You are on page 1of 18

Original Article

New brands diluting the personality


of existing brands
Received (in revised form): 7th February 2013

Minkyung Choy
is a PhD candidate at Seoul National University. Her research interests are consumer behavior, brand management and
marketing communications.

Jae II Kim
is a Professor of Marketing at Seoul National University. His research interests are consumer behavior, brand management
and qualitative research.

ABSTRACT Marketing literature emphasizes the threats copycats pose to market


leading brands and demonstrates that copycats that are more identical to the leader
brand are evaluated more positively. This research examines whether a new brand
with varying degrees of brand element similarity changes consumer perception of
the personality of an existing brand. The associative network theory is used to
explain how a new brand affects an existing brand. A total of 160 participants were
randomly assigned to the condition of a 2(similarity: high, low)×2(familiarity: high,
low) between-subject design and completed questionnaires. When consumers are
unfamiliar with a senior brand (that is, an existing pioneer brand in a specific product
category), a junior brand (that is, a new entrant brand to the same product category)
with similar brand elements reinforces personality of the senior brand, whereas a
low-similarity junior brand dilutes them. When consumers are familiar with the
senior brand, the emergence of a junior brand reinforces senior brand personality,
regardless of whether the junior brand is similar or dissimilar to the senior brand.
The dilution of senior brand personality attributed to the emergence of a dissimilar
or unique new brand lowers consumers’ attitude toward the senior brand and
consequently decrease their purchase intention. Our findings contradict what is often
brand management’s gut reaction. The results indicate that when a new brand in
the same product category enters the market with a differentiation strategy (that
is, unique brand name and package design), the new brand can dilute the personality
of an existing brand and affect consumer’s subsequent behaviors. Also, the results
suggest that marketing effort to increase consumers’ familiarity with brands is critical
to the first comer brands.
Journal of Brand Management (2013) 20, 590–607. doi:10.1057/bm.2013.2;
Correspondence: published online 5 April 2013
Minkyung Choy
College of Business Administra-
tion, Seoul National University,
Kwanak-ro,
Keywords: brand similarity; brand personality; brand dilution; attitude; purchase intention
Kwanak-gu, Seoul 151-916,
South Korea
E-mail: mk_choy@hanmail.net

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607

www.palgrave-journals.com/bm/
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

INTRODUCTION an existing pioneer brand in a specific prod-


Consumers often find themselves buying uct category or the first comer in its prod-
products by mistake. They believe that they uct category. It is the first user of trademarks
are buying the product that they usually (for example, brand name, slogan and sym-
purchase, only to find later that what they bol) and owns such trademarks (Simonson,
bought only resembles the original product. 1994; Pullig et al, 2006). Junior brand is
Similarity in image, color, brand name and defined as a new entrant brand to the same
product packaging makes the recognition product category or the second comer
of an original product difficult. within the senior brand’s product category,
Undifferentiated and copycat products and it possesses attributes similar or dis-
flood the market, and this practice is wide- similar to those of the senior brand (Pullig
spread in many industries. When a product et al, 2006). Junior brand is a competing
of a leading company successfully penetrates party that makes consumers mistakenly
the market, other companies also see the pos- believe it is the senior brand because the
sibility of entering the same market by offer- junior brand often uses similar marks already
ing a similar product. In reality, legal disputes being used by an existing party (Simonson,
between the original trademark holder and 1994; Pullig et al, 2006).
the other party imitating the original are Specifically, the main objective of this
common because brand imitation damages research is to investigate whether similarity
the brand image of the original. in terms of brand elements, such as brand
The effect of copycat practice on copy- name and package design, between a senior
cat brands has been extensively studied, but and a junior brand dilutes or reinforces sen-
research on the effect of this practice on ior brand personality. To explain how a
original brands is limited (Horen and junior brand causes the dilution and rein-
Pieters, 2012a). Although marketing litera- forcement of senior brand personality, we
ture on copycat practice has argued that employed the associative network theory
copycats gain benefits from imitating orig- that suggests that associative constructs of
inal brands, a recent study (Horen and brands compete against one another for
Pieters, 2012a) showed the possibility of the retrieval in a shared brand network. This
opposite. A question emerges on whether study also examines whether high familiar-
a new brand imitating another weakens or ity with the senior brand alleviates the dilu-
strengthens the values associated with the tion of senior brand personality. Finally,
imitated brand. Previous studies on brand this study identifies whether the dilution or
dilution focus on the weakening of asso- reinforcement of senior brand personality
ciations between a brand and its product results in lower or higher attitude toward
category (Pullig et al, 2006). However, the senior brand compared with the junior
inasmuch as the emotional, rather than brand, consequently affecting the intention
functional, benefit of a brand is gaining con- of consumers to buy the senior brand.
siderable attention, studies on brand dilution
take a more diverse point of view, such as THEORETICAL BACKGROUND AND
the symbolic aspect of a brand. This research HYPOTHESES
focuses on the weakening of the association
between a brand and its personality which Brand dilution and associative
is a component of brand image. network theory
This study examines brand dilution in Brand dilution focuses on protecting the
the relationship between a senior brand and value of the existing brand. Based on prior
a junior brand. Senior brand is defined as research (Pattishall, 1984; Simonson, 1993),

