You are on page 1of 11

Brain (1997), 120, 2207–2217

Cognitive deficits in Huntington’s disease are


predicted by dopaminergic PET markers and brain
volumes
L. Bäckman,1,2,3 T.-B. Robins-Wahlin,2,3 A. Lundin,4 N. Ginovart4 and L. Farde4
1Department of Psychology, Göteborg University, Göteborg, Correspondence to: Lars Bäckman, Stockholm Gerontology
2Stockholm Gerontology Research Centre, 3Department of Research Center, Olivecronas väg 4, S-113 82 Stockholm,
Clinical Neuroscience and Family Medicine, Section of Sweden
Geriatrics, Karolinska Institute and 4Department of
Clinical Neuroscience, Section of Psychiatry, Karolinska
Institute, Stockholm, Sweden

Summary
The main aim of this study was to investigate the of the variance across the majority of cognitive tasks. In
relationship between dopaminergic markers and brain addition, frontal volume showed a strong relationship with
volumes for striatal and cortical structures, and cognitive all cognitive tasks. D1 binding and volume measurements
performance in patients with Huntington’s disease and for the temporal cortex and thalamic volume showed
control subjects. We used PET and MRI data as predictors associations with a select number of cognitive tasks. The
of performance in tasks assessing executive function, overall data pattern is consistent with the view that
visuospatial ability, episodic memory, verbal fluency, per- Huntington’s disease may be characterized as a frontostria-
ceptual speed and reasoning. The dopamine neurotransmis- tal dementia, in which cognitive deficits may result from
sion parameters (D1 and D2 receptor density and dopamine pathological changes at multiple sites in the frontostriatal
transporter density) and the volumetric measurements for circuitry.
caudate and putamen accounted for substantial portions
Keywords: Huntington’s disease; cognition; dopaminergic markers; PET; MRI

Abbreviations: ANOVA 5 analysis of variance; DAT 5 dopamine transporter; R-OCF 5 Rey–Osterrieth’s Complex Figure;
TMT 5 Trail Making Test; WCST 5 Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Introduction
Huntington’s disease is a progressive, neurodegenerative learning (Fedio et al., 1979; Martone et al., 1984) and verbal
disorder with a typical clinical onset between 30 and 50 fluency (Butters et al., 1978; Wexler, 1979; Tröster et al.,
years of age. The clinical manifestations of Huntington’s 1989; Rosser and Hodges, 1994). Importantly, processing
disease involve prominent motor dysfunction and cognitive speed is a central component in many of the tasks described
deterioration, as well as psychiatric symptoms and personality above. Thus, the observed patterns of results are consistent
changes (for reviews, see Brandt and Butters, 1986; Brown with the notion that Huntington’s disease is characterized by
and Marsden, 1988). Huntington’s disease-related deficits a reduction in mental tempo (Brandt and Butters, 1986;
have been reported in tasks assessing a variety of cognitive Brown and Marsden, 1988).
skills, including episodic memory (Caine et al., 1977; Butters It is of interest to relate the cognitive deterioration to
et al., 1978; Moses et al., 1981), executive functions and the specific neuropathology in Huntington’s disease. The
mental flexibility (Boll et al., 1974; Josiassen et al., 1983; neuropathology is characterized by a loss of medium spiny
Taylor and Hansotia, 1983; Butters et al., 1985), visuospatial neurons, the predominant neuronal population in the striatum
ability (Fedio et al., 1979; Mayeux et al., 1983; Brouwers (Graveland et al., 1985; Goto et al., 1989). A more recent
et al., 1984; Mohr et al., 1991; Filoteo et al., 1995), procedural observation is that frontal gyri become atrophic along with
© Oxford University Press 1997
2208 L. Bäckman et al.

Table 1 Volumes, D1- and D2-receptor binding potential and DAT binding of different brain structures in patients with
Huntington’s disease and control subjects
Structure Volume (mm3)* D 1* D2* DAT*

Huntington’s Control Huntington’s Control Huntington’s Control Huntington’s Control


disease subjects disease subjects disease subjects disease subjects

Caudate
Mean 2065.20 3834.20 0.67 1.13 1.22 2.08 0.51 1.13
SD 603.67 672.92 0.28 0.10 0.35 0.14 0.27 0.11
Putamen
Mean 3447.60 5361.80 0.84 1.28 1.48 2.46 0.60 1.27
SD 396.98 885.58 0.30 0.17 0.29 0.19 0.28 0.12
Frontal cortex
Mean 14835.20 18190.60 0.31 0.30 – – – –
SD 1590.65 726.53 0.06 0.03 – – – –
Temporal cortex
Mean 13482.40 16138.40 0.35 0.46 – – – –
SD 1574.05 2136.66 0.04 0.07 – – – –
Thalamus
Mean 4832.60 5965.20 – – – – – –
SD 624.01 607.65 – – – – – –

Adapted from Ginovart et al. (1997). *Values represent the means of both hemispheres because no significant asymmetry was observed.

