You are on page 1of 3

Scientific Commentaries Brain 2014: 137; 308–312 | 309

replication more effectively than cells from patients with LHON.


Collectively, these findings indicate that mitochondrial mass in- References
creases in response to LHON mutations and large increases may Barboni P, Carbonelli M, Savini G, Ramos Cdo V, Carta A, Berezovsky A,
be protective in asymptomatic carriers. Presumably, the increased et al. Natural history of Leber’s hereditary optic neuropathy: longitudinal
amount of mitochondrial compensates for the defect of respiratory analysis of the retinal nerve fiber layer by optical coherence tomography.
chain complex I in the patients. Ophthalmology 2010; 117: 623–27.
DiMauro S, Schon EA. Mitochondrial disorders in the nervous system.
Although this excellent translational research study by Giordano
Annu Rev Neurosci 2008; 31: 91–123.
et al. (2014) strongly supports the notion that mitochondrial bio- Giordano C, Iommarini L, Giordano L, Maresca A, Pisano A,
genesis is protective in LHON, proof of this concept will require Valentino ML, et al. Efficient mitochondrial biogenesis drives incom-
identification of the factor(s) responsible for activating mitochon- plete penetrance in Leber’s hereditary optic neuropathy. Brain 2014;
drial proliferation and experimental manipulation of the biogenesis 137: 335–53.
Giordano C, Montopoli M, Perli E, Orlandi M, Fantin M, Ross-
factors in cells and animal models. If validated and extended,
Cisneros FN, et al. Oestrogens ameliorate mitochondrial dysfunction
these observations will have clinical diagnostic and therapeutic im- in Leber’s hereditary optic neuropathy. Brain 2011; 134: 220–34.
plications, as noted by the authors. For example, they note that Larsson NG, Andersen O, Holme E, Oldfors A, Wahlstrom J. Leber’s
levels of mitochondrial DNA in white blood cells of LHON muta- hereditary optic neuropathy and complex I deficiency in muscle. Ann

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at Belgorod State University on February 6, 2014


tion carriers may prove to be a biomarker predictive of developing Neurol 1991; 30: 701–8.
blindness. Identification of the pathway responsible for mitochon- Leber TH. Ueber hereditäre und congenital-angelegte Sehnervenleiden.
Graefe’s Archiv Ophthalmol 1871; 17: 249–91.
drial DNA biogenesis will provide a pharmacological target to pre-
Newman NJ. Hereditary optic neuropathies: from the mitochondria to
vent blindness in unaffected mutation carriers. Studies have the optic nerve. Am J Ophthalmol 2005; 140: 517–23.
already demonstrated promising therapeutic effects of activating Viscomi C, Bottani E, Civiletto G, Cerutti R, Moggio M, Fagiolari G, et al.
mitochondrial biogenesis in mouse models of mitochondrial dis- In vivo correction of COX deficiency by activation of the AMPK/PGC-
eases with defects of the respiratory chain (Wenz et al., 2008; 1alpha axis. Cell Metab 2011; 14: 80–90.
von Graefe A. Ein ungewöhnlicher Fall von hereditäre amaurose. Arch
Viscomi et al., 2011). Remarkably, Giordano et al. have shown
Ophthalmol 1858; 4: 266–8.
that asymptomatic LHON mutation carriers may already be taking Wallace DC. A new manifestation of Leber’s disease and a new explan-
advantage of enhancing mitochondrial mass to prevent blindness. ation for the agency responsible for its unusual pattern of inheritance.
Brain 1970; 93: 121–32.
Michio Hirano, MD Wallace DC, Singh G, Lott MT, Hodge JA, Schurr TG, Lezza AMS, et al.
Columbia University Medical Centre Mitochondrial DNA mutation associated with Leber’s hereditary optic
neuropathy. Science 1988; 242: 1427–30.
New York, NY USA
Wenz T, Diaz F, Spiegelman BM, Moraes CT. Activation of the PPAR/
E-mail: mh29@cumc.columbia.edu PGC-1 alpha pathway prevents a bioenergetic deficit and effectively
improves a mitochondrial myopathy phenotype. Cell Metab 2008; 8:
doi:10.1093/brain/awu005 249–56.

