You are on page 1of 17

Article

Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search


by Single Neurons in the Human Brain
Highlights Authors
d A new class of neurons in the human MTL signal target Shuo Wang, Adam N. Mamelak,
detection during visual search Ralph Adolphs, Ueli Rutishauser

d Response of target neurons is invariant to visual category Correspondence


d MFC target neurons precede MTL target neurons by 200ms shuo.wang@mail.wvu.edu (S.W.),
radolphs@hss.caltech.edu (R.A.),
d Target neurons do not depend on explicitly defined targets ueli.rutishauser@cshs.org (U.R.)

In Brief
Wang et al. reveal a new class of neurons
in the human medial temporal lobe that
signals target detection during visual
search. These neurons have invariant
response to visual category, predict
detected or missed targets, do not
depend on explicitly defined targets, and
likely have a frontal origin.

Wang et al., 2018, Current Biology 28, 1–12


July 9, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092
Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

Current Biology

Article

Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search


by Single Neurons in the Human Brain
Shuo Wang,1,2,* Adam N. Mamelak,3 Ralph Adolphs,2,5,6,* and Ueli Rutishauser3,4,5,*
1Department of Chemical and Biomedical Engineering, and Rockefeller Neuroscience Institute, West Virginia University, 1 Medical Center Dr,

Morgantown, WV 26506, USA


2Computation and Neural Systems, California Institute of Technology, 1200 E California Blvd, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
3Departments of Neurosurgery and Neurology, Cedars-Sinai Medical Center, 8700 Beverly Blvd, Los Angeles, CA 90048, USA
4Center for Neural Science and Medicine, Department of Biomedical Sciences, Cedars-Sinai Medical Center, 8700 Beverly Blvd, Los Angeles,

CA 90048, USA
5Division of Biology and Biological Engineering, California Institute of Technology, 1200 E California Blvd, Pasadena, CA 91125, USA
6Lead Contact

*Correspondence: shuo.wang@mail.wvu.edu (S.W.), radolphs@hss.caltech.edu (R.A.), ueli.rutishauser@cshs.org (U.R.)


https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

SUMMARY actions to reach the goal [1–4]. Search depends on top-down


modulation of bottom-up visual processes to detect whether
Neurons in the primate medial temporal lobe (MTL) the current location contains the target [2, 5–12]. While classi-
respond selectively to visual categories such as cally investigated for its role in long-term memory [13], the MTL
faces, contributing to how the brain represents stim- is also involved in mediating ongoing behaviors by facilitating vi-
ulus meaning. However, it remains unknown whether sual and spatial working memory and scene integration pro-
MTL neurons continue to encode stimulus meaning cesses [14–18]. For example, patients with MTL lesions are
impaired in visual search due to an inability to organize explora-
when it changes flexibly as a function of variable
tion patterns and properly maintain a representation of the
task demands imposed by goal-directed behavior.
search target [17, 18]. Similarly, such patients are impaired in
While classically associated with long-term memory, oddity-judgment tasks that require the inspection of complex
recent lesion and neuroimaging studies show that scenes [19]. A key contribution of the MTL to ongoing behavior
the MTL also contributes critically to the online guid- is through its support of visual and spatial working memory
ance of goal-directed behaviors such as visual [15, 16, 20, 21], which is essential in visual search to maintain
search. Do such tasks modulate responses of neu- the representation of the target and to organize an efficient
rons in the MTL, and if so, do their responses mirror search without revisits. In the presence of distractors (which in
bottom-up input from visual cortices or do they reflect our task are fixations on non-targets), patients without a func-
more abstract goal-directed properties? To answer tional MTL are impaired in some working memory tasks
these questions, we performed concurrent record- [14, 16, 22–24]. During search, this manifests in more revisits
of the target [17]. Similarly, evidence from neuroimaging
ings of eye movements and single neurons in the
[20, 25] and intracranial recordings [21, 26] indicates that persis-
MTL and medial frontal cortex (MFC) in human neuro-
tent activity within the MTL supports visual working memory
surgical patients performing a memory-guided visual in such tasks. Together, this body of work shows that the
search task. We identified a distinct population of MTL—and in particular, the hippocampus—supports goal-
target-selective neurons in both the MTL and MFC directed behavior. However, it remains largely unknown how
whose response signaled whether the currently such modulation by behaviorally relevant goals is implemented.
fixated stimulus was a target or distractor. This In higher-order visual cortex, the response of neurons is
target-selective response was invariant to visual cate- modulated by whether the preferred stimulus of a neuron is
gory and predicted whether a target was detected or currently a target of a search [9, 12, 27]. This modulation in visual
missed behaviorally during a given fixation. The cortex is in addition to a neuron’s visual tuning, such that a cell’s
response latencies, relative to fixation onset, of MFC response is informative about the presence or absence of only a
specific target [2, 11, 12, 28]. Furthermore, this neuronal target
target-selective neurons preceded those in the MTL
response predicts whether a search target will be missed
by 200 ms, suggesting a frontal origin for the target
[2, 28]. The origins of such modulation are thought to be primarily
signal. The human MTL thus represents not only fixed the frontal cortex [4, 29–34]. However, higher-level visual re-
stimulus identity, but also task-specified stimulus sponses in macaques are in addition also modulated by the
relevance due to top-down goal relevance. MTL in tasks requiring memory [35, 36]. Despite this observation,
the role of the MTL in search has remained unclear because no
INTRODUCTION previous study has obtained single-neuron recordings from
this region during memory-guided visual search.
Goal-directed behaviors such as visual search depend on hold- We used simultaneous single-neuron recordings and eye
ing in mind a desired outcome while deploying sequences of tracking performed in neurosurgical patients to investigate how

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd. 1


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A B C D
7 500

Fixation Duration (ms)


6 450

5 400
Target

RT (s)
Search Cue Present ? Absent
1 sec 4 350
Correct
RT 3 300
Up to 14 sec +
2 250
1 sec
ITI 1
1-2 sec TP TA T D

Figure 1. Task and Behavior


(A) Task structure. The search cue is shown for 1 s, immediately followed by the search array. Subjects are instructed to indicate by button press whether the
target is present or absent (timeout 14 s). Trial-by-trial feedback is given immediately after button press (‘‘correct,’’ ‘‘incorrect,’’ or ‘‘time out’’), followed by a blank
screen for 1–2 s.
(B and C) Example visual search arrays with fixations indicated. (B) Sample stimuli. Each circle represents a fixation. Green circle: first fixation. Magenta circle: last
fixation. Yellow line: saccades. Blue dot: raw gaze position. Red box: target. (C) Boxplot of reaction time. TP: target-present; TA: target-absent.
(D) Boxplot of fixation duration. T: fixations on targets; D: fixations on distractors. On each box, the central mark is the median, the edges of the box are the 25th
and 75th percentiles, the whiskers extend to the most extreme data points the algorithm considers to be not outliers, and the outliers are plotted individually.

signals related to target detection interact with visual sensory 0.2 Hz, and we restricted our analysis to this subset. In the ana-
signals in the human MTL during active search. Subjects were in- lyses below, we pool neurons from both areas and commonly
structed to locate a visual target shown among 23 other stimuli in refer to them as ‘‘MTL neurons.’’ The words ‘‘cell’’ and ‘‘neuron’’
an array during instructed visual search. The target changed trial are used interchangeably.
by trial, requiring rapid implementation of changing goals. We
used a range of stimuli, including objects and faces, as search Target-Selective Neurons in the MTL
targets [37]. This approach enabled us to identify category-tuned To investigate the neural substrates of target detection, we
neurons [38–40] and to test whether the response of these neu- aligned neuronal responses at fixation onset and compared
rons was modulated by target relevance. the response of each neuron between fixations on distractors
and targets. We first analyzed responses to all fixations onto
RESULTS targets, whether or not they were detected. As the response,
we used the mean firing rate in a time window starting
Task and Behavior 200 ms before fixation onset and ending 200 ms after fixation
Patients (n = 8, 11 sessions; Table S1) performed a visual search offset (next saccade onset). The duration of this window was
task. In each trial, subjects reported by button press whether the on average 690 ms ± 45.0 ms (mean ± SD across sessions).
search target was present or absent (Figures 1A and 1B; 80% of 50 cells (21.9%; binomial p < 1020; 19 and 31 from the amyg-
all trials contained the target). The search target (cue) changed dala and hippocampus, respectively) had a response that
for every trial and was displayed for 1 s immediately preceding differed significantly between fixations on targets versus dis-
the search array. Therefore, patients had to keep the target item tractors in target-present trials (two-tailed t test, p < 0.05).
in working memory throughout the search. Patients performed We identified two types of such ‘‘target-selective’’ (TS) neu-
well with an average reporting accuracy of 92.0% ± 7.52% rons: one type had a greater response to targets relative to
(mean ± SD across sessions; 91.4% ± 8.47% for target-present tri- distractors (target-preferring; 27/50 cells, Figures 2A–2C),
als and 94.5% ± 6.02% for target-absent trials, two-tailed paired t and the second had a greater response to distractors relative
test, p = 0.18). Considering only correct trials, the average reaction to targets (distractor-preferring; 23/50, Figures 2D and 2E;
time (RT, relative to search array onset) for target-present trials note that these distractor-preferring cells still carried informa-
was significantly faster compared to target-absent trials tion about targets; see STAR Methods). The proportion of TS
(1.92 s ± 0.76 s versus 3.96 s ± 1.56 s, two-tailed paired t test, cells identified was highly significant (permutation test, p <
p < 104; Figure 1C). The accuracy and RT of the patients was 0.001): a control permutation test shuffling the labels of targets
comparable to that of healthy participants who had performed and distractors revealed 11.43 ± 3.34 neurons expected
the same task in a previous study [37], confirming that our patients’ by chance (mean ± SD; 6.44 ± 2.51 target-preferring and
behavioral performance was not impaired (two-tailed two-sample 5.00 ± 2.17 distractor-preferring). This result demonstrates
t test, p values = 0.17 and 0.11 for accuracy and RT, respectively). that a subset of MTL neurons encode whether the present fix-
Consistent with prior reports [41], dwell times of fixations on tar- ation landed on a target (see Figure S3 for example neuronal
gets (384 ± 88.4 ms) were significantly longer than those on dis- responses aligned at button-press).
tractors (302 ± 63.0 ms; paired two-tailed t test, t[10] = 2.72, p = To assess whether the response of TS cells could be attributed
0.022; Figure 1D; see STAR Methods for more fixation analysis). to target detection alone, or in addition also encoded visual stim-
ulus category, we next compared their response between fixa-
Electrophysiology tions on identical objects as a function of whether the fixated
We recorded in total 317 single neurons in the human amygdala object was a target or a distractor in that trial. This comparison
and hippocampus. 228 neurons had an average firing rate > revealed that the TS neurons that increased their activity for