the striatum (Webb and Trzepacz, 1987; Jernigan et al., basal ganglia serve a key function in integrating information
1991). The striatum is densely innervated by dopaminergic through cortico-striato-thalamocortical circuits (Bruyn et al.,
fibres and contains a high density of postsynaptic D1 and D2 1979; Gerfen, 1989; Parent, 1990). Thus, given the
receptors, which are located on the medium spiny neurons Huntington’s disease-related degeneration of the striatal
(Seeman et al., 1993; Hall et al., 1994). D1 is the predominant nuclei, as well as their projections to cortical and subcortical
dopamine receptor in the neocortex (Murray et al., 1992). areas (Bruyn et al., 1979; Graveland et al., 1985; Alexander
Post-mortem examinations and in vivo studies with PET have et al., 1986; Reiner et al., 1988; Goto et al., 1989; Parent
demonstrated marked reductions of D1 and D2 receptors in and Hazrati, 1995), widespread effects on cognitive functions
the striatum of Huntington’s disease patients (Hägglund et al., are to be expected in this disease. Attempts at linking the
1987; Filloux et al., 1990; Richfield et al., 1991; Sedvall subcortical and cortical changes in Huntington’s disease
et al., 1994; Turjanski et al., 1995). Moreover, a recent PET with cognitive performance and functional ability have been
study demonstrated loss of D1 receptors in the frontal cortex relatively successful. Relationships between CT-based
of Huntington’s disease patients (Sedvall et al., 1994). measures of caudate atrophy and indices of both functional
Biochemical research indicates a Huntington’s disease- and cognitive ability have been reported (Shoulson et al.,
related dysfunction in the presynaptic dopamine neurons that 1982; Sax et al., 1983; Starkstein et al., 1988). Bamford
ascend from the midbrain (Ferrante and Kowall, 1987). The et al. (1989) found strong associations between CT measures
dopamine transporter (DAT) is a protein located of striatal and frontal atrophy and cognitive test performance
presynaptically on dopaminergic nerve terminals (Kuhar in a group of early-stage Huntington’s disease patients.
et al., 1990; Giros et al., 1992), and may thus serve as a Starkstein et al. (1992) observed that MRI-based measures
marker for the presynaptic neuron. of caudate and frontal atrophy correlated with a factor
In a recent PET and MRI study of Huntington’s disease indexing memory and speed of processing in mild to moderate
patients, we found a parallel reduction of D1 and D2 receptor Huntington’s disease patients, although no relationships were
binding sites in the striatum. These reductions were related found with measures of putaminal or thalamic atrophy. Brandt
to the duration of the illness (Ginovart et al., 1997). The et al. (1995) recently reported that an MRI-based measure
density of D1 receptors was also reduced in the temporal of left caudate atrophy was related to memory for item and
cortex, but not in the frontal cortex. In addition, a Huntington’s contextual information in mild to moderate Huntington’s
disease-related reduction of DAT was found in the caudate disease patients; no reliable relationships with measures of
and putamen. The MRI assessment showed striatal atrophy, other basal ganglia structures were obtained. In another recent
along with reduced volumes of the frontal cortex and thalamus report, Harris et al. (1996) found a somewhat different
(see Table 1). outcome. In their study, Huntington’s disease-related deficits
Striatal regions are topographically organized with in various cognitive tasks (i.e. visual and verbal episodic
abundant reciprocal connections to the neocortex (Graybiel memory, Trail Making and Stroop tests) were related to
and Ragsdal, 1979; Crosson, 1992) and thalamus (Dom et al., volumetric measurements of both caudate and putamen. In
1976; Jayarman, 1984). There is substantial evidence that the addition, measures of regional cerebral blood flow in caudate,
PET and cognition in Huntington’s disease 2209

putamen and thalamus, derived from single photon emission between 2 and 8 years (mean 6 SD 5 5.6 6 2.5 years).
computed tomography, correlated with performance in several Exclusion criteria for the control subjects included history of
of the cognitive tasks. Interestingly, the best overall predictor mental disorder, significant somatic condition, abnormal MRI
of cognitive performance was a global measure of atrophy, and current or previous abuse of drugs or alcohol. No subject
namely the percentage of cerebrospinal fluid volume in the was on medication, and subjects were not allowed to drink
whole brain. These results suggest a more global influence any alcohol during 48 h prior to the testing.
of an impaired cortico-striato-thalamocortical system on The Huntington’s disease patients had a mean Mini-
Huntington’s disease-related cognitive deficits than has been Mental State Examination (Folstein et al., 1975) score of
implied in prior research. 25.8 6 1.3, indicating a mild to moderate global cognitive
The present investigation represents an extension of the impairment. One Huntington’s disease patient had depressive
PET and MRI study reported by Ginovart et al. (1997) (see symptoms at the time of the investigation.
Table 1). The main aim was to explore the relationship
between dopamine neurotransmission parameters and
cognitive performance in Huntington’s disease patients and
MRI and PET assessment
A detailed description of the MRI and PET assessment was
normal control subjects. To our knowledge, this is the first
provided by Ginovart et al. (1997). Briefly, a head fixation
attempt to relate dopaminergic PET markers to cognitive
system was used which allowed transfer of positioning from
functioning in Huntington’s disease. An additional purpose
MRI to PET, and repeated PET assessments with the same
was to relate volumetric MRI measurements to cognitive
positioning of the head (Bergström et al., 1981). MRI
performance. In this way, potential differences between the
examinations were performed using an MR Signa Advantage
neurotransmitter parameters and volumetric measurements
System (General Electric, Signa 1.5 T) at the Karolinska
with regard to the effects on cognitive functioning could be
Hospital, providing 124 contiguous slices of the entire brain
determined. The cognitive battery was designed to tap a wide
with a resolution in the axial plane of 1.5 mm.
variety of cognitive domains known to be implicated in
PET studies were performed on a Siemens ECAT Exact
Huntington’s disease. The battery involved tests of executive
HR 47 (Wienhard et al., 1994). Each subject participated in
function, visuospatial ability, episodic memory, verbal
three PET measurements performed on the same day. The
fluency, perceptual speed, and reasoning. Although we
PET measurements followed the procedure described by
expected a general relationship between the biological and
Farde et al. (1994, 1995). The radioligands used were
cognitive variables, a final objective was to examine the
[11C]SCH 23390 for D1 receptors, [11C]raclopride for D2
relative predictive power of the biological indices for
receptors and [11C]β-CIT for DAT (Farde et al., 1986, 1987,
performance in the respective cognitive tests.
1994, 1995). Regions of interest were drawn manually on
the MRI images. The following structures were delineated:
caudate (the head), putamen, thalamus, frontal cortex,
Method temporal cortex and cerebellum. The MRI-defined regions
of interest were used to calculate structural volumes. The
Subjects
regions of interest drawn on the MRI images were transferred
Five patients with Huntington’s disease (three men and two
to the corresponding PET images to obtain time curves for
women) and five healthy control subjects (three men and
regional brain radioactivity.
two women) were included in the study. The subjects gave
[11C]SCH 23390 and [11C]raclopride binding to D1 and D2
informed consent after the nature and possible consequences
receptors, respectively, was calculated using a ratio method
of the study were described, and the study was approved by the
(Farde et al., 1986, 1995). The cerebellum was used as a
Ethics and Radiation Safety Committees of The Karolinska
reference region for the free radioligand concentration (F),
Hospital, Stockholm. The groups were matched on age and
since this region contains negligible densities of D1 and D2
educational background. The Huntington’s disease group had
receptors (Hall et al., 1988; Cortés et al., 1989). Specific
a mean age (6 SD) of 49.4 6 7.6 years and had completed
binding (B) was defined as the total binding in a region of
an average of 11.5 6 1.5 years of formal education. The
interest reduced by F. The B : F ratio was calculated for
control subjects had a mean age of 48.0 6 7.8 years and
each region of interest, providing an index of the density of
averaged 11.9 6 0.7 years of schooling. The clinical diagnosis
available receptors (Farde et al., 1986, 1995). [11C]β-CIT
of Huntington’s disease was based on the presence of the
was analysed using a multiple-time graphic analysis (Patlak
typical motor and cognitive symptoms in patients with a
et al., 1983; Patlak and Blasberg, 1985). This analysis
family history of Huntington’s disease. The diagnosis was
provides an influx constant (Ki), which is linearly related to
confirmed by molecular genetic analysis showing a CAG
the density of the DAT (Ginovart et al., 1997).
repeat length of .37 in the gene locus for Huntington’s
disease. The exact number of CAG repeats was, however,
not known to any of the investigators, according to the Cognitive testing
Huntington Study Group policy on non-disclosure of CAG The cognitive test battery was administered by an experienced
trinucleotide repeat length. The duration of illness varied psychometrician (T.-B.R.-W.) at the Stockholm Gerontology
2210 L. Bäckman et al.

