You are on page 1of 2

PROFILE

https://edition.pagesuite-professional.co.uk/html5/reader/production/default.aspx?pubname=&edid=558600cb-8de9-4616-9242-f8f3b41da315

Specific Language Impairment (SLI)


This article originally appeared in the May 2018
issue of Open Access Government

versus Speech Sound Disorders (SSD)


The important differences between Specific Language Impairment (SLI) in children and Speech
Sound Disorders (SSD) in children are placed under the spotlight by Mabel L. Rice, Fred & Virginia

A
Merrill Distinguished Professor of Advanced Studies at the University of Kansas

round the world young chil- they are hearing. Their first job is to SSD can be obvious to adult listeners
dren are expected to learn refine them to match the ones they of young children. Young children’s
the language they overhear in need and to drop the ones that may attempts to talk can be unintelligible,
conversations around them. This is a not matter and to develop the motor especially when they are very young.
robust, spontaneous ability of humans, control needed to do that. If unintelligibility persists as children
unlike, for example, reading, which age, it becomes noticeable and a
must be explicitly taught. One promi- Humans at birth are equipped with matter of concern because it is
nent scholar of children’s language motor movements for breathing, not “typical.” Some mispronunciations
acquisition once put it this way: “… sucking and swallowing (basic func- are understandable but regarded as
there is virtually no way to prevent it tions for survival), along with a wide immature, such as “wabbit” for “rabbit”,
from happening short of raising a child range of sounds. Some vocalisations “thoup” for “soup”, or “bawoon” for
in a barrel.”1 serve communicative purposes, and “balloon.” Scholars have tracked the
some are biological (such as burps or order in which children learn their
Although true of most children, chil- coughs). Between one and two years speech sounds and have developed
dren with Specific Language Impair- of age, babies master more refined age norms for evaluating whether a
ment (SLI) are the exceptions to this tongue, lips and palatal movements child meets age expectations4. The
assumption. These are children who do needed for speaking the words and prevalence of SSD in 4-6-year-old
not have overt neurodevelopmental sentences of a language. This children in population-based cohorts
disorders, hearing impairments, or requires a fine-tuned synchrony of is approximately 3-6%5 and the condi-
other obvious causes of developmental muscles, sound perception and the tion appears to resolve in 75% of
disorders and who live in ordinary cognitive centres of the brain. The children by age 66.
families. Yet they are later than other output is a sequenced speech
children in learning a language and pattern, such as what we hear in a People often assume SSD is the same
are at risk for persistent low language phone conversation. as SLI, such that children’s speech
abilities into adulthood. The best abilities reflect their underlying
estimates for the prevalence of SLI are The second part of the human language abilities or vice versa. This is
7-10% of children at school entry (5-6 language capacity is more covert, a not true. In the most precise study of
years). 2,3
matter of cognitive processes in the a population-based sample of 5-year-
brain that do not require a speech old children, the co-occurrence of
SLI is often confused with Speech production system. For example, deaf speech and language impairments,
Sound Disorders (SSD) in children. children can acquire a language once adjusting for age expectations,
Between one and five years of age system that can be expressed in phys- was estimated at less than 2%5. For
children are learning two distinctly ical signs of the hands, face and body the children with SLI, speech impair-
different parts of the human language postures. Language emerges in young ment was evident in approximately
capacity. One is the production of children first in short utterances 5-8% of the children. The authors
speech sounds needed in their native that lengthen with age. In English, lan- concluded that SSD and SLI are inde-
language. In the beginning, children guage emerges as one or two words pendent; they are not likely to co-occur.
can produce more sounds than they at a time, which relatively quickly Thus, SSD is not a diagnostic marker
need for the language or languages expand to phrases or sentences. of SLI and presumably, the two condi-

310
PROFILE

tions do not share a common causal says “why ah de wabbits wed?” The References

pathway. substitution of w/r in “rabbit” and 1. Pinker S. Language learnability and language development. Cam-

“red”, along with the omission of the bridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1984.

