You are on page 1of 66

IDIOMA INGLÉS

TEXTOS DE LECTURA

PARA

MÓDULO 2

Este material está siendo procesado y evaluado internamente y ha


sido diseñado únicamente para uso interno de la cátedra.

1
INDEX

TEXT 1 ………………………………………………….. page 2

TEXT 2 …………………………………………………… page 6

TEXT 3 …………………………………………………… page 9

TEXT 4 …………………………………………………… page 11

TEXT 5 …………………………………………………… page 16

TEXT 6 …………………………………………………… page 20

TEXT 7 …………………………………………………… page 25

TEXT 8 …………………………………………………… page 30

TEXT 9 ………………………………………………….. page 38

TEXT 10 ………………………………………………… page 47

TEXT 11 ………………………………………………… page 59

TEXT 12 …………………………………………………. page 62


TEXT 1
https://www.psychologicalscience.org/observer/studying-people-in-their-local-environments

5 Studying People in Their Local Environments


NEIL A. LEWIS, JR.
Psychological science is ostensibly interested in the behavior and mental
processes of a variety of people, not just processes related to college students
and Mechanical Turk workers responding to surveys over the internet. Gaining
10 the generalizable knowledge we seek requires studying a variety of people in a
variety of contexts; it is the only way to know whether and how generalizable
our findings really are.

And if we want our research to inform social policies, then it should include a
broader composition of people and situations. Only then can we understand
15 how policies that are generated from our findings might differentially affect
individuals and subgroups.

Vehicles to Move Us Forward


The implications I’ve just outlined are the main reasons my colleagues and I
have been shifting our scientific work to include more mobile research methods.
20 In the Department of Communication at Cornell University, we have a mobile
research lab that allows us to diversify both the samples and settings in which
we conduct social scientific research. The lab was originally funded by the
National Institutes of Health, specifically for the purposes of including diverse
and hard-to-reach populations in research on health messaging.

2
25 The lab is the size of a small RV and is fully equipped with five private data
collection stations. It enables us to recruit and study people in their own
environments. To date, my colleagues and I have used it to study youth and
adults from rural and urban settings throughout the Northeast region of the
United States. For one set of studies, my colleagues wanted to examine how
30 socioeconomically disadvantaged youth and adult smokers respond to different
kinds of warning labels on cigarette packages. So they took the lab to a variety
of urban and rural communities to recruit participants. They not only learned
about how individuals attended to and processed the labels (via eye-tracking
and surveys), but also how living in those different environments affects
35 people’s smoking decisions. These lessons are important for both the science
and any policies that result from it.

My own ongoing research with the mobile lab is combining a variety of


methodological techniques including geographic air-quality mapping, eye-
tracking, and surveys to examine how people make sense of, and are motivated
40 to respond to, information about the environmental health hazards in their
surroundings. My collaborator in systems engineering used transportation data
to develop fine-grained maps depicting levels of air pollution in different
neighborhoods of the greater New York City area. Since we have rich
information about people’s differential exposure to pollutants, as well as data
45 about the demographic composition of neighborhoods, we have been taking the
lab to neighborhoods of differential exposure and composition to examine how
residents of those neighborhoods respond to the information as functions of
both their individual characteristics and features of their local environments.
Conducting the study in this way allows us to learn about relevant psychological
50 processes in an ecologically valid way.

As researchers, the mobile lab has had several benefits. First, it has forced us
to think more critically about factors that influence participants, and their
implications for both the theories we can advance and any practical knowledge
generated from our work. It provides vivid reminders that people are embedded
55 in broader ecological systems and that we must think carefully about how
multiple dimensions of those systems interact with individuals when developing
our models. That modeling and theorizing inevitably leads to important
discussions about measurement, construct validity, and generalizability across
samples and settings.

60 Human Rights Considerations

Using the mobile lab has also reminded us of another important lesson. In the
1948 Universal Declaration of Human Rights, the United Nations General
Assembly declared that “everyone has the right freely to…share in scientific
65 advancement and its benefits” (UNESCO, 2005). When we take the mobile lab
into (particularly underserved) communities, it provides opportunities for people

3
to exercise that human right — to have their perspectives reflected in the
scientific record … a record that often influences the policies and practices that
govern their society. That reality is not lost on our participants. I have been
70 deeply moved by people’s gratitude for being allowed to participate in our study
because, to use their words, “no one usually cares what [they] think.” It is as if
they have learned from scientists that, to borrow from The Op-Ed Project
(2017), “some people narrate the world; other people have their world narrated
for them;” that there are some people who get to be part of the scientific record
75 and others who do not.

To quote Audrey Squire (2015), “historical exclusivity often has a way of turning
into present and institutionalized tragedy. Whose story gets told matters.” What
I’ve learned from using the mobile lab is that when we make decisions about
who to include in our studies, and which environments to study, we are
80 (implicitly) making decisions about whose psychological processes matter to us.
And those decisions have important implications for the knowledge we create,
and the policies and practices that are developed as a result of that knowledge.

The mobile lab was co-funded by the National Institutes of Health, Cornell
College of Agriculture and Life Sciences, and College of Human Ecology; some
85 of the ongoing research described in this article is funded by the David R.
Atkinson Center for a Sustainable Future.

References
90
Brick, C., Freeman, A. L. J., Wooding, S., Skylark, W. J., Marteau, T. M., &
Spiegelhalter, D. J. (2018). Winners and losers: communicating the potential impact of
policies. Palgrave Communications, 4(69). doi:10.1057/s41599-018-0121-9.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of human development: Experiments by nature
95 and design. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press.
Byrne, S., Greiner Safi, A., Kemp, D., Skurkca, C., Davydova, J., Scolere, L., &
Niederdeppe, J. (2017). Effects of varying color, imagery, and text of cigarette package
warning labels among socioeconomically disadvantaged middle school youth and adult
smokers. Health Communication. doi:10.1080/10410236.2017.1407228.
100 Flake, J. K., Pek, J., & Hehman, E. (2017). Construct validation in social and
personality research: Current practice and recommendations. Social Psychological and
Personality Science, 8(4), 370-378.
Fried, E. I., & Flake, J. K. (2018). Measurement matters. Observer, 31(3).
Giner-Sorolla, R. (2019). From crisis of evidence to a “crisis” of relevance? Incentive-
105 based answers for social psychology’s perennial relevance worries. European Review of
Social Psychology, 30, 1-38.

4
Goroff, D. L., Lewis, N. A., Jr., Scheel, A. M., Scherer, L. D., & Tucker, J. A. (2018,
November 1). The Inference Engine: A grand challenge to address the context
sensitivity problem in social science research. doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/j8b9a

110 Rohrer, J. M. (2018). Thinking clearly about correlations and causation: Graphical
causal models for observational data. Advances in Methods and Practices in
Psychological Science, 1(1), 27-42.
Simons, D. J., Shoda, Y., & Lindsay, D. S. (2017). Constraints on generality (COG): A
proposed addition to all empirical papers. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(6),
115 1123-1128.
Skitka, L. J., & Sargis, E. G. (2006). The internet as psychological laboratory. Annual
Review of Psychology, 57, 529-555.
The Op-Ed Project (2017, December 2). Write to change the world. New York, NY.
UNESCO.

120 UNESCO (2005). Universal declaration on bioethics and human rights. Retrieved
from: http://www.unesco.org/new/en/social-and-human-
sciences/themes/bioethics/bioethics-and-human-rights/

5
12/3/2019 How does implicit bias by physicians affect patients' health care?

TEXT 2
CE CORNER

How does implicit bias by physicians affect


patients' health care?
Research is exploring how specific factors affect patients’ perception of treatment
By Tori DeAngelis
March 2019, Vol 50, No. 3
Print version: page 22

Overview
CE credits: 1
Learning objectives: After reading this article, CE candidates will be able to: 
1. Discuss research that suggests some health-care providers have implicit bias toward various patient groups.
2. Discuss how certain combinations of physicians and patients lead to poorer interactions.
3. Describe possible interventions to improve patient-physician interactions.

For more information on earning CE credit for this article, go to www.apa.org/ed/ce/resources/ce-corner.aspx (/ed/ce/resources/ce-corner) .

The theory of aversive racism, first posed in the 1970s, encompasses some of the most widely studied ideas in social psychology. According to
theory developers Samuel L. Gaertner, PhD, of the University of Delaware, and John F. Dovidio, PhD, of Yale University, people may hold negative
nonconscious or automatic feelings and beliefs about others that can differ from their conscious attitudes, a phenomenon known as implicit bias.
When there’s a conflict between a person’s explicit and implicit attitudes—when people say they’re not prejudiced but give subtle signals that they
are, for example—those on the receiving end may be left anxious and confused.

Lab studies have long tested these ideas in relation to employment decisions, legal decisions and more.
In 2003, the concepts received an empirical boost from “Unequal Treatment,” (https://www.nap.edu/catalog/10260/unequal-treatment-confronting-racial-
and-ethnic-disparities-in-health-care) a report from an Institute of Medicine (IoM) panel made up of behavioral scientists, physicians, public health
experts and other health professionals. The report concluded that even when access-to-care barriers such as insurance and family income were
controlled for, racial and ethnic minorities received worse health care than nonminorities, and that both explicit and implicit bias played potential
roles.
“The report really opened a lot of doors to further research on bias in care,” says Dovidio, who served on the IoM panel.
Psychologists and others are now building on the IoM findings by exploring how specific factors, including physicians’ use of patronizing language
and patients’ past experiences with discrimination, affect patients’ perception of providers and care. Research is also starting to look at how
implicit bias affects the dynamics of physician-patient relationships and subsequent care for patients with particular diseases, such as cancer and
diabetes.
Tackling this topic can be difficult because of the real-world challenges of getting medical professionals to engage in these studies, researchers
say. Another problem is that the main measure used to assess implicit bias, the Implicit Association Test (IAT), has come under fire in recent years
for reasons including poor test-retest reliability and the argument that higher IAT scores do not necessarily predict biased behavior.  
While this disagreement remains to be resolved, researchers are starting to use other measures and techniques to assess implicit bias, as well as
new methodologies to track patient attitudes and outcomes. And while the predictive power of the IAT may be relatively small, in the
aggregate, even small effects can have large consequences for minority patients (see Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
(https://psycnet.apa.org/PsycARTICLES/journal/psp/108/4) , Vol. 108, No. 4, 2015).

Implicit bias is called implicit for a reason—it’s not easy to capture or to fix, says Michelle van Ryn, PhD, an endowed professor at Oregon Health &
Science University (OHSU). But it is worth a deeper dive because of its implications for patient treatment on both a personal and a health-care
level, she says.
“Implicit bias creates inequalities through many difficult-to-measure pathways, and as a consequence,people tend to underestimate its impact,”
says van Ryn. “This kind of research is essential in making real progress toward health-care equality.”

How bias plays out


One of the first psychologists to apply theories of aversive racism and implicit bias in a real-world medical setting is social psychologist Louis A.
Penner, PhD, senior scientist at Wayne State University’s Karmanos Cancer Institute. Along with Dovidio, Gaertner and others, he asked patients
and physicians before a medical appointment about their race-related attitudes, and measured physicians’ implicit bias. The researchers also
video-recorded patients and physicians during the appointment and asked them to complete questionnaires afterward.
The team found that black patients felt most negatively toward physicians who were low in explicit bias but high in implicit bias, demonstrating the
validity of the implicit-bias theory in real-world medical interactions, says Penner (Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 46, No. 2, 2010).

https://www.apa.org/print-this
6
12/3/2019 How does implicit bias by physicians affect patients' health care?

Researchers are also examining ways that providers may inadvertently demonstrate such bias, including through language. In a study in Social
Science & Medicine (Vol. 87, 2013), Nao Hagiwara, PhD, at Virginia Commonwealth University, and colleagues found that physicians with higher
implicit-bias scores commandeered a greater portion of the patient-physician talk time during appointments than did physicians with lower scores.
Those findings are consistent with research by Lisa A. Cooper, MD, of Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine and colleagues, who found
that physicians high in implicit bias were more likely to dominate conversations with black patients than were those lower in implicit bias, and that
black patients trusted them less, had less confidence in them, and rated their quality of care as poorer (American Journal of Public Health, Vol.
102, No. 5, 2012).

The individual words that physicians use can also signal implicit bias, Hagiwara has found. She looked at physicians’ tendency to use first-person
plural pronouns such as “we,” “ours” or “us” when interacting with black patients. According to social psychology theories related to power
dynamics and social dominance, people in power use such verbiage to maintain control over others of lesser power. In line with those theories,
she found that physicians who scored higher in implicit bias spoke more of these words than colleagues lower in implicit bias, using language
such as, “We’re going to take our medicine, right?” (Health Communication, Vol. 32, No. 4, 2017).

Specific diseases and populations


Another line of research is investigating physician and patient attitudes among patients with specific diseases. This work is shedding more light on
the role that patients may play in poor communication and relationship outcomes, and eventually aims to show whether poor communication
affects health outcomes.
In a study of black cancer patients and their physicians, Penner, Dovidio and colleagues found that, overall,
providers high in implicit bias were less supportive of and spent less time with their patients than providers low
in implicit bias. And black patients picked up on those attitudes: They viewed high-implicit-bias physicians as
less patient-centered than physicians low in this bias. The patients also had more difficulty remembering what
their physicians told them, had less confidence in their treatment plans, and thought it would be more difficult to
follow recommended treatments (Journal of Clinical Oncology, Vol. 34, No. 24, 2016).
In another study, Penner and colleagues looked more specifically at how past discrimination may influence
black cancer patients’ perception of care and their reactions to it. Patients who reported high rates of past discrimination and general suspicion of
their health care talked more during sessions, showed fewer positive emotions and rated their physicians more negatively than those who
reported less past discrimination and lower suspicion (Social Science & Medicine, Vol. 191, 2017).
“Individually and jointly, the race-related attitudes of both nonblack physicians and their black patients negatively affect what transpires during
their medical interactions and the outcomes that follow them,” Penner says.
Meanwhile, Hagiwara is focusing on black patients with Type 2 diabetes as part of a four-year study funded by the National Institute of Diabetes
and Digestive and Kidney Diseases (BMJ Open, Vol. 8, e022623, 2018). She and colleagues will assess the role of physician communication
behaviors as they relate to patients’ trust in and satisfaction with their providers, and then see how those interactions relate to health outcomes.
In addition to using surveys and video recordings of patient-physician interactions, the team will attempt to gain a deeper understanding of patient
reactions than previous studies. They’ll do this first by having patients view the videos without interruption as the team gathers their physiological
responses, including heart rate, skin conductance and eye gaze. Then, patients will watch the video a second time, stop the videos whenever
they have a positive or negative reaction to them, and explain why. The team will also stop the videos in places where they recorded patients’
physiological responses and ask patients additional questions to ascertain possible nonconscious responses. Six months later, the team will
examine how those findings influence health behaviors and outcomes by examining patients’ lab values, diabetes complications, and self-
reported treatment adherence—the first study to directly assess such health outcomes.
Focusing intensively on one disease “will help our understanding of the role of implicit bias in clinical outcomes,” Hagiwara says.

Medical students and more


While most implicit-bias studies in health-care treatment have been conducted with black patients and nonblack providers, other researchers are
investigating implicit bias in relation to other ethnic groups, people with obesity, sexual and gender minorities, people with mental health and
substance use disorders, older adults and people with various health conditions.
Medical school is one arena where this work is taking place. OHSU’s van Ryn, who is founder and head of a translational research company called
Diversity Science (http://www.diversityscience.org/)

in Portland, Oregon, is principal investigator in a long-term study of medical students and residents examining whether and how the medical
school and residency training environments might influence future doctors’ racial and other biases. For the past eight years, she, Dovidio and
colleagues have been surveying a cadre of 4,732 medical students attending 49 of the nation’s 128 allopathic medical schools, who first entered
medical school in 2010.
The study (http://www.changestudy.org/) , funded by a number of sources, including the National Institutes of Health, asks students on a regular basis
about their implicit and explicit attitudes toward racial and other minorities, and how these views might change over time.
In several studies using this data set, the team has found that student reports of organizational climate, contact with minority faculty and patients,
and faculty role-modeling were more strongly related to changes in implicit and explicit bias than their experiences with formal curricula or formal
training (Journal of General Internal Medicine (https://link.springer.com/journal/11606) , Vol. 30, No. 12, 2015). These include studies headed by health
services researcher Sean Phelan, PhD, of the Mayo Clinic, that examine medical student reactions to patients who are obese and who identify as
LGBT. In prospective studies of the initial medical student cohort, he found results similar to those involving race: for example, that students with
lower implicit-bias scores were more likely to have had frequent contact with LGBT faculty, residents, students and patients, and that those with
higher scores were more likely to have been exposed to faculty who exhibited discriminatory behavior (Journal of General Internal Medicine, Vol.
32, No. 11, 2017). 
In terms of race, van Ryn’s team also found that students who entered medical school with lower implicit-bias scores and many positive
experiences with people of different races were likely to build on those experiences during medical school, says Dovidio.

https://www.apa.org/print-this
7
12/3/2019 How does implicit bias by physicians affect patients' health care?
“It’s like a ripple effect,” he says. “They come into medical school with more positive racial attitudes, so during medical school they feel less
interracial anxiety and interact in more positive ways with patients. And those experiences of contact in medical school have an additive effect that
goes over and above their earlier contact experiences.”

How to intervene
Given the nonconscious and emotional nature of implicit bias, it is not easy to overcome. As a result, designing
interventions is tricky, Dovidio says. For example, he, van Ryn and their colleagues found that formal diversity
training in medical school has little or no effect on students’ levels of implicit bias over time. “It doesn’t do harm,
but it doesn’t do anything positive either,” he says.

Such findings suggest the importance of using psychological methods to address psychological problems,
Penner adds. “The goal of interventions shouldn’t be to confront physicians with their implicit bias and get them
to change it,” he says, “but rather to make it less important in their interactions.”
Promising strategies include those aimed at getting physicians to see a patient as an individual rather than as a stereotyped member of a group,
helping patients become more engaged with their treatment and fostering patients’ sense of being “on the same team” as their doctor (Journal of
General Internal Medicine, Vol. 28, No. 9, 2013).
Researcher Jeff Stone, PhD, a professor of psychology at the University of Arizona, is using some of these ideas in workshops he’s developed for
medical students. “For them, this is about how to improve their skills as a doctor or nurse,” he says. “We don’t just expose them to these ideas and
leave it at that—we have them practice them.”
For example, the workshop uses the strategy of individualizing patients to encourage medical students to question stereotypes about a patient’s
ethnic group, such as the notion that Hispanics don’t adhere to medical advice. Instead, a medical student may be told to ask all patients specific
questions about adherence, like whether they have finished all of their medications or have made an appointment for a referral. Stone has just
completed a study related to this work and is now examining whether changes in implicit bias correspond with better treatment of patients in the
clinic.

Another promising intervention, the prejudice habit-breaking intervention, is based on a theory developed by Patricia G. Devine, PhD, and William
T.L. Cox, PhD, of the University of Wisconsin—Madison. The intervention, which adopts the premise that bias, whether implicit or explicit, is a habit
that can be overcome with motivation, awareness and effort, includes experiential, educational and training components. A study by Patrick S.
Forscher, PhD, of the University of Arkansas, and colleagues found that compared with controls, people who received the intervention were more
likely after 14 days to feel concern about the targets of prejudice and to label biases as wrong, though that awareness later declined. However, in
a subsample of original participants two years later, those who received the intervention were more likely than controls to object to an online
essay endorsing racial stereotyping, the team found (Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (https://urldefense.proofpoint.com/v2/url?u=https-
3A__doi.org_10.1016_j.jesp.2017.04.009&d=DwMFaQ&c=7ypwAowFJ8v-mw8AB-
SdSueVQgSDL4HiiSaLK01W8HA&r=u2yC65FR6T3iE_qlT4nO9MrRdsmVbskhUsmYSvuD2zw&m=OpBA0cMgWG-
7pyj9Hlzc534Yno8gZZOI7vhN27kko74&s=-24Vp02BezoXM8B-YQk9sKae6BeRwd0dydEOtmWmXOY&e=) , Vol. 72, 2017).

What's next?
Psychologists who study implicit bias in health care acknowledge there is much more to learn. That includes discovering ways that patient-
physician interactions might lead to poorer health outcomes down the road, and conducting research on other populations besides black patients
and nonblack physicians. On a more discrete level, it includes achieving a better understanding of how situational factors like stress and time
constraints could activate bias and influence treatment decisions.
Researchers also acknowledge that individual interventions are just one way to reduce providers’ implicit bias. Equally important are systemic
interventions, the mission of van Ryn’s company, Diversity Science. The company helps organizations apply the best findings and interventions on
implicit bias to create inclusive cultures. Ways they do this include conducting climate assessments using evidence-based tools and
questionnaires, giving leaders feedback on that data, and providing ongoing training for all employees, including case demonstrations and
refreshers.

Also important is conducting this work with other disciplines and recognizing that environmental factors such as access to transportation and
proximity to toxic environments can play significant roles in health disparities, says Dovidio.
“When you put together physicians’ implicit bias, geography, patient attitudes, the patient-doctor interaction and organizational, historical and
structural factors,” he says, “you get a holistic picture of what can cause health disparities and specific avenues to remedy them. Understanding
how these processes contribute jointly to health-care disparities,” he adds, “is necessary for addressing such a persistent and complex problem—
one with life-or-death consequences.” 

SHARE   (https://twitter.com/share?url=https%3a%2f%2fwww.apa.org%2fmonitor%2f2019%2f03%2fc
THIS (#) corner&via=APA&text=How+does+implicit+bias+by+physicians+affect+patients%27+health+care
Find this article at:
https://www.apa.org/monitor/2019/03/ce-corner

https://www.apa.org/print-this
8
Impacting Lives, Communities and Systems

TEXT 3
Aqua Vision: Addressing the needs of individuals with visual
impairments through community-built aquatic programming
Minnie Teng

P articipating in aquatic group exercises yields benefits beyond


physical fitness—it also improves mental health (Ourania,
Georgia, Ioannis, & Marina, 2011) and social inclusion (Dolan,
for the pilot project in Vancouver. Personnel from CNIB
also helped to spread the word. Partnering with community
organizations shows acknowledgment that the knowledge
2016). Occupational therapists work with clients to promote and experience of community members are as valued as the
health and overcome physical, social and emotional barriers to knowledge and experience of occupational therapists (Doll,
maximize clients’ quality of life (Mitchell & Unsworth, 2004). 2010). As a student occupational therapist, I led the pilot
This article illustrates an example of a student occupational project, applied for various community grants to help fund it,
therapist engaging in community practice by identifying barriers and hosted fundraisers. The pilot project eventually received a
to participation through community immersion and forming grant from the University of British Columbia.
partnerships with relevant organizations to develop a program
that addresses these barriers (Doll, 2010).
Aqua Vision
Several meetings involving myself, stakeholders (such as
Identifying a need through community immersion BC Blind Sports) and interested participants took place to
As a person with intermittent strabismus, a visual disorder, I discuss how aquafit could be adapted to enhance accessibility.
volunteered for a support group for older adults experiencing Throughout the planning and implementation process, staff
vision loss. One of the topics that repeatedly came up members of BC Blind Sports who are visually impaired as
during meetings was the limited number of physical and well as individuals from the visually impaired community were
social activities these older adults have access to. Many of involved. They contributed suggestions, such as to decrease
the participants mentioned a desire to engage in exercise, the volume of music during aquafit sessions to better enable
as most used to be physically and socially active. Being an participants to hear instructions, to avoid the use of words
aquafit instructor, I suggested aquafit, also known as water such as “this” and “that” (as these terms are visual references)
aerobics. One man expressed interest in returning to aquafit and to have the instructor wear a bright headband or cap for
classes, but due to his limited vision he was not able to see visual contrast. One challenge persisted—how could clients
the instructor. Another woman shared her experience of ensure they are at a safe distance from others? An idea came
accidentally hitting another person when she tried aquafit. to me one evening when I stepped into the shower—we could
Many participants expressed interest in aquafit, especially as try using non-slip mats as landmarks! Each participant is given
this activity can mitigate symptoms of other health conditions a mat that sticks to the bottom of the pool, to use as a tactile
present in this population, such as arthritis. Thus, I contacted landmark to gauge how far she or he can freely and safely
British Columbia (BC) Blind Sports, a charity dedicated to move her or his arms and legs (see below picture).
providing sports and recreation opportunities for people Over a year passed between the inception of the idea of
with visual impairments, to inquire about aquafit programs adapted aquafit and the actual implementation of Aqua Vision. In
adapted for the visually impaired, but found that there was no the winter of 2016, the first accessible aquatic exercise program in
such programming. Through my volunteer and community BC for the visually impaired was piloted—and it turned out to be
immersion experience, barriers to participation in aquafit and a great success!
a need for accessible aquafit for the visually impaired were
identified.

