You are on page 1of 4

J Neurol (2008) 255:1545–1548

DOI 10.1007/s00415-008-0978-4 ORIGINAL COMMUNICATION

S. Fonville Accuracy and inter-observer variation


H. B. van der Worp
P. Maat in the classification of dysarthria
M. Aldenhoven
A. Algra from speech recordings
J. van Gijn

Received: 2 January 2008 ■ Abstract Background Dysar- groups of nine observers, or four
Received in revised form: 3 April 2008 thria may be classified as flaccid, paired groups with similar levels of
Accepted: 18 April 2008 spastic, ataxic, hypokinetic, chore- clinical experience. Together, the
Published online: 3 September 2008 atic, dystonic, or mixed. We hypo- observers in a given group rated all
thesized that in routine neurologi- 100 recordings. Results The accu-
cal practice the reliability and racy of the classification was poor
S. Fonville, MD accuracy of perceptual analysis (35 % were classified correctly) and
Dept. of Neurology alone in the classification of dysar- the inter-observer agreement be-
Onze Lieve Vrouwe Gasthuis thria is low and that this classifica- tween paired groups low (κ 0.16 to
Amsterdam, The Netherlands
tion is mainly based on the clinical 0.32). The level of experience in
Dr. H. B. van der Worp, MD, PhD (쾷) · context rather than on the percep- neurology did not have a signifi-
M. Aldenhoven, MD · A. Algra, MD, PhD ·
J. van Gijn, MD, FRCP, FRCPE tion of speech. We therefore stud- cant influence. Conclusion Neuro-
Dept. of Neurology ied the accuracy and the inter- logical trainees as well as experi-
Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience observer agreement in the enced neurologists have great
University Medical Center Utrecht classification of dysarthrias on the difficulty in identifying specific
Heidelberglaan 100
3584 CX Utrecht, The Netherlands
basis of perceptual analysis alone. types of dysarthria on the basis of
Tel.: +31-30/2509111 Methods Seventy two neurologists perceptual analysis alone. In clini-
Fax: +31-30/2522782 and neurological trainees classified cal practice this probably means
E-Mail: h.b.vanderworp@umcutrecht.nl recorded speech samples of 100 pa- that most neurologists will classify
P. Maat, MD tients as flaccid, spastic, ataxic, dysarthria in the context of other
Dept. of Neurology extrapyramidal, or mixed dysar- features from neurological exami-
St. Elisabeth Hospital Tilburg thria, or as not dysarthric. All nation or ancillary investigations.
Tilburg, The Netherlands
observers were blinded to the pa-
A. Algra, MD, PhD tients’ final diagnosis, which was ■ Key words dysarthria ·
Julius Centre for Health Sciences
and Primary Care based on all clinical features and interobserver variation ·
University Medical Center Utrecht investigations. In the analysis the neurological examination
Utrecht, The Netherlands observers were arranged in eight

flaccid, spastic, ataxic, hypokinetic, choreatic, dystonic,


Introduction or mixed [7]. This classification rests largely on clinico-
anatomic distinctions proposed by Darley, Aronson and
Dysarthria is defined as a disturbance in the pronuncia- Brown [3, 4].
tion of spoken language. Articulation, respiration, reso- The perception of dysarthria depends on the analytic
nance, prosody and phonation may be compromised by ear of the clinician. In practice, however, dysarthria will
a range of inborn or acquired structural anomalies af- usually be classified in the context of other disturbances
fecting the vocal tract, but the term “dysarthria” is usu- found at neurological examination or imaging. Although
JON 2978

ally confined to articulation disorders caused by a neu- neurological textbooks stress the importance of dysar-
rological impairment. Dysarthria may be classified as thria as an aid in the diagnostic process, little is known
1546

about the accuracy of perceptual analysis alone for the them had a specific training in distinguishing different types of dys-
arthria. All observers were blinded to the patients’ clinical diagnosis.
identification of different types of dysarthria. Among Each observer listened in a separate session to the recordings on
experienced speech/language pathologists and graduate one compact disc and classified the types of dysarthria by filling out
students, a study from the USA found an overall accu- a standard form. Each recording could be repeated once before a final
racy in identifying the type of dysarthria in only 19 % classification was made. Within any observer group, each of the nine
compact discs was evaluated by a single member of that group. Each
and 56 % of the cases [9]. An accuracy of 71 % was found compact disc was therefore evaluated by eight different observers
in a French study, but the investigators only distin- (four pairs with a similar level of experience).
guished hypokinetic, spastic, and ataxic dysarthrias [1].
We hypothesized that in routine neurological prac-
tice the reliability and accuracy of perceptual analysis to ■ Classification
identify the different types of dysarthria are low. We Because of the low prevalence of the choreatic and dystonic dysar-
therefore studied the inter-observer variability and ac- thrias in our centre, we combined hypokinetic, choreatic, and dys-
curacy of perceptual analysis alone to assess its value as tonic dysarthrias into one category of ‘extrapyramidal’ dysarthrias. A
an aid in the diagnosis and classification of dysarthria. sixth group of samples without dysarthria was added. This group in-
cluded patients with different Dutch dialects, as well as patients with
In addition, we investigated the influence of neurologi- speech disorders without a physical cause (such as those with a con-
cal experience. version disorder). Each sample could therefore be classified as a “flac-
cid”, “spastic”, “ataxic”, “extrapyramidal”, or “mixed dysarthria”, or as
“no dysarthria”. Patients with mixed dysarthria were those with dif-
ferent types of dysarthria combined, such as a combined flaccid and
Patients and methods spastic dysarthria in patients with ALS.

