You are on page 1of 13

Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology Copyright 2000 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

2000, Vol. 68, No. 5, 830-842 0022-006X/00/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//0022-006X.68.5.830

Assessing Symptoms of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in


Children and Adults: Which Is More Valid?

Stephen V. Faraone Joseph Biederman


Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Massachusetts General Hospital
Massachusetts Mental Health Center, and Harvard Institute and Harvard Medical School
of Psychiatric Epidemiology and Genetics

Jennifer A. Feighner Michael C. Monuteaux


Massachusetts General Hospital Massachusetts General Hospital
and Harvard School of Public Health

The assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults has been a source of
controversy. The authors tested competing ideas by evaluating familial transmission among adult and
nonadult relatives of ADHD children. They analyzed ADHD symptom data collected by structured
interviews from the members of 280 ADHD and 242 non-ADHD families. For both past and current
symptoms, both the boys' and girls' families showed significantly more familial aggregation for adult
relatives than for nonadult relatives. The results were similar for inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive
symptoms and for relatives with and without psychiatric comorbidity. The results provide further
evidence for the validity of adult ADHD and support the intriguing idea that, from a familial perspective,
the assessment of ADHD may be more valid in adults than in children. They do not support the idea that
parents of ADHD children are biased to report ADHD symptoms in themselves because of their exposure
to an ADHD child.

Despite media attention and the success of popular books, from a pattem of consistent data. For psychiatric disorders, stan-
investigators continue to debate the validity of attention deficit dard validation criteria include clinical correlates, treatment re-
hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in adults. Some assert that most sponse, laboratory studies, follow-up studies, and family history.
cases of ADHD remit in adulthood; they conclude that adult Our prior reviews of these validity criteria suggest ADHD may be
ADHD is very rare (Hill & Schoener, 1996; Shaffer, 1994). If so, a valid disorder (Faraone, 2000; Spencer et al., 1994; Spencer,
most referred cases must be due to referral artifacts or missed Biederman, Wilens, & Faraone, 1998).
differential diagnoses. In contrast, others claim that many cases of If adult ADHD is valid, we would expect the children of ADHD
ADHD persist into adulthood and that ADHD in adults should be adults to have an elevated prevalence of ADHD (Faraone, Tsuang,
considered a valid disorder (Barkley, 1997; Murphy & Barkley, & Tsuang, 1999; Faraone & Tsuang, 1995). This has been found
1996a; Spencer, Biederman, Wilens, & Faraone, 1994). by the two extant family studies of adult ADHD (Biederman, et al.,
Given this skepticism about the validity of adult ADHD, re- 1995; Manshadi, Lippmann, O'Daniel, & Blackman, 1983). These
searchers have sought to assess the validity of the disorder using studies both produced the same intriguing result: The risk of
the criteria of Robins and Guze (1970). In their view, the validity ADHD among children of ADHD adults was much higher than the
of any psychiatric disorder derives not from a single study, but risk for ADHD among relatives of children with ADHD. For
example, we found a 57% prevalence of ADHD among children of
ADHD adults, which was much higher than the 15% prevalence of
ADHD among siblings of ADHD children (Biederman et al.,
Stephen V. Faraone, Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit, Child Psychi-
atry Service, Massachusetts General Hospital (MGH); Harvard Medical 1995). These results are somewhat counterintuitive. If adult
School; Massachusetts Mental Health Center; and Harvard Institute of ADHD is an uncertain diagnosis, fraught with the difficulties of
Psychiatric Epidemiology and Genetics. Joseph Biederman, Pediatric Psy- retrospective recall and self-referral biases, there should be more
chopharmacology Unit, Child Psychiatry Service, MGH; and Harvard false positive cases of ADHD among adults than among children.
Medical School. Jennifer A. Feighner, Pediatric Psychopharmacology If that were the case, then evidence for familial transmission
Unit, Child Psychiatry Service, MGH. Michael C. Monuteaux, Pediatric should be lower in families sampled through adults compared with
Psychopharmacology Unit, Child Psychiatry Service, MGH; and Depart- those sampled through children. Yet the opposite is true.
ment of Epidemiology, Harvard School of Public Health. The high familial loading of adult ADHD suggests that genes
This work was supported in part by National Institutes of Health Grants
(or other familial risk factors) may play a smaller role in the
R01MH57934, R01HD37694, R13MH59126, and R01MH41314.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Stephen etiology of remitting ADHD than they do for persistent ADHD.
V. Faraone, Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit (ACC 725), Massachu- We tested this persistence hypothesis in two ways. In a prospective
setts General Hospital, 15 Parkman Street, Boston, Massachusetts 02114. study, we examined 140 ADHD boys and 120 non-ADHD boys at
Electronic mail may be sent to sfaraone@earthlink.net. a baseline assessment and completed a 4-year follow-up study. By

