You are on page 1of 9

Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87

www.elsevier.com/locate/b&c

Central and autonomic nervous system integration in emotion


Dirk Hagemann,a,b,* Shari R. Waldstein,b,c and Julian F. Thayera
a
Laboratory of Personality and Cognition, National Institute on Aging, National Institutes of Health, Gerontology Research Center,
5600 Nathan Shock Drive, Baltimore, MD 21224, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, Baltimore County, 1000 Hilltop Circle, Baltimore, MD 21250, USA
c
Division of Gerontology, Department of Medicine, University of Maryland School of Medicine and Geriatrics Research Education and Clinical Center,
Baltimore Veterans Affairs Medical Center, 22 South Greene Street, Baltimore, MD 21201, USA

Accepted 26 September 2002

Abstract

Emotions involve physiological responses that are regulated by the brain. The present paper reviews the empirical literature on
central nervous system (CNS) and autonomic nervous system (ANS) concomitants of emotional states, with a focus on studies that
simultaneously assessed CNS and ANS activity. The reviewed data support two primary conclusions: (1) numerous cortical and
subcortical regions show co-occurring activity with ANS responses in emotion, and (2) there may be reversed asymmetries on
cortical and subcortical levels with respect to CNS/ANS interrelations. These observations are interpreted in terms of a model of
neurovisceral integration in emotion, and directions for future research are presented.
Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.

Keywords: Autonomic nervous system; Central nervous system; Dynamical models; Emotion; Hemispheric lateralization; Inhibitory control

1. Introduction (ANS) correlates of emotion. Although several investi-


gations have noted emotion-specific autonomic response
Emotions involve a complex mix of cognitive, affec- patterns (e.g., Ekman, Levenson, & Friesen, 1983; Sin-
tive, behavioral, and physiological responses (e.g., Bir- ha, Lovallo, & Parsons, 1992), the majority of studies
baumer & O € hman, 1993; Oatley & Jenkins, 1996). At a suggest greater similarities than differences among
physiological level of analysis, much current research in physiological activation patterns during various emo-
affective neuroscience seeks to elucidate the neural net- tional states (for reviews see Cacioppo, Berntson, Lar-
works that underlie emotion. In this regard, results of sen, Poehlmann, & Ito, 2000; Neumann & Waldstein,
numerous investigations of the central nervous system 2001; Stemmler, 1996).
(CNS) concomitants of emotion suggest the involvement Study of the linkage between central and autonomic
of multiple cortical (e.g., frontal, temporal, and parietal) correlates of emotion is relevant to numerous fields of
and subcortical (e.g., basal ganglia, thalamus, amygdala, investigation including affective neuroscience, neurocar-
and hippocampus) regions across a variety of positive diology, psychophysiology, and behavioral medicine.
and negative emotions such as happiness, anxiety, anger, Yet, relatively few studies have simultaneously examined
sadness, disgust (for reviews, see Borod, 2000; Lane & CNS and ANS responses during emotion. In this paper,
Nadel, 2000). we provide an overview of such investigations. Previ-
A separate body of psychophysiological research has ously, it has been suggested that the right hemisphere
sought to identify patterns of autonomic nervous system may be dominant in eliciting autonomic responses during
emotion (e.g., Borod & Madigan, 2000; Gainotti, 1989;
* Wittling & Roschmann, 1993). However, others have
Corresponding author. Present address: Fachbereich I—Psychol-
ogie, Universit€
at Trier, Universit€atsring 15, Trier 54286, Germany.
proposed more complex models of association between
Fax: +49-651-201-3956. emotion-related central and autonomic response patterns
E-mail address: hagemann@uni-trier.de (D. Hagemann). (Lane & Jennings, 1995; Lane & Schwartz, 1987; Thayer

0278-2626/03/$ - see front matter Ó 2003 Elsevier Science (USA). All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00011-3
80 D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87