Partnering up with community organizations


BC Blind Sports and the Canadian National Institute of the
Blind (CNIB) are leading organizations in BC that provide
services for people with visual impairments. These two
organizations employ staff members who are visually impaired
and are experts in providing advice on how to improve
accessibility for the visually impaired. When I approached the
staff of BC Blind Sports, they were very excited about the idea
of accessible aquafit and agreed to provide partial funding Participants using non-slip mats during an Aqua Vision session

OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY NOW VOLUME 19.4


9
Occupational therapists as social innovators problem solve, collaborate with community groups and then to
Following the success of Aqua Vision, other community develop programs that make a difference.
groups in British Columbia reached out and inquired about
providing accessible aquatic services for specific populations. References
This community practice experience thus led to my founding Dolan, S. (2016). Benefits of group exercise. http://www.acsm.org/public-
of the Aquafit for All Association, a non-profit organization information/articles/2016/10/07/benefits-of-group-exercise
providing accessible aquatic opportunities for people of all Doll, J. D. (2010). Program development and grant writing in occupational
abilities. Occupational therapists possess unique knowledge therapy: Making the connection. Sudbury, MA: Jones & Bartlett Learning.
and skills related to identifying personal and environmental Mitchell, R., & Unsworth, C.A. (2004). Role perceptions and clinical reasoning
factors that impact people’s engagement in important of community health occupational therapists undertaking home visits.
and meaningful occupations. Using our understanding of Australian Occupational Therapy Journal, 51, 13-24. doi:10.1111/j.1440-
1630.2004.00372.x
the impact occupations have on health and well-being,
occupational therapists have a role to play as social Ourania, M., Georgia, Y., Ioannis, T., & Marina, M. (2011). Psychological and
physiological effects of aquatic exercise program among the elderly. The
innovators—to identify unmet needs in the community, Sport Journal, 14.

About the author


Minnie Teng, BSc, is a first-year student in the Master of Occupational Therapy program at the University of British Columbia. She is the founder
of the Aquafit for All Association (aquafitforall.org) and can be reached at: minnie.teng@aquafitforall.org

Cover photo credit: Vickie Teng


Photo submitted by: Minnie Teng

Minnie says: “The image depicts


an Aqua Vision session where adults
We’re an
with visual impairments participate in important part
aquafit (water aerobics). This project
is the first of its kind, and means a of your team.
lot to these individuals as for some participants, this is the
only exercise they are able to participate in due to their
vision challenges.”
products & services
that support you.

• Mobility Aids • Medication Reviews


• Diabetes Care • Aids for daily living
• Nutritionals • Compression Hosiery

Visit Pharmasave.com

22 OCCUPATIONAL THERAPY NOW VOLUME 19.4


10
TEXT 4
2 Interconnections between social
representations and intervention
Denise Jodelet
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 05:31 13 March 2017

In human sciences, and particularly in social psychology, the notion of inter-


vention is acknowledged as a practice that corresponds to an explicit and
intentional project of a deliberate act of change. Differing from application, as
we will see further on, intervention is based on research aiming at determining,
on various levels, all the elements of a field in which the activity of an individual
or collective subject is carried out, in order to encourage transformation, to the
benefit of the latter (Dubost 2007). It can have, in this regard, a non-conformist,
reformative, or adaptive purpose.
This chapter puts forward some reflections of a theoretical nature on the means
through which the study of social representations (SRs) can articulate itself with
intervention practices. After having considered the forms that intervention takes,
I intend to show that they all implicitly or explicitly refer to a certain knowledge
of social representations and, in most cases, to an act upon them. Following, I
will suggest a model for the analysis of social representations found in the space
of concrete life, thus allowing an organic connection between its study and the
practices of intervention.

Theoretical perspectives and application


When working in concrete social situations or communities, the relation between
research and intervention is founded on two principles. First of all, the golden
rule of qualitative methodologies: the necessary honesty that the researcher
must demonstrate when carrying out fieldwork in order to not treat the people
who are providing information like inert matter from which data is extracted.
This requirement involves giving back the obtained results and presenting,
for discussion, the interpretations and conclusions that were found. Moreover,
it allows verifying the adequacy between the researchers’ interpretations of
their observations and the meaning given by the subjects under study to their
own behaviour. Second, such a procedure encourages participative research; it
can be useful to the process of social reflexivity, through which the knowledge
produced during the course of the study is taken into account and adopted by
the community, thus modifying it. These considerations bring us to differentiate

11
Denise Jodelet
various forms and conditions of intervention, in which the role attributed to the
study of social representations must be clarified.
One first assertion is that if a study which uses the approach or the theory of
social representations (TSR) does not always imply a perspective of intervention,
on the contrary, all intervention necessarily presupposes consideration of SRs.
In order to clarify such relations that present certain ambiguities, it is important
to examine in detail the meaning of these two statements referring to more
general questions such as the relation between theory and application, or between
research and practice or action, or research and intervention.
Concerning independence between the study of social representations and
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 05:31 13 March 2017

application or intervention, it is worth emphasizing that the Theory of Social


Representations (Moscovici 1961) constitutes a scientific field in itself and does
not need to resort to application and even less to intervention in order to develop.
As a model of social thought, a theory of common sense, a psycho-sociology of
knowledge, this theory has to do with empirical material drawn from different
places, laboratory as well as social field. But it does not presuppose application
as an objective. The data are useful for the development of theoretical models to
analyse social products of a mental and symbolic type, without always implying
the concern of returning to the observed reality in order to use them. Moreover,
certain authors come to think that the purity of the theoretical propositions
would exclude a perspective of application or pragmatic use of them, in fields
close to social psychology (Garnier and Rouquette 2000). We find here the old
opposition between a conception of research considered fundamental and ‘pure’,
and another conception of research considered applied and often devalued.
However, one can question the legitimacy of this theoretical isolation, which
would result in a lack of social relevance. Effectively, in the fields of applied
psychology, such as health, education, social work, environment, etcetera, the resort
to the SRs approach has proved to be among the most productive. In these fields
the research is based on the characteristics of representational phenomena isolated
by the theory, for example: the conflicts between different forms of knowledge
(common sense versus scientific knowledge, traditional versus modern knowledge),
or the power of beliefs or ideologies to guide practices, or the importance of
communication in the orientation of behaviour and social relations. The appli-
cation of theoretical and methodological models ensures a better understanding of
the processes developing in these areas. Taking into consideration the results thus
obtained, we can say that application contributes to theoretical progress.
And, in fact, many contemporary social psychologists take applied research
as a new path towards theoretical research focused on problems of current
times (Himmelweit and Gaskell 1990), because there exists a complementarity
between research practices focused on theory and social problems, or because
the understanding acquired in a particular application field is useful for the
opening, the evaluation, and the development of theoretical models. This means
that theoretical progress is reached thanks to the applied research directed to
concrete situations that take into account cultural and social contexts along with
the elements involved in interaction and discursive practices.

12
Interconnections between social representations and intervention
From application to intervention
The claim that there is no intervention which does not take into account SRs,
refers to the fact that the fields of application are domains of knowledge as well
as of action. It is not rare, although by no means obligatory, that application
takes the form of an intervention. Researchers deal with the study of SRs not
only as a toolbox to understand their reality, but also as a path of action upon it,
thus illustrating Lewin’s principle (1963) ‘No action without research; no research
without action.’ The concern related to a practice of intervention is linked to the
desire to modify the state of a field of action and transform the behaviours of
its actors towards improved adaptation or satisfaction. In this regard, the TSR
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 05:31 13 March 2017

offers openings. The definition of SRs as a filter through which to read the world,
a guide of action and an orientation of behaviours and communications, can
induce a reasonable objective of utilizing the theory’s achievements in order to
encourage a change based on the modification of SRs.
The case is even more evident in psycho-sociological or sociological models,
specifically devoted to practices that aim at change in defined social spaces (insti-
tutions, organizations, social movements, communities, etcetera). The latter are
the basis of concrete groups to which individuals pertain; they ensure a mediation
between personal and group life. Intervention thus appears both as the end and
the means of scientists, whose methodology takes the form of research-action.
The objectives of these research-actions are different: to solve concrete problems
experienced by social subjects and develop democracy in organizations and
communities, as Lewin advocated; to respond to social demands; to encourage
social progress through the adoption of practices related to public policies, as
in the case of health; to contribute to the empowerment of individuals and
groups who are alienated in their work, or socially underprivileged, dominated or
oppressed; to accomplish political goals, etcetera.
All these perspectives on intervention in favour of social change, no matter
what the scale, assume reference to SRs to be lay forms of knowledge and support
for identification. To illustrate this assumption one can simply consider the way
in which communitarian psychology, liberation psychology, sociology of social
movements make references to phenomena alluding to SRs. Fals Borda, Paulo
Freire, Ignacio Martìn Baró, Alain Touraine, to name only the most important,
emphasize that all social intervention whose objective is social transformation
depends on groups’ potentialities among which figures their proper knowledge. All
intervention focused on change of social reality implies an emphasis on popular
knowledge, the necessity of taking it into account in the interaction between the
researchers and the social groups. Also appears the importance of working on lay
forms of knowledge, in terms of consciousness-raising and formulation of new
necessities and identities.
Such positions represent a challenge for the approach of SRs and force us to
reflect upon the forms of a more organic integration in the processes of inter-
vention. The psycho-sociological and sociological models assume SRs to be linked
to social behaviours or objects of manipulation. But they neither analyse their

13
80 Denise Jodelet
dynamic nor consider them as complex systems of thought encompassing subjec-
tivity in relation to others and society in a defined context.

Three forms of interconnection between social


representations and intervention
The interconnection between SRs and processes of intervention can take on
more or less slight and intentional forms in relation, or not, with practices. I will
examine three of these forms: when the exploration of SRs produces an effect of
modification in the way of thought; when the transformation of practices affect
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 05:31 13 March 2017

SRs’ state; when the intervention on SRs is intentionally directed to producing


change in the subjects’ way of being or behaviour.
Research focusing on SRs identification becomes an intervention due to the
contingencies of the research practice itself, particularly in the case of quali-
tative research. As in all research, the observed–observer relationship constitutes
an original situation that transforms the object and the subject under study
(Jodelet 2003). Such a process is particularly visible in in-depth interviews with
the close interaction between the researcher and his or her interlocutor. There
the subjects discover that through their narrations they elicit things they never
imagined they would think, or become conscious of forms of reasoning or
processes that were unclear to them. This type of intervention is, in a certain
way, shaped by the interview technique which facilitates the subject’s expression,
as Merleau-Ponty has stated (2004: 29): ‘A true interview gives me access to
thoughts of which I was not aware, not capable, and I sometimes feel as if
followed in a path unknown to me and that my words, thrown back to me by
the other, are being clarified for me’. In this case, the intervention is not made
in the perspective of change, but it can have a consciousness-raising effect, even
if not intentionally provoked.
It results from this that the exploration of SRs offers a resource that certain
models use, as in the case of clinical psycho-sociology, directed towards the
re-signification of the subjects’ situations and life experiences, without necessarily
presupposing a radical change. Psycho-sociologists consider that the space of
intervention ‘implies, before anything, a method of reshaping representations,
positions and behaviours based on an elaboration on their meanings. What
changes is the view of the social actors on situations, on themselves and on
others, which finds itself displaced by the work of elucidation […] In this sense, it
would be more adequate to use the term mobility, mobility of representations, of
thought, and, consequently, of practice’ (Giust-Desprairies 2004: 80).
On the contrary, there exist processes by which intervention aiming at a change
of individual or group behaviours will bring modifications to SRs. Such interven-
tions do not have the objective of studying SRs or taking them into account, but
modify them in a way that can or cannot be intentional. We can find examples
of this in behavioural therapies or in certain models of experimental social
psychology, such as the model of ‘commitment’, based on Festinger’s cognitive
dissonance theory. Suggested by Joule and Beauvois (1998), with the name ‘the

14
Interconnections between social representations and intervention 81
foot in the door’, intervention produces a behavioural change through the initial
adoption of a behaviour associated with the desired practice. The technique,
which was applied to problems of public health in the case of AIDS, does
not focus on the representational dimension and considers its modifications as
collateral, secondary and marginal effects.
However, on the other hand, this relation between practice and SRs constitutes
the object of a theoretical elaboration suggested by the researchers of the Aix-en-
Provence School, who attribute changes in SRs to the influence of practices
(Abric 1994). In this case, the transformation of practices, whether it be sponta-
neous linked to the evolution of social and cultural models, or intentional due to
Downloaded by [Florida Atlantic University] at 05:31 13 March 2017

the intervention of the researcher or the institutions in power, has a direct effect
upon the organization and meanings of SRs. We do find in that case a striking
illustration of the influence of intervention on SRs.
The third form of relation between SRs and intervention can be found
in numerous models that aspire to action at the level of society in terms of
social change. As I said previously, this is found in psychology (communitarian,
liberation, socio-clinical) as well as in psycho-sociology and sociology (of social
movements, of work, of organizations or institutions, etcetera). These disci-
plines advocate, in one way or another, knowledge of representations and
an action upon them, in order to base, mediate or direct intervention. The
authors postulate that social practice depends on the vision that social subjects,
individuals or groups, have of their reality through the interpretation that they
make of it. The intervention project consists in exploring social constructs elabo-
rated by the subjects, detaching those which obstruct or facilitate the desired
practices in order to correct or reinforce them, or reaffirm the importance of
those that support identity and favour a way of life and action which is both
authentic and in accordance with their necessities. Such an objective supposes
an identification of representations that are distributed in a given space of life
or action, and a particular work on the actors’ way of thinking in order to
modify the interpretations and, consequently, the practices. In these models of
intervention, even without always being mentioned as such or analysed in a
theoretical manner, social representations serve to find the means to help social
actors, groups or communities, to improve their life or working conditions, to
lead a satisfactory existence, fulfil their desires, respect their rights, strengthen
their power, or achieve objectives optimal for their well-being. Work on repre-
sentations is conceived in different ways.

15
TEXT 5
This article was downloaded by: 10.3.98.93
On: 09 Mar 2019
Access details: subscription number
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: 5 Howick Place, London SW1P 1WG, UK

Routledge International Handbook of Migration Studies

Steven J. Gold, Stephanie J. Nawyn

Psychological acculturation

Publication details
https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9780203863299.ch3
Marc Bornstein
Published online on: 13 Dec 2012

How to cite :- Marc Bornstein. 13 Dec 2012, Psychological acculturation from: Routledge
International Handbook of Migration Studies Routledge
Accessed on: 09 Mar 2019
https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/doi/10.4324/9780203863299.ch3

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR DOCUMENT

Full terms and conditions of use: https://www.routledgehandbooks.com/legal-notices/terms

This Document PDF may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproductions,
re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or
accurate or up to date. The publisher shall not be liable for an loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages
whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

16
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.93 At: 20:49 09 Mar 2019; For: 9780203863299, chapter3, 10.4324/9780203863299.ch3 TEXT 5

3
Psychological acculturation
Perspectives, principles, processes,
and prospects

Marc Bornstein

Introduction
Acculturation traditionally includes “those phenomena which result when groups of individuals
having different cultures come into continuous first-hand contact, with subsequent changes in
the original culture patterns of either or both groups” (Redfield et al. 1936: 149–50, emphasis
added). Since this formative definition was first advanced, the sociological and anthropological
origins of acculturation theory and research have engendered continuing and appropriate focus
on group-level acculturation. However, it is well to recall that individuals do the actual migration
and adjustment. More than 200 million people today are said to live outside their country of
origin. That number tallies to 1 in ~30 individuals living on earth.
For the vast majority of international migrants, leaving their native country to settle in a new
country engenders daunting alternatives between allegiance to and association with one way of
life that includes family, social, and economic connections against usually contrasting economic,
philosophical, religious, and political conditions or investments. When considered in this way,
migration and acculturation constitute thoroughly transforming forces on individual people. On
this argument, we contend that a more encompassing approach to acculturation must embrace
dual processes of group cultural and individual psychological adjustment that result from contact
between two or more groups and their individual members.
This brief chapter outlines some prominent principles, processes, and prospects of this per-
spective on individual-level psychological acculturation. We first review relevant general theory about
migration and acculturation. We then differentiate individual-level from group-level acculturation.
Individual-level acculturation is not a uniform process as implied by a group-level approach.
Next, we distinguish and discuss variability of different sorts that constitutes the heart of indivi-
dual psychological acculturation. For brevity’s sake, we provide selected, rather than exhaustive,
illustrations. Psychological acculturation raises methodological, disciplinary, and policy con-
siderations, and we overview those also. Finally, we point to some profitable future directions
of theory development and empirical inquiry in the area of psychological acculturation.
Migration signifies physical relocation between geographic locales; acculturation signifies

17
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.93 At: 20:49 09 Mar 2019; For: 9780203863299, chapter3, 10.4324/9780203863299.ch3 TEXT 5 Psychological acculturation

psychological adjustment. Acculturation is certainly a group phenomenon, and some aspects


of acculturation submit to group-level analysis; acculturation is also an individual phenom-
enon, and other aspects are better understood at an individual level. This chapter focuses on
the latter.

Acculturation: a group and individual phenomenon

Acculturation
When an individual from one culture emigrates to a new one, that individual conveys his or her
original culture. Acculturation is the study of how people with one culture negotiate adjustment
as they settle and adapt in a new culture. Twentieth-century theory and research on acculturating
groups and individuals was initially characterized by unidimensional and unidirectional models of
change where immigrants were seen to relinquish their culture of origin as they acquired a new
culture of destination. In short, acculturation equated to assimilation (Gordon 1964). Accumu-
lating evidence eventually suggested that most acculturating individuals adopt cognitions and
practices of the new culture while simultaneously retaining those of their old (e.g., celebrating
holidays of the culture of destination as well as holidays of the culture of origin). That is,
individual immigrants have (varying degrees of) competence in two cultures.

Group and individual


Acculturation takes place on both group and individual planes. Group-level processes and effects
provide a deeper understanding of global acculturation experiences and help to identify social
forces (e.g., attitudes toward immigrants, immigration policies) and aggregate acculturation tra-
jectories. However, just as different immigrant groups retain and adopt culture-specific cognitions
and practices differently, so too do different individual immigrants in a group. Indian migrants to
the United Kingdom may be considered to have acculturated because a large proportion of
Indians have, for example, learned to speak English. However, individuals within the migrant
Indian community vary widely in the ways they have adapted and differ considerably in their
level of acculturation, as for example in their English-language proficiency.
From the example just given, it is plain that the two planes of acculturation do not necessarily
change in lockstep, and there are good reasons for adopting a multi-level perspective of indi-
viduals nested within groups—and therefore of acculturation transpiring at the two levels. Fur-
thermore, each level certainly informs and influences the other. Compare the acculturation
histories of newer Mexican immigrants in New York City with more established Dominicans.
Mexicans are scattered across neighborhoods with low co-ethnic concentration, compared to
Dominicans many of whom have lived in a predominant urban enclave for over 50 years
(Yoshikawa 2011). Mexicans arrived in a period without a pathway to citizenship; in contrast,
many Dominicans experienced amnesty in the late 1980s following the Immigration Reform
and Control Act. Recent migrants from the Dominican Republic are much more likely to have
family members in the United States with residency or citizenship and accompanying language
and systems navigation skills than recent migrants from Mexico. Mexican parents and their
young children have lower availability of supports for child care and finances as well as fewer
multi-generational family networks. For children, this means that grandparents and other older
family members with English-language skills are far more likely to be present in Dominican
households than in Mexican households. In this way, individual acculturation patterns are
influenced by group histories of migration. Individual immigrants from the former Soviet

18
Downloaded By: 10.3.98.93 At: 20:49 09 Mar 2019; For: 9780203863299, chapter3, 10.4324/9780203863299.ch3
M. Bornstein TEXT 5
Union (FSU) to Israel in the 1990s experienced reduced migration-related trauma because their
exodus comprised whole families.
In brief, acculturation involves complex processes that occur in individuals and in groups. On
the group plane, acculturation involves changes in social structures and institutions and in cul-
tural practices. On the individual plane, acculturation involves changes in a person’s cognitions
and practices broadly construed. At the group level, acculturation encompasses social change in
demographic, health, and economic systems in society and affects civic, educational, social ser-
vice, and legal systems. At the individual level, migrants often think, feel, and behave differently
from the native-born in a culture of destination and they also differ from one another on
indices of health, well-being, education, and so forth.

Psychological acculturation
International migration is not a single, discrete event involving movement from one geo-
graphically and socially bounded locality to another. Rather, international migration entails
dynamic exchanges, multiple domains, diverse resources, and imperative needs that are simul-
taneously unique and indigenous to multiple settings. Migration and acculturation are inherently
individual experiences that precipitate thoroughgoing changes of social identity and self.
Immigrants must negotiate new cultures and learn to navigate new systems. Just to communicate
effectively in their culture of settlement requires of immigrants new competencies in speaking,
listening, reading, and writing. Such transformations entail gaining new knowledge as well as
adjusting ingrained life scripts. Immigrants face multiple challenges in acculturating to a new
society—including deciding which cognitions or practices to retain from their culture of origin
and which to adopt from their culture of destination.
Not every individual enters into, participates in, or accommodates in acculturation in the
same way; one individual in a group may follow a course toward fuller assimilation, whereas
another in the same group may strive for a bicultural equilibrium. Individual differences are the
hallmark of psychological acculturation, and there is great variability in the ways individuals go
about acculturating as well as in the levels and types of acculturation they achieve. Here, we
discuss several kinds of variability that principally define psychological acculturation. Migration
transcends identity in the sense of who am I, and raises considerations of where do I fit and
what are my present and future roles within my new society. Individual level analyses focus on
images of self within place or context.
Along the way, considerations of variability elicit methodological, disciplinary, and policy
questions. For example, most studies measure acculturation at a single point in time and gen-
erally employ a cross-sectional research design. This methodological orientation leads to an
artificially cropped “snapshot” of acculturation and invariably to portrayals of acculturation
status as static. In reality, however, acculturation in individuals is dynamic and nonlinear, as
acculturating individuals retain some cognitions or practices of their culture of origin and
undergo periods of stabilization as well as change. Immigrants’ modes of acculturation may vary
over time as a function of ongoing experiences in their new culture, or new developments in
the original culture. Examinations of acculturation true to its process nature call for microgenetic
and longitudinal designs, preferably with multiple waves.

19
TEXT 6

CASE ANALYSES
FOR ABNORMAL
PSYCHOLOGY
Learning to Look Beyond
the Symptoms
Second edition

Randall E. Osborne, Joan Esterline Lafuze,


and David V. Perkins

20
TEXT 6

INTRODUCTION

1 A Guiding Theme
If there is one statement that guides the nature and structure of this casebook, it
is “What you see isn’t necessarily what you get.” It is easy and perhaps efficient
to assume that what we see is reality. If we see a man on the street kick a dog,
for example, it is quite easy (and possibly even “safe” assuming you might have
to walk by this man) to conclude that this is an aggressive person. But what type
of information have we used to draw this rapid conclusion? We have based our
entire decision about the kind of person he is on one piece of behavioral evi-
dence. Although, certainly, what people do (how they act) can indicate the type
10 of person they are, there are other pieces of evidence available to us that we can
use to verify or nullify the accuracy of the impressions we have formed.