Syncope: how the EEG helps in understanding


clinical findings
Although it is now widely accepted that the key to accurate et al., 2002, 2006; Wieling et al., 2009; Tannemaat et al.,
diagnosis and risk stratification of syncope is a thoughtful 2013).
and scrupulous history, exactly what is meant by the history In this issue of Brain, van Dijk et al. (2014) provide fascinating
remains unclear, and moving it from experts to front-line work- and informative insights into why some symptoms cluster with
ers has proven difficult. Partly this is because syncope is simply a each other in patients with vasovagal syncope. The authors per-
symptom, like fever, with a plethora of potential causes. Partly formed detailed EEG and videometric analyses of 69 patients with
as well this reflects the multitude of somewhat overlapping positive responses to head-up tilt table testing. The EEG provides
symptoms and signs for the most common form, the ‘faint’ or an objective marker of brain dysfunction during the cerebral
vasovagal syncope. There are several clinical features that are hypoperfusion that accompanies syncope. For nearly 60 years
known to be helpful in the differential diagnosis of loss of con- investigators have described EEG patterns during provoked reflex
sciousnesses, based on quantitative symptom studies. These for syncope (Gastaut and Fischer-Williams, 1957; Ammirati et al.,
example help distinguish epileptic convulsions and pseudosyn- 1998; Sheldon et al., 1998; Martinez-Fernandez et al., 2008).
cope from syncope, but such studies were aimed at clinical de- Two patterns have been described. A ‘slow-flat-slow’ pattern is
cision-making. They reported just the most highly significant characterized by an initial slow phase in which delta waves appear
clinical points and do not help clinicians make sense of the and wave amplitude increases, a sudden flattening of the EEG,
welter of symptoms that clinical experience suggests (Sheldon and a return to normal brain activity through a slow phase.
310 | Brain 2014: 137; 308–312 Scientific Commentaries

A ‘slow’ pattern consists only of an increasing and decreasing observed in syncope associated with profound bradyarrhythmia
slowing. The slow-flat-slow pattern was considered to be a sign or asystole during implantable loop recordings (Petkar et al.,
of more severe cerebral hypoperfusion and has been observed 2012). Therefore, in the absence of control groups, such as pa-
more commonly in cardio-inhibitory syncope compared with the tients with non-syncopal loss of consciousness and with syncope
other subtypes, and associated with the presence of convulsive from other causes, it may be a while before we are confident that
movements during syncope. However, with the exception of these clinical sign patterns are useful in the differential diagnosis of
small studies and the specific evaluation of convulsive-like move- transient loss of consciousness. Moreover, signs that can easily be
ments during syncope, few studies have investigated both EEG observed after intensive review of a video might not be easily
patterns and signs and symptoms during syncope. recalled and described by a bystander after a ‘real life’ syncope
Van Dijk et al. (2014) describe signs and haemodynamic and episode. Second, the video analysis might be biased by both the
EEG changes using videometric data in 69 cases of tilt-induced absence of blinding to EEG results and the camera position, which
vasovagal syncope. Their results confirm that EEG flattening is a did not allow the analysis of peripheral movements.
sign of more severe cerebral hypoperfusion than EEG slowing In conclusion, from a clinical standpoint, this study can help us
alone. Indeed, patients with the slow-flat-slow EEG pattern had in recognizing syncope associated with a deeper hypoperfusion,
a lower minimum blood pressure, longer ECG pauses and duration which might be more symptomatic and require more diagnostic

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at Belgorod State University on February 6, 2014