2 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A B C D E

F G H I J K

Figure 2. TS Cells Signal Top-Down Goal Relevance but Are Not Visually Tuned
(A–C) Neurons that increase their firing rate when fixating on targets, but not distractors (selection by two-tailed t test in a time window of 200 ms before fixation
onset to 200 ms after fixation offset: all p values < 1011).
(D and E) Neurons that decrease their firing rate when fixating on targets but not distractors (all p values < 103). Fixations are sorted by fixation duration (black line
shows start of the next saccade). t = 0 is fixation onset. Asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference between fixations on targets and distractors in that bin (p <
0.05, two-tailed t test, Bonferroni-corrected; bin size = 50 ms).
(F and G) Mean response to fixations on each visual object as a function of whether the object is currently a target or a distractor (two example neurons are shown).
Each data point is a different object. Gray bars (upper right) show the number of data points above and below the diagonal. In these two examples, the response to
most objects was stronger when the object was a target (sign-test: [F] p = 1.40 3 105; [G] p = 5.35 3 104), indicating that the response was predominantly
modulated trial-by-trial by target relevance rather than visual identity. Note that these two example neurons are the same as in (B) and (C).
(H–K) Single-fixation analysis using the TSI. (H) Shown is the cumulative distribution of the single-fixation response of fixation-aligned target- and distractor-
preferring neurons for fixations on targets and distractors (n = 50 neurons). (I) TSI summary. TS: target-selective cells; NTS: non-TS cells. Error bars denote one
SEM across cells. Chance performance was calculated by permutation tests by shuffling the label of target and distractor. Error bars denote one SD across
permutations. KS test was used to compare TS cells with non-TS cells and permutation test was used to compare against chance performance. Asterisk (*)
indicates a significant difference. *** p < 0.001. n.s.: not significant. (J) Detected versus missed targets. FDetect: detected targets. FMiss: missed targets.
D: distractors. (K) Temporal response characteristics of TS neurons. FAll: all fixations on targets. FFirst: the first fixation on target. DBefore: fixations on distractors
before the first fixation on target. DAfter: fixations on distractors after the first fixation on target. Asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference between fixation types.
*** p < 0.001.
See also Figure S1, S2, S3, and S4.

targets did so even when comparing target and distractor fixa- TS Neurons Predict Behavioral Target Detection
tions that landed on visually identical objects (n = 27 neurons, We next investigated the relationship between the response of
two-tailed paired t test on difference in firing rate against 0: TS cells and behavior by quantifying the response of TS cells
p = 0.0019; Figures 2F and 2G show two example neurons, during individual fixations using a target-selectivity index (TSI).
both p < 0.001, sign-test). Similarly, the distractor-preferring The TSI is equal to the neuronal response during an on-target fix-
TS neurons increased their firing rate to distractors compared ation, normalized by the average response during fixations on
to targets when only considering fixations on identical visual ob- distractors (see Equations 1 and 2 and STAR Methods). As ex-
jects (n = 23, two-tailed paired t test on difference in firing rate pected, the TSI for TS cells was significantly larger during fixa-
against 0: p < 0.001). Because in this comparison the visual cate- tions on targets compared to fixations on distractors (n = 50,
gory of the stimuli is identical, this result indicates that the differ- two-tailed two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov [KS] test, p <
ential response of TS neurons must be driven by target detection 10122; Figures 2H and S4C; this finding is also true for target-
alone. and distractor-preferring neurons considered separately, see

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 3


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A B C D E

F G H I

J K L M N

Figure 3. Category Cells Are Fixation Sensitive and Visually Tuned during Search and Are Modulated by Top-Down Goal Relevance
(A–C) Example neurons that increased firing rate to face cues (selection by two-tailed t test during search cue presentation: p < 103).
(D and E) Example neurons that increased firing rate to non-face cues (p < 104). Each raster (upper) and peristimulus time histogram (PSTH) (lower) is shown with
color coding as indicated. Trials are aligned to cue presentation (gray lines). Trials within each category are sorted according to reaction time (black line).
Waveforms for each unit are shown in the raster plot. In the PSTH plot, asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference between the response to face-target trials and
non-face-target trials in that bin (p < 0.05, two-tailed t test, Bonferroni-corrected; bin size = 250 ms). Shaded area denotes ± SEM across trials.
(F and G) Cue-aligned average PSTH of all neurons that significantly increased firing rate for face cues (F) and non-face cues (G), respectively.
(H and I) Object category selectivity tuning showing ordered average responses from the best to the worst stimulus. Neurons with category selectivity were
identified by one-way ANOVA across 14 object categories (p < 0.05). Neurons that did not reach statistical significance (unselected neurons) were shown for
comparison purposes. Responses were normalized by the response to the best stimulus. Asterisks (*) in (H) indicate significant difference between selected and
unselected neurons: + p < 0.1, * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, and *** p < 0.001. (I) Amygdala (solid line) and hippocampal (dashed line) category neurons showed a similar
category tuning. No significant differences in category selectivity were found between amygdala and hippocampal neurons at all stimulus levels (two-tailed two-
sample t test, all p values > 0.16), although unselected amygdala neurons showed a stronger visual selectivity.
(J–N) Summary of DOS. (J) DOS during cue presentation (left) and fixations on targets during search (right). Face: face-selective neurons. Category: category-
selective neurons. Note that DOS values are dependent on the number of categories, making DOS values of face-selective and category-selective cells not
comparable. Error bars for category neurons denote one SEM across cells. Chance performance was calculated by permutation tests by shuffling the label of
categories. Error bars for chance values denote one SD across permutations. Asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference by permutation test. *** p < 0.001. (K)
Correlation of DOS (evaluated with 14 object categories) between cue presentation and search. Each circle represents a neuron. Magenta circles denote
the category-selective neurons (n = 32) and gray circles denote the unselected neurons (n = 196). The magenta line is the linear fit for category-selective neurons

(legend continued on next page)


4 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018
Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