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 591
Choy and Kim

Morrin and Jacoby (2000, p. 266) describe if the new information exhibits little seman-
brand dilution as ‘lessening the uniqueness tic relatedness to existing nodes in the net-
or distinctiveness of a famous brand in the work (Morrin and Jacoby, 2000).
minds of consumers … and weakening a Consider an example from Morrin and
famous brand’s propensity to bring to mind Jacoby (2000), wherein a consumer is
relevant associations’. Studies on brand familiar with the Hyatt Hotel brand and
dilution define dilution as the weakening has formed a brand network for Hyatt in
of associations between the brand and any his/her long-term memory. The consumer
of its aspects (Morrin and Jacoby, 2000; is exposed to information about Hyatt Legal
Jacoby, 2001). This perspective assumes Services either through advertising, word-
that a brand’s value or equity is something of-mouth communication or direct experi-
that exists in the brand knowledge or mem- ence. The knowledge structures of
ory network of consumers (Pullig et al, consumers will be altered such that an addi-
2006). tional Hyatt node is created and attached
Dilution has two different forms, namely to the category node for legal services. Such
(1) tarnishment and (2) blurring (Simonson, an alteration of consumer knowledge struc-
1993). Tarnishment corresponds to obvious ture is expected to weaken the relative
damage to a senior brand because of an strength of the original brand network.
attached negative association resulting from Given that the Hyatt name no longer
the emergence of a junior brand. Blurring belongs to the first user of the mark, the
refers to a gradual lowering of the identity first user no longer owns the Hyatt name
and uniqueness of a senior brand because in his/her long-term memory. Thus, when
of the emergence of a junior brand. exposed to the Hyatt name, consumers may
Although both forms seem to describe dilu- instead activate the legal services network
tion as the negative effect of the introduc- rather than the hotel network for the Hyatt
tion of a junior brand on brand associations, mark. A response competition may also
this study defines dilution based on the sec- occur, thus impeding the retrieval of the
ond form, not only because blurring is less Hyatt hotel network.
obvious and less frequently studied (Pullig The theory of associative network sug-
et al, 2006), but also because it expectedly gests that people store brand information
gives more subtle insight to the market, in their long-term memory as a pattern of
such as understanding the gradually low- linkages between concept nodes, similar to
ered values associated with a brand. associations between the brand and its
The logic of a junior brand affecting a attributes (Anderson, 1983). Even when
senior brand is relevant to the way consum- two brands are unrelated to each other in
ers store and retrieve brand information the aspect of ownership or producer, they
from their knowledge structure (Morrin create links to each other by sharing simi-
and Jacoby, 2000; Pullig et al, 2006). The lar brand elements, such as brand names,
associative network theory holds that infor- logos and slogans (Pullig et al, 2006). Even
mation stored in long-term memory con- if consumers have separate cognitive net-
sists of networks containing nodes that are works for each of the senior and junior
connected by links. The associations include brands, some shared attributes connect the
relations such as category membership and two networks (Pullig et al, 2006).
possession of characteristics (Morrin and Given that both senior and junior brands
Jacoby, 2000). When additional associa- exist in a shared brand network, focusing
tions are added to a pre-existing network, on the senior brand activates associations
the retrieval speed slows down, particularly connected with the junior brand (Anderson

592 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

et al, 2000), and vice versa. All associative et al, 2000). Typical users are most likely
constructs compete against one another for to rely on heuristics such as color schemes,
activation, and associations of a junior lettering or product placement to aid their
brand, which are new in the brand knowl- purchase decision (Simonson, 1994). Thus,
edge structure found in the memory of the similarity of the brand name, design or
consumers, could thus affect the degree of any of the various attributes can be consid-
activation of senior brand associations ered to contribute to consumer confusion.
(Burke and Srull, 1988). In other words, in Keller (2003) suggests six criteria in
a shared brand network, adding a similar choosing brand elements: memorability,
new construct results in activation among meaningfulness, likeability, transferability,
closely related existing constructs, thus adaptability and protectability. When a sen-
increasing the degree of activation of senior ior brand chooses brand elements, a crite-
brand associations (Humphreys et al, 2000). rion to be considered carefully against
Adding a dissimilar construct, on the other copycat imitation is ‘protectability’, which
hand, decreases the degree of activation of is the extent to which a brand element is
senior brand associations. ensured both legally and competitively
from trademark infringement. Logos, sym-
bols, slogans and jingles are highly pro-
Brand similarity and brand dilution tected from infringement, whereas brand
Copycats imitate the characteristics of orig- names and packaging are protected only
inal brands, such as brand name, logos, within certain limits because they can be
symbols and packages, and free ride on the easily reproduced (Keller, 2003). In their
brand equity of the original product. The studies, Horen and Pieters (2012a) posit
characteristics of a brand are called brand that leader brands and copycat brands differ
elements, which identify and differentiate in brand name and package design. They
one brand from another. Brand elements reasoned that cases of trademark infringe-
include brand names, URLs, logos, char- ment often deal with these brand elements,
acters, spokespeople, slogans and jingles which are important features of the trade
(Keller, 2003). A good choice of brand ele- appearance of brands. Thus, brand similar-
ments promotes the formation of strong, ity in this study is operationalized as the
favorable and unique brand associations similarity in brand name and package design
(Keller, 2003). Hence, copycats imitate the between two brands.
brand elements of a leader brand (that is, a Literature on trademark infringement
senior brand with high awareness, familiar- has emphasized the potential harm that
ity or reputation) to take advantage of the copycats pose to senior brands (Morrin and
authorized positive associations and mar- Jacoby, 2000). This potential harm is based
keting efforts of the leader brand (Horen on the idea that the more the copycat brand
and Pieters, 2012a). resembles the original leader brand, the
Trademark is the combination of unique more likely that it will create brand confu-
brand elements that, when taken together, sion and the more likely it is for consumers
create a product impression that consumers to evaluate the copycat brand positively
will consistently associate with that brand (Loken et al, 1986; Warlop and Alba, 2004).
(Rutherford et al, 2000). If a new brand is Thus, research on the practice of imitating
designed and dressed as a senior brand in products has focused on demonstrating
terms of shape, color, design, labeling, potential brand confusion when copycat
packaging or texture, that will affect con- products closely resemble the original
sumers’ response in several ways (Rutherford (Simonson, 1994).

