You are on page 1of 12

Journal ofhttp://hpq.sagepub.

com/
Health Psychology

Eating Your Words: Discursive Psychology and the Reconstruction of Eating


Practices
Sally Wiggins, Jonathan Potter and Aimee Wildsmith
J Health Psychol 2001 6: 5
DOI: 10.1177/135910530100600101

The online version of this article can be found at:


http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/6/1/5

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

Additional services and information for Journal of Health Psychology can be found at:

Email Alerts: http://hpq.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts

Subscriptions: http://hpq.sagepub.com/subscriptions

Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav

Permissions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav

Citations: http://hpq.sagepub.com/content/6/1/5.refs.html

>> Version of Record - Jan 1, 2001

What is This?

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 5

Eating Your Words: Journal of Health Psychology


Copyright © 2001 SAGE Publications
London, Thousand Oaks and New Delhi,
Discursive Psychology [1359–1053(200101)6:1]
Vol 6(1) 5–15; 015308

and the Reconstruction


of Eating Practices
Abstract
Psychological research into
eating practices has focused
mainly on attitudes and
behaviour towards food, and
disorders of eating. Using
experimental and
SALLY WIGGINS, JONATHAN POTTER, questionnaire-based designs,
& AIMEE WILDSMITH these studies place an emphasis
Loughborough University, UK on individual consumption and
cognitive appraisal, overlooking
the interactive context in which
food is eaten. The current
S A L LY W I G G I N S has a degree in psychology from the article examines eating
University of Dundee. She is currently carrying out practices in a more naturalistic
doctoral research at Loughborough University on environment, using mealtime
eating practices and food evaluations. conversations tape-recorded by
families at home. The empirical
is Professor of Discourse
J O NAT H A N P O T T E R data highlight three issues
Analysis at Loughborough University. His main concerning the discursive
research interests are in theoretical, metatheoretical construction of eating practices,
and methodological features of discursive psychology. which raise problems for the
His most recent book is Representing Reality (London: existing methodologies. These
Sage, 1996). are: (1) how the nature and
evaluation of food are
A I M E E W I L D S M I T H has a degree in social psychology negotiable qualities; (2) the use
from Loughborough University. She is currently of participants’ physiological
carrying out doctoral research on eating and eating states as rhetorical devices; and
disorders. (3) the variable construction of
norms of eating practices. The
article thus challenges some key
assumptions in the dominant
literature and indicates the
virtues of an approach to eating
AC K N OW L E D G E M E N T S . The authors would like to thank Jackie practices using interactionally
Abell, Wendy Stainton Rogers and Carla Willig for their helpful
based methodologies.
comments on an earlier draft of the article.

COMPETING INTERESTS: None declared.


Keywords
ADDRESS. Correspondence should be directed to: conversation analysis, discourse
S A L LY W I G G I N S ,
Discourse and Rhetoric Group, Department of
Social Sciences, Loughborough University, Loughborough,
analysis, discursive psychology,
Leicestershire, LE11 3TU, UK food and eating, methodology,
[Tel. 01509 228870; Fax 01509 223944; email: S.K.Wiggins@lboro.ac.uk] rhetoric
5

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 6

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 6(1)

T H E F I E L D of food and eating research has the subsequent eating behaviour of dieters and
expanded rapidly in recent decades, particularly non-dieters. This type of research suggests that
in relation to health, and embodied identities the physiological effects of different foods and
(e.g. Hill & Franklin, 1998; Lupton, 1996; the cognitive appraisal of eating behaviours are
Malson, 1998). The vibrancy and variety of such primary concerns of the research. For example,
work is due in part to the range of psychological participants may be asked to taste a food and
and sociological approaches adopted and the use then indicate on a rating scale the extent to
of established methodologies. In this article, the which they feel full, or satiated (e.g. Rolls &
focus is on the dominant psychological Hetherington, 1990). Measures such as these
approach, which concentrates on the cognitive may then be used to give an indication of an indi-
and behavioural aspects of an individual’s eating vidual’s eating habits, and his or her attitudes
behaviour (e.g. Mizes & Christiano, 1995; toward particular foods (e.g. Rogers & Blundell,
Rodin, 1990). While this perspective has greatly 1990).
influenced eating research, it will be argued that The implicit assumptions within this type of
the use of individual methodologies has pre- research can be summarized as follows:
vented an examination of the interactional
• Physiological states are accessible through
nature of eating (though see Ochs, Pontecorvo,
quantifiable, external measures
& Fasulo, 1996, for developments in this area).
• Each measurement is taken to be an accurate
By using empirical examples, the current article
representation of an internal state
aims to highlight certain phenomena that appear
• Participant responses are treated as being
to be absent in research using experimental
related to, and therefore predictors of, actual
methods, and thus illustrate potential problems
eating behaviours.
with current methodologies. It is to be argued
that these instances raise issues that are funda-
mental to research on eating practices. Attitudes and taste
To begin with, we provide a broad overview of preferences
the main topics of eating research and the
Similar assumptions are present in studies that
assumptions embedded within the methodologi-
seek to assess attitudes or preferences that
cal framework. These topics can be classed as
people may have towards foods (e.g. Clarke &
follows: consumption behaviour; attitudes and
Palmer, 1983; Ogden & Thomas, 1999). The aim
taste preferences; and the links between eating
of this type of research is often to educate people
and body image. Each type of research is typi-
into ‘healthier’ eating habits, or to determine
cally based on experimental, cognitive or clinical
why people have particular attitudes towards
methods, which place an emphasis on individual
different foods (Nash, 1990; Rogers & Blundell,
behaviour (e.g. Bolles, 1990; Rolls & Hethering-
1990). Methods used for this type of research
ton, 1990; Wardle, 1995; Williamson, Barker,
typically draw on questionnaire or rating-scale
Bertman, & Gleaves, 1995). We now consider
designs, in which food tasting may or may not be
these topics in turn.
a component. For example, Ogden and Chanana
(1998) used questionnaires to determine the
Consumption behaviour relationship between ethnicity and weight
concern, with respect to beliefs about food and
This first type of study involves participants con-
eating. The assumptions implicit within attitude
suming foods in a controlled environment and
research are therefore that:
completing rating scales concerning the food
and their perception of physiological and cogni- • Individuals possess a fixed attitude towards
tive states (e.g. Rodin, 1990; Wardle & Beales, food/eating, based on an internal, cognitive
1988). This approach is concerned with the state
means by which food cues are perceived, experi- • Use of appropriate methods will provide
enced and cognitively appraised by the indi- access to such attitudes, and to participants’
vidual (see Rodin, 1990, for a brief review). For ‘true’, underlying beliefs
example, Wardle and Beales (1988) tested the • Attitudes are the result of individual
effect of eating a ‘preload’ amount of food on appraisal, preferences and motivations.
6

