You are on page 1of 9

Addiction Research & Theory

ISSN: 1606-6359 (Print) 1476-7392 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/iart20

What time is it? Temporal psychology measures


relate differently to alcohol-related health
outcomes

Michael T. McKay, John L. Perry, Jon C. Cole & Frank C. Worrell

To cite this article: Michael T. McKay, John L. Perry, Jon C. Cole & Frank C. Worrell (2018) What
time is it? Temporal psychology measures relate differently to alcohol-related health outcomes,
Addiction Research & Theory, 26:1, 20-27, DOI: 10.1080/16066359.2017.1309032

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1309032

Published online: 29 Mar 2017.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 77

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 2 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=iart20

Download by: [University of New England] Date: 29 November 2017, At: 07:52
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY, 2018
VOL. 26, NO. 1, 20–27
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/16066359.2017.1309032

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

What time is it? Temporal psychology measures relate differently to


alcohol-related health outcomes
Michael T. McKaya, John L. Perryb, Jon C. Colea and Frank C. Worrellc
a
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK; bDepartment of Sport, Health, and Exercise Science, University
of Hull, Hull, UK; cGraduate School of Education, University of California, Berkeley, CA, USA

ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY


Background: Scores on temporal psychology measures continue to be associated with alcohol-related Received 4 October 2016
problems; however, few studies have examined these associations using more than one temporal Revised 15 March 2017
psychology instrument. This omission is potentially problematic as not all instruments measure the Accepted 16 March 2017
same dimensions of temporal psychology. The present study aimed to address this gap in the
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

literature. KEYWORDS
Methods: Participants (N ¼ 410) were opportunistically sampled from a University in the United Alcohol-related problems;
Kingdom and completed the following questionnaires: The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test; mental health; time
The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; The Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory; The Temporal perspective; consideration
Focus Scale; The Adolescent Time Inventory-Time Attitudes Scale; and The Consideration of Future of future consequences;
Consequences Scale-14. time attitudes
Results: Results showed weak to moderate correlations (with one exception, CFC-F and ZTPI-F)
between scores on the various temporal scales, indicating that they measure related but distinct con-
structs. Structural equation modelling and linear regression revealed that AUDIT scores were most
strongly associated with present hedonism, symptoms of anxiety were most strongly associated with
negative feelings about the present, and symptoms of depression were more moderately associated
with a broader range of indicators.
Conclusions: Cognitive and affective dimensions of temporal psychology relate differently to different
health outcomes, and researchers need to be explicit and clear in reporting findings.

Introduction focus on affect and assess an individual’s emotional and


evaluative feelings toward the past, the present, and the
Despite knowledge of the potential harms that may result,
future (Andretta et al. 2013). Temporal focus concerns cog-
young adults across a range of countries and cultures con-
nitions and describes the extent to which people characteris-
tinue to drink heavily during single sessions (Adhikari et al.
tically devote their attention to perceptions of the past,
2010; Hodgins et al. 2016). A major problem in preventing
present, and future (Bluedorn 2002). Finally, consideration
alcohol-related harm and other health compromising behav-
iours is that the negative consequences are in the future of future consequences (CFC) refers to the extent that
(e.g., increased risk of developing cancer) whereas the posi- individuals consider the potential outcomes of present
tive consequences are in the present (e.g., intoxication). behaviours, and the degree to which that consideration
On the other hand, health-promoting behaviours often influences those behaviours (Strathman et al. 1994).
require opportunity costs in the present (e.g., exercise) for A number of studies have reported a significant and
positive future benefits that may not be realised (e.g., a lon- meaningful relationship between a lower future orientation
ger life). It therefore makes sense that temporal constructs (and higher present orientation) and more problematic alco-
would be meaningfully related to health-related behaviours. hol use. Both time perspective more broadly, and the other,
Time perspective is conceptualised in the Zimbardo Time more specific constructs (e.g., time attitudes, temporal focus
Perspective Inventory (ZTPI; Zimbardo & Boyd 1999), as an and CFC) have been found to relate meaningfully to alcohol
individual difference variable which examines how thoughts use so that, broadly speaking, a foreshortened orientation
about and feelings towards the past, present and future influ- toward the future and a higher present orientation are sig-
ence human behaviour. Others (e.g., Shipp et al. 2009) have nificantly associated with increased alcohol use and/or
described time perspective as an umbrella term for a range greater alcohol-related problems. This relationship has been
of narrower temporal constructs including time attitudes, found to be true in studies of time perspective more broadly
temporal focus, and consideration of future consequences. using the ZTPI (e.g., Klingeman 2001; Wills et al. 2001;
Although time perspective is a broad area, time attitudes Apostolidis et al. 2006; Daugherty & Brase 2010;

CONTACT Michael. T. McKay Michael.McKay@Liverpool.ac.uk Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Liverpool, Eleanor Rathbone Building,
Bedford Street South, Liverpool, L69 7ZA
ß 2017 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY 21

Barnett et al. 2013; Cole et al. 2016); consideration of future Measures


consequences (Beenstock et al. 2011; McKay et al. 2013;
The 56-item Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (ZTPI;
McKay et al. 2015); temporal focus (McKay et al. 2012);
Zimbardo & Boyd 1999) assesses individual differences on
time attitudes (McKay et al. 2016b; Wells et al. 2016) and
future orientation (Robbins & Bryan 2004). In addition, five subscales: Past-Negative (PN) and Past-Positive (PP),
others have shown that a past negative time perspective is measures a negative and positive view of the past, respect-
associated with greater alcohol-related problems (Linden ively (e.g., 'Painful past experiences keep being replayed in my
et al. 2014; Chavarria et al. 2015). mind', or, 'It gives me pleasure to think about my past');
Additionally, given the high co-morbidity between alco- Present-Hedonistic (PH), measures a risk-taking approach to
hol-related problems and mental ill health, a number of life oriented toward present pleasure (e.g., 'Ideally, I would
researchers in a range of populations have reported mean- live each day as if it were my last'); Present-Fatalistic (PF),
ingful relationships between time perspective (assessed using assesses a pessimistic view of life (e.g., 'It doesn’t make sense
the ZTPI) and measures of mental ill-health. Indeed, this lit- to worry about the future since there is nothing I can do
erature is dominated by studies using the ZTPI, with rela- about it anyway'); and Future (F), assesses preparation for
tively few studies examining the relationship between the future and orientation to future outcomes ('Meeting
symptoms of psychopathology and other temporal psych- tomorrow’s deadlines and doing other necessary work comes
ology measures. Accordingly, significant relationships have before tonight’s play). All items were answered on a 5-point
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