■ Patients
■ Statistical analysis
We included native Dutch-speaking patients with any type of dysar-
thria who presented at the Departments of Neurology or Neurosur- For each level of neurological experience, the inter-observer agree-
gery of the University Medical Center of Utrecht, the Netherlands, ment between the two groups was calculated with kappa statistics [2,
between August 2002 and January 2004. We excluded all patients suf- 5]. In general, κ represents the strength of agreement above the agree-
fering from aphasia or apraxia of speech, patients with inborn or ac- ment expected by chance according to the following rating: < 0, poor;
quired anatomical deformities in the oropharyngeal region, and pa- 0–0.20, slight; 0.21–0.40, fair; 0.41–0.60, moderate; 0.61–0.80, substan-
tients who were unable to read a text. We also included control patients tial; and > 0.80, excellent [8]. We used the formula [95 % confidence
without dysarthria. All patients gave consent for participation in the interval (CI) = κ ± (1.96*standard error)] to calculate the correspond-
study. In every patient, two authors (second and last) decided the ing 95 % CIs.
‘true’ classification of the dysarthria by agreeing on the type of dys-
arthria based on perceptual analysis and comparing this with all
other clinical features and with the results of imaging and neurophys- Results
iological studies.
Approval from the local medical ethics review board was not re-
quired, given the present criteria. On the basis of the combination of all clinical findings
and the results of ancillary investigations, 19 of the 100
samples were classified as flaccid dysarthria, 17 as spas-
■ Recordings tic, 15 as ataxic, 12 as extrapyramidal, and 20 as mixed.
Seventeen samples were rated as not dysarthric. Overall,
All subjects were asked to read a standardized Dutch text, entitled the observers classified 35 % of the samples correctly
“Loos alarm op het strand” (“False alarm on the beach”) and contain-
ing 142 words. Of all 172 samples recorded, 100 were selected on ac- (Table 1).
count of the technical quality of the recording, the audibility of the Neurological trainees with less than 36 months of ex-
speech disorder, and the presence of a certain clinical diagnosis. The perience and neurologists with more than 10 years of
100 recordings were divided randomly into nine groups, each consist- experience classified the samples slightly more often in-
ing of either 11 or 12 recordings, which were transferred to separate
compact discs. correctly, but differences with the other observer groups
were not statistically significant. There was no consis-
tent pattern in the confusion of one type of dysarthria
■ Observers for the other (Table 2).
Inter-observer agreement in the classification of dys-
A total of 36 neurologists and 36 neurological trainees from three arthria was fair in neurological trainees with more than
regional hospitals and four university hospitals participated in the
study. On the basis of their clinical experience, we divided them into 36 months of experience and slight in the other observer
four pairs of observer groups, each group consisting of 9 observers: groups (Table 3). There was considerable overlap be-
1) two groups of neurological trainees with less than 36 months of tween the confidence intervals.
experience, 2) two with neurological trainees who had more than 36
months of experience (in the Netherlands, neurological training takes
6 years), 3) two with neurologists who had less than 10 years of expe-
rience after board certification, and 4) two with neurologists having
more than 10 years of experience after board certification. None of
1547

Table 1 Accuracy of the classification of dysarthria


related to the level of neurological experience Final classification* Neurological Neurological Neurologists Neurologists Mean
trainees trainees < 10 years of > 10 years of (%)
< 36 months of > 36 months of experience (%) experience (%)
experience (%) experience (%)

Flaccid 40 40 53 37 42
Spastic 29 38 38 29 34
Ataxic 33 40 47 37 39
Extrapyramidal 29 42 25 20 29
Mixed 13 10 20 13 14
No dysarthria 50 56 50 59 54

Total 32 37 39 33 35
* The final classification of dysarthria was based on the combination of all clinical findings and ancillary investiga-
tions

Table 2 Confusion matrix: classification of dysar-


thria by all observers combined compared with the Final classification* Classification by observers
final classification
Flaccid Spastic Ataxic Extrapyra-midal Mixed No dysarthria
(%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%)