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 7

WIGGINS ET AL.: PSYCHOLOGY AND EATING PRACTICES

Eating and body image contexts. So rather than looking at individual


consumption, the emphasis is on studying eating
The third main topic of eating research is based on
practices as they occur in the context of social
the individual’s perception of his or her body
interaction. This has involved the collection of
image, and the links that this may have with eating
empirical data from more ‘naturalistic’ environ-
behaviour and its disorders (e.g. Heatherton,
ments, in the form of mealtime conversations. A
Herman, Polivy, King, & McGree, 1988; Mizes
discursive, social constructionist approach is
& Christiano, 1995). For example, Hill and
adopted (e.g. Billig, 1987; Edwards, 1997;
Franklin (1998) used rating scales and body
Edwards & Potter, 1993; Potter, 1996; Potter &
image diagrams to determine the dieting beliefs
Wetherell, 1987). The aim is to examine the con-
and behaviours of daughters and their mothers,
structive nature of discourses concerned with
and the transmission of food values. Studies of
food and eating, and the ways in which these are
this kind typically categorize participants as
used to build identities in interaction. An
‘dieters’ or ‘non-dieters’, in an attempt to
examination of the data highlighted certain
examine the processes of ‘restraint’ that are
phenomena, which receive little emphasis in the
thought to pervade dieters’ lifestyles (e.g.
current literature. It is worth repeating at this
Kennett & Nisbet, 1998; Stunkard & Messick,
stage that the emphasis here is on using these
1985; Wardle & Beales, 1988).
instances simply to question dominant assump-
The measurement of eating attitudes is often
tions. They can be broken down into three
based on participants’ questionnaire responses,
themes, each of which is concerned with the con-
and the distinction made between ‘restrained’
struction of an aspect of eating:
and ‘unrestrained’ eaters is predominantly
defined as being the midpoint of the response 1. The object of eating; the food itself. How can
distribution (Herman & Polivy, 1980). Use is the nature of food be flexibly built up and
made of pictorial body image diagrams, in order transformed?
to determine participants’ perception of their 2. The participants’ physiology (e.g. state of
appearance (e.g. Monteath & McCabe, 1997). hunger). How can this be constructed and
This type of research therefore assumes that: rhetorically deployed in interaction?
3. The practice of eating, and the notion of
• Eating behaviour can be characterized as
‘restraint’. How can restraint (or lack of
‘restrained’ or ‘unrestrained’ using appropri-
restraint) be manufactured in sequences
ate measures
of interaction in ways which account for, and
• Participants’ body image is based on percep-
justify, different activities?
tual and cognitive processes, and these can be
represented pictorially
• Participant responses are representative of Method
internal states and are independent of other
Materials and participants
individuals.
Tape-recorded conversations from family meal-
The common assumptions of the three types of times were used as the data for this study. Three
eating research can be summarized as follows: families were recruited via non-academic per-
eating behaviour is treated as an individual sonal contacts to record the conversations them-
activity involving perceptual and cognitive selves using a portable tape-recorder. Recording
appraisals which directly influence eating styles; was carried out over a seven-day period for each
quantifiable measurements can be used to access family, in order for the participants to become
internal states and thus predict eating behav- acclimatized to the equipment. The full corpus is
iour; and participant responses are truthful and over 15 hours of recorded conversation.
representative of internal states. Families with adolescent daughters were
Having outlined briefly some of the assump- chosen to allow for easier access to ‘eating’ talk,
tions of existing research on eating, we will now as adolescence was deemed to be a period when
consider an alternative approach to eating prac- socialization into food and eating habits often
tices. The current article examines the inter- occurs (see Davies & Furnham, 1986; Ochs,
actional nature of eating practices in everyday Pontecorvo, & Fasulo, 1996). Additionally,