been reported between time perspective and suicidal ideation Likert-type scale (1 ¼ Very Unlike Me, 5 ¼ Very Like Me).
(Laghi et al. 2009; Van Beek et al. 2011) and psychopath- The Consideration of Future Consequences Scale-14
ology (e.g., Zimbardo & Boyd 1999; Van Beek et al. 2011; (Joireman et al. 2012) is made up of seven positively worded
Anagnostopoulos & Griva 2012; Papastamatelou et al. 2015; items (CFC-Future; e.g., 'I consider how things might be in
McKay et al. 2016a). In contrast, relatively little is known the future, and try to influence those things with my day to
about the relationship of time attitudes temporal focus, and day behaviour'), and seven negatively worded items (CFC-
CFC to psychopathology. In one recent study, McKay et al. Immediate; e.g., ''I only act to satisfy immediate concerns, fig-
(2017) reported that those with a positive time attitudes pro- uring the future will take care of itself'). Essentially, CFC-
file scored meaningfully lower on symptoms of depression Immediate can be understood as an assessment of present
than peers, while elsewhere, consideration of immediate con- orientation, with CFC-Future an assessment of consequences
sequences, but not consideration of future consequences was in the more distant future. Responses were on a 7-point
meaningfully associated with symptoms of anxiety (McKay Likert-type scale from 1 (very unlike me) to 7 (very like me).
et al. 2016c). We are not aware of any research between tem- In their development of the scale, Joireman et al. (2012)
poral focus as conceptualised by Shipp et al. (2009), and reported that scores on the two factors were highly reliable:
symptoms of psychopathology. CFC-Future (a ¼ .82) and CFC-Immediate (a ¼ .82).
Within the temporal psychology literature, few studies The Adolescent Time Inventory Time Attitudes Scale
have included more than one measure of these temporal (TAS; Mello & Worrell 2007) is a 30-item instrument with
dimensions. Accordingly, the relationship among temporal six 5-item subscales assessing Past Negative attitudes (PaN),
measures is somewhat unclear as is the understanding of Past Positive attitudes (PaP), Present Negative attitudes
whether or not these constructs relate similarly to health (PrN), Present Positive attitudes (PrP), Future Negative atti-
behaviours, or whether there are differential relations along tudes (FN), and Future Positive (FP) attitudes. TAS scores
the lines of cognition and affect. The present study is there- are scored on a 5-point Likert scale with verbal and numer-
fore potentially important in helping to understand the ical anchors (1 ¼ Totally Disagree, 5 ¼ Totally Agree). As pre-
subtleties of research in this area, and the degree to which viously noted, in adolescents and adults, TAS scores have
all or only some dimensions of temporal psychology relate been shown to be internally consistent and structurally valid
to alcohol-related problems. (Alansari et al. 2013; Worrell et al. 2013; Mello et al. 2016),
and there has also been evidence of convergent and discrim-
inant validity (Worrell & Mello 2009).
Method The Temporal Focus Scale (Shipp et al. 2009) is a 12-item
scale assessing cognitive engagement with the past, present
Participants
and future. The scale consists of four Past (e.g., 'I think
Participants were 410 adults aged 18–75 (M ¼ 24.83, about things from my past'); Current (e.g., 'My mind is on
SD ¼ 10.52; 45.4% male) recruited as part of an undergradu- the here and now'); and Future ('I think about times to
ate project by six Masters Degree students in a University in come') items. Cronbach’s alphas for TFS scores ranged from
the North West of England, through opportunistic and 0.74 to 0.89 (Shipp et al. 2009). Convergent validity evidence
snowball sampling. Participants were 98% Caucasian, and no for the three TFS subscale scores was demonstrated through
compensation was offered for participation in the study. A correlations with other pre-existing measures of time per-
battery of questionnaires was completed in paper-and-pencil spective, including the ZTPI (Shipp et al. 2009).
format under the supervision of at least one of the students. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS;
The study was given ethical approval by the relevant univer- Zigmond & Snaith 1983) was used to screen participants for
sity ethics committee and all participants gave informed depression (e.g., 'I still enjoy the things I used to enjoy') and
consent. anxiety (e.g., 'I get sudden feelings of panic'). The HADS
22 M. T. MCKAY ET AL.

scores for anxiety (HADS-A) and depression (HADS-D) Table 1. Descriptive statistics for major variables.
range from zero to 28. There is evidence of validity and Variable M SD Min. Max. Skew Kurtosis a x (95% CI)
internal consistency (HADS-A: a ¼ .83; HADS-D: a ¼ .82) Outcomes
AUDIT 10.96 6.70 0.00 31.00 .51 .30 .83 .84 (.81, .86)
for HADS scores, and equivalent levels of sensitivity (.80) Anxiety 6.66 4.09 0.00 17.00 .40 .56 .82 .83 (.80, .85)
and specificity (.80; for a review, see Bjelland et al. 2002). Depression 3.93 3.20 0.00 14.00 .78 .07 .70 .71 (.65, .75)
The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT; ZTPI
Saunders et al. 1993) was used to assess problematic alcohol Past negative 3.24 .73 1.44 4.89 .21 .63 .82 .83 (.80 (.86)
Past positive 3.00 .62 1.20 4.80 .04 .01 .70 .70 (.64, .75)
use in the sample. AUDIT is a 10-item questionnaire with Present hedonistic 3.32 .48 1.94 4.67 .16 .11 .79 .79 (.76, .82)
reliable scores that yield valid inferences across different Present fatalistic 2.58 .62 1.10 4.44 .22 .08 .72 .75 (.71, .79)
contexts and cultures (e.g., de Meneses-Gaya et al. 2009). Future 3.27 .50 1.85 4.77 .10 .03 .73 .73 (.69, .77)
When used to detect problematic alcohol use in a population CFCS
CFC-I 3.70 .96 1.29 6.00 .03 .63 .82 .83 (.81, .85)
of university undergraduates, AUDIT demonstrated good CFC-F 4.59 .88 1.57 7.00 .14 .14 .82 .83 (.79, .86)
sensitivity (.94) and specificity (.92; Adewuya 2005). AUDIT TAS
scores can range from 0 to 40. Past positive 3.69 .74 1.00 5.00 .63 1.07 .88 .88 (.85, .91)
Past negative 2.20 .90 1.00 5.00 .66 .11 .90 .89 (.86, .91)
Present positive 3.84 .69 1.00 5.00 .76 1.00 .89 .89 (.86, .91)
Data analysis Present negative 2.33 .78 1.00 5.00 .41 .24 .85 .84 (.82, .87)
Future positive 3.92 .66 1.80 5.00 .56 .44 .89 .88 (.86, .91)
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

