack of association between This suggests that the correlation between cognitive sophistication
GPA and SP use does not necessarily undermine these earlier stud- and Smartphone usage cannot be explained by a mere usage
ies. Rather, this null result serves as a strong contrast to the robust reporting bias among the least analytic participants. In other
associations between SP use and our cognitive style and ability words, there was no evidence that cognitive misers were simply
measures. more likely to overestimate time spent doing a given task. More-
Boredom proneness did not relate to either online SP use or over, the specificity of the associations between cognitive miserli-
search engine use despite previous evidence that it is associated ness and Smartphone use (i.e., search engine use, but not social
with internet addiction in a non-clinical sample (Nichols & Nicki, media and online entertainment use) supports the hypothesis that
2004). This null result suggests that boredom proneness may not the association between cognitive miserliness and the extended
play a strong role in general internet use. More importantly for mind is specific to a reliance on outside informational sources.
present purposes, this excludes boredom proneness as a potential
mediator between cognitive miserliness and SP use. 5.1. Cognitive style and Smartphone use

5. General discussion That those less willing to think analytically are more prone to
heavy Smartphone search engine use suggests that people may
We provide evidence that the tendency to rely on intuitive be prone to look up information that they actually know or could
rather than analytic thinking is related to a modern form of cogni- easily learn, but are unwilling to invest the cognitive cost associat-
tive miserliness whereby information is sought from an external ed with encoding and retrieval. Recall that a common characteris-
source (i.e., one’s Smartphone). This pattern was replicated and tic across our cognitive style measures was the presence of an
qualified across three studies and among both Mechanical Turk™ intuitive response that must be overridden to obtain the correct
workers in the United States and Canadian undergraduate stu- answer. Translating this to searching for information on one’s
dents. Moreover, in Study 2, performance on cognitive style mea- phone, it would seem that a feeling of uncertainty could prompt
sures was associated with search engine use on computers, but one to reach for their Smartphone, rather than engage in more
only among those who did not own a Smartphone. In addition, analytic thought. This supposition is consistent with recent
boredom proneness was ruled out as a potential moderator. These research implicating metacognitive feelings of rightness as sources
data suggest that those prone to cognitive miserliness in particular of analytic engagement (Thompson et al., 2011, 2013). Similarly,
can supplant potentially biased and irrational intuitive thinking one might choose not to engage in costly elaborative encoding,
(Kahneman, 2011; Stanovich, 2004) with outsourced information as they know that knowledge can be procured externally.
sources via the Smartphone (Sparrow et al., 2011).
A number of informative null results also emerged from our 5.2. Cognitive ability and Smartphone use
analysis. First, across all three studies, there were generally no dif-
ferences between Smartphone owners and non-owners for the Although the tendency to seek knowledge and information is
cognitive measures. The two exceptions in Study 3 were relatively often equated with intelligence, cognitive ability was associated
small differences that were not consistent across cognitive style with less Smartphone use and less time spend using online search
and ability measures. This general lack of a difference suggests that engines. This connection between cognitive ability and Smart-
owning and (presumably) using a Smartphone is not sufficient to phone use may be reflective of the possibility that more knowl-
strongly alter analytic thinking skill or disposition. However, it is edgeable individuals are less likely to require online information
important to note that the strongest effects of Smartphone usage search when confronted with a problem in everyday life. Our abil-
emerged for those who fell into the highest usage group. Thus, it ity measures included numerical and vocabulary related problems,
is possible that a high level of Smartphone use is required for the and these seem likely types of information that one might search
Smartphone to have negative effects on analytic thinking. We note, for, such as calculating a tip online, or looking up the meaning of
N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480 479

a word. The ability to search for information online, on the go, has mind might have detrimental consequences to cognition (see Carr,
presumably equated the facts and knowledge available for use, 2010). While the lack of overall difference between Smartphone
although differences in online search efficiency are likely (Ford, owners and non-owners is counter to this possibility, further
Miller, & Moss, 2005). While our findings support the idea that research is required before any concrete conclusions can be made.
highly intelligent individuals are less likely to require this external An important issue for future research is to attempt to understand
knowledge, further examination of the association between every- the complex interplay between personality factors (Lane &
day information search and cognitive ability is necessary to come Manner, 2011) and cognitive factors such as those measured here
to any definitive conclusions. that determine the use of Smartphones.