Reseach Centre. Subjects were tested individually. The time word every 5 s. Subjects were told to remember as many
elapsed between the brain imaging and the cognitive words as possible for a subsequent free recall test. The words
assessment averaged 1.60 6 0.89 days for the Huntington’s were presented bimodally; that is, they were shown on printed
disease patients and 2.00 6 2.23 days for the control subjects cards consecutively, and simultaneously read aloud by the
(P . 0.50). The total testing time for the cognitive battery examiner. Following presentation of the last word in the list,
was ~3 h. an oral free recall test was given. The recall task was
The test battery was designed to measure a broad spectrum self-paced.
of cognitive functions, including executive functions [Trail
Making Test (TMT), Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
and Tower of Hanoi], episodic memory [word recall and Controlled Oral Word Association Test
Rey–Osterrieth’s Complex Figure (R-OCF)—memory]; The Controlled Oral Word Association Test (Benton and
visuospatial ability (Block Design and R-OCF—copy); verbal Hamsher, 1989) was administered to assess verbal fluency.
fluency (Controlled Oral Word Association Test), perceptual In this test, subjects are asked to produce as many words as
speed (Digit Symbol) and reasoning (Picture Arrangement). possible beginning with the letters F, A and S, with the
The respective tests used are described below. exception of proper names, numbers, and words with the
same suffix. One minute was allowed for each letter.

Picture Arrangement, Block Design and Digit Rey–Osterrieth’s Complex Figure


Symbol The R-OCF (Rey, 1941; Osterreith, 1944) was given in two
Three tests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale— parts: copy and memory. Subjects were given a blank sheet
Revised (WAIS-R) (Wechsler, 1981) were used: Picture of paper, and another sheet of paper on which the R-OCF
Arrangement, Block Design and Digit Symbol. Standard was printed. They were instructed to copy the figure as
rules for administration were employed, and the tests were accurately as possible, with no time restrictions. Following
scored according to WAIS-R criteria. The maximum score completion, the examiner removed both the original and the
for Picture Arrangement was 20, for Block Design it was 51 copied figure. Completion time was recorded. After a
and for Digit Symbol 93. retention interval of 4 min during which the Controlled Oral
Word Association Test was completed, subjects were again
given a blank sheet of paper, and asked to reproduce the R-
Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B OCF from memory. Recall was self-paced. The copy and
A version of the TMT from the Halstead–Reitan Battery memory tasks of the R-OCF were scored according to
(Halstead, 1947; Reitan and Davidson, 1974; Reitan, 1979) standardized criteria (Taylor, 1959; Lezak, 1995). The
was given in two parts, A and B. For both parts, subjects maximum score for each of these variables was 36.
were presented with a white sheet of paper on which circles
were distributed. In part A, the circles were numbered from
1 to 25 and subjects were asked to draw lines to connect the
The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Subjects were given two packs of 64 cards in the WCST
25 circles in correct order (i.e. 1–2–3– . . . –25). In part B,
(Berg, 1948; Grant and Berg, 1948; Milner, 1963). One to
the circles contained numbers from 1 to 13 and letters from
four instances of each of four symbols in four different
A to L. Subjects were instructed to connect the consecutively
colours were printed on each card (e.g. one red triangle, two
numbered and alphabetically lettered circles, by alternating
green stars, three yellow crosses and four blue circles). The
between the two sequences (i.e. 1–A–2–B– . . . –L–13). In
subject’s task was to sort the cards according to a principle
both parts, subjects were told to connect the circles as fast
inferred from the examiner’s responses to the subject’s
as possible. The number of seconds needed to finish each
placement of the cards. The test involved three principles for
part was registered. The first error observed was immediately
sorting: colour, symbol or number. The examiner responded
pointed out by the examiner, and the subject was required to
only ‘right’ or ‘wrong’ according to the subject’s placement
correct the error. Thereafter, the subject was asked to continue
of the card, and did not answer any questions. After 10
in the proper sequence. From the second error onwards the
consecutive correct placements, the examiner shifted the
subject was not corrected. Performance time was unlimited.
principle. The test continued until the subject had made six
runs of ten correct placements, or when all 128 cards were
placed. In the current study, the number of perseverative
Word recall errors was used as the criterion variable.
A total of 20 concrete nouns from different taxonomic
categories were used. The 20 words were equivalent with
respect to visual and tactual imagery, meaningfulness and The Tower of Hanoi
frequency, as determined from a normative Swedish study The puzzle of the Tower of Hanoi (Butters et al., 1985)
(Molander, 1984). The list was presented at a rate of one consisted of three pegs, forming a triangle on a plastic square
PET and cognition in Huntington’s disease 2211