Future negative 2.10 1.14 1.00 4.00 .47 .26 .78 .78 (.73, .81)
To determine measurement error, we first examined the fac-
TFS
tor structure of each measurement scale using confirmatory
Past 3.85 1.14 1.50 7.00 .98 .34 .83 .83 (.79, .87)
factor analysis, independent cluster models (CFA-ICM). Hu Current 4.77 .99 2.00 7.00 .06 .30 .65 .65 (.57, .70)
and Bentler’s (1999) guidance for adequacy of model fit was Future 4.48 1.18 1.00 7.00 .29 .49 .79 .80 (.76, .83)
broadly employed. Specifically, incremental fit indices (CFI ZTPI: Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory; CFCS: Consideration of Future
and TLI) close to .90 and absolute fit indices (SRMR and Consequences Scale; CFC-I: Consideration of Future Consequences Scale -
Immediate; CFC-F: Consideration of Future Consequences Scale - Future; TAS:
RMSEA) close to .05 were considered to represent adequate Time Attitudes Scale; TFS: Temporal Focus Scale.
model fit. However, these were not employed as golden rules
(Marsh et al. 2004; Perry et al. 2015).
All analyses were conducted in Mplus 7 (Muthen & consistency (see Table 1) with the exception of TFS-current
Muthen 2012), employing the robust maximum likelihood (a and x < .65). Descriptive statistics are presented in Table 1
estimator (MLR) to guard against departure from multivari- and intercorrelations are reported in Table 2.
ate normality. To determine the relative capacity of each Before conducting the main analyses, we examined effects
temporal measure to predict AUDIT, HADS-A, and HADS- for age and sex to determine if it was necessary to control
D scores, we tested three multiple linear regression models, for these. For age, bootstrapped bivariate correlations indi-
inputting the outcome as the dependent variable and all tem- cated a significant negative relationship with AUDIT score
poral measures as independent variables. The temporal (r ¼ .29, p < .001, 95% CI ¼ .36, .18) and HADS-A
measures were entered in the second step, after age and sex (r ¼ .15, p < .01, 95% CI ¼ .24, .07). There was no asso-
were entered in Block 1 to account for their variance. ciation between age and HADS-D (r ¼ .07, p ¼ .17, 95%
Finally, we examined the extent to which each scale was able CI ¼ .02, .16). Also, independent samples t-tests confirmed
to account for variance in AUDIT score, HADS-A and two sex differences. Specifically, males presented higher
HADS-D scores over and above other scales. We applied AUDIT scores, t(408) ¼ 5.05, p < .001, 95% CI ¼ 1.90, 4.50,
Ferguson’s (2009) guidelines to interpret correlation coeffi- and females self-reported higher HADS-A scores,
cients. Accordingly, the recommended minimum practical t(408) ¼ 2.15, p < .05, 95% CI ¼ 1.62, .05. There was no
effect (RMPE) is a coefficient of .20, a coefficient of .50 sex difference in HADS-D scores, t(408) ¼ .35, p ¼ .73,
represents a moderate effect size, and a coefficient of  .80 95% CI ¼ .73, .56. Consequently, we controlled for age and
represents a strong effect. These effect sizes were also used sex in remaining analyses predicting AUDIT score and
to interpret beta coefficients in the regression models. HADS-A.

Results Confirmatory factor analyses


Descriptive statistics To estimate measurement error, we conducted CFA-ICM on
Data were first screened for outliers, missing data, and uni- each scale. The ZTPI presented a poor model fit:
variate normality. There were no outliers or missing data, and v2(1474) ¼ 2891.69, p < .001, CFI ¼ .710, TLI ¼ .697,
no problems were observed regarding univariate skewness SRMR ¼ .075, RMSEA ¼ .048 (.046, .051). Issues with the
(<2) or kurtosis (<2; see Table 1). To estimate internal con- factorial validity have been highlighted previously (e.g., see
sistency, we calculated both alpha and also omega point esti- Worrell et al. 2016). The CFCS-14 CFA-ICM indicated a
mates and confidence intervals, as recommended by Dunn reasonable model fit: v2(76) ¼ 190.09, p < .001, CFI ¼ .924,
et al. (2013), using the MBESS package (Kelley & Lai 2012) in TLI ¼ .909, SRMR ¼ .051, RMSEA ¼ .061 (.050, .071), with
R (R Development Core Team 2012) with 1,000 bootstrap some error indicated by the absolute fit indices. All items
samples. All variables demonstrated acceptable internal significantly loaded onto their latent variable. Model fit for
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY 23

Table 2. Inter-correlations among scores on temporal measures.