& Lane, 2000); many of these models focus on both tween pleasant and unpleasant states. For example, a
cortical and subcortical interconnections (such as fron- variety of discrete emotions such as happiness, sadness,
tal–subcortical systems). We present results from the anger, fear, and disgust have been elicited with proce-
literature on concomitant CNS and ANS response to dures such as presentation of film clips (Lane, Reiman,
emotional stimuli to suggest the viability of the latter Ahern, & Thayer, 2000), presentation of pictures of re-
position. We discuss findings from lesion studies, visual spective facial expressions (Schneider et al., 1995), recall
half field studies, and investigations that have directly and re-experience of personal life episodes (Waldstein
measured CNS activity with the spontaneous electroen- et al., 2000), anticipation of electric shock (Slomine,
cephalogram (EEG), evoked potentials (EPs), or neu- Bowers, & Heilman, 1999), or hypnotic induction (de
roimaging techniques, along with measures of ANS Pascalis, Ray, Tranquillo, & DÕAmico, 1998). In con-
activation. We also highlight a recent model of neuro- trast, pleasant and unpleasant emotions have typically
visceral integration (Thayer & Lane, 2000) to enhance the been induced by the presentation of affective slides (e.g.,
findings of the present review. This model relies on Cuthbert, Schupp, Bradley, Birbaumer, & Lang, 2000;
principles of dynamical systems to suggest that different Lane et al., 1997; Palomba, Angrilli, & Mini, 1997).
neural networks are flexibly recruited according to situ- Third, the studies described in this review have used a
ational demands to integrate the central and autonomic variety of variables to measure ANS activity. The ANS
response to emotional stimuli. We use findings from our has two main branches—the sympathetic and parasym-
overview of the CNS/ANS literature and the theoretical pathetic (vagal) nervous systems—that innervate visceral
position of the model of neurovisceral integration to organs, blood vessels, and glands, and which exert op-
provide suggestions for future research directions. posite effects on the innervated organs (for an overview,
Prior to the review of relevant studies, some remarks see Lovallo & Sollers, 2000). The most frequently used
on the definition of ‘‘emotion’’ and additional technical measure of ANS activity is heart rate (HR), which is
issues may be helpful. First, defining ‘‘emotion’’ has antagonistically affected by sympathetic and parasym-
been a controversial issue, both historically (e.g., Ep- pathetic activity. This dual influence renders consider-
stein, 1984), and currently (e.g., Scherer, 2000). For the able ambiguity to the interpretation of HR responses,
purpose of the present review, we use a working defi- but inclusion of other cardiovascular measures can help
nition that focuses on the functional aspects of emotions to avoid this problem: HR variability and particularly
(e.g., Frijda, 1986, 1988; Levenson, 1988; see Thayer & its high frequency component primarily reflect cardiac
Lane, 2000): Emotions may be characterized as an or- parasympathetic activity whereas measures of myocar-
ganismic response to an environmental event that facil- dial performance indicate primarily b-adrenergic sym-
itates the rapid mobilization for action. This response pathetic activity. Another widely used ANS measure is
involves multiple systems of the organism, such as cog- blood pressure, which is also influenced by sympathetic
nitive, behavioral, and autonomic sub-systems. When and parasympathetic activity, and skin conductance
these response systems are efficiently coordinated, they response (SCR), which primarily reflects sympathetic
allow for goal-directed behavior in the service of flexible activity.
adaptation of the organism to changing environmental
demands.
Second, several researchers have classified emotional 2. Studies of CNS and ANS correlates of emotion
responses in terms of a number of discrete ‘‘basic emo-
tions’’ such as surprise, interest, happiness, rage, fear, 2.1. Lesion studies
sadness, and disgust (e.g., Ekman, 1984; Izard, 1977;
Tomkins, 1962). In contrast, other investigators have Most lesion studies have contrasted autonomic re-
argued that emotions—at least on the level of subjective sponses in individuals with right versus left hemispheric
experience—may be more parsimoniously described by brain lesions. Results suggest a critical role of the right
only two dimensions, which are ‘‘valence’’ (pleasant– hemisphere in mediating the ANS response to emotional
unpleasant) and ‘‘arousal’’ (low–high intensity) (e.g., stimuli. In this regard, many, though not all (e.g., Slo-
Larsen & Diener, 1992; Russell, 1980). Several re- mine et al., 1999), studies demonstrated a diminished
searchers have suggested that these two dimensions are SCR or HR response to pleasant or unpleasant stimuli
hierarchically related to discrete emotions (e.g., Diener, in right brain-damaged patients compared with left
Smith, & Fujita, 1995; Russell & Barrett, 1999). None- brain-damaged patients or control subjects (Andersson
theless, these different theoretical orientations have gi- & Finset, 1998; Caltagirone, Zoccolotti, Originale, Da-
ven rise to different emotion induction procedures for niele, & Mammucari, 1989; Heilman, Schwartz, &
experimental research (for general overviews, see Gerr- Watson, 1978; Meadows & Kaplan, 1994; Morrow,
ards-Hesse, Spies, & Hesse, 1994; Oatley & Jenkins, Vrtunski, Kim, & Boller, 1981; Zoccolotti, Caltagirone,
1996), and concomitant physiology has been contrasted Benedetti, & Gainotti, 1986; Zoccolotti, Scabini, &
in various experiments between basic emotions, or be- Violani, 1982).
D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87 81