Forming Impressions—The Road to Bias and Back Again


Daniel Gilbert and his colleagues (e.g., Gilbert, Pelham, & Krull, 1988) have sug-
gested that the cognitive resources necessary for forming impressions of others is
limited. We cannot expend more of those resources (such as the amount of time
we have to consider what someone else is like) than we have available. At the
same time that we are expending mental resources to form impressions, we are
also utilizing mental resources to complete other cognitive tasks, such as trying
to remember what we were supposed to pick up at the grocery store. Thus, we
20 may not always have the mental resources available to truly consider the kind of
person we are observing or with whom we are interacting.
In addition, Gilbert and colleagues (1988) suggest that forming an accurate
impression requires a two-step process. To the extent that an individual does
not complete both steps, the resulting impression that is formed will be biased

21
TEXT 6

Introduction

in some fashion. The two steps required for forming an accurate impression are:
making a dispositional inference, and engaging in situational correction.
When making a dispositional inference, the person perceiving the behavior of
another will assume that the behavior indicates the type of person that is being
observed. With our example of the man kicking the dog, the perceiver (the per-
30 son witnessing the event and attempting to form an impression) will judge the
behavior at face value. In other words, if a man kicks a dog, he is an aggressive
person.
The second step in the impression formation process requires more effort
(therefore it will require the expenditure of more of our cognitive resources)
than the first. It is quite easy to assume that the behavior defines the person. It
is quite another thing, however, to assess the situation in which that behavior
is occurring and judge whether the situation could have had any impact on
that behavior. According to Gilbert and colleagues (1988), many impressions are
formed solely on the basis of the dispositional inference because perceivers are
40 often too busy or not highly enough motivated to engage in the more effortful
process of situational correction. In our dog example, situational correction will
require the perceiver to assess the situation and make appropriate adjustments to
his or her impression of the man.
Perhaps we could ask a few people who witnessed the same event what they
think the behavior indicates. Maybe some of these people will have more infor-
mation about what happened than we do. It is possible that one of the other
“witnesses” observed what led up to the kicking episode. Would you change
your opinion of the man, for example, if you discovered that his son is cower-
ing behind his leg and the dog had tried to bite the 5-year-old? Probably. In this
50 case, the situational-correction process would cause us to change our impression
of the man. What would happen to your impression if the other people present
tell you that the man was yelled at by his girlfriend and he just “stormed up to
the dog and kicked it in the ribs” after that? Maybe you would become even
more certain that this is an aggressive person. The point we are making is this:
When someone engages in a behavior, we have to know more than just what the
behavior was in order to understand why that behavior occurred.
Once biased impressions have formed, they are extremely resistant to change.
A teacher who has heard from another teacher that an incoming student is a
“behavior problem” will have a difficult time resisting the use of that biased
60 impression in interactions with that student. But recovery from bias is possible.
Gilbert and Osborne (1989) suggested individuals can correct biased impressions,
but only under a specific set of circumstances. Specifically, the person who has
formed the impression must have both: (a) the cognitive resources available to
reconsider that impression and, (b) must be motivated to expend the mental
energy that changing that impression will require (Osborne & Gilbert, 1992).
A clinician wanting to form an accurate impression of a client must focus
effort on doing so. This may require her or him to talk with the individual prior

22
TEXT 6

Introduction

to reading case history information. In this manner the clinician may begin to
form an impression without allowing prior information to introduce bias into that
70 process. If the clinician reads the case history first and a previous doctor has sug-
gested that the client might suffer from schizophrenia, could that prior conclusion
interfere with the current clinician’s ability to objectively observe and interview
that client? Yes. The beliefs that people have, whether those persons are clinicians
or laypersons, directly affect the assumptions those persons will make. Some-
times those assumptions—and the conclusions that will be drawn based on those
assumptions—will be valid and accurate. Many times, however, they will not be.

Treating the Symptoms Versus Seeking the Cause


Thousands of people a day will go to a medical doctor with some illness. Let us
say that you have some bacterial infection that is causing a severe cut on your
80 finger to hurt. The doctor may prescribe an over-the-counter painkiller to ease
the pain. Although this medication may provide temporary relief from the pain
caused by the infection, will it “cure” the problem? No. If the doctor is going to
provide you with a cure, it will have to be in the form of an antibiotic that can
“kill” the infectious agent.
This same analogy applies in understanding the relationship between the
symptoms of a disorder and the nature of the underlying cause. It would behoove
us to provide you with an example to clarify the point. If you saw someone sit-
ting on a bench waiting for the city bus and constantly waving his hands by his
ears, you might think that this is odd behavior. If you fall victim to this pattern
90 of thinking, however, you will have made the first mistake that will make it dif-
ficult to truly understand abnormal psychology. As a general rule in abnormal
psychology, the symptoms we can observe (in this case the waving of the hands)
are only a reflection of the underlying problem.
If we did not seek the causal explanation for why this man is waving his
hands by his ears, we may not truly provide him with the assistance he needs.
Maybe we decide to use a behavioral modification technique to get him to stop
engaging in the waving behavior. Would this “cure” the man? Certainly not.
What if we discover that the man is suffering from paranoid schizophrenia and
he is hearing voices telling him to do bad things. Suddenly, waving his hands by
100 his ears does not seem so odd anymore. In fact, given that he is hearing voices,
the behavior might make perfect sense. Since he is unaware of the source of the
voices, he may be attempting to shoo the sounds away.

The Components of Disorders


In order to aid you in connecting the affective, behavioral, and cognitive symp-
toms of each disorder, we will provide case information that focuses on all three.
After presenting this case information, then, each chapter will progress into

23
TEXT 6

Introduction

critical questions that will influence the diagnosis that is selected, cover potential
treatment options and the strengths and disadvantages of each, provide infor-
mation on case progress or setbacks, and conclude with information about the
110 typical prognosis (long-term probability of success) for such a case.
Major issues that confront clinical psychologists will be woven into the
cases. Clinical psychologists often confront difficulties in diagnosing various
disorders that are strikingly similar. How psychologists go about resolving
those issues will be incorporated into the text so that you can learn to engage
in the critical ref lection process that accurate diagnosis requires. Part of this
difficulty comes from the intricate underlying biology of the brain. A num-
ber of the disorders discussed will involve many of the same brain structures
or neurotransmitters or both. Only subtle differences, then, may determine
which disorder is at play.
120 We also incorporate into the text the relationship between diagnosis and treat-
ment. After exploring the potential diagnoses a clinician may consider based on
the prevalent symptoms in each case, potential treatment plans will be discussed.
These discussions of diagnosis and treatment are important, as they illustrate the
critical analysis process that clinicians must use. Treatment decisions are com-
plex and many crucial concepts must be considered in developing, maintaining,
assessing, and altering a treatment program.
You will become actively involved in the development of each case. Most
chapters will include “Critical Thinking and Questioning” pauses that engage you
in the analysis process. These reflection questions will also serve as springboards
130 into the next section of the chapter, as you are encouraged to reflect on unresolved
questions and issues that will then become focal points for discussion in that next
section of the chapter. You will be given the opportunity to utilize diagnostic
information and suggest a diagnosis before it is actually revealed in the chapter.
The same will occur when treatment strategies are discussed. The section follow-
ing the diagnosis will discuss the choices the clinician made and why those choices
were made. The emphasis is more on the critical analysis process and much less on
the specifics of actual treatment.
It is extremely important that you understand our use of terminology
throughout this text. Several times it might seem as if we do not make defini-
140 tive statements. You will notice that we often use terms such as “may be caused
by,” “might be indicative of,” or “normally is associated with.” Such cautious
wording is not an attempt to avoid making absolute statements. The nature of
mental illness and the relatively rapid increase in knowledge about these illnesses,
however, requires us to be cautious with our word choices. We have attempted
to provide you with the most accurate and most complete, yet most expansive,
coverage of the illnesses as we can. For this reason, many of the cases will include
a discussion of multiple potential causes as well as the multiple treatment paths
that may be considered.

24
TEXT 7

OCCUPATIONAL
THERAPY
with Aging Adults
Promoting Quality of Life
through Collaborative Practice
Karen Frank Barney, PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA
Professor Emerita
Director, Reentry Program
Saint Louis University Prison Program
College of Arts & Sciences

Chair Emerita
Department of Occupational Science & Occupational Therapy
Edward & Margaret Doisy College of Health Sciences
Saint Louis University
St. Louis, Missouri

Margaret A. Perkinson, PhD, FGSA, FAGHE, FSfAA


Associate Research Scientist
Department of Sociology and Anthropology
University of Maryland Baltimore County
Baltimore, Maryland

Editor-in-Chief
Journal of Cross-Cultural Gerontology

Emeritus Faculty and Director of Gerontology Component


NAPA-OT Field School in Antigua, Guatemala

Copyright © 2016 by Elsevier, Inc. All


rights reserved
25
TEXT 7 CHAPTER 4
Theoretical Models Relevant to Gerontological
Occupational Therapy Practice
Clare Hocking PhD, OTR/L, Phyllis Meltzer, PhD

CHAPTER OUTLINE
Being Aged: The Emergence of Theories of Aging in community settings and supporting their role in “teaching
History of Gerontology Theories: Gerontology as a skills for living.”50 Ideas concerning healthful aging have a
Discipline much longer history. Taoist, Islamic, Greco-Roman, Hebrew,
Being Aged: Gerontological and Psychological Models and Christian scriptures have provided observations about
of Aging health and aging, and through the centuries the different
Disengagement Theory schools of thought became intertwined. For example,
Successful Aging nineteenth-century philosophers influenced the thinking of
Activity Theory twentieth-century biologists.1
Continuity Theory Occupational perspectives, such as those occupational ther-
Productive Aging apists and occupational scientists might propose, are largely
Reflections on Active Aging absent from the multidisciplinary discourse on aging. For ex-
Related Theories
ample, in the authoritative textbook Handbook of Theories of
Critical Perspectives
Aging,9 the only mention of occupations besides caregiving
International Uptake of Active Aging
relates to hunting and foraging, which apparently influenced
Models to Explain Being Occupied
the evolutionary determination of the human lifespan.39 In this
Being in Context: Lawton’s Ecological Model
chapter, the focus is squarely on occupational perspectives of
Models of Functioning
aging. This chapter brings together theoretical understandings
Summary
of being aged, being occupied, being in context, and, as is the
OBJECTIVES case for some individuals, being affected by a health condition.
s Identify theories and models of aging; gain understand-
To set the scene we consider a range of theories of aging,
ing of the validity of and exceptions to universal applica- dwelling on those that speak to older people’s engagement in
tions of each theory the everyday world of doing. Acknowledging older people as
s Envision possible applications of theories and models of occupational beings, we provide an overview of the theories
aging to occupational therapists’ gerontology-based occupational therapists draw from to explain the dynamic
practice relationship between people, their environment, and their
s Interpret perspectives/theories of aging from an occupa- occupations, and how the things people do in older age affect
tional viewpoint their health and well-being. To further explicate the context
s Interpret information derived from aged persons them- of older people’s participation in occupations, a frequently
selves regarding their views on elements of successful cited model that explains environmental influences on older
aging
adults’ occupational engagement is described. Finally, be-
s Compare similarities between the development of the
disciplines of gerontology and occupational therapy
cause older age may be accompanied by the onset of chronic
s Discuss attempts to define and identify successful aging health conditions, that theoretical basis is supplemented by
from a variety of viewpoints the World Health Organization’s International Classification
of Functioning, Disability and Health (ICF), which explains
the relationship between participation in the activities of
everyday life and having a health condition.71
Older age can be conceptualized from multiple perspectives:
as a biological progression, a developmental process, a social
Being Aged: The Emergence of Theories
phenomenon, and a lived experience. Although phenome-
of Aging
nological understandings of living in advanced age are
relatively new, efforts have been made to bring the various Theories of aging, which attempt to answer the questions of
perspectives together into a holistic understanding of aging. how and what changes are part of the aging process, have a
Occupational therapists are attuned to those efforts. Biopsy- very long history. Drawing on Hippocrates (460-377 BCE),
chosocial theory, for example, was promoted to occupational Aristotle (384-322 BCE) is credited with being the first West-
therapists in the mid-1970s as being well suited to working ern person to propose firm ideas about aging.1 The idea that

26
SECTION I Conceptual Foundations of Gerontological Occupational Therapy TEXT 7
aging was not a disease but a normal stage of life during guide the creation of programs and practices to enhance the
which disease may occur is also longstanding, having been lives of the aging. The goal became the search to identify the
proposed by Seneca (4 BCE-65 CE), who emphasized quality determinants of “successful aging.” Early pioneers include
of life over a diseased but long life. Cicero also maintained James Birren, one of the founders of gerontology during the
that old age might have advantages if people maintain their 1940s, who established much of the framework of modern
strength and interest in life.1 gerontology theory.10 Havighurst30 defined one of the key
Current understandings of aging have been shaped by ger- concepts, successful aging, as a feeling of happiness and
ontology, which emerged as a scientific field of inquiry in the satisfaction with one’s current and past life, and Butler13
United States with the publication of E. V. Cowdry’s Problems considered life review and reminiscence as meaningful.
of Aging: Biological and Medical Aspects in 1939. The book was Rowe and Kahn58 affected the vocabulary of gerontology by
a compendium of the work of experts from various medical identifying the differences between “usual” and “successful”
and biological disciplines who reviewed “the current state of aging. Recently, older adults themselves, in focus groups
theories and knowledge on aging in their disciplines.”1 A great and research surveys, are creating their own definitions of
number of “competing theories about senescence, explicating successful aging: considering themselves successful despite
continuities and changes from the cellular to the societal lev- chronic physical conditions and functional difficulties.64
els”1 followed, spurred by the founding of two professional Successful aging theories currently include, among others,
organizations in the 1940s—the American Geriatrics Society the concepts of productivity,58 adaptation,12 and resilience.29
and the Gerontological Society of America—which encour- New theories continue to be formulated in collaboration
aged researchers to create theories with cooperation from with allied health professions and social scientists.
researchers in other disciplines.
Sixty years later, the biomedical perspective prevails. Gov- Being Aged: Gerontological and Psychological
ernments and individuals are alert to the lifestyle factors that
Models of Aging
give rise to heart disease, diabetes, and cognitive impair-
ments, and the need to actively manage those conditions to We discuss a range of psychosocial models of aging in the
stave off disability and death. Explanatory models, such as following sections, as these more clearly incorporate occupa-
psychoneuroimmunology, sefi to explain individuals’ sus- tional perspectives. We start with disengagement theory,
ceptibility to chronic and infectious diseases in terms of the which is the most controversial.
links between their beliefs, psychological and stress re-
sponses, and neurologic and endocrine functioning. These Disengagement Theory
views overshadow life-course and biodemographic perspec- Disengagement theory is important, as it was the first theory
tives, which acknowledge the influence of cumulative in- of aging proposed by gerontologists. Disengagement theory
equalities related to gender, race, low socioeconomic status, purported that it was natural and inevitable that older people
and sexual orientation on the early onset and poor outcomes would withdraw from society and increasingly focus on per-
of chronic health conditions.9 Public policy perspectives also sonal meanings. Although allowing that there would be
reveal how old-age policies are shaped by social processes variations in timing,22 the theory postulated that this with-
and in turn shape the experience of being aged.9 drawal was mutual—members of society would also disen-
Psychological theories of aging are also pervasive. Perhaps gage from older people as their abilities, knowledge, and
the best known was developed by Erik Erikson in his middle skills deteriorated and they ceased to perform the social roles
age.23 Erikson’s theory of ego development encompassed the associated with work, family, and marriage.18
lifespan and proposed that each stage, from infancy to old Fifty years later, however, disengagement theory seems too
age, represented a choice or crisis. Old age, he believed, led to accepting of the biomedical perspective that the years beyond
integrity and wisdom. Integrity should be the “dominant syn- retirement are characterized by increasing decrepitude, senile
tonic disposition, in search of balance with an equally perva- degeneration of physical and cognitive capacities, and patho-
sive sense of despair,” whereas wisdom is a “detached concern logic processes. It also seems to perpetuate the ageist attitudes
with life itself.”23 Joan Erikson, in her later years, stated in a prevalent in Western societies, which frame older people as
documentary film that these elements of the theory were not having less to offer than younger people and force them to
necessarily right—in fact, they were “just plain wrong.” She withdraw, whether they wish to or not.25 It is also controver-
revised the theory of old age to include coping and facing sial because potentially mediating factors such as race, gen-
death as important components.21 der, and social status were not recognized.1 Further, disen-
gagement theory may be based on a misinterpretation—that
the withdrawal from society it postulates is not preparation
History of Gerontology Theories: Gerontology
for death, but rather a preference for interacting with the
as a Discipline
people closest to them. That possibility, as framed by socio-
The emergence of gerontology as a discipline parallels that emotional selectivity theory, suggests that people who per-
of occupational therapy in time and focus. During and fol- ceive their time to be limited are more focused on the pres-
lowing World War II, psychologists and people in related ent, prioritize happiness over new learning, and sefi
disciplines became aware of the need for theories that could “emotionally rich interactions with significant others.”61 The

27
TEXT 7 CHAPTER 4 Theoretical Models Relevant to Gerontological Occupational Therapy Practice

theory is given weight by evidence demonstrating its applica- They concluded that higher level of education was the most
bility to younger adults with life-threatening illness, who important socioeconomic factor because it explained correla-
similarly perceive their time to be limited. Disengagement tions between successful aging and hous›old and personal
theory has largely been abandoned.25 income, which the authors concluded was the result, for the
most part, of educational opportunities.
Successful Aging There is some agreement emerging from the research that
The concept of “active aging,” which is diametrically opposed control of the social environment and material well-being are
to disengagement theory, has been theorized since the early requirements for successful aging. Equally, research has not
1950s. It has taken on many guises over the years. One of supported other assumptions, such as that minority older
these guises, the theory of successful (as opposed to normal) adults (in the United States) are disadvantaged compared
aging, was initially presented as a fact.31 Defined as a feeling with the larger population and that the additive effects of
of happiness and satisfaction with one’s current and past marginalization are greater in old age than in middle age.
life,30 some researchers operationalized successful aging as Rowe and Kahn’s 1987 definition of successful aging has also
“life satisfaction, morale, happiness, and mental health.”46 been critiqued for not addressing ethnic differences, differ-
More recent iterations have defined successful aging as “the ences in experiences of life events and coping mechanisms,69
ability to maintain three key b›aviors or characteristics: low and differences in how older adults themselves perceive suc-
risk of disease and disease-related disability, high mental and cessful aging. “Finally, criticism has been leveled on the suc-
physical function, and active engagement with life.”60 The cessful aging paradigm for vesting too much responsibility
hierarchical nature of these components suggests that the within the individual for achieving this normatively desirable
absence of disease and disability, and concomitant risk fac- state, thus risking further marginalization of high-risk seg-
tors, makes it easier to maintain physical and mental func- ments of society, such as the poor and older women.”69
tions, which in turn indicates the potential for activity. The
aging adult must actually do some of the activities, whether Activity Theory
pertaining to social engagement with other people or pro- Havighurst and colleagues’30 activity theory proposed
ductive b›avior. that normal aging is typified by continuing engagement in
However, social science researchers have challenged the meaningful occupations and relationships. The specific rela-
parameters Rowe and Kahn58 proposed, arguing that success- tionship predicted was between social activities and life satis-
ful aging encompasses larger arenas of b›avior and condi- faction, whereby higher levels of satisfaction would be associ-
tions. Older adults themselves certainly have a broader per- ated with a higher frequency of activity and with informal
spective. For instance, Strawbridge and associates64 found social activities rather than formal or solitary activities.49
that 867 older adults participating in the Alameda County Activity theory was more formally developed by Lemon,
Study rated themselves as aging successfully despite chronic Bengtson, and Peterson,44 who specified the relationship bet-
physical conditions and functional difficulties that meant that ween activity and well-being.
none of them met Rowe and Kahn’s criteria. Similarly, older Activity theory is partially supported by evidence from
adults who discussed successful aging in focus groups in research. There is substantial support for a positive relation-
California placed little emphasis on genetics, longevity, func- ship between the number and frequency of activities older
tion, independence, or the absence of disease/disability. people participate in and their survival, functional and cogni-
Rather, in their opinion, successful aging centered on four tive status, physical health, psychological well-being, social
interrelated components: attitude/adaptation, security/stabil- status, maintenance of skills and knowledge, and satisfaction
ity, health/wellness, and engagement/stimulation, It also re- with life. Evidence from the Aging in Manitoba Study (AIM),
quired a positive attitude, realistic perspective, and the ability which is the largest and longest-running study on aging in
to adapt.57 In studies that have recruited an older cohort, Canada, showed that levels of everyday activity reported
however, such as the Leiden longitudinal study of people aged six years after the initial survey were positively related to hap-
85⫹, participants identified the effects that “longevity, physi- piness, as well as better function and reduced mortality. How-
cal, cognitive, psychological and social health and function- ever, participation in solitary occupations was also related to
ing” have on successful aging, and also acknowledged that it happiness. Participants who engaged in handwork hobbies,
is a process of adaptation involving “effective coping, living playing or listening to music, attending the theater, or reading
circumstances (finances, neighborhood) and also overall life and writing were happier six years later than those who
satisfaction.”12 Ongoing exploration may validate older peo- did not.49
ple’s multidimensional definitions and contribute to theory Other studies, however, have reported that some occupa-
building. tions that might be considered solitary, such as listening to
Correlation studies also bring new understandings of what is the radio and watching television, are negatively associated
required for successful aging. For instance, in their study of suc- with well-being. Also, because the studies do not establish
cessful aging among 1825 Korean older adults, Jang, Choi, and causality, it is not clear whether good health enables higher
Kim37 measured a range of factors, including the presence of levels of activity, being more active promotes good health, or
chronic diseases, physical functioning, history of mental illness both. There is also some evidence that older people’s activity
and social activity participation, and subjective well-being. levels can be irrational and excessive.33,53 In addition, the