830
VALIDITY OF ASSESSINGADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 831

mid-adolescence, 85% of the ADHD boys continued to have the threshold to define deviance at each age will reduce the number of
disorder; 15% remitted. The prevalence of ADHD was signifi- diagnosable cases among older individuals.
candy higher among the relatives of persistent ADHD probands This issue was addressed by Heiligenstein, Conyers, Berns,
compared with relatives of remitted ADHD probands (Biederman, Miller, and Smith (1998). They examined ADHD diagnoses and
et al., 1996). Parents of persistent ADHD probands were 20 times symptom scores among 448 college students not selected for any
more likely to have ADHD than parents of controls whereas psychiatric diagnosis. Four percent of the students met DSM-IV
parents of nonpersistent ADHD probands showed only a fivefold criteria for ADHD. They then defined ADHD as deviance from the
increased risk. Similarly, siblings of persistent ADHD probands norm: Students were defined as having ADHD if their total symp-
were 17 times more likely to have ADHD than siblings of controls, tom score exceeded the 93rd percentile of the sample. The subjects
whereas siblings of nonpersistent ADHD probands showed only a who met this criterion showed clinically significant symptoms
four-fold increased risk (Faraone, Biederman, & Monuteaux, (Heiligenstein, Guenther, Levy, Savino, & Fulwiler, 1999; Heiligen-
2000). In a retrospective study, we compared ADHD adolescents stein & Keeling, 1995). Thus, the authors argued that the DSM-IV
having retrospectively reported childhood onset ADHD with criteria were too stringent because they did not identify many
ADHD children. The relatives of adolescent probands had higher subjects who were both deviant from the norm and showed symp-
rates of ADHD compared with the relatives of child probands toms warranting clinical attention.
(Biederman et al., 1998). Thus, a prospective study of children and The choice of a symptom threshold also affects estimates of the
a retrospective study of adolescents suggest that, when ADHD persistence of ADHD into adulthood. For example, Fischer (1997)
persists into adolescence and adulthood, it is highly familial. followed 148 hyperactive children for 15 years. At a mean age
Taken together, these data suggest that, from a familial perspec- of 21, the self-report of these participants indicated that only 3%
five, not only is the adult ADHD diagnosis valid, but it might met criteria for ADHD as defined by the revised 3rd edition of the
actually be more valid than the childhood diagnosis. By more valid DSM (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987). But
we mean that a validity coefficient indexing the familial transmis- when they used a psychometric criterion to define ADHD (symp-
sion of ADHD would be greater for adults than children. This idea toms exceeding the 93rd percentile of severity of the control
makes a straightforward prediction: When selecting families group), 25% met criteria for ADHD. We reported similar findings
through ADHD children, the evidence for familial transmission from our longitudinal study of ADHD boys (Biederman, Mick, &
should be greater when examining the risk to adult relatives than Faraone, 2000). We found that by the age of 19, 38% of children
it is when examining the risk to non-adult relatives. Although this had the full ADHD diagnosis, 72% showed persistence of at least
prediction is straightforward, testing it is complicated by uncer- one-third of the symptoms required for the diagnosis, and 90%
tainties about the correct symptoms threshold for defining ADHD showed evidence of clinically significant impairment.
in adults. These considerations have led to the idea that ADHD be recast
Although extensive psychometric studies provided empirical as a norm-referenced rather than a criterion-referenced diagnosis
support for the symptom thresholds used to diagnose ADHD in (Barkley, 1998; Faraone, 2000). This idea suggests that one deal
children (Lahey et al., 1994), little is known about the validity of with the diagnosis of ADHD as one does the constructs of adaptive
these symptom thresholds for adult ADHD. Barkley (1998) sug- functioning or intelligence. For example, one would not have
gested that applying current ADHD criteria to adults is not devel- adults complete the Wechsler Intelligence Scales for Children and
opmentally sensitive. The fourth edition of the Diagnostic and then conclude that intelligence increases with age. Instead, one
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV; American Psy- uses different test batteries for different age groups; within a single
chiatric Association, 1994) recognizes developmental changes in battery, a score is considered high or low in reference to people of
the expression of ADHD in several ways. It cautions diagnosti- the same age.
cians that, with maturation, symptoms become less conspicuous. These uncertainties about the developmental course of ADHD
Older children may be restless and fidgety but not overly hyper- symptoms raise a key question about the decline of ADHD symp-
active. With age, inattention may predominate as school tasks toms over time: Does this decline reflect remission of the disorder,
require increasing levels of attention. The DSM also specifies that decreases in severity with age, or both? Or is this decline an
symptoms are considered present only if they are maladaptive and artifact of measurement because the symptoms used to define
inconsistent with developmental level. The DSM-IV also includes ADHD in children are not sensitive1 measures of ADHD in adult-
the category of ADHD in partial remission for individuals (espe- hood? Although the answers to these questions must await future
cially adolescents and adults) who currently have symptoms but no research, the issues they raise suggest that studies of adult ADHD
longer meet full criteria. examine the disorder in a manner that attends to the potential
The net effect of developmental changes is to make it more reduced sensitivity of the diagnosis in adulthood.
difficult for ADHD children to meet criteria for the disorder as In summary, family studies suggest not only that the adult
they get older. This situation was shown empirically by Murphy
ADHD diagnosis is valid, but also that it might actually be more
and Barkley (1996b), who used self-report rating scales to diag-
valid than the childhood diagnosis (i.e., ADHD that persists into
nose ADHD in 720 adults applying for or renewing their drivers adulthood is more familial than ADHD that remits in childhood).
licenses in the state of Massachusetts. In this population sample of
Our prior work has shown (a) children of clinically referred
adults, they found a systematic decrease in the prevalence of ADHD adults have very high rates of ADHD (Biederman et al.,
ADHD symptoms with age. Although this decrease may indicate
true remission of symptoms with age, it may also indicate a
measurement problem: reduced sensitivity of ADHD symptoms l In this context, sensitivity is the probability that the diagnosis of
with age. If the latter is true, then using the same symptom ADHD will correctly identify an ADHD adult as having ADHD.
832 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