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 8

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 6(1)

female adolescents are often the focus of con- associated with food on such occasions (e.g.
cerns about eating and dietary behaviour Douglas & Nicod, 1974; Goode, Curtis, &
(Grogan & Wainwright, 1996; Hill & Franklin, Theophano, 1984; Otnes, 1991). In relation to
1998), so it seemed appropriate to consider this, our first data example is taken from near the
daughters in particular. end of a family mealtime, in which Sue, the
The tapes were all transcribed to a ‘first pass’ mother, begins to clear away the dinner plates.
level that captured the words used and some Also present are her two daughters, Chloe and
basic features of the delivery of talk. Passages of Emily, who are in their early teenage years. The
interaction that involved talk about food, and extract begins immediately following a conver-
negotiations of what to eat or not to eat, were sation about decorating the house.
transcribed more fully using the scheme devel-
oped by Gail Jefferson (see Appendix for tran- Extract 1: SKW/A1a/M1
scription notation). 1 Sue: >Come on< there was only a
↑tiny bit of (.)of
Analytic procedure
2 ↓salmon just ↑ea:t salmon
The analytic approach is derived from discursive
psychology and conversation analysis (Edwards 3 Chloe: ↑N:o its fo:ul
& Potter, 1992; Hutchby & Wooffitt, 1998;
4 (2.0)
Potter, 1997, 1998). The data corpus was examined
with a concern for the constructive and action- 5 Emily: I’ve eaten ↑mine
orientated nature of the participants’ talk; how
6 Sue: Ye:ah ↑you’ve eaten ↓yours
the participants themselves made sense of, and
orientated towards, each other’s utterances. 7 (1.0)
Notes were made during the transcription
8 Chloe: I’ve been try:ing but (mine’s
process, and during further readings of tran-
inedible)
script and listenings to the tape. Of particular
interest were points where the material seemed The first point highlighted by this fragment of
to depart from assumptions about eating in the conversation is that the family members here are
standard literature. These points were discussed not abstractly consuming foods as individuals—
between the authors and in data sessions with their mealtime is an interactive event, where
researchers working on other materials. Three there is a strong sense of involvement with each
issues were raised that highlighted potential other’s actions. Similarly, there are related
limitations with previous research. For this activities taking place within the talk, such as
article a small number of extracts were selected urging, offering, and negotiating consumption.
from the larger corpus of data, on the basis that Such activities are a part of everyday conversa-
they illustrated these limitations. Extracts from tion and interaction (Edwards, 1997) and in this
one family were chosen to simplify the exposi- instance they become bound up with the prac-
tion. tice of eating. For example, Chloe’s mother
encourages her to eat the salmon (lines 1–2),
which Chloe resists by stating that the salmon is
Analysis and discussion
‘fo:ul’ (line 3). Constructing the food in this
We will examine three issues in detail: (1) con- way—as being unpleasant—allows Chloe to
struction of the object; (2) construction of the provide an account for why she is not eating her
individual; and (3) construction of the behav- food. Eating, or not eating, is seen here as some-
iour. Each issue relates to the discursive con- thing for which Chloe is being held accountable.
struction of eating practices within interaction. So, not only are there negotiations within the
interaction, but these are also bound up with the
Issue 1: Constructing the construction of the food. In giving reasons for
object—food eating or not eating the food, its nature is simul-
Sociological literature on food and eating has taneously constructed and evaluated. One way
highlighted the importance of the structure of in which descriptions can be evaluative is to
mealtimes and the interactional meanings offer a particular representation of the object at
8