28
SECTION I Conceptual Foundations of Gerontological Occupational Therapy
TEXT 7
evidence does not clearly show whether different kinds of The link between remaining productive and successful ag-
activities confer different kinds of benefits. For example, pro- ing lies in its outcomes. For example, older Japanese women
ductive activities might confer a sense of competence and in the northern Okinawan village of Kijaha are the main con-
usefulness, but if they are not physically demanding, they tributors to the occupation of basho-fu weaving. The fiber
might not provide the known benefits of exercising. Finally, they use requires intense labor to prepare it for the weaving
the proposed association between life satisfaction and social loom. As the women age they contribute less-intense physical
rather than solitary occupations has not been widely labor, but remain active in some aspects of the preparation
researched and is not clearly supported.49 processes.69 In exchange for their continuing skilled contri-
bution, they receive symbolic capital,11 which refers to the
Continuity Theory amount of honor and prestige possessed by a person with
Atchley’s4,5 continuity theory elaborated on activity theory by regard to existing social structures. Symbolic capital relates to
introducing a life-course perspective. It proposes that older the opinions of others as well as the individual, and it is ac-
adults persist with the activities, b›aviors, opinions, beliefs, crued “through self-maintenance of health, continued engage-
preferences, and relationships that characterized them in ment with society (in various cultural manifestations), and
earlier stages of their lives, and that doing so is an adaptive pursuit of productive activity.”69 Through their participation in
strategy for managing changes in their physical, social, and basho-fu, the women are honored as living cultural treasures.
mental status and the life events associated with growing
older. That is, with the support of their network of relation- Reflections on Active Aging
ships and social roles, older adults make decisions that pre- Since the 1960s, postmodern perspectives have influenced
serve occupations that are highly meaningful and other ac- research and theory development in at least two ways. First,
tivities that characterize their daily routines to sustain their researchers give more credence to people’s perception of their
self-concept and lifestyle.40,43 Continuity theory is descrip- own lives. Reflecting that credibility, members of a reference
tive, focusing on the relationship between the things people group of older adults living in the United Kingdom were
do and their psychological functioning, rather than the ex- asked for their opinion of active aging; they reported that
tent of their involvement in various occupations.53 It rests on they prefer the notion of “comfortable, healthy aging.”16 Sec-
the assumption that people’s personalities are stable, and that ond, there is increasing tolerance for diversity. That mind
personality influences the roles individuals assume, their in- shift plays out in the recognition that active aging may mani-
terest in those roles, and their life satisfaction.22 fest differently in early and later old age. Accordingly, the
Although the theory is supported to some extent by re- concept of active aging has been criticized for representing
search findings, it does not account for the diverse outlooks youthful or middle-aged perspectives that have been sup-
older people have on their everyday activities and the future, ported by research that has canvassed the views of people in
the active choice some make to relinquish activities that early old age.16
worry them, or the transfer of tasks to younger people.16 It Picking up on such concerns, researchers now recognize
does not encompass documented reductions in the range of that older adults’ perspective of being active may be less
activities with declining health or changes in the kinds of oc- about actively working to achieve goals and more related to
cupations people participate in as they age,49 such as the shift taking pleasure in everyday occupations, relationships, and
toward activities that require less physical effort, and from events.16 Researchers have also argued for the need to inves-
outdoors to indoor activities. It also fails to account for older tigate how the “oldest old” age successfully, based on mea-
adults who initiate new occupations, such as widows who sured differences between the younger and older cohorts.
take up new hobbies and exercise routines, and men who People in the younger group are more physically and men-
begin to shop, clean, and cook after losing a spouse.53 tally fit than those of previous generations, retain the ability
to learn, have greater emotional intelligence and wisdom
Productive Aging than any other age group, and maintain their ability to adjust
In the 1980s, active aging was reinterpreted as “productive to changed circumstances and health status. Among the old-
aging,” and in the United States, in the economic retrench- est old, even the healthiest have been found to have severely
ments of the Reagan administration, it was recast as civic impaired ability to learn and show declines in life satisfaction,
engagement—“volunteerism that places the responsibility for affect, identity, and psychological and medical status.
solving social problems on the shoulders of American volun- Although there is no agreed-on demarcation between the
teers while government retreats.”47 Productive aging refers to young old and those living in advanced old age, a population
“all activities … that create goods or services of value,”58 in- approach might place the transition at the chronologic age
cluding paid, unpaid, self-defined, culturally defined, and where 50% of a cohort have died. In developed nations, that
other activities. The concept of productive aging is supported would be 75 to 80 or 80 to 85 years of age, depending on how
by reports regarding the activities of older adults, such as one the cut-off was calculated.7
from the MacArthur Study of Successful Aging, which as-
serted that most older people do some productive work and The rest of the chapter intentionally omitted.
that “all in all, the amount of such work is substantial; and …
much of it continues throughout life.”58

29
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835
DOI 10.1186/s12913-017-2778-y

TEXT 8
RESEARCH ARTICLE Open Access

The impact of a social prescribing service


on patients in primary care: a mixed
methods evaluation
Dawn Carnes1,2* , Ratna Sohanpal1, Caroline Frostick3, Sally Hull1, Rohini Mathur1, Gopalakrishnan Netuveli3,
Jin Tong3, Patrick Hutt4 and Marcello Bertotti3

Abstract
Background: Social prescribing is targeted at isolated and lonely patients. Practitioners and patients jointly develop
bespoke well-being plans to promote social integration and or social reactivation. Our aim was to investigate: whether
a social prescribing service could be implemented in a general practice (GP) setting and to evaluate its effect
on well-being and primary care resource use.
Methods: We used a mixed method evaluation approach using patient surveys with matched control groups
and a qualitative interview study. The study was conducted in a mixed socio-economic, multi-ethnic, inner city London
borough with socially isolated patients who frequently visited their GP. The intervention was implemented by ‘social
prescribing coordinators’. Outcomes of interest were psychological and social well-being and health care resource use.
Results: At 8 months follow-up there were no differences between patients referred to social prescribing and the
controls for general health, depression, anxiety and ‘positive and active engagement in life’. Social prescribing patients
had high GP consultation rates, which fell in the year following referral. The qualitative study indicated that most patients
had a positive experience with social prescribing but the service was not utilised to its full extent.
Conclusion: Changes in general health and well-being following referral were very limited and comprehensive
implementation was difficult to optimise. Although GP consultation rates fell, these may have reflected regression
to the mean rather than changes related to the intervention. Whether social prescribing can contribute to the
health of a nation for social and psychological wellbeing is still to be determined.
Keywords: Mixed methods, Evaluation, Social prescribing, Primary care

Background prescription, walking groups and the introduction of


Since the 1990s there has been a shift from the concept health trainers, with some evidence for behaviour change
of the biomedical health care model to the biopsychoso- [2–4]. These aim to help people manage their chronic
cial model of understanding health states and disease, condition, prevent more serious health problems devel-
particularly for non-communicable chronic illnesses oping, and contribute to addressing health inequalities
such as back and neck pain [1]. In the last few years by building social support networks.
there has been an emergence of interventions focusing The emergence of these interventions are in part due
on the social component of care, such as social prescrib- to the aging population, increases in chronic conditions,
ing, art on prescription, exercise/physical activity on levels of social isolation and the growing burden of pro-
viding health care [5]. There is scope for providing new
* Correspondence: d.carnes@qmul.ac.uk; Dawn.carnes@hefr.ch and innovative interventions to promote the self-
1
Queen Mary University of London, Barts and The London School of management of chronic conditions potentially reducing
Medicine and Dentistry, Centre for Primary Care and Public Health, 58 Turner
St, London E1 2AB, UK
the need for physician led care. Despite the increase in
2
University of Applied Sciences Western Switzerland, School of Health socially oriented health services, their effectiveness
Sciences, Route des Cliniques 15, 1700 Fribourg, Switzerland remains uncertain: a review of 12 evaluations of UK
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article

© The Author(s). 2017 Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0
International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and
reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to
the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver
(http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

30
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835
TEXT 8

Fig. 1 Description of barriers and facilitators to social engagement via the social prescribing service

social prescribing services showed that the rigour of postal questionnaire survey. For health care resource use
evaluations was limited and that none of the evaluations we searched electronic patient records and compared
had an adequate control group [6]. However some of the those referred into social prescribing with a propensity
service evaluation reports indicated some beneficial matched control group. The evaluation period was from
changes in anxiety, depression, wellbeing, social isolation the service inception, Feb 1st 2014 to January 31st 2016.
and general practice attendance [7, 8].
There is no standard definition of social prescribing The setting
but we describe it as: a non-medical referral, or linking The social prescribing service was piloted in the London
service, to help people identify their social needs and Borough of City and Hackney which is characterised by
develop ‘well-being’ action plans to promote, establish or an extreme range of socio-economic deprivation and
re-establish integration and support in their communi- affluence and a considerable ethnic mix [9].
ties, with the aim of improving personal wellbeing. Three areas in the borough were included and were
In January 2014 the London Borough of City and Hack- assigned a social prescribing coordinator. The coordinators
ney Clinical Commissioning Group (CCG) commissioned were trained in social work and employed by a managing
a pilot project for a social prescribing service in three third sector (not-for-profit) organisation commissioned to
areas comprising 22 primary care general practices. The implement the service. Three social prescribing coordina-
aim of the social prescribing service was to improve tors were appointed and worked in the 22 GP surgeries
patient well-being and increase personal self-efficacy enrolled.
shown by a reduction in primary health care resource use.
The aim of the evaluation was twofold: i) to assess the Population
effect of the service on mental wellbeing and primary The population of interest were patients in general
health care resource use and ii) to assess the whether practices who were frequent attenders and, or socially
the service could be implemented as intended. isolated. People were not referred if they were in acute
The aim of this paper is to present data about the crisis, at risk to self and/or others, had uncontrolled
effect of the service on the people referred and the addictions or uncontrolled mental health problems.
implementation of the service from a patient
perspective. The social prescribing service
Patients were referred to a Social prescribing coordin-
Methods ator. At the first meeting with the coordinator, the
We used a mixed methods approach to evaluate the patients discussed their personal circumstances and if
service and test its effect on patients. For the service possible a mutually determined well-being action plan
evaluation we monitored activity in the service and we was devised. The action plan contained goals for im-
interviewed patients to explore their views and experi- proving patient wellbeing, in some cases this involved
ences of the service. To compare the effect on patient referring patients to community organisations and
reported health related outcomes we used a matched services. If necessary a volunteer was assigned to help
controlled group to assess ‘non-exposed’ patients using a the patient achieve their goals. Volunteers were trained

31
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

by the Social prescribing coordinators to assist in the de- ethnic group, general practice (by Index of Multiple
livery of the service and provide additional support to Deprivation) and the presence of co-morbidities
clients. Patients could receive up to six sessions with the (cardiovascular disease, respiratory and mental
social prescribing coordinator and as many contacts with health conditions). Using anonymised consultation
the volunteer as required. and prescribing data we compared annual GP
consultations and number of medications prescribed
Evaluation of effect of service on patients (antidepressants, antipsychotics, anxiolytics,
nonsteroidal anti-inflammatory drugs and opioid
a) Patient reported mental wellbeing analgesics) for the year prior and the year following
All patients referred to the social prescribing service the date of referral between social prescribing
were sent a questionnaire by the independent attenders and their matched controls.
evaluation research team, prior to their first Analysis: We used non-parametric statistics and
appointment (where possible) and after eight linear regression to compare the social prescribing
months. Patients were asked basic demographic group with the controls.
information about: age, sex, employment status,
education status, English language fluency, and living Evaluation of the service from a patient perspective
alone or not. We collected data about general health In this report we present information about the patients
[10], wellbeing [11], anxiety and depression [12], and their perspectives of the service.
number of regular activities, accident and emergency
visits in last 3 months and positive and active a) Activity
engagement in life [13]. The eight month follow-up The commissioned service provider was requested
questionnaire also included questions about to keep monthly records of the number of people
satisfaction. referred into the service (by GP and practice), the
The control group consisted of a randomly selected sex, age, number and type of contacts with the social
group of patients from neighbouring areas not prescriber and volunteers and places that people
involved in the social prescribing scheme. People were referred to in the community setting.
were selected and matched by: age, older than 23 b) Interviews with patients
and younger than 85 years, GP attendance (last We used a phenomenological approach to capture
3 months) and at least one of the following: patient experience, beliefs and opinions at one point
depression, anxiety, type 2 diabetes. Exclusion in time. We tried to access patients who had fully
criteria were: palliative care and housebound. engaged with the social prescribing service (2 or
A sample of 3000 people were invited to take part in more contacts), partially engaged (1 contact) and
a questionnaire survey about their health and those who did not engage at all (0 contacts). We
wellbeing with a view to getting around a 10% randomly sampled 100 patients from each category
response rate (based on prior surveying experience to approach for interview. Subsequently we aimed to
in this population) to match the number of patients interview 20 patients by using purposive sampling to
expected to be referred into the social prescribing maximise the variety and range of patients
service (300 over a 12 month period). These interviewed in terms of sex, age and ethnicity.
participants were given the same questionnaire as We conducted semi-structured interviews covering:
those referred into social prescribing and were lifestyle (to establish their levels of social isolation),
followed up at 8 months. the way they were referred into the service, what
Analysis: We compared questionnaire mean scores they knew about the service, their level of
from the social prescribing patients and the controls engagement in, and experience of the service and
at baseline and at 8 months. recommendations for the future. Four of the authors
b) Primary health care resource use (DC, MB, CF, RS) conducted the interviews by
Primary health care use data were collected telephone and face to face where possible. Copious
electronically and anonymously from patient health notes were taken, interviews were not transcribed
care records. All GP referrals into the social verbatim. Signed consent was obtained from each
prescribing scheme were flagged by a unique participant.
identification code. Matched controls were identified Analysis: We familiarised ourselves with the content
from the referring practice populations. For every of the interviews and organised the data by
patient referred, up to 20 matched controls with responses to questions (the topic areas). By
similar demographic characteristics were identified. consensus we agreed on emerging themes and
Demographics used to match patients were: age, sex, sub-themes. Data was aligned with each theme and

32
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

sub-theme and any dissonant data or data left over Table 2 Demographic profile of participant and control patients
was considered separately. responding to the survey at baseline
Characteristics Control Group Intervention Group P-value
Results n = 302 n = 184
Quantitative evaluation Age (Median (IQR)) 58 (20) 56 (22) 0.376a
Twenty-two general practices referred patients into the so- Gender (n (%))
cial prescribing service (range: patients per practice 1–108). Female 164 (54) 103 (59)
The mean number of patients referred per month was 45 Male 137 (46) 72 (41) 0.354b
(range 25–59). A total of eighty-two community organisa-
Ethnicity (n (%))
tions were used in the delivery of the service, although most
participants were sent to 10% of these. The community or- White 170 (58) 88 (49)
ganisations were diverse and reflected the different interests Non-White 123 (42) 90 (51) 0.070b
of people, for example exercise classes, cookery lunch clubs, Living arrangement (n (%))
library visits, religious groups and ping pong. Nineteen Alone 106 (37) 101 (60)
volunteers were trained, 10 were used. With others 180 (63) 66 (40) < 0.001b
Of the patients referred (Table 1), 17% had more than
Work status (n (%))
one contact with the service, 14% had no contact at all
and the remainder had one contact. Patient reasons for Not paid 153 (53) 162 (91)
non-engagement following referral included: declined to Paid 136 (47) 17 (9) < 0.001b
participate, reason unknown, uncontactable, other com- Education (n (%))
mitments, ill health, moved away, unclear of reason for Up to 16 years 111 (39) 100 (58)
referral. 17 years or above 175 (61) 72 (42) < 0.001b
a b
Median test, Chi square test
Patient reported outcomes
The questionnaire response rate for the social prescrib- treatment effect our social prescribing service had on
ing group at baseline was 39% (184/475) and at 8 months people in the study. Both the non-adjusted and adjusted
38% (69/181). For the controls the response rate at base- models (taking into account the different demographic
line was 10% (302/3000) and at 8 months 42% (127/302) profiles) showed that the social prescribing service did
(Table 2). not have any statistically significant effects on patients’
The control and intervention groups differed in three general and mental health, wellbeing and active living
ways, the control group were more likely to be living changes. However there was a reduction in the number
with others, in paid work and were in full time educa- of activities between baseline and follow-up indicating a
tion for longer. negative effect.

Baseline data and change at 8 months Health care resource use


There was no statistically significant difference in any Across the participating general practices, the study
outcome between baseline and 8 months (Tables 3 and 4). identified 381 patients referral to social prescribing. For
Both the social prescribing group and the control these 381 participants, 7540 controls, matched by age, sex,
group showed positive changes in anxiety (though not ethnicity and co-morbidities were identified (Table 5).
depression) over the 8 months period. However, the The annual GP consultation rate in those referred to
control sample was in better mental health at baseline social prescribing was significantly higher than in con-
(Table 3). trols both before and in the year following the date of
The change in patient reported outcome scores for referral to social prescribing. The GP consultation rate
general health, depression, anxiety, wellbeing and active within controls was higher after their matched compara-
engagement in life were analysed using a linear regres- tor referral date compared to before, whilst the GP con-
sion model (Table 4). This type of analysis predicts what sultation rate for those referred into social prescribing

Table 1 Engagement in service (Feb 2014 – Mar 2015)


Consultations between patient and social prescribing coordinator/volunteer Number (%) of people referred into social prescribing (n = 585)
No contact 81 (14)
Single consultation 405 (69)
Between 2 and 4 consultations 79 (14)
Between 5 and 6 consultations 20 (3)

33
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

Table 3 Comparison of outcome variables between baseline and 8 month follow-up


Outcomes Control Group Intervention Group
Baseline Follow-up Baseline Follow-up
n Mean (S.D.) n Mean (S.D.) n Mean (S.D.) n Mean (S.D.)
a
General health score 296 3.3 (1.00) 127 3.3 (1.02) 184 2.8 (1.00) 65 2.7 (0.95)
HADS Anxiety score (range 0–21)b 287 8.1 (5.47) 124 7.6 (5.43) 175 11.3 (5.02) 63 11.2 (5.02)
b
HADS Depression score (scale 0–21) 295 6.7 (5.22) 124 5.9 (5.22) 174 9.9 (5.08) 64 10.1 (5.06)
HADS score (scale 0–41)b 286 14.8 (9.88) 122 13.4 (9.99) 169 21.1 (9.57) 63 21.3 (9.36)
Wellbeing (past week) (range 0–6) 300 3.6 (1.52) 126 3.9 (1.44) 184 2.8 (1.47) 65 2.8 (1.44)
Active engagement in life score (scale 0–20)c 293 13.7 (3.92) 121 14.1 (3.89) 179 13.5 (3.88) 62 13.5 (3.83)
Number of regular activities (range 0–6) 302 2.8 (2.24) 126 2.9 (2.27) 184 1.9 (1.66) 43 1.3 (1.31)
A&E visits in past 3 months 289 0.3 (0.79) 121 0.5 (1.15) 47 0.3 (0.68)
a
General health scores 1 = very bad; 5 = very good. bAnxiety and depression Scores between 0 and 7 in both anxiety and depression scales are considered normal,
with 8–10 borderline and 11 or over indicating clinical ‘caseness’. cHeiQ Scale is between 5 and 20: 5 = poorly integrated; 20 = well integrated

was lower after the referral date compared to before would have liked more information about the service and
(Table 5). 62% (39/63) would recommend the service to others.
The analysis showed that the number of medications
prescribed to cases was significantly higher for those re- Patient interviews
ferred into social prescribing both before and after the Of the randomly selected 100 clients from the three differ-
intervention. The number of medications prescribed in- ent groups: full, partial and non-attenders, to be inter-
creased slightly in the controls after the referral date but viewed: fifteen people responded to the letters and
the number of medications prescribed in those referred consented to be interviewed. Of these, five people were
to social prescribing remained stable (Table 5). available for interview. The remainder were too busy,
non-contactable or did not want to participate. Examples
Qualitative evaluation for non-participation included: moved, unwell, in hospital.
Satisfaction with the service As our sampling method only generated five inter-
Most clients (55% 35/60)) were satisfied with the social views, we asked the managing organisation to contact an
prescribing service they received at 8 months, 70% (42/60) additional participants. This resulted in the completion
of 15 additional interviews which were well balanced be-
tween sex, ethnicity and age (63% were aged 50 years or
Table 4 Effect of social prescribing on general and mental
health, wellbeing and active living over) but all had engaged in the service. Of those inter-
viewed two people had been invited to social prescribing
Linear regression model on outcome differences (between baseline and
follow-up) against treatment group but did not attend, six attended one or two sessions the
Outcomes Non-adjusted Adjusteda remainder (7) three or more.
Coef. (95% Conf. Coef. (95% Conf.
Two strong themes emerged from the data about: I)
Interval) Interval) Processes and procedures and II) Engagement and
General health score −0.029 (−0.312, 0.127 (−0.221, outcomes. No dissonant data was found.
0.253) 0.475)
HADS Anxiety score (range 0–21) −0.542 (−1.837, −0.119 (−0.847, Theme I. Processes and procedures
0.752) 1.609) Sub themes included i) patients being overwhelmed by
HADS Depression score 0.679 (−0.566, 0.857 (−0.737, their care provision and ii) appropriateness and timing
(range 0–21) 1.924) 2.451) of the referral. This included GP ‘parentalism’ and where
HADS score (range 0–41) 0.232 (−2.113, 0.906 (−2.144, patient’s thresholds of needs were too high (or low).
2.577) 3.957)
Wellbeing (past week) −0.089 (−0.569, −0.013 (−0.623, “My GP knows me so well he probably just referred me
(range 0–6) 0.391) 0.596)
because he thought it would be good for me”
Active engagement in life score 0.023 (−0.957, −0.073 (−1.278, (Pt partially engaged)
(range 0–20) 1.004) 1.131)
Number of regular activitiesb −0.856 (−1.518, −0.897 (−1.729,
−0.194) −0.065)
a
Adjusted with control variables, including age, sex, ethnicity, work status and
living arrangement
“I had too many other things going on [family crises]”
b
p = 0.012 for non-adjusted model and p = 0.035 for adjusted model (Pt not engaged)

34
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

Table 5 Comparison of GP consultation and medication use before and after referral date between those referred to social
prescribing and controls
Social prescribing N control Median (IQR) N referred into Median (IQR) Two-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum
social prescribing (Mann-Whitney) test for
non-parametric data
Annual GP consultation rate before referral 7540 2.9 (0.6–5.8) 377 8.3 (5.8–12.1) p < 0.001
Annual GP consultation rate after referral 7540 3.3 (0–6.4) 377 7.3 (4.7–10.7) p < 0.001
Two-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum (Mann-Whitney) p = 0.014 p = 0.001
test for non-parametric data
No. of medications 6 months before referral 7540 0 (0–1) 377 2 (1–3) p < 0.001
No. of medications 6 months after referral 7540 0 (0–1) 377 2 (1–3) p < 0.001
Two-sample Wilcoxon rank-sum (Mann-Whitney) p = 0.022 p = 0.156
test for non-parametric data

Some interviewees were not sure what social prescribing of a social prescribing coordinator was more than
was and who the service delivery organisation was, logistical coordination but important for facilitating the
despite having been referred. self-management of life skills and thus the health condi-
tions. Wellbeing co-ordinators dealt with a range of
“I have no idea who or what you are talking about, needs from straightforward sign-posting to, what was in
but sounds a good idea, I don’t know why I was essence, a more intensive coaching-style intervention.
referred……..” (Pt not engaged) Some of the most positive outcomes reported by
patients resulted from experiencing sessions which
Names used for social prescribing coordinators included: allowed them the time to explore their situation more
wellbeing coordinators, managing organisation support, fully and work collaboratively to set realistic goals for
social prescribers, counsellors, navigators, link workers, the future.
supporters, members of the general practice team.
The coordinators established themselves as part of the “It’s done me a world of good, taken me out of the
general practice services, in part because the one to one house, given me a routine and given me a sense of
consultations that happened in the general practice purpose and …hope. It’s given me back my confidence”
surgeries. As a consequence users did not recognise the (Pt engaged)
term social prescribing but only remembered their
coordinators. This was mainly because the people we
interviewed saw so many different health care profes-
sionals they had lost track of who they were seeing. “It [social prescribing] gave me the motivation to think
I might be ready to go back to work” (Pt engaged)
“I don’t know who she was [in terms of health care
professional]……I can’t remember her name…..errr but
she was very nice” (Pt engaged)
“It [a voluntary organisation return to work scheme]
allowed me to keep my hand in, so when I was ready
to go back to work [this meant] I wouldn’t have not
“The problem is there are lots of services and lots of been working since 2012………….I’ve [now] got
names, I get confused” (Pt partially engaged) references and skills that are current” (Pt engaged)

Theme II. Engagement and outcome Overall interpretive analysis


The sub themes focused on the coordinator and patient Figure 1, illustrates the process of social prescribing in
relationship and understanding of the service. Where City and Hackney. It shows three phases in the process,
contact with wellbeing coordinators was established, this first with the GP, then the social prescriber and finally
left a lasting impression either because expectations the patient’s/client’s entry into the voluntary sector or
were surpassed or because expectations were unmet the community. The ‘successful’ clients moved through
(often illustrating a lack of understanding about the ser- each stage and emerged after exposure to all three stages
vice). The role of coordinators seemed to work best as ready to move on or requiring further support. The
when they addressed some of the entrenched health and reasons for not going through each stage are shown in
well-being issues patients had, illustrating that the role the orange boxes and include: lack of understanding,