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 593
Choy and Kim

However, a recent research on copycat- oval logo of Bertolli olive oil or the lilac
ting (Horen and Pieters, 2012a) suggests a wrapper of Milka chocolate), copycats
possibility that copycats could be less dan- exhibit a type of literal similarity (Horen
gerous to leader brands than commonly and Pieters, 2012b). Theme imitation
believed. Horen and Pieters (2012a) found involves copying the semantic meaning or
that the appraisal of copycats with a higher theme of a leader brand, such as the ‘wild-
degree of similarity increases only when cat’ theme of the Puma sports brand or the
consumers evaluate them in the absence of ‘traditional, family-produced olive oil’
an explicit comparison with leader brands. theme of the Bertolli brand, but presenting
When direct comparison with a leader it in a visually different way (Horen and
brand is conducted, copycats closely similar Pieters, 2012b).
to the leader brand receive less positive Using a large variety of product catego-
reviews compared with copycats that are ries (for example, yogurt, bottled water,
moderately similar to the leader brand. athletic shoes, spreadable butter and milk
Although they demonstrated that copycats chocolate), Horen and Pieters (2012b)
can gain or lose from their resemblance of showed that participants perceive feature
the leader brand, they remain silent on how copycats as less acceptable and more unfair
copycat similarity affects the reputation of than theme copycats, as predicted. The
the leader brand. Pullig et al (2006) focused authors argued that displaying literal simi-
on the change in senior brands resulting larity through imitation of the unique fea-
from the emergence of junior brands. They tures of a leader brand is more likely to
demonstrated that the association with a activate a clear representation of this brand
senior brand and its distinctive aspects is (that is, ‘This looks exactly like A’) because
reinforced when category similarity with these features are directly linked to the
the junior brand is high and diluted when leader brand. Therefore, feature imitations
category similarity is low. Their results sug- are likely to be considered inappropriate
gest that dilution is least likely to occur in and unacceptable. In addition, feature imi-
copycat strategies when a copycat emerges tations are perceived to cause aversion in
in a similar category as the senior brand. consumers, which will cause them to have
A recent study argues that copycats most a negative view of the copycat (Horen and
often imitate the perceptual feature of a Pieters, 2012b).
leader brand, but they can also imitate Theme imitations, on the other hand,
underlying meanings or themes of the are more implicit and less evident than fea-
leader brand (Horen and Pieters, 2012b). ture imitations because underlying mean-
The former is called feature imitation and ings or themes are indirectly linked to the
the latter is called theme imitation. leader brand (Horen and Pieters, 2012b).
According to Horen and Pieters (2012b), Furthermore, because themes are not exclu-
feature imitation occurs through imitation sively associated with the imitated leader
of the letters of the leader brand’s name or brand but also with other brands, theme
through imitation of the distinctive percep- imitations are considered more acceptable
tual features of the leader brand’s package and less deceptive than the imitation of dis-
design. By replacing one or more letters of tinctive perceptual features (Horen and
the name or by rearranging them (for Pieters, 2012b). The studies imply that imi-
example, ‘Dogiva’ dog biscuits from Godiva tating the underlying meaning or theme
chocolate or ‘Wumart’ from Wal-Mart), or conveyed by the leader brand is a more
by subtly or blatantly copying product effective copycatting strategy than imitating
appearance (for example, the red and white its unique perceptual features.

594 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

Dilution of brand personality According to Aaker (1996a), both product-


Brand dilution refers to the weakening of related factors, such as product category,
associations between the brand and any of price and physical attributes, as well as non-
its aspects. The number and nature of a product-related factors, such as user imagery,
given brand’s associations are thought to be symbols, marketing communication and
an indicator of its equity (Aaker, 1991; word of mouth, form brand personality.
Keller, 1993). Thus, copycats unfairly take In this research, packaging and brand name
advantage of associations related to leader correspond to the product- and non-prod-
brands and consequently get a free ride on uct-related factors, respectively.
the authorized equity of leaders. Among According to associative network theory,
various brand-related associations, one the logic of brand dilution, adding a simi-
important symbolic brand association is lar new construct results in activation
brand personality. Brands provide symbolic among closely related existing constructs,
meanings or benefits to consumers because thus increasing the degree of activation of
consumers incorporate human personality senior brand associations, while adding a
traits in a brand. Aaker (1997, p. 347) refers dissimilar construct decreases the degree of
to brand personality as ‘the set of human activation of senior brand associations.
characteristics associated with a brand’. She Thus, we hypothesize that the emergence
exploited personality traits used in psychol- of a similar junior brand would promote
ogy and marketing literature, conducted the activation of the associations of a senior
qualitative research intended for American brand, thus reinforcing the senior brand
consumers, and developed five brand per- personality. The emergence of a dissimilar
sonality dimensions, namely, sincerity, junior brand, in contrast, interrupts the
excitement, ruggedness, competence and activation of associations of a senior brand,
sophistication. Numerous researchers con- resulting in the dilution of the senior brand
sider brand personality as an important fac- personality.
tor from the perspective of brand-related
association (for example, Aaker, 1991; Hypothesis 1a: A high-similarity junior
Keller, 1993; Kotler, 2002). Thus, distinctly brand reinforces senior brand person-
positive brand personality facilitates distinc- ality.
tive brand association.
A favorable brand personality is thought Hypothesis 1b: A low-similarity junior
to encourage consumers in active process- brand dilutes senior brand personality.
ing (Biel, 1992), create grounds for product
differentiation (Aaker, 1996b), and increase
consumer preference and usage (Sirgy, The role of brand familiarity in brand
1982). In other words, brand personality personality dilution
makes a given brand stand out in the crowd People who are familiar with a certain
(Freling and Forbes, 2005) and plays an object learn, understand and appraise new
important role in differentiation. Thus, information about the object better than
when a new brand enters a market, brand those who are unfamiliar with the object
personality becomes one of the important (Celsi and Olson, 1988). Brand familiarity
brand-related associations distinguishing reflects the degree of a consumer’s direct
the new brand from existing brands. and indirect experience with a brand (Alba
Brand personality dilution is operational- and Hutchinson, 1987; Kent and Allen,
ized in this study as the lessening of asso- 1994; Campbell and Keller, 2003). Murphy
ciations between a brand and its personality. and Wright (1984) argue that in perceiving