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 9

WIGGINS ET AL.: PSYCHOLOGY AND EATING PRACTICES

the expense of all others; i.e. they implicitly argue no direct negotiation about eating the food, but
against other (potential) descriptions (Billig, rather a negotiation over how the food may be
1987; Potter, 1996). Therefore, it is not only the defined. By using different expressions and
act of eating that is being negotiated, but also the emphases in their talk, the speakers are able to
nature of the food itself. How one describes the construct the food in quite different ways. This
food is related to how the food will then be simple, yet powerful, use of discourse demon-
treated, for example, whether it will be classed as strates how evaluations may be made about food
something that one should, or could, eat. through what seem to be merely observational
Negotiating about the nature of food is thus a comments. For example, by stating that: ‘There’s
continuous process, requiring the joint efforts of >↑so much< tu:na in Mum’ (line 1), Chloe not
the individuals involved. Each turn of talk can only presents a description of the food, she also
serve to give a new definition, and therefore con- displays an orientation to it in a particular way;
struction, of the food or activity. In Extract 1, for in this context ‘so much’ is hearable as ‘too
example, Emily’s utterance ‘I’ve eaten ↑mine’ much’. By looking now at the other speaker,
(line 5), contrasts with Chloe’s construction (‘its Mark, we can see how he constructs the food
fo:ul’; line 3), through its sequential position in differently, and simultaneously offers a more
the conversation. By stating that she was able to positive evaluation. In other words, the meal is
eat the food, Emily’s talk redefines it as being defined as being ‘nice’ (line 3), and as containing
edible. We can confirm this interpretation of the a lot of tuna for a good reason (i.e. it’s a tuna
interaction by looking at the next turn in the dish). Describing the food as being either ‘tuna
conversation, in which Sue repeats Emily’s with pasta’, or ‘pasta with tuna’ (lines 6 and 7)
statement as if suggesting that it supported her sets up a particular evaluative construction of
argument (line 6). This orientation demon- the food.
strates that it was treated as a reconstruction of We have seen, therefore, that food (as any
the food as edible, rather than foul; hence the other object) can be negotiated, defined and
‘problem’ lies with Chloe, and not the food. constructed in talk, and that this is an ongoing,
A further example of the construction of food jointly achieved process. In contrast, previous
can be seen in the next extract of conversation studies have tended to treat food as an object to
below. This section is taken from near the start be individually appraised, and responded to—
of a family mealtime, and involves a brief through eating it, or not eating it. However, if
exchange between the father, Mark, and his constructions of food may be variable, and pro-
daughter Chloe. It follows a lull in the conversa- duced in interaction, this raises problems with
tion, before Chloe makes a comment about the the assumptions highlighted earlier. Using an
meal itself. experimental methodology, which requires par-
ticipants to give a unitary response on a particu-
lar variable (e.g. niceness), places constraints on
Extract 2: SKW/A1a/M2
the way in which the food may be constructed.
1 Chloe: There’s >↑so much< tu:na in
What has been overlooked is the fluidity and
Mum
scope of food construction. Predefining the
2 (1.0) nature of food restricts this practice, and alters
the meaning of both the food and its
3 Mark: It’s ↑ni:ce (0.4) it’s—it’s: tuna
consumption.
pasta (0.4)
4 that’s why there’s so much tuna Issue 2: Constructing the
>in it< individual—physiology
Let us now consider what psychologists would
5 (4.0)
typically conceptualize as the physiological
6 Chloe: °It’s° <tuna with pasta (0.2) not dimensions of eating; that is, phenomena such as
pasta with hunger, satiety and taste. Our focus, again, is
with the way these things are constructed in talk.
7 tuna>
The following fragment of conversation is
This extract differs from the first in that there is taken from another mealtime, about halfway
9

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 10

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 6(1)

through the meal. Mark, the father, is clearing least, treated as adequate as a reason for her
away the dinner plates, when the conversation failing to eat all of her meal. Upon being ques-
turns to the food left on his daughter’s (Chloe’s) tioned by her father, Chloe then goes on to
plate. Also present are Emily, the other daugh- produce a more elaborate account of her inter-
ter, and their mother, Sue (who does not speak nal state using references to appetite and the
in this extract). presence of others to achieve this.
Reporting one’s physiological state can there-
Extract 3: SKW/A2a/M5 fore involve more than describing internal
1 Mark: ↑Wh:y ↓don’t you want this sensations, which is the assumption in many
Chlo↑e? psychological studies (e.g. Birch, 1990; Rogers &
Blundell, 1990). Here we have seen an instance
2 (1.2)
where some further formulation is required, and
3 Chloe: °I’m ↑fu:ll° in particular some report of evidence, in order
for the report to be treated as an acceptable
4 (2.0)
account. In this respect, physiological states are
5 Mark: ↑Why are you always full you treated as negotiable. For example, Mark (line
↓two 5) starts to treat his daughters’ fullness as a move
in an argument. Yet an internal sensation, such
6 (2.4)
as fullness or taste preference, is generally
7 Mark: I ca:n’t underst:and at↓your regarded in current research as something
age(.)I purely individual. What is being suggested here
is that in practical situations such ‘states’ can be
8 Emily: na—
open to public discussion; that is, they can be
9 Mark: used to be eat↓ing, negotiated, disputed and argued for or against.
Descriptions of one’s physiological state, then,
10 Emily: Haven’t got very big ↑appetites=
are not simply descriptions, but resources within
11 Chloe: =E—↑Emmie’s no:t (.) tha::t interaction, available to all participants. Physio-
(0.8) logical accounts can be used to answer ques-
tions, requests, or to justify behaviour; to treat
12 ↓f:ull all the time but my=
them as merely representational would be to
13 Mark: =But you keep ea:ting things in underestimate their orientation to action.
Let us illustrate this by contrasting the way a
14 betwe:en ↑meals
physiological state can be constructed in a con-
15 Chloe: Look versation with how it is defined in a consumption
questionnaire. For example, constructing ‘hun-
16 Chloe: ↓Mum (.) can you tell him my
ger’, or ‘satiety’, as unitary physiological states
appe↓tites
in experimental terms may underestimate the
17 gone variety of ways in which these sensations can be
evoked in everyday discourse (see Lupton, 1996,
18 (2.4)
p. 33). Talking about being ‘full’, in Extract 3,
19 Emily: °You’ve just said it° provided Chloe with an account or justification
for a particular course of action (i.e. not eating
20 (1.8)
all of her food). The use of questionnaires and
21 Chloe: >No but< she’s been ↓here so rating scales may obscure the flexibility around
she can the meanings of physiological accounts. Using
an approach which can deal systematically with
22 ↑pro:ve it
natural discourse is one way to reveal these flex-
As before, this extract illustrates an account of ible constructions.
food which is jointly produced. In this case, the Our next example takes another ‘physiologi-
conversation is concerned with Chloe’s physio- cal state’—taste—and shows how it is variably
logical state—how full she feels. Simply stating constructed in interaction. Extract 4, below, is
that she is full (line 3) is not, on this occasion at taken from near the end of a family mealtime,
10