1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
1 ZTPI-PN – .29 .09 .24 .05 .12 .01 .21 .34 .25 .31 .17 .25 .27 .17 .10
2 ZTPI-PP – .23 .18 .18 .13 .18 .41 .16 .02 .10 .04 .01 .21 .06 .13
3 ZTPI-PH – .35 .29 .40 .14 .18 .07 .08 .03 .18 .03 .08 .24 .02
4 ZTPI-PF – .30 .49 .32 .04 .21 .23 .28 .26 .45 .10 .05 .26
5 ZTPI-F  .39 .51** .16 .10 .13 .07 .15 .25 .10 .02 .25
6 CFCS-I  .36 .06 .14 .16 .17 .20 .34 .10 .12 .21
7 CFCS-F – .07 .02 .08 .01 .30 .29 .14 .01 .42
8 TAS-PaP – .73 .38 .27 .30 .27 .12 .22 .18
9 TAS-PaN – .46 .52 .33 .46 .08 .29 .14
10 TAS-PrP – .80 .58 .53 .14 .31 .12
11 TAS-PrN – .43 .54 .17 .29 .05
12 TAS-FP – .69 .04 .26 .45
13 TAS-FN – .07 .30 .40
14 TFS-P – .01 .27
15 TFS-C – .31
16 TFS-F –
ZTPI: Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory (PN: Past Negative; PP: Past Positive; PH: Present Hedonistic; PF: Present Fatalistic; F: Future); CFCS: Consideration of
Future Consequences Scale (I: Immediate; F: Future); TAS: Time Attitudes Scale (PaN: Past Negative; PaP: Past Positive; PrN: Present Negative; PrP: Present Positive;
FN: Future Negative; FP: Future Positive); TFS: Temporal Focus Scale (P: Past; C: Current; F: Future).
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

Bolded coefficient is that reaching Ferguson’s (2009) threshold for moderate effect size.

the TAS was good: v2(390) ¼ 709.16, p < .001, CFI ¼ .939, Table 3. Linear regression analyses using backward elimination showing
retained variables only.
TLI ¼ .932, SRMR ¼ .040, RMSEA ¼ .045 (.039, .050) with all
Variable Scale B (95% CI) SE B t R2
items presenting a significant and substantive loading onto
Dependent variable: AUDIT score .26
their respective latent factor. Finally, we examined the factor Past negative ZTPI 1.90 (2.85, 0.95) .48 .21 3.93
structure of the TFS, which indicated some measurement Present hedonistic ZTPI 5.91 (4.50, 7.32) .72 .42 8.24
error in model fit: v2(51) ¼ 170.71, p < .001, CFI ¼ .892, Present fatalist ZTPI 2.51 (3.60, 1.42) .55 .23 4.53
Future ZTPI 1.48 (2.89, 0.07) .72 .11 2.06
TLI ¼ .860, SRMR ¼ .074, RMSEA ¼ .076 (.063, .088). CFC-F CFCS-14 0.83 (1.63, 0.02) .41 .11 2.01
However, all items presented a significant and substantive Past positive TAS 1.76 (0.84, 2.67) .47 .20 3.77
loading on their hypothesised factor. Future positive TAS 1.50 (2.56, 0.54) .51 .15 3.03
Past TFS 0.70 (0.18, 1.22) .26 .12 2.67
Dependent variable: Anxiety .25
Multiple linear regression Present hedonistic ZTPI .93 (1.33, 0.12) .41 .11 2.25
CFC-I CFCS-14 .42 (0.03, 0.85) .22 .10 1.95
CFC-F CFCS-14 .56 (0.14, 0.99) .22 .12 2.59
To determine which of the temporal variables were the Past positive TAS .95 (1.45, 0.44) .26 .17 3.69
strongest predictors of AUDIT HADS-A and HADS-D Present negative TAS 1.66 (1.17, 2.14) .24 .32 6.85
scores we tested three separate multiple linear regression Past TFS .70 (0.38, 1.02) .16 .20 4.32
models using backward elimination. This procedure required Dependent variable: Depression .26
Past negative ZTPI .60 (1.09, 0.10) .25 14 2.37
entering all 16 factors from the ZTPI, CFCS-14, TAS, and Past positive ZTPI .48 (1.09, 0.05) .27 .09 1.78
TFS before iteratively removing the weakest explaining fac- Present hedonistic ZTPI .83 (1.53, 0.14) .35 .12 2.36
tors until a model is produced containing only predictor Future ZTPI .62 (1.31, 0.07) .35 .10 1.76
CFC-I CFCS-14 .41 (0.06, 0.77) .18 .12 2.30
factors with a statistically significant beta value. We acknow- CFC-F CFCS-14 .53 (0.15, 0.92) .20 .15 2.71
ledge that stepwise, autostop regression models can be prob- Past positive TAS .40 (0.86, 0.06) .24 .09 1.72
lematic in describing the proportion of model fit because it Present positive TAS .73 (1.44, 0.02) .36 .16 2.02
Present negative TAS .63 (0.00, 1.25) .32 .15 1.96
uses unconstrained least squares in its estimation. This Future negative TAS .67 (0.03, 1.10) .27 .12 2.09
means that the R2 value should be treated with caution. For Past TFS .38 (0.11, 0.64) .13 .13 2.81
the purpose of our analysis, however, we were interested in ZTPI: Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory; CFCS-14: Consideration of Future
the extent to which temporal variables predicted variance Consequences Scale-14; TAS: Time Attitudes Scale; TFS: Temporal Focus Scale.
p < .05;
relative to each other. For this, backward elimination is p < .01;
appropriate. p < .001;
The results of these analyses are presented in Table 3. Practically significant results italicised.
The first analysis examined AUDIT score as the dependent
variable. Model 1 explained 26.5% of overall variance in were removed in the iterative process until Model 11,
AUDIT scores, F(16, 393) ¼ 8.88, p < .001. The iterative which explained 25.4% of the total variance in anxiety,
backward elimination removed variables explaining a negli- F(6, 403) ¼ 22.84, p < .001. Of the retained variables, the
gible amount of variance until Model 9, which explained PrN scale from the TAS was the largest predictor of vari-
25.9% of total AUDIT variance, F(8, 401) ¼ 17.50, p < .001). ance. The final regression analyses had HADS-D score as the
Of the eight retained variables, the most meaningful pre- dependent variable. Model 1 explained 26.4% of variance,
dictor of AUDIT score was the PH scale from the ZTPI. F(16, 393) ¼ 8.83, p < .001. Five variables were iteratively
The second regression analysis entered HADS-A as eliminated up to Model 6, which explained 25.8% of the
the dependent variable. Model 1 explained 26.5% of the overall variance in HADS-D score, F(11, 398) ¼ 12.57,
overall variance, F(16, 393) ¼ 8.87, p < .001. Ten variables p < .001. In this equation, variance was more evenly
24 M. T. MCKAY ET AL.