1995), and (b) in both retrospective (Biederman et al., 1998) and childhood diagnoses. We collected data on nearly all the parents: 99% for
prospective (Biederman, Faraone, Milberger, et al., 1996; Faraone, control boys, 100% for boys with ADHD, 98% for control girls, and 98%
2000) studies, w h e n A D H D persists into adolescence and young for girls with ADHD. Assessments of parents were masked with regard to
adulthood, it is highly familial. proband diagnosis. All parents signed a written consent form prior to
participation in the study.
Our prior studies have addressed the implications of persistence
To be given a diagnosis of adult ADHD, the participant must have met
by looking at the relatives o f persistent and nonpersistent A D H D
the following criteria: (a) On the basis of their retrospective report, par-
probands. They have not, however, examined persistence in the ticipants must have met DSM-III-R criteria for a diagnosis of ADHD by
relatives. If our hypothesis about persistence is correct, then evi- the age of 7; (b) they must have had at least five DSM-III-R symptoms of
dence for familial transmission should be greater w h e n examining ADHD at the time of assessment; and (c) they must have described a
the risk to adult relatives (parents and adult siblings) than it is chronic course of ADHD symptoms from childhood to adulthood. To elicit
w h e n examining the risk to nonadult relatives (other siblings). ADHD symptoms, we used the ADHD module from the Kiddie- SADS-E,
Thus, this report sought to extend our prior work by testing this wording questions in the past tense. Thus, the symptoms used to assess
prediction. Moreover, given the uncertainties about h o w to diag- childhood ADHD and current adult ADHD were identical. We assessed
nose A D H D in adults, the present article sought to address this chronicity of symptoms by inquiring about the presence of symptoms
during childhood, adolescence, and adulthood.
issue in a manner that would deal with the effects o f varying
The five interviewers had undergraduate degrees in psychology; they
s y m p t o m thresholds. W e did so by using receiver operating char-
were trained to high levels of interrater reliability. The principal investi-
acteristic (ROC) analyses o f A D H D symptoms among relatives gator, Joseph Biederman supervised the interviewers throughout the study.
ascertained in our two previously reported family studies o f We computed kappa coefficients of agreement by having three experi-
A D H D (Biederman et al., 1999; Biederman et al., 1992). enced, board-certified child and adult psychiatrists diagnose participants
from audio-taped interviews made by the assessment staff. On the basis
Method of 61 interviews, all disorders achieved kappas higher than 0.82, with the
exception of alcohol abuse, which was 0.75. The mean kappa was 0.90.
Participants Kappas of 1.0 were obtained for ADHD (95% confidence interval: 0.8-
1.0), substance abuse (0.7-1.0), and substance dependence (0.7-1.0). Al-
We analyzed data from two family-genetic studies of ADHD that we though these kappa coefficients showed high interrater agreement, they did
have presented in previous publications (Biederman et al., 1999, 1992). not demonstrate the validity of the diagnoses. Full information regarding
Our family study of ADHD boys selected two groups of index male kappa coefficients is available on request.
participants: 140 ADHD probands and 120 boys without ADHD compar- A committee of four board-certified child and adult psychiatrists re-
isons. These groups had 454 and 368 first-degree biological relatives, solved all diagnostic uncertainties. The committee members were blind to
respectively. Our family study of ADHD girls studied two groups of index the participants' ascertainment group, ascertainment site, and all data
female participants: 140 ADHD probands and 122 girls who did not have collected from other family members. For children older than 12 years, the
ADHD. These groups had 417 and 369 first-degree biological relatives, diagnosticians combined data from interviews of children with data from
respectively, who provided data. interviews of mothers about their children by considering a diagnostic
All probands were between the ages of 6 and 17. Potential probands criterion positive if endorsed in either interview.
were excluded if they had been adopted or if their nuclear family was not
available for study. We excluded children if they had major sensorimotor
handicaps (e.g., paralysis, deafness, blindness), psychosis, autism, or an Statistical Analysis
estimated Full-Scale IQ score less than 80. Also, to minimize the potential
confounds of extreme social adversity, we excluded participants from the Our analyses sought to test one main hypothesis: When selecting fam-
lowest Hollingshead-Redlich socioeconomic class. Each of the ADHD ilies through ADHD children, the evidence for familial transmission should
probands met diagnostic criteria for current ADHD at the time of the be greater for the risk to adult relatives than for the risk to nonadult
clinical referral; at the time of recruitment, each had active symptoms of relatives. We used a combination of logistic regression and ROC analysis
the disorder. to provide a full examination of how varying symptom thresholds might
Two independent sources provided the proband children. We selected affect evidence for familial transmission. Before testing our study hypoth-
psychiatrically referred ADHD probands from consecutive referrals to the esis, we examined demographic and clinical features of the sample and
Pediatric Psychopharmacology Unit at the Massachusetts General Hospital determined if these were associated with either ADHD or age of relative.
(MGH). At MGH, we recruited normal controls from the Pediatric Ambu- These analyses used logistic regression (for binary outcomes) or linear
latory Service. The Harvard Community Health Plan provided both pedi- regression (for continuous outcomes).
atrically referred ADHD and control probands. We screened controls only To test our hypothesis, we estimated several sets of logistic regression
for the presence of ADHD. models separately for boy and girl proband families. Each model used
proband status (ADHD and controls) as the dependent variable and number
Procedure of ADHD symptoms in relatives as the independent variable. We let p
denote the probability that the relative comes from an ADHD family (i.e.,
Psychiatric assessments of participants and siblings relied on the Sched- a family ascertained through a participant with ADHD). Then, log[p/(1 -
ule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children: p)] is the logit ofp. Logistic regression models the logit as a linear function
Epidemiologic Version (Kiddie SADS-E; Orvaschel & Puig-Antich, 1987). of the number of ADHD symptoms in the adult and nonadult relatives. For
For the probands nonadult siblings, and adult siblings, we collected each model, the magnitude and significance of familial transmission was
maternal- and self-reports of symptoms, except for children younger assessed by the estimated coefficient for the symptom variable as a pre-
than 12 years of age, who were not directly interviewed. The assessment dictor of group status. This regression coefficient (/30 indicates the change
personnel were blind to participant diagnosis and ascertainment site. For in the logit associated with an increase of one in the relative's ADHD
parents, we collected self-reports of symptoms using the Structured Clin- symptom count. Exp(/3i) is the odds ratio, that is, the ratio of the odds that
ical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID; Spitzer, Williams, Gibbon, & First, a relative with N symptoms comes from an ADHD family compared with
1990), supplemented with modules from the Kiddie SADS-E covering a relative who has N - 1 symptoms. Thus, the odds ratio is an index of the
VALIDITY OF ASSESSING ADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 833

