You are on page 1of 6

Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Psychiatry Research
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/psychres

Affective touch awareness in mental health and disease relates


to autistic traits – An explorative neurophysiological investigation
Ilona Croy a,b,n, Helen Geide b, Martin Paulus c, Kerstin Weidner a, Håkan Olausson b
a
Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Germany
b
Center for Social and Affective Neuroscience, Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden
c
Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, USA

art ic l e i nf o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Affective touch is important for social interaction within families and groups and there is evidence that
Received 21 April 2016 unmyelinated C tactile fibers are involved in this process. Individuals with autism spectrum disorders
Received in revised form show alterations in the perception and processing of affective touch. sThus, we hypothesized that af-
29 August 2016
fective touch awareness based on C tactile fiber activation is impaired in individuals with high levels of
Accepted 8 September 2016
autistic trait. The pleasantness perception of optimal and suboptimal C tactile stimuli was tested in an
Available online 13 September 2016
explorative study in 70 patients recruited from an outpatient psychotherapy clinic and 69 healthy
Keywords: comparison subjects. All participants completed questionnaires about autistic traits, depressive symp-
Autism tomatology, childhood maltreatment, and about the daily amount of touch. Relative to comparison
Touch
subjects, patients reported engaging in touch less frequently in daily life and rated touch less pleasant.
Social
Reduced valence ratings of touch were explained by childhood maltreatment but not by any particular
C-tactile
Relationship disorder or depression severity. Among all tested variables, the affective touch awareness correlated with
Childhood maltreatment autistic traits only - in patients as well as in comparison subjects. Taken together, individuals with mental
health issues have a lower baseline of expression and reception of affective touch. Autistic traits and
childhood maltreatment modulate the experience of affective touch.
& 2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Taken together, interpersonal touch has a pronounced function in


social behavior, such as the creation and strengthening of bonds
Interpersonal touch is highly important for human interaction (for review see (McGlone et al., 2014) and (Gallace and Spence,
and “provides the most emotional of our tactile experiences” 2010)).
(Gallace and Spence, 2010). Touch is sensed from the very first The ability to process and perceive the whole range of tactile
moments of life, when the newborn is comforted by the mother´s stimulations may be a precondition for the creation of social bonds
embrace and remains pivotal during the whole lifespan (Sehlstedt and in consequence for the maintenance of mental health. Indeed,
et al., 2015). Interpersonal touch is not only a powerful way to a positive attitude towards interpersonal touch is negatively cor-
convey emotions between individuals (Hertenstein et al., 2006), related to social reticence (Fromme et al., 1989) and socially high-
but also decreases stress perception (Grewen et al., 2003) and anxious people experience interpersonal touch more aversive than
stress related cortisol response (Ditzen et al., 2007). Moreover, low anxious ones (Wilhelm et al., 2001). Individuals with autism, a
even subtle interpersonal touch can have strong behavioral con- disorder involving the lack of functional social interaction, are
sequences: Waiters for instance receive higher tips when touching known to dislike interpersonal touch and 70% of those also exhibit
their customers (Crusco and Wetzel, 1984), basketball players tactile sensory-perceptual abnormalities (Baranek et al., 2006). An
perform better the more they touch each other at the beginning of interesting study on postnatal depression further showed, that
the season (Kraus et al., 2010), and being touched by a nurse de- such mothers have problems in creating a rewarding tactile in-
creases levels of stress before surgery (Whitcher and Fisher, 1979). teraction with their baby, which leads to decreased positive re-
sponse in the babies (Malphurs et al., 1996).
n
Interestingly, research from the last decade opens new possi-
Corresponding author at: Department of Psychotherapy and Psychosomatic
bilities to examine the processing and perception of tactile sti-
Medicine, Technische Universität Dresden, Fetscherstr. 74, 01307 Dresden,
Germany. mulations. This research shows that there is a group of un-
E-mail address: Ilona.croy@tu-dresden.de (I. Croy). myelinated afferent nerve fibers, which are specialized in the

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.09.011
0165-1781/& 2016 Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
492 I. Croy et al. / Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496