Table 4. Hierarchical regression analyses with AUDIT score as the dependent variable.
Scale/variable B (95% CI) SE b t VIF Tolerance DR2
Model 1a
Block 1 .11
Block 2 ZTPI .14
Past negative .20 (1.05, 1.44) .63 .02 .31 2.26 .44
Past positive 1.67 (3.16, .18) .76 .14 2.21 2.11 .47
Present hedonistic 5.61 (4.11, 7.11) .76 .40 7.37 1.54 .65
Present fatalist 2.28 (3.53, 1.04) .63 .21 3.60 1.71 .59
Future .78 (2.27, .71) .76 .06 1.04 1.68 .60
Model 1b
Block 1 .24
CFCS .01
CFC-I .32 (.47, 1.10) .40 .05 .80 1.69 .59
CFC-F 1.06 (1.96, .15) .46 .14 2.30 1.84 .54
Model 1c
Block 1 .21
Block 2 TAS .04
Past negative .19 (1.15, 1.53) .68 .03 .28 4.37 .23
Past positive 2.27 (.89, 3.64) .70 .27 3.25 3.67 .27
Present negative .37 (1.83, 1.08) .74 .04 .51 3.83 .26
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

Present positive .90 (2.58, .78) .86 .09 1.06 4.01 .25
Future negative .52 (1.95, .92) .73 .05 .71 2.89 .35
Future positive 1.31 (2.78, .15) .74 .13 1.76 2.82 .36
Model 1d
Block 1 .24
Block 2 TFS .01
Past .46 (.15, 1.07) .31 .08 1.49 1.44 .69
Current .04 (.66, .74) .36 .01 .11 1.42 .71
Future .03 (.63, .70) .34 .01 .10 1.83 .55
All other temporal scales other than one entered in Block 2 entered in Block 1.
p < .05;
p < .01;
p < .001;
Practically significant results italicised.

distributed among retained variables than in other regression Table 5. Hierarchical regression analyses with HADS-A as the dependent
variable.
analyses.
To determine the relative contributions of each scale in Scale/variable B (95% CI) SE b t DR2
predicting AUDIT, HADS-A and HADS-D scores, we con- Model 2a
Block 1 .25
ducted a further four linear regression models, entering a ZTPI .03
different temporal measure last in each one. In Model 1a of Past negative 1.36 (.62, 2.11) .38 .23 3.59
this analysis, CFCS, TAS, and TFS variables were entered in Past positive .29 (1.18, .61) .46 .04 .63
Present hedonistic .53 (1.43, .37) .46 .06 1.16
Block 1 using the enter method with AUDIT score as the Present fatalist .07 (.81, .68) .38 .01 .17
dependent variable. ZTPI variables were then entered in Future .58 (.31, 1.48) .46 .07 1.28
Block 2. The change in R2 was then examined to determine Model 2b
Block 1 .28
the variance explained by the final scale over and above that CFCS .01
already accounted for by the other scales. For Model 1 b of CFC-I .36 (.11, .83) .24 .08 1.50
this analysis, CFCS variables were entered in Block 2, for CFC-F .40 (.15, .94) .28 .08 1.43
Model 2c
Model 1c, TAS variables were entered in Block 2, and for Block 1 .24
Model 1d, TFS variables were entered last. This process was TAS .04
repeated with HADS-A scores (Models 2a, 2 b, 2c, and 2d) Past negative .24 (1.05, .57) .41 .05 .59
Past positive .46 (1.28, .37) .42 .9 1.09
and HADS-D scores (Models 3a, 3 b, 3c, and 3d) as the Present positive .83 (.04, 1.71) .45 .16 1.87
dependent variables. Present negative .38 (1.39, .64) .51 .06 .73
The results demonstrated that no one single measurement Future positive .21 (.65, 1.07) .44 .04 .48
Future negative .19 (1.07, .69) .45 .03 .43
scale was substantively better at predicting variance than the Model 2d
combined rest. The exception to this was for AUDIT score Block 1 .27
(Table 4), where the largest DR2 was observed when the TFS .02
Past .37 (.01, .74) .19 .10 1.99
ZTPI variables were inserted after the other scales. Most Current .22 (.64, .20) .21 .05 1.04
importantly, PH was a strong positive predictor. For HADS- Future .27 (.13, .67) .20 .08 1.31
A (Table 5) and HADS-D (Table 6), the TAS variables made All other temporal scales other than one entered in Block 2 entered in Block 1.
p < .05;
the largest contribution to total R2, though these were rela- p < .001;
tively small to moderate. Practically significant results italicised.
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY 25

Table 6. Hierarchical regression analyses with HADS-D as the dependent the TAS, and negatively related to PF on the ZTPI. These
variable.
findings provide convergent validity evidence in support of
Scale/Variable B (95% CI) SE b T DR2 the two CFCS-14 factors, making them directly relevant to
Model 3a an increasing number of studies using the CFCS-14 (for a
Block 1 .23
ZTPI .04 review, see Joireman & King 2016). The medium-sized cor-
Past negative .29 (.29, .89) .30 .06 .98 relations that CFC-F manifests with TAS-FP (positive) and
Past positive 1.36 (2.07, .66) .36 .24 3.79 TAS-FN (negative) suggest that CFC-F is assessing a hopeful
Present hedonistic .62 (1.33, .09) .36 .09 1.72
Present fatalist .07 (.66, .52) .30 .01 .23 and optimistic future, rather than just a basic future
Future .31 (1.02, .39) .36 .05 .87 orientation.
Model 3b Patterns of relationships provided some support for TAS
Block 1 .25 scores with the CFCS and TFS scores as reported below, but
CFCS .01
CFC-I .62 (.06, .69) .19 .10 1.67 only with two ZTPI scores. PN (ZTPI) and PaN (TAS) were
CFC-F .43 (.00, .86) .22 .12 1.96 meaningfully related as were PP (ZTPI) and PaP (TAS). PF
Model 3c (ZTPI) was more strongly correlated with the Future sub-
Block 1 .20 scales on the TAS than the Present subscales, providing
TAS .06
Past negative .39 (1.03, .25) .32 .11 1.20 more evidence that the ZTPI’s present fatalism construct is
Past positive .18 (.83, .48) .33 .04 .53 also tapping into future time perspective. The ZTPI’s F scale
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