foot and five circular plastic blocks. The starting peg was group. The only exception to this pattern was word recall,
positioned next to the subject where all five blocks were in which the group difference did not approach conventional
arranged, according to size, to form a tower (the smallest significance.
block on the top and the largest on the bottom). Subjects
were asked to move the blocks as fast as possible from the
starting peg to either of the two other pegs, maintaining the
tower form. They were allowed to move only one block at Relationships between brain imaging
a time, and could never place a larger block on the top of a parameters and cognitive performance
smaller one. A minimum of 31 moves was needed for optimal Blockwise hierarchical regression analysis
solution of the task. Completion time was registered. The outcomes of the analyses of the predictive effects of the
caudate and putamen data were identical; when the block
involving D1, D2 and DAT was entered first in the equation,
Data analysis it made a significant contribution to all cognitive variables
The cognitive data were entered into separate analyses (all P , 0.05), except perseverations in the WCST and word
of variance (ANOVAs), with group (Huntington’s disease recall (all P . 0.20). The amount of variance accounted for
patients, control subjects) as a between-subjects variable. by this block was considerable, with r2-values ranging from
For each statistically reliable effect, η2 was calculated to 0.67 to 0.91. The volume data did not add to the amount of
determine the proportion of variance in cognitive performance explained variance in any instance (all P . 0.20). However,
that was accounted for by group membership. when the order of entry of predictors was reversed, the
We used the dopamine parameters and brain volume data volume data contributed reliably to all cognitive measures
initially reported by Ginovart et al. (1997) as correlates of (all P , 0.05), except word recall, perseverative errors,
cognitive performance. These relationships may best be TMT-A and TMT-B (all P . 0.20). The r2-values for the
determined using homogeneous groups, due to potential volume data varied between 0.48 and 0.70 across the seven
aggregation bias for combined data. However, examining tasks for which the relationship was reliable. In these
within-group relationships was not judged to be feasible in analyses, there was no significant contribution of the block
the present study, because of the relatively small sample of dopamine parameters (all P . 0.10). This pattern of
sizes. Thus, in order to maximize statistical power, the data results can be understood from the fact that there were
used in the regression analyses were collapsed across group. sizeable correlations among the data on D1, D2 and DAT,
To determine the predictive value of the dopamine and brain volume in both caudate (r2-values ranging from
neurotransmission and morphological data for cognitive 0.76 to 0.94, all P , 0.05) and putamen (r2-values ranging
performance, we first conducted blockwise hierarchical from 0.73 to 0.89, all P , 0.05). Thus, once the blocks of
regression analyses for each structure and cognitive variable. dopaminergic markers or volume variables were taken into
To examine the relative influence of the dopamine and brain account, not much variance was left that could be explained
volume data on cognitive performance, two sets of regressions by the remaining block. Yet it should be noted that the
were performed for caudate and putamen, In the first set, the neurotransmitter parameters accounted for a greater portion
D1, D2 and DAT data were entered first in one block, followed of the variance in cognitive performance than the striatal
by the brain volume data in the second block. In the second volume variables across all measures. In addition, the
set, order of entry between the two blocks was reversed. For neurotransmitter parameters made a contribution to nine
frontal and temporal cortex, the same strategy was applied, cognitive measures, whereas the volume variables were
although only D1 and brain volume data were available. related to seven cognitive measures.
Finally, for thalamus, only volume data were available Frontal volume was related to performance in all cognitive
to predict cognitive performance. In additional regression tasks, irrespective of whether it was entered before or after
analyses, we investigated the relative importance of the D1 in frontal cortex (r2-values ranging between 0.50 and
different biological variables for different cognitive tasks, by 0.80; P , 0.05). D1 in frontal cortex was unrelated to
entering all biological variables in stepwise forward fashion. performance across all measures (P . 0.50). Thalamic
volume contributed significantly to performance in verbal
fluency, R-OCF-copy, R-OCF-memory and Tower of Hanoi
Results (r2-values between 0.41 and 0.54; all P , 0.05), but not to
the remaining cognitive tasks (P . 0.20). Finally, when
entered first in the equation, D1 in temporal cortex made a
Cognitive data reliable contribution to performance in Picture Arrangement,
The performance by Huntington’s disease patients and control TMT-A, TMT-B and the Tower of Hanoi (P , 0.05), with
subjects in the cognitive tests is shown in Table 2. As can no explanatory variance added by temporal volume (P .
be seen from this table, ANOVAs revealed significant effects 0.10). However, as was true with caudate and putamen,
favouring the control subjects for 10 of the 11 tests, suggesting reversal of order of entry between the dopamine and volume
a rather global cognitive deficit in the Huntington’s disease measures resulted in the opposite pattern of results, again
2212 L. Bäckman et al.

Table 2 Summary statistics of cognitive performance in patients with Huntington’s disease


and control subjects
Test Huntington’s disease Control subjects F P η2

Digit Symbol 23.00 6 6.32 49.40 6 8.44 31.31 0.001 0.80


Block Design 18.40 6 7.83 38.60 6 9.76 13.03 0.01 0.62
Picture Arrangement 4.80 6 4.60 18.20 6 1.31 39.21 0.001 0.83
Trail Making, part A 102.60 6 43.32 32.60 6 8.08 12.61 0.01 0.61
Trail Making, part B 222.40 6 85.48 60.60 6 8.91 17.72 0.01 0.69
Tower of Hanoi 3081.20 6 675.16 906.60 6 387.97 38.99 0.001 0.83
Verbal Fluency 20.00 6 7.79 51.40 6 8.14 38.32 0.001 0.83
ROCF—copy 29.00 6 2.00 35.20 6 1.10 36.96 0.001 0.83
ROCF—memory 15.20 6 5.26 28.40 6 2.79 24.54 0.01 0.75
Word Recall 8.00 6 3.39 10.60 6 1.67 2.36 .0.05 –
Perseverative Errors 35.60 6 21.85 7.60 6 2.70 8.09 0.05 0.50

Data are shown as mean 6 SD.

Table 3 Stepwise regression analyses for predicting cognitive performance


Task Predictor variables Incremental r2 Cumulative r2 β P

Digit Symbol DAT putamen 0.74 0.74 0.86 0.001


Block Design DAT putamen 0.66 0.66 0.82 0.01
Picture Arrangement DAT putamen 0.78 0.78 0.88 0.001
Tower of Hanoi DAT putamen 0.82 0.82 20.91 0.001
D1 temporal 0.09 0.91 20.42 0.03
Verbal Fluency DAT putamen 0.78 0.78 0.88 0.001
D1 caudate 0.11 0.89 0.39 0.04
Trail Making, part A DAT putamen 0.63 0.63 20.79 0.01
Trail Making, part B Volume frontal 0.80 0.80 20.90 0.001
Ray Osterreith, copy Volume putamen 0.65 0.65 0.80 0.01
Ray Osterreith, memory Volume frontal 0.69 0.69 0.83 0.01
Word Recall Volume frontal 0.51 0.51 0.72 0.02
Perseverative Errors Volume frontal 0.70 0.70 20.84 0.01

DAT 5 dopamine transporter binding.