Figures S4D–S4I). This result confirms that the single-fixation Thus, the response of TS cells remained sensitive to the type of
response of TS cells is strong enough for single-fixation analysis stimulus fixated (target or distractor) even after the target had
(also see Figure S4A and S4B for ROC analysis). Permutation already been fixated. However, the TSI for distractor fixations
tests by shuffling the label of target and distractor further increased significantly for those that were fixated after the target
confirmed our results: TS cells (30.6% ± 17.3%, mean ± SD was already fixated at least once compared to those before the
across cells; Figure 2I) had a significantly higher TSI compared first on-target fixation (KS-test: p < 109; Figure 2K). This indi-
to chance (8.68% ± 1.09%, p < 0.001), whereas the TSI of all cates that despite subjects not stopping the search, the presence
non-TS cells (12.1% ± 10.8%; Figure 2I) did not differ from of the target did influence the response of TS cells immediately
chance (12.3% ± 0.83%, p = 0.60). following the first fixation on the target, which here resulted in
We first tested whether TS neurons responded differently to TS cells responding to some degree also to distractor fixations
targets as a function of whether the target was detected by the that followed the first on-target fixation.
subject (detected versus misses, where misses were target fixa-
tions for which patients failed to press the target-present button Category Cells Are Modulated by Target Detection
immediately; see STAR Methods). TS neurons had a significantly We next asked whether the previously described category-se-
higher response to targets that were detected relative to those lective cells in the MTL [39] might also be modulated by detection
that were missed (TSI: 31.1% ± 18.6% versus 16.9% ± 79.5%, of the target. We identified two types of category cells: face-se-
mean ± SD across cells, KS-test, p < 0.001; Figure 2J; see [28] lective and general visual category-selective responses. We
for a similar analysis in macaques). Nevertheless, the response tested separately for face-selective cells because of their prom-
of TS neurons to missed targets was significantly larger than inence in the MTL [42]. We then proceeded to compare the
that to distractors (TS index: 16.9% ± 79.5% versus 0.0% ± response of both types of category cells between fixations on
0.0% (by default); KS-test, p < 107; Figure 2J). Thus, TS cells targets and distractors to determine whether their response
distinguished targets from distractors even when targets were discriminated targets from distractors, in addition to visual
not behaviorally detected by button press. Nevertheless, the categories.
maximal response of TS cells was only evoked if an on-target fix- To identify face-selective neurons, we compared the response
ation coincided with behavioral detection of the target, suggest- between face and non-face stimuli during cue presentation. The
ing that the target detection signal may be graded, from a response of 39 neurons (17.1%; p < 1011) had a significant
strength insufficient for behavioral choice, to a strength sufficient response difference (31/39 increased their firing rate for faces
for the behavioral choice and typically accompanied by compared to non-faces, whereas 8/39 increased their firing
conscious recognition of the target. rate for non-faces compared to faces but did not further distin-
guish non-face categories; see Figures 3A–3E and S5 for exam-
Temporal Response Characteristics of TS Neurons ples and Figures 3F and 3G for group average). To select cate-
Above, we considered all fixations on distractors regardless of gory-selective neurons, we grouped the objects into 14 visual
whether they occurred before or after the target had been fixated categories (see STAR Methods). The response of 32 neurons
intermittently. However, in a subset of 25.3% ± 19.1% of trials, (14.0%; p < 107) co-varied significantly as a function of visual
subjects continued to fixate on at least one of the distractors after category during cue presentation (one-way ANOVA of 14 cate-
they first fixated on the target (note that above, fewer fixations gories, p < 0.05). Figure 3H shows the ordered responses from
were considered as misses because we required at least 3 fixa- the best to the worst stimulus for these neurons. Compared to
tions before button press). We next used these trials to explore the unselected neurons, category-selective neurons showed
how TS neurons responded to targets and distractors as a conse- steeper changes from the best to the worst stimulus, a difference
quence of fixating on targets. As expected, TS neurons had a that was significant at all stimulus levels (two-tailed two-sample t
significantly higher TSI when comparing all target fixations test, all p values < 0.022; FDR corrected) except the best and
(30.6% ± 17.3%, mean ± SD across cells) to the subset of worst stimuli (Figure 3H; similar results were obtained for amyg-
distractor fixations that preceded the first target fixation dala and hippocampal neurons separately, see Figure 3I).
(0.34% ± 4.56%; KS-test: p < 1020; Figure 2K). This was also To summarize the response of category neurons during
true when only considering the first target fixation in each trial search, we used the depth of selectivity (DOS) index (see
compared to the distractor fixations that preceded this first target STAR Methods and Equation 3) [43]. DOS values range between
fixation (26.2% ± 16.0% versus 0.34% ± 4.56%; KS-test: p < 0–1, with 1 indicating tuning to only a single category (note
1020; Figure 2K). On the other hand, when only considering dis- the dependence of DOS values on the number of categories,
tractor fixations that occurred after the first fixation on a target making DOS values of face-selective and category-selective
(21.0% ± 36.4%), TS neurons still showed a significantly higher cells not comparable). During the cue period, the mean DOS of
TSI for all targets (30.6% ± 17.3%; KS-test: p < 0.001; Figure 2K). face-selective and category-selective cells was 0.42 ± 0.14

(r = 0.65, p < 104) and the gray line is the linear fit for all neurons (r = 0.65, p < 1028). (L) DOS for targets versus distractors. T: fixations on targets. D: fixations on
distractors. Error bars denote one SEM across cells. Asterisk (*) indicates a significant difference by two-tailed paired t test. *** p < 0.001. (M) Correlation of DOS
(evaluated with 14 object categories) between fixations on targets and distractors. Each circle represents a neuron. Magenta circles denote the category-se-
lective neurons (n = 32) and gray circles denote the unselected neurons (n = 196). The magenta line is the linear fit for category-selective neurons (r = 0.59,
p = 0.00039) and the gray line is the linear fit for all neurons (r = 0.63, p < 1025). (N) DOS for TS neurons did not differ between target and distractor fixations.
Legend follows (J). n.s.: not significant.
See also Figure S5.

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 5


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A B

C D

Figure 4. Population Summary for MTL


(A) Most selective neurons were either only TS (n = 38) or only face-selective (n = 27) neurons, with a minority qualifying as both (n = 12).
(B) Most selective neurons were either only TS (n = 43) or category-selective (n = 25) neurons, with a minority qualifying as both (n = 7).
(C and D) Response of all recorded neurons as a function of visual selectivity (C) DOS index for face selectivity; (D) DOS index for category selectivity; chance DOS
was subtracted to correct for baseline) and target selectivity (TSI; chance TSI was subtracted to correct for baseline). Each circle represents a neuron. Color of the
circle indicates classification of the neuron (Red: target neurons. Brown: visually selective neurons. Blue: both. Gray: neither).

(chance: 0.14; permutation test: p < 0.001; Figure 3J) and 0.68 ± versus 0.20 ± 0.15 for target and distractor, respectively; two-
0.16 (chance: 0.58; permutation test: p < 0.001; Figure 3J), tailed paired t test: p = 0.00047; category-selective neurons:
respectively. We found that category cells identified during the 0.76 ± 0.16 versus 0.62 ± 0.17, p < 104; Figure 3L). Also, visual
cue period remained visually selective during search: using selectivity was significantly correlated between fixations on tar-
neuronal responses aligned at fixation onset, selectivity re- gets and distractors when considering all neurons, regardless
mained significantly above chance for both face-selective of whether they were category cells (DOS for face-selectivity:
(mean DOS 0.36 ± 0.22 versus 0.22 expected by chance; permu- r = 0.39, p < 1011; DOS for category-selectivity: r = 0.63, p <
tation test: p < 0.001; Figure 3J) and category-selective neurons 1025; Figure 3M). Together, this result shows that category cells
(mean DOS 0.76 ± 0.16 versus 0.69 expected by chance; p < are modulated both by sensory information about the visual ob-
0.001; Figure 3J). Also, the DOS index of category cells ject category and by target detection during the search task.
computed during search and during cue presentation was corre-
lated (face-selective: r = 0.44, p = 0.0047, category-selective: r = TS and Category Cells Are Largely Distinct
0.65, p < 104, all: r = 0.65, p < 1028; Figure 3K), indicating that We next investigated whether target cells were also visual cate-
their tuning remained stable. Note that the analysis of category- gory-selective [39]. The DOS values of TS neurons were not
cell activity during search is statistically independent of cell se- larger than those expected by chance (mean DOS 0.49 ± 0.21
lection because category cells were selected based only on their versus 0.49 expected by chance; evaluated during cue presen-
response during the cue period. This result shows that category tation period with 14 object categories; permutation test by
cells selected for tuning to the cue remained visually selective scrambling object identity: p = 0.58; Figure 3N). This result
when considering fixation-onset triggered responses. shows that the response of TS cells was, on average, not visually
We next compared the selectivity of category cells during tuned.
search between fixations on targets versus distractors. We Were there cells that were both TS and category cells? We
found that category cells were significantly more tuned during found a small number of such cells: only 12 (5.26%) of neurons
fixations on targets compared to distractors (numbers are DOS were both TS and face-selective cells (Figure 4A), and only 7
index values; face-selective neurons: 0.36 ± 0.22 [mean ± SD] (3.07%) of neurons were both TS and category-selective cells

6 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A B C D E

Figure 5. TS Neurons in the Pre-SMA Respond before TS Neurons in the MTL


(A and B) Example TS neurons from pre-SMA.
(C and D) Two TS neurons simultaneously recorded in the pre-SMA (C) and MTL (D). Legend conventions as in Figures 2A–2E.
(E) Cumulative firing rate for TS neurons from the pre-SMA (dotted lines; n = 31 neurons) and MTL (solid lines; n = 27 neurons). Note that only TS neurons that had a
greater firing rate for targets than distractors are shown. Shaded area denotes ± SEM across neurons. Red: fixations on targets. Blue: fixations on distractors. Top
bars show clusters of time points with a significant difference (one-tailed pairwise t test; p < 0.01; FDR-corrected; cluster size > 10 time points). Arrows indicate
the first time point of the significant cluster. Magenta: MTL neurons. Black: pre-SMA neurons.
(F) Difference in cumulative firing rate (same data as shown in [E]) Shaded area denotes ± SEM across neurons. Arrows indicate the first time point of the sig-
nificant cluster. Magenta: MTL neurons. Black: pre-SMA neurons.
See also Figure S6.