degree of familial transmission. An odds ratio of 1.0 signifies no familial disorders. Major depression and anxiety disorders were more prev-
transmission. As the odds ratio increases above 1.0, so does evidence for alent in the adult sample, and ADHD was more prevalent in the
the familial transmission of ADHD. We assessed the significance of/3 i child sample. This latter effect was seen for both the hyperactive-
(and hence of the odds ratio) by a z score ([Ji/SE[~]). impulsive and inattentive total symptom counts, whether measured
We tested two logistic regression models for current ADHD symptoms.
currently or in the past.
Model 1 was as follows: number of current ADHD symptoms for adult
Table 2 presents the same information for relatives of ADHD
relatives (age greater than 18) as a predictor of proband diagnosis. Model 2
was as follows: number of current ADHD symptoms for nonadult relatives girls. Relatives of ADHD probands were slightly younger than
as a predictor of proband diagnosis. Our hypothesis predicted that the odds relatives of controls, and the adult relatives came from a somewhat
ratios that index familial transmission should be greater for Model 1 lower social class than the child relatives. Relatives of ADHD girl
compared with Model 2. We tested this hypothesis using a likelihood ratio probands were at higher risk for ADHD, major depression, and
chi-square test. We also tested a parallel set of models for the relative's past anxiety disorders but not conduct disorder. ADHD was more
history of ADHD symptoms. Then, to explore the effects of ascertainment prevalent in the child compared with the adult sample of relatives.
site and psychiatric comorbidity, we repeated these analyses after stratify- These lower rates of ADHD were seen for both the hyperactive-
ing the participants on these variables. We also separately examined past
impulsive and inattentive total symptom counts whether measured
and current symptoms for the two main clusters of ADHD symptoms:
currently or in the past.
hyperactive-impulsive and inattentive.
For analyses of current symptoms, we excluded child or adult relatives
who were being treated for ADHD at the time of assessment. The numbers
(and percentages of total relatives) excluded were as follows: 33 child
Analyses of Current Symptoms
relatives of girl probands (15%), 16 adult relatives of girl probands
Table 3 provides a summary of the logistic regression analyses.
(3%), 10 child relatives of boy probands (4%), and 5 adult relatives of boy
probands (1%). For the analyses of current symptoms, the results were identical for
Because relatives from the same family are not statistically independent the relatives of boy and girl probands. Both showed statistically
from one another, standard logistic regression methods will produce inac- significant familial aggregation for adult relatives but not for
curate p values. To address this intrafamily clustering, we adjusted vari- nonadult relatives. Moreover, for both samples, the differences
ance estimates using Huber's (1967) formula as implemented in STATA between the adult and nonadult samples were statistically signifi-
(Stata Corporation, 1997). This formula is a theoretical bootstrap that cant: boys, X~(1, N = 776) = 10.9, p = .001; girls, X~(1, N =
produces robust statistical tests. The method works by entering the cluster 735) = 11.6, p < .001. The pattern of results can be seen in the
scores (i,e., sum of scores within families) into the formula for the estimate
odds ratios. These ratios indicate the increased odds of being a
of variance. The resulting p values are valid even when observations are
relative of an ADHD proband afforded by an increment of one in
not statistically independent of one another.
We used the logistic regression models to generate ROC curves as the level of the relative's ADHD symptoms count.
follows. Each model allows us to compute two statistics that assess the The implications of the logistic regression analyses can be seen
degree to which each possible number of ADHD symptoms in relatives in Figure 1. Panel A shows ROC curves separately for adult and
discriminates the relatives of ADHD probands from the relatives of control nonadult relatives of boy probands. If ADHD were not familial,
probands. These statistics are sensitivity and the false positive rate. In this the points on the ROC curve would fall on the diagonal line
context, sensitivity refers to the proportion of ADHD probands that have a connecting the [0, 0] and [1, 1] points. Points lying above the
relative with N or more symptoms. Thefalse positive rate is the proportion diagonal provide evidence for familial transmission.
of control probands that have a relative with N or more symptoms. We can
Each point on the graph indicates, for a different diagnostic
then compute a pair of sensitivity and false positive rate statistics for each
threshold, N, the proportion of ADHD probands that have a rela-
number of ADHD symptoms. To plot the ROC curve, all possible pairs of
sensitivity and false positive rate values are joined into one curve by tive with N or more symptoms (the vertical axis) and the propor-
plotting sensitivity on the vertical axis and the false positive rate on the tion of non-ADHD probands that have a relative with N or more
horizontal axis (McNeil & Hanley, 1984). symptoms (the horizontal axis). For example, the point labeled
If ADHD symptoms are familial, then the ROC curve should rise above "A" in Panel A of Figure 1 shows that 30% of ADHD probands
the diagonal line connecting the [0, 0] and [1, 1] points of the ROC graph. have an adult relative with two or more current symptoms. In
If, as our hypothesis predicts, symptoms in adult relatives are more familial contrast, only 10% of control probands have an adult relative with
than symptoms in child relatives, then the ROC curve for adults should rise two or more current symptoms. If we move down the ROC curve
above the curve for children. That is, the area under the adult relative's
toward the [0, 0] point, the next point we encounter tells us that
curve should be greater than the area under the child relative's curve. We
21% of ADHD probands, but only 6% of controls, have an adult
tested this difference using the method of Hanley and McNeil (1983).
relative with three or more current symptoms. The point labeled
"B" shows that 30% of ADHD and 25% of control probands have
Results a nonadult relative with two or more current symptoms. Moving
down the ROC curve toward the [0, 0] point, the next point we
Clinical and Demographic Data
encounter tells us that 23% of ADHD probands and 19% of
Table 1 presents the clinical and demographic features of the controls have a nonadult relative with three or more current
adult and child relatives of ADHD boys. It shows that, compared symptoms.
with control probands the relatives of ADHD probands came from Consistent with the logistic regression analyses, the area under
lower social classes and less intact families. Adult relatives did not the adult ROC curve is greater than the area under the nonadult
differ from child relatives on these demographic features. Table 1 curve, indicating that ADHD is more familial in the adult com-
also shows that the relatives of ADHD probands were at higher pared with the nonadult relatives (z = 3.0, p = .001). The data
risk for ADHD, conduct disorder, major depression, and anxiety from the sample of families selected through girl probands show
834 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

r--,, . . . . o.o, ..
oc~
II II II II II II .~
~1, ,," .o.~.~.
~.. ~.. II II II II

I I I I I I
II I I ,-..;<-ie,ie,i II II II II II II
~" II II II II II II
t~l t"l t'-I C',I t"q I"1 ~

8 i
~,
. Jl
• II
~, II
. . . .
II II V
vvvvvv ~?
',,ID (",1 ~ r~ t",l oo ~

H IJ II II II II "~-~
I~1 II II II II II Jl
,.., ,.., ,.., ,.., .~ ,.., ~'~

.=
H

<

~.~

N~
..~ II

4
II

<

r..) £.'~1 it~ £E"d

~E

,-.:..q

,:::1 ;>
VALIDITY OF ASSESSING ADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 835

V II II II V II
II II • • • II
~ ~ "~ ~ :

IIII ~ 1 II I, fl I, fl II ~*
~ II II II II II II ~ ~ ~.~

~ II . . . .
~ II ~ II II II V
I ~ ~
I1~1 --~m~
II II II II II II N

H
~ ~ .~
<
t~

Q
i 0=
.<
&
~a

¢D m~

.~
II

~D
<

~a
i ~ 0 0 0

.~ II

co
'-C

.,<

o o
:~°
°~
~'~ ~ ~ *- ~.

~ ..
~a

~ . ~ o ~.=.=
~ = ' = ~ ~
836 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

Table 3
Results of Logistic Regression Models by Gender of Proband
Description of model Statistical results

Sample Source of symptoms Type of relative Odds ratio z p

Boys Current Adult 1.4 5.2 <.001


Current Nonadult 1.1 1.6 .12
Past Adult 1.3 6.3 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 2.9 .003
Girls Current Adult 1.3 6.0 <.001
Current Nonadult 1.0 0.4 .68
Past Adult 1.3 7.7 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 2.7 .008