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 11

WIGGINS ET AL.: PSYCHOLOGY AND EATING PRACTICES

with the same family members as in the previous physiological accounts than the representation
examples. Once again, as the plates are cleared of individual sensations. The analytic examples
away, attention turns to what has, or has not, discussed here suggest that these apparently
been eaten by the daughters. fixed states can be used both flexibly and rhetor-
ically in interaction. This sharply contrasts with
results found in experimental situations—
Extract 4: SKW/A1b/M4
perhaps because of the presence of others, but
1 Emily: I’ve ↑only left the vegeta↓bl:es
more importantly, by the way in which the
2 Mark: °Well° I want you to eat the ‘attitude’ itself is regarded. Constructing evalu-
↑vegetables: ations is an activity that participants accomplish
themselves—they are not just passively respond-
3 that’s the whole point
ing to internal, cognitive or physiological states
4 Chloe: mmmrrrr↓gghhhhhh (Potter & Wetherell, 1987).
5 (1.2)
Issue 3: Constructing the
6 Mark: (have a bit of chicken) behaviour—refusing food
The final issue is concerned with the way eating
7 Sue: ↑They were n:ice vegetable::s
practices are categorized as ‘normal’ or
8 Emily: <Com:e ↓on Chl::oe>= ‘restrained’ (e.g. Herman & Polivy, 1980). As
discussed in the introduction, this is often based
9 Mark: =They’re ↑lovely they ↓were (.)
on questionnaire responses about behaviour and
they were
attitudes towards food, and places constraints on
10 co:oked in the ↑wok (.) they the way in which the behaviour may be defined
weren’t b:oiled by the participants themselves. We now consider
an extract of conversation in which such an issue
11 or anything like ↓that
arises. This extract was taken from near the start
This section of conversation provides an illus- of a family mealtime, in which Sue (the mother)
tration of how taste can be constructed as being asks Chloe (her daughter) about her day at
an objective quality of food. Mark speaks about school.
the vegetables as being ‘n:ice’ (line 7) and
‘↑lovely’ (line 9), as if this was how they really Extract 5: SKW/A1a/M2
are, regardless of individual tastes. By depicting 1 Sue: What >did you< have for ↑lunch
them in this way, an argument is produced as to t’day?
why Emily should be eating them. The ‘point’ of
2 (2.0)
eating appears to be negotiated in terms of
quantity and quality. Emily claims that she has 3 Chloe: I had a chi:cken pi::e.
eaten most of her dinner, leaving ‘only’ the veg-
4 (3.0)
etables (therefore emphasizing the quantity
eaten). Mark then replies to this by stating that 5 Chloe: (2 syllables)=
the ‘whole point’ (line 3) of eating the meal is to
6 Mark: =An’ what
eat the vegetables, thus stressing the type of food
to be eaten (with the emphasis on the quality). 7 Chloe: A choc:olate do:ughnut
The quality of the food is further emphasized in ((smiley voice))
lines 9–11, in which the method of cooking is
8 Sue: I heard you (.) sha:med
used by Mark to account for how ‘lovely’ they
your↓self.
were (line 9). This offers the construction that
one’s sensory experience of the food is depen- 9 Mark: For ↑lunch
dent on external, rather than internal factors.
10 Chloe: ↓No but I ↑didn’t have any
What we have tried to show here are the ways
↓break I came in
in which attitudes and preferences about foods
are more complex than is suggested in much 11 and ev’rybody was .hh buying
current research, and that there is more to like (.) pizzas
11

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 12

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 6(1)