suggesting substantial colinearity between the two types of Although the strong relationships between the biological
biological indices. indices and cognitive performance were largely due to
substantial group differences for both sets of variables,
positive associations were also seen within groups. Illustrative
Stepwise regression analyses examples of these relationships are portrayed in Figs 1 and
After having established that many of the biological variables 2. These figures show the relationship between DAT in
and, in particular, that the data pertaining to dopamine putamen and Tower of Hanoi and frontal volume and TMT-
parameters in caudate and putamen as well as frontal volume B, respectively.
were generally related to cognitive performance, we evaluated
the relative importance of the biological indices for specific
cognitive tasks. To accomplish this goal, we conducted Discussion
additional regression analyses for each of the cognitive It is well established that Huntington’s disease is associated
variables. To control for colinearity among the biological with specific neuropathology in striatal and neocortical brain
variables, both within and between brain areas, all biological regions (Graveland et al., 1985; Ferrante and Kowall, 1987;
variables were entered in stepwise forward fashion. The Webb and Trzepacz, 1987; Goto et al., 1989; Filoux et al.,
outcome of the stepwise regressions is shown in Table 3. 1990; Jernigan et al., 1991; Richfield et al., 1991) as well
The general impression from this table is that DAT in putamen as cognitive impairment (Butters et al., 1978; Fedio et al.,
and frontal volume were the most important predictors of 1979; Josiassen et al., 1983; Taylor and Hansotia, 1983;
cognitive performance, with DAT in putamen being the major Rosser and Hodges, 1994; Lange et al., 1995; Lawrence
predictor in six tasks, and frontal volume being the sole et al., 1996). In the research reported here, we sought to link
predictor in four tasks. The only exception to this pattern the neuropathology of Huntington’s disease to the cognitive
was R-OCF-copy, in which the volume measure of putamen deficits that accompany this disease. This was accomplished
emerged as a single predictor of performance. by examining the relationship of D1, D2 and DAT densities
PET and cognition in Huntington’s disease 2213

There were several intriguing relationships between the


neurotransmitter and brain volume measures and cognitive
performance. The D1, D2 and DAT data for both caudate and
putamen showed strong relationships to all cognitive tasks,
except word recall and perseverations in the WCST. Similar
relationships were seen between the morphological data for
the striatal structures and cognitive performance, although
there was no association with the TMT in these analyses.
Note that the order of entering the dopamine binding
parameters and the volume data determined which type of
biological index would show a relationship to cognitive
performance. Specifically, when the dopaminergic markers
were entered first in the regressions, the volume data made no
additional contribution to the cognitive variation. Conversely,
when the volume data were entered first, D1, D2 and DAT
did not add any explanatory variance. This pattern of results
Fig. 1 Relationship between DAT in putamen and Tower of indicates that both the neurotransmitter and volume data may
Hanoi (completion time) in patients with Huntington’s disease serve as markers of the pathophysiological process that
(closed squares) and control subjects (open squares). underlies impaired cognitive functioning in Huntington’s
disease.
Furthermore, the volume of the frontal cortex was strongly
related to performance in all 11 cognitive tasks, whereas
temporal volume and D1 in temporal cortex were related to
performance in four tasks only: Tower of Hanoi, Picture
Arrangement, TMT-A and TMT-B. As with the analysis of
the striatal data, order of entry determined, for the temporal
cortex, which type of index that would show a reliable
relationship with cognitive performance. Finally, thalamic
volume was associated with performance in four tasks:
R-OCF-copy, R-OCF-memory, verbal fluency and Tower
of Hanoi.
The overall pattern of results indicates that the
dopaminergic markers as well as the brain volume data were
related to performance in the majority of cognitive tasks
employed. Among the biological indices, measures of frontal
volume and dopamine binding in the striatum showed the
Fig. 2 Relationship between frontal volume and Trail Making, strongest and most consistent relationships with cognitive
part B (completion time) in patients with Huntington’s disease performance. It is arguable that the cognitive deficits shown
(closed squares) and control subjects (open squares). by the current Huntington’s disease patients are largely
attributable to striatal and frontal lesions, given that these
as well as measurements of structural brain volumes (Ginovart patients exhibited marked reductions in dopamine binding in
et al., 1997) to cognitive performance in Huntington’s disease the striatum as well as frontal volume (Ginovart et al., 1997).
patients and control subjects. Thus, the present results are consistent with the view that
The cognitive data indicated Huntington’s disease-related Huntington’s disease may be characterized as a frontostriatal
impairments across all domains assessed (i.e. executive dementia (Robbins et al., 1994; Lawrence et al., 1996).
function, visuospatial skill, episodic memory, verbal fluency, Whereas the observed relationships between striatal and
perceptual speed and reasoning). In fact, with the exception frontal volumes and cognitive impairment in Huntington’s
of word recall, all cognitive measures showed statistically disease replicate prior research (Shoulson et al., 1982; Sax
reliable differences between Huntington’s disease patients and et al., 1983; Starkstein et al., 1988, 1992; Bamford et al.,
control subjects. However, for word recall, the performance 1989; Brandt et al., 1995), the associations between the
advantage of the control subjects did not approach dopamine neurotransmission parameters and cognitive
conventional significance. This pattern of results, indicating performance constitute novel findings.
a rather global cognitive deficit in mild to moderate stages Although the cognitive data pattern may reflect
of Huntington’s disease, is in agreement with prior research Huntington’s disease-related lesions at several levels of the
(for reviews, see Brandt and Butters, 1986; Brown and frontostriatal circuitry (Lawrence et al., 1996), stepwise
Marsden, 1988). regression analyses, which controlled for colinearity among
2214 L. Bäckman et al.