the same pattern of results (see Panel B of Figure 1; z = 3.4, p < nonadult relatives, both clusters of symptoms showed statistically
.001). significant familial aggregation for both adult and nonadult rela-
tives. The odds ratios show that, for both types of symptoms, the
Analyses of Past Symptoms odds ratios were greater for adult compared with nonadult rela-
tives. These differences between the adult and nonadult samples
For the analyses of past symptoms, Table 3 shows that the were statistically significant: current inattentive symptoms, )d~(1,
results for the boys and girls samples were very similar. Both N = 1,510) = 21.5, p < .001; past inattentive symptoms, X~(1,
showed statistically significant familial aggregation for both adult N = 1,575) = 18.7, p < .001; current hyperactive-impulsive
and nonadult relatives. Notable was that the odds ratios show that symptoms, X~(1, N = 1,511) = 22.9, p < .001; past hyperactive-
the direction of effect was similar for boys and girls. Both showed impulsive symptoms, X~I(I, N = 1,575) = 18.1, p < .001.
greater odds ratios for adult compared with nonadult relatives. Table 6 shows the results stratified by psychiatric comorbidity.
These differences between the adult and nonadult samples were For the analyses labeled "No Diagnosis," we included only rela-
statistically significant: boys, X~(1, N = 791) = 5.3, p = .021; tives who did not have any of the following disorders: conduct
girls, X~l(1, N = 784) = l l . 4 , p < .001. disorder, major depressive disorder with severe impairment, mul-
Figure 2 shows the ROC curves for past symptoms. As we saw tiple anxiety disorder, or antisocial personality disorder. Relatives
for current symptoms, the results from the boys and girls samples with one or more of these diagnoses were included in the analyses
are similar. The areas under both adult ROC curves are greater labeled "At Least One Diagnosis." These analyses combined rel-
than the areas under the corresponding nonadult curves, indicating atives of boy and girl probands. With the exception of current
that ADHD is more familial in the adult compared with the symptoms for nonadult relatives, both clusters of symptoms
nonadult relatives (boys: z = 2.2, p = .014; girls: z = 3.1, p < showed statistically significant familial aggregation for both adult
.001). and nonadult relatives. The odds ratios show that the direction of
effect was similar for both the comorbid and non-comorbid groups.
Analyses of Ascertainment Source, Symptom Clusters, and Both show greater odds ratios for adult compared with nonadult
Psychiatric Comorbidity relatives. These differences between the adult and nonadult sam-
ples were statistically significant--no diagnoses, current symp-
Table 4 shows that the results were very similar when we
toms, X~I(1, N = 1,090) = 13.6, p < .001; no diagnoses, past
stratified the samples by ascertainment source using analyses that
symptoms, X~I(1, N = 1,120) = 15.6, p < .001; and one or more
combined relatives of boy and girl probands. With the exception of
diagnoses, current symptoms, X~l(1, N = 386) = 5.9, p = . 0 1 5 -
current symptoms for nonadult relatives, both clusters of symp-
with the exception of the case of past symptoms for relatives
toms showed statistically significant familial aggregation for both
having one or more diagnosis, X~(1, N = 418) = 2.5, p = 0.114.
adult and nonadult relatives. The odds ratios show that the direc-
tion of effect was similar for both ascertainment sites. Both show
greater odds ratios for adult compared with nonadult relatives. Analyses of Reporter Bias Effects
These differences between the adult and nonadult samples were
statistically significant: M G H current symptoms, X~(1, N = Because adult relatives of ADHD children may be aware of the
653) = 24.4, p < .001; M G H past symptoms, X~(1, N = ADHD symptoms in their proband children, they may be biased to
700) = 14.1, p < .001; health maintenance organization (HMO) report ADHD symptoms in themselves. If so, our finding of
current symptoms, X~I(1, N = 858) = 6.2, p = .013; HMO past greater familiality in adult relatives might be due to this reporter
symptoms, X~I(1, N = 875) = 10.1, p = .002. bias effect. This type of reporter bias makes a testable prediction:
Table 5 shows that the results were very similar when inatten- The adult relatives of ADHD children should have a greater
tive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms were considered sepa- number of symptoms than the nonadult relatives. Inspection of
rately. The Appendix shows how these clusters were defined with Tables 1 and 2 shows that this prediction does not hold. For both
DSM-III-R symptoms. These analyses combine relatives of boy the boys and girls samples, the mean number of symptoms re-
and girl probands. With the exception of current symptoms for ported by adult relatives of ADHD probands was nonsignificantly
VALIDITY OF ASSESSING ADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 837

IA: Current Symptoms Among Relatives of Boy ADHD and Control Probands
o denotes adult relatives, A denotes child relatives
1-

,9-

,8

.7-

.6-

.5-
.4-
.3-

.2"

.1-

O-
o' '1 .'2 ~3 14 .
'5 '.6 .; .'8 ~9 ;
False Positive Rate
IB: Current Symptoms Among Relatives of Girl ADHD and Control Probands
o denotes adult relatives, A denotes child relatives
1

,9

.8-

.7-

.5-

.4-
r~
.3"

.2-

.I"

0-
i |
~) •I1 .2' .3= .4 .5 •'6 ,'7 .I8 ~9 1
False Positive Rate

Figure 1. Receiver operating characteristic curves for current ADHD symptoms among relatives of boy and
girl ADHD participants. ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

less than the mean number of symptoms reported by nonadult The pattern of results supporting this hypothesis was seen for
relatives. This was true for current and past symptoms and for both current and past assessments of total ADHD symptoms and
inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (all ps > .05). for inattentive and hyperactive-impulsive symptoms considered
separately. Moreover, our results cannot be accounted for by
Discussion gender, psychiatric comorbidity, or ascertainment source because
we found the same pattern of results for the subgroups defined by
Our results support the study hypothesis: When selecting fam-
these variables.
ilies through ADHD children, evidence for familial transmission is
We also considered the possibility that ADHD in children biases
greater when examining ADHD symptoms in adult relatives than
the self-reports of ADHD in their adult relatives. The adult rela-
it is when examining symptoms in nonadult relatives. Thus, our
tives of ADHD children are usually aware of the ADHD symptoms
results provide further evidence for the validity of adult ADHD
and continue to support the intriguing idea that, from a familial in the child. That knowledge may bias them to report ADHD
perspective, the diagnosis of ADHD is more valid in adults than it symptoms in themselves. If that occurs, then the rates of ADHD
is in children. among adult relatives of ADHD children would be spuriously
838 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

2A: Past Symptoms Amon~ Relatives of Boy ADHD and Control Probands
o denotes adult relatives, A denotes child relatives
1

.8

.8-

.7

~ .4-
r~
.3-

.2"

.1

0
!
I . .
0 '1 2 '3 ,I 5
I 16
7
= .i9
1'
False Positive Rate
2B: Past Symptoms Among Relatives of Girl ADHD and Control Probands
o denotes adult relatives, A denotes child relatives
1 -

.9-

.8-

.7-

.~ .5-

.4-

.3-

.2 ~

,I-

0-
¢
.
0 . .
.1 . .
.2 . .3 . .4 .5
False Positive Rate
. . '7. . '8 ,, 1,

Figure 2. Receiver operating characteristic curves for past ADHD symptoms among relatives of boy and girl
ADHD participants. ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.

high, leading to the incorrect conclusion that adult ADHD is more toms in control relatives (Faraone et al., 1999). Our finding that
familial than child ADHD. But if ADHD adults are biased to ADHD is more familial among adult relatives is due to symptom
overreport symptoms, then the adult relatives of ADHD children differences between child and adult relatives of controls. Because
should have had a greater number of symptoms than the child the adult relatives of controls had fewer symptoms than the child
relatives. That was not the case. In fact, the adult relatives tended relatives of controls, the ADHD versus control comparison is
to report fewer symptoms, although the difference was not statis- greater for adult relatives.
tically significant. This feature of our data can be seen by inspecting the ROC
We have made two statements that appear contradictory but are curves. Recall that each successive point on the ROC shows the
not: (a) Adult ADHD symptoms are more familial than child sensitivity and false positive rate for successive ADHD symptom
ADHD symptoms, and (b) the number of ADHD symptoms does thresholds. For example, the point labeled "A" in Panel A of
not differ between adult and child relatives. The statements do not Figure 1 shows that 30% of ADHD probands and 10% of control
contradict one another because any index of familial transmission probands have an adult relative with two or more current symp-
must measure differences between two quantities: the number of toms. Compare this point with the point labeled "B," which shows
ADHD symptoms in ADHD relatives and the number of symp- that 30% of ADHD and 25% of control probands have a nonadult
VALIDITY OF ASSESSING ADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 839