In contrast, several recent investigations that exam- 2.3. Spontaneous electroencephalography (EEG) studies
ined patients with more distinct cerebral lesions suggest
that a right/left distinction in hemispheric dominance for A promising approach for the study of central and
central and autonomic linkages during emotion is too autonomic integration in emotion utilizes the sponta-
simplistic. Tranel and Damasio (1994) found that bi- neous EEG to localize cortical activation while assessing
lateral lesion of the ventromedial prefrontal region, an index (or indices) of autonomic function. In a recent
unilateral lesion of the right inferior parietal region, and investigation of self-generated happy and sad emotions
unilateral left or right lesion of the anterior cingulate and a relaxation condition, de Pascalis et al. (1998)
gyrus were associated with a diminished SCR to emo- found that bilateral arousal at the midfrontal cortex,
tional stimuli. This observation suggests that structures arousal asymmetry at the precentral cortex (i.e., right-
of both hemispheres are important sites for the neural sided asymmetry during sadness and left-sided asym-
networks that link emotional stimuli to ANS responses. metry during relaxation), and HR responses all followed
Bechara et al. (1995) found that bilateral lesion of the the same response pattern across all conditions. Both
amygdala prevented the acquisition of a conditioned midfrontal and relative right-sided precentral cortical
aversive SCR. However, other studies have suggested a activation and HR were largest in the sad and smallest
lack of involvement of the amygdala, or hippocampal in the relaxation condition. However, there was no co-
regions, in the SCR to emotional stimuli (Tranel & occurrence of cortical arousal and HR across the three
Damasio, 1989; Tranel & Hyman, 1990). conditions for parietal sites.
However, there are problems with lesion studies. For Further studies extended these findings. Schmidt,
example, lesion studies are often limited by small sample Fox, Schulkin, and Gold (1999) found that shy, as
sizes. In addition, because naturally occurring lesions compared to non-shy, children showed a greater in-
typically do not respect neuroanatomical boundaries (see crease in right midfrontal cortical activation and a
Kolb & Taylor, 2000), these types of investigations yield greater increase in HR during a task designed to induce
interpretive difficulties with respect to definitive locali- self-presentation anxiety. However, no group differences
zation of function. As well, the considerable plasticity of were noted in the left midfrontal, parietal, and occipital
the brain allows post-lesion relocation of functions, scalp regions, or in a measure of cardiac vagal tone.
which also hampers the interpretation of findings. Demaree, Harrison, and Rhodes (2000) found increases
in bilateral frontal cortical activity and HR immediately
2.2. Visual half field studies before and after a cold pressor test in high hostile, but
not low hostile, men. In a recent study of young adults,
Central and autonomic responses have also been ex- Waldstein et al. (2000) noted positive correlations be-
amined in visual half field studies in normal subjects (for tween left and right midfrontal cortical activation and
a review, see Wittling, 1995). In these studies, a visual HR during an anger recall condition. Subjects who
stimulus is presented selectively to the right or left displayed a lateralized right midfrontal cortical activa-
hemisphere and the ANS response is measured. Similar tion response during anger-inducing tasks also showed
to the lesion studies, several investigations in this area enhanced blood pressure responses. In addition, ex-
also suggested a right-hemispheric dominance for HR ploratory analyses indicated that men who showed a
responses to (or in anticipation of) unpleasant stimuli lateralized left midfrontal cortical activation response
(Dimond & Farrington, 1977; Hugdahl, Franzon, An- during happiness-inducing tasks also displayed the
dersson, & Walldebo, 1983; Spence, Shapiro, & Zaidel, greatest concomitant increase of systolic blood pressure
1996), although findings related to cardiovascular re- and HR. Davidson, Marshall, Tomarken, and Henri-
sponse to pleasant stimuli were inconsistent (Dimond & ques (2000) examined social phobics and normal con-
Farrington, 1977; Wittling, 1990). trols during anticipation of public speaking. The social
In contrast, several recent visual half field studies phobics showed increased state anxiety and increased
challenge the hypothesis of right-hemispheric domi- relative right-sided hemispheric activity during antici-
nance for ANS responses. Some preliminary evidence pation of the speech, whereas controls showed no sub-
has suggested a right hemispheric dominance for emo- stantive changes. However, the corresponding increase
tion-induced sympathetic nervous system response (as in HR and blood pressure during anticipation was
measured by various indices of myocardial function), similar in both groups, which suggests a lack of corre-
and a left hemisphere dominance for the parasympa- spondence between CNS and ANS measures in this
thetic response (as assessed by HR variability) (Wittling, study. Using a very different experimental paradigm,
Block, Genzel, & Schweiger, 1998a; Wittling, Block, Gilbert et al. (1999) noted that one month of smoking
Schweiger, & Genzel, 1998b). Unfortunately, visual half abstinence was associated with decreased cortical acti-
field studies are limited in number. As well, this research vation, particularly at bilateral parietal sites, and a de-
approach does not allow for a localization of function creased HR. However, there were no significant changes
within the cerebral hemispheres. in positive or negative mood.
82 D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87

In sum, the spontaneous EEG studies have employed blood pressure during a neutral control task and an un-
very different types of samples and have utilized dispa- pleasant emotional state that was induced with a mental
rate experimental paradigms. It therefore is not sur- stress task. Subjects who showed an increased rate pres-
prising that the findings are somewhat heterogeneous. sure product (which is predominantly due to sympathetic
Nonetheless, preliminary evidence suggests a positive activity) during the emotional state also showed in-
association between an increased bilateral or right-sided creased brain activation in the cerebellum, the periaqu-
activation of the anterior cortex and increased HR or eductual gray, right inferior frontal gyrus, and middle
systolic blood pressure in unpleasant emotional states. frontal gyrus/orbitofrontal cortex. Lane et al. (1997)
Interestingly, none of these studies observed similar as- noted that both pleasant and unpleasant picture presen-
sociations for posterior sites. However, the evidence for tation were associated with greater activation of left
pleasant emotional states is not consistent, and pre- prefrontal cortex, left thalamus, left hypothalamus, and
liminary evidence suggests the possibility of sex differ- left midbrain structures compared with neutral picture
ences. These findings require replication before definitive presentation. These valence-independent effects were
interpretations are tenable. In addition, major limita- accompanied by greater SCR to pleasant and unpleasant
tions of the spontaneous EEG include the inability to pictures than to neutral stimuli. In a further investiga-
record subcortical activity, low spatial resolution, and tion, Lane, Chua, and Dolan (1999) examined separately
mediocre temporal resolution (in the second range). the effects of valence and arousal on regional blood flow
and autonomic activity. Irrespective of valence, highly
2.4. Evoked potential studies arousing stimuli (relative to low arousing stimuli) elicited
both greater SCR, in addition to activation in the medial
Two recent studies evaluated evoked potential (EP) prefrontal cortex, right anterior temporal cortex, right
and autonomic response to emotional stimuli. EP allows extrastriate cortex, as well as in the left amygdala and left
a registration of brain activity with a temporal resolu- thalamus. More recently, Damasio et al. (2000) induced
tion in the millisecond range. Palomba et al. (1997) sadness, happiness, anger, and fear with recall of per-
noted a large positive correlation between the amplitude sonal life experience, and compared simultaneously reg-
of the electrocortical response at vertex (averaged across istered PET, HR, and SCR with a neutral control
a time range between 600 and 900 ms after picture onset) condition. In addition to a multitude of emotion-specific
and HR deceleration during presentation of pleasant, effects on regional blood flow, there was common acti-
unpleasant, and neutral pictures. In another study, vation during all four emotions (compared with the
Cuthbert et al. (2000) noted that the amplitude of the neutral state) in the right insula, the left midbrain, and the
electrocortical response (averaged across anterior and left mesial cerebellum, in addition to a common deacti-
posterior sites and across a time range between 400 and vation in the right lateral frontal lobe. Increases in HR
1000 ms after picture onset) to pleasant, unpleasant, and SCR were also noted during sadness, happiness,
and neutral pictures was associated with an increased anger, and fear as compared to the neutral control con-
SCR, but not with the HR response. The contradictory dition. Lane et al. (2000) induced happiness, sadness, and
findings of these two studies do not permit conclusions disgust using film clips and personal recall tasks. HR
at present. Although EP methods have superior tem- variability was correlated with regional brain activation
poral resolution compared with other measures of CNS during the emotion conditions, a neutral condition, and
activity, EPs are also not well suited for the registration the subtraction of neutral from emotion conditions. Of
of subcortical activity, and have a low spatial resolution. particular note, a positive correlation between HR vari-
ability and regional brain activation in the medial pre-
2.5. Neuroimaging studies frontal cortex and the left posterior orbitofrontal/
anterior insular cortex was found for the subtraction
Major advantages of neuroimaging methodology comparison. More specifically, emotional arousal was
include the co-registration of cortical and subcortical associated with a decrease in HR variability and con-
activity and good spatial resolution. Several recent in- comitant decreases in brain activation in these regions.
vestigations simultaneously collected measures of re- To date, relatively few studies have simultaneously
gional cerebral blood flow with 15 O-water positron measured CNS activation using neuroimging procedures
emission tomography (PET), and measures of ANS ac- and ANS activity during emotional states. Nonetheless,
tivation during the induction of emotions. Schneider the preliminary evidence suggests a co-occurrence of left
et al. (1995) found that sad and happy moods, induced by subcortical activation and increased SCR during both
a combined imagination and picture presentation task, pleasant and unpleasant emotional states. These sub-
were associated with different patterns of regional brain cortical structures comprise the amygdala, thalamus,
activation, but similar HR accelerations. There were, in and hypothalamus, in addition to midbrain regions of
addition, no significant correlations between cerebral the left hemisphere. For cortical structures, however,
blood flow and HR. Soufer et al. (1998) recorded HR and such lateralization is less clear. Some findings point to a
D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87 83