Present positive .35 (.34, 1.04) .35 .09 1.00 was not meaningfully related to either FP or FN, and PH
Present negative .63 (1.43, .17) .41 .14 1.56
Future positive .57 (.11, 1.25) .35 .12 1.65 was not related to either PrP or PrN.
Future negative .31 (1.01, .38) .35 .06 .88 The pattern of correlations between TFS and ZTPI sub-
Model 3d scales was supportive of their theoretical relationships, with
Block 1 .25 TFS-Past having its largest correlations with PP and PN,
TFS .01
Past .26 (.03, .55) .15 .09 1.77 TFS-Present having its largest correlation with PH, and TFS-
Current .03 (.37, .30) .17 .01 .19 Future having its largest correlations with F and PF. The
Future .24 (.08, .55) .16 .09 1.47 correlation between TFS-Future and PF, that is present fatal-
All other temporal scales other than one entered in Block 2 entered in Block 1. ism, is particularly interesting, as fatalism involves anticipat-
p < .001; Practically significant results italicised.
ing negative events in the future. Nonetheless, all of these
correlations fell short of .30. TFS-Future was meaningfully
related to CFC-F, but TFS-Current was not related to CFC-I.
The TFS-Current and Future scores were related to TAS pre-
Discussion
sent and future scores, respectively, in keeping with hypothe-
The present study used a relatively large sample from a ses and theory (e.g., valences changed with positive and
University setting to examine the relationship between scores negative TAS subscales). However, TFS-Current scores were
on four temporal constructs and scores on alcohol-related also related to TAS-Future scores, and TFS and TAS scores
problems and symptoms of both anxiety and depression. In on the past were not meaningfully associated.
order to achieve these goals, the first aim of the study was to It could be argued that the most noteworthy finding from
examine the psychometric integrity of scores on all of the the correlation matrix is the relative lack of meaningful rela-
measures. Results indicated that, with the exception of the tionships among the temporal measures, supporting their
TFS-current factor, the reliability of scores on all measures conceptual and theoretical independence (perhaps with the
exception of the relationship between CFCS-F and ZTPI-F).
were acceptable. One issue of concern was the lower than
Indeed, even meaningful correlations indicated at most 24%
optimal nature of the fit indices for the ZTPI.
shared variance across temporal constructs, supporting our
contention that time perspective (ZTPI), attitudes toward
Intercorrelations among temporal constructs time periods (TAS), consideration of future consequences
(CFCS), and cognitive engagement with time periods (TFS)
Applying Ferguson’s (2009) criteria, there was only one are related but distinct constructs.
moderate-sized inter-scale correlation between temporal
dimensions, namely that between ZTPI-future, and CFC-
future. This finding speaks to the relative independence of Associations with symptoms of psychopathology
the four temporal constructs used in this study, which is We applied the criteria of Ferguson (2009) to interpret these
important in a literature that is becoming increasingly frac- associations. Accordingly a recommended minimum effect
tured by modified and shortened forms of scales and size for b (or a practically significant effect size) is  .2, a
increasing numbers of measurement options. Results of the moderate effect size  .5, and a strong effect  .8. Based on
correlation matrix as a whole are intuitive and in line with the bs in Table 3 and the relatively low level of variance
previous research. explained, we concluded that the relationship between the
In this study, the CFC-I factor was meaningfully and temporal variables and the three outcome measures is not
positively related to both of the ZTPI’s present subscales, PH particularly strong. There are no temporal dimensions with
and PF, and negatively to the F subscale; the CFC-F factor moderate or higher effect sizes in the depressive symptom-
was positively related to F on the ZTPI and TFS and FP on atology model, and only two in the anxiety model.
26 M. T. MCKAY ET AL.

Accordingly, higher levels of anxiety symptomatology are measure of the outcome variables of interest. Limitations
meaningfully associated with a greater past temporal focus notwithstanding, this study makes several contributions.
and a higher present negative attitude. First, the results indicate that scores on the ZTPI, CFCS-14,
We are not aware of any other studies having examined TFS, and TAS are internally consistent. Second, the study
the relationship between depression or anxiety symptomatol- establishes the related and unique aspects of four temporal
ogy and TFS scores; however, Shipp et al. (2009) reported a constructs that are used in the literature, with the pattern of
meaningful (r ¼ .63) correlation between ZTPI past nega- correlations indicating overlap where theory would suggest
tive and TFS past, but a smaller sized (r ¼ .27) correlation (e.g., in the same time period).
between ZTPI past positive and TFS past, suggesting that the Finally, the findings suggest that some temporal con-
TFS past dimension is tapping a negative appraisal of the structs are related in statistically and practically significant
past. The correlations in the present study also suggest a ways to symptoms of anxiety and alcohol-related problems,
greater relationship between TFS past and ZTPI past nega- but less so to symptoms of depression. With the caveat that
tive than past positive, although the degree of difference is this is a single study, we can state that anxiety is more
less than in Shipp et al. (2009). Elsewhere, studies have sug- closely related to negative affect in the present and a cogni-
gested that repetitive negative thinking, particularly in terms tive focus on the past, whereas problematic alcohol use is
of worry and rumination, is associated with elevated levels of predicted by present hedonism. On the other hand, multiple
psychiatric symptomatology (McEvoy et al. 2013). It there- time constructs combine to predict 25% of the variance in
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