Table 4
Results of Logistic Regression Models by Ascertainment Source

Description of model Statistical results

Source Source of symptoms Type of relative Odds ratio z

HMO Current Adult 1.2 5.2 <.001


Current Nonadult 1.1 1.2 .231
Past Adult 1.2 6.7 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 1.4 .153
MGH Current Adult 1.4 5.6 <.001
Current Nonadult 1.0 0.3 .735
Past Adult 1.3 6.9 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 3.4 .001

Note. HMO = health maintenance organization; MGH = Massachusetts General Hospital.

relative with two or more current symptoms. These points tell us consistent with our prior report from a different sample. In that
that, for the identical symptom threshold used to define ADHD study, we compared symptom rates between 26 clinically referred
(two or more), the sensitivity for adult relatives is identical to that ADHD adults who had ADHD children and 49 clinically referred
for child relatives. In contrast, the false positive rate for child ADHD adults who did not have ADHD children (Faraone, Bied-
relatives is more than twofold that of adult relatives. erman, & Mick, 1997). We hypothesized the following: If having
If we move down the ROC curve toward the [0, 0] point, the an ADHD child biased an adult to report ADHD symptoms, then
next point we encounter on the adult relative curve tells us that ADHD adults having ADHD children should have reported more
21% of ADHD probands but only 6% of controls, have an adult symptoms than ADHD adults who did not have ADHD children.
relative with three or more current symptoms. Moving down the We rejected that hypothesis by showing that the number of symp-
child ROC curve, we see that 23% of ADHD probands and 19% of toms reported by ADHD adults did not differ between those who
controls have a nonadult relative with three or more current symp- did and did not have ADHD children. Moreover, no individual
toms. Thus, at the threshold of three or more symptoms, child and symptom was more frequent among the ADHD adults who had
adult relatives have a similar sensitivity. But, as we saw for points ADHD children compared with those who did not have ADHD
A and B, the child relatives have a greater false positive rate than children. Thus, having an ADHD child did not bias ADHD adults
the adult relatives. to overreport ADHD symptoms.
The false positive rate for each symptom threshold is the pro- Our results also address the idea that ADHD should be recast as
portion of control probands that have a relative whose symptoms a norm-referenced rather than a criterion-referenced diagnosis
exceed the threshold. Thus, from a familial perspective, we have (Barkley, 1998; Faraone, 2000). As we reviewed in the introduc-
shown that the diagnosis of ADHD yields more false positives in tion, some have argued that the symptom threshold for diagnosing
nonadult than adult relatives. This idea is counterintuitive given adult ADHD should be lower than that for diagnosing ADHD in
contemporary skepticism about the diagnosis of adult ADHD youths. The ROC curves addressed this issue because if a symptom
(Shaffer, 1994). If the self-report of adult ADHD is heavily influ- threshold that defines a specific point also defines a familial form
enced by recall biases and reports in the media, we should have of ADHD, then that point should lie above the diagonal line
found it to be less valid and more prone to false positive reports connecting the [0, 0] and [1, 1] points. All of the four curves for
than the diagnosis of ADHD among youths. adult relatives notably show the same finding: All of the points on
Although our data cannot rule out the possibility of some biased the ROC lie above the diagonal. Thus, from a familial perspective,
reports of ADHD in adults, our evidence against reporter bias is any of these points could be used as a symptom threshold to define

Table 5
Results of Logistic Regression Models for ADHD Symptom Clusters

Description of model Statistical results

ADHD symptom cluster Source of symptoms Type of relative Odds ratio z

haattentive Current Adult 1.5 6.9 <.001


Current Nonadult 1.1 1.2 .241
Past Adult 1.4 9.3 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 2.9 .003
Hyperactive/Impulsive Current Adult 1.5 6.9 <.001
Current Nonadult 1.1 1.0 .318
Past Adult 1.5 9.2 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.2 3.7 <.001

Note. ADHD = attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.


840 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

Table 6
Results of Logistic Regression Models Stratified by Psychiatric Comorbidity

Description of model Statistical results

Diagnostic statusa Source of symptoms Type of relative Odds ratio z p

No diagnoses Current Adult 1.3 5.1 <.001


Current Nonadult 1.0 1.0 .332
Past Adult 1.3 7.2 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 2. l .038
At least one diagnosis Current Adult 1.2 4.0 <.001
Current Nonadult 1.0 0.0 .974
Past Adult 1.2 5.2 <.001
Past Nonadult 1.1 2.0 .047

aRefers to the presence of at least one of the following: conduct disorder, major depressive disorder with severe
impairment, multiple anxiety disorder, or antisocial personality disorder.