(Figure 4B). The proportion of cells that qualified as both was had a spontaneous firing rate of > 0.2 Hz, and 211 neurons from
not greater than expected from independence of these two the aMCC, 162 of which had a firing rate of > 0.2 Hz. We found 51
attributes, i.e., TS neurons had a similar percentage of category TS neurons in the pre-SMA (Figures 5A and 5B; 39.5%; binomial
neurons as the entire population (c2-test: p = 0.25 and p = 0.99, p < 1050; 31 cells had a greater response to targets relative to
respectively; Figures 4A and 4B), and category neurons had a distractors) and 42 TS neurons in the aMCC (25.9%, binomial
similar percentage of TS neurons as the entire population p < 1050; 18 cells had a greater response to targets relative to
(p = 0.23 and p = 0.99, respectively). A scatterplot of TSI versus distractors). We next proceeded to compare the onset latency,
DOS for all recorded neurons further showed that TS neurons relative to fixation onset, of the TS neurons between MFC
and category neurons were largely separate populations (Fig- and MTL. This revealed that pre-SMA TS neurons responded
ures 4C and 4D show DOS values during cue period for face-se- significantly earlier than MTL TS neurons (Figures 5E and 5F;
lective and category-selective neurons, respectively). Further- pre-SMA: 163 ms relative to fixation onset; MTL: 44ms; permu-
more, TSI and DOS were not correlated for all cells (r = 0.061, tation test: p = 0.005; see Figures 5C and 5D for individual exam-
p = 0.36), TS cells (r = 0.062, p = 0.67), category cells ples). This result was similar for aMCC TS neurons, which also
(r = 0.057, p = 0.76), or cells that were qualified as both TS responded significantly earlier than MTL TS neurons (Figure S6A
and category (r = 0.21, p = 0.66; similar results were found and S6B; aMCC: 140 ms relative to fixation onset; MTL: 44 ms;
using face-selective DOS). Lastly, a two-way ANOVA (TS X cate- permutation test: p = 0.042). This result held when restricting
gory) of the entire population of neurons confirmed that neither analysis to neurons recorded simultaneously (Figure S6C and
face-selective cells (Figure 4C; TSI: p = 0.95; DOS: p = 0.81; S6D; permutation test: p = 0.023). Together, these data show
main effects p < 106 as by selection) nor category-selective that target-relevance signals are available earlier in MFC
cells (Figure 4D; TSI: p = 0.60; DOS: p = 0.88; main effects p < compared to MTL, suggesting that the MTL target signal might
1020 as by selection) interacted with TS cells. Together with represent top-down input from the frontal cortex. In addition,
the preceding selectivity analysis, this result suggests that TS TS neurons in MFC also differentiate between detected versus
and category cells are largely distinct. missed targets like MTL TS neurons (see STAR Methods).

MTL TS Neurons Respond Later than MFC TS Neurons MTL Neurons Encode Category Signals Earlier than
Where do the MTL target detection signals originate? To explore Target Detection Signals
the origins of the target detection signal, we also recorded from Was the response latency of category and TS cells in the MTL
two medial frontal brain areas: the pre-supplementary motor different? To answer this question, we selected a population of
area (pre-SMA) and the anterior mid-cingulate cortex (aMCC). category neurons whose response differentiated between fixa-
Nine patients (10 sessions) performed the identical task. We re- tions on faces and non-faces during search (two-tailed two-sam-
corded in total 182 single neurons in the pre-SMA, 129 of which ple t test; n = 27; 17 neurons had a greater response for faces and

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 7


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

A Pop-out Task B C
80

Pop−out [normalized firing rate %]


60
Target
Present ? Absent 40
20
Correct
RT 0
Up to 14 sec +
−20
1 sec −40
ITI −60
1-2 sec
−80
−50 0 50
Standard [normalized firing rate %]

Figure 6. MTL TS Neurons Do Not Require an Explicit Search Target


(A) Task structure for the control pop-out task. The target was defined as an ‘‘oddball’’: there was either one face among vehicles or one vehicle among faces.
Subjects were instructed to indicate by button press as soon as they determined whether there was an oddball (the same button press as the standard task). Trial-
by-trial feedback is given immediately after button press.
(B) Sample stimuli of pop-out task. Each circle represents a fixation. Green circle: first fixation. Magenta circle: last fixation. Yellow line: saccades. Blue dot: raw
gaze position. Red box: target. In this example, the target (oddball) is a face.
(C) Scatterplot of the mean normalized firing rate between the standard and pop-out task. Each circle represents the mean normalized firing rate difference
between fixations on targets and fixations on distractors for a neuron. Magenta circles denote the TS neurons (derived from the standard task; n = 28) and gray
circles denote the unselected neurons. The magenta line is the linear fit for TS neurons and the gray line is the linear fit for all neurons that had an overall firing rate >
0.2 Hz for both tasks in comparison (n = 157).

we focused on this subset here). We found that category neurons dent from selection): neurons had above-baseline discrimination
with faces as the preferred stimulus responded significantly faster of targets versus distractors in the pop-out task (two-tailed one-
than TS neurons (Figure S6E and S6F; category: 155 ms relative sample t test: p = 0.037). Together, the consistency between the
to fixation onset; TS: 44 ms; permutation test: p = 0.026) relative to standard and pop-out tasks suggests that MTL neurons encode
fixation onset. Similarly, we found that face-selective neurons search goals irrespective of goal format.
selected during cue presentation (Figures 3A–3G) also responded Lastly, we compared the onset latency of MFC TS neurons
earlier than TS neurons during search (Figures S6G–S6I). (n = 29; 20 from the pre-SMA and 9 from the aMCC) and MTL
Together, our results show that MTL neurons encode visual infor- TS neurons (n = 9) in the pop-out task. We found that MFC TS
mation earlier than target-detection responses. neurons responded significantly earlier than MTL TS neurons,
Interestingly, even though category cells became selective just as had been the case in the standard task (Figure S6K and
before TS cells, when we separately analyzed DOS in each S6L; MFC: 160 ms relative to fixation onset; MTL: 16 ms; per-
consecutive 50 ms time window, we found that DOS increased mutation test: p = 0.008). This result suggests that MFC neurons
immediately after TS cells became selective (44 ms; Fig- can signal task relevance in at least two different ways: through
ure S6M). This result further supports the idea that category neu- instructions held in working memory and by detection of oddball
rons are modulated by goal relevance signals in the MTL. stimuli. It is also worth noting that MTL TS neurons responded
earlier in the pop-out task (16 ms) compared to the standard
MTL TS Neurons Do Not Depend on Explicitly Defined task (44 ms; permutation test: p = 0.028), but MFC TS neurons
Targets responded with a similar latency (standard task: 171 ms,
To further characterize TS cells, we conducted a comparison pop-out task: 160 ms; permutation test: p = 0.58), suggesting
‘‘pop-out’’ task while recording from the same neurons as the that MTL neurons receive top-down signals faster when less
above ‘‘standard’’ task (9 sessions, 157 MTL neurons with an over- working memory is involved (i.e., Dt between MFC and MTL;
all firing rate > 0.2 Hz). In this pop-out task (Figures 6A and 6B), no standard task: 215 ms, pop-out task: 176 ms).
explicit search cue was given. Instead, the target was defined as
an ‘‘oddball’’: there was either one face among vehicles or one DISCUSSION
vehicle among faces. Patients were instructed to indicate by but-
ton press as soon as they determined whether there was an Our results reveal two distinct visually responsive populations of
oddball (target). This allowed us to examine target or distractor re- neurons in the human MTL. One population shows response
sponses in the absence of an explicitly provided search cue. selectivity to visual object categories including faces as
We examined whether the subset of TS neurons selected from described before [39, 42, 44]. A second population, described
the standard task for which we also recorded the pop-out task here for the first time, shows response selectivity to targets
(28 TS neurons; 9 target-preferring and 19 distractor-preferring) and distractors in memory-based visual search. Our results sug-
also signaled target detection in the pop-out task. We found that gest a population-level response in the MTL that represents the
this was the case: TS cell responses were significantly correlated meaning of visual stimuli by encoding both their category mem-
between the standard and pop-out tasks (Figure 6C; r(28) = 0.49, bership and their task relevance. Differential latency analysis
p = 0.0082 for TS neurons and r(157) = 0.25, p = 0.0015 for all suggests that the target detection signal in the MTL likely origi-
neurons). The correlation results were further confirmed by com- nates from frontal cortex. In addition, in the MTL, visual category
parison to chance performance (note that this test was indepen- information was available earlier than the target-detection