35
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

lack of perceived need, overwhelmed by other health clients actually went to which activities, organised by
needs, logistical problems getting out and about. which voluntary organisations and how many times they
went. The impact of the social prescribing service in the
Discussion community organisations is not discernible but we do
All the participating general practices referred patients know that a large number of diverse organisations, activ-
into the service. Of the patients referred 69% received at ities and events were recommended.
least one contact, either by telephone or face to face,
only 17% received two or more contacts. This limited Findings in relation to other studies
exposure to the service may partly explain the lack of The findings for social prescribing type of initiatives are
impact on outcomes. comparable to other trials and evaluations of self-
Those referred into social prescribing seemed to fit the management programmes [14, 15] in that the evidence
referral criteria. They consulted more frequently and for effectiveness is inconsistent, small to moderate, at
were prescribed more medication than the controls, and best, and only on some outcomes [6, 7, 16]. Health care
were significantly more likely to be living alone and resource use and subsequent evidence of reduction, and
unemployed. They were more anxious, depressed, they hence cost, remain powerful indicators for commis-
rated their general health and wellbeing worse than con- sioners to fund these sorts of interventions. Two other
trols but interestingly their level of positive and active studies reported promising data about health care re-
engagement in life were about the same. source use reduction as did we but whilst this data
There were no significant changes in general health, shows promise as with the other two studies the results
wellbeing, anxiety, depression, levels of positive and have to be viewed with caution. Our qualitative study
active engagement in life over time in either the social elicited strong positive narratives similar to case studies
prescribing or the control groups. A finding which is dif- reported in other evaluations [17–19] but the quantita-
ficult to explain is the reduction in number of activities tive data did not support or reflect the strength of these
in the intervention arm. In contrast the qualitative study narratives throughout the whole referred group.
showed there were strong and powerful narratives about Due to the complex nature of these patients, long term
the impact social prescribing had on some patients. multiple health states and social conditions, resolution
The consultation data needs interpreting with care be- perhaps is not the end goal but better quality of life and
cause: i) there were a large number of controls, so a /or mental wellbeing. Given the discrepancy between the
small rise in the median value for consultation rate over qualitative and quantitative literature on social prescrib-
time for controls (2.9 to 3.3) was statistically significant ing, it could be that the standard health outcome mea-
and ii) the statistically significant drop in median GP sures do not capture the ‘non-health’ related outcomes
consultation rate from 8.3 to 7.3 was in part, because that reflect patient priorities and their perspective of their
GPs referred patients because they had higher than own health and wellbeing [20]. Another point worth mak-
average rates of attendance (alongside perceived social ing, is that interventions of this nature that require the
isolation) so the identified changes may represent regres- person’s active participation, engagement and commit-
sion to the mean, rather than a change related to the ment might be ‘exposure’ or ‘dose’ dependent, at present
efficacy of the intervention. Without evidence from a little is known about levels of patient exposure and inter-
randomised controlled trial it would be premature to vention fidelity that might affect outcome [21–24].
conclude that social prescribing reduced GP consult-
ation rates. Implications for practice and research
Fidelity, making sure the service is delivered as it should
Strengths and limitations of this evaluation be can be difficult, the problems encountered in the im-
The major strength of this evaluation is that it had two plementation of this social prescribing service have been
control groups: one for the comparison of patient experienced by others and recommendations to optimise
reported outcomes by questionnaire, and the other for service reported elsewhere [22]. From our evaluation
primary health care resource use using electronic patient experience we would emphasise the following:
records. This is the most comprehensive control group
comparison to date. 1. The social prescribing has a patient recognisable
A weakness however is the response rates, as with any identity
evaluation of this nature in communities where English 2. Coordinators are located in the GP surgery
fluency and literacy is varied, it is difficult to collect data 3. Co-ordinators have non-clinical training, strong
via postal questionnaires. The response rate at 8 months interpersonal and motivational skills.
from those referred into social prescribing was only 14% 4. Assessment of outcomes are those important to
(69/475). Furthermore, we do not have data about which patients

36
Carnes et al. BMC Health Services Research (2017) 17:835 TEXT 8

Further research is needed from the GPs point of view. intervention arm data collection, MB, CF conducted the control arm
We propose that being able to share the needs of highly intervention, DC, MB, RS, CF conducted the evaluation interviews, SH, RM
conducted the search and did the analysis for GP consultation and
dependent patients with a social prescribing service is prescribing data, JT, GN conducted the analysis of the control data and the
valuable in itself. Further work may need to be done to linear regression models. PH advised the research team on general practice,
establish the right measurement tools and the appropri- read and commented on all drafts of the manuscript. All authors read and
approved the final manuscript.
ate timescale for data collection and a cluster rando-
mised controlled trial with a full health economic Ethics approval and consent to participate
analysis might provide more robust evidence for policy This study was approved by the University of East London Ethics Committee
and is consistent with the Declaration of Helsinki.
makers and commissioners thinking of this type of Consent to participate in the study was gained from participants in the
service provision. More work is also needed to ensure questionnaire survey and the qualitative interviews. Participants received
optimal delivery of social interventions to understand information about the study and the research study team was available at
any point to clarify participant questions to ensure consent was informed.
their potential effects. The concept of delivering social Participants were ensured that all data would be anonymised and no
interventions on the theoretical assumption that build- identifiable information would be published or reported. For the propensity
ing social self-efficacy can relieve congestion in the GP matched controls we using existing data that was de-identified and
de-linked to any other identifiable data before acquisition and analysis,
surgery may be misguided. Perhaps a better conception therefore individual consent was not required.
is to give value to self-efficacy and social capital and
consider other mechanisms to reduce attendance at GP Consent for publication
There are no details or images requiring written informed consent in this
surgery. publication.

Competing interests
Conclusion The authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Changes in general health and well-being following re-
ferral were very limited. Comprehensive implementation
Publisher’s Note
was difficult to optimise and possibly explains the poor Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
quantitative outcomes in comparison with the positive published maps and institutional affiliations.
narratives reported by those fully engaging with the ser-
Author details
vice. Although GP consultation rates fell, these may have 1
Queen Mary University of London, Barts and The London School of
reflected regression to the mean rather than changes Medicine and Dentistry, Centre for Primary Care and Public Health, 58 Turner
St, London E1 2AB, UK. 2University of Applied Sciences Western Switzerland,
related to the intervention. Social prescribing is still in
School of Health Sciences, Route des Cliniques 15, 1700 Fribourg,
relative infancy and the health benefit of social and psy- Switzerland. 3University of East London, Institute for Health and Human
chological well-being as part of the overall health of a Development, Water Lane, Stratford, London E15 4LZ, UK. 4City and Hackney
Clinical Commissioning Group, Queensbridge Group General Practice, 24
nation strategy is still to be determined.
Holly Street, London E8 3XP, UK.
Abbreviations
Received: 4 April 2017 Accepted: 6 December 2017
A&E: Accident and emergency department; CCG: Clinical Commissioning
group; GP: General practitioner; HADS: Hospital anxiety and depressions
scale; NHS: National Health Service; UK: United Kingdom
References
1. Waddell G. The back pain revolution. New York: Churchill Livingstone; 1999.
Acknowledgments ISBN 0-443-06039-8
We would to thank: Prof Heather Cage, Kate Homer, Gail Findlay, Kevin 2. Thomson L, Camic PM, Chatterjee HJ. Social prescribing: a review of
Sheridan for their contribution to this study. community referral schemes. London: Universtiy College London,
Canterbury Christ Church University; 2015. Available at: http://www.
Funding artsandminds.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2015/10/Social_Prescribing_
City and Hackney Clinical Commissioning Group, London, which is part of Review_2015.pdf. Accessed 13.12.2017.
the National Health Service UK. The funding body did not play a role in the 3. Sanchez A, Bully P, Martinez C, et al. Effectiveness of physical activity
design, collection, analysis or interpretation of data. promotion interventions in primary care: a review of reviews. Prev Med.
2014;76:S56–67.
Availability of data and materials 4. Jepson RG, Harris FM, Platt S, et al. The effectiveness of interventions to
The dataset(s) supporting the conclusions of this article is available from the change six health behaviours: a review of reviews. BMC Public Health.
corresponding author (Dawn Carnes d.carnes@qmul.ac.uk). An anonymised 2010;10:538.
data set from the questionnaire surveys can be made available to 5. Global Burden of Disability Group (GDB), HALE collaborators. Global,
researchers working in this field who have: a valid research question / regional and national disability adjusted life years for 306 diseases and
objective, can clearly state the purpose of use and who are in a position to injuries and health life expectancy for 188 countries 1990-2013: quantifying
sign a QMUL and UEL standard data sharing agreement. the epidemiological transition. Lancet. 2015;386(10009):2118.
They must be able to ensure their institution can comply with the data 6. Wilson P, Booth A. Evidence to inform the commissioning of social
protection and security standards expected for handling and storing medical prescribing. University of York Centre for reviews and dissemination. 2015
data. (Feb). Available at: https://www.york.ac.uk/media/crd/Ev%20briefing_social_
prescribing.pdf. Accessed 13.12.17.
Authors’ contributions 7. Kimberlee R, Jones M, Powell J. Measuring the economic impact of the
DC and MB designed the study. DC wrote the first and managed the wellspring healthy living centre's social prescribing wellbeing programme
production of subsequent drafts of the manuscript. Each author contributed for low level mental health issues encountered by GP services. Project
to their appropriate parts of the manuscript. DC, RS conducted the Report. South West Forum, UK. 2013. Available from: http://www.

37
Received: 19 March 2018 Revised: 16 August 2018 Accepted: 19 August 2018

DOI: 10.1002/da.22867

RESEARCH ARTICLE TEXT 9

Prediction of major depressive disorder onset in college


students

David D. Ebert1 Claudia Buntrock1 Philippe Mortier2


Randy Auerbach3,4 Kiona K. Weisel1 Ronald C. Kessler5
Pim Cuijpers6 Jennifer G. Green7 Glenn Kiekens2 Matthew K. Nock8
Koen Demyttenaere3,4 Ronny Bruffaerts3,4

1 Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Friedrich–Alexander University Erlangen–Nüuremberg, Erlangen, Germany

2 Department of Neurosciences, Universitair Psychiatrisch Centrum KU Leuven, Belgium

3 Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

4 Center for Depression, Anxiety and Stress Research, McLean Hospital, Belmont, MA, USA

5 Department for Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA

6 EMGO Institute for Health and Care Research, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, the Netherlands

7 School of Education, Boston University, Boston, MA, USA

8 Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA

Correspondence
David Daniel Ebert, Clinical Psychology and Background: Major depressive disorder (MDD) in college students is associated with substantial
Psychotherapy, Friedrich–Alexander University burden.
Erlangen–Nüremberg Nägelsbachstraße 25a,
90152 Erlangen, Germany. Aims: To assess 1-year incidence of MDD among incoming freshmen and predictors of MDD-
Email: david.ebert@fau.de incidence in a representative sample of students.
Funding information
National Institute of Mental Health, Method: Prospective cohort study of first-year college students (baseline: n = 2,519, 1-year
Grant/Award Number: NIMH;R01MH070884; follow-up: n = 958)
the Pfizer Foundation; Bristol-Myers
Squibb; Eli Lilly and Company; Ortho-McNeil Results: The incidence of MDD within the first year of college was 6.9% (SE = 0.8). The most
Pharmaceutical; GlaxoSmithKline; the US Public important individual-level predictors of onset were prior suicide plans and/or attempts (OR = 9.5).
Health Service, Grant/Award Numbers:
The strongest population-level baseline predictors were history of childhood–adolescent trauma,
R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, R01
DA016558; the Fogarty International Center, stressful experience in the past 12 months, parental psychopathology, and other 12-month mental
Grant/Award Number: FIRCA R03-TW006481; disorder. Multivariate cross-validated prediction (cross-validated AUC = 0.73) suggest that 36.1%
the Pan American Health Organization; John D.
of incident MDD cases in a replication sample would occur among the 10% of students at highest
and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation
predicted risk (24.5% predicted incidence in this highest-risk subgroup).

Conclusions: Screening at college entrance is a promising strategy to identify students at risk of


MDD onset, which may improve the development and deployment of targeted preventive inter-
ventions.

KEYWORDS
depression, epidemiology, health services, mood disorders, suicide/self-harm

This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs License, which permits use and distribution in any
medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non-commercial and no modifications or adaptations are made.

c 2018 The Authors. Depression and Anxiety published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Depress Anxiety. 2018;1–11. wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/da


38
TEXT 9 EBERT ET AL .

1 INTRODUCTION
Key Points
Major depressive disorder (MDD) is one of the leading causes of dis-
• We investigated whether MDD during the first year of col-
ability worldwide (Vos et al., 2012) and also one of the most common
lege can be predicted using baseline data.
mental disorders among college students (Auerbach et al., 2016, 2018;
• Strongest predictors on the individual level were prior sui-
Farabaugh et al., 2012). Depression in college students is associated
cidal behaviors, but when taking also the prevalence of the
with lower academic performance (Hysenbegasi, Hass, & Rowland,
risk factor into account (OR ≥ 2/PARP ≥ 15%), preventive
2005), substantial role impairment (Alonso et al., 2018), increased
approaches should focus on students with traumatic expe-
risk for college dropout (Arria et al., 2013), increased levels of anxi-
riences, a recent break-up with a romantic partner, serious
ety (Rawson, Bloomer, & Kendall, 1994), physical illness, decreased
ongoing arguments with people close to them, and those
physical activity, unsafe sexual behavior, increased levels of smoking
with recent stressful life events. A multivariate risk predic-
(Cranford, Eisenberg, & Serras, 2009), alcohol and drug dependency,
tion algorithm was able to predict the incidence of MDD,
poorer quality of life, self-harming behaviors (Serras, Saules, Cranford,
with 36.1% of all cases occuring in the 10% of students at
& Eisenberg, 2010), and an increased risk of suicide (Eisenberg, Hunt, &
the highest predicted risk.
Speer, 2013). Together, this underscores the importance of developing
tools that identify students at greatest risk to develop MDD during • Screening at college entrance is promising to identify stu-
this critical period of development. dents at high risk for MDD onset, which may improve
Early identification of students at risk for MDD may allow to effec- the development and deployment of targeted preventive
tively deploy preventive interventions during college and thereby interventions.
reduce the incidence, prevalence, severity, duration, and consequences
of future depressive episodes as well as of other mental disorders (van
Zoonen et al., 2014). To support clinical decision-making and resource
allocation, universities need tools that accurately identify students at aimed at identifying college freshmen at highest risk for MDD onset
high risk of developing depression in the near future. over the subsequent 12 months.
Although there is a fair amount of studies that estimate the preva-
lence of MDD among college students, studies on the incidence of
MDD among representative incoming students are much scarcer. 2 METHOD
Zivin, Eisenberg, Gollust, & Golberstein (2009) estimated the inci-
dence of depression in college students at approximately 5% per Longitudinal data were obtained from the Leuven College Surveys
year, but they did not identify risk parameters that predict MDD (LCS), which are part of the WHO World Mental Health Surveys Inter-
incidence. national College Student project (WMH-ICS). Full procedures of the
Several studies of risk indicators for MDD among college students LCS have been reported elsewhere (Mortier et al., 2017). In the aca-
have been carried out, but most of them were limited by being based demic year 2014–2015, all 4,130 Dutch-speaking incoming freshmen
on cross-sectional rather than prospective data and thus, cannot disen- aged 18 years or older were invited to participate in the baseline sur-
tangle the cause from the effect (Brandy, Penckofer, Solari-Twadell, & vey. The inclusion of the baseline sample consisted of three consec-
Velsor-Friedrich, 2015; Leino & Kisch, 2005). Moreover all prior stud- utive stages, with different refusal conversion strategies to increase
ies focused only on the coefficients of individual predictors rather than final response rates: In the first stage, the baseline survey was part of a
developing composite risk measures (Mahmoud, Staten, Hall, & Lennie, routine psychomedical check-up. All incoming freshmen (i.e., complete
2012). Furthermore, individual-level effect sizes merely identify spe- enumeration or census sampling) were sent a standard invitation let-
cific risk indicators for individuals. However, for prevention purposes, ter for this check-up. This means that all units of the freshmen popu-
it is important to select risk indicators that are associated with the lation were eligible to complete the survey on a desktop computer in
largest potential health gain at population level. Risk indicators that the waiting room of the students’ mental health center. One reminder
will lead to the largest population health gain should not only be linked letter for the medical check-up was sent by mail by the students’ men-
to heightened risk of developing a depression on an individual-level, tal health center. In a second stage, nonrespondents to the first stage
but should also be prevalent in the target population. By estimating the were personally contacted using customized emails containing unique
population attributable risk proportion (PARP), it is possible to identify electronic links to the survey. Two reminder emails were sent with a
the number of cases that would not occur in the population, if a specific 1-week interval. By implementing this stage, we removed the physical
risk indicator were eliminated. barrier between the initial nonrespondents and the mental health cen-
The aims of the current study were to: (1) estimate the 1-year inci- ter, since the survey could then be completed on a personal computer
dence of MDD among college freshmen who were without MDD in the at home. The third stage was identical to the second stage, but addi-
12 months prior to college enrollment, and further, among these indi- tionally included an incentive, that is, a raffle for store credit coupons.
viduals to, (2) examine individual-level, and (3) population-level pre- Two reminder emails were sent with a 1-week interval. When includ-
dictors of 1-year incidence of MDD. Finally, we aimed (4) to evaluate ing the reminder (e)mails used in each stage, the maximum amount of
the prediction accuracy of a baseline multivariate risk prediction model contacts was set to eight attempts. A total of 2,519 students completed

39
EBERT ET AL .
TEXT 9
the baseline survey, equivalent to a baseline response rate (RR) of 61%. variables, “rarely” was used for all items, except bully victimization
Students were contacted for a follow-up survey 12 months after the which had a cut-off of “sometimes” (Nansel et al., 2001).
baseline assessment, using a similar sampling design to the one used at
baseline. Personalized emails with unique electronic links to the survey 3.3 Stressful events experienced in the past
were sent, including up to seven reminder emails. Beginning with the 12 months
fifth reminder email, emphasis was put on a store credit coupon raf-
fle. The survey was delivered fully digital and all survey sections were Stressful events were assessed using 12 items taken from well-

presented in a precise predetermined order. validated screeners (Bray & Hourani, 2007; Brugha & Cragg, 1990;

Of the students who responded at baseline, 958 (38%) responded Vogt, Proctor, King, King, & Vasterling, 2008). Items assessed life-

to the follow-up survey (57.5% response rate after adjusting for threatening illness, accidents or death of a family member or close

nonparticipation due to college dropout). Earlier reports of the friend, interpersonal events (i.e., break-up with a romantic partner,

WMH-ICS initiative have shown that lifetime- (22.4%, 95%CI: 21.2– serious betrayal by someone else than partner), physical or sexual

23.7) and 12-month mental disorders (19.1%, 95%CI: 17.9–20.2) are assault, and legal problems (i.e., time spent in jail).

somewhat lower in the LCS sample, than average prevalence rates


of the countries included in the first prevalence estimates assessed 3.4 Twelve-month mental disorder
in eight participating WMH-ICS countries (Auerbach et al., 2018).
The CIDI Screening Scales (CIDI-SC) (Kessler & Üstün, 2004; Kessler
The study's protocol was approved by the University Hospital Leuven
et al., 2013) were used to assess two mood disorders (major depressive
Biomedical Ethical Board (B322201215611). Informed consent was
disorder and [hypo]manic episodes), two anxiety disorders (general-
obtained from all subjects who participated in the study. Students who
ized anxiety disorder and panic disorder), and drug use disorder (abuse
reported any past year STB or nonsuicidal self-injury were presented
or dependence either on cannabis, cocaine, or any other street drug,
with links to local mental health resources.
or on a prescription drug either used without a prescription or used
more than prescribed to get high, buzzed, or numbed out). The CIDI-SC
3 MEASURES consists of a range of DSM-IV-based screening scales containing well-
validated self-report items that were developed to deliver reliable esti-
The WMH-ICS survey instrument includes multiple screening instru- mates of mental disorder diagnoses. Concordance with blinded clinical
ments measuring a wide range of mental health outcomes. The diagnoses in clinical reappraisal studies were in the range AUC = 0.70–
included assessments are described below. 0.78 (Kessler et al., 2013). Information on lifetime and 12-month MDD
was assessed by asking respondents about age, age of onset of MDD,

3.1 Socio-demographic variables and most recent age with MDD.


The Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (Saunders,
Socio-demographic characteristics of freshmen were obtained from
Aasland, Babor, De la Fuente, & Grant, 1993) is a 10-item screening
the KU Leuven student administration office, including gender, age,
tool developed by the WHO to determine alcohol consumption, risk
nationality, parent financial situation, parent education, familial
for alcohol dependence, and alcohol-related harm. The AUDIT is
composition, university group membership, and secondary school
well-validated in college students (DeMartini & Carey, 2012). Consis-
educational type.
tent with prior recommendations (Babor, Higgins-Biddle, Saunders,
& Monteiro, 2001), the AUDIT was used to identify students with
3.2 Parental psychopathology and traumatic 12-month “risky or hazardous drinking” and students with 12-month
experiences in childhood–adolescence “risk for alcohol dependence.”
A modified version of the Columbia Suicidal Severity Rating Scale
Traumatic experiences in childhood and adolescence (i.e., prior to the
(Posner et al., 2011) was used to assess 12-month suicidal thoughts
age of 17) were assessed using 19 items adapted from the Composite
and behaviors (STB), including suicidal ideation, suicide plans, and sui-
International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) (Kessler & Üstün, 2004),
cide attempts. After the assessment of outcomes with suicidal intent,
the Adverse Childhood Experience Scale (Felitti et al., 1998), and
12-month nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) also was assessed (Nock,
the Bully Survey (Swearer & Cary, 2003). Items assessed parental
Holmberg, Photos, & Michel, 2007).
psychopathology (i.e., any serious mental or emotional problems,
substance abuse, suicidal thoughts and behaviors or death by suicide,
criminal activities or interpersonal violence), physical abuse, emotional 3.4.1 Analyses
abuse, sexual abuse, neglect, bully victimization (i.e., direct verbal or All analyses were performed with SAS version 9.4, Mplus version 7.4,
physical bullying, indirect bullying or cyberbullying), and dating vio- and R version 3.3.2. Nonresponse propensity weights (Rosenbaum
lence. Items were rated on a 5-point Likert scale (“never,” “rarely,” & Rubin, 1983) were used to adjust for possible bias caused by final
“sometimes,” “often,” and “very often”). Confirmatory factor analysis nonresponse. Multiple imputation by chained equations (van Buuren,
using our data showed a strong unidimensional structure of responses 2007) was used to adjust for survey attrition and within-survey
(Comparative Fit Index = 0.991; Tucker–Lewis = 0.988; root mean item nonresponse. All analyses were conducted in the subsample
square of approximation = 0.019). To obtain dichotomously coded without 12-month MDD at baseline. Lifetime MDD was added as