detection of interpersonal touch. These so called C tactile afferents 2. Methods


are located in the hairy skin of the body and respond to slowly
moving, caressing types of touch (Löken et al., 2009). They re- 2.1. Participants
spond optimally at a stimulus temperature of about 32 °C, i.e. si-
milar to human skin temperature (Ackerley et al., 2014b). The C Seventy patients presenting to outpatient psychotherapy (60
women, 10 men, aged 24–70 years, mean 46.0 712.0 years SD)
tactile fibers do not only have distinct response properties, but
were included in the study. The patients were recruited by four
also project to distinct brain areas, mainly to the posterior insula
psychotherapists and diagnosed by their attending psychothera-
cortex (Björnsdotter et al., 2009). Further, C tactile targeted stimuli
pist according to the international classification of mental and
activate regions known to be involved in reward processing such behavioral disorders, version 10 (Dilling et al., 1991). The sample
as the putamen and the orbitofrontal cortex (Sailer et al., 2016; was unbalanced in gender reflecting the distribution in outpatient
Olausson et al., 2002), and regions implied in processing of social psychotherapy. The majority of patients suffered from mood and
stimuli, such as the social superior temporal sulcus (Bennett et al., affective disorders (ICD 10 F3: n¼ 24 patients), somatoform dis-
2013; Voos et al., 2013). orders (F45: n¼ 18 patients), disorders of personality (F60: n¼22
Microneurography recordings from nerves innervating the patients), post traumatic stress disorder (F43.1: n ¼4 patients), or
human forearm skin show that C tactile fibers respond optimally anxiety disorders (F40/41: n¼ 1 patient) as primary diagnosis.
to stimulation with a velocity of 1–10 cm/s; stimulations per- Most patients (n¼ 45) were diagnosed with more than one mental
formed with a velocity of 0.3 cm/s or slower or with a velocity of disease. Mood and affective disorders (F3: n ¼18 patients), post
30 cm/s or faster are less effective in activating C tactile fibers and traumatic stress disorder (F43.1 n ¼20 patients) and anxiety dis-
especially fast stimulation does not excite those fibers (Ackerley orders (F40/41: n ¼10 patients) were frequently observed co-
morbidities. As we intended to have a broad spectrum of diseases,
et al., 2014b; Löken et al., 2009; Olausson et al., 2010). Accordingly,
no diagnosis was excluded. Patients reported significantly more
light, slow stroking stimulation performed with a velocity of about
adverse childhood experience compared to comparison subjects
1–10 cm/s is perceived more pleasant than stimulation with a fast
(CTQ: emotional abuse: p o0.001; physical abuse: p o0.001, sex-
velocity of 30 cm/s or with a superslow velocity of 0.1 or 0.3 cm/s.
ual abuse: p¼ 0.02; emotional neglect: p o0.001, physical neglect:
With those response characteristics, C tactile fibers are highly po 0.001). Patients and comparison subjects did not differ in
sensitive to human interpersonal stroking touch (Olausson et al., medication, except for antidepressants (43% in patients; 0% in
2010). The velocity dependent pleasantness rating curve has been comparison subjects), analgesics (17% in patients; 4% in compar-
replicated several times - on different body parts (Ackerley et al., ison subjects) and tranquilizer (13% in patients; 0% in comparison
2014a), with handheld as well as robot guided stroking stimula- subjects; compare Table 1).
tion (Triscoli et al., 2013), and for touch in various visual (Ellingsen Physical conditions that are likely to impact touch perception,
et al., 2013) or olfactory conditions (Croy et al., 2014). However, such as diabetes, diseases of liver, kidney, neurological diseases,
there are individual differences, which suggest that several top- history of brain surgery, and any skin diseases on tested side (left
down modulatory processes play a substantial role in the sub- dorsal forearm) were exclusion criteria. Further, all patients were
jective perception of the afferent information carried by C tactile required to be fluent in German, in order to understand the
questionnaires.
fibers. While most people strongly prefer slow over fast stroking,
Sixty-nine healthy participants were recruited by public an-
some are rather indifferent. We hypothesize that any abnormal-
nouncements and among the acquaintances of the examiners (56
ities in C tactile touch perception are likely seen in the domain of
women, 13 men, aged 21–67 years, mean 45.6 712.5 years SD).
social behavior and mental health. The same exclusion criteria as for the patients were applied. Fur-
Although studies on touch in psychiatric disorders are rare, it ther, participants of the control group were required not to be
has been reported that adults with autism are more sensitive than under any psychological treatment and were screened for mental
healthy controls to certain aspects of touch including vibration, health with the short version of the patient health questionnaire
texture perception and thermal pain (Cascio et al., 2008). There are (Löwe and Spitzer, 2001; Spitzer et al., 1999).
a few published examinations on C tactile processing and autism. The study was conducted according to the Declaration of
Those show that the perception and cortical processing of slow
stroking, C tactile targeted stimuli is reduced in adults with high Table 1
Medication in patients and control subjects. The number of participants within a
traits of autism compared to adults with low autistic scores (Voos
group is displayed for each group of drugs.
et al., 2013), and in autistic compared to normally developed
children (Kaiser et al., 2015). In line, recent findings point in the Controls Patients
N N
direction, that autism is related to small fiber loss - affecting C
tactile fibers (Silva and Schalock, 2016). A study in mice further Analgesics 2 13
shows that experimental reduction of peripheral tactile sensitivity Antihypertensives 13 14
Antidiabetics 0 0
leads to enhanced anxiety behavior and reduced social interaction
Lipid lowering drugs 0 0
in mice (Orefice et al., 2016). Thyroid drugs 4 10
Here, we examined if C tactile perception is abnormal in a Ulcus drugs 2 8
broader range of mental disorders and if such abnormality might Arthritis drugs 0 2
Anticoagulants 0 3
provide a functional biomarker of mental disease. In order to Vitamins 2 4
capture the effect of a variety of mental disorders on affective Antidepressiva 0 30
touch perception, an explorative study was set up including pa- Neuroleptica 1 6
Tranquilizer 0 9
tients with various diseases and symptom severity. As adverse Anticonvulsants 0 3
childhood experience is related to mental disorders (Anda et al., Hormonal substances 12 9
2006), childhood experience was further assessed in all Immunosuppressant agents 0 4
Other medication 2 11
participants.
I. Croy et al. / Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496 493