adult ADHD. We do not suggest that clinical symptom thresholds viewed as the extreme expression of a trait that varies quantita-
be chosen soley on familial data. Studies addressing the clinical tively in the population, clinicians need ADHD symptom thresh-
significance of specific thresholds, in the context of appropriate olds for clinical purposes as much as blood pressure thresholds are
normative data, are needed. needed to define hypertension. Our data cannot provide the opti-
This finding is in line with the idea that we should view ADHD mal symptom threshold for adult ADHD, but they do highlight the
as a dimensional trait, not a discrete category. There are several need for future research to examine this issue in more detail.
lines of evidence that suggest strongly that a dimensional perspec- Our results must be considered in the context of some method-
tive on ADHD is valid. First, many studies have found an excellent ologic limitations. We do not know to what degree our findings
correspondence between dimensional measures of ADHD (e.g., will generalize to nonreferred ADHD children in the community,
scales derived from the Child Behavior Checklist, the Conners and although raters were blind to the diagnosis of the probands
Scales, and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire) and the parents were not. Moreover, the retrospective assessments of
categorical diagnosis of ADHD (Biederman et al., 1993; Bieder- childhood-onset ADHD and the assessments of current ADHD
man, Faraone, Mick, Moore, & Lelon, 1996; Bird, et al., 1987; symptoms were made by the same interviewer. Thus, it is possible
Boyle et al., 1997; Chen, Faraone, Biederman, & Tsuang, 1994; that the interviewer's knowledge of the adult's childhood symp-
Edelbrock, 1986; Hudziak, 1997). These studies suggest that chil- toms might have contaminated the interviewer's ratings of the
dren with ADHD are at one extreme of a quantitative dimension, adult's current symptoms. This possibility cannot account for the
and that on this quantitative dimension, there is no obvious bimo- differences we observed between child and adult relatives, but it
dality that separates ADHD children from non-ADHD children. might have led to an overestimate of current ADHD symptoms in
Further support for the validity of a quantitative approach to adults who reported childhood symptoms.
ADHD is the fact that quantitative measures of ADHD are highly Another limitation of our work is that we did not collect parental
heritable (about 70%) and as heritable as the ADHD diagnosis reports or school records to confirm our retrospective diagnoses of
(Edelbrock, Rende, Plomin, & Thompson, 1995; Goodman & ADHD. It is possible that troubled adults who have been influ-
Stevenson, 1989a, 1989b; Judy et al., 1997; Sherman, Iacono, & enced by media reports of ADHD may recognize ADHD symp-
McGue, 1997; Silberg et al., 1996; Stevenson, 1992; Thapar, toms in themselves, and this might bias their recall of childhood
Hervas, & McGuffin, 1995). symptoms. One safeguard against such bias was our use of a
More important, twin studies have used mathematical modeling control group, because such biases should have equally affected
techniques to test directly the hypothesis that the clinical diagnosis the adults from ADHD and control families. Nevertheless, future
of ADHD is the extreme of a quantitative trait. Gjone, Stevenson, studies of adult ADHD would further strengthen the knowledge
and Sundet (1996) applied a mathematical model to determine if base about adult ADHD by collecting informant reports or school
the heritability of attention problems increased with their severity. records of childhood symptoms.
If ADHD accounted for the heritability of the broader dimension Despite these limitations, our two family studies of ADHD boys
of attention problems, then heritability should increase with in- and girls provide further support for the hypothesis that ADHD is
creasing severity. However, Gjone et al. found that heritability did more familial in adult samples. This finding shores up the validity
not change with severity. They concluded that there was in the of retrospective diagnoses of adult ADHD and suggests that,
population a continuously distributed dimension of genetic liabil- compared with the childhood diagnosis, the adult diagnosis may be
ity to attention problems. A similar approach was applied by Levy more valid and thus more informative for studies of the disorder.
et al. (1997). Like Gjone et al., they concluded that, with regard to
the genetic components of its etiology, ADHD was best viewed as
References
the extreme of a behavior that varies genetically throughout the
entire population rather than as a categorical disorder. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical man-
Although available data support the idea that ADHD can be ual of mental disorders (3rd ed., rev.). Washington, DC: Author.
VALIDITY OF ASSESSING ADULT ADHD SYMPTOMS 841

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man- Implications for theories of diagnosis. Current Directions in Psycholog-
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiat- ical Science, 9, 33-36.
ric Association. Faraone, S., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (1997). Symptom reports by adults
Barkley, R. (1997). Age-dependent decline in ADHD: True recovery or with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Are they influenced by
statistical illusion? The ADHD Report, 5, 1-5. attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in their children? Journal of
Barkley, R. A. (1998). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder." A handbook Nervous and Mental Diseases, 185, 583-584.
for diagnosis and treatment. New York: Guilford Press. Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Monuteaux, M. C. (2000). Toward
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Doyle, A., Krifcher Lehman, B., Kraus, I., guidelines for pedigree selection in genetic studies of attention deficit
Perrin, J., & Tsuang, M. T. (1993). Convergence of the Child Behavior hyperactivity disorder. Genetic Epidemiology, 18, 1-16.
Checklist with structured interview-based psychiatric diagnoses of Faraone, S. V., Tsuang, D., & Tsuang, M. T. (1999). Genetics and mental
ADHD children with and without hyperactivity. Journal of Child Psy- disorders: A guide for students, clinicians, and researchers. New York:
chology and Psychiatry, 34, 1241-1251. Guilford Press.
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Keenan, K., Benjamin, J., Krifcher, B., Faraone, S. V., & Tsuang, M. T. (1995). Methods in psychiatric genetics.
Moore, C., Sprich-Buckminster, S., Ugaglia, K., Jellinek, M. S., Stein- In M. Tohen, M. T. Tsuang, & G. E. P. Zahner (Eds.), Textbook in
gard, R., Spencer, T., Norman, D., Kolodny, R., Kraus, I., Perrin, J., psychiatric epidemiology (pp. 81-134). New York: Wiley.
Keller, M. B., & Tsuang, M. T. (1992). Further evidence for family- Fischer, M. (1997). The persistence of ADHD into adulthood: It depends
genetic risk factors in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Patterns of on whom you ask. The ADHD Report, 5, 8-10.
comorbidity in probands and relatives psychiatrically and pediatrically Gjone, H., Stevenson, J., & Sundet, J. (1996). Genetic influence on
referred samples. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49, 728-38. parent-reported attention-related problems in a Norwegian general pop-
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Mick, E., Moore, P., & Lelon, E. (1996). ulation twin sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Child behavior checklist findings further support comorbidity between Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 588-598.
ADHD and major depression in a referred sample. Journal of the Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989a). A twin study of hyperactivity: I. An
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 734 -742. examination of hyperactivity scores and categories derived from Rutter
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Keily, K., teacher and parent questionnaires. Journal of Child Psychology and
Guite, J., Ablon, S., Reed, E. D., & Warburton, R. (1995). High risk for Psychiatry, 30, 671-689.
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children of parents with
Goodman, R., & Stevenson, J. (1989b). A twin study of hyperactivity: II.
childhood onset of the disorder: A pilot study. American Journal of
The aetiological role of genes, family relationships and perinatal adver-
Psychiatry, 152, 431-435.
sity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 691-709.
Biederman, J., Faraoue, S., Mick, E., Williamson, S., Wilens, T., Spencer,
Hanley, J. A., & McNeil, B. J. (1983). A method of comparing the areas
T., Weber, W., Jetton, J., Kraus, I., Pert, J., & Zallen, B. (1999). Clinical
under receiver operating characteristic curves derived from the same
correlates of ADHD in females: Findings from a large group of pediat-
cases. Radiology, 148, 839-843.
ricaily and psychiatrically referred girls. Journal of the American Acad-
Heiligenstein, E., Conyers, L. M., Berns, A. R., Miller, M. A., & Smith,
emy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 966-975.
M. A. (1998). Preliminary normative data on DSM-IV attention deficit
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Milberger, S., Curtis, S., Chen, L., Marts,
hyperactivity disorder in college students. Journal of American College
A., Ouellette, C., Moore, P., & Spencer, T. (1996). Predictors of per-
Health, 46, 185-188. (Published erratum appears in Journal of American
sistence and remission of ADHD: Results from a four-year prospective
College Health, 46, 213)
follow-up study of ADHD children. Journal of the American Academy
Heiligenstein, E., Guenther, G., Levy, A., Savino, F., & Fulwiler, J. (1999).
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 35, 343-351.
Psychological and academic functioning in college students with atten-
Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Taylor, A., Sienna, M., Williamson, S., &
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of American College
Fine, C. (1998). Diagnostic continuity between child and adolescent
ADHD: Findings from a longitudinal clinical sample. Journal of the Health, 47, 181-185.
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 37, 305-313. Heiligenstein, E., & Keeling, R. P. (1995). Presentation of unrecognized
Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in college students. Journal of
of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition American College Health, 43, 226-228.
and symptom subtype. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 816-818. Hill, J., & Schoener, E. (1996). Age-dependent decline of attention deficit
Bird, H., Canino, G., Gould, M., Ribera, J., Rubio-Stipec, M., Woodbury, hyperactivity disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 1143-
M., Huertas-Goldman, H., & Sesman, M. (1987). Use of the Child 1146.
Behavior Checklist as a screening instrument for epidemiological re- Huber, P. J. (1967). The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under
search in child psychiatry: Results of a pilot study. Journal of the non-standard conditions. Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 26, 207-213. on Mathematical Statistics and Probability, 1, 221-233.
Boyle, M., Offord, D., Racine, Y., Szatmari, P., Sanford, M., & Fleming, Hudziak, J. J. (1997). Identification of phenotypes for molecular genetic
J. (1997). Adequacy of interviews vs checklists for classifying childhood studies of common childhood psychopathology. In K. Blum & E. P.
psychiatric disorder based on parent reports. Archives of General Psy- Noble (Eds.), Handbook of psychiatric genetics (pp. 201-217). Boca
chiatry, 54, 793-799. Raton, FL: CRC Press, Inc.
Chen, W., Faraone, S., Biederman, J., & Tsuang, M. (1994). Diagnostic Judy, L., Silberg, J., Meyer, J., Maes, H., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Rutter,
accuracy of the Child Behavior Checklist scales for attention deficit M., Reynolds, C., Heath, A., Truett, K., Meale, M., Erikson, M., Loeber,
hyperactivity disorder: A receiver-operating characteristic analysis. R., & Hewitt, J. (1997). Genetics and developmental psychopathology:
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1017-1025. II. The main effects of genes and environment of behavioral problems in
Edelbrock, C. (1986). Behavioral ratings of children diagnosed for atten- the Virginia Twin study of Adolescent behavioral development. Journal
tion deficit disorder. Psychiatric Annals, 16, 36-40. of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Discipline, 38, 965-980.
Edelbrock, C., Rende, R., Plomin, R., & Thompson, L. A. (1995). A twin Lahey, B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L.,
study of competence and problem behavior in childhood and early Hynd, G., Barkley, R., Newcorn, J., Jensen, P., Richters, J., Garfinkel,
adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 775-785. B., Kerdyk, L., Frick, P., Ollendick, T., Perez, D., Hart, E., Waldman, I.,
Faraone, S. V. (2000). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: & Shaffer, D. (1994). DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperac-
842 FARAONE, BIEDERMAN, FEIGHNER, AND MONUTEAUX