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 595
Choy and Kim

similarity between two brands, a person recalls the brand’s category better after
unfamiliar with one or both brands might being exposed to diluting stimuli.
perceive them as mere replications of the In sum, as a consumer directly or indi-
same product category. In contrast, a per- rectly interacts with a certain object more
son familiar with one or both brands might frequently, he or she becomes more famil-
differentiate one brand from the other bet- iar with the object. Consumer’s knowledge
ter. Thus, in the case of less familiar brands, structure of a familiar object is stable because
consumers may have little experience with associative nodes related to the object are
these brands and possess an unstable knowl- strongly connected or linked to one another
edge structure about such brands, which and are not easily affected by external fac-
can be easily affected by external factors tors. High familiarity with a brand suggests
(Crocker et al, 1984). In contrast, the that the strength of links among the brand-
knowledge structure of highly familiar related associations is high in the consum-
brands remains stable in consumer memory ers’ brand knowledge structure. Thus,
and does not change easily. highly familiar senior brands would not be
Brand familiarity captures the brand affected by such factors as the emergence
knowledge structures of consumers regard- of copycats or distinctive new brands. We
ing the brand associations that exist in their predict that in the case of high-familiarity
memory (Campbell and Keller, 2003). senior brand, brand personality dilution
Consumers have various types of associa- attributed to a low-similarity junior brand
tions for familiar brands through first-hand would be alleviated.
experience, experiences of family or friends,
or company marketing communications Hypothesis 2a: When the familiarity
such as advertising (Campbell and Keller, of consumers with a senior brand is
2003). On the contrary, consumers lack low, a high-similarity junior brand
many associations for unfamiliar brands reinforces senior brand personality,
because of the absence of these kinds of whereas a low-similarity junior brand
experiences. Based on the associative net- dilutes senior brand personality.
work theory, the stronger the linkage
between a brand and its associations, the Hypothesis 2b: When the familiarity of
more likely it is that these associative con- consumers with a senior brand is high,
structs are activated (Pullig et al, 2006). the effect of a low-similarity junior
The ‘co-occur’ method, a learning brand on the dilution of senior brand
method of consumers’ brand association personality diminishes.
network formation, suggests that as the fre-
quency of occurrence of brand-related
association increases through direct or indi- The effect of brand personality dilution on
rect experience, the linkage between the attitude and purchase intention
brand and its related associations becomes When two brands (or a senior brand and
stronger (Anderson, 1983). Greater famili- junior brand) share a common attribute or
arity with brand-related associations rein- a common brand image, consumers believe
forces the strength of the linkage, resulting that they can enjoy similar experiential
in an increased probability of activation of benefits from either of the two (Lau and
the associations. Morrin et al (2006) have Phau, 2007). Eventually, this perception
shown that if a consumer is familiar (versus influences the choice probability or pur-
unfamiliar) with the association between a chase intention of consumers. Personality
brand and its product category, he or she dilution of a low-familiarity senior brand

596 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

would negatively affect a consumer’s buy- brand. The consumer attitude toward the
ing intention because the actual brand senior brand will thus become more favo-
choice of a consumer often depends on his rable than that toward the junior brand,
or her perceived uniqueness of brand asso- increasing purchase intention for the senior
ciations (Keller, 2003). Pullig et al (2006) brand. Hence, the following mediation
argue that a valid measure of dilution cap- effect hypothesis is given:
tures changes in brand knowledge and
shows that dilution significantly reduces Hypothesis 3: Attitude toward a senior
both the probability of a senior brand being brand compared with a junior brand
included in consumer’s consideration set mediates the relationship between
and the consumers’ buying intention for senior brand personality and the
the senior brand. Thus, we expect that the change in purchase intention for the
dilution or reinforcement of the senior senior brand.
brand personality will affect the buying
intention of consumers.
The strength of an association deter- METHOD
mines consumer attitude and affects the
degree to which attitude affects later behav- Overview
ior (Fazio, 1986). Strong, favorable and The empirical study examines the idea that
unique brand associations in consumer the dilution and reinforcement of the sen-
memory generate high perceived quality, a ior brand personality depend on the brand
positive attitude and generally positive similarity between senior and junior brands,
affect (Esch et al, 2006). A weak association as well as the degree of familiarity of senior
does not activate preference for an encoun- brands. Furthermore, the empirical study
tered object (Fazio, 1986). The intention tests whether the dilution or reinforcement
to buy a specific brand is positively affected of the senior brand personality affects the
by the attitude of a consumer toward the buying intention of consumers for the sen-
same brand (Laroche et al, 1996), and thus ior brand.
the attitude of consumers toward a brand We predicted that the emergence of a
influences their purchase intention. We high-similarity junior brand will activate
predict that consumer attitude will mediate the associations for a senior brand, thus
the relationship between dilution or rein- reinforcing the senior brand personality.
forcement of a senior brand personality and In contrast, the emergence of a low-simi-
consumer purchase intention. larity junior brand interrupts the activation
In sum, in the case of brand dilution, a of associations for a senior brand, thus
decline in the activation of associations with resulting in the dilution of the senior brand
senior brand resulting from the emergence personality. However, highly familiar sen-
of a unique junior brand increases consum- ior brands will not be affected by the emer-
ers’ attitude toward the junior brand but gence of similar or distinctive junior brands.
lowers their attitude toward the senior The dilution of the senior brand personal-
brand. This weakened attitude toward the ity is attributed to the emergence of a
senior brand compared with the junior dissimilar or unique junior brand, which
brand reduced consumer intention to buy lowers consumers’ attitude toward the
the senior brand. In contrast, in the case of senior brand compared with the junior
brand reinforcement, the emergence of a brand. Consequently, the buying intention
similar junior brand does not reduce the of consumers for the senior brand will
activation of associations with a senior decrease.