Helsinki and approved by the local ethical board of the University three repetitions of each picture. The relationship between touch
of Dresden (EK 65022014). pleasantness ratings and stroking velocity and differences in
overall or velocity dependent pleasantness ratings were analyzed
2.2. Experimental setting and procedure with a full factorial repeated measurement ANOVA with the be-
tween subject factor group (2) and the within subject factor ve-
Participants were asked to sit in a comfortable chair in front of locity (5). For control purposes, the same analysis was repeated
a computer screen with their left arm in prone position on a pillow with picture pleasantness ratings. Data is presented after Green-
positioned on the left side of the chair. The stimuli were applied to house-Geisser correction to adjust for violations of the sphericity
the subject's left dorsal forearm. On the subject's right side, a vi- assumption.
sual analog rating scale (VAS) was placed, which was used by the Several analyses of covariance were conducted: first, to ex-
participants to rate the stimuli. amine whether medication significantly affected the subjective
The stroking was performed in proximo-distal direction by a perception of C tactile afferent stimulation, the analysis of touch
trained experimenter (author HG) with a flat, 50 mm wide, soft ratings was repeated with the additional covariate medication (yes
brush made of fine, smooth, goat's hair. Prior to the experiment no). This was done for 1) intake of any medication 2) all medica-
she was trained in delivering the stimuli with a constant force of tions that differed significantly between groups. Second, the same
0.4 N using a scale. The experimenter marked an area of 10 cm on analysis was repeated under inclusion of age and gender as cov-
the participants forearm and brush stroking was applied within ariate, as age and gender effects in the perception of C tactile sti-
this area. During the experiment, the experimenter was visually muli have been previously observed (Croy et al., 2014; Sehlstedt
guided regarding brushing velocity by a visual meter (on a monitor et al., 2016).
not visible to the subject). In total, 15 stroking stimuli were pre- Two outcome variables were defined: 1) the overall touch
sented: Stroking with 0.3; 1; 3; 10 and 30 cm/s. Each stroking pleasantness calculated as the mean of all touch pleasantness
velocity was repeated three times and the order of the stimuli was ratings and 2) the affective touch awareness calculated as
pseudorandomized. Interstimulus interval was set to 10 s. The the difference of pleasantness ratings between C tactile opti-
pleasantness ratings obtained from stroking performed by hand mized and non-optimized stroking (3 vs 30 cm/s, compare
under such controlled conditions are comparable to high-precision (Ackerley et al., 2014b; Löken et al., 2009; Olausson et al.,
brushing delivered by a robot (Triscoli et al., 2013). After each 2010)), weighted by the overall pleasantness ratings (affe-
stimulus, participants rated the pleasantness on the VAS scale with ctive touch awareness ¼ Xpleasantness3 cm/s  Xpleasantness30 cm/s)*
the endpoints 0 ¼not pleasant at all to 10 ¼extremely pleasant. (∑(Xpleasantness0.3 cm/s , Xpleasantness1 cm/s, Xpleasantness3 cm/s,
To control for general rating bias in mental disorders, such as Xpleasantness10 cm/s, Xpleasantness30 cm/s)/5). Over the whole sam-
tendency to less pleasant ratings, all participants were asked to ple, there was a weak correlation between both scores (r ¼ 0.2,
rate how pleasant they perceived five different pictures taken from p ¼ 0.01). Normal distribution of those variables was checked
the international affective picture system (Lang et al., 1999). Five with the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, and patients and compar-
pictures of different hedonic quality (spider 1202; smiling baby ison subjects were compared using independent sample t-test.
2071; clothespins 7052; gold bars 8500; dirty toilet 9301) were Six regression analyses were performed with automatic linear
presented for 3 s each on a computer screen with an interstimulus modeling: two for the whole sample, two for patients, and two
interval of 10 s. Each picture was presented three times and pre- for comparison subjects with the target variable A) overall
sentation order was randomized within and between participants. touch pleasantness and B) affective touch awareness. The pre-
After each picture presentation, participants rated the perceived dictors age, sex, depression score, autism spectrum quotient
pleasantness on the VAS presented above. score, amount of interpersonal touch, and childhood maltreat-
ment were included. For childhood maltreatment, the mean of
2.3. Questionnaires the whole questionnaire was used. Focusing on patients, the
same model was computed and extended by a predictor of the
In order to get a broad overview over the patients symptoma- diagnoses observed more than 10 times (F3; F43; F40/41; F45;
tology and potential factors that could influence touch perception, F60). Group differences between the reported frequencies of
all participants answered the following questionnaires: 1) BDI II interpersonal touch were analyzed with the non-parametric
(Beck et al., 1996; Hautzinger et al., 2006) which is a worldwide Mann-Whitney test. The reported frequencies of interpersonal
used reliable and valid instrument which asks about depression touch were correlated to the overall touch pleasantness and to
symptoms with 21 items and was used to measure depression affective touch awareness using Spearman correlation in pa-
severity. 2) The Autism spectrum quotient (Woodbury-Smith et al., tients and comparison subjects, separately.
2005) questionnaire which measures high functioning autism with
50 items that relate to social and communication skills, imagina-
tion, attention to details, and tolerance of change. 3) The 28 item 3. Results
childhood trauma questionnaire CTQ (Bernstein et al., 1997),
which measures childhood maltreatment retrospectively on the 3.1. Patients liked touch less, but there were no group differences in
scales emotional and physical neglect and emotional, physical and affective touch awareness or in hedonic perception of control pictures
sexual abuse. 4) Participants were also asked about their re-
lationship status and with an unstandardized 1-item question There was a significant main effect of velocity (F[137,4] ¼72.9,
about “how often they experience physical contact to other per- po 0.001, ŋ2 ¼0.35), with velocities in the intermediate range
sons” (410 times/day, 45 times/day, 45 times/week, weekly, being rated as more pleasant compared to fast or very slow ve-
less than weekly). locities (Fig. 1), and there was a significant quadratic fit of the
pleasantness curve (F[138,1]¼ 191.6, p o0.001, ŋ2 ¼0.58). Ratings
2.4. Statistical analysis differed significantly between groups (F[138,1] ¼5.9, p ¼0.018,
ŋ2 ¼ 0.04), with patients rating touch less pleasant compared to
Data was analyzed with SPSS 21. The pleasantness ratings of controls. However, there was no significant interaction between
touch were averaged over the three repetitions of each velocity, velocity and group (F[138,1]¼1.7, p ¼0.18). Thus, patients differed
and the pleasantness ratings of pictures were averaged over the from comparison subjects in overall touch pleasantness (T[138] ¼
494 I. Croy et al. / Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496