co-occurrence of greater relative left-sided activation of be noted that definitive conclusions are hampered by the
the frontal lobe and increased SCR during pleasant and limited number of emotional states assessed, and the
unpleasant emotion. However, other findings suggest a absence of appropriate statistics to quantify and directly
co-occurrence of right frontal lobe, insular, anterior test for correspondence in CNS and ANS responses (for
temporal lobe, and extrastriate cortical activation and exceptions, see Cuthbert et al., 2000; Lane et al., 2000;
increased SCR or rate pressure product during pleasant Palomba et al., 1997; Schneider et al., 1995; Soufer et al.,
and unpleasant states. Considered together, these find- 1998; Waldstein et al., 2000). In addition, it is important
ings again indicate that a left/right dichotomy is too to note that there is variability in findings across studies.
simplistic to describe CNS/ANS relations in emotion, Second, results of the studies reviewed herein suggest
and suggest that localization on the anterior–posterior that a simple left/right dichotomy with respect to hemi-
and the cortical–subcortical dimensions is also impor- spheric specialization for the autonomic component of
tant. Interestingly, the preliminary evidence also sug- the emotional response is probably untenable. The em-
gests that the co-occurrence of CNS and ANS activity pirical data suggest that cortical and subcortical asym-
may be mediated by the arousal component of emo- metries in the CNS/ANS processing of emotional
tional stimuli, rather than by valence. These observa- information may be reversed. The lesion and spontane-
tions require replication, particularly because many of ous EEG studies reviewed above provide some evidence
these studies are limited by relatively small sample sizes for right-sided cortical involvement in ANS responses to
(particularly in relation to the number of relevant vari- unpleasant stimulation (de Pascalis et al., 1998; Schmidt
ables), which hampers statistical conclusions. Although et al., 1999; Tranel & Damasio, 1994; Waldstein et al.,
the spatial resolution of neuroimaging methods is good 2000). In contrast, results of neuroimaging studies sug-
compared with EEG and EP methods, PET has a low gest a greater involvement of left-hemispheric subcortical
temporal resolution (in the minute range). structures (e.g., amygdala, hypothalamus, and thalamus)
in ANS responses to emotional stimuli irrespective of
valence (Damasio et al., 2000; Lane et al., 1999; Lane
3. Conclusion et al., 1997). It thus appears that neuroimaging studies
generally point to left subcortical activation during
The data reviewed above support two primary con- emotional arousal, whereas electrophysiological evidence
clusions. First, there is ample support for the notion that suggests right cortical activation at least for unpleasant
numerous cortical and subcortical regions show co-oc- emotion.
curring activity with ANS responses in emotion. Such a Such a reversal of cortical/subcortical activation
coupling between CNS and ANS activity may be in- asymmetry may be explained by the joint function of
ferred for frontal, temporal, parietal, and anterior cin- two inhibitory mechanisms. Specifically, the mechanism
gulate cortices together with subcortical structures of ipsilateral inhibition (e.g., Tucker, 1981; Tucker,
including the amygdala, thalamus, hypothalamus, and 1984) suggests that activation of a cortical region results
the midbrain. In this regard, lesion studies indicate that in inhibition of efferent subcortical structures. This is
the ventromedial prefrontal region, the right inferior related to Hughlings JacksonÕs classic principle of ‘‘hi-
parietal region, and the anterior cingulate gyrus are erarchic integration through inhibition’’ (Jackson, 1879;
important sites for the neural networks that link pleas- Tucker, Derryberry, & Luu, 2000). In addition, the
ant and unpleasant stimuli to the SCR (Tranel & mechanism of contralateral inhibition (e.g., Sackheim
Damasio, 1994), and that selective bilateral lesions of et al., 1982) suggests that activation of a cortical area
the amygdala prevent the acquisition of conditioned leads to an inhibition of homologous contralateral cor-
aversive SCR (Bechara et al., 1995). In addition, spon- tex. The parallel functioning of both mechanisms readily
taneous EEG studies suggest a positive association be- implies a reversal of cortical and subcortical activation
tween an increased bilateral or right-sided activation of asymmetries as is suggested in this review (see Liotti &
the anterior cortex and increased HR or systolic blood Tucker, 1995; for a combination of these mechanisms).
pressure in unpleasant emotional states (de Pascalis However, the present evidence for the proposed reversal
et al., 1998; Schmidt et al., 1999; Waldstein et al., 2000). of cortical/subcortical activation asymmetry in emotion
Finally, neuroimaging evidence reveals a co-occurrence should be explicitly tested in future studies.
of activation in cortical (frontal, insular, and anterior We suggest that the present conclusions, derived from
temporal), subcortical (amygdala, thalamus, and hypo- the extant empirical literature, can be enriched by con-
thalamus), and midbrain structures and increased SCR sideration of a recent theoretical model of neurovisceral
and HR during pleasant and unpleasant emotional integration in emotion. This model was elaborated by
states (Damasio et al., 2000; Lane et al., 1999; Lane Thayer and Lane (2000), who proposed a network of
et al., 2000; Lane et al., 1997). Importantly, although the neural structures that generate, receive, and integrate
observed covariation of CNS and ANS activity across internal and external information in the service of goal-
different emotion induction tasks is suggestive, it must directed behavior and organism adaptability. One such
84 D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87