Flaccid 42 6 16 11 13 13
Spastic 17 34 11 16 15 8
Ataxic 10 18 39 4 12 18
Extrapyramidal 15 12 16 29 5 24
Mixed 19 25 20 15 14 9
No dysarthria 14 19 8 4 3 54
* The final classification of dysarthria was based on the combination of all clinical findings and ancillary investiga-
tions

Table 3 Inter-observer agreement in the classification of dysarthrias


In a British study testing the inter-rater reliability for
Observer groups Cohen’s 95 % CI the clinical assessment of stroke, the inter-observer vari-
Kappa ation was smaller among physicians than among medi-
Neurological trainees < 36 months of experience 0.16 0.06–0.27
cal students [6]. We had also expected to find a large ef-
fect of the variable ‘clinical experience’ on the accuracy
Neurological trainees > 36 months of experience 0.32 0.21–0.44
of the classification as well as on the inter-observer vari-
Neurologists < 10 years of experience 0.20 0.09–0.31
ation. Although there was a trend towards a better inter-
Neurologists > 10 years of experience 0.19 0.09–0.30 observer agreement among neurological trainees with
more than 36 months of experience, neither the accu-
racy of identification nor the inter-observer agreement
Discussion differed significantly between groups. This finding is
difficult to explain. One factor may be that regardless of
The 72 neurologists and neurologists in training par- the level of experience, neurologists tend to classify dys-
ticipating in the present study classified only 35 % (range arthria in the context of other characteristics found on
32–39 %) of the speech samples correctly, which demon- neurological examination or imaging, and they do not
strates that classifying dysarthria based on perceptual receive specific training or accumulate experience in the
analysis alone is highly unreliable. This is in keeping identification of speech disorders from perceptual anal-
with a previous study in the USA of two groups of expe- ysis alone.
rienced speech/language pathologists, who correctly Remarkably, the same applies to speech pathologists,
identified only 19 % and 56 % of the dysarthria types, because in the American study mentioned above no dif-
despite the use of a standardized form including differ- ference was found in the accuracy of identification be-
ent characteristics of each type of dysarthria [9]. A tween those with a minimum of five years of clinical
slightly higher accuracy was found in a French study, but experience and those in training [9].
this included only hypokinetic, spastic, and ataxic dys- The observers distinguished 54 % (range 50–59 %) of
arthrias [1]. the subjects without dysarthria correctly from subjects
1548

with dysarthria. The fact that this number did not ap- analysis alone. In clinical practice this means that most
proach 100 % in experienced neurologists is probably neurologists will classify dysarthria in the context of
explained by the inclusion of subjects with different other features from the neurological examination or an-
Dutch dialects and those with speech disorders without cillary investigations. In general clinical practice, the
a localisable (physical) cause. ‘Mixed dysarthria’ ap- classification of the type of dysarthria should therefore
peared to be the most difficult type to classify (14 % clas- not be overestimated.
sified correctly, range 10–20 %), probably because par-
ticipants classified speech samples according to the type ■ Conflict of interest The authors declare no conflict of interest.
of dysarthria that predominated. ■ Acknowledgement We thank all patients, neurologists, and neuro-
In conclusion, neurological trainees as well as expe- logical trainees who participated in this study.
rienced neurologists have great difficulty in identifying
specific types of dysarthria on the basis of perceptual

References
1. Auzou P, Ozsancak C, Jan M, Menard JF, 4. Darley FL, Aronson AE, Brown JR 7. Kent RD (2000) Research on speech
Eustache F, Hannequin D (2000) Evalua- (1969) Clusters of deviant speech motor control and its disorders: a
tion of motor speech function in the dimensions in the dysarthrias. J Speech review and prospective. J Commun
diagnosis of various forms of dysar- Hear Res 12:462–496 Disord 33:391–428
thria. Rev Neurol (Paris) 156:47–52 5. Fleiss JL (1971) Measuring nominal 8. Landis JR, Koch GG (1977) The mea-
2. Cohen J (1960) A coefficient of agree- scale agreement among many raters. surement of observer agreement for
ment for nominal scales. Educ Psychol Psychol Bull 76:378–382 categorial data. Biometrics 33(1):
Meas 20:37–46 6. Hand PJ, Haisma JA, Kwan J, Lindley RI, 159–174
3. Darley FL, Aronson AE, Brown JR Lamont B, Dennis MS, Wardlaw JM 9. Zyski BJ, Weisiger BE (1987) Identifica-
(1969) Differential diagnostic patterns (2006) Interobserver agreement for the tion of dysarthria types based on per-
of dysarthria. J Speech Hear Res 12: bedside clinical assessment of sus- ceptual analysis. J Commun Disord 20:
246–296 pected stroke. Stroke 37:776–780 367–378

You might also like