2.4, p¼ 0.017, d ¼0.36), but not in affective touch awareness(T awareness to autistic traits
[138] ¼0.8, p¼ 0.4). Inclusion of medication as covariate in the
model did not change the effects and there was no significant The overall touch pleasantness was not very well related to any
main or interaction effect of medication. Inclusion of age and of the predictors. In the whole sample, a diagnose of personality
gender did not change the effects and there was no significant disorder was related to reduction of overall touch pleasantness, (F
main or interaction effect of age or gender. [151,1] ¼13.3, p o0.001) explaining 7.5% of variance in total. Spe-
The overall touch pleasantness and affective touch awareness cifically for the patients group, overall touch pleasantness was
related to personality disorders as well (F[69,1] ¼5.3, p ¼0.025,
were fairly normally distributed in both groups. Five of the com-
10.8% explained variance). For comparison subjects on the other
parison subjects (7.2%) scored o/¼ 0 in affective touch awareness,
hand, sex had a weak relation in tendency, with women rating
indicating that they did not prefer C tactile targeted over non C
touch more pleasant than men. However this result did not reach
tactile targeted touch. Nine of the patients (12.9%) scored o /¼0.
significance level (F[68,1]¼ 3.6, p ¼0.06, 3.8% explained variance),
In contrast to the touch ratings, there was no significant main effect
and the result has to be interpreted with caution due to the un-
of group (F[138,1]¼ 0.15, p¼ 0.7) and no significant group*stimulus balanced gender distribution.
interaction (F[138,1]¼0.1, p¼1) for the picture ratings. Affective touch awareness on the other hand was related to the
autism spectrum quotient, both, in patients and in comparison
subjects. Across all participants, a model that included the autism
3.2. Overall touch perception related to diagnosis, affective touch
spectrum quotient (F[149,1] ¼7.3, p ¼0.007) and the history of
childhood maltreatment (CTQ: (F[149,1]¼4.2, p ¼0.042) explained
9.2% of the variance. Low scores in the autism spectrum quotient
and high scores in history of childhood maltreatment were related
to enhanced affective touch awareness. In the patients, the model
explained 12.3% of the variance and was mainly influenced by the
autism spectrum quotient (F[69,1]¼ 7.4, p ¼0.008). For comparison
subjects, the model explained 6.0% in total and only autism
spectrum quotient (F[69,1] ¼5.3, p ¼0.025), but none of the other
variables, contributed. Hence, autism spectrum quotient was cor-
related to affective touch perception in patients (r¼  0.33,
p¼ 0.006; Fig. 2) as well as in comparison subjects (r ¼  0.27,
p¼ 0.025; Fig. 2). Further, a diagnose of personality disorder con-
tributed to the model, but did not reach significance (F[149,1]¼ 3.7,
p¼ 0.057), and likewise a diagnose of post traumatic stress dis-
order contributed, but did not reach significance (F[69,1] ¼3.1,
p¼ 0.083).
An exploratory analysis was conducted to examine whether
there was a similar relationship in the opposite direction: did af-
fective touch awareness also relate to the autism spectrum quo-
tient? Therefore, autism spectrum quotient was used as target and
affective touch awareness as well as all the previous variables
served as predictors. For patients, the model with best fit (27.3%
Fig. 1. Affective touch perception in psychotherapeutic outpatients (N ¼ 70) and explained variance) included depression scores (F[69,1]¼ 14.1,
healthy comparison subjects (N¼ 69). A) Patients and comparison subjects pre- po 0.001), affective touch awareness (F[69,1] ¼6.0, p ¼0.017) and
ferred intermediate, C tactile targeted, stroking velocities (po 0.001). Patients rated
all velocities as less pleasant than comparison subjects (p¼ 0.018). There were no
sex (F[69,1] ¼4.3, p ¼0.041). For comparison subjects the model
significant differences in the shape of the curve between patients and comparison with best fit (15.0% explained variance) included the same vari-
subjects. ables as for the patients; depression scores (F[68,1]¼6.6,