40
TEXT 9 EBERT ET AL .

a covariate in all analyses. Incidence was reported as a weighted can be seen in Table 3, the burden of mental disorders in the sample
proportion (%) and associated standard error (SE). Logistic regres- was quite high. Approximately, one-third of all students experienced
sion analysis was used to test the individual-level strength of the at least one 12-month disorder (35.2%), and 28.6% reported exactly
association between baseline risk indicators and the onset of MDD. one, 5.3% exactly two, and 1.3% three or more 12-month mental dis-
All analyses were adjusted for lifetime history of MDD. Measures of orders. Approximately, half of the sample (52.5%) reported at least
association were reported as odds ratios (OR) and associated 95% one traumatic experience before the age of 17; with 28.4% experienc-
confidence intervals (95%CI). Firth's penalised likelihood estimation ing parental psychopathology as the most reported type of traumatic
was applied to avoid overfitting and inconsistent estimators due to experience, followed by bully victimization (25.4%). Every second stu-
data sparseness (Firth, 1993). The population-level impact of baseline dent also reported at least one 12-month stressful life event (52.5%,
risk indicators on the onset of MDD was estimated by population Table 2).
attributable risk proportions (PARPs) (Krysinska & Martin, 2009)
using the predicted probabilities resulting from the logistic regression
equations as a summary predictor (Nock, Borges, & Ono, 2012). 4.2 Twelve-month incidence of MDD during college
PARPs could be interpreted as the proportion of cases that would freshman year
be prevented if the targeted risk indicator were fully blocked in the
Twelve-month prevalence of MDD at baseline was 11.0% (95%CI:
population.
10.0–12.0%, n = 277/2,519). Among the remaining cases without 12-
Finally, a multivariate model was estimated, including socio-
month MDD (n = 2,242), lifetime prevalence of MDD was only 3.5%
demographic variables, childhood–adolescent traumatic events,
(95%CI: 2.9–4.2%; n = 79). All analyses were restricted to the 2,242
12-month stressful experiences, 12-month risk for mental disorders,
students who had no history of MDD during the 12 months prior to the
and lifetime history of MDD. Nagelkerkes pseudo-R2 was used as
baseline survey. The incidence of depressive disorder in the first year
a measure of total effect size. Based on the multivariate equation,
after college matriculation was estimated at 6.9% (95%CI: 5.3–8.4%;
individual-level predicted probabilities were created, receiver oper-
n = 154/2,242). Most of these cases were first-onset incidence cases
ating characteristic (ROC) curves were generated, and to evaluate
(94.15%; n = 145/154).
prediction accuracy area under the curve (AUC) values were cal-
culated. Predicted probabilities were then discretized into deciles
(10 groups of equal size ordered by percentiles) and cross-classified
4.3 Individual- and population-level predictors of
with observed cases to visualize the concentration of risk associated
12-month MDD incidence
with high composite predicted probabilities. We defined sensitivity
as the proportion of cases found among predefined proportions Models adjusting for lifetime MDD at baseline (Tables 1–3) revealed
of respondents (e.g., 10%) with the highest predicted probabilities. the following key findings. First, socio-demographic variables did not
Positive predictive value (PPV) was defined as the probability of significantly predict the onset of MDD in students in their first year
actually developing an MDD when estimated among the 10% of of college. Second, the most important predictors of MDD onset at
respondents with the highest predicted probabilities. We used the the individual level were 12-month suicide plans and/or attempts
method of leave-one-out cross-validation (Efron & Gong, 1983) to (OR = 9.55), sexual abuse prior to the age of 17 (OR = 8.01), three or
correct for the over-estimation of prediction accuracy when both more 12-month mental disorders other than MDD (OR = 6.27), three
estimating and evaluating model fit in a single sample. Although or more 12-month stressful events (OR = 4.29), and 12-month gener-
leave-one-out cross-validation shows a downward bias of true predic- alized anxiety disorder (OR = 4.11). However, the impact of these pre-
tion accuracy compared to other cross-validation techniques (Smith, dictors at population level were all small (PARP < 12%) due to the low
Seaman, Wood, Royston, & White, 2014), this method was preferred prevalence of these predictors.
as it allows for the straightforward cross-validation of multiple Third, large proportions of MDD onset were attributable to any 12-
imputed datasets. month mental disorder at baseline (other than MDD, PARP = 25.6%),
any childhood–adolescent trauma (PARP = 31.5%), and any stressful
experience in the past year (PARP = 34.5%). Specific associations
regarding stressful experiences included: break-up with a romantic
4 RESULTS
partner, romantic partner cheated, serious betrayal by someone else
than partner, and serious ongoing arguments or break-up with a
4.1 Sample description
friend or family member (median OR = 2.7; median PARP = 13.5%).
Descriptive characteristics of the sample can be found in Table 1. The In relation to any childhood–adolescent trauma, specifically parental
majority of the sample was female (54.5%), only few participants (6%) psychopathology, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, and dating violence
were of non-Belgian nationality and 15.3% of the students indicated (median OR = 2.7; median PARP = 12.5%) were significantly associ-
that they were raised in households, in which their parents financial ated with MD. With regard to being at risk for comorbid mental health
situation was difficult. Parental education was high for both parents issues, specific associations included generalized anxiety disorder,
for the majority of the students (63.4%), only few students (14.8%) nonsuicidal self-injury, suicidal ideation, and suicide plans and/or
indicated that neither of their parents had a high education level. As attempts (median OR = 3.9; median PARP = 6.7%).

41
EBERT ET AL .
TEXT 9
TA B L E 1 Socio-demographic Variables as Baseline Predictors for Depression Onset during Follow-up

Bivariate Modela
Prevalence Subsample no. 12-m MDD
n (w) % (w) (SE) OR 95%CI PARP (%)
I. Socio-demographic variables
Being male 1,021 45.5 0.9 0.68 (0.41–1.12) −14.4
Age > 18 years 495 22.1 0.8 1.40 (0.83–2.37) 7.0
Non-Belgian nationality 134 6.0 0.4 1.48 (0.63–3.45) 3.2
Parents’ financial situation difficult 344 15.3 0.7 1.05 (0.53–2.07) 1.0
Parental educationb
Both parents high 1,422 63.4 1.0 (ref) – –
Only one parent high 487 21.7 0.8 0.95 (0.54–1.68) −0.8
None of parents high 333 14.8 0.7 1.03 (0.51–2.10) 0.7
c
Nonintact familial composition 472 21.0 0.8 1.23 (0.68–2.20) 4.3
College-related socio-demographics
University Group membership
Human Sciences 1,171 52.2 0.9 (ref) – –
Science & Technology 623 27.8 0.8 0.61 (0.35–1.07) −12.7
Biomedical Sciences 448 20.0 0.7 0.61 (0.31–1.17) −9.3
Non-GSE pre-educational level 131 5.8 0.4 1.33 (0.49–3.57) 1.9

Note. Significant odds ratios/PARPs are shown in bold (𝛼 = 0.05); OR = odds ratio; PARP = population attributable risk proportion; GSE = general secondary
education.
a
The bivariate associations are based on a separate model for each row, with the variable in the row as the only predictor in the model, adjusted for lifetime
MDD at baseline.
b
High degree of parental education defined as holding a college bachelor degree or more.
c
Nonintact familial composition defined as parents being divorced or separated.

Fourth, a positive dose–response relationship was found within 5 DISCUSSION


each domain, with MDD risk among students who had three or more
risk indicators substantially elevated both at the individual level (ORs 5.1 Main findings
between 3.4 and 6.3) and at the population level (PARPs between 4.0
This prospective study examined the onset of MDD in a large represen-
and 16.2%). Finally, when considering both individual- and population-
tative sample of college students. In the first year of college, the inci-
level effects, the most important risk indicators (i.e., OR ≥ 2 and PARP
dence of MDD was estimated at 6.9%. Among the 10% of students with
≥ 15%) were any traumatic experience prior to the age of 17, break-up
the highest predicted risk of MDD onset based on our model, approxi-
with a romantic partner in the past year, serious ongoing arguments or
mately one out of four developed MDD. Suicidal plans and/or attempts
break-up with a friend or family member in the past year and three or
were most strongly associated with MDD onset at the individual
more stressful life events in the last 12 months.
level. The largest population-level effects, however, were found for
any 12-month mental disorder at baseline (PARP = 25.6%), a history
4.4 Multivariate model for MDD onset during of any childhood–adolescent trauma (PARP = 31.5%), and stressful
freshman year experiences in the past 12 months (PARP = 34.5%).
The total effect size (Nagelkerke pseudo-R2) of risk indicators was
0.23. The prediction model had a reasonable performance with a cross-
5.2 Limitations
validated AUC of 0.73 (SE = 0.04). The 10% of students at highest
predicted risk for subsequent onset of MDD within the first 12 months Several limitations are noteworthy. First, response rates were moder-
after college matriculation included 36.1% (SE = 6.1) of all observed ate (61.0% at baseline; 57.5% at follow-up). However, these response
MDD cases (Table 4). The probability of MDD onset in this 10% of rates compare favourably to those achieved in other large-scale
respondents was 24.7% (SE = 4.9). The only significant predictors in prospective college student surveys (39–44%) (Eisenberg et al.,
the final model, when adjusted for all other risk domains, were suicidal 2013; Paul, Tsypes, Eidlitz, Ernhout, & Whitlock, 2015). In addition,
ideation (OR = 2.88; 95%CI = 1.10–7.56; PARP = 4.4%) and suicide state-of-the-art missing data techniques were applied to increase
plans and/or attempts (OR = 6.77; 95%CI = 1.55–29.62; PARP = 3.9%). the representativeness of the findings. Nonetheless it is possible that
A full overview of the multivariate estimates can be found in the systematic nonresponse might have biased results. Second, baseline
supplementary materials. risk for mental disorders was not assessed by diagnostic interviews

42
TEXT 9 EBERT ET AL .

TA B L E 2 Childhood–Adolescent Traumatic Experiences and 12-Month Stressful Experiences as Baseline Predictors for Depression Onset
during Follow-up

Bivariate Modela
Prevalence Subsample no. 12-m MDD
n (w) % (w) (SE) OR 95%CI PARP (%)
II. Twelve-month stressful experiences
Life-threatening illness or 481 21.5 1.1 1.18 (0.60–2.34) 3.9
injury of a friend or family
member
Death of a friend or family 437 19.5 1.0 1.07 (0.52–2.20) 1.7
member
Break-up with a romantic 394 17.7 1.0 2.63 (1.37–5.09) 20.3
partner
Romantic partner cheated 87 3.9 0.5 3.81 (1.11–13.07) 8.5
Serious betrayal someone else 210 9.4 0.7 2.35 (1.07–5.17) 10.4
than partner
Serious ongoing arguments or 284 12.7 0.8 2.78 (1.46–5.31) 16.7
break-up with friend or
family member
Life-threatening accident 22 1.0 0.3 2.60 (0.21–32.11) 1.9
Seriously physically assaulted 65 2.9 0.4 1.22 (0.18–8.09) 1.2
Sexually assaulted or raped 8 0.4 0.1 2.26 (0.13–38.25) 0.6
Any serious legal problem 44 1.9 0.3 2.20 (0.31–15.73) 2.6
Any stressful event 1,177 52.5 1.2 2.12 (1.20–3.75) 34.5
Number of stressful experiences
0 1,065 47.5 1.2 (ref) – –
1 620 27.6 1.1 1.53 (0.83–2.82) 8.8
2 355 15.9 0.9 2.04 (1.01–4.13) 9.8
3+ 202 9.0 0.7 4.29 (1.85–9.96) 16.2
F-test (p-value)b F = 2.69
(0.046)
III. Traumatic experiences (≤ age 17)
Parental psychopathology 637 28.4 1.1 1.96 (1.13–3.39) 19.7
Physical abuse 96 4.3 0.5 2.01 (0.74–5.45) 4.0
Emotional abuse 329 14.7 0.8 2.51 (1.34–4.71) 16.4
Sexual abuse 18 0.8 0.2 8.01 (1.64–39.06) 3.6
Neglect 116 5.2 0.5 1.44 (0.51–4.05) 2.4
Bully victimization 570 25.4 1.1 1.19 (0.68–2.09) 4.5
Dating violence 115 5.1 0.5 2.94 (1.03–8.40) 8.5
Any traumatic experience 1,141 50.9 1.2 2.00 (1.17–3.43) 31.5
Number of traumatic experiences
0 1,099 49.1 1.2 (ref) – –
1 688 30.7 1.1 1.46 (0.80–2.66) 9.0
2 272 12.1 0.8 2.57 (1.28–5.15) 11.4
3+ 181 8.1 0.7 3.43 (1.52–7.74) 11.7
F-test (p-value)b F = 3.04
(0.029)
Note. Significant odds ratios/PARPs are shown in bold (𝛼 = 0.05); OR = odds ratio; PARP = population attributable risk proportion.
a
The bivariate associations are based on a separate model for each row, with the variable in the row as the only predictor in the model, adjusted for lifetime
MDD at baseline.
b Cochran–Armitage trend test. The F-test evaluates significance (𝛼 = 0.05) of 200 pooled Cochran–Armitage 𝜒 2 (3) linear trend tests.

43
EBERT ET AL .
TEXT 9
TA B L E 3 Twelve-Month Mental Disorders as Baseline Predictors for Depression Onset during Follow-up

Bivariate Modela
Prevalence Subsample no. 12-m MDD
n (w) % (w) (SE) OR 95%CI PARP (%)
IV. Twelve-month mental disorders
Generalized anxiety disorder 70 3.1 0.3 4.11 (1.75–9.70) 7.5
Panic disorder 23 1.0 0.2 0.94 (0.07–12.05) 0.2
Broad mania 22 1.0 0.2 2.75 (0.58–12.98) 1.6
Low risk for alcohol use disorder 1,660 74.0 0.8 (ref) – –
Risky or hazardous drinking 505 22.5 0.8 1.17 (0.69–1.96) 3.3
Risk for alcohol dependence 77 3.4 0.4 1.34 (0.39–4.63) 1.2
Drug abuse/dependence 25 1.1 0.2 1.93 (0.39–9.41) 1.0
Nonsuicidal self-injury 161 7.2 0.5 2.53 (1.28–5.02) 8.7
No STB 2,154 96.1 0.4 (ref) – –
Suicidal ideation 64 2.9 0.3 3.76 (1.65–8.57) 5.9
Suicide plans and/or attempts 24 1.1 0.2 9.55 (2.96–30.78) 5.0
Any mental disorder 790 35.2 0.9 2.12 (1.34–3.36) 25.6
Number of mental disorder
0 1,452 64.8 0.9 (ref) – –
1 641 28.6 0.9 1.76 (1.09–2.86) 14.3
2 119 5.3 0.4 3.34 (1.55–7.19) 7.8
3+ 30 1.3 0.2 6.27 (1.85–21.33) 4.0
F-test (p-value)c F = 4.16
(0.006)
Note. Significant odds ratios/PARPs are shown in bold (𝛼 = 0.05); STB = suicidal thoughts and behaviors; OR = odds ratio; PARP = population attributable
risk proportion.
a The bivariate associations are based on a separate model for each row, with the variable in the row as the only predictor in the model, adjusted for lifetime

MDD at baseline.
b Cochran–Armitage trend test. The F-test evaluates significance (𝛼 = 0.05) of 200 pooled Cochran–Armitage 𝜒 2 (3) linear trend tests.

but with self-report measures and a categorical cut-off scoring system. that includes additional predictors. A related limitation is that we
The latter measures were well-validated screening scales used in prior used conventional research analysis methods to develop the risk
general population surveys that have shown high concordance with model. It is likely that we will be able to improve on this performance
with blinded clinical diagnoses in clinical reappraisal studies (Kessler in planned cross-national analyses using machine learning methods
et al., 2010). However, it remains unknown whether screening scale (Kessler et al., 2016, 2017). Finally, we did not assess serious life
performance is different among college students. Although we plan to events during follow-up. Therefore, we do not know which MDD
carry out clinical reappraisal studies to address this limitation in future incidence cases are due to baseline vulnerability and which due to
iterations of the WMH college surveys, this has not yet been done exposure to random traumas that could not be predicted at baseline
and caution is consequently needed in interpreting results regarding (e.g., sexual assault, death of a parent). Such information would not
prevalence estimates. Third, the survey was conducted among fresh- only be important to inform about strategies to improve prediction
men in one Belgian college. The findings might not generalize to college accuracy of the algorithm, but also relevant for the development of
students from other universities in different countries or cultures. appropriate prevention strategies. This should be explored in future
Finally, although we included a large set of known risk indicators for studies.
MDD onset, some important risk indicators were not assessed, such
as subsyndromal depression, chronic somatic conditions, personality
5.3 Implications for clinical practice and future
traits/disorders, psychotic experiences/disorders, poor self-perceived
research
health, low emotion regulation skills, low self-esteem, low resilience,
and neuroticism (Berking, Wirtz, Svaldi, & Hofmann, 2014; Cole & Our study has relevant implications for clinical practice and future
Dendukuri, 2003; Ebert, Hopfinger, & Berking, 2017; Korten, Comijs, research. First, to the best of our knowledge, this study is among the
Lamers, & Penninx, 2012; Pelkonen, Marttunen, Kaprio, Huurre, & first that prospectively estimated the 1-year incidence proportion of
Aro, 2008; Wild et al., 2016). As a result, the strength of the composite MDD in students during their first year of college. The reported inci-
risk index found here should be considered a lower bound estimate dence proportion is somewhat higher than the estimated incidence of
compared to the estimate that might be obtained in future research MDD among college students based on the World Health Organization

44
TEXT 9 EBERT ET AL .

TA B L E 4 Concentration of Risk of Depression Cases in Different The assigned predicted probabilities could then be used as a way to
Proportions of Incoming Freshmen at Highest Predicted Risk based on delineate those at highest risk for onset of MDD in the following year.
a Multivariate Modela Including all Risk Factors
However, although risk-prediction algorithms might be of high value
Depression Onset for detecting students at risk, for developing mental health problems,
% at Highest
Predicted Risk Sensitivity (%[SE])b PPV (%[SE])c it should be noted that relying only on procedures based on students
100 100.0 (0.0) 6.9 (0.8) self-reports might be not sufficient to detect students at risk, and other
90 96.0 (2.4) 7.3 (0.9) measures such as staff training and awareness campaigns should not

80 91.8 (3.4) 7.9 (1.0) be neglected. Students considered to be at high risk could be offered
preventive interventions, for example delivered through the internet
70 87.4 (4.1) 8.6 (1.2)
(Buntrock et al., 2016, 2017; Ebert et al., 2018; Harrer et al., 2018).
60 82.7 (4.8) 9.5 (1.3)
Based on our model, over one third of MDD cases will occur in the 10%
50 77.2 (5.2) 10.6 (1.5)
of students at highest predicted risk. However, this does not imply that
40 71.0 (5.9) 12.2 (1.8)
students at lower risk do not warrant preventive interventions. More
30 63.1 (6.3) 14.4 (2.3)
research is needed to obtain information on the needs of students
20 52.4 (6.5) 17.9 (3.0)
who are associated with different risk levels and which interventions
10 36.1 (6.1) 24.7 (4.9) work best at varying levels. Due to high comorbidity rates between
a See the model in the supplementary materials covering multivariate model
emotional disorders (Beekman et al., 2000) and overlapping risk
construction (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2). Model-based AUC values factors (de Graaf, Bijl, Smit, Vollebergh, & Spijker, 2002), such studies
were 0.78 [SE = 0.03] for depression onset. Cross-validated AUC values
were 0.73 [SE = 0.04].
should also explore relative advantages of disorder versus trans-
b Sensitivity = proportion of depression cases found among the row % of diagnostic and individual tailored preventive interventions (Weisel
respondents at highest predicted risk, based on cross-validated predicted et al., 2018). In addition, clinical outcome and cost-effectiveness
probabilities.
research based on varying risk thresholds should be conducted so
c
Positive predictive value (PPV) = probability of effectively developing a
depression when being among the row % at highest predicted risk, based that intervention decisions derived from the prediction model are
on cross-validated predicted probabilities. evidence-based.
Third, the population-level estimates offered relevant insights into
(WHO) World Mental Health Surveys (Auerbach et al., 2016). Differ- the design of future interventions. Based on individual-level effect
ences may be explained by geographical or methodological differences sizes, one could argue preventive interventions should focus on stu-
(i.e., adjustment for college attriters or the use of retrospective designs dents who have been either sexually abused or who had suicide plans
in the WHO surveys). Our data suggest that the first year in college or attempts (OR > 8). However, the impact of these factors on a pop-
constitutes a risk period for the onset of MDD. In fact, the vast majority ulation level overall was very low (PARP < 5%) due to low prevalence.
of observed MDD cases were incidence cases (94.8%), thus this period In contrast, targeting students who experience any childhood–
in life seems to be an opportune point in time to intervene preventively. adolescent trauma, such as emotional abuse, could have a beneficiary
Second, our study further adds to the cumulating evidence that the effect for about one third of subsequent depression onsets. Also, the
development of risk-prediction for psychiatric disorders is feasible incidence of depression among these students designated to be at high
(Bernardini et al., 2017) and provides evidence that a multivariate risk (24.7%) is sufficiently high that the cost-effectiveness of a preven-
prediction model can be a useful tool to accurately predict the tive intervention has a reasonable chance of being within an actionable
onset of MDD during college. Prediction accuracy (AUC = 0.73) was range. Likewise, targeting students whose parents have a mental disor-
comparable to the few prediction algorithms that have been evaluated der could potentially reduce one fifth of depression cases (19.7%). Tar-
for depression within a general population (AUC = 0.71) (Nigatu, Liu, geting students at college entry who broke recently up with a romantic
& Wang, 2016) and primary care samples (AUC = 0.82) (Bellon et al., partner, could have preventive effects for one fifth of subsequent MDD
2011) and are also comparable to other fields of medicine (Karnes cases. In general, offering such specific interventions, subsequent to a
et al., 2017; ten Haaf et al., 2017). However, to achieve optimal perfor- screening at college student entrance, might result in a more develop-
mance, recalibration of models is needed prior to applying the models mental approach to the prevention of depression during adolescence
to a new population. As predictors included in the model contribute and emerging adulthood which may ultimately help decrease the large
to a model's calibration capacity, it is important to develop target burden associated with this disorder in young people.
group-specific prediction algorithms because predictors for the risk Finally, prevalence estimates of STBs were, potentially due to the
of MDD onset and their predicted values may differ among different exclusion of MDD baseline cases in the present study, somewhat lower
population segments (i.e., college students). The risk prediction algo- than recent estimates of STB cross-national prevalence rates (Mortier
rithm could be used to predict future MDD among incoming freshmen. et al., 2018). These low prevalence rates lead, as stated above, to a com-
More research on the validation of such specific risk prediction models parable low proportion of MDD cases in the population attributable to
is warranted; nevertheless, it is a promising methodology and enables STBs. However, due to the disabling nature of STBs and their adverse
interesting opportunities for the development of individualized consequences, there nevertheless is a clear need for interventions
approaches for MDD in emerging adults. Data on self-reported risk that are specifically designed to reach this underreached population
factors could easily be collected by means of regular student surveys. (Mortier et al., 2018) and help affected students to cope effectively.