12 and a—a slice of pi:zza and a (.) a conversation is taken from the middle of a
family mealtime, in which Chloe is referring to
13 cho:colate (.) e:r sli::ce for
friends of hers at school. The discussion from
break=
which it is taken centres around how much food
14 Mark: =For break= other people eat, and how this may relate to
their body shape.
15 Chloe: =And then for lunch they
have like a (0.2) a—
Extract 6: SKW/A1a/M2
16 Mark: A:h 1 Chloe: <She ↑does (0.6) she does: (0.6)
she
17 Chloe: a↑nother piece of pizza and a—
(0.2) an’ a(.) 2 does eat a lo:t (0.2) but the:n (.) so
18 chocolate slice and °↑two 3 does Ja:ne>
lunches >in one
4 Mark: Well Jane doesn’t—
19 day<° I mean (.) even when I
5 Sue: ↑Jane doesn’t ↑d:o anything
↑am (.) li:ke
6 Chloe: No but—
20 Mark: mm
7 Sue: (Or) play spo:rt
21 Chloe: (0.2) my <pi:ggy self> (0.2)
don’t eat ↑that 8 Chloe: I know but Jane ea:ts: a lo:t
22 mu:ch In this sequence, an individual’s eating habits is
defined using a reference to, or comparison with,
As in the other extracts, what we can see here is those of another person. The notion of eating a
the production of an account—in this case it is an lot of food (as in lines 1 and 2) is given meaning
account of what was eaten by Chloe, and others, through being defined as a relative quality. As
at school that day. Through describing the food was indicated earlier, what may be seen as
in a particular way, Chloe is able to construct a restrained requires a ‘normal’ level with which
definition of what is ‘normal’ in this situation. to compare it. In this instance, what may be seen
For example, in using expressions such as ‘every- as excessive also requires a sense of what is to be
body’ (line 11) and ‘they have like’ (line 15), she expected from others. This is similarly bound up
displays such eating practices as being general, with notions of sport and activity in relation to
frequently occurring activities (see Pomerantz, eating habits. Jane is described as eating a lot,
1986, and Edwards, 2000, for how such expres- despite being inactive (lines 4–7), suggesting
sions can be used to ‘normalize’ accounts). The that this is not the ‘norm’ in this situation. The
constructive element of talk, then, offers a construction of others’ behaviour is hereby
means of defining both the behaviour of self and defined as being dependent on a comparative
of others. This has a rhetorical function in that source.
one can portray a particular version of events in The above extract also highlights the issue of
a way that justifies one’s actions (Potter, 1996). accountability in relation to eating habits. As in
In the extract above, Chloe is able to account for Extract 5, in which Chloe was held accountable
her own behaviour (eating the doughnut) for eating a doughnut, in this latter extract it is
through comparing her actions to those of Jane whose behaviour is under debate. Her
others. By producing an account of what is eating habits are being assessed in relation to her
‘normal’ she can then construct her own actions physical activity rather than simply her physio-
as being somewhat restrained in comparison, logical state. This idea of being held accountable
and thus defend her behaviour against criticism for what one eats is an important aspect of inter-
(see Edwards, 1994; Smith, 1978, on the con- action, particularly in relation to body image and
struction of normalizing accounts). the ‘thin ideal’ (Davies & Furnham, 1986;
Our final data example further illustrates the Grogan and Wainwright, 1996). In Zdrodowksi’s
rhetorical nature of talk, with respect to the (1996) paper, for example, she noted how
eating habits of others. The following section of the eating behaviour of women classed as
12

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 13

WIGGINS ET AL.: PSYCHOLOGY AND EATING PRACTICES

‘overweight’ was always accounted for in terms approach to eating research, that using data col-
of their size. If they ate a lot, they were ‘greedy’, lected from natural situations. Moreover, study-
and so it was no surprise that they were ‘fat’. ing eating as it occurs in everyday life has
Conversely, though, if they ate only a little, it illustrated how it may be redefined as an inter-
was because they were on a diet—due to their actional practice rather than an individual
size. Similarly, Wetherell (1996) found that behaviour. Our analysis is meant to show both
teenage girls talked about body image and the possibility and the potential for studying
eating in terms of accountability, and that con- eating in situ rather than via retrospective
nections were made pervasively between the accounts or experimental simulations. Second,
foods eaten and body size. Rather than being we have used these data examples to highlight
fixed, though, these accounts were variable, and some fundamental issues that are largely absent
the girls drew on different repertoires as the in the dominant psychological literature on
rhetorical context altered. This latter study also eating and eating disorders.
suggests that using a discursive framework may The three issues were based around the key
open up new avenues of research in this area. element of discursive construction in inter-
There is therefore more to the notion of action, and on how talk about eating practices is
‘restraint’, or refusing food, than is suggested by rhetorically and collaboratively formulated. We
the questionnaire designs typically used in showed some of the ways in which eating is not
current research. It can be used as a resource in simply an abstract, individual activity, but is
interaction, to account for, justify and explain folded into social interaction and daily routine.
behaviours—both one’s own, and those of Talking about food and eating involves con-
others. ‘Norms’ of eating are often constructed structing descriptions of food, body shape and
in relation to restricted eating practices, though activities, which can be used to accomplish a
these are often used retrospectively in accounts range of tasks (refusing and accepting, account-
rather than existing to predetermine the behav- ing for appetite and so on). The current method-
iour (e.g. Herman & Polivy, 1980). As an ology used in consumption research neglects this
example of constructing norms, Beach (1996) aspect of eating practices, relying instead mainly
demonstrated how an individual with bulimia on studies of isolated individuals. Our concern is
nervosa constructed her behaviour as ‘normal’ the extent to which such research makes predic-
by developing descriptions which invoked social tions about eating which extrapolate from studies
norms and everyday events in particular ways. which treat it as a decontextualized, desocial-
Malson (1998) also argued this point in her study ized, individual activity.
of the discourses of anorexia nervosa, which It might be argued that work of this kind is
highlighted the rhetorical and subjective nature focused on questions about how eating is done
of accounts of eating behaviour. Both of these as a social practice, but has little to say about the
studies have illustrated the constructive qualities more fundamental motivational concerns of tra-
of discourse, and offer an alternative methodol- ditional eating research. It is certainly the case
ogy by which to examine eating practices. that discursive psychology is avoiding the factors
In illustrating how eating behaviour can be and effects model that is typical elsewhere in
variably constructed in interaction, we have psychology (for some arguments as to why, see
highlighted some important related issues— Potter & Edwards, in press). However, the
those of accountability, justification, and the attention to people’s situated actions is attention
construction of ‘norms’ of behaviour. Not only to their issues of motive and accountability. Our
do these show the complexity of accounts, but material is threaded through with concerns
they also raise questions about the methods and about why to eat or not. Thus, in a discursive
theories used in traditional research on eating psychological approach to eating ‘motivations’,
behaviour. ‘causes’ and ‘intentions’ become topics for study
in themselves (Edwards, 1997; Potter, 1998).
This study of the ‘why’ of eating as a partici-
Conclusions
pants’ concern may have implications to psycho-
In summary, then, we have tried to achieve logical models of motivation on a theoretical
two things. First, we have introduced a new level (for example, highlighting issues to do with
13