predictor variables, showed that some relationships between the current findings both with regard to Huntington’s disease-
the biological and cognitive variables were stronger than related effects and potential general effects.
others. In particular, DAT in putamen emerged as the most
powerful predictor in Digit Symbol, Block Design, Picture
Arrangement, Tower of Hanoi, verbal fluency and TMT-A, Acknowledgements
whereas frontal volume showed the strongest relationship We wish to thank the staff of the PET centre at the Karolinska
with R-OCF-memory, word recall, TMT-B, and perseverative hospital for their assistance. This research was supported
errors from the WCST. To the extent that the striatum plays by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health
a critical role in fast cognitive performance (Cummings, (MH41205–8) and the Swedish Medical Research Council
1986; Brown and Marsden, 1988; Almkvist et al., 1992; (09114) to L.F., the NHR Foundation and Gadelius
Crosson, 1992), it is noteworthy that the rate at which Minnesfond to A.L., and the Swedish Council for Research
information could be processed or retrieved is extremely in the Humanities and the Social Sciences (F 67/95) to L.B.
important in all tasks that were predicted by the DAT
putamen variable.
Of further interest is that both lesion studies (Moscovitch, References
1982; Schacter, 1987; Incisa della Rochetta and Milner, 1993; Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL. Parallel organization of
Wheeler et al., 1995) and PET activation research with functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex.
normal subjects (Squire et al., 1992; Shallice et al., 1994; [Review]. Annu Rev Neurosci 1986; 9: 357–81.
Tulving et al., 1994; Rugg et al., 1996) indicate a strong Almkvist O, Wahlund LO, Andersson-Lundman G, Basun H,
frontal involvement in the encoding and subsequent retrieval Bäckman L. White-matter hyperintensity and neuropsychological
of information from episodic memory, as assessed in, for functions in dementia and healthy aging. Arch Neurol 1992; 49:
example, word recall and R-OCF-memory. Similarly, there 626–32.
is evidence that proficient executive functioning, as assessed
Andreasen NC, Flaum M, Swayze V 2d, O’Leary DS, Alliger R,
in tasks such as the WCST and TMT-B, draws on the integrity
Cohen G, et al. Intelligence and brain structure in normal individuals
of the frontal cortex (Milner, 1963; Weinberger et al., 1986; [see comments]. Am J Psychiatry 1993; 150: 130–4. Comment in:
Janowsky et al., 1989; Segalowitz et al., 1992; Tranel et al., Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 456–7.
1994; Nagahama et al., 1996). Although these specific
relationships may be theoretically plausible, we would like Bamford KA, Caine ED, Kido DK, Plassche WM, Shoulson I.
Clinical-pathologic correlation in Huntington’s disease: a
to caution that (i) the overall pattern of results indicated a
neuropsychological and computed tomography study. Neurology
relationship between most biological and cognitive variables,
1989; 39: 796–801.
and (ii) the sample sizes used to infer these relationships
were relatively small. Thus, at present, the safest conclusion Benton AL, Hamsher K de S. Multilingual aphasia examination.
is that the current data reflect a general influence of an Iowa City, Iowa: AJA Associates, 1989.
impaired frontostriatal system on the cognitive deficits that Berg EA. A simple objective test for measuring flexibility in
accompany Huntington’s disease. The fact that the present thinking. J Gen Psychol 1948; 39: 15–22.
results suggest that lesions at multiple sites in the cortico-
Bergström M, Boethius J, Eriksson L, Greitz T, Ribbe T, Widen
striato-thalamocortical circuitry (i.e. caudate, putamen, frontal
L. Head fixation device for reproducible position alignment in
cortex and thalamus) may be associated with Huntington’s transmission CT and positron emission tomography. J Comput
disease-related cognitive impairment should be highlighted. Assist Tomogr 1981; 5: 136–41.
Finally, it should be noted that the strong relationships
between the biological and cognitive variables were largely Björklund A, Lindvall O. Dopamine-containing systems in the
CNS. In: Björklund A, Hökfelt T, editors. Handbook of chemical
due to marked group differences for both sets of variables.
neuroanatomy, Vol. 2: classical transmitters in the CNS, Part 1.
Unfortunately, the relatively small sample sizes made within-
Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1984: 55–122.
group regressions less meaningful. However, an interesting
observation was that, for many cognitive tasks, relationships Boll TJ, Heaton R, Reitan RM. Neuropsychological and emotional
were seen also within groups. This pattern was especially correlates of Huntington’s chorea. J Nerv Ment Dis 1974; 158: 61–9.
pronounced in the patients, possibly due to the generally Brandt J, Butters N. The neuropsychology of Huntington’s disease.
greater variability for both the biological and cognitive Trends Neurosci 1986; 9: 118–20.
variables in this group (see Figs 1 and 2). These findings
Brandt J, Bylsma FW, Aylward EH, Rothlind J, Gow CA. Impaired
may suggest a disease-related as well as a general association
source memory in Huntington’s disease and its relation to basal
between the neurotransmitter parameters and brain volumes, ganglia atrophy. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17: 868–77.
on the one hand, and cognitive performance, on the other.
Evidence for a relationship between volume measurements Brouwers P, Cox C, Martin A, Chase T, Fedio P. Differential
of various cortical and subcortical structures and performance perceptual-spatial impairment in Huntington’s and Alzheimer9s
dementias. Arch Neurol 1984; 41: 1073–6.
on intelligence tests has been reported in healthy adults
(Andreasen et al., 1993). Future research should substantiate Brown RG, Marsden CD. ‘Subcortical dementia’: the
PET and cognition in Huntington’s disease 2215