fore is intuitive that higher levels of anxiety would be associ- depression. These findings need to be replicated and we also
ated with higher present negative time attitudes. It is recommend examining mediation models involving variables
therefore not surprising that we see a practically significant that temporal constructs are more closely related to as well
relationship between higher ZTPI past negative and symp- as other psychopathology constructs.
toms of anxiety in the linear regression in Table 5.
Although none of the temporal measures was practically
significant for symptoms of depression in the backward Disclosure statement
elimination (Table 3), it is noted that a lower past positive is The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are
associated with depressive symptoms in Table 6. This latter responsible for the content and writing of this article.
fidning is in keeping with the rumination literature discussed
immediately above (e.g., McEvoy et al. 2013).
References
Adhikari P, Batts D, Harrold T. 2010. Drinking patterns in Australia,
Associations with alcohol-related problems 2001–2007. Canberra: Australian Institute of Health and Welfare.
Adewuya AO. 2005. Validation of the Alcohol Use Disorders
Compared to the results for psychopathological symptoms, Identification Test (AUDIT) as a screening tool for alcohol-related
the meaningful results for AUDIT score are more numerous, problems among Nigerian university students. Alcohol Alcohol.
although notably, the variance explained by the model over- 40:575–577.
all was similar. In line with past studies, present hedonism Alansari M, Worrell FC, Rubie-Davies C, Webber M. 2013. Adolescent
Time Attitude Scale (ATAS) scores and academic outcomes in sec-
was a relatively strong, positive predictor of AUDIT score
ondary school females in New Zealand. IJQRE. 1:251–274.
(e.g., Daugherty & Brase 2010; Keough et al. 1999). This Anagnostopoulos F, Griva F. 2012. Exploring time perspective in Greek
finding makes sense, as many of the ZTPI present hedonism young adults: Validation of the Zimbardo Time Perspective
items concern risk-taking and impulsivity. Elsewhere, the Inventory and relationships with mental health. Soc Indic Res.
other three practically significant results suggest that higher 106:41–59.
Andretta JR, Worrell FC, Mello ZR, Dixson DD, Baik SH. 2013.
AUDIT scores in these university students are significantly
Demographic group differences in adolescents' time attitudes.
associated with a general sense of well-being, so lower pre- J Adolesc. 36:289–301.
sent fatalistic, lower past negative, and higher present posi- Apostolidis T, Fieulaine N, Simonin L, Rolland G. 2006. Cannabis use,
tive. The emerging picture is that a higher level of alcohol- time perspective and risk perception: evidence of a moderating
related problems is associated with a positive, rather than a effect. Psychol Health. 21:571–592.
Barnett E, Spruijt-Metz D, Unger JB, Rohrbach LA, Sun P, Sussman S.
negative, temporal disposition. These results in Table 3 are
2013. Bidirectional associations between future time perspective and
supported by the results of the linear regression in Table 4, substance use among continuation high-school students. Subst Use
where ZTPI present hedonistic (positively) and present fatal- Misuse. 48:574–580.
istic (negatively) are significantly and practically associated Beenstock J, Adams J, White M. 2011. The association between time
with AUDIT score. However, this time, a higher present perspective and alcohol consumption in university students: cross-
sectional study. Eur J Public Health. 21:438–443. doi:10.1093/eurpub/
positive time attitude is also practically significant.
ckp225
Bluedorn AC. 2002. The human organization of time: temporal realities
and experience. Stanford (CA): Stanford University Press.
Limitations and conclusion Bjelland I, Dahl AA, Haug TT, Neckelmann D. 2002. The validity of
A major limitation of the current study was that all of the the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. An updated literature
review. J Psychosom Res. 52:69–77.
variables were self-reported. It will be useful for future stud- Chavarria J, Allan NP, Moltisanti A, Taylor J. 2015. The effects of pre-
ies to include externally-assessed outcomes (e.g., ratings by sent hedonistic time perspective and past negative time perspective
others). It will also be useful to include more than one on substance use consequences. Drug Alcohol Depend. 152:39–46.
ADDICTION RESEARCH & THEORY 27

Cole JC, Andretta JR, McKay MT. 2016. The relationship between tem- McKay MT, Andretta JR, Cole JC. 2017. How individuals feel about the
poral profiles and alcohol-related problems in university undergradu- past, present and future bears little relation to alcohol-related prob-
ates: results from the United Kingdom. Addict Behav. 55:15–18. lems, anxiety, and depression: a person-centered analysis in a
Daugherty JR, Brase GL. 2010. Taking time to be healthy: predicting University sample. Subst Use Misuse [Internet]. Available from:
health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10826084.2016.1263662
Personal Indiv Differ. 48:202–207. Mello ZR, Worrell FC. 2007. The adolescent time inventory: english.
De Meneses-Gaya C, Waldo Zuardi A, Loureiro SR, Crippa JAS. 2009. Unpublished scale. University of California, Berkeley. Available
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): an updated sys- from: https://faculty.sfsu.edu/zmello/content/adolescent-time-
tematic review of psychometric properties. Psychol Neurosci. inventory
2:83–97. Mello ZR, Zhang JW, Barber SJ, Howell RT, Worrell FC. 2016.
Dunn TJ, Baguley T, Brunsden V. 2013. From alpha to omega: a prac- Psychometric properties of time attitude scores in young, middle,
tical solution to the pervasive problem of internal consistency esti- and older adult samples. Personal Indiv Differ. 101:57–61. http://dx.
mation. Br J Psychol. 105:399–412. doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.037
Ferguson CJ. 2009. An effect size primer: a guide for clinicians and Muthen LK, Muthen BO. 2012. Mplus user’s guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles
researchers. Profess Psychol Res Pract. 40:532–538. (CA): Muthen & Muthen.
Hodgins DC, von Ranson KM, Montpetit CR. 2016. Problem drinking, Papastamatelou J, Unger A, Giotakos O, Athanasiadou F. 2015. Is time
gambling and eating among undergraduate university students. perspective a predictor of anxiety and perceived stress? Some prelim-
What are the links? Int J Ment Health Addict. 14:181–199. inary results from Greece. Psychol Stud. 60:468–477.
Joireman J, King S. 2016. Individual differences in the consideration of Perry J, Nicholls A, Clough P, Crust L. 2015. Assessing model fit:
future and (more) immediate consequences: a review and directions Caveats and recommendations for confirmatory factor analysis and
Downloaded by [University of New England] at 07:52 29 November 2017