functional unit is the central autonomic network (CAN; parallel, distributed pathways, which allows for multiple
Benarroch, 1993, 1997). Functionally, this network is an avenues to a given autonomic response (e.g., increased
integrated component of an internal regulation system sympathetic or decreased parasympathetic activity or
through which the brain controls visceromotor, neuro- any combination of the two). Moreover, within the
endocrine, and behavioral responses that are critical for CAN, both direct and indirect pathways can modulate
goal-directed behavior and adaptability (Benarroch, the output to the preganglionic sympathetic and para-
1993). Structurally, the CAN involves a number of sympathetic neurons. Furthermore, the activity of the
structures throughout the neuraxis including the ante- CAN is state dependent and thus sensitive to initial
rior cingulate, insular, and ventromedial prefrontal conditions (see Glass & Mackey, 1988).
cortices, the central nucleus of the amygdala, the pa- The model of neurovisceral integration proposes that
raventricular and related nuclei of the hypothalamus, the CAN (or related systems that have been identified by
the periaquaductal gray matter, the parabrachial nu- other researchers such as the anterior executive region of
cleus, the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS), the nucleus Devinsky, Morrell, & Vogt, 1995; or the ‘‘emotion cir-
ambiguus, the ventrolateral medulla, the ventromedial cuit’’ of Damasio, 1998) constitutes a network of CNS
medulla, and the medullary tegmental field. These structures that is associated with the processes of re-
structures are reciprocally interconnected such that in- sponse organization and selection, and serves to mod-
formation flows in both directions—top-down and bot- ulate psychophysiological resources in both emotion
tom-up. The primary output of the CAN is mediated and attention (Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Thayer &
through the preganglionic sympathetic and parasym- Friedman, 1997). Thus, according to the model, the core
pathetic neurons. These neurons innervate the heart via neural ÔwetwareÕ underpinning cognitive, affective, and
the stellate ganglia and the vagus nerve. Interestingly, physiological regulation may be one and the same.
many of the CAN brain regions have been found to be Additional structures are flexibly recruited in the service
associated with autonomic responses in the literature of specific behavioral adaptations. This sparsely inter-
reviewed above. connected neural network allows for maximal organism
The CAN also has many features of a dynamical flexibility in adapting to rapidly changing environmental
system. There is a growing consensus both inside and demands. When this network is either completely un-
outside of cognitive neuroscience that living organisms coupled or rigidly coupled the organism is less able to
are self-organizing dynamical systems (e.g., van Gelder, dynamically assemble the appropriate neural support
1998). This has important implications for attempts to structures to meet a particular demand and is thus less
localize specific functions to specific structures. First, adaptive.
dynamical systems flexibly recruit system components The main conclusions and caveats of this review, in
into context specific functional units. Thus any given conjunction with the model of neurovisceral integration
system element or structure may serve numerous but not in emotion, suggest a number of future directions for
necessarily fixed functions. Second, there is considerable research in this new area of affective neuroscience. First,
overlap between the functions and structures associated given the suggestion of widespread cortical and sub-
with a wide range of behaviors. For example, in the cortical involvement, and the possibility of reversed
context of a system designed to maximize adaptability in asymmetries on cortical and subcortical levels, research
the service of goal directed behavior, cognition, emo- on central and autonomic integration in emotion should
tion, and physiological activities must be coordinated utilize neuroimaging as the preferred method for as-
and built upon the same neural Ôwetware.Õ Discrete be- sessing CNS activity. Second, there is ample evidence
haviors and functions emerge when distributed activity that the emotional ANS response includes a complex
across system levels is recruited into functional networks pattern of sympathetic and parasympathetic activation
to meet environmental challenges and task demands. (for a review, see Cacioppo et al., 2000), and preliminary
And third, in the context of a dynamical system, in- evidence suggests that different ANS components may
hibitory processes may hold particular importance for have different CNS concomitants (Wittling et al., 1998a,
flexible system responding. Unlike the prevailing ho- 1998b). Given that frequently used ANS measures like
meostatic model, dynamical systems models propose HR or blood pressure are antagonistically influenced by
that the system normally functions far from equilibrium. the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous systems
Accordingly, from a dynamical systems perspective, (Berne & Levy, 2001), future research will greatly benefit
emotional (or other) states may reflect modes of relative from examination of both sympathetic and parasym-
stability that occur in the constant flow of organism– pathetic activity. Furthermore, evaluation of distinct
environment interactions. sympathetic response patterns (i.e., cardiac versus vas-
Because the components of the CAN are reciprocally cular) would be useful.
interconnected, this allows for continuous positive and Third, there is some evidence that men and women
negative feedback interactions and integration of auto- differ in the experience and expression of emotions (see
nomic responses. Next, the CAN comprises a number of Hagemann et al., 1999; and the literature cited there),
D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87 85