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 597
Choy and Kim

High- and low-familiarity senior brands Design, sample and stimulus


were selected through the pretest. The A total of 167 undergraduate and graduate
main study tests the hypothesis by manipu- students at a major university in Korea par-
lating the degree of verbal (that is, brand ticipated in the study in exchange for com-
name) and visual (that is, package design) pensation. The students were randomly
similarity of the junior brand. Participants assigned to the condition of a 2(similarity:
were provided a questionnaire containing high versus low)×2(familiarity: high versus
the pictorial stimulus of the brands and low) between-subjects design. After removing
questions that concern our research inter- questionnaires with incomplete responses,
ests. Participants were first shown a picture data from 160 participants (97 males, Mage =
of the high- or low-familiarity senior brand 23.93) were used.
before answering the brand personality Given the purpose of this research, we
items. Thereafter, the participants were chose a product category in which brand
exposed to the high- or low-similarity jun- personality is considered as an important
ior brand and were asked to answer a list driver in decision-making. Based on previ-
of questions. Finally, participants were ous literature (Onkvisit and Shaw, 1987),
again asked to answer questions regarding we identified product categories that com-
the senior brand. The following sections municate certain images to users, such as
provide the details of the pretest and main automobiles, food and beverage, and
experiment. clothes. In addition, categories that are both
symbolic and utilitarian, such as automo-
Pretest biles, beverages and athletic shoes, were
Brand personality is formed through the also considered (Ratchford, 1987; Aaker,
direct or indirect experience of consumers 1997). Given the characteristics of the stu-
with the brand (Plummer, 2000). In the dent sample and the purpose of this research,
present research, real brands were used as beverage was selected as the appropriate
senior brands, whereas fictitious brands product category for the experiment.
were used as junior brands to rule out
potential confounding effects on participant Manipulation
attitude toward existing junior brands (Lau As experimental stimuli of senior brands, a
and Phau, 2007). High- and low-familiarity picture of an actual brand containing its
senior brands were selected through the brand name and product packaging was
pretest. used. To manipulate the familiarity level of
A list of beverage brands was presented senior brands, aside from the pretest for
to 20 participants (11 females, Mage = 23.7) selecting the appropriate brands, a short
drawn from the same subject pool as the description about the market position of the
main study. They evaluated familiarity with brand was also provided. The amount of
the six listed brands on five dimensions information was even for all conditions.
using a 7-point scale (1 = not at all, 7 = To manipulate the junior brand similar-
extremely): heard of, know, understand, ity conditions into high and low, we used
familiar with and have experience with this pictorial stimuli. A research assistant adept
brand. Consequently, Black Bean Thera-Tea in using the Photoshop program created
(a bottled tea) was selected as the high- images of high- and low-similarity junior
familiarity senior brand (Mhigh = 5.26), and brands. Pictorial stimuli of high-similarity
Vono (an instant cup soup) was selected as junior brands contains similar brand name
the low-familiarity senior brand (Mlow = 3.52; and package design with the senior brand,
t = 4.242, P = 0.000). whereas those of low-similarity junior

598 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

brands contains different and unique brand vious responses. Brand personality items
name and package design.1 were the same as the previous, except for
the order of items to minimize the rele-
Procedure vance of prior measurements. The set of
This research utilized a before–after exper- questions about attitude toward and inten-
imental design, enabling the researchers to tion to buy the senior brand were then
focus on the change in consumer percep- repeated. On the last page, participants
tion about the personality of the senior answered the items for manipulation check
brand attributed to the emergence of a jun- and demographics. Finally, they were
ior brand. Given that imitators generally debriefed and thanked.
enter the market at a lower price compared
with a market leader (Warlop and Alba, Measures
2004), the effect of the price factor needs All items were measured using a 7-point
to be controlled. Although beverages are scale (1 = not at all, 7 = extremely). Brand
relatively in the low-involvement product similarity between two brands is operation-
category, and price is not a decisive factor alized as the similarity of brand elements,
when consumers make their choices, the namely brand name and package design.
prices of the two brands were placed on The participants rated brand similarity on
the same level. three dimensions (Pan and Lehmann, 1993;
First, the participants were shown the Howard et al, 2000): brand name, package
pictorial stimulus of the high- or low-famil- design and overall similarity (Cronbach’s
iarity senior brand. Below the picture, a  = 0.92).
short write-up was provided stating that Given that brand personality dimensions
this brand is a pioneer brand in its product reflect a distinctive cultural background of
category. Participants were instructed to consumers (Aaker et al, 2001), four dimen-
respond to 14 items on the four brand per- sions, namely competence, passion, sincer-
sonality dimensions for the senior brand. ity and friendliness, were selected from
Next, they were instructed to answer items previous studies done in Korea (for exam-
on familiarity with and purchase intention ple, Kim and Ahn, 2000). Each dimension
for the senior brand. has four (reliable, skillful, safe, capable;
In the next page of the questionnaire, a Cronbach’s  = 0.80), four (lively, adven-
brief statement introduced a new brand, turous, appealing, cheerful; Cronbach’s
explaining that this brand has entered the  = 0.80), three (honest, sincere, faithful;
market of the same product category as the Cronbach’s  = 0.81) and three (friendly,
brand on the previous page. Then, the generous, warm; Cronbach’s  = 0.77) sub-
image of the fictitious junior brand, con- items, respectively. The senior brand per-
taining the brand name and packaging, and sonality index was created by averaging the
a short write-up about the junior brand values of [post-exposure value – pre-expo-
were presented. Participants were instructed sure value] for all brand personality dimen-
to respond to the same personality items sions. If participants rated senior brand
and attitude toward the junior brand. personality lower after being exposed to
In the subsequent page of the question- the junior brand, the post-exposure value
naire, senior brand personality was again is smaller than the pre-exposure value.
measured. This time, however, participants A negative ( − ) value of the index means
were asked to answer the same personality that senior brand personality is diluted,
items following their current feelings and whereas a positive ( + ) value means that
thoughts without trying to recall their pre- senior brand personality is reinforced.