8 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

response. Lastly, a control experiment suggested that the MTL TS cells reflect a top-down signal within the MTL that originates
target response is independent of the format of the search goals. in the frontal cortex. Here, this conclusion rests on latency differ-
TS cells constitute a single-neuron correlate of an aspect of ences alone. Future experiments are needed to test the causality
MTL function that so far has received relatively little attention: of this pathway more directly.
the online guidance of behavior [45]. Despite an absence of de- We found that MFC neurons responded to targets and MTL
mands on long-term memory, lesion studies have consistently neurons responded to faces 150 ms prior to fixation onset,
shown that patients with MTL lesions exhibit deficits in suffi- suggesting that the neurons respond to what is currently at-
ciently demanding visual search tasks [17, 18]. A critical compo- tended, but not yet fixated. Given the observed fixation duration
nent of visual search is working memory, which is required to (Figure 1D; 300 ms), patients must have begun processing the
keep in mind the current search goal and to keep track of visited next to-be-fixated item in the middle of the present fixation,
locations. It has been suggested that the effects of MTL lesions consistent with prior reports that subjects can identify a search
are due to the increasingly recognized role of the MTL in support- target at least one fixation in advance [58]. Here, we found
ing memory for even brief periods of time (seconds) when neuronal responses supporting such look-ahead processing.
distractors and other competing demands are present Recordings from monkey superior colliculus have even shown
[16, 26, 46]. In contrast, here, we provide evidence for a second that the process of target selection can encompass at least
role of the MTL during search: the detection of goal relevance. two future saccade targets [59].
This result is compatible with the finding that patients with MTL Visually selective cells are a prominent feature of the human
lesions miss targets more frequently and fixate distractors longer MTL [38–40, 60]. Category cells increase their firing rate shortly
than controls [47]. Our subjects sometimes also missed targets after the onset of only a set of preferred stimuli shown in isolation.
despite directly looking at them, in which case the activity of The latency, amplitude, and duration of this activity increase is
TS cells was reduced. Together, our data provide direct evi- modulated by a variety of factors that include anatomical loca-
dence for a role of the human MTL in the implementation of tion, tuning sparsity, speed of stimulus presentation, and visual
behaviorally relevant goals. awareness [46, 50, 61]. These properties of category cells can
We found that the response of TS cells during on-target fixa- all be explained using a feedforward view of sensory processing.
tions was modulated by whether the subject detected the target. In contrast, we now show here that category-cell activity is
In addition to goal relevance, their response was thus indicative modulated by the top-down factor of search goal relevance. A
of the choice made by the subject. Note that a similar relation- second form of top-down modulation that modulates category-
ship has been observed for visually selective neurons in the infe- cell activity is spatial attention [42]. We also observed such mod-
rotemporal cortex of macaques [28]. Here, in contrast, we show ulation in our task because category-cell activity was sensitive to
that non-visually selective TS neurons exhibit this relationship. the currently fixated object. Our subjects actively searched the
This finding adds to the increasing evidence that the activity of array, a situation where the current gaze position is an accurate
MTL neurons reflects choices made about visual stimuli rather indication of the location of spatial attention (except for brief mo-
than the sensory input [48] and that it may track visual awareness ments of time preceding saccades [28, 62, 63]). Together, our re-
of the presented stimulus [49–52]. sults reveal that during active search, category-cell activity is
Modulation of neural activity by goal relevance has been jointly modulated by the two top-down factors of spatial atten-
observed in a number of other brain areas. For example, in mon- tion and goal relevance.
keys, in both single-saccade tasks [2, 53, 54] and tasks with natu- Searching for a face in a crowd is perhaps one of the most
ralistic free viewing [9, 27, 28], temporal lobe cortical neurons common visual search tasks that humans perform, making it
show an enhanced response to visual stimuli presaccadically important to independently investigate face cells. Here, we
when the stimulus in the receptive field becomes the target. Simi- found that face cells were strongly modulated by the top-down
larly, during delayed match-to-sample tasks at fixation, visual re- factors of spatial attention and goal relevance. This result was
sponses to ‘‘match’’ stimuli in IT and perirhinal cortex are modu- also true when restricting our analysis to face cells recorded in
lated by goal relevance [11, 12]. Here, we provide four new pieces the amygdala, which reveals that the social inference processes
of data relative to this literature. First, visually tuned cells in the hu- that are thought to rely on face cells in the amygdala [64, 65] can
man MTL are similarly modulated by goal relevance, a response be modulated by goal relevance. In conclusion, our results pro-
which they might inherit from their input [2, 11, 12]. Although visu- vide important insights into how the brain detects goal-relevant
ally tuned cells responded with a shorter latency than TS cells, targets in the environment. While behavioral work has long indi-
their selectivity increased after TS cells became selective, further cated that the MTL is important in the online control of behavior,
confirming the modulation of TS cells (cf. Figure S6M). Second, it has so far remained unclear what specifically its contribution is.
non-visually tuned target-selective cells were indicative of goal Here, we provide direct evidence for one such contribution: the
relevance alone, a kind of response that has not previously detection of goal-relevant targets.
been documented in the MTL of either humans or macaques.
Third, we identified TS cells in the MFC that responded signifi- STAR+METHODS
cantly earlier than those of simultaneously recorded TS cells in
the MTL. Fourth, MTL neurons encoded search goals irrespective Detailed methods are provided in the online version of this paper
of goal format. Responses indicating behavioral relevance have and include the following:
been investigated intensively in the frontal cortex, in particular
the frontal eye fields [55–57]. This information is thought to top- d KEY RESOURCES TABLE
down modulate other areas [34]. Our findings suggest that MTL d CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 9


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

d EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS 8. Shulman, G.L., McAvoy, M.P., Cowan, M.C., Astafiev, S.V., Tansy, A.P.,
d METHOD DETAILS d’Avossa, G., and Corbetta, M. (2003). Quantitative analysis of attention
B Stimuli and detection signals during visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3384–
3397.
B Task
B Electrophysiology 9. Bichot, N.P., Rossi, A.F., and Desimone, R. (2005). Parallel and serial neu-
ral mechanisms for visual search in macaque area V4. Science 308,
B Eye tracking
529–534.
d QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
10. Rutishauser, U., and Koch, C. (2007). Probabilistic modeling of eye move-
B Fixation analysis
ment data during conjunction search via feature-based attention. J. Vis. 7,
B Spikes
5.
B Single-neuron ROC analysis
11. Miller, E.K., and Desimone, R. (1994). Parallel neuronal mechanisms for
B TSI
short-term memory. Science 263, 520–522.
B Depth of selectivity (DOS) index and visual selectivity
12. Pagan, M., Urban, L.S., Wohl, M.P., and Rust, N.C. (2013). Signals in infe-
B Differential latency
rotemporal and perirhinal cortex suggest an untangling of visual target in-
d DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY formation. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1132–1139.
13. Squire, L.R., Stark, C.E., and Clark, R.E. (2004). The medial temporal lobe.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 279–306.
14. Hartley, T., Bird, C.M., Chan, D., Cipolotti, L., Husain, M., Vargha-Khadem,
Supplemental Information includes six figures and one table and can be found
F., and Burgess, N. (2007). The hippocampus is required for short-term
with this article online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092.
topographical memory in humans. Hippocampus 17, 34–48.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 15. Graham, K.S., Barense, M.D., and Lee, A.C. (2010). Going beyond LTM in
the MTL: a synthesis of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings on
We thank all patients for their participation, Juan Xu for creating the array item the role of the medial temporal lobe in memory and perception.
labels, and Ming Jiang for helping with the analyses. This research was sup- Neuropsychologia 48, 831–853.
ported by the Rockefeller Neuroscience Institute, the Autism Science Founda- 16. Jeneson, A., and Squire, L.R. (2011). Working memory, long-term mem-
tion and the Dana Foundation (to S.W.), the Simons Foundation (Simons ory, and medial temporal lobe function. Learn. Mem. 19, 15–25.
Collaboration on the Global Brain Award 543015SPI to R.A.), an NSF CAREER
17. Warren, D.E., Duff, M.C., Jensen, U., Tranel, D., and Cohen, N.J. (2012).
award (1554105 to U.R.), and the NIMH (R01MH110831 to U.R. and Conte
Hiding in plain view: lesions of the medial temporal lobe impair online rep-
Center P50MH094258 to R.A.). The funders had no role in study design,
resentation. Hippocampus 22, 1577–1588.
data collection/analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
18. Yee, L.T.S., Warren, D.E., Voss, J.L., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D., and Cohen,
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS N.J. (2014). The hippocampus uses information just encountered to guide
efficient ongoing behavior. Hippocampus 24, 154–164.
S.W., R.A., and U.R. designed experiments and wrote the paper. S.W., A.N.M., 19. Lee, A.C., Buckley, M.J., Pegman, S.J., Spiers, H., Scahill, V.L., Gaffan, D.,
and U.R. performed research. S.W. and U.R. analyzed data. All authors dis- Bussey, T.J., Davies, R.R., Kapur, N., Hodges, J.R., and Graham, K.S.
cussed the results and contributed toward the manuscript. (2005). Specialization in the medial temporal lobe for processing of objects
and scenes. Hippocampus 15, 782–797.
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS 20. Ranganath, C., and D’Esposito, M. (2001). Medial temporal lobe activity
associated with active maintenance of novel information. Neuron 31,
The authors declare no competing interests. 865–873.
21. Johnson, E.L., and Knight, R.T. (2015). Intracranial recordings and human
Received: February 5, 2018
memory. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 31, 18–25.
Revised: April 3, 2018
Accepted: April 27, 2018 22. Aggleton, J.P., Shaw, C., and Gaffan, E.A. (1992). The performance of
Published: June 14, 2018 postencephalitic amnesic subjects on two behavioural tests of memory:
concurrent discrimination learning and delayed matching-to-sample.
REFERENCES Cortex 28, 359–372.
23. Holdstock, J.S., Gutnikov, S.A., Gaffan, D., and Mayes, A.R. (2000).
1. Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G.L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stim- Perceptual and mnemonic matching-to-sample in humans: contributions
ulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 201–215. of the hippocampus, perirhinal and other medial temporal lobe cortices.
2. Chelazzi, L., Miller, E.K., Duncan, J., and Desimone, R. (1993). A neural Cortex 36, 301–322.
basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex. Nature 363, 345–347. 24. Olson, I.R., Moore, K.S., Stark, M., and Chatterjee, A. (2006). Visual work-
3. Wolfe, J.M., and Horowitz, T.S. (2004). What attributes guide the deploy- ing memory is impaired when the medial temporal lobe is damaged.
ment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 1087–1097.
495–501. 25. Schaefer, A., Braver, T.S., Reynolds, J.R., Burgess, G.C., Yarkoni, T., and
4. Miller, B.T., and D’Esposito, M. (2005). Searching for ‘‘the top’’ in top- Gray, J.R. (2006). Individual differences in amygdala activity predict
down control. Neuron 48, 535–538. response speed during working memory. J. Neurosci. 26, 10120–10128.
5. Chelazzi, L., Duncan, J., Miller, E.K., and Desimone, R. (1998). Responses 26. Kaminski, J., Sullivan, S., Chung, J.M., Ross, I.B., Mamelak, A.N., and
of neurons in inferior temporal cortex during memory-guided visual Rutishauser, U. (2017). Persistently active neurons in human medial frontal
search. J. Neurophysiol. 80, 2918–2940. and medial temporal lobe support working memory. Nat. Neurosci. 20,
6. Wolfe, J.M. (1994). Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search. 590–601.
Psychon. Bull. Rev. 1, 202–238. 27. Mazer, J.A., and Gallant, J.L. (2003). Goal-related activity in V4 during free
7. Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2000). A saliency-based search mechanism for overt viewing visual search. Evidence for a ventral stream visual salience map.
and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Res. 40, 1489–1506. Neuron 40, 1241–1250.