and preliminary evidence also suggests that the associ- complexity (Friedman & Thayer, 1998; Thayer &
ation between CNS and ANS activity during emotional Friedman, 2002). This perseverative activity reflects the
states might be moderated by sex (Waldstein et al., inability of the system to flexibly assemble the necessary
2000). Thus, future studies might further examine this structures into appropriate functional units in response
individual difference variable. Additional dimensions of to changing environmental demands.
individual differences (e.g., personality) might also in- Next, different methods are needed to examine the
fluence CNS/ANS integration in emotion. dynamics of the physiological activity that support
Fourth, although numerous brain regions display emotional states. The common procedure of averaging
activation during emotion, this does not necessarily over large periods of time may obscure important cor-
imply that each of these regions has direct involvement tical and autonomic dynamics that in themselves may be
in autonomic responding. In this regard, associations the distinguishing features of different emotional and
between individual differences in CNS and ANS re- behavioral states (cf. Vaadia et al., 1995). An additional
sponses during emotional states should be directly corollary is that the typical strategy of averaging data
quantified and tested, preferably with multivariate over individuals also obscures the dynamics and speci-
methods (for an example of a cross-conditions correla- ficity associated with individual differences in the phys-
tion approach, see Cuthbert et al., 2000; and for exam- iological correlates of emotion (Friedman, Santucci,
ples of cross-subjects correlations, see Palomba et al., Curtis, & Pumphrey, 1999).
1997; Schneider et al., 1995; Waldstein et al., 2000). It Last, the neural structures that support emotional
must be noted, however, that multivariate statistics re- behavior need not differ from those that support other
quire larger sample sizes than those employed in most of types of behavior. Indeed, behavior of all types is most
the studies reviewed above. certainly built upon the same physiological wetware.
Fifth, a main conclusion of the present paper is that This presents a challenge for researchers to identify ex-
different basic emotions might share common CNS and perimental paradigms that will allow us to examine the
ANS concomitants, which are linked to the arousal di- system in a more fully integrated fashion.
mension of emotion. A rigorous test of this notion will
include not only induction variations with respect to
arousal and valence, but will also test the alternative
hypothesis that different basic emotions share only a few Acknowledgment
neural (CNS and ANS) concomitants. The envisioned
study on CNS/ANS integration in emotion will there- The Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft supported
fore sample activity in multiple cortical and subcortical this work through Grant Ha 3044/1-1 to the first author.
regions and from several sympathetic and parasympa-
thetic response systems during the induction of several
basic emotions and control conditions. Interrelations References
between CNS and ANS activity will then be examined
with multivariate statistics. This integrated approach Andersson, S., & Finset, A. (1998). Heart rate and skin conductance
will help to consolidate and extend our knowledge reactivity to brief psychological stress in brain-injured patients.
on the physiological concomitants of the emotional re- Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 645–656.
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., &
sponse. Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double association of conditioning and
Finally, the model of neurovisceral integration in declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus
emotion may have important implications for the study in humans. Science, 269, 1115–1118.
of central and autonomic integration in emotion. First, Benarroch, E. E. (1993). The central autonomic network: Functional
attempts to localize the cortical concomitants of emo- organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clinic Proceed-
ings, 68, 988–1001.
tion in any simple way may be untenable. The literature Benarroch, E. E. (1997). The central autonomic network. In P. A. Low
suggests that a number of neural structures and auto- (Ed.), Clinical autonomic disorders (2nd ed., pp. 17–23). Philadel-
nomic changes are associated with emotional states. phia: Lippincott–Raven.
Indeed, across investigations, sometimes the same cen- Berne, R. M., & Levy, M. N. (2001). Cardiovascular physiology. St.
tral and autonomic activation patterns are associated Louis: Mosby.
Birbaumer, N., & O € hman, A. (1993). The structure of emotion. Seattle:
with different emotions and sometimes different central Hogrefe and Huber Publishers.
structures and autonomic changes are associated with Borod, J. (Ed.). (2000). The neuropsychology of emotion. New York:
the same emotion. These findings can be reconciled in Oxford University Press.
the context of dynamical systems models where struc- Borod, J. C., & Madigan, N. K. (2000). Neuropsychology of emotion
tures dynamically organize to meet the demands of and emotional disorder: An overview and research directions. In J.
C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion (pp. 3–28). New
specific situations. Healthy systems are characterized by York: Oxford University Press.
this emotional complexity and diversity whereas un- Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., Larsen, J. T., Poehlmann, K. M., &
healthy systems show perseverative activity and a lack of Ito, T. A. (2000). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis,
86 D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87