The long-term ramifications of the expanding externalization of
5.3. Limitations our cognitive functions to device remain unseen. Rather than adju-
dicate between those hopeful of the benefits afforded by such
Our data indicate a robust and replicable relation between rea- advances and those fearful of the costs, we hope that our data will
soning performance and high levels of Smartphone use. However, serve as a call to search out both the positive and negative cogni-
the causal explanation of this finding that we have hypothesized tive consequences such media will impart, as there is a demonstra-
– that cognitive miserliness leads individuals to rely on their ble, replicable relation between cognitive factors and the use of
Smartphone as an external mind – requires further experimental Smartphones that raises many questions that cannot be ‘Googled’
work. While this seems to be the most plausible account of our for an answer. In the face of increasing accessibility to increasingly
data (as discussed above), the results reported herein should be powerful personal technological devices (e.g., smart watches, Goo-
considered preliminary. Outside of experimental work, it is also gle Glass), it is time for more psychologists to accept and embrace
possible that the association between cognitive miserliness and that our cognition has transcended skull and skin (Clark &
Smartphone use is explained by some third variable that has not Chalmers, 1998). It remains unclear as to whether such meshing
been assessed here. We have ruled out boredom proneness and of mind and media is a first step toward a new era of postbiological
have illustrated the specificity of the association, but, again, further intelligence (Dick, 2003) or if such changes will have negative con-
work is necessary. sequences for our cognition (Carr, 2010). Time is of the essence, for
In line with previous work (e.g., Barr et al., 2014; Pennycook we ‘‘rapidly. . . approach the final phases of the extensions of man—
et al., 2014), we have labeled the cognitive tasks that cue incorrect the technological simulation of consciousness, when the creative
intuitive responses ‘‘cognitive style’’ measures and those that more process of knowing will be collectively and corporately extended
directly assess specific abilities (i.e., numeracy and verbal intelli- to the whole of human society.’’ (McLuhan, 1964, p. 19).
gence) ‘‘cognitive ability’’ measures. Although we used six different
cognitive measures (four cognitive style and two cognitive ability) Appendix A. Supplementary material
across our three studies, all six measures were relatively short and,
as a consequence, should not be considered full and extensive mea- Supplementary data associated with this article can be found, in
sures of ability and style. We contend that the measures employed the online version, at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.029.
here are sufficient to evidence a basic association between cogni-
tive miserliness in both forms; however, future research with more References
extensive measures – including full intelligence measures and
thinking disposition questionnaires – is necessary to determine Baron, J. (1998). Judgment misguided: Intuition and error in public decision making.
whether cognitive ability or style are differentially influential fac- City: Oxford University Press.
Barr, N., Pennycook, G., Stolz, J. A., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). Reasoned connections: A
tors in determining the use of Smartphones as an extended mind. dual-process perspective on creative thought. Thinking & Reasoning, 21, 61–75.
Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s mechanical turk: A
5.4. Conclusions and future directions new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological
Science, 6, 305.
Campitelli, G., & Gerrans, P. (2014). Does the cognitive reflection test measure
Technological advances have long figured prominently in the cognitive reflection? A mathematical modeling approach. Memory & Cognition,
history of psychological science. Smartphones seem to represent 42, 434–447.
Carr, N. (2010). The shallows: How the internet is changing the way we think, read and
another technology that can fundamentally change psychology,
remember. W.W. Norton.
though unlike neuroimaging or computational technology that Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension.
afforded new ways of answering old questions, this advance inter- Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
acts with, and might also change, the very thing which we aim to Clark, A., & Chalmers, D. (1998). The extended mind. Analysis, 7–19.
Dawes, R. M. (1976). Shallow psychology. In J. S. Carroll & J. W. Payne (Eds.),
understand. Acknowledging the impending integration of man and Cognition and social behavior (pp. 3–11). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
machine will result in a broader understanding of human experi- De Neys, W., & Franssens, S. (2009). Belief inhibition during thinking: Not always
ence and cognition, though the work has only just begun. Given winning but at least taking part. Cognition, 113, 45–61.
De Neys, W., & Glumicic, T. (2008). Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of
the frenetic pace of such technological advances, we believe it thinking. Cognition, 106, 1284–1299.
imperative that more researchers consider reliance on Smart- Dick, S. J. (2003). Cultural evolution, the postbiological universe and SETI.
phones as a new kind of thinking, as our collective psychology International Journal of Astrobiology, 2, 65–74.
Evans, J. St. B. T. (1989). Bias in human reasoning: Causes and consequences. City:
may change alongside the technological advances. In light of our Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
data, construing Smartphone use as a form of cognitive miserliness Evans, J. St. B. T. (2009). How many dual-process theories do we need? One, two, or
is a potentially fruitful means of framing the way in which we off- many? In J. Evans & K. Frankish (Eds.), In two minds: Dual processes and beyond
(pp. 33–54). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
load cognitive effort within an ecologically valid framework. Evans, J. St. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. City: Psychology
In his short story, Harrison Bergeron, Vonnegut Jr. (1961) fore- Press.
cast a future in which personal technological devices are used to Evans, St. J. B. T., & Stanovich, K. E. (2013). Dual-process theories of higher
cognition: Advancing the debate. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 8(3),
suppress the abilities of the most able, to be more commensurate
223–241.
with the level of their less capable societal counterparts. It seems Farmer, R., & Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness: The development and
that the opposite has become reality, whereby the Smartphone correlates of a new scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 50, 4–17.
operates to provide information to those less willing and able to Ford, N., Miller, D., & Moss, N. (2005). Web search strategies and human individual
differences: Cognitive and demographic factors, Internet attitudes, and
think analytically, thus acting to diminish disadvantage. However, approaches. Journal of the American Society for Information Search and
it is also worth considering that the ability to rely on the external Technology, 56, 741–756.