10 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

28. Sheinberg, D.L., and Logothetis, N.K. (2001). Noticing familiar objects in parametrically encodes the intensity of specific facial emotions and their
real world scenes: the role of temporal cortical neurons in natural vision. categorical ambiguity. Nat. Commun. 8, 14821.
J. Neurosci. 21, 1340–1350. 49. Kreiman, G., Fried, I., and Koch, C. (2002). Single-neuron correlates of
29. Bichot, N.P., Thompson, K.G., Chenchal Rao, S., and Schall, J.D. (2001). subjective vision in the human medial temporal lobe. Proc. Natl. Acad.
Reliability of macaque frontal eye field neurons signaling saccade targets Sci. USA 99, 8378–8383.
during visual search. J. Neurosci. 21, 713–725. 50. Quiroga, R.Q., Mukamel, R., Isham, E.A., Malach, R., and Fried, I. (2008).
30. Everling, S., Tinsley, C.J., Gaffan, D., and Duncan, J. (2002). Filtering of Human single-neuron responses at the threshold of conscious recogni-
neural signals by focused attention in the monkey prefrontal cortex. Nat. tion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 3599–3604.
Neurosci. 5, 671–676. 51. Rey, H.G., Fried, I., and Quian Quiroga, R. (2014). Timing of single-neuron
31. Buschman, T.J., and Miller, E.K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up con- and local field potential responses in the human medial temporal lobe.
trol of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science Curr. Biol. 24, 299–304.
315, 1860–1862. 52. Reber, T.P., Faber, J., Niediek, J., Boström, J., Elger, C.E., and Mormann,
32. Rossi, A.F., Bichot, N.P., Desimone, R., and Ungerleider, L.G. (2007). Top F. (2017). Single-Neuron Correlates of Conscious Perception in the Human
down attentional deficits in macaques with lesions of lateral prefrontal cor- Medial Temporal Lobe. Curr. Biol. 27, 2991–2998.e2.
tex. J. Neurosci. 27, 11306–11314. 53. Tolias, A.S., Moore, T., Smirnakis, S.M., Tehovnik, E.J., Siapas, A.G., and
33. Zhou, H., and Desimone, R. (2011). Feature-based attention in the frontal Schiller, P.H. (2001). Eye movements modulate visual receptive fields of
eye field and area V4 during visual search. Neuron 70, 1205–1217. V4 neurons. Neuron 29, 757–767.
34. Tomita, H., Ohbayashi, M., Nakahara, K., Hasegawa, I., and Miyashita, Y. 54. Ogawa, T., and Komatsu, H. (2004). Target selection in area V4 during a
(1999). Top-down signal from prefrontal cortex in executive control of multidimensional visual search task. J. Neurosci. 24, 6371–6382.
memory retrieval. Nature 401, 699–703.
55. Sakagami, M., and Niki, H. (1994). Encoding of behavioral significance of
35. Naya, Y., Yoshida, M., and Miyashita, Y. (2001). Backward spreading of visual stimuli by primate prefrontal neurons: relation to relevant task con-
memory-retrieval signal in the primate temporal cortex. Science 291, ditions. Exp. Brain Res. 97, 423–436.
661–664.
56. Schall, J.D., Hanes, D.P., Thompson, K.G., and King, D.J. (1995). Saccade
36. Naya, Y., Sakai, K., and Miyashita, Y. (1996). Activity of primate inferotem- target selection in frontal eye field of macaque. I. Visual and premovement
poral neurons related to a sought target in pair-association task. Proc. activation. J. Neurosci. 15, 6905–6918.
Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 93, 2664–2669.
57. Hasegawa, R.P., Matsumoto, M., and Mikami, A. (2000). Search target se-
37. Wang, S., Xu, J., Jiang, M., Zhao, Q., Hurlemann, R., and Adolphs, R. lection in monkey prefrontal cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1692–1696.
(2014). Autism spectrum disorder, but not amygdala lesions, impairs social
58. Kotowicz, A., Rutishauser, U., and Koch, C. (2010). Time course of target
attention in visual search. Neuropsychologia 63, 259–274.
recognition in visual search. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, 31.
38. Fried, I., MacDonald, K.A., and Wilson, C.L. (1997). Single neuron activity
59. Shen, K., and Pare , M. (2014). Predictive saccade target selection in supe-
in human hippocampus and amygdala during recognition of faces and ob-
rior colliculus during visual search. J. Neurosci. 34, 5640–5648.
jects. Neuron 18, 753–765.
60. Quiroga, R.Q., Reddy, L., Kreiman, G., Koch, C., and Fried, I. (2005).
39. Kreiman, G., Koch, C., and Fried, I. (2000). Category-specific visual re-
Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain.
sponses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nat.
Nature 435, 1102–1107.
Neurosci. 3, 946–953.
61. Mormann, F., Kornblith, S., Quiroga, R.Q., Kraskov, A., Cerf, M., Fried, I.,
40. Rutishauser, U., Ye, S., Koroma, M., Tudusciuc, O., Ross, I.B., Chung,
and Koch, C. (2008). Latency and selectivity of single neurons indicate hi-
J.M., and Mamelak, A.N. (2015). Representation of retrieval confidence
erarchical processing in the human medial temporal lobe. J. Neurosci. 28,
by single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe. Nat. Neurosci. 18,
8865–8872.
1041–1050.
62. Rolls, E.T., Aggelopoulos, N.C., and Zheng, F. (2003). The receptive fields
41. Wenzel, M.A., Golenia, J.-E., and Blankertz, B. (2016). Classification of Eye
of inferior temporal cortex neurons in natural scenes. J. Neurosci. 23,
Fixation Related Potentials for Variable Stimulus Saliency. Front. Neurosci.
339–348.
10, 23.
63. Zirnsak, M., and Moore, T. (2014). Saccades and shifting receptive fields:
42. Minxha, J., Mosher, C., Morrow, J.K., Mamelak, A.N., Adolphs, R.,
anticipating consequences or selecting targets? Trends Cogn. Sci. 18,
Gothard, K.M., and Rutishauser, U. (2017). Fixations Gate Species-
621–628.
Specific Responses to Free Viewing of Faces in the Human and
Macaque Amygdala. Cell Rep. 18, 878–891. 64. Adolphs, R. (2010). What does the amygdala contribute to social cogni-
tion? Ann. N Y Acad. Sci. 1191, 42–61.
43. Rainer, G., Asaad, W.F., and Miller, E.K. (1998). Selective representation of
relevant information by neurons in the primate prefrontal cortex. Nature 65. Rutishauser, U., Mamelak, A.N., and Adolphs, R. (2015). The primate
393, 577–579. amygdala in social perception - insights from electrophysiological record-
ings and stimulation. Trends Neurosci. 38, 295–306.
44. Rutishauser, U., Tudusciuc, O., Neumann, D., Mamelak, A.N., Heller, A.C.,
Ross, I.B., Philpott, L., Sutherling, W.W., and Adolphs, R. (2011). Single- 66. Sasson, N.J., Dichter, G.S., and Bodfish, J.W. (2012). Affective responses
unit responses selective for whole faces in the human amygdala. Curr. by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images
Biol. 21, 1654–1660. related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE 7, e42457.
45. Voss, J.L., Bridge, D.J., Cohen, N.J., and Walker, J.A. (2017). A Closer 67. Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention.
Look at the Hippocampus and Memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 577–588. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 194–203.
46. Kornblith, S., Quian Quiroga, R., Koch, C., Fried, I., and Mormann, F. 68. Itti, L., Koch, C., and Niebur, E. (1998). A Model of Saliency-Based Visual
(2017). Persistent Single-Neuron Activity during Working Memory in the Attention for Rapid Scene Analysis. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell.
Human Medial Temporal Lobe. Curr. Biol. 27, 1026–1032. 20, 1254–1259.
47. Warren, D.E., Duff, M.C., Tranel, D., and Cohen, N.J. (2011). Observing 69. Brainard, D.H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spat. Vis. 10,
degradation of visual representations over short intervals when medial 433–436.
temporal lobe is damaged. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 3862–3873. 70. Rutishauser, U., Mamelak, A.N., and Schuman, E.M. (2006). Single-trial
48. Wang, S., Yu, R., Tyszka, J.M., Zhen, S., Kovach, C., Sun, S., Huang, Y., learning of novel stimuli by individual neurons of the human hippo-
Hurlemann, R., Ross, I.B., Chung, J.M., et al. (2017). The human amygdala campus-amygdala complex. Neuron 49, 805–813.

Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018 11


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

71. Rutishauser, U., Ross, I.B., Mamelak, A.N., and Schuman, E.M. (2010). 73. Rutishauser, U., Schuman, E.M., and Mamelak, A.N. (2008). Activity of hu-
Human memory strength is predicted by theta-frequency phase-locking man hippocampal and amygdala neurons during retrieval of declarative
of single neurons. Nature 464, 903–907. memories. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 105, 329–334.
72. Rutishauser, U., Schuman, E.M., and Mamelak, A.N. (2006). Online detec-
tion and sorting of extracellularly recorded action potentials in human 74. Wang, S., Tudusciuc, O., Mamelak, A.N., Ross, I.B., Adolphs, R., and
medial temporal lobe recordings, in vivo. J. Neurosci. Methods 154, Rutishauser, U. (2014). Neurons in the human amygdala selective for
204–224. perceived emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, E3110–E3119.