of loss of consciousness and more often had asystole than those and therapeutic effort. Moreover, the description of symptoms
patients with the slow EEG pattern. In addition, the authors ana- that have rarely been described as associated with syncope, such
lysed several clinical signs occurring during syncope, according to as oral automatisms, might eventually help with the differential
the EEG pattern of the patient and the phase in which they diagnosis of transient loss of consciousnesses with atypical clinical
occurred. As already underlined in previous studies, pallor, sweat- patterns.
ing and having the eyes open during syncope occurred in almost
every patient: the five patients with eyes closed during syncope Monica Solbiati1 and Robert Sheldon2
1
were all in the slow pattern group, suggesting that eye opening Medicina ad Indirizzo Fisiopatologico, Dipartimento di Scienze
might not occur if the brain hypoperfusion is mild. Dilated pupils, Biomediche e Cliniche ‘L. Sacco’, Ospedale ‘L. Sacco’, Università
making sounds, jerks, eye movements and oral automatisms were degli Studi di Milano, Milano, Italy
2
observed in 450% of the population. The authors propose a clas- Libin Cardiovascular Institute of Alberta, University of Calgary,
sification of clinical signs based on their relation to the EEG phase. Calgary, AB, Canada
Type A signs occur during the first slow phase, are present during
the flat phase and stop in the second slow phase. They include Correspondence to: Robert Sheldon, MD, PhD, FRCP(C),
loss of consciousness, eye opening and general stiffening with Professor of Cardiac Sciences,
Libin Cardiocascular Institute of Alberta of the University of Calgary,
possible arm raising. Type B signs occur only during EEG slowing
3280 Hospital Drive NW,
and they likely require some cortical involvement. They include
Calgary, AB, Canada, T2N4N1
myoclonic jerks, nonsensical talking, non-verbal sounds, and au- E-mail: sheldon@ucalgary.ca
tomatisms. Patients also reported auras and near-death experi-
ences. Type C signs occur only during EEG flattening and are doi:10.1093/brain/awt363
mainly roving eye movements and stertorous breathing, suggest-
ive of subcortical generation. Finally, type D signs, including jaw
dropping and snoring, are less specific and may occur in either
phase.
References
The van Dijk et al. (2014) paper provides an important contri- Ammirati F, Colivicchi F, Di Battista G, Garelli FF, Santini M.
Electroencephalographic correlates of vasovagal syncope induced by
bution to our understanding of syncope symptoms. It offers a
head-up tilt testing. Stroke 1998; 29: 2347–51.
unique opportunity to analyse reflex syncope symptoms and Gastaut H, Fischer-Williams M. Electro-encephalographic study of syn-
signs related to EEG changes, and therefore to clarify the patho- cope: its differentiation from epilepsy. Lancet 1957; 273: 1018–25.
physiology of some of the clinical events related to syncope. It Martinez-Fernandez E, Garcı́a FB, Gonzalez-Marcos JR, Peralta AG,
provides insight into why some symptoms are clustered with each Garcia AG, Deya AM. Clinical and electroencephalographic features
of carotid sinus syncope induced by internal carotid artery angioplasty.
other. Moreover, signs that have never been described as syn-
Am J Neuroradiol 2008; 29: 269–72.
cope-related could now help clinicians to distinguish syncope Petkar S, Hamid T, Iddon P, Clifford A, Rice N, Claire R, et al. Prolonged
from other causes of loss of consciousness. implantable electrocardiographic monitoring indicates a high rate of
Nevertheless, before being able to extend the study’s findings to misdiagnosis of epilepsy–REVISE study. Europace 2012; 14: 1653–60.
everyday clinical practice, we must consider that the study has a Sheldon RS, Koshman ML, Murphy WF. Electroencephalographic
findings during presyncope and syncope induced by tilt table testing.
few weaknesses. First, a very selected population in a highly moni-
Can J Cardiol 1998; 14: 811–16.
tored setting was analysed. The same findings may not be true in Sheldon R, Rose S, Connolly S, Ritchie D, Koshman ML, Frenneaux M.
either spontaneous vasovagal syncope or syncope from other Diagnostic criteria for vasovagal syncope based on a quantitative
causes. For example, the onset of partial seizures has been history. Eur Heart J 2006; 27: 344–50.
Scientific Commentaries Brain 2014: 137; 308–312 | 311

Sheldon R, Rose S, Ritchie D, Connolly SJ, Koshman MS, Lee MA, et al. van Dijk JG, Thijs RD, van Zwet E, Tannemaat MR, van Niekerk J,
Historical criteria that distinguish syncope from seizures. J Am Coll Benditt DG, et al. The semiology of tilt-induced reflex syncope in
Cardiol 2002; 40: 142–8. relation to electroencephalography changes. Brain 2014; 137: 576–85.
Tannemaat MR, van Niekerk J, Reijntjes RH, Thijs RD, Sutton R, van Wieling W, Thijs RD, van Dijk N, Wilde AAM, Benditt DG, van Dijk JG.
Dijk JG. The semiology of tilt-induced psychogenic pseudosyncope. Symptoms and signs of syncope: a review of the link between physi-
Neurology 2013; 81: 752–8. ology and clinical clues. Brain 2009; 132: 2630–42.

A (mini) pill for stroke?