Fig. 2. Affective touch awareness in healthy comparison subjects (N ¼69) and in patients (N¼ 70). Affective touch awareness represents the perceived pleasantness dif-
ference between C tactile optimized and suboptimized stimuli. High scores indicate a preference for C tactile optimized stroking stimuli, and low scores indicate a preference
for C tactile suboptimal stimuli. Affective touch awareness was negatively related to autistic traits, both in comparison subjects (r¼  0.27, p ¼ 0.025) and in patients
(r¼  0.33, p ¼ 0.006).
I. Croy et al. / Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496 495

Fig. 3. Frequency of interpersonal touch in healthy comparison subjects (N ¼69) and patients (N¼ 70). Overall, patients report significantly (p o 0.001) less interpersonal
touch compared to comparison subjects (compare the two large pie diagrams). This pattern was independent of whether participants were single, in a relation or in a
relationship with shared household (compare the six small plots).

p ¼0.013), affective touch awareness (F[68,1] ¼5.0, p¼ 0.028), and possibilities are that patients avoid interpersonal touch, live in
sex (F[68,1] ¼3.1, p ¼0.081). relationships that are characterized by reduced body contact, or
exhibit memory bias. Nevertheless, given that interpersonal touch
3.3. Patients reported less interpersonal touch presumably strengthens social bonds, a reduced amount seems
not beneficial for patients with mental disorders.
Most of the healthy comparison subjects reported to be tou- The experimental condition was designed to enable valid
ched more than 10 times a day, and none of them reported in- comparisons between ratings of C tactile optimal and sub-optimal
terpersonal touch less than 5 times a week. Most of the patients stimuli as reflected by the affective awareness score. Our patients
however, reported to be touched more than 5 times a day and and comparison subjects showed the typical velocity dependent
there were patients reporting to be touched less than weekly pleasantness rating curve (compare for instance (Ackerley et al.,
(Fig. 3). Overall, patients reported significantly less interpersonal 2014a, 2014b; Ellingsen et al., 2013; Löken et al., 2009; Olausson
touch as compared to controls (Z¼4.9, p o0.001, compare Fig. 3). et al., 2010; Triscoli et al., 2013). However, consistent with pre-
Regardless of whether patients lived alone, were in a relationship vious observations, there were some people in both groups who
or were in a relationship with a shared household, and regardless had a negative affective touch awareness score, indicating that
of whether patients lived together with children or not, they re- they rated non-C tactile targeted stroking as more pleasant than C
ported less interpersonal contact compared to healthy controls tactile targeted stroking. The affective touch awareness was sig-
(Fig. 3). Overall, there were no significant correlations between the nificantly and specifically related to the autism spectrum quotient.
reported amount of interpersonal touch and overall touch plea- However, sample characteristics, ascertainment approach, and the
santness or affective touch awareness. lack of other covariates limits the degree to which these results
might generalize. Nevertheless, our finding is consistent with
previous studies showing that healthy adult people with autistic
4. Discussion traits (measured with the same autism spectrum questionnaire as
in the current study) as well as children with diagnosed autism
Both patients and healthy comparison subjects showed the spectrum disorder have a reduced appraisal of C tactile targeted
expected velocity dependent pleasantness curve for brush strok- stroking stimuli (Kaiser et al., 2015; Voos et al., 2013).
ing, indicating that C tactile targeted stroking velocities were In contrast to these studies (Kaiser et al., 2015; Voos et al., 2013),
preferred over slower and faster velocities (Löken et al., 2009). we did not limit our sample to a specific diagnosis but examined a
However, patients rated touch generally less pleasant than com- representative sample of outpatient psychotherapeutic patients. In-
parison subjects. This effect was especially pronounced in patients terestingly, we found for both patients and healthy comparison
with disorders of personality. The attenuation of overall plea- subjects that autistic traits and not depression severity or other de-
santness of touch might be due to specific reductions of hedonic mographic characteristics (sex and age) related to the affective touch
tone of tactile stimulation or rely on other influencing factors such awareness. These findings suggest that quantifying the degree of
as being less comfortable in interpersonal interactions in general. affective touch awareness might be a potential trans-diagnostic
However, one needs to be careful to directly relate the hedonic biomarker for autistic traits. In addition, adverse childhood experi-
tone of C tactile stimulation to the preference of touch in daily life. ence was also related to affective touch awareness in both groups.
It is not clear whether the subjective preference of a variety of However, this effect was weaker and, surprisingly, negatively corre-
different interpersonal touch situations directly relate to hedonic lated to affective touch awareness, i.e. the more adverse childhood
evaluation of C tactile stimulation. This needs to be evaluated in experiences the higher the affective touch awareness.
future studies. Social impairment is one of the key domains of the autism
In line with the reduced appraisal of touch, patients reported a spectrum quotient and it has been suggested, that C tactile per-
generally reduced amount of interpersonal touch frequency. This ception is important for social communication and bonding (Löken
was true regardless of whether patients were single, in a re- and Olausson, 2010; McGlone et al., 2014). This notion is not only
lationship, or in a relationship with shared household. Inter- underpinned by the typically social situation of touch but also by
personal touch reflects emotional bonds between people and neural pathways. C tactile fibers project to target areas of posterior
those who are emotionally close also allow others to touch them insula, OFC (Olausson et al., 2002) and also activate superior
more frequently (Suvilehto et al., 2015). However, the reason for a temporal cortex (Bennett et al., 2013; Voos et al., 2013). The su-
reduction in frequency of reported touch is unclear. Among the perior temporal sulcus is considered a “social brain area” (Pelphrey
496 I. Croy et al. / Psychiatry Research 245 (2016) 491–496