neuropsychological evidence. [Review]. Neuroscience 1988; 25: et al. Visual attention and perception in patients with Huntington’s
363–87. disease: comparisons with other subcortical and cortical dementias.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1995; 17: 654–67.
Bruyn GW, Dots GT, Dom R. Huntington’s chorea: current
neuropathological status. In: Chase TN, Wexsler NS, Barbeau A, Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. ‘Mini-mental state’. A
editors. Huntington’s disease. New York: Raven Press, 1979: 83–93. practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the
clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189–98.
Butters N, Sax D, Montgomery K, Tarlow S. Comparison of the
neuropsychological deficits associated with early and advanced Gerfen CR. The neostriatal mosaic: striatal patch-matrix organization
Huntington’s disease. Arch Neurol 1978; 35: 585–9. is related to cortical lamination. Science 1989; 246: 385–8.
Butters N, Wolfe J, Martone M, Granholm E, Cermak LS. Memory Ginovart N, Lundin A, Farde L, Halldin C, Backman L, Swahn
disorders associated with Huntington’s disease: verbal recall, verbal CG, et al. PET study of pre- and post-synaptic dopaminergic markers
recognition and procedural memory. Neuropsychologia 1985; 23: for the neurodegenerative process in Huntington’s disease. Brain
729–43. 1997; 120: 501–14.
Caine ED, Ebert MH, Weingartner H. An outline for the analysis of Giros B, el Mestikawy S, Godinot N, Zheng K, Han H, Yang-Feng
dementia: the memory disorder of Huntington’s disease. Neurology T, et al. Cloning, pharmacological characterization, and chromosome
1977; 27: 1087–92. assignment of the human dopamine transporter. Mol Pharmacol
Cortés R, Camps M, Gueye B, Probst A, Palacios JM. Dopamine 1992; 42: 383–90.
receptors in human brain: Autoradiographic distribution of D1 and Goto S, Hirono A, Rojas-Corona RR. An immunohistochemical
D2 sites in Parkinson syndrome of different etiology. Brain Res investigation of the human neostriatum in Huntington’s disease.
1989; 483: 30–8. Ann Neurol 1989; 25: 298–304.
Crosson BA. Subcortical functions in language and memory. New Grant DA, Berg EA. A behavioral analysis of degree of
York: Guilford, 1992. reinforcement and ease of shifting to new responses in a Weigl-
Cummings JL. Subcortical dementia: neuropsychology, type card-sorting problem. J Exp Psychol 1948; 38: 404–11.
neuropsychiatry, and pathophysiology. [Review]. Br J Psychiatry Graveland GA, Williams RS, DiFiglia M. Evidence for degenerative
1986; 149: 682–97. and regenerative changes in neostriatal spiny neurons in Huntington’s
Dom R, Malfroid M, Baro F. Neuropathology of Huntington’s disease. Science 1985; 227: 770–3.
chorea: studies of the ventrobasal complex of the thalamus. Graybiel A, Ragsdale CW Jr. Fiber connections of the basal ganglia.
Neurology 1976; 26: 64–8. In: Cuenod M, Kreutzberg GW, Bloom FE, editors. Development
Farde L, Hall H, Ehrin E, Sedvall G. Quantitative analysis of D2 and chemical specificity of neurons. Progress in brain research, Vol.
dopamine receptor binding in the living human brain by PET. 51. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1979: 239–83.
Science 1986; 231: 258–61. Hägglund J, Aquilonius SM, Eckernäs SÅ, Hartvig P, Lundquist H,
Farde L, Halldin C, Stone-Elander S, Sedvall G. PET analysis of Gullberg P, et al. Dopamine receptor properties in Parkinson’s
human dopamine receptor subtypes using 11C-SCH 23390 and 11C disease and Huntington’s chorea evaluated by positron emission
raclopride. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987; 92: 278–84. tomography using 11C-N-methyl-spiperone. Acta Neurol Scand
1987; 75: 87–94.
Farde L, Halldin C, Müller L, Suhara T, Karlsson P, Hall H. PET
study of (11C)b-CIT binding to monoamine transporters in the Hall H, Farde L, Sedvall G. Human dopamine receptor subtypes—
monkey and human brain. Synapse 1994; 16: 93–103. in vitro binding analysis using 3H-SCH 23390 and 3H raclopride. J
Neural Transm 1988; 73: 7–21.
Farde L, Hall H, Pauli S, Halldin C. Variability in D2 dopamine
receptor density and affinity: a PET study with [11C]raclopride in Hall H, Sedvall G, Magnusson O, Kopp J, Halldin C, Farde L.
man. Synapse 1995; 20: 200–8. Distribution of D1- and D2-dopamine receptors, and dopamine and
its metabolites in the human brain. Neuropsychopharmacology 1994;
Fedio P, Cox CS, Neophytides A, Canal-Frederick G, Chase TN. 11: 245–56.
Neuropsychological profile of Huntington’s disease: patients and
those at risk. In: Chase TN, Wexsler NS, Barbeau A, editors. Halstead WC. Brain and intelligence. Chicago: University of
Huntington’s disease. New York: Raven Press, 1979: 239–55. Chicago Press, 1947.
Ferrante RJ, Kowall NW. Tyrosine hydroxylase-like Harris GJ, Aylward EH, Peyser CE, Pearlson GD, Brandt J, Roberts-
immunoreactivity is distributed in the matrix compartment of normal Twillie JV, et al. Single photon emission computed tomographic
human and Huntington’s disease striatum. Brain Res 1987; 416: blood flow and magnetic resonance volume imaging of basal ganglia
141–6. in Huntington’s disease. Arch Neurol 1996; 53: 316–24.
Filloux F, Wagster MV, Folstein S, Price DL, Hedreen JC, Dawson Incisa della Rocchetta A, Milner B. Strategic search and retrieval
TM, et al. Nigral dopamine type-1 receptors are reduced in inhibition: the role of the frontal lobes. Neuropsychologia 1993;
Huntington’s disease: a postmortem autoradiographic study using 31: 503–24.
(3H) SCH 23390 and correlation with (3H) forskolin binding. Exp
Janowsky JS, Shimamura AP, Kritchevsky M, Squire LR. Cognitive
Neurol 1990; 110: 219–27.
impairment following frontal lobe damage and its relevance to
Filoteo JV, Delis DC, Roman MJ, Demadura T, Ford E, Butters N, human amnesia. Behav Neurosci 1989; 103: 548–60.
2216 L. Bäckman et al.

Jayaraman A. Thalamostriate projections—an overview. In: Parent A. Extrinsic connections of the basal ganglia. [Review].
McKenzie JS, Klemm RE, Wilcock LN, editors. The basal ganglia. Trends Neurosci 1990; 13: 254–8.
Advances in behavioral biology, Vol. 27. New York: Plenum Press,
Parent A, Hazrati LN. Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I.
1984: 69–86.
The cortico-basal-ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop. [Review]. Brain
Jernigan TL, Salmon DP, Butters N, Hesselink JR. Cerebral structure Res Brain Res Rev 1995; 20: 91–127.
on MRI. Part II: specific changes in Alzheimer9s and Huntington’s
Patlak CS, Blasberg RG. Graphical evaluation of blood-to-brain
diseases. Biol Psychiatry 1991; 29: 68–81.
transfer constants from multiple-time uptake data: generalizations.
Josiassen RC, Curry LM, Mancall EL. Development of J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1985; 5: 584–90.
neuropsychological deficits in Huntington’s disease. Arch Neurol
Patlak CS, Blasberg RG, Fenstermacher JD. Graphical evaluation
1983; 40: 791–6.
of blood-to-brain transfer constants from multiple-time uptake data.
Kuhar MJ, Sanchez-Roa P, Wong DF, Dannals RF, Grigoriadis DE, J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 1983; 3: 1–7.
Lew R, et al. Dopamine transporter: biochemistry, pharmacology
Reiner A, Albin RL, Anderson KD, D’Amato CJ, Penney JB, Young
and imaging. [Review]. Eur Neurol 1990; 30 Suppl 1: 15–20.
AB. Differential loss of striatal projection neurons in Huntington
Lange KW, Sahakian BJ, Quinn NP, Marsden CD, Robbins TW. disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1988; 85: 5733–7.
Comparison of executive and visuospatial memory function in
Reitan RM. Manual for administration of neuropsychological test
Huntington’s disease and dementia of Alzheimer type matched for
batteries for adults and children. Tucson (AZ): Reitan
degree of dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1995; 58:
Neuropsychological Laboratory, 1979.
598–606.
Reitan RM, Davison LA. Clinical neuropsychology: current status
Lawrence AD, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Rosser AE, Lange KW,
and applications. Washington (DC): V. H. Winston, 1974.
Robbins TW. Executive and mnemonic functions in early
Huntington’s disease. Brain 1996; 119: 1633–45. Rey A. L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie
traumatique. Arch Psychol 1941; 28: 286–340.
Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd ed. New York:
Oxford University Press, 1995. Richfield EK, O9Brien CF, Eskin T, Shoulson I. Heterogeneous
dopamine receptor changes in early and late Huntington’s disease.
Martone M, Butters N, Payne M, Becker JT, Sax DS. Dissociations
Neurosci Lett 1991; 132: 121–6.
between skill learning and verbal recognition in amnesia and
dementia. Arch Neurol 1984; 41: 965–70. Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Lange KW, Lees AJ, Leigh
PN, et al. Cognitive deficits in progressive supranuclear palsy,
Mayeux R, Stern Y, Rosen J, Benson F. Is ‘subcortical dementia’ a
Parkinson’s disease, and multiple system atrophy in tests sensitive
recognizable clinical entity? Ann Neurol 1983; 14: 278–83.
to frontal lobe dysfunction. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;
Milner B. Effects of different brain lesions on card sorting. Arch 57: 79–88.
Neurol 1963; 9: 90–100.
Rosser AE, Hodges JR. Initial letter and semantic category fluency
Mohr E, Brouwers P, Claus JJ, Mann UM, Fedio P, Chase TN. in Alzheimer9s disease, Huntington’s disease, and progressive
Visuospatial cognition in Huntington’s disease. Mov Disord 1991; supranuclear palsy [see comments]. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
6: 127–32. 1994; 57: 1389–94. Comment in: J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
1995; 58: 520–1.
Molander B. Imagery, visual and tactual dimensions of imagery, and
meaningfulness: Swedish norms for 858 nouns. (Umeå Psychological Rugg MD, Fletcher PC, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Dolan RJ.
Reports No. 178). Umeå (Sweden): University of Umeå, Department Differential activation of the prefrontal cortex in successful and
of Psychology, 1984. unsuccessful memory retrieval. Brain 1996; 119: 2073–83.
Moscovitch M. Multiple dissociations of function in amnesia. In: Sax DS, O9Donnell B, Butters N, Menzer L, Montgomery K, Kayne
Cermak LS, editor. Human memory and amnesia. Hillsdale (NJ): HL. Computed tomographic, neurologic, and neuropsychological
Lawrence Erlbaum, 1982: 337–70. correlates of Huntington’s disease. Int J Neurosci 1983; 18: 21–36.
Moses JA Jr, Golden CJ, Berger PA, Wisniewski AM. Schacter DL. Memory, amnesia, and frontal lobe dysfunction.
Neuropsychological deficits in early, middle, and late stage Psychobiology 1987; 15: 21–36.
Huntington’s disease as measured by the Luria-Nebraska
Sedvall G, Karlsson P, Lundin A, Anvret M, Suhara T, Halldin C,
Neuropsychological Battery. Int J Neurosci 1981; 14: 95–100.
et al. Dopamine D1 receptor number—a sensitive PET marker for
Murray AM, Ryoo H, Joyce JN. Visualization of dopamine D3-like early brain degeneration in Huntington’s disease. Eur Arch
receptors in human brain with (125I) epidepride. Eur J Pharmacol Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1994; 243: 249–55.
1992; 227: 443–5.
Seeman P, Guan HC, Van Tol HHM, Niznik HB. Low density of
Nagahama Y, Fukuyama H, Yamauchi H, Matsuzaki S, Konishi J, dopamine D4 receptors in Parkinson9s, schizophrenia, and control
Shibasaki H, et al. Cerebral activation during performance of a card brain striata. Synapse 1993; 14: 247–53.
sorting test. Brain 1996; 119: 1667–75.
Segalowitz SJ, Unsal A, Dywan J. CNV evidence for the
Osterrieth PA. Le test de copie d’une figure complexe. Arch Psychol distinctiveness of frontal and posterior neural processes in a traumatic
1944; 30: 206–356. brain-injured population. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1992; 14: 545–65.
PET and cognition in Huntington’s disease 2217