tivity disorder in children and adolescents. American Journal of Psychi- Sherman, D., Iacono, W., & McGue, M. (1997). Attention deficit hyper-
atry, 151, 1673-1685. activity disorder dimensions: A twin study of inattention and impulsivity
Levy, F., Hay, D., McStephen, M., Wood, C., & Waldman, I. (1997). hyperactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adoles-
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A category or a continuum? cent Psychiatry, 36, 745-753.
Genetic analysis of a large-scale twin study. Journal of the American Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Meyer, J., Maes, H., Hewitt, J., Simonoff, E.,
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 737-744. Pickles, A., & Loeber, R. (1996). Genetic and environmental influences
Manshadi, M., Lippmann, S., O'Daniel, R., & Blackman, A. (1983). on the covariation between hyperactivity and conduct disturbance in
Alcohol abuse and attention deficit disorder. Journal of Clinical Psy- juvenile twins. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 803-
chiatry, 44, 379-380. 816.
McNeil, B. J., & Hanley, J. A. (1984). Statistical approaches to the analysis Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Wilens, T., & Faraone, S. (1994). Is attention
of receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves. Medical Decision deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults a valid disorder? Harvard Review
Making, 4, 137-150. of Psychiatry, 1, 326-335.
Murphy, K., & Barkley, R. (1996a). Prevalence of DSM-IV symptoms of Spencer, T., Biederman, J., Wilens, T. E., & Faraone, S. V. (1998). Adults
ADHD in adult licensed drivers: Implications for clinical diagnosis. with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a controversial diagnosis.
Journal of Attention Disorders, 1, 147-161.
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 59, 59-68.
Murphy, K., & Barkley, R. A. (1996b). Attention deficit hyperactivity
Spitzer, R. L., Williams, J. B., Gibbon, M., & First, M. B. (1990).
disorder adults: comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Comprehen-
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R: Non-Patient Edition
sive Psychiatry, 37, 393-401.
(SCID-NP, Version 1.0). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
Orvaschel, H., & Puig-Antich, J. (1987). Schedule for Affective Disorders
and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children: Epidemiologic 4th Version. Press.
Fort Lauderdale, FL: Nova University, Center for Psychological Stata Corporation (1997). Stata reference manual: Release 5. College
Study. Station, TX: Author.
Robins, E., & Guze, S. B. (1970). Establishment of diagnostic validity in Stevenson, J. (1992). Evidence for a genetic etiology in hyperactivity in
psychiatric illness: Its application to schizophrenia. American Journal of children. Behavior Genetics, 22, 337-344.
Psychiatry, 126, 983-987. Thapar, A., Hervas, A., & McGuffin, P. (1995). Childhood hyperactivity
Shaffer, D. (1994). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults. Amer- scores are highly heritable and show sibling competition effects: Twin
ican Journal of Psychiatry, 151, 633-638. study evidence. Behavior Genetics, 25, 537-544.

Appendix

DSM-III-R ADHD Symptom Clusters

Inattentive symptoms Hyperactive/impulsive symptoms

Shifts activities Difficulty remaining seated


Difficulty sustaining attention Is fidgety
Difficulty following instructions Has difficulty playing quietly
Easily distracted Talks excessively
Loses things Interrupts
Does not listen Blurts out
Difficulty waiting turn
Acts before thinking

Note. DSM-III-R = Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., rev.); ADHD = attention
deficit and hyperactivity disorder.

Received May 24, 1999


Revision received January 24, 2000
Accepted January 31, 2000 •

You might also like