12 Current Biology 28, 1–12, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

STAR+METHODS

KEY RESOURCES TABLE

REAGENT or RESOURCE SOURCE IDENTIFIER


Software and Algorithms
MATLAB R2013b MathWorks https://www.mathworks.com/products/
matlab.html
OSort N/A http://www.rutishauserlab.org/osort
Psychophysics toolbox PTB3 N/A http://psychtoolbox.org
Other
Neuralynx Neurophysiology System Neuralynx (https://neuralynx.com) Cat# ATLAS 128
EyeLink Eye Tracker SR Research (https://www.sr-research.com) Cat# 1000 Plus Remote
Ad-Tech 8-contact Microelectrode Ad-Tech (https://adtechmedical.com/ Cat# WB09R-SP00X-014
subdural-electrodes)
Cedrus Response Box Cedrus (https://cedrus.com/) Cat# RB-844

CONTACT FOR REAGENT AND RESOURCE SHARING

Further information and requests for resources should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the Lead Contact, Ralph Adolphs
(radolphs@hss.caltech.edu).

EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS

There were 11 sessions from 8 patients in total. Three patients did two sessions. All sessions had simultaneous eye tracking. All par-
ticipants provided written informed consent according to protocols approved by the institutional review boards of the Cedars-Sinai
Medical Center and the California Institute of Technology. We also compared our patients’ behavioral task performance with that of 8
healthy individuals to confirm that our patients performed normally in the task. These healthy individuals have been characterized in a
previous study [37].

METHOD DETAILS

Stimuli
We used 20 distinct visual search arrays. In each array there were 24 items whose spatial locations were randomized between the
20 arrays. 12 items contained faces (faces and people with different postures, emotions, ages, and genders, etc.) and 12 items did
not contain faces (furniture, toys, food, etc.) (see Figure 1B for an example). These face and non-face items composing the array stim-
uli have been characterized and described previously [37, 66]. From each array stimulus, we randomly assigned 4 face items and
4 non-face items as targets (on 8 distinct trials). For each array, we also had 2 target-absent trials, i.e., the target was not among
the objects in the search array (one target-absent trial with a face target, and one with a non-face target). Therefore, in total we
had 100 trials with face targets and 100 trials with non-face targets, and 20% of trials were target-absent trials. The entire task
was separated into two blocks. Each block had 100 trials. Patients finished at least one block. Importantly, low-level properties of
face and non-face items were equalized within each search array. The face and non-face items did not differ in standard low-level
saliency as quantified by the Itti-Koch model [67, 68], distance to center or size (all p values > 0.79). In the analysis of visual tuning to
object category, we further categorized the items into 14 finer categories: face, clock, vehicle, furniture, electronics, stationery, sign,
plant, toy, sport, bag, comb, clothes, and food.
In the pop-out task, we only used face items and vehicle items from those in the standard task. There were still 24 items in total in
each search array, but each array was generated online with a randomly selected subset of face and vehicle items. The spatial loca-
tion of each item was also randomly decided online. Target-present arrays had one face among vehicles or one vehicle among faces,
and target-absent arrays had homogeneously all faces or all vehicles.

Task
The task (Figures 1A and 1B) has been described in a previous study [37]. A target was presented for 1 s followed by the search array.
Patients were instructed to find the item in the array that matched the target and explicitly told that the array might or might not contain
the target. The search array stayed up for at most 14 s, or until the subject responded by a Cedrus button box, either by pushing the

Current Biology 28, 1–12.e1–e4, July 9, 2018 e1


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

left button to indicate that the target was found in the array, or by pushing the right button to indicate that the target was absent in the
array. A feedback message (‘Correct’ or ‘Incorrect’) was then displayed for 1 s. Subjects were instructed to respond as quickly and
accurately as possible. If subjects did not respond within 14 s after array onset, a message ‘Time Out’ was displayed. An inter-trial-
interval (ITI) was jittered between 1 to 2 s. The array and target orders were completely randomized for each subject. Subjects prac-
ticed 5 trials before the experiment to familiarize themselves with the task. In the end, the overall percentage of correct answers was
displayed to patients as a motivation.
The control pop-out task used different search arrays (Figure 6B) but the same task as the standard task, except that targets were
pre-defined and thus there was no search cue. Patients were still instructed to report target presence as quickly and accurately as
possible. Each block had 100 trials.
Patients sat approximately 60 cm from an LCD display with a 17-inch screen (screen resolution: 1024 3 768). The refresh rate of
the display was 60Hz and the stimuli occupied the center of the display (31.5 3 25.4 visual angle). Stimuli were presented using
MATLAB with the Psychtoolbox 3 [69] (http://psychtoolbox.org).

Electrophysiology
We recorded bilaterally from implanted depth electrodes in the amygdala, hippocampus, aMCC, and pre-SMA from patients with
pharmacologically intractable epilepsy. Target locations were verified using post-implantation structural MRIs as shown in Figure S1.
At each site, we recorded from eight 40 mm microwires inserted into a clinical electrode as described previously [70, 71]. Efforts were
always made to avoid passing the electrode through a sulcus, and its attendant sulcal blood vessels, and thus the location varied but
was always well within the body of the targeted area. Microwires projected medially out at the end of the depth electrode and exam-
ination of the microwires after removal suggests a spread of about 20-30 degrees. The amygdala electrodes were likely sampling
neurons in the mid-medial part of the amygdala and the most likely microwire location is the basomedial nucleus or possibly the
deepest part of the basolateral nucleus. Bipolar wide-band recordings (0.1-9kHz), using one of the eight microwires as reference,
were sampled at 32kHz and stored continuously for offline analysis with a Neuralynx system. The raw signal was filtered with
zero-phase lag 300-3kHz bandpass filter and spikes were sorted using a semi-automatic template matching algorithm as described
previously [72]. Units were carefully isolated and recording and spike sorting quality were assessed quantitatively (Figure S2).

Eye tracking
Patients were recorded with a remote non-invasive infrared Eyelink 1000 system (SR Research, Canada). One of the eyes was
tracked at 500Hz. The eye tracker was calibrated with the built-in 9-point grid method at the beginning of each block. Fixation extrac-
tion was carried out using software supplied with the Eyelink eye tracking system. Saccade detection required a deflection of greater
than 0.1 , with a minimum velocity of 30 /s and a minimum acceleration of 8000 /s2, maintained for at least 4 ms. Fixations were
defined as the complement of a saccade, i.e., periods without saccades. Analysis of the eye movement record was carried out offline
after completion of the experiments.
Rectangular regions of interest (ROI) were used to define array items in the search array [37]. The ROI boundaries tightly encom-
passed the entire array item and varied between items, depending on the item size. Fixations within the ROIs were counted as falling
on items. Each fixation was treated individually, and multiple consecutive fixations that fell on the same array item were counted as
discrete samples.
Patients occasionally failed to consciously detect targets, conditional on targets having been fixated. We defined ‘‘misses’’ as fix-
ations that landed on the target even though the target was not detected. We excluded the last 3 fixations landing on the target for
misses because they corresponded to target detection [37].

QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS

Fixation analysis
We included fixations from target-present trials only to analyze target response, and fixations from both target-present and target-
absent trials to analyze visual selectivity. We included all trials, but qualitatively the same results were derived when analyzing correct
trials only. We drew rectangular regions of interest (ROIs) that tightly encompassed the array items (Figure 1B). 64.4% ± 3.63%
(mean ± SD across sessions) fixations were on items (the rest were in margins), and multiple consecutive fixations within the
same array item were counted as discrete samples. 18.0% ± 5.09% fixations from target-present trials fell onto targets. To analyze
target response, we contrasted fixations on targets to all other fixations in target-present trials, including those in the margins, which
were broadly defined as ‘‘distractors.’’ Qualitatively the same results were derived when only including fixations fully within the item
ROIs. To analyze visual selectivity, we only considered fixations fully within the item ROIs.
Trials in which targets had ever been fixated (regardless of whether they had been detected by button press), had on average
5.09 ± 1.35 and 0.65 ± 0.63 fixations on distractors before and after the first fixation on target, respectively. Furthermore, patients
occasionally failed to consciously detect targets even when they had been fixated. We defined ‘‘misses’’ as fixations that landed
on the target even though the trial was not immediately terminated by button press from the subject, i.e., at least 3 fixations away
from button press. We excluded the last 3 fixations landing on the target for misses because they corresponded to target detection
[37]. 7.38% ± 6.18% of target-present trials contained such misses.

e2 Current Biology 28, 1–12.e1–e4, July 9, 2018


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

Spikes
Only units with an average firing rate of at least 0.2Hz (entire task) were considered. Only single units were considered. Trials were
aligned to stimulus onset or button press. Fixations were aligned to fixation onset. Average firing rates (PSTH) were computed by
counting spikes across all trials in consecutive 250 ms bins and across all fixations in consecutive 50 ms bins. Pairwise comparisons
were made using a two-tailed t test at p < 0.05 and Bonferroni-corrected for multiple comparisons in the group PSTH.

Single-neuron ROC analysis


Neuronal ROCs were constructed based on the spike counts in a time window 500 to 500 ms around button press for trial-wise
analysis, and in a time window of 200 ms before fixation onset to 200 ms after fixation offset for fixation-wise analysis. We varied
the detection threshold between the minimal and maximal spike count observed, linearly spaced in 20 steps. The AUC of the
ROC was calculated by integrating the area under the ROC curve (trapezoid rule). The AUC value is an unbiased estimate for the
sensitivity of an ideal observer that counts spikes and makes a binary decision based on whether the number of spikes is above
or below a threshold. We defined the category with higher overall firing rate as ‘true positive’ and the category with lower overall firing
rate as ‘false positive’. Therefore, the AUC value was always above 0.5 by definition.