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 14

JOURNAL OF HEALTH PSYCHOLOGY 6(1)

the way taste and hunger can be interactionally References


negotiated) and on a methodological level (for Beach, W. A. (1996). Conversations about illness:
example, highlighting some of the ways tra- Family preoccupations with bulimia. New Jersey:
ditional measures constitute their topic). Lawrence Erlbaum.
In conclusion, it is hoped that this brief intro- Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and thinking: A rhetorical
duction to a discursive approach to food and approach to social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge
eating will open up new avenues of research. We University Press.
have tried to highlight features of eating that Birch, L. L. (1990). ‘The control of food intake by
have been disregarded in traditional young children: The role of learning’ In E. D.
approaches. We hope that further studies of Capaldi & T. L. Powley (Eds.), Taste, experience and
feeding. Washington, DC: American Psychological
eating and interaction would start to map out the
Association, 116–135.
organization of food-related practices: accepting Bolles, R. C. (1990). A Functionalistic Approach to
and refusing food; complimenting and criticiz- Feeding. In E. D. Capaldi & T. L. Powley (Eds.),
ing; linking and separating food from issues of Taste, experience and feeding. Washington, DC:
health, body shape and pleasure. American Psychological Association, 3–13.
Clarke, M. G., & Palmer, R. L. (1983). Eating attitudes
and neurotic symptoms in university students.
Appendix British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 299–304.
Davies, E., & Furnham, A. (1986). The dieting and
Transcription notation body shape concerns of adolescent females. Journal
This was based on a Jefferson-style system of tran- of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27(3), 417–428.
scription (see Hutchby & Wooffitt, 1998, for a Douglas, M., & Nicod, N. (1974). Taking the biscuit:
recent summary), providing the necessary detail The structure of British meals. New Society, 33,
required to analyse the conversations: 744–747.
Edwards, D. (1994). Script formulations: An analysis
(.) A dot in a bracket indicates a pause of of event descriptions in conversation. Journal of
less than two-tenths of a second. Language and Social Psychology, 13(3), 211–247.
: Colons indicate an extension of the pre- Edwards, D. (1997). Discourse and cognition. London:
ceeding vowel sound. The more colons Sage.
there are, the greater the extent of the Edwards, D. (2000). ‘The Logic of Extreme Case
stretching. Formulations: Softeners, Investments and Irony’.
(()) Words in double brackets refer to the Research on Language and Social Interaction, 23(4),
transcriber’s comments on features of 347–373.
the talk. Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psych-
.hh A full stop before one or more ‘h’s indi- ology. London: Sage.
cates a speaker in-breath. Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1993). Language and causa-
Salmon Underlining indicates stress or emphasis tion: A discursive action model of description and
in the speech. attribution. Psychological Review, 100(1), 23–41.
(2.0) Numbers in brackets refer to pauses in Goode, J. G., Curtis, K., & Theophano, J. (1984). Meal
tenths of a second. formats, meal cycles and menu negotiation in the
(mine’s) Words in brackets indicate the tran- maintenance of an Italian-American community. In
scriber’s best estimate of unclear speech. M. Douglas (Ed.), Food and the social order: Studies
[] Square brackets indicate the beginning of food and festivities in three American communi-
and end of overlapping talk. ties. New York: Russell Sage Foundation, 143–218.
= Equal signs indicate continuous talk Grogan, S., & Wainwright, N. (1996). Growing up in
between speakers. the culture of slenderness: Girls’ experiences of
° Degree signs enclose talk which is lower body dissatisfaction. Women’s Studies International
in volume relative to the surrounding Forum, 19(6), 665–673.
talk. Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., Polivy, J., King, G. A.,
↑↓ Pointed arrows indicate a marked rising & McGree, S. T. (1988). The (mis)measurement of
or falling in speech intonation. restraint: An analysis of conceptual and psycho-
>< ‘Greater than’ and ‘less than’ signs metric issues. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,
enclose speech which is noticeably faster 97(1), 19–28.
than the surrounding talk. When the Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1980). Restrained eating.
order is reversed (<>) this indicates In A. J. Stunkard (Ed.), Obesity. Philadelphia: W. B.
slower speech. Saunders, 208–225.
14