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 599
Choy and Kim

Items used to measure the degree of similarity manipulation. As expected, the


brand familiarity encompass both first- and main effect of brand similarity was signifi-
second-hand experiences. Based on previ- cant (Mlow = 3.25, Mhigh = 5.91; F(1, 156) =
ous research (Srinivasan and Ratchford, 234.02, P < 0.001) such that those in the
1991; Mishra et al, 1993; Cowley and high-similarity condition perceive the given
Mitchell, 2003), brand familiarity was junior brand more similar to the senior
measured using the following five items: I brand than those in the low-similarity con-
have heard of the brand name a lot, I am dition. Neither the main effect of brand
knowledgeable about the brand, I have an familiarity nor the brand similarity by brand
understanding of the brand, I am familiar familiarity interaction was significant.
with the brand and I have experience with In addition, senior brand familiarity
the brand (Cronbach’s  = 0.93). manipulation was validated through an
When dilution occurs and participant ANOVA with the same factors and par-
attitude toward the junior brand is higher ticipant senior brand familiarity ratings as
than that toward the senior brand, the value the dependent variable. As expected, the
of [senior brand attitude – junior brand main effect of brand familiarity was sig-
attitude] becomes negative. In contrast, the nificant (Mlow = 2.80, Mhigh = 4.84; F(1, 156) =
value is positive when dilution does not 87.68, P < 0.001) such that those in the
occur. To measure attitude toward the sen- high-familiarity condition found the given
ior and junior brands, participants were senior brand more familiar than those in
asked to evaluate the following three the low-familiarity condition. Neither the
dimensions (Ruth, 2001): favorable, posi- main effect of brand similarity nor the
tive and attractive (Cronbach’s  = 0.93). brand familiarity by brand similarity inter-
To capture change in purchase intention, action was significant.
participants rated their buying intention for
the senior brand before and after being Hypotheses testing
exposed to the junior brand. The negative
value of [ post-exposure value – pre-expo- Test of brand personality dilution
sure value] implies a decrease in purchase To demonstrate the effect of brand element
intention for the senior brand because of similarity between senior and junior brands
the emergence of the junior brand. The on senior brand personality, as well as the
participants were instructed to answer the moderating role of senior brand familiarity,
following three items to measure their buy- we employed a 2(brand similarity)×2 (brand
ing intentions (Mackenzie et al, 1986; familiarity) ANOVA on the index of senior
Zauberman et al, 2009): probability of buy- brand personality. Brand personality dilu-
ing this brand when buying, probability of tion is significantly different depending on
using this brand when it becomes available the similarity conditions (F(1, 156) = 5.05,
and intention to purchase this brand P < 0.05). The mean value of the senior
(Cronbach’s  = 0.87). brand personality index is negative in the
low-similarity condition (Mlow = − 0.01),
RESULTS whereas the value is positive in the high-
similarity condition (Mhigh = 0.21). The
Manipulation check results indicate that when a junior brand is
An ANOVA with the brand similarity and similar to a senior brand, senior brand per-
brand familiarity as the factors and partici- sonality is reinforced. When a junior brand
pants’ brand similarity ratings as the depend- is different from a senior brand, the senior
ent variable was employed to validate brand brand personality is diluted, although the