The non-overlap of SLI and SSD carries final /r/ in the word “are,” are not 2. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien
M. The prevalence of specific language impairment in kinder-
implications for public health services unusual speech errors in the speech garten children. J Speech Hear Res. 1997;40:1245-1260.
and for scientific studies of the nature of young boys.
3. Norbury CF, Gooch D, Wray C, et al. The impact of nonverbal ability
and origins of SLI and SSD. on prevalence and clinical presentation of language disorder:
Such errors are quite noticeable evidence from a population study. Journal of Child Psychology &
Psychiatry. 2016;57(11):1247-1257.
• A big issue for public health services although they often do not interfere
is that children with SLI are likely to with adults’ understanding of the 4. Goldman R, Fristoe M. Goldman Fristoe Test of Articulation-2
(2nd Edition). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service;
be overlooked as needing language intended meanings. The other boy, 2000.
intervention services, perhaps in part dressed in green, asks a question 5. Shriberg LD, Tomblin JB, McSweeny JL. Prevalence of speech
because of the fact that SSD may be formulated in the adult grammar as: delay in 6-year-old children and comorbidity with language

obvious to adults/caregivers, but SLI “why is/are that rabbit/those rabbits impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:1461-1481.

is not7. red?” The boy says “Why that red?”, a 6. Shriberg LD. Five subtypes of developmental phonological
disorders. Clinical Communication Disorders 1994;4:38-53.
sentence consistent with the gram-
• SSD is likely to resolve with age mar rules for children this age with
7. Rice ML. Specific Language Impairment in Children. https://ww-
wopenaccessgovernmentorg/specific-language-impairment-in-
(children are likely to “outgrow” it) SLI7. He demonstrates a deficiency in chi 2017.
whereas SLI is likely to persist into sentence structure, with the omission 8. Catts HW, Fey ME, Weismer SE, Bridges MS. The relationship
adulthood . 7
of the obligatory copula form of BE between language and reading abilities. In: Tomblin JB, Nippold

(“is”’ or “are”) and the substitution of a


MA, eds. Understanding individual differences in language
development across the school years New York City, New York:
• At school entry, SLI predicts later pronoun (“that”) for the common Psychology Press; 2014:144-165.
reading impairments8 whereas SSD noun “rabbit.” Furthermore, the 9. Sices L, Taylor HG, Freebairn L, Hansen A, Lewis B. Relationship
predicts weakly, if at all, once specification of singular versus plural between speech-sound disorders and early literacy skills in

adjusted for co-occurring language for the noun phrase is vague because
preschool-age children: Impact of comorbid langauge impair-
ment. J Dev Behav Pediatr,. 2007;6:438-447.
impairments.9,10 the noun information is underspeci-
10. Hayiou-Thomas ME, Carroll JM, Leavett R, Hulme C, Snowling
fied. His articulation of speech sounds MJ. When does speech sound disorder matter for literacy? The
• Scientific studies of children’s com- is at adult levels and his meaning is role of disordered speech errors, co-occurring language impair-

munication problems in medical con- effectively conveyed.


ment and family risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry. 2017;58(2):197-205.
ditions should differentiate between
11. Rice ML, Russell JS, Frederick T, et al. Risk for speech and language
SLI and SSD. A recent study of children Although the speech sound errors of impairments in pre-school aged HIV-exposed uninfected children
exposed to Human Immunodefi- the child with SSD are likely to be with in utero combination antiretroviral exposure. The Pediatric

ciency Virus (HIV) is the first report of noticed and to generate attempts by Infectious Disease Journal. Accepted Dec 2017.

SSD outcomes compared to primary adults to correct the problem, the


language impairments (i.e., without grammar errors of the child with SLI
other developmental disorders). The are less likely to be noticed or under-
risk for language impairments in the stood as flags for concern. Yet it is the
children was higher than population child in green who is at higher risk of
norms but the risk for SSD was not adverse developmental outcomes
elevated.11 than the child in purple, who is more
likely to “outgrow” the SSD and less
Examples of how children talk can likely to encounter problems with
illustrate the differences between SLI literacy, school achievement, or
and SSD. Consider two 5-year-old long-term persistence of subtle but Mabel L. Rice
boys. They are talking about a picture very important elements of grammar Fred & Virginia Merrill Distinguished
of red rabbits. One, dressed in purple, and vocabulary. Our research and Professor of Advanced Studies
has an SSD, apparent in his mispro- our service systems will be improved University of Kansas
nunciations of the speech sounds by increased recognition of the Tel: +1 785 864 4570
needed to say “why are the rabbits important differences between SLI mabel@ku.edu
red.” Within his speech system, he and SSD.

311

You might also like