& J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. Glass, L., & Mackey, M. C. (1988). From clocks to chaos. Princeton,
173–191). New York: The Guilford Press. NJ: Princeton University Press.
Caltagirone, C., Zoccolotti, P., Originale, G., Daniele, A., & Mam- Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Maier, S., Becker, G., L€ urken, A., &
mucari, A. (1989). Autonomic reactivity and facial expression of Bartussek, D. (1999). The assessment of affective reactivity using
emotion in brain-damaged patients. In G. Gainotti, & C. Caltag- films: Validity, reliability, and sex differences. Personality and
irone (Eds.), Emotions and the dual brain (pp. 204–221). Berlin: Individual Differences, 26, 627–639.
Springer. Heilman, K. M., Schwartz, H., & Watson, R. T. (1978). Hypoarousal
Cuthbert, B. N., Schupp, H. T., Bradley, M. M., Birbaumer, N., & in patients with the neglect syndrome and emotional indifference.
Lang, P. J. (2000). Brain potentials in affective picture processing: Neurology, 28, 229–232.
Covariation with autonomic arousal and affective report. Biological Hugdahl, K., Franzon, M., Andersson, B., & Walldebo, G. (1983).
Psychology, 52, 95–111. Heart-rate responses (HRR) to lateralized visual stimuli. Pavlovian
Damasio, A. R. (1998). Emotion in the perspective of an integrated Journal of Biological Science, 18, 186–198.
nervous system. Brain Research Reviews, 26, 83–86. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.
Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, Jackson, J. H. (1879). On affections of speech from diseases of the
L. L. B., Parvizi, J., & Hichwa, R. D. (2000). Subcortical and brain. Brain, 2, 203–222.
cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Kolb, B., & Taylor, L. (2000). Facial expression, emotion, and
Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049–1056. hemispheric organization. In R. Lane, & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive
Davidson, R. J., Marshall, J. R., Tomarken, A. J., & Henriques, J. B. neuroscience of emotion (pp. 62–83). New York: Oxford University
(2000). While a phobic waits: Regional brain electrical and Press.
autonomic activity in social phobics during anticipation of public Lane, R. D., Chua, P. M.-L., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Common effects of
speaking. Biological Psychiatry, 47, 85–95. emotional valence, arousal and attention on neural activation
Demaree, H. A., Harrison, D. W., & Rhodes, R. D. (2000). during visual processing of pictures. Neuropsychologia, 37, 989–
Quantitative electroencephalographic analyses of cardiovascular 997.
regulation in low- and high-hostile men. Psychobiology, 28, 420– Lane, R. D., & Jennings, J. R. (1995). Hemispheric asymmetry,
431. autonomic asymmetry, and the problem of sudden cardiac death.
de Pascalis, V., Ray, W. J., Tranquillo, I., & DÕAmico, D. (1998). EEG In R. J. Davidson, & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain asymmetry (pp.
activity and heart rate during recall of emotional events in 271–304). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
hypnosis: Relationships with hypnotizability and suggestibility. Lane, R. D., & Nadel, L. (Eds.). (2000). Cognitive neuroscience of
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 29, 255–275. emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.
Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contributions of Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Ahern, G. L., & Thayer, J. F. (2000).
anterior cingulate cortex to behavior. Brain, 118, 279–306. Activity in medial prefrontal cortex correlates with vagal compo-
Diener, E., Smith, H., & Fujita, F. (1995). The personality structure nent of heart rate variability during emotion. Psychosomatic
of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 130– Medicine, 62, 1286.
141. Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Ahern, G.
Dimond, S. J., & Farrington, L. (1977). Emotional response to films L., Davidson, R. J., & Schwartz, G. E. (1997). Neuroanatomical
shown to the right or left hemisphere of the brain measured by correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia,
heart rate. Acta Psychologica, 41, 255–260. 35, 1437–1444.
Ekman, P. (1984). Expression and nature of emotion. In K. Scherer, & Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Induction of lateralized
P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 319–343). Hillsdale: sympathetic input to the heart by the CNS during emotional
Erlbaum. arousal: A possible neurophysiologic trigger of sudden cardiac
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic death. Psychosomatic Medicine, 49, 274–284.
nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with the
221, 1208–1210. circumplex model of emotion. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of
Epstein, S. (1984). Controversial issues in emotion theory. Review of personality and social psychology: Emotion (Vol. 13, pp. 25–59).
Personality and Social Psychology, 5, 64–88. Newbury Park: Sage.
Friedman, B. H., & Thayer, J. F. (1998). Autonomic balance revisited: Levenson, R. W. (1988). Emotion and the autonomic nervous system:
Panic anxiety and heart rate variability. Journal of Psychosomatic A prospectus for research on autonomic specificity. In H. L.
Research, 44, 133–151. Wagner (Ed.), Social psychophysiology and emotion: Theory and
Friedman, B. H., Santucci, A. K., Curtis, E. M., & Pumphrey, B. G. clinical applications (pp. 17–42). Chichester: Wiley.
(1999). Idiodynamic profiles of cardiovascular activity. Psycho- Liotti, M., & Tucker, D. M. (1995). Emotion in asymmetric cortico-
physiology, 36, S52. limbic networks. In R. J. Davidson, & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University asymmetry (pp. 389–423). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
Press. Lovallo, W. R., & Sollers III, J. J. (2000). Autonomic nervous system.
Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist, 43, In G. Fink (Ed.), Encyclopedia of stress (Vol. 1, pp. 275–284). San
349–358. Diego: Academic Press.
Gainotti, G. (1989). The meaning of emotional disturbance resulting Meadows, M. E., & Kaplan, R. F. (1994). Dissociation of autonomic
from unilateral brain injury. In G. Gainotti, & C. Caltagirone and subjective responses to emotional slides in right hemisphere
(Eds.), Emotions and the dual brain (pp. 147–167). Berlin: Springer. damage. Neuropsychologia, 32, 847–856.
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental Morrow, L., Vrtunski, P. B., Kim, Y., & Boller, F. (1981). Arousal
induction of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. response to emotional stimuli and laterality of lesion. Neuropsych-
British Journal of Psychology, 85, 55–78. ologia, 19, 65–71.
Gilbert, D. G., McClernon, F. J., Rabinovich, N. E., Dibb, W. D., Neumann, S. A., & Waldstein, S. R. (2001). Similar patterns of
Plath, L. C., Hiyane, S., Jensen, R. A., Meliska, C. J., Estes, S. L., cardiovascular response during emotional activation as a function
& Gehlbach, B. A. (1999). WWEEG, physiology, and task-related of affective valence and arousal and gender. Journal of Psychoso-
mood fail to resolve across 31 days of smoking abstinence: matic Research, 50, 245–253.
Relations to depressive traits, nicotine exposure, and dependence. Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding emotion. Cam-
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 7, 427–443. bridge, MA: Blackwell.
D. Hagemann et al. / Brain and Cognition 52 (2003) 79–87 87