et al., 2004), and it is possible that there is a shift of touch pro- Crusco, A.H., Wetzel, C.G., 1984. The midas touch the effects of interpersonal touch
cessing from affective to discriminative networks in people with on restaurant tipping. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 10 (4), 512–517.
Dilling, H., Mombour, W., Schmidt, M.H., 1991. Internationale Klassifikation psy-
prominent autistic traits (Kaiser et al., 2015). However, a labeled chischer Stö rungen: ICD-10, Kapitel V (F, klinisch-diagnostische Leitlinien).
line view of affective and discriminative touch very much sim- Ditzen, B., Neumann, I.D., Bodenmann, G., von Dawans, B., Turner, R.A., Ehlert, U.,
Heinrichs, M., 2007. Effects of different kinds of couple interaction on cortisol
plifies neuronal mechanisms of perception and spinal dorsal horn and heart rate responses to stress in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 32 (5),
signal integration is likely to contribute but is yet poorly under- 565–574.
stood (Abraira and Ginty, 2013). However, recent research in- Ellingsen, D.-M., Wessberg, J., Eikemo, M., Liljencrantz, J., Endestad, T., Olausson, H.,
Leknes, S., 2013. Placebo improves pleasure and pain through opposite mod-
dicates that impairments of peripheral tactile nerve fiber proces- ulation of sensory processing. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 110 (44), 17993–17998.
sing are related to social behavior in mice (Orefice et al., 2016). Fromme, D.K., Jaynes, W.E., Taylor, D.K., Hanold, E.G., Daniell, J., Rountree, J.R.,
Peripheral C tactile signaling and affective touch perception are Fromme, M.L., 1989. Nonverbal behavior and attitudes toward touch. J. Non-
verbal Behav. 13 (1), 3–14.
remarkably well correlated; the frequency of nerve firing corre- Gallace, A., Spence, C., 2010. The science of interpersonal touch: an overview.
lates at r4 0.9 with pleasantness ratings (Löken et al., 2009). Im- Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34 (2), 246–259.
Grewen, K.M., Anderson, B.J., Girdler, S.S., Light, K.C., 2003. Warm partner contact is
paired affective touch awareness is observed in people with re- related to lower cardiovascular reactivity. Behav. Med. 29 (3), 123–130.
duced C fiber density (Morrison et al., 2011). Moreover, the pre- Hautzinger, M., Keller, F., Kühner, C., 2006. Beck Depressions-inventar (BDI-II).
ference of fast or slow stroking is a rather stable trait with a retest Harcourt Test Services Frankfurt.
Hertenstein, M.J., Keltner, D., App, B., Bulleit, B.A., Jaskolka, A.R., 2006. Touch
reliability of 0.77 (Croy et al., 2015). Further studies are needed communicates distinct emotions. Emotion 6 (3), 528.
that examine the specificity and sensitivity of affective touch Kaiser, M.D., Yang, D.Y., Voos, A.C., Bennett, R.H., Gordon, I., Pretzsch, C., Beam, D.,
Keifer, C., Eilbott, J., McGlone, F., Pelphrey, K.A., 2015. Brain mechanisms for
awareness in patients with autism spectrum disorders.
processing affective (and nonaffective) touch are atypical in autism. Cereb.
In perspective, such studies may promote the utility of affective Cortex.
touch awareness as a diagnostic marker for autism and reveal Kraus, M.W., Huang, C., Keltner, D., 2010. Tactile communication, cooperation, and
performance: an ethological study of the NBA. Emotion 10 (5), 745.
clinical usability. Already now, therapeutic applications of slow Lang, P.J., Bradley, M.M., Cuthbert, B.N., 1999. International Affective Picture System
stroking touch for enhancement of social processing in autism (Iaps): Instruction Manual And Affective Ratings. The Center for Research in