480 N. Barr et al. / Computers in Human Behavior 48 (2015) 473–480

Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Seli, P., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Analytic
Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42. cognitive style predicts religious and paranormal belief. Cognition, 123,
Hong, F. Y., Chiu, S. I., & Hong, D. H. (2012). A model of the relationship between 335–346.
psychological characteristics, mobile phone addiction and use of mobile phones Sánchez-Martínez, M., & Otero, A. (2009). Factors associated with cell phone use in
by Taiwanese University female students. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, adolescents in the community of Madrid (Spain). Cyber Psychology & Behavior,
2152–2159. 12, 131–137.
Huang, M. H., & Hauser, R. M. (1998). Trends in Black–White test score differentials: Schwartz, L. M., Woloshin, S., Black, W. C., & Welch, H. G. (1997). The role of
II. The WORDSUM vocabulary test. In U. Neisser (Ed.), The rising curve: Long- numeracy in understanding the benefit of screening mammography. Annals of
term gains in IQ and related measures (pp. 303–332). Washington, DC: American Internal Medicine, 127, 966–972.
Psychological Association. Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. (1995). Decision making. In E. E. Smith & D. N. Osherson
Jacobsen, W. C., & Forste, R. (2011). The wired generation: Academic and social (Eds.). Thinking (Vol. 3, pp. 77–100). City: MIT Press.
outcomes of electronic media use among university students. Cyberpsychology, Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological
Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 275–280. Bulletin, 119, 3–22.
Junco, R., & Cotton, S. R. (2011). Perceived academic effects of instant messaging Sparrow, B., Liu, J., & Wegner, M. (2011). Google effects on memory: Cognitive
use. Computers & Education, 56, 370–378. consequences of having information at our fingerprints. Science, 333, 776–778.
Junco, R., & Cotton, S. R. (2012). No. A 4 U: The relationship between multitasking Stanovich, K. E. (2004). The Robot’s rebellion: Finding meaning in the age of Darwin.
and academic performance. Computers & Education, 59, 505–514. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded Stanovich, K. E. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The psychology of rational
rationality. American Psychologist, 58, 697–720. thought. London, UK: Yale University Press.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Strauss, Giroux. Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and
Kahneman, D., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational
and biases. Cambridge University Press. Psychology, 89, 342–357.
Lane, W., & Manner, C. (2011). The impact of personality traits on smartphone Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought.
ownership and use. International Journal of Business and Social Science, 2, 22–28. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127, 161–188.
Lepp, A., Barkley, J. E., & Karpinski, A. C. (2014). The relationship between cell phone Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning:
use, academic performance, anxiety, and Satisfaction with Life in college Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23,
students. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 343–350. 645–726.
Lepp, A., Li, J., Barkley, J. E., & Salehi-Esfahani, S. (2015). Exploring the relationships Taylor, S. E. (1981). The interface of cognitive and social psychology. In J. H. Harvey
between college students’ cell phone use, personality and leisure. Computers in (Ed.), Cognition, social behavior, and the environment (pp. 189–211). Hillsdale, NJ:
Human Behavior, 43, 210–219. Erlbaum.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Ammirati, R., & Landfield, K. (2009). Giving debiasing away: Can Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J., & Pennycook, G. (2011). Intuition, reason, and
psychological research on correcting cognitive errors promote human welfare? metacognition. Cognitive Psychology, 63, 107–140.
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 390–398. Thompson, V. A., Prowse Turner, J., Pennycook, G., Ball, L., Brack, H., Ophir, Y., et al.
Mann, S., & Robinson, A. (2009). Boredom in the lecture theatre: An investigation (2013). The role of answer fluency and perceptual fluency as metacognitive cues
into the contributors, moderators and outcomes of boredom amongst for initiating analytic thinking. Cognition, 128, 237–251.
university students. British Educational Research Journal, 35, 243–258. Toplak, M. V., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The cognitive reflection test as a
McLuhan, M. (1964). Understanding media: the extensions of man. New York: predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition,
McGraw Hill. 39, 1275–1289.
Miller, G. (2012). The smartphone psychology manifesto. Perspectives in Vonnegut, K. Jr., (1961). Harrison Bergeron. The Magazine of Fantasy & Science fiction,
Psychological Science, 7, 221–237. 5–10.
Nichols, L. A., & Nicki, R. (2004). Development of a psychometrically sound internet Yen, C., Tang, T., Yen, J., Lin, H., Huang, C., & Liu, S. (2009). Symptoms of problematic
addiction scale: A preliminary step. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 18, cellular phone use, functional impairment and its association with depression
381–384. among adolescents in Southern Taiwan. Journal of Adolescence, 32, 863–873.
Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). The role
of analytic thinking in moral judgements and values. Thinking & Reasoning, 20,
188–214.

You might also like