45
EBERT ET AL . TEXT 9
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Beekman, A. T., de Beurs, E., van Balkom, A. J., Deeg, D. J., van Dyck, R., & van
Tilburg, W. (2000). Anxiety and depression in later life: Co-occurrence
The Leuven College Survey was carried out in conjunction with the
and communality of risk factors. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157,
World Health Organization World Mental Health (WMH) survey initia- 89–95. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.157.1.89
tive and is a part of the World Mental Health International College Stu- Bellón, J. A., de Dios Luna, J., King, M., Moreno-Kustner, B., Nazareth, I.,
dent project. The WMH survey is supported by the National Institute Monton-Franco, C., … Torres-Gonzalez, F. (2011). Predicting the onset
of Mental Health (NIMH; R01MH070884), the John D. and Catherine of major depression in primary care: International validation of a risk
prediction algorithm from Spain. Psychological Medicine, 41, 2075–2088.
T. MacArthur Foundation, the Pfizer Foundation, the US Public Health
https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291711000468
Service (R13-MH066849, R01-MH069864, and R01 DA016558),
Berking, M., Wirtz, C. M., Svaldi, J., & Hofmann, S. G. (2014). Emo-
the Fogarty International Center (FIRCA R03-TW006481), the
tion regulation predicts symptoms of depression over five years.
Pan American Health Organization, Eli Lilly and Company, Ortho- Behaviour Research and Therapy, 57, 13–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/
McNeil Pharmaceutical, GlaxoSmithKline, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. j.brat.2014.03.003
A complete list of all within-country and cross-national WMH pub- Bernardini, F., Attademo, L., Cleary, S. D., Luther, C., Shim, R. S., Quar-
lications can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmh/. In tesan, R., & Compton, M. T. (2017). Risk prediction models in psy-
chiatry. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 78, 572–583. https://doi.org/
Belgium specifically, these activities were supported by the Belgian
10.4088/JCP.15r10003
Fund for Scientific Research (11N0514N/11N0516N/1114717N), the
Brandy, J. M., Penckofer, S., Solari-Twadell, P. A., & Velsor-Friedrich, B.
King Baudouin Foundation (2014-J2140150-102905), and Eli Lilly
(2015). Factors predictive of depression in first-year college students.
(IIT-H6U-BX-I002). In Germany, the preparation of this paper was Journal of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 53, 38–44.
funded by BARMER. https://doi.org/10.3928/02793695-20150126-03
Bray, R. M., & Hourani, L. L. (2007). Substance use trends among active
DECLARATION OF INTEREST duty military personnel: Findings from the United States Department of
Defense Health Related Behavior Surveys, 1980–2005. Addiction, 102,
None. 1092–1101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01841.x
Brugha, T. S., & Cragg, D. (1990). The list of threatening experiences:
ORCID The reliability and validity of a brief life events questionnaire.
Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 82, 77–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/
David D. Ebert https://orcid.org/0000-0001-6820-0146
j.1600-0447.1990.tb01360.x
Claudia Buntrock https://orcid.org/0000-0002-4974-5455
Buntrock, C., Berking, M., Smit, F., Lehr, D., Nobis, S., Riper, H., … Ebert,
Philippe Mortier https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2113-6241
D. (2017). Preventing depression in adults with subthreshold depres-
Randy Auerbach https://orcid.org/0000-0003-2319-4744 sion: Health-economic evaluation alongside a pragmatic randomized
Kiona K. Weisel https://orcid.org/0000-0003-1800-0044 controlled trial of a web-based intervention. Journal of Medical Internet
Ronald C. Kessler https://orcid.org/0000-0003-4831-2305 Research, 19, e5. https://doi.org/10.2196/jmir.6587

Pim Cuijpers https://orcid.org/0000-0001-5497-2743 Buntrock, C., Ebert, D. D., Lehr, D., Smit, F., Riper, H., Berking, M., & Cuijpers,
P. (2016). Effect of a web-based guided self-help intervention for pre-
Glenn Kiekens https://orcid.org/0000-0001-8747-3385
vention of major depression in adults with subthreshold depression a
Ronny Bruffaerts https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0330-3694 randomized clinical trial. JAMA - Journal of the American Medical Associa-
tion, 315, 1854–1863. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2016.4326
REFERENCES Cole, M. G., & Dendukuri, N. (2003). Risk factors for depression among
elderly community subjects: A systematic review and meta-analysis.
Alonso, J., Mortier, P., Auerbach, R. P., Bruffaerts, R., Vilagut, G., Cuijpers, P.,
American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 1147–1156. https://doi.org/
… Kessler, R. C. (2018). Severe role impairment associated with men-
10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1147
tal disorders: Results of the WHO World Mental Health Surveys Inter-
national College Student Project. Depression and Anxiety, 35, 802–814. Cranford, J. A., Eisenberg, D., & Serras, A. M. (2009). Substance use behav-
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22778 iors, mental health problems, and use of mental health services in a
probability sample of college students. Addictive Behaviors, 34, 134–
Arria, A. M., Caldeira, K. M., Vincent, K. B., Winick, E. R., Baron, R. A., &
145. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.09.004
O'Grady, K. E. (2013). Discontinuous college enrollment: Associations
with substance use and mental health. Psychiatric Services, 64, 165–172. de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., Smit, F., Vollebergh, W. A. M., & Spijker, J. (2002).
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ps.201200106 Risk factors for 12-month comorbidity of mood, anxiety, and substance
use disorders: Findings from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey
Auerbach, R. P., Alonso, J., Axinn, W. G., Cuijpers, P., Ebert, D. D.,
and Incidence Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 620–629.
Green, J. G., … Bruffaerts, R. (2016). Mental disorders among
https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.620
college students in the World Health Organization World Mental
Health Surveys. Psychological Medicine, 46, 2955–2970. https://doi.org/ DeMartini, K. S., & Carey, K. B. (2012). Optimizing the use of the AUDIT for
10.1017/S0033291716001665 alcohol screening in college students. Psychological Assessment, 24, 954–
963. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028519
Auerbach, R. P., Mortier, P., Bruffaerts, R., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Cuijpers,
P., … & Murray, E. (2018). WHO World Mental Health Surveys Inter- Ebert, D. D., Hopfinger, L., & Berking, M. (2017). Emotion regulation pro-
national College Student Project: Prevalence and predictors of mental tects against recurrence of depressive symptoms following inpatient
disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(7), 623–638. care for major depressive disorder. Behavior Therapy, 48, 739–749.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2017.03.003
Babor, T., Higgins-Biddle, J. C., Saunders, J. B., & Monteiro, M. G. (2001). The
alcohol use disorders identification test: Guidelines for use in primary Ebert, D. D., Van Daele, T., Nordgreen, T., Karekla, M., Compare, A., Zarbo,
care. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization. C., … Baumeister, H. (2018). Internet- and mobile-based psychological

46
Music Therapy
TEXT 10

Today
WFMT online journal
Volume 14, No. 1

Music Therapy Today publishes articles that are related to music therapy
education, practice, and research. Categories may include, but are not li-
mited to Editorials, Presidential Notes, Position Statements, Curriculum
Reports, Clinical Case Studies, Research Reports, Service Projects, World
Congresses Proceedings, Interviews, Book Reviews, and Online Resources.

2018 WFMT. All rights reserved. ISSN: 1610-191X

47
TEXT 10

Implementing a Song as a Reward for


Transition from Free-Play Time to a
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Group Activity
Kumi Sato
Shigeki Sonoyama

Abstract Resumen

Even though there are substantial numbers A pesar de que hay un número considerable
of studies investigating transition strategies, de estudios que investigan estrategias de tran-
there is still an interest in and the need for sición, todavía existe un interés y la necesidad
research on transition due to its frequent oc- de investigar sobre la transición debido a su
currence in everyday life. Research suggests frecuente ocurrencia en la vida cotidiana. Los
implementation of music as a prompt effec- estudios existentes sobre este tema sugieren
tive to promote smooth transitions; how- la implementación de la música como un
ever, the effect of music implemented as a medio eficaz para promover transiciones sin
reward has not been discussed yet. Since re- problemas; sin embargo, el efecto de la mú-
wards are used as commonly as prompts in sica implementada como recompensa no se
educational settings, the current study exam- ha discutido aún. Debido a que las recompen-
ined the effect of a song as a reward for sas se utilizan con tanta frecuencia como pau-
younger children with developmental disabi- tas en entornos educativos, el estudio actual
lities during transitions, in comparison with comparó el efecto de una canción como re-
the effect of a song as a prompt. The parti- compensa para niños con trastornos del desa-
cipants were three children at the age of 3 or rrollo durante las transiciones, con el efecto de
4, who required support to initiate and com- una canción como aviso. Los participantes
plete transitions, especially from free-play eran tres niños con edades de 3 ó 4 años, que
time to a group activity. The result indicates requerían apoyo para iniciar y completar las
two of the participants initiated the transi- transiciones, especialmente del tiempo de
tion faster when the song was implemented juego libre a una actividad grupal. El resultado
as a prompt (Intervention A), whereas the indica que dos de los participantes iniciaron la
time they took after initiation of the transi- transición más rápidamente cuando la can-
tion was reduced when the song was imple- ción se implementó como aviso (Intervención
mented as a reward (Intervention B). For the A), mientras que el tiempo que tomaron des-
other participant, implementing the song as pués del inicio de la transición se redujo
a reward was effective to decrease the time cuando la canción se implementó como re-
needed for initiating and completing the compensa (Intervención B). Para el otro par-

48
TEXT 10

transition. The effect of these interventions ticipante, la implementación de la canción


on their independence during the transition como recompensa fue efectiva para disminuir
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

will be discussed also. el tiempo necesario para iniciar y completar la


transición. También se discutirá el efecto de
Keywords: song, reward, transitions, children estas intervenciones en su nivel de indepen-
with developmental disabilities, single-sub- dencia durante la transición.
ject research design
Palabras clave: canción, recompensa, transi-
ciones, niños con discapacidades del desarrollo.

Implementing a song as a reward for transi- ving a larger selection of alternative rewards
tion from free-play time to a group activity as would be beneficial for practitioners because
interest in and needs of therapeutic use of they can attempt to find which type of re-
music is growing in the area of education, re- wards is suitable for the child and provide the
searchers have investigated its effect to teach best support. Contrary to importance and
children, especially individuals with disabili- popularity of rewards in educational settings
ties who need special support. Since learning (Hoffmann, Huff, Patterson, & Nietfeld, 2009),
in a similar environment, which children with- research on use of music as a reward is limited
out disabilities have, is one of the general in music therapy literature. Lim (2010) stated
goals for children with disabilities, research in that music stimuli worked as both a prompt
music therapy has shown how music can as- and an automatic reward, but the function
sist them acquiring necessary or expected of music as a reward was not directly exa-
skills (Katagiri, 2009; De Mers, Tincani, Van mined in this study. Although research has
Norman, & Higgins, 2009; Register, Darrow, explored effective use of music as a prompt,
Standley, & Swedberg, 2007). Some studies potential effect of music as a reward has not
were conducted in a school or home setting investigated yet.
so that the participants can maintain the skills
after termination of the music therapy inter- Research on effective strategies incorpora-
vention without additional training (Kern, ting rewards is needed in other area of dis-
Wakeford, & Aldridge, 2007; Kern, Wolery, & ciplines also. Sterling-Turner and Jordan
Aldridge, 2007; Pasiali, 2004; Register & (2007) conducted a literature review of re-
Humpal, 2007). Music was used to deliver a search on interventions to support transi-
cue or create a structure in these studies; in tions in individuals with autism, and they
other word, music was provided to promote pointed out most of the available studies exa-
specific behaviors before the target behaviors mined interventions using some kinds of
occur. prompts including verbal prompts/audio
cues, visual support, and video priming.
In practical educational settings, however, re- They argued the need for further studies in-
wards are selected and offered as much as vestigating consequent components in tran-
prompts, depending on social context and sitions. Even though there is a substantial
environmental conditions, to enhance appro- number of empirical studies about promo-
priate behaviors of children. Therefore, ha- ting smooth transitions, interest in research

49
TEXT 10

associated with transitions is ongoing due to The effect of music in promoting smooth
its frequent occu-rrence in daily life and the transitions has been examined also (Gad-
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

likelihood that children with disabilities find berry, 2011; Register & Humpal, 2007). The
it challenging. results of these studies demonstrated musi-
cal interventions decreased transition times
A few studies about the effect of rewards in as well as increased independence in the par-
transition times include the research con- ticipants during transitions. In the guideline
ducted by Waters, Lerman, and Hovanetz for identifying appropriate transition support,
(2009). The participants were two 6-year music and singing are also listed as an exam-
old boys diagnosed with autism, and they ple of auditory prompts besides verbal war-
had difficulty terminating a preferred activ- nings and timers (Hume, Sreckovic, Snyder, &
ity and initiating a non-preferred activity. Carnahan, 2014). However, music was incor-
The results indicate that a visual schedule, porated as a prompt in these models. There-
which is a commonly used prompt, would fore, the current study will examine the effect
not be effective by itself, and it should be of music provided as a reward in transition
combined with appropriate rewards and times, compared to the effect of music pro-
limited access to preferred activities, that vided as a prompt.
is extinction. Cote, Thompson, and McKer-
char (2005) provided three typically deve- Furthermore, since transition requires a se-
loping toddlers with interventions for tran- quence of tasks including terminating an
sitioning from the play area to the toileting engaged activity, physically moving or shif-
area. They found that the participants’ ting attention, and preparing for the next
compliance increased if access to preferred activity, what part of transition a child find
activities was not allowed after the initial it challenging should be different. Some
instruction; moreover, the effect was even children might need support to start physi-
more significant when a reward (e.g. a toy cally moving even though they can finish
to carry with) was delivered with the ex- the previous activity without any prompts
tinction procedure, compared to when a (Sterling-Turner & Jordan, 2007). Others
verbal warning was given 2 minutes prior might need prompts to terminate an en-
to a transition. Hanley, Tiger, and Ingvarsson gaged activity though they can quickly clean
(2009) investigated strategies to increase up and move to a different area once they
preschoolers’ selection of non-preferred finish the previous activity. Hume, Srec-
but academically important activities during kovic, Snyder, and Carnahan (2014) argued
free-play time. Although their research was how important it is to individualize transi-
not conducted in scheduled transitions, tion support because the best intervention
they encouraged the participants to transi- and when it should be implemented varies
tion from a preferred activity to a non-pre- depending on the child’s chronological/de-
ferred activity in the free-play period. The velopmental age or abilities. In addition to
results revealed embedded reinforcement, comparing the effect of music as a reward
such as decorating the activity area with and its effect as a prompt during transitions,
popular children’s cartoon characters, in- the authors will deeply discuss how diffe-
creased and maintained the participants’ rently each participant in this study, who
engagement in originally non-preferred ac- had different learning needs, responded to
tivities. the interventions.

50
TEXT 10

Method (b) pseudonyms would be used as information


of their child would be kept strictly confiden-
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Setting tial; and (c) they had a right to withdraw from


the study if they thought their child would not
This study was conducted at a day treatment benefit by participating in it. All of the parents
facility in Japan, which provides services for were willing to have their child participate in
children with developmental disabilities un- the study and signed the consent form.
der 6 years old. Children and their parents
can decide how many days in a week they use Kenta was a 4-year-old boy diagnosed with
the services depending on the child’s learning Autism Spectrum Disorders. His score on the
needs. Most of the children uses the services Tsumori-Inage Infant Developmental Scale,
approximately 3 hours a day to learn self-care which is a parent questionnaire–based test
or academic skills, which cannot be ad- commonly used in Japan (Kurita, Osada,
dressed on an individual basis at regular pre- Shimizu, & Tachimori, 2003), was 75 indicating
school/kindergarten and go to regular pre- the severity of the disability was mild. Impro-
school/kindergarten for the rest of the day. ving attention span was one of the challenges
Some children come to the facility to practice for him and he required frequent prompts to
learning in a group before they start going to stay on task. Sometimes he didn’t use the
regular preschool/kindergarten. bathroom even though he spontaneously said
he wanted to use it and went into the room.
After a circle time in the morning, at this faci- Kenta could engage in a task/activity relatively
lity, each child follows a schedule shown on longer if there were no peers around; in other
own schedule board, which is planned for in- words, he was greatly influenced by other
dividual learning needs to be addressed. The peers’ behaviors. When he heard the timer,
basic schedule includes: circle time, toilet, which was used as the signal for cleaning up
hand washing, snack, pre-academic tasks, at the day treatment facility, he often said “It’s
free-play time (i.e. unconstructed play), group time to clean up” or “Let’s put toys away” to
activity, lunch, tooth brushing, and going peers. However, he didn’t finish playing if
home. In this daily schedule, not all of the chil- other peers were still engaging in the play.
dren were able to have smooth transitions
from free-play time to group activity. There- Sara was a 4-year-old girl who had no speci-
fore, music intervention was implemented to fied developmental delays. According to the
encourage children finish playing, putting toys Tsumori-Inage Infant Developmental Scale
away, and having a seat for the next activity. she took at 27 months, her developmental
age was diagnosed as 21 months (DQ 78). At
Participants the time of the study, Sara had no difficulty
understanding verbal directions or expressing
The purpose of this study and its procedure her needs in words except when she had
was clearly explained to parents of three chil- tantrums. She also spoke to her peers often
dren who needed support during transitions though she rarely played interactively with
from free-play time to group activity. Their pa- them. Sara was able to perform most of the
rents understood: (a) this study was approved tasks independently without much physical
by the University of Tsukuba Ethics Committee support; however, she spent long time com-
for Research in the Faculty of Human Sciences; pleting a task since her movements were

51
TEXT 10

slower generally. She had a tendency to re- Beat


fuse new or unfamiliar activities because of
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

her anxious personality. 4/4 64


3/4 3
Masa was a 3-year-old boy diagnosed with no
specified developmental delays. His DQ score 2/4 31
on the Tsumori-Inage Infant Developmental
others** 2
Scale, that he took at 26 months was 62.
However, by the time of the study, he was **These two songs are played in 6/8 beats.
able to communicate verbally with adults or
peers. Although he had the ability to perform Tempo (BPM)
most of the tasks, the child care aides consis-
tently needed to provide prompts for him. Presto (168-208) 2
Masa particularly had difficulty in finishing his Allegro (120-168) 48
play. He understood he was supposed to put
toys away if he heard the sound of a timer, Moderato (108-120) 16
but he often said “I don’t want to” or “Wait!” Andante (76-108) 23
and refused to finish playing.
Adagio (66-76) 3
Songs
Largo (40-66) 8

The authors conducted a brief analysis of chil-


dren songs to investigate some characteristics Based on the brief analysis, two original songs
in popular and well-known children songs in were composed for this study; “Clean Up, Up,
this culture. A CD set with 100 children songs Up!” was to use as a prompt to encourage
(Minnna, 2014) was selected for the analysis children putting toys away, and “Well Done,
because it included both new and traditional Finished!” was to use as a reward to praise
songs. Table 1 shows the characteristics of for cleaning up (See Appendix). Both songs
well-known children songs. It implies songs in were composed in a major key in 4/4 beats
a major key, in 4/4 beats, and at a faster and played at a faster tempo between 120
tempo are preferred. Moreover, 53 out of 100 and 168 BPM. Additionally, repeated sounds
songs used repeated sounds, words, or ono- were included in the lyrics to create some
matopoeia to create some rhythmic patterns. rhythmic patterns.

Table 1. Brief analysis of 100 children songs in Prior to the study, many of the child care aides
Japan. reported they felt uncomfortable leading the
songs because they didn’t have an advance
Key music educational background. Some studies
major 92 indicated live music was more effective than
recorded music (Gadberry, 2011). However,
minor 3 since the authors placed importance on de-
veloping teaching strategies, which can be im-
others* 5
plemented easily without special instruction,
*Others include traditional folk songs using spe- the songs were recorded instrumentally and
cial melodic scales. played with a CD player for the child care aides

52
TEXT 10

to sing along in this study. Two recorded CD player when the timer rang, instead of pro-
were placed at the facility for the child care viding a verbal direction. For Intervention B
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

aids to learn the songs. (the song provided as a reward), the basic
procedures during were identical to those in
Procedure the baseline phase except that the child care
aides sang “Well Done, Finished!” along with
This study was conducted using a single-sub- the participants after they had a seat, instead
ject research design across participants. The of providing verbal praises. In the follow-up
data was collected for four weeks in each session, the procedure was identical to the
phase and follow up data was taken one baseline phase.
month after the termination of interventions.
The latency until the participant initiated the Interobserver agreement. The first author
cleaning task, the total transition time until the watched the video recordings of all sessions
participant required to have a seat for the next as the first observer. Then, the second ob-
activity, the number and kinds of prompts pro- server, who was a doctoral student studying
vided, toy categories the participant was play- special education, watched the video recor-
ing with, and the child care aide who sup- dings and collected the data for 40% of all
ported the participant were recorded. A video sessions for Masa, 38% of sessions for Kenta,
camera was set up in the corner of the room and 35% of sessions for Sara. The IOA rate
to record the latency, the total transition time, was assessed by the length of time each par-
and the prompts later; in addition, the first au- ticipant required after the initiation of the
thor was present every time to record the toy cleaning task, which was calculated by sub-
categories and the child care aides on a data tracting the latency from the total time taken.
collection sheet. The mean duration per occurrence IOA was
97% for Masa, 96% for Kenta and Sara.
Baseline. When a timer rang as a signal to fi-
nish playing and put toys away, the child care The number of verbal, visual, and physical
aides gave a verbal prompt such as “Please prompts were recorded separately by the
put your toy away.” Then, they waited for 1 same observers. Any verbal directions were
minute without providing any other prompts defined as verbal prompts, and visual cues in-
to see if the participants spontaneously start cluding pointing, showing a picture card, pre-
cleaning up. Additional prompts were given senting a box to put the toys in, or demonstra-
as much as the participants needed to finish ting the cleaning task were defined as visual
playing, put toys away, and have a seat for the prompts. Physical contacts including tapping,
next activity after the 1-minute interval. Ver- holding a hand, or holding the participant up
bal praises were provided for completing the were defined as physical prompts. The exact
cleaning task as it had been practiced at the agreement IOA for Masa was 67% in verbal
facility. prompts, 100% in visual prompts, and 78% in
physical prompts. The exact agreement IOA for
Interventions and follow-up. The basic pro- Kenta was 60% in verbal prompts, 100% in vi-
cedures during Intervention A (the song pro- sual prompts, and 100% in physical prompts.
vided as a prompt) were identical to those in The exact agreement IOA for Sara was 80% in
the baseline phase except that the song verbal prompts, 60% in visual prompts, and
“Clean Up, Up, Up!” was played with a CD 100% in physical prompts.

53
TEXT 10
Results quired the participants to complete the
cleaning task, physically move to another
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Figure 1 shows the total transition time as area, and have a seat for the next activity.
well as latency to initiation of the cleaning The number of data points is different since
task, which required the participants to fi how many days in a week each child used
nish an engaged activity, and the time after the facility was decided by their parents, de-
initiation of the cleaning task, which re- pending on their learning needs. Kenta re-
quired slightly less time for the transition
during Intervention A, while the transition
time Sara took was reduced in Intervention
B. The total time Masa spent on the transi-
tion decreased when the song was imple-
mented as a reward; however, it increased
again as the intervention continued.

Table 2 shows the mean time of before (i.e.


latency) and after initiation of the cleaning
task in each phase respectively. Kenta and
Masa initiated the cleaning task faster du-
ring Intervention A; on the other hand, in
regard to the time required after initiation
of the task, their compliance was increased
in Intervention B, when the song was deli-
vered as a reward. They did not spend much
time once they finished playing and started
putting toys away. Sara was stably able to
start putting toys away within 100 seconds
during Intervention B. Although she com-
pleted the cleaning task quickly during
Baseline without considering the toy cate-
gories she was playing, she required less
time after initiation of the task in Interven-
tion B, in a comparison between Interven-
tion A and B.

Table 3 shows the average number of


prompts provided for each participant. For
Sara and Masa, the number was the mini-
Figure 1. The filled circles represent the total
mum in every kind of prompts during Inter-
transition time. Two different dotted areas
vention B. Although the number of prompts
represent the latency to initiation of the
cleaning task (low density) and the time af- provided for Kenta decreased in Intervention
ter initiation of the cleaning task (high den- A, it increased again in Intervention B. He re-
sity) respectively. The blank circle in the fol- quired more verbal and physical prompts in
low-up session represents the latency. this phase compared to baseline.

54
TEXT 10

Table 2. The mean of latency and the time after initiation of the task in each phase.
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Baseline Intervention A Intervention B


Latency 158.7 72.8 146.4
Kenta
After 121.7 141.0 94.7

Latency 227.5 199.2 41.6


Sara
After 34.8 137.0 102.8

Latency 209.9 141.9 176.0


Masa
After 107.0 160.6 80.2

Table 3. The average number of prompts provided for each participant.