spectrum diseases (Silva et al., 2015) are in line with our ob- Psychophysiology, University of Florida.
Löken, L.S., Olausson, H., 2010. The skin as a social organ. Exp. Brain Res. 204 (3),
servations. Another interesting perspective is the research on ef- 305–314.
ferent C fiber function. Studying both branches of the post- Löken, L.S., Wessberg, J., McGlone, F., Olausson, H., 2009. Coding of pleasant touch
by unmyelinated afferents in humans. Nat. Neurosci. 12 (5), 547–548.
ganglionic C-fiber system will clarify, whether affective touch re- Löwe, B.P., Spitze, R.L., 2001. Gesundheitsfragebogen für Patienten PHQ-D: Manual
lates to autonomic dysfunction and cardiovascular risk in patients (Komplettversion und Kurzform). Pfizer GmbH.
with psychiatric disorders. Malphurs, J.E., Field, T.M., Larraine, C., Pickens, J., Pelaez-Nogueras, M., Yando, R.,
Bendell, D., 1996. Altering withdrawn and intrusive interaction behaviors of
In summary, the findings from the current study point toward depressed mothers. Infant Ment. Health J. 17 (2), 152–160.
affective touch awareness as a biological marker that is closely McGlone, F., Wessberg, J., Olausson, H., 2014. Discriminative and affective touch:
related to autistic traits rather than as a vulnerability marker for sensing and feeling. Neuron 82 (4), 737–755.
Morrison, I., Loken, L.S., Minde, J., Wessberg, J., Perini, I., Nennesmo, I., Olausson, H.,
mental disorders in general. 2011. Reduced C-afferent fibre density affects perceived pleasantness and
empathy for touch. Brain: J. Neurol. 134 (Pt 4), 1116–1126.
Olausson, H., Lamarre, Y., Backlund, H., Morin, C., Wallin, B., Starck, G., Ekholm, S.,
Strigo, I., Worsley, K., Vallbo, Å., 2002. Unmyelinated tactile afferents signal
Acknowledgements touch and project to insular cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 5 (9), 900–904.
Olausson, H., Wessberg, J., Morrison, I., McGlone, F., Vallbo, A., 2010. The neuro-
physiology of unmyelinated tactile afferents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 34 (2),
This study was supported by a grant of the Marcus och Amalia 185–191.
Wallenbergs Minnesfond to IC (MAW 2014.000). Orefice, L.L., Zimmerman, A.L., Chirila, A.M., Sleboda, S.J., Head, J.P., Ginty, D.D.,
2016. Peripheral mechanosensory neuron dysfunction underlies tactile and
behavioral deficits in mouse models of ASDs. Cell.
Pelphrey, K.A., Morris, J.P., Mccarthy, G., 2004. Grasping the intentions of others:
References the perceived intentionality of an action influences activity in the superior
temporal sulcus during social perception. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16 (10), 1706–1716.
Sailer, U., Triscoli, C., Häggblad, G., Hamilton, P., Olausson, H., Croy, I., 2016. Tem-
Abraira, V.E., Ginty, D.D., 2013. The sensory neurons of touch. Neuron 79 (4), poral dynamics of brain activation during 40 min of pleasant touch.
618–639. NeuroImage.
Ackerley, R., Carlsson, I., Wester, H., Olausson, H., Wasling, H.B., 2014a. Touch per- Sehlstedt, I., Ignell, H., Backlund Wasling, H., Ackerley, R., Olausson, H., Croy, I.,
ceptions across skin sites: differences between sensitivity, direction dis- 2016. Gentle touch perception across the lifespan. Psychol. Aging 31 (2), 176.
crimination and pleasantness. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8. Sehlstedt, I., Ignell, H., Wasling, H.B., Ackerley, R., Olausson, H., Croy, I., 2015. Gentle
Ackerley, R., Wasling, H.B., Liljencrantz, J., Olausson, H., Johnson, R.D., Wessberg, J., touch perception across the lifespan. Psychol. Aging.
2014b. Human C-tactile afferents are tuned to the temperature of a skin- Silva, L., Schalock, M., 2016. First skin biopsy reports in children with autism show