for future research. Social Personal Psychol Compass. 10:313–326. exploratory structural equation modeling. Meas Phys Educ Exerc Sci.
doi:10.1111/spc3.12252 19:12–21.
Joireman J, Shaffer MJ, Balliet D, Strathman A. 2012. Promotion orien- R Core Team. 2012. R: A language and environment for statistical com-
tation explains why future-oriented people exercise and eat healthy: puting. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
evidence from the two-factor consideration of future consequences- Available from: http://www.R-project.org/
14 scale. Personal Social Psychol Bull. 38:1272–1287. Robbins RN, Bryan A. 2004. Relationships between future orientation,
Kelley K, Lai K. 2012. MBESS: MBESS. R package version 3.3.2. impulsive sensation seeking, and risk behavior among adjudicated
Available from: http://CRAN.R-project.org/package¼MBESS adolescents. J Adolesc Res. 19:428–445.
Keough KA, Zimbardo PG, Boyd JN. 1999. Who's smoking, drinking, Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, dela Fuente JR, Grant M. 1993.
and using drugs? Time perspective as a predictor of substance use. Development of the Alcohol Use disorders Identification Test
Basic Appl Social Psychol. 21:149–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/ (AUDIT): WHO Collaborative Project on early detection of persons
S15324834BA210207. with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction. 88:791–804.
Klingeman H. 2001. The time game: temporal activity on emotional Shipp AJ, Edwards JR, Schurer-Lambert L. 2009. Conceptualization and
well-being among older Australian women: cross-sectional and longi- measurement of temporal focus: the subjective experience of the
tudinal analysis. Time Soc. 10:303–328. past, present, and future. Org Behav Hum Dec Process. 110:1–22.
Laghi F, Baiocco R, D’Alessio M, Gurrieri G. 2009. Suicidal ideation Sircova A, van de Vijver FJR, Osin E, Milfont TL, Fieulaine N, Kislali-
and time perspective in high school students. Eur Psychiatry. Erginbilic A; 54 members of the International Time Perspective
24:41–46. Research Project. 2014. A global look at time: a 24-country study of
Linden AN, Lau-Barraco C, Hollis BF. 2014. Associations between psy- equivalence of the Zimbardo Time Perspective Inventory. SAGE
chological distress and alcohol outcomes as mediated by time per- Open. 4:1–12.
spective orientation among college students. Mental Health Subst Strathman A, Gleicher F, Boninger DS, Edwards CS. 1994. The consid-
Use. 7:134–143. eration of future consequences: weighing immediate and distant out-
Marsh HW, Hau KT, Wen Z. 2004. In search of golden rules: comment comes of behavior. J Personal Social Psychol. 66:742–752.
on hypothesis-testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit Van Beek W, Berghuis H, Kerkhof A, Beekman A. 2011. Time perspec-
indexes and dangers in overgeneralizing Hu and Bentler's (1999) tive, personality and psychopathology: Zimbardo's Time Perspective
findings. Struct Equ Modeling, 11, 320–341. Inventory in psychiatry. Time Soc. 20:364–374.
McEvoy PM, Watson H, Watkins ER, Nathan P. 2013. The relationship Wells KE, Morgan GM, Worrell FC, Sumnall HR, McKay MT. 2016.
between worry, rumination, and comorbidity: evidence for repetitive The influence of time attitudes on alcohol-related attitudes, behav-
thinking as a transdiagnostic construct. J Affect Disord. 151:313–320. iors, and subjective life expectancy in early adolescence: A longitu-
McKay MT, Percy A, Cole JC. 2013. Present orientation, future orienta- dinal examination using latent transition mover-stayer analysis. Int J
tion and alcohol use in Northern Irish adolescents. Addict Res Behav Develop. (DOI: 10.1177/0165025416679740)
Theory. 21:43–51. Wills TA, Sandy JM, Yaeger AM. 2001. Time perspective and early-
McKay MT, Dempster M, Mello ZR. 2015. Does consideration of future onset substance use: A model based on stress-coping theory. Psychol
consequences: moderate the relationship between aggression and Addict Behav. 15:118–125.
alcohol use in adolescents? Results from the United Kingdom. Worrell FC, Mello ZR. 2009. Convergent and discriminant validity of
Addict Res Theory. 23:372–379. time attitude scores on the Adolescent Time Perspective Inventory.
McKay MT, Percy A, Goudie AJ, Sumnall HR, Cole JC. 2012. The tem- Diskurs Kindheits Und Jugendforschung [Res Child Adoles
poral focus scale: factor structure and association with alcohol use in Develop]. 4:185–196.
a sample of Northern Irish school children. J Adolesc. 35:1361–1368. Worrell FC, Mello ZR, Buhl M. 2013. Introducing English and German
McKay MT, Cole JC, Andretta JR. 2016a. Temporal profiles relate versions of the Adolescent Time Attitude Scale (ATAS). Assessment.
meaningfully to anxiety and depression in university undergraduates. 20:496–510.
Personal Indiv Differ. 101:106–109. Worrell FC, Temple EC, McKay MT, Zivkovi  c U, Perry JL, Mello ZR,
McKay MT, Percy A, Cole JC, Worrell FC, Andretta JR. 2016b. The Musil B, Cole JC. 2016. A theoretical approach to resolving the psy-
relationship between time attitudes profiles and self-efficacy, sensa- chometric problems associated with the Zimbardo Time Perspective
tion seeking, and alcohol use: an exploratory study. Personal Indiv Inventory. Eur J Psychol Assess. (Epub ahead of print). doi:10.1027/
Differ. 97:203–209. 1015-5759/a000313
McKay MT, Perry JL, Percy A, Cole JC. 2016c. Evidence for the reli- Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. 1983. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression
ability and validity, and some support for the practical utility of the Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 67:361–370.
two-factor consideration of future consequences scale-14. Personal Zimbardo PG, Boyd JN. 1999. Time perspective: A valid, reliable indi-
Indiv Differ. 98:133–136. vidual-differences metric. J Personal Social Psychol. 77:1271–1288.

You might also like