600 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

absolute value is small. These findings sup- 0.3

Senior brand personality index


High-familiarity

port Hypotheses 1a and 1b. 0.2


Low-familiarity

The planned contrasts revealed signifi-


0.1
cant differences between the low-similarity
condition and high-similarity conditions 0
High-similarity Low-similarity
(Mlow = − 0.18, Mhigh = 0.17; F(1, 156) = -0.1
6.90, P < 0.01) when senior brand familiar-
-0.2
ity is low (see Table 1). However, when
senior brand familiarity is high, the com- Figure 1: The role of brand familiarity in brand personality
dilution.
parison between the low-similarity condi-
tion and high-similarity conditions was not
significantly different (Mlow = 0.17, Mhigh =
0.24; F(1, 156) = 0.28, P > 0.50). This pat- Additional analysis: Test of brand personality
tern confirmed that when senior brand dilution on each brand personality
familiarity is high, senior brand personality dimension
is reinforced in both high- and low-similar- We further analyze the abovementioned
ity conditions. In contrast, when senior effects on each of the four brand personal-
brand familiarity is low, senior brand per- ity dimensions. The effects of similar brands
sonality is reinforced in the high-similarity were examined on each brand personality
condition, whereas senior brand personality dimension to test Hypotheses 1a and lb.
is diluted in the low-similarity condition. With regard to competitiveness, the senior
These results indicate that when the famil- brand personality did not present significant
iarity of consumers with a senior brand is differences between low- and high-similar-
high, brand personality is reinforced by ity conditions (Mlow = 0.20, Mhigh = 0.22;
the emergence of a junior brand, regardless F(1, 156) = 0.02, P > 0.50). Junior brands,
of whether the junior brand is similar or regardless of the level of similarity, rein-
dissimilar to the senior brand. However, forced the competence personality of the
when the familiarity of consumers with senior brand. In terms of passion, the dif-
a senior brand is low, a similar junior ference between low- and high-similarity
brand reinforces senior brand personality, conditions was marginally significant,
whereas a dissimilar junior brand dilutes implying that the similar junior brand rein-
senior brand personality. These findings forced the passion personality of the senior
provide support for Hypotheses 2a and 2b brand while the distinctive junior brand
(see Figure 1). diluted the passion personality of the senior
brand (Mlow = − 0.30, Mhigh = 0.03; F(1, 156) =
3.63, P < 0.10). On sincerity, the senior
brand personality did not present a signifi-
Table 1: Mean (SD) of senior brand personality cant difference between low- and high-
similarity conditions (Mlow = 0.26, Mhigh =
Familiarity
0.46; F(1, 156) = 1.56, P > 0.10). In both
High Low the low- and high-similarity conditions, the
Similarity High 0.24 (0.59)a 0.17 (0.58)a
junior brand reinforced the sincerity per-
Low 0.17 (0.54)a − 0.18 (0.67)b sonality of the senior brand. On the friend-
liness dimension, the difference between
Note: ( + ) value denotes the reinforcement of brand low- and high-similarity conditions was
personality, and ( − ) value denotes the dilution of brand
personality. Differing superscripts denote significant significant, implying that the similar junior
differences at P < 0.01 within a column. brand reinforced the friendliness personality

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 601
Choy and Kim

of the senior brand whereas the unique

− 0.28 (0.99)c
0.07 (0.75)a
junior brand diluted the friendliness per-

Low
sonality of the senior brand (Mlow = − 0.17,

Friendliness
Mhigh = 0.17; F(1, 156) = 5.30, P < 0.05).
The same planned contrasts were con-

− 0.06 (0.88)a
0.28 (1.06)a
ducted to examine Hypotheses 2a and 2b

High
on each brand personality dimension. The
results are presented in Table 2. In the case
of the high-familiarity senior brand, low-

Within a column, differing superscripts (a,b) denote significant differences at P < 0.01; differing superscripts (a,c) denote marginal significance at P < 0.10.
and high-similarity conditions did not signi-

− 0.03 (0.92)c
0.37 (0.81)a
ficantly differ in all of the four personality

Low
dimensions. In the case of the low-familiar-
ity senior brand, the passion dimension
Sincerity

showed a significant difference between


0.55 (1.09)a low- and high-similarity conditions (Mlow =
0.56 (1.04)a − 0.53, Mhigh = 0.12; F(1, 156) = 7.51, P < 0.01)
High

Note: ( + ) value denotes the reinforcement of brand personality, and ( − ) value denotes the dilution of brand personality.
except in the competence dimension (Mlow =
0.11, Mhigh = 0.16; F(1, 156) = 0.09, P > 0.50).
Familiarity

The sincerity dimension (Mlow = − 0.03,


Mhigh = 0.37; F(1, 156) = 3.39, P < 0.10) and
− 0.53 (0.91)b
0.12 (0.96)a

the friendliness dimension (Mlow = − 0.28,


Low

Mhigh = 0.07; F(1, 156) = 2.85, P < 0.10)


showed marginally significant difference bet-
Passion

ween low- and high-similarity conditions.


− 0.07 (1.12)a
− 0.07 (1.22)a
High

Test of the effect of brand personality


dilution on attitude and purchase intention
Attitude toward the senior brand compared
0.16 (0.74)a
0.11 (0.87)a

with the junior brand was hypothesized to


Table 2: Mean (SD) of senior brand personality on each dimension

Low

mediate the relationship between the senior


brand personality and change in purchase
Competence

intention for the senior brand. A set of


regression equations were set up (see Table 3)
0.28 (0.67)a
0.29 (0.75)a

and a series of regression analyses were con-


High

ducted to examine the role of attitude


toward the senior brand compared with the
junior brand as a potential mediator follow-
ing Baron and Kenny (1986).
The results indicate that (a) senior brand
personality dilution results in lower attitude
High
Low

toward the senior brand compared with the


junior brand (1 = 0.50; t = 2.53, P < 0.05);
(b) lower attitude toward the senior brand
compared with the junior brand results
Similarity

in decreased purchase intention for the


senior brand (2 = 0.20; t = 4.55, P < 0.001);