In this issue of Brain, Bushra Wali et al. (2014) describe the effi- infarct volume may be a poor guide to functional improvement,
cacy of progesterone in an animal model of ischaemic stroke and, and in many animal models (Howells et al., 2010) the neurobe-
in so doing, provide data of importance for clinical translation, havioural consequences of focal cerebral ischaemia resolve rapidly,
while at the same time raising the bar for high quality reporting meaning that long-term efficacy is difficult to measure. Using a

Downloaded from http://brain.oxfordjournals.org/ at Belgorod State University on February 6, 2014


of in vivo research (Wali et al., 2014). combination of behavioural endpoints, Wali et al. (2014) demon-
The development of new treatments for stroke is a tricky busi- strate firstly that there is a sustained behavioural deficit 3 weeks
ness (O’Collins et al., 2006), and so finding an established drug after the injury, and secondly that the efficacy of progesterone
that might have therapeutic efficacy is a tantalizing prospect. was retained at these later time points. This response appeared
Abundant preclinical data support a neuroprotective effect of pro- to be independent of the delay to treatment—that is, the benefi-
gesterone in experimental stroke and traumatic brain injury, and a cial effect of treatment initiated 6 h after the induction of focal
phase III trial is under way. Given that the safety profile of pro- ischaemia was retained whether outcome was measured 3 days, 9
gesterone is already known, such reprovisioning would accelerate days or 21 days later. Conversely, the lack of benefit of treatment
drug development. A potential confounding influence is sex- and initiated 24 h after ischaemia was largely consistent across times of
age-related differences in circulating progesterone. Women who outcome measurement, although it is a simplification to say there
experience stroke are usually post-menopausal, and modelling the was no efficacy with late treatment; for locomotion (distance tra-
menopause in animals is generally achieved by ovariectomy. In a velled in the open field) and for measures of gait quality (stride
meta-analysis of the effects of progesterone in animal models of length, paw print area, limb swing speed) the data are consistent
focal cerebral ischaemia, Wong et al. (2013) found that proges- with a biologically important effect, which the experiment was not
terone increased mortality in young female hormonally-intact ani- powered to detect. As the authors point out, if efficacy is sus-
mals, and had no effect on lesion volume in adult ovariectomized tained beyond 6 h this would substantially broaden the pool of
females (Wong et al., 2013). In human studies, patients taking patients potentially eligible for progesterone treatment. This effi-
hormone replacement therapy (either oestrogen alone or oestro- cacy at longer delays to treatment suggests effects on regener-
gen plus progesterone) at the time of their stroke seemed to have ation and repair as well as neuroprotection, consistent with reports
a worse outcome (Bath and Gray, 2005); however, the effect of that progesterone promotes post-ischaemia synaptogenesis (Zhao
progesterone on its own is unknown. et al., 2011), increases circulating endothelial progenitor cells (Li
Wali et al. (2014) avoid the confounding effect of circulating et al., 2012), and promotes neurogenesis (Barha et al., 2011) and
progesterone and effects of ovariectomy by restricting attention to neural regeneration (Li et al., 2012).
aged (12-month-old) male rats. They systematically explore the The most important thing about this study, however, is the
limits to efficacy in ways that will help inform clinical trial effort made by the authors to minimize the risk that their findings
design. For delay to treatment, the authors establish that efficacy are confounded by bias (Kilkenny et al., 2010; Landis et al.,
is retained at 6-h delays but lost at 24-h delays, thus validating the 2012). By randomly allocating animals to the various experimental
use of a 6-h window in clinical trials. For drug dose, they show groups, they ensure that the only systematic differences between
maximum efficacy at the lowest dose tested [16 mg/kg on Day 1 the groups were those designed into the experiment. Of course,
(in two doses 5–6 h apart), 8 mg/kg for the next 6 days], with there will be differences between laboratory animals, but the in-
32 mg/kg (Day 1)/16 mg/kg thereafter marginally less effective, vestigators performed a power calculation to ensure that their
and 64 mg/kg (Day 1)/32 mg/kg thereafter clearly less effective. experiment was large enough to account for these chance differ-
These doses are at the higher end of those identified in the review ences and still see any treatment effects of a size considered im-
of Wong et al. (2013) who found no dose-response relationship. portant. The authors take care to report the number of animals
These dosage regimes are, however, loosely analogous to those recruited to each experimental cohort, and the number surviving
that have been used in some clinical trials in traumatic brain injury, ischaemia surgery and to the time that outcome is measured. This
where the total daily dose has ranged from 2 mg/kg to 12 mg/kg. allows the reader to judge whether improved outcome may be
The study by Wali et al. (2014) is also important in that it because of a healthy survivor effect (O’Collins et al., 2011), and
explores efficacy in aged animals with outcomes measured at ex- is particularly important where a drug has previously been re-
tended times after the stroke has occurred. One difficulty in as- ported to increase mortality. Finally, by blinding the assessment
sessing the efficacy of drugs used to treat stroke is that residual of outcome the authors give us greater confidence that the

You might also like