Palomba, D., Angrilli, A., & Mini, A. (1997). Visual evoked potentials, Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. New York:
heart rate responses and memory to emotional pictorial stimuli. Springer.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, 27, 55–67. Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1989). Intact electrodermal skin conduc-
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of tance response after bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia,
Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178. 27, 381–390.
Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Core affect, prototypical Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1994). Neuroanatomical correlates of
emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: Dissecting the electrodermal skin conductance response. Psychophysiology, 31,
elephant. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 805– 427–438.
819. Tranel, D., & Hyman, B. T. (1990). Neuropsychological correlates of
Sackheim, H. A., Greenberg, M. S., Weiman, A. L., Gur, R., bilateral amygdala damage. Archives of Neurology, 47, 349–355.
Hungerbuhler, J. P., & Geschwind, N. (1982). Hemispheric Tucker, D. M. (1981). Lateral brain function, emotion, and concep-
asymmetry in the expression of positive and negative emotions. tualization. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 19–46.
Archives of Neurology, 39, 210–218. Tucker, D. M. (1984). Lateral brain function in normal and disordered
Scherer, K. R. (2000). Psychological models of emotion. In J. C. Borod emotion: Interpreting electroencephalographic evidence. Biological
(Ed.), The neuropsychology of emotion (pp. 137–162). New York: Psychology, 19, 219–235.
Oxford University Press. Tucker, D. M., Derryberry, D., & Luu, P. (2000). Anatomy and
Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N. A., Schulkin, J., & Gold, P. W. (1999). physiology of human emotion: Vertical integration of brain stem,
Behavioral and psychophysiological correlates of self-presentation limbic, and cortical systems. In J. C. Borod (Ed.), The neuropsy-
in temperamentally shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, chology of emotion (pp. 56–79). New York: Oxford University
119–135. Press.
Schneider, F., Gur, R. E., Mozley, L. H., Smith, R. J., Mozley, P. D., Vaadia, E., Haalman, I., Abeles, M., Bergman, H., Prut, Y., Slovin,
Censits, D. M., Alavi, A., & Gur, R. C. (1995). Mood effects on H., & Aertsen, A. (1995). Dynamics of neuronal interactions in
limbic blood flow correlate with emotional self-rating: A PET study monkey cortex in relation to behavioural events. Nature, 373, 515–
of oxygen-15 labeled water. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 518.
61, 265–283. van Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science.
Sinha, R., Lovallo, W. R., & Parsons, O. A. (1992). Cardiovascular Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 21, 615–628.
differentiation of emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 54, 422–435. Waldstein, S. R., Kop, W. J., Schmidt, L. A., Haufler, A. J., Krantz,
Slomine, B., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1999). Dissociation D. S., & Fox, N. A. (2000). Frontal electrocortical and cardiovas-
between autonomic responding and verbal report in right and left cular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology,
hemisphere brain damage during anticipatory anxiety. Neuropsy- 55, 3–23.
chiatry Neuropsychology and Behavioral Neurology, 12, 143–148. Wittling, W. (1990). Psychophysiological correlates of human brain
Soufer, R., Bremner, J. D., Arrighi, J. A., Cohen, I., Zaret, B. L., Burg, asymmetries. Neuropsychologia, 28, 457–470.
M. M., & Goldman-Rakic, P. (1998). Cerebral cortical hyperac- Wittling, W. (1995). Brain asymmetry in the control of autonomic–
tivation in response to mental stress in patients with coronary physiologic activity. In R. J. Davidson, & K. Hugdahl (Eds.), Brain
artery disease. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, asymmetry (pp. 305–357). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
6454–6459. Wittling, W., Block, A., Genzel, S., & Schweiger, E. (1998a).
Spence, S., Shapiro, D., & Zaidel, E. (1996). The role of the right Hemisphere asymmetry in parasympathetic control of the heart.
hemisphere in the physiological and cognitive components of Neuropsychologia, 36, 461–468.
emotional processing. Psychophysiology, 33, 112–122. Wittling, W., Block, A., Schweiger, E., & Genzel, S. (1998b).
Stemmler, G. (1996). Psychophysiologie der Emotionen. Zeitschrift f€ur Hemisphere asymmetry in sympathetic control of the human
psychosomatische Medizin, 42, 235–260. myocardium. Brain and Cognition, 38, 17–35.
Thayer, J. F., & Friedman, B. H. (1997). The heart of anxiety: A Wittling, W., & Roschmann, R. (1993). Emotion-related hemisphere
dynamical systems approach. In A. Vingerhoets (Ed.), The (non) asymmetry: Subjective emotional responses to laterally presented
expression of emotions in health and disease (pp. 39–49). Amster- films. Cortex, 29, 431–448.
dam: Springer. Zoccolotti, P., Caltagirone, C., Benedetti, N., & Gainotti, G. (1986).
Thayer, J. F., & Friedman, B. H. (2002). Stop that! Inhibition, Pertubation des responses vegetatives aux stimuli emotionnels au
sensitization and their neurovisceral concomitants. Scandinavian cours des lesions hemispheriques unilaterales. Encephale, 12, 263–
Journal of Psychology, 43, 123–130. 268.
Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral Zoccolotti, P., Scabini, D., & Violani, C. (1982). Electrodermal
integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of responses in patients with unilateral brain damage. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 61, 201–216. Clinical Neuropsychology, 4, 143–150.

You might also like