stroking caress. J. Neurosci. 34 (8), 2879–2883. loss of C-tactile fibers. J. Neurol. Disord. 4 (262), 2.
Anda, R.F., Felitti, V.J., Bremner, J.D., Walker, J.D., Whitfield, C., Perry, B.D., Dube, S.R., Silva, L.M., Schalock, M., Gabrielsen, K.R., Budden, S.S., Buenrostro, M., Horton, G.,
Giles, W.H., 2006. The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experi- 2015. Early intervention with a parent-delivered massage protocol directed at
ences in childhood. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 256 (3), 174–186. tactile abnormalities decreases severity of autism and improves child-to-parent
Baranek, G.T., David, F.J., Poe, M.D., Stone, W.L., Watson, L.R., 2006. Sensory ex- interactions: a replication study. Autism Res. Treat., 2015.
periences questionnaire: discriminating sensory features in young children Spitzer, R.L., Kroenke, K., Williams, J.B., Patient Health Questionnaire Primary Care
with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. J. Child Psychol. Study Group, 1999. Validation and utility of a self-report version of PRIME-MD:
Psychiatry 47 (6), 591–601. the PHQ primary care study. JAMA 282 (18), 1737–1744.
Beck, A.T., Steer, R.A., Brown, G.K., 1996. Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio. Suvilehto, J.T., Glerean, E., Dunbar, R.I., Hari, R., Nummenmaa, L., 2015. Topography
Bennett, R.H., Bolling, D.Z., Anderson, L.C., Pelphrey, K.A., Kaiser, M.D., 2013. fNIRS of social touching depends on emotional bonds between humans. Proc. Natl.
detects temporal lobe response to affective touch. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9 Acad. Sci., 19231.
(4), 470–476. Triscoli, C., Olausson, H., Sailer, U., Ignell, H., Croy, I., 2013. CT-optimized skin
Bernstein, D.P., Ahluvalia, T., Pogge, D., Handelsman, L., 1997. Validity of the stroking delivered by hand or robot is comparable. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 7,
childhood trauma questionnaire in an adolescent psychiatric population. J. Am. 208.
Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36 (3), 340–348. Voos, A.C., Pelphrey, K.A., Kaiser, M.D., 2013a. Autistic traits are associated with
Björnsdotter, M., Löken, L., Olausson, H., Vallbo, Å., Wessberg, J., 2009. Somatotopic diminished neural response to affective touch. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8 (4),
organization of gentle touch processing in the posterior insular cortex. J. 378–386.
Neurosci. 29 (29), 9314–9320. Whitcher, S.J., Fisher, J.D., 1979. Multidimensional reaction to therapeutic touch in a
Cascio, C., McGlone, F., Folger, S., Tannan, V., Baranek, G., Pelphrey, K.A., Essick, G., hospital setting. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 37 (1), 87.
2008. Tactile perception in adults with autism: a multidimensional psycho- Wilhelm, F.H., Kochar, A.S., Roth, W.T., Gross, J.J., 2001. Social anxiety and response
physical study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38 (1), 127–137. to touch: incongruence between self-evaluative and physiological reactions.
Croy, I., Angelo, S.D., Olausson, H., 2014. Reduced pleasant touch appraisal in the Biol. Psychol. 58 (3), 181–202.
presence of a disgusting odor. PLoS One 9 (3), e92975. Woodbury-Smith, M.R., Robinson, J., Wheelwright, S., Baron-Cohen, S., 2005.
Croy, I., Triscoli, C., Luong, A., Sehlstedt, T., Sailer, U., Olausson, H., 2015. The In- Screening adults for asperger syndrome using the AQ: a preliminary study of its
dividual Preferred Velocity of Touch as a Stable Measurement. IASAT, London. diagnostic validity in clinical practice. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 35 (3), 331–335.

You might also like