TSI
In target-preferring neurons (n = 27), the normalized firing rate (i.e., the firing rate was normalized by dividing by average baseline
(the firing rate 1000 ms before cue onset) across all trials, separately for each unit) was 1.41 ± 0.28 and 1.03 ± 0.17 for fixations
on targets and distractors, respectively (two-tailed paired t test, p = 9.94 3 1011), whereas in distractor-preferring neurons
(n = 23), the normalized firing rate was 0.71 ± 0.24 and 0.92 ± 0.26 for fixations on targets and distractors, respectively (two-tailed
paired t test, p = 4.83 3 107). This result shows that the subset of TS neurons that have a larger response to distractors than targets
are decreasing their activity for targets rather than increasing their activity for distractors.
We quantified for each neuron whether its response differed between fixations on targets and fixations on distractors using a
single-fixation TSI (Equation 1). The TSI facilitates group analysis and comparisons between different types of cells (i.e., target-
and distractor-preferring cells in this study), as motivated by previous studies [44, 73, 74]. The TSI quantifies the response during
fixation i relative to the mean response to fixations on distractors and baseline (a 1 s interval of blank screen right before target
cue onset). The mean response and baseline was calculated individually for each neuron.
FRi  meanðFRDistractor Þ
TSIi = ,100% (Equation 1)
meanðFRBaseline Þ
For each fixation i, which can be either target or distractor, TSIi is the baseline normalized mean firing rate (FR) during an interval
from 200 ms before fixation-onset to 200 ms after fixation offset (the same time interval as cell selection). Different time intervals were
tested as well, to ensure that results were qualitatively the same and not biased by particular spike bins.
If a neuron distinguishes fixations on target from fixations on distractors, the average value of TSIi will be significantly different from
0. Since target-preferring neurons have more spikes in fixations on targets and distractor-preferring neurons have more spikes in
fixation on distractors (the selection process is described above), on average TSIi is positive for target-preferring neurons and nega-
tive for distractor-preferring neurons. To get an aggregate measure of activity that pools across neurons, TSIi was multiplied by 1 if
the neuron is classified as a distractor-preferring neuron (Equation 2). This makes TSIi on average positive for both types of target
neurons. Notice that the factor 1 depends only on the neuron type, which is determined by t tests on fixations as described above,
but not fixation type. Thus, negative TSIi values are still possible.
FRi  meanðFRDistractor Þ
TSIi =  ,100% (Equation 2)
meanðFRBaseline Þ
After calculating TSIi for every fixation, we subsequently averaged all TSIi of fixations that belong to the same category. By defi-
nition, the average value of TSIi for fixation on distractors will be equal to zero because the definition of TSIi is relative to the response
to fixation on distractors (see Equation 2). The mean baseline firing rate was calculated across all trials. The same FRDistractor was
subtracted for both types of fixations.
The cumulative distribution function (CDF) was constructed by calculating for each possible value x of the TSI how many examples
are smaller than x. That is, F(x) = P(X % x), where X is a vector of all TSI values. The CDF of fixations on targets and distractors were
compared using two-tailed two-sample Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests. All error bars are ± SE unless indicated otherwise.
8.95% ± 3.19% of all fixations in target-present trials that landed on an item (not in the blank) were consecutive (i.e., on the same item
as the previous fixation), and 48.5% ± 17.5% of these consecutive fixations were fixations on targets. Because there were fewer fixations
on targets than on distractors overall, the percentage of consecutive fixations on targets (24.6% ± 8.08%) was significantly higher than
that on distractors (5.77% ± 2.99%; two-tailed paired t test: p < 105). However, even though there were multiple fixations on targets, the
fixations were not always consecutive: we found that in 28.6% ± 16.1% of trials with multiple fixations on targets, there were distractors in
between fixations on targets, resulting in an average of 0.71 ± 0.58 fixations (absolute count) on distractors between fixations on targets.
Similar results could be derived when excluding consecutive fixations on targets and distractors: we could still identify 37 TS neurons
(16.2%; binomial p < 1010) with an average TSI of 32.4% ± 19.8%. Lastly, when using only the first fixations on targets and distractors,
we could still identify 30 TS neurons (13.2%; binomial p < 106) with an average TSI of 31.0% ± 18.8%.

Current Biology 28, 1–12.e1–e4, July 9, 2018 e3


Please cite this article in press as: Wang et al., Encoding of Target Detection during Visual Search by Single Neurons in the Human Brain, Current
Biology (2018), https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.092

Moreover, we confirmed our results by excluding fixations in the margins (i.e., not on an item) of the search array (only including
fixations fully within the item ROIs): we could identify 37 TS neurons (16.2%; binomial p < 1010) with an average TSI of 45.6% ±
29.0%. Notably, our TS neurons did not have a significant difference in firing rate between fixations on distractors and margins
(two-tailed paired t test: p = 0.74).
Lastly, similar to MTL neurons, aMCC TS neurons also had a significantly higher response to targets that were detected relative to
those that were missed (TSI: 34.2% ± 29.0% versus 13.2% ± 66.9%, mean ± SD across cells, KS-test, p = 0.0081), and the response
of aMCC TS neurons to missed targets was significantly larger than that to distractors (TSI: 13.2% ± 66.9% versus 0.0% ± 0.0% (by
default); KS-test, p < 106). Similarly, we found that pre-SMA TS neurons also had a significantly higher response to targets that were
detected relative to those that were missed (TSI: 84.2% ± 112.5% versus 8.51% ± 92.3%, mean ± SD across cells, KS-test, p <
107). In addition, however, the response of pre-SMA TS neurons to missed targets was significantly lower than that to distractors (TS
index: 8.51% ± 92.3% versus 0.0% ± 0.0%; KS-test, p < 0.05), indicating that the response to missed targets was suppressed.
Notably, when we compared between brain areas, we found that TSI of neurons from the aMCC did not differ significantly from
that from the MTL (p = 0.53 for detected targets, p = 0.84 for missed targets, and p = 0.91 for the difference between detected
and missed targets), whereas TSI of neurons from the pre-SMA was significantly higher for detected targets (p = 0.0014) and in partic-
ular the difference between detected and missed targets (p = 0.012; but not missed targets only: p = 0.16). Together, this shows that
TS neurons in MFC differentiate between detected versus missed targets like MTL TS neurons. In addition, these data suggest that
the pre-SMA was more strongly correlated with behavioral detection of targets relative to the MTL.

Depth of selectivity (DOS) index and visual selectivity


We quantified the DOS for each neuron:
P .
n
n j = 1 rj rmax
DOS = (Equation 3)
n1
where n is the number of categories (n = 2 and 14 for face-selective and category-selective neurons, respectively), rj is the mean
response to category j, and rmax is the maximal mean response. DOS varies from 0 to 1, with 0 indicating an equal response to all
categories and 1 exclusive response to one but none of the other categories. Thus, a DOS value of 1 is equal to maximal sparseness.
When we selected visually selective neurons, we used a tighter fixation time window of 0 to 300 ms after fixation onset to ensure
that visual inputs were restricted to fixations. However, using the identical time window for target neurons that included saccades
before and after fixations, we derived qualitatively the same results.
To assess statistical significance, we estimated the null distribution by first randomly shuffling the category labels of fixations/
target cues and then computing DOS. We used 1000 runs for the permutation analysis. We compared the observed DOS with
this null distribution of DOS to obtain p values.

Differential latency
We binned spike trains into 1-ms bins and computed the cumulative sum. We then averaged the cumulative sums for fixations on
targets and distractors, respectively. We here only analyzed target-preferring neurons because distractor-preferring neurons had
different temporal dynamics. We then compared, at every point of time, whether the cumulative sums of a group of neurons were
different (p < 0.01, one-tailed pairwise t test; FDR corrected). The first point of time of the significant cluster (cluster size > 10
time points) was used as the estimate of the differential latency. Note that this method is not sensitive to differences in baseline firing
rate between neurons because the latency estimate is pairwise for each neuron individually.
To compare latency between TS and category neurons, we selected a population of 27 neurons that differentiated fixations on
faces versus non-faces during search (two-tailed two-sample t test, p < 0.05), so that they were comparable to TS neurons (i.e.,
both were selected based on fixations during search). The majority of this subset of neurons (17 neurons) had a greater response
for faces than non-faces and we focused on the neurons with faces as preferred stimulus. In addition, we confirmed our results
with the category neurons selected during cue presentation. We examined face-selective neurons, which had a clear preferred
and non-preferred category. We conducted two analyses, with fixations on targets. The first one focused on face-selective neurons
with faces as the preferred stimulus (i.e., neurons that increased their firing rate for faces). The second one combined face-selective
neurons with faces as preferred stimulus and face-selective neurons with non-faces as the preferred stimulus by inverting the latter,
so that the preferred response of all face-selective neurons was a firing rate increase.
To assess statistical significance, we estimated the null distribution by first randomly shuffling the labels for groups and then
repeated the above latency analysis. We used 1000 runs for the permutation analysis. We compared the observed latency difference
between groups with this null distribution of latency difference to obtain p values.

DATA AND SOFTWARE AVAILABILITY

The spike detection and sorting toolbox OSort was used for data processing, which is available as open source. Data and custom
MATLAB analysis scripts are available upon reasonable request.

e4 Current Biology 28, 1–12.e1–e4, July 9, 2018

You might also like