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014


01 Wiggins (jk/d) 16/11/00 9:52 am Page 15

WIGGINS ET AL.: PSYCHOLOGY AND EATING PRACTICES

Hill, A. J., & Franklin, J. A. (1998). Mothers, daugh- Potter, J. (1998). Qualitative and discourse analysis. In
ters and dieting: Investigating the transmission of N. Schooler (Ed.), Comprehensive clinical psych-
weight control. British Journal of Clinical Psychol- ology, volume 3: Research methods. Amsterdam:
ogy, 37, 3–13. Elsevier Science, 117–144.
Hutchby, I., & Wooffitt, R. (1998). Conversation Potter, J., & Edwards, D. (in press). Discursive social
analysis: Principles, practices and applications. psychology. In P. Robinson & H. Giles (Eds.),
Cambridge: Polity Press. Handbook of language and social psychology.
Kennett, D. J., & Nisbet, C. (1998). The influence of London: Wiley.
body mass index and learned resourcefulness skills Potter, J., & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social
on body image and lifestyle practices. Patient Edu- psychology: Beyond attitudes and behaviour. London:
cation and Counselling, 33, 1–12. Sage.
Lupton, D. (1996). Food, the body and the self. Rodin, J. (1990). Behaviour: Its definition and
London: Sage. measurement in relation to dietary intake. In G. H.
Malson, H. (1998). The thin woman: Feminism, post- Anderson (Ed.), Diet and behaviour: Multi-
structuralism and the social psychology of anorexia disciplinary approaches. London: Springer-Verlag,
nervosa. London: Routledge. 57–71.
Mizes, J. S., & Christiano, B. A. (1995). Assessment of Rogers, P. J., & Blundell, J. E. (1990). Psychobiological
cognitive variables relevant to cognitive behavioural bases of food choice. In M. Ashwell (Ed.), Why we eat
perspectives on anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. what we eat: 12th annual conference of the British
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 95–105. Nutrition Foundation. London: BNF, 31–40.
Monteath, S. A., & McCabe, M. P. (1997). The influ- Rolls, B. J., & Hetherington, M. (1990). A behavioural
ence of societal factors on female body image. scientist’s perspective on the study of diet and
Journal of Social Psychology, 137(6), 708–727. behaviour. In G. H. Anderson (Ed.), Diet and
Nash, P. (1990). The influence of nutritional awareness behaviour: Multidisciplinary approaches. London:
on consumer food choice. In M. Ashwell (Ed.), Why Springer-Verlag. 209–217.
we eat what we eat: 12th annual conference of the Smith, D. (1978). K is mentally ill: The anatomy of a
British Nutrition Foundation. London: BNF, 60–78. factual account. Sociology, 12, 23–53.
Ochs, E., Pontecorvo, C., & Fasulo, A. (1996). Social- Stunkard, A. J., & Messick, S. (1985). The three-factor
ising taste. Ethnos, 61(1–2), 7–46. eating questionnaire to measure dietary restraint,
Ogden, J., & Chanana, A. (1998). Explaining the effect disinhibition and hunger. Journal of Psychosomatic
of ethnic group on weight concern: Finding a role for Research, 29(1), 71–83.
family values. International Journal of Obesity, 22, Wardle, J. (1995). The assessment of obesity: Theor-
641–647. etical background and practical advice. Behaviour
Ogden, J., & Thomas, D. (1999). The role of familial Research and Therapy, 33(1), 107–117.
values in understanding the impact of social class on Wardle, J., & Beales, S. (1988). Control and loss of
weight concern. International Journal of Eating Dis- control over eating: An experimental investigation.
orders, 25, 273–279. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97(1), 35–40.
Otnes, P. (1991), What do meals do? In E. L. Furst, Wetherell, M. (1996). Fear of fat: Interpretative reper-
R. Prattala, M. Ekstrom, L. Holm, & U. Kjaernes toires and ideological dilemmas. In J. Maybin &
(Eds.), Palatable worlds: Sociocultural food studies. N. Mercer (Eds.), Using English: From conversation
Oslo: Solum Forlag, 97–108. to canon. London: Routledge.
Pomerantz, A. (1986). Extreme case formulations: A Williamson, D. A., Barker, S. E., Bertman, L. J., &
way of legitimising claims. Human Studies, 9, 219–229. Gleaves, D. H. (1995). Body image, body dysphoria
Potter, J. (1996). Representing reality: Discourse, and dietary restraint: Factor structure in non-clinical
rhetoric and social construction. London: Sage. subjects. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1),
Potter, J. (1997). Discourse analysis as a way of 85–93.
analysing naturally occurring talk. In D. Silverman Zdrodowski, D. (1996). Eating out: The experience of
(Ed.), Qualitative research: Theory, method and eating in public for the ‘overweight’ woman. Women’s
practice. London: Sage. Studies International Forum, 19(6): 655–664.

15

Downloaded from hpq.sagepub.com at University of New England on October 29, 2014

You might also like