Shallice T, Fletcher P, Frith CD, Grasby P, Frackowiak RS, Dolan encoding/retrieval asymmetry in episodic memory: Positron
RJ. Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal emission tomography findings [see comments]. [Review]. Proc Natl
episodic memory. Nature 1994; 368: 633–5. Acad Sci USA 1994; 91: 2016–20. Comment in: Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 1994; 91: 1989–91.
Shoulson I, Plassche W, Odoroff C. Huntington disease: caudate
atrophy parallels functional impairment [abstract]. Neurology 1982; Turjanski N, Weeks R, Dolan R, Harding AE, Brooks DJ. Striatal
32 Suppl: A143. D1 and D2 receptor binding in patients with Huntington’s disease
and other choreas. Brain 1995; 118: 689–96.
Squire LR, Ojemann JG, Miezin FM, Petersen SE, Videen TO,
Raichle ME. Activation of the hippocampus in normal humans: a Ungerstedt U. Stereotaxic mapping of the monoamine pathways in
functional anatomical study of memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA the rat brain. Acta Physiol Scand 1971; Suppl 367: 1–48.
1992; 89: 1837–41. Webb M, Trzepacz PT. Huntington’s disease: correlations of mental
Starkstein SE, Brandt J, Folstein S, Strauss M, Berthier ML, status with chorea. Biol Psychiatry 1987; 22: 751–61.
Pearlson GD, et al. Neuropsychological and neuroradiological Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-
correlates in Huntington’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation, 1981.
1988; 51: 1259–63.
Weinberger DR, Berman KF, Zec RF. Physiologic dysfunction of
Starkstein SE, Brandt J, Bylsma F, Peyser C, Folstein M, Folstein dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia [see comments]. Arch
SE. Neuropsychological correlates of brain atrophy in Huntington’s Gener Psychiatry 1986; 43: 111–24. Comment in: Arch Gener
disease: a magnetic resonance imaging study. Neuroradiology 1992; Psychiatry 1991; 48: 282–3.
34: 487–9.
Wexler NS. Perceptual-motor, cognitive, and emotional
Taylor EM. Psychological appraisal of children with cerebral deficits. characteristics of persons at risk for Huntington’s disease. In: Chase
Cambridge (MA): Harvard University Press, 1959. TN, Wexler NS, Barbeau A, editors. Huntington’s disease. New
Taylor HG, Hansotia P. Neuropsychological testing of Huntington’s York: Raven Press, 1979: 257–71.
patients: clues to progression. J Nerv Ment Dis 1983; 171: 492–6. Wheeler MA, Stuss DT, Tulving E. Frontal lobe damage produces
Tranel D, Anderson SW, Benton A. Development of the concept of episodic memory impairment. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1995; 1:
‘executive function’ and its relationship to the frontal lobes. In: 525–36.
Boller F, Grafman J, editors. Handbook of neuropsychology, Vol. Wienhard K, Dahlbom M, Eriksson L, Michel C, Bruckbauer T,
9. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1994; 125–48. Pietrzyk U, et al. The ECAT EXACT HR: Performance of a new
Tröster AI, Salmon DP, McCullough D, Butters N. A comparison high resolution positron scanner. J Comput Assist Tomogr 1994;
of the category fluency deficits associated with Alzheimer9s and 18: 110–8.
Huntington’s disease. Brain Lang 1989; 37: 500–13.
Tulving E, Kapur S, Craik FI, Moscovitch M, Houle S. Hemispheric Received May 22, 1997. Accepted July 21, 1997

You might also like