Baseline Intervention A Intervention B


verbal 3.7 2.4 6.0

Kenta visual 3.0 1.0 1.6

physical 1.3 0.6 1.4

verbal 3.5 4.6 2.0

Sara visual 2.5 3.8 1.4

physical 1.3 1.6 0.6

verbal 5.6 5.7 5.1

Masa visual 4.4 1.0 1.0

physical 2.7 2.9 2.4

Discussion tive for them to complete the cleaning task


and be ready for the next activity in shorter
The total transition time required for the time. On the other hand, for Sara, imple-
transition decreased in either Intervention menting a song as a reward was effective to
A or B in every participant. However, how finish playing as well as be ready for the next
these musical interventions functioned was activity. In terms of how independently the
different in each participant. The mean time participants completed the transition, Table
of before and after initiation of the cleaning 3 suggests the amount of support Sara and
task implies implementing a song as a prompt Masa required was decreased as the study
was effective for Kenta and Masa to finish continued, and it can be concluded they
playing and start putting toys away, whereas learned what is expected during the transi-
implementing a song as a reward was effec- tion.

55
TEXT 10

Detailed discussion on each participant song implemented in Intervention B and sang


it along with a smile, he required more
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

Kenta. Kenta regularly told his peers to put prompts to initiate the cleaning task. There-
toys away when he heard the sound of the fore, it is difficult to conclude that musical in-
timer though he did not initiate the cleaning terventions helped him improve his inde-
task until the child care aides gave him the di- pendence during the transition. Since he still
rection. He had difficulty staying on task, and needed a certain amount of support to stay
his behaviors were highly influenced by other engaged, the song as a prompt would be
peers’ behaviors; for example, he stopped his more effective for him, in terms of the total
hands and stared at a peer for a while if the transition time.
peer started crying. Kenta often played with
Masa, and he followed Masa especially for Sara. Sara strongly refused to finish paying
the initiation of the cleaning task. If Masa and put toys away at first. She expressed her
started the cleaning task faster, he could start anger not only by saying “No” but also by cry-
it faster. If Masa took time to finish playing, ing loudly or thrashing her arm and legs.
Kenta needed time to finish playing. Thus, while she spent time regulating herself,
the child care aides put most of the toys away
When a song was implemented as a prompt and left one piece for her to complete the
during Intervention A, Kenta stared at the cleaning task. Due to this flexible response to
child care aide singing the song, and he was her behaviors, the time required after initia-
able to start putting toys away immediately af- ting the cleaning task was significantly shorter
ter the song. He sometimes stopped his hands in the baseline phase, besides the difference
if he saw peers still playing, yet he could in the toy categories she preferred playing
restart the cleaning task and have a seat with with in each phase. The child care aides had
a few verbal prompts usually. He said “Let’s been struggling to have her finish playing
start (the next activity)” and invited peers to without difficulty and put all the toys away by
come after him when he became ready for the herself.
next activity. Kenta’s behaviors during Inter-
vention B were very similar to what was ob- In the beginning of Intervention, A, Sara
served in the baseline phase. He told his peers stared at the child care aide singing the song,
or himself to put toys away when he heard the yet after a while, she started refusing to finish
sound of the timer; however, he could not playing. However, those behaviors were gra-
spontaneously initiate the cleaning task until dually decreased, and she did not exhibit any
prompts were provided after the 1-minute in- behaviors, which refuse the cleaning task, at
terval. One of the reasons why the total tran- all after Day 8. One possible reason for this
sition time dropped dramatically in the follow- sudden change is she preferred playing with
up session is that Masa was absent on that the same toy from that point. It was a set of
day. If Kenta was playing alone, he followed finger puppets, and she did not have to share
directions without difficulty. it with other peers like blocks; therefore, she
might have been highly satisfied with her
Kenta needed fewer prompts in Intervention playtime before finishing the play. Sara liked
A, yet this is partly because he had the same the song implemented as a reward and some-
child care aide for support during this phase. times sang it by herself although she had
Although he showed a great interest in the never sung a song during music activities.

56
TEXT 10

Sara was able to put all the pieces of the toy, the song gradually, however, and this could
which she preferred playing in the second be the reason why the total transition time
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

half of the study, back to the original place by he required increased towards the end
herself. She spent longer time to complete again.
the cleaning task since her movements were
slow, yet the child care aides respected her In the second half of the study, Masa often
independence and did not provide additional had a seat with a toy in his hand immediately
help unless she asked for it. The decrease in after he heard the sound of the timer. He was
the number of prompts suggests her inde- redirected to put the toy back to the original
pendence during the transition improved place as the cleaning task was a part of the
throughout the study. Although the toy cate- transition practiced at the facility, yet his be-
gories are considered as one of the factors havior told he became able to shift his atten-
which reduced the total transition time, a tion to the next activity in shorter time. His
song as a reward was more effective to ad- behaviors were easily changed by health con-
dress her learning needs generally. ditions or feelings as well as how much time
he could have for free-play time on the day,
Masa. At the beginning of the study, Masa re- thus his performance during the transition
fused to finish playing almost every time he was variable overall. If he could not have a
heard the sound of the timer. He knew the smooth transition, he usually had difficulty
timer meant it was time to put toys away be- engaging in an activity later on the day. Al-
cause he asked the child care aides “Did it though there was not a significant decrease
ring?” and then said “I’m not coming.” In ad- in the total transition time, some changes,
dition, even though he put toys away, he which would probably lead to the decrease in
could not have a seat immediately after that. the time in the future, were observed in his
He lied down on the floor and rolled around behaviors.
until the child care aid took him to the chair.
Therefore, finishing the play and having a seat Limitations and implications
for the next activity were both challenging for
him. In order to closely examine the difference in
the effects between two interventions, Inter-
During Intervention A, the frequency of his vention A and B should have been repeated
verbal refusals to finish playing decreased in an A-B-C-B-C design; furthermore, the
gradually, and Masa became able to initiate combined effect needs to be investigated in
the cleaning task with a few verbal prompts. addition to the independent effect of each in-
He sometimes sang a part of the song along tervention. However, besides this study was
with the recorded music. However, he still conducted at the end of a school year, Sara
had difficulty having a seat, thus the time re- and Masa decided to terminate the use of the
quired after the initiation of the cleaning task services and go to regular kindergarten/
did not improve much. When the song was preschool in the next school year. Additio-
implemented as a reward in Intervention B, nally, since it was carried out during winter,
Masa showed a great interest in the song. He some participants were absent for a longer
said “yay!” and had a seat immediately if the period due to their health conditions, and
child care aide told him he was going to sing thus it was difficult to collect data as much as
the song. Masa became less interested in scheduled.

57
TEXT 10

The authors respected the management po- ventions and extinction on toddlers’ com-
licy of the facility and determined not to as- pliance during transitions. Journal of Ap-
MUSICTHERAPYTODAy, Volume 14, No. 1, 2018

sign a specific child care aid to each partici- plied Behavior Analysis, 38, 235-238.
pant. If each participant had the same child De Mers, C. L., Tincani, M., Van Norman, R. K.,
care aid throughout the study, however, the & Higgins, K. (2009). Effects of music ther-
data, especially the number of prompts taken apy on young children’s challenging be-
to analyze the change in their independence, haviors: A case study. Music Therapy Pers-
would have been more validate. pectives, 27(2), 88-96.
Gadberry, D. L. (2011). The effect of music on
In addition, the sample size was limited be- transitions and spoken redirections in a
cause there were not many children using the preschool classroom (Doctoral disserta-
services more than three days a week regu- tion). Retrieved from ProQuest Central;
larly at the time of study. Recruiting more par- ProQuest Dissertations & Theses Global.
ticipants at the day treatment facility or other (Order No. 3458216)
educational support centers is needed for fu- Hanley, G. P., Tiger, J. H., & Ingvarsson, E. T.
ture research to increase research validity as (2009). Influencing preschoolers’ free-play
well as examine if the findings could be gener- activity preferences: An evaluation of sa-
alized. tiation and embedded reinforcement.
Hoffmann, K. F., Huff, J. D., Patterson, A. S., &
The result of this study implies music as a Nietfeld, J. L. (2009). Elementary teachers’
prompt could be effective to encourage initia- use and perception of rewards in the
ting a task whereas music as a reward could classroom. Teaching and Teacher Educa-
be effective to completing a task. Further- tion, 25, 843-849.
more, it suggests how and when musical in- Hume, K., Sreckovic, M., Snyder, K., & Carna-
terventions should be implemented would be han, C. R. (2014). Smooth transitions: Hel-
different depending on individual learning ping students with autism spectrum disor-
needs. Since rewards are commonly used in der navigate the school day. Teaching Ex-
practical educational settings as well as ceptional Children, 47, 35-45.
prompts, additional research is needed to ex- Katagiri, J. (2009). The effect of background
plore more strategies to implement music as music and song texts on the emotional un-
a reward so that educators, parents, and other derstanding of children with autism. Jour-
specialists working for children with special nal of Music Therapy, 46(1), 15-31.
needs can have a larger repertoire of alterna- Kern, P., Wakeford, L., & Aldridge, D. (2007). Im-
tive teaching strategies. It is highly recom- proving the performance of a young child
mended future research considers use of with autism during self-care tasks using em-
recorded music so that those who are not mu- bedded song interventions: A case study.
sic therapists or who do not have an advance Music Therapy Perspectives, 25(1), 43-51.
music educational background can incorpo- Kern, P., Wolery, M., & Aldridge, D. (2007).
rate the strategy without special training. Use of songs to promote independence in
morning greeting routines for young chil-
References dren with autism. Journal of Autism & De-
velopmental Disorders, 37, 1264-1271.
Cote, C. A., Thompson, R. H., & McKerchar, P. Kurita, H., Osada, H., Shimizu, K., & Tachimori,
M. (2005). The effects of antecedent inter- H. (2003). Validity of DQ as an estimate of

58
TEXT 11

5 News

A few questions about ‘mental disorders’

How many times have you seen a video on Facebook about mental
disorder? How about an over-reaching headline on the same topic,
10 something like “Depression: Solved by science”?

21 August 2018

Such communications can be great. They package complex issues into bite-
size pieces and keep the public informed about a vital research topic. They also
have a dark side.

15 By oversimplifying things, and showing us lots of images of brains, these kinds


of communications instil us, the viewers and readers, with confidence.
Confidence that science understands mental disorders and that neuroscience in
particular will soon solve them.

Unfortunately this confidence is not warranted. Psychiatry is effectively in crisis,


20 at least in regards to its diagnostic system. The DSM-5 (the Holy Bible big-bad-
list of official mental disorders) is riddled with issues and confidence in it is
waning. One brief example: there are currently so many criteria for post-
traumatic stress disorder that you can meet those criteria in 636,120 different
ways.

25 In 2013, Thomas Insel, then director of the National Institute of Mental Health
(NIMH) in the US, put it plain when he said: “Patients with mental disorders
deserve better.”

Responding to such issues, many researchers are putting their confidence in


neuroscience. Insel himself wrote the above statement while releasing the

59
30 NIMH’s solution to the diagnostic crisis: a research funding framework called
RDoC. RDoC explicitly focuses on understanding the brain, putting topics like
culture and values largely to the side. Why? Because the first of RDoC’s stated
core assumptions is that mental disorders are brain disorders.

Don’t get me wrong, studying the brain has huge potential, but this is a big
35 assumption. The truth is that the fundamental question “What is mental
disorder?” is still hotly debated. It is this question that interests me.

I’ve been working from a school of thought known as 3e Cognition (or


sometimes ‘enactive embodiment’). I’ll spare you the details. Effectively, 3e
Cognition takes the emphasis off the brain alone and instead considers the
40 whole person – as a brain and body system – trying to survive and flourish
within their physical and social environment.

This view recognises the importance of the brain but allows for a more
comprehensive perspective; through the brain and body and out to culture and
environment. Taking such a perspective, it is difficult to see how mental
45 disorders can simply be brain disorders, as per the assumptions of RDoC. With
its broader perspective, the assumptions of 3e Cognition seem a much better
basis from which to seek to understand mental disorder.

It can also help us determine what counts as mental disorder and what doesn’t.
If you have a heart attack, we can all easily agree this is a bad thing. But the
50 story is different when it comes to mental disorder.

If you are seeing things that aren’t there, this may be psychosis. Alternatively, it
might be a valued spiritual experience. These two options are likely to look the
same in the brain. But if you consider the person’s culture and values, these are
very different things.

55 I have been trying to show that 3e thinking can help with this distinction. It
allows us to talk about the biological stuff going on but also the individual’s
values and culture. All under a single scientifically minded framework.

For example, say ‘Mary’ is seeing visions of her dead grandmother. Is this
mental illness? 3e thinking would encourage us to ask, “Is this working for this
60 person?” Perhaps Mary values these experiences, perhaps it is helping her with
other difficulties in her life.

My PhD has so far been concerned with trying to understand what the concept
of metal disorder looks like from a 3e perspective. As I move into the second
half of my project, I am starting to shift my focus to the question ‘If this is what
65 mental disorders are, then how should we best seek to explain them?’

60
The field of mental health is at a crossroads. We have acknowledged many
issues in the way we recognise and understand disorder. We need to respond
to this, but blind confidence is dangerous in the face of complexity. If we only
focus on the brain, I’m sure we will discover some amazing things, but our
70 understanding of mental disorders will remain incomplete, and we will thereby
be doing a disservice to those that suffer them.

Kristopher Nielsen is a member of the Explanation of Psychopathology and


Crime research lab at Victoria University of Wellington. This article is an
adaptation of his presentation as a finalist in the University’s Three Minute
75 Thesis competition.

61
Anthropological Forum
A Journal of Social Anthropology and Comparative Sociology

TEXT 12
ISSN: 0066-4677 (Print) 1469-2902 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/canf20

Mind Change: How Digital Technologies Are


Leaving Their Mark on Our Brains, by Susan
Greenfield

Julian Ch Lee

To cite this article: Julian Ch Lee (2015) Mind Change: How Digital Technologies Are Leaving
Their Mark on Our Brains, by Susan Greenfield, Anthropological Forum, 25:3, 315-317, DOI:
10.1080/00664677.2015.1021442

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00664677.2015.1021442

Published online: 24 Mar 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 143

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=canf20

Download by: [Purdue University Libraries]


62
Date: 23 March 2016, At: 21:17
Book Reviews
for kayaks and umiaks, quivers for arrows, and The title of Susan Greenfield’s new book, Mind 1
even nets for catching birds; thread was made Change, intends to suggest that the presence of
from their sinews, needles and scraping tools digital technologies is having social and psycho-
from their bones, and their antlers were beauti- logical impacts that are similar in scale to climate
fully and intricately carved. Sealskin was likewise change. And in the same way as the present and
used for tents and for covering kayaks, and seal forecasted impacts of climate change are largely
intestines, especially from the Bearded Seal, negative, so too are the impacts of the ‘screen
furnished waterproof undergarments. The culture’ which is arising from the ever-present
ivory of walruses, whenever this could be and always-on digital devices to which most
obtained, was used mainly in making decora- now reach for and engage with within 15
tions. Whales, particularly the Beluga, gave fat minutes of consciousness each day (17).
(used for fuel and light) as well as meat and Books that treat the impact of digital technol- 2
skin, and their stomachs were used as bags. ogies on people and society can often be highly
Downloaded by [Purdue University Libraries] at 21:17 23 March 2016

Parts of the Arctic Hare were valued in shamanic optimistic, wholly pessimistic or seek place
rituals. Descriptions and illustrations of all this themselves between the cyber-utopians and
are accompanied in the book by traditional cyber-dystopians. Greenfield’s book clearly
tales featuring animals, some entertaining, falls within the second category, and surveys
some didactic, some rather sad. the array of concerns that she has about the
There is a table of Inuit terms for animals and impacts of the hyper-connected state of society
their parts, an appendix of his biological collec- and the impacts of spending so much time
tions now in the Smithsonian, and a glossary. engaging with a screen, the behavioural out-
There are photos of many of the specimens comes of which she refers to as ‘screen culture’.
(including the material culture items), and a While many who teach undergraduates can
large number of atmospheric black-and-white share anecdotes of student inattentiveness in 3
photos, many of them taken by Turner class as they browse Facebook, and have
himself. Bryony Anderson’s drawings of the heard student complaints about the length of
animals add flavour. There is a map on pages even modest readings, Greenfield draws on
12–13; I wish this could have been more acces- an array of research findings, particularly in
sible—perhaps a frontispiece—because I con- the fields of psychology and neuroscience, to
stantly needed to look at it to remind myself support her perspective that digital technol-
of where particular places were. This is my ogies are having largely negative impacts on
only criticism of what is otherwise a well- people, including on their attention spans. A
produced, informative, and important book. key premise of this perspective is that a key
TEXT 12 feature of the brain is its neuroplasticity, that
is, its ability to be shaped, both functionally
Mind Change: How Digital Technologies Are and physically, by the environment with
Leaving Their Mark on Our Brains, by Susan which it engages. Ipso facto, if the present
Greenfield. Rider, London, 2014, xv + 368 pp., environment of many brains is one of immer-
preface, figures, tables, notes, further reading, sion in interactions taking place on screens,
index. ISBN 978-1-84604-430-4 (hardback). that environment must be conditioning
people’s brains in particular ways, with
JULIAN CH LEE impacts feeding through to everything from
School of Global, Urban and Social Studies their identities to their societies.
RMIT University Those familiar with neuroscience might find
© 2015 Julian ch lee Chapters 5 and 6 to be a fairly standard and 4
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00664677.2015.1021442 brief introduction to the subject. However, as

63
Book Reviews
Greenfield moves into Chapters 7 and 8, ‘How network where inflated versions of one’s own
the brain becomes a mind’ and ‘Out of your life can be posted, and validated (123–126).
mind’, one begins to see where she is going The impacts to which Greenfield refers are
with her argument. In short, she argues that not just left to stand to reason. Rather, 6
what we understand to be a human ‘mind’, as throughout, she draws on available research
opposed to just a brain, relies on maturing that provides evidence for the personal
from an on-going focus on the present and social impacts she believes digital tech-
moment, as is the case for young children, nologies are creating. For example, she cites
towards a greater encompassment of the research that indicates that social network
more abstract past and future. However, the use is associated with lower levels of
kinds of environments created by digital tech- empathy in people (135–141). However,
nologies and the internet continually reinforce while she is always careful to note that corre-
an in-the-moment state of consciousness lation is different from causation, as people
Downloaded by [Purdue University Libraries] at 21:17 23 March 2016

which does not cultivate a strong sense of self, with lower levels of empathy might be
inward self-knowledge, and wider context drawn to social networking because of diffi-
into which to embed present experiences. culties in real-world experiences, she also
Greenfield argues that they instead promote presents arguments for how and why, to con-
more emotional and in-the-moment con- tinue with the example of social networks a
sciousness seen in children and many schizo- little further, the paucity of non-verbal cues
phrenics, and not that seen in otherwise in social networks would stifle the develop-
‘normal’ adults (99). Digital technology is ment of empathy.
therefore leading us to ‘lose our minds’. In an area of study where there is a great deal
In the remainder of the book Greenfield of opinion often unbacked by evidence, Mind 7
5 explores how three areas of digital technology Change is an engaging survey of some of the
foster this. Those areas are social networking, psychological and social impacts of our immer-
video games, and surfing the web. In each she sion in digital technologies and interactions
describes how the logics and experiences of with screens. Her pessimistic orientation
each of these areas cultivates an array of qual- cannot just be dismissed as being just a biased
ities, almost all of which are socially or indivi- opinion when she has assembled so much evi-
dually undesirable. With respect to social dence to support her viewpoint.
networking, for example, the use of Facebook, However, as one reads Mind Change, one
which Greenfield uses to stand in for social net- wonders whether social scientists who have
8
working per se, is unlike most ‘real-world’ inter- long written about digital communities would
actions in that there are always people to concur, and whether Greenfield’s evidence is
interact with at a given moment, and that infor- sufficient to contradict the views of researchers
mation shared is subject to people’s approval or who have come to more sanguine, or at least
otherwise, which can be instant, thus possibly equivocal, views about the impacts of digital
instantly satisfying a craving for affirmation, technology. It is hard to tell because Greenfield
even when one’s posts are idealised or distorted seldom engages with the findings and opinions
representations of one’s life. And here begins a of such researchers. Readers could therefore
possible vicious cycle where one compares the develop a feeling that Greenfield has not
inflated achievements and experiences of addressed the other side of the argument suffi-
others with one’s own mundane life, thus ciently enough to be fully convincing.
damaging self-esteem, leading one to feel ever An example of where some social scientists
less inclined to engage with others offline, and
to confine oneself ever more to the social
would depart from Greenfield is in her use of
the term the ‘real-world’, as opposed to the
9

64
Book Reviews
online world. Many would contest this div- viii+187pp.,
ision as an overly stark separation of the figures, index. ISBN 978-1- 1186-5628-0
‘real’ and the digital. Others would prefer to (paperback).
refer to ‘online’ and ‘offline’ worlds or selves, LARA MCKENZIE
rather than use the word ‘real’ with its negative Anthropology and Sociology
or dismissive implications about the online The University of Western Australia
world. Generally, however, one can see the © 2015 Lara McKenzie
point Greenfield is making. For example, ‘In http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00664677.2014.989940
real life, actions always have consequences
and, as we know only too well, cannot be What is blood? Can we theorise blood? What
reversed. Unlike video games, no one can would a theory of blood, spanning cultures
become undead … ’ (222). and histories, look like? These are questions
Another aspect of Mind Change that might that Janet Carsten asks in the introductory
10
Downloaded by [Purdue University Libraries] at 21:17 23 March 2016

dissatisfy some is the rapidity with which some chapter of her edited volume Blood will out,
topics are addressed. It is a broad ranging originally published as a special issue in the
book and some phenomena are treated quite Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute
cursorily. An example is her discussion, or (2013). To date, Carsten argues, blood lacks
more accurately, mention, of trolls and trolling. anthropological and sociological theoris-
Trolling receives a paragraph (154) and is ations. Yet it cuts across the realms of politics,
depicted without much nuance, unlike, for religion, kinship, and the body, having a
example, The Dark Net by Jamie Bartlett, who ‘propensity to travel within, between, and
discusses the viewpoints of trolls he has inter- beyond all of these’ (2). In this volume,
viewed, leading the reader to a greater under- Carsten and her fellow contributors work
standing, although not necessarily acceptance, towards answering the questions she poses,
of people who troll and their motivations. developing an ‘anthropology of blood’ which
Mind Change therefore might be better encapsulates the many unstable meanings,
11 thought of as a provocation, an intervention associations, and anatomies of blood.
intended to stoke debate on an issue which The book works as a series of case studies,
she regards as of great significance and moving across time, space, and various facets
concern. That she takes a particular angle of social life, with Carsten highlighting conti-
and that she has a particular argument are nuities as well as discontinuities in blood’s
both clear, and therefore readers should con- symbolism. Some chapters are global in their
sider its merit in view of other angles and reach. Kath Weston, for instance, discusses
opposing arguments in order to reach their the metaphors of blood in global economic
conclusions. The book should be of interest discourse, reflected in the use of terms like
to academics interested in a passionate but lifeblood, liquidity, and circulation. Such
pessimistic assessment of the impact of metaphors, she argues, envision global finan-
digital technologies. It should also be of inter- cial institutions and their corresponding
est to non-academics, and has certainly been social relations in terms of ‘the integrated,
written for such an audience in mind. self-regulating, self-sustaining qualities of a
biological life-form’ (33). The use of organic
analogies such as these help to establish ‘the
Blood will out: Essays on liquid transfers and economy’ as a natural and unquestionable
flows, edited by Janet Carsten. Journal of the force (33). Weston shows how the metaphor
Royal Anthropological Institute Special Issue of blood underscores a ‘diagnostic regime’
Book Series. Wiley Blackwell, Malden, 2013, that aims at ensuring economic ‘health’, thus

65

You might also like