602 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

Table 3: Regression equations for mediation analysis one (that is, package design) changes con-
Equations sumer perception of the personality of the
senior brand, which is a component of
(a) ATT=1 + 1SBP + e1 brand image. This change in perception
(b) PI=2 + 2ATT + e2
(c) PI= 3 + 3SBP + e3 affects consumer attitude and buying inten-
(d) PI=4 + 41SBP + 42ATT + e4 tion. The results show that when consumers
are unfamiliar with the senior brand, a jun-
SBP: senior brand personality.
PI: change in senior brand purchase intention.
ior brand with similar brand elements, such
ATT: attitude toward senior brand compared with junior as brand name and package design, rein-
brand. forces personality of the senior brand.
Note: x are constants and ex are errors. In contrast, a low-similarity junior brand
dilutes personality of the senior brand. When
consumers are highly familiar with the sen-
(c) senior brand personality dilution has a ior brand, the emergence of a junior brand
direct effect on change in purchase inten- reinforces senior brand personality, regard-
tion for the senior brand (3 = 0.32; t = 2.79, less of whether the junior brand is similar
P < 0.01); and (d) when attitude toward the or dissimilar to the senior brand. Finally, the
senior brand compared with the junior findings demonstrate that the dilution of
brand is included in the model as a media- senior brand personality attributed to the
tor, the effect of senior brand personality emergence of a dissimilar or unique junior
on the change in purchase intention was brand lowers consumers’ attitude toward the
reduced but remained significant (41 = 0.23; senior brand compared with the junior
t = 2.06, P < 0.05), whereas the effect of brand, consequently decreasing their current
attitude toward the senior brand compared purchase intention for the senior brand as
with the junior brand on the change in opposed to their previous purchase intention
purchase intention showed only a minimal before seeing the unique junior brand.
change and remained significant (42 = 0.18; We further examined the dilution effect
t = 4.10, P < 0.001). The direct effect of the on each of the four brand personality
senior brand personality on the change in dimensions. In the competence and sincer-
purchase intention was significant, and ity dimensions, the senior brand personality
it remained significant when the attitude was not significantly different between low-
was controlled for, but it was significantly and high-similarity conditions and was rein-
reduced in magnitude, indicating a signifi- forced in both conditions. The results
cant partial mediation (Sobel z = 2.22, suggest that even when a low-similarity jun-
P < 0.05). The results provide evidence that ior brand emerges, the competence and
attitude toward the senior brand compared sincerity personalities of the senior brand are
with the junior brand partially mediates the reinforced because of the idea that the sen-
effect of senior brand personality on the ior brand is a pioneer and the true original
change in purchase intention, supporting does not change. In contrast, a distinctive
Hypothesis 3. junior brand dilutes the passion and friend-
liness personalities of the senior brand
DISCUSSION because consumers may perceive the senior
brand as outdated and boring.
Conclusion
This research suggests that a junior brand Implications
with varying degrees of similarity of verbal In the context of brand imitation, previous
element (that is, brand name) and visual research has placed more weight on

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 603
Choy and Kim

benefits that copycats get and less on the consequently increases the intention of
effect on original brands. Marketing literature consumers to buy the new brand.
emphasizes the threats that copycats pose to The finding that the personality of a
market-leading brands (Morrin and Jacoby, highly familiar senior brand is not diluted
2000). Previous studies have demonstrated by the emergence of a junior brand, regard-
that copycats that are more identical to the less of brand element similarity, suggests
leader brand are evaluated more positively that marketing efforts to increase consumer
(Loken et al, 1986; Warlop and Alba, 2004). familiarity with a senior brand are critical.
Although Horen and Pieters (2012a) showed Diluting the personality of a leader brand
that copycats can gain or lose from their is difficult, even for a distinctive new brand,
resemblance to the leader brand, only the because that brand is a pioneer in its prod-
possibility that the evaluation of the leader uct category and is salient with high level
brand might unexpectedly gain from blatant of familiarity or reputation in the memory
imitation practice has been suggested. The of consumers. In this case, a differentiation
present research demonstrated that in the case strategy of a junior brand would only rein-
of low-familiarity senior brand, when con- force (rather than dilute) the personality of
sumers perceive high degrees of similarity the senior brand. Thus, through marketing
between a junior and a senior brand, their strategies, such as repeated advertisements
perception of the senior brand personality is or sales promotions to reach out to a wide
reinforced. In contrast, low similarity between range of consumers, the first comer brand
the senior and junior brands dilutes senior needs to exert an effort to increase con-
brand personality. sumer familiarity with the brand as opposed
Pullig et al (2006), in the context of brand to potential competitors or imitators.
dilution, examined consumer perception of The results of further analysis on each
senior brands but focused on the weakened brand personality dimension suggest that
associations between brand names and prod- when a distinctive junior brand emerges, a
uct category. While they employed only a senior brand can maintain an image of
verbal element (that is, brand name), the competent pioneer and true original while
present study included a visual element (that losing its passionate image and being per-
is, package design), as well. Furthermore, in ceived as outdated and boring. Thus, for a
the selection of appropriate brand elements senior brand, conveying an image of endur-
for this imitation context, we applied the ing passion and never boring through mar-
protectability criterion. keting communications is an effective way
Practical implications emerge from this to be a more attractive brand to consumers.
study. First, our findings contradict ‘what
is often brand management’s gut reaction: Limitations and future research
that highly similar junior brands are the As experimental stimuli of a beverage
most serious threats’ (Pullig et al, 2006, brand, ‘Black Bean Thera-Tea’ was used for
p. 65). The results indicate that when a new the high-familiarity senior brand while
brand in the same product category enters ‘Vono’ was used for the low-familiarity sen-
the market with a differentiation (that is, ior brand. These brands pertain to bottled
low similarity) strategy, the new brand can tea and instant cup soup, respectively.
dilute the personality of an existing brand. Although these two brands are known as
Newcomers entering with their own unique beverage products in the country where
brand name and package design can increase this study was conducted, such brands can
consumer attitude toward them compared be perceived as different categories in other
with the existing brand. This strategy countries. Thus, future study needs to use

604 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

categories that are more commonly package design, and an interestingly novel
perceived as the same categories across experience with a new brand might result
various country contexts in high- and low- in the dilution of the personality of the
familiarity conditions. leader brand. Future research to investigate
This research used one product category these factors could provide more useful
based on criteria suggested by previous insights for marketing managers.
studies. As mentioned in the method sec-
tion, beverage is one of product categories ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
where brand personality is an important This study was supported by the Institute
factor in decision-making. Although previ- of Management Research at Seoul National
ous studies on brand dilution showed no University.
difference across different product catego-
ries (for example, Morrin and Jacoby, 2000;
Pullig et al, 2006), the inability to reflect NOTE
1 A more detailed description of these materials is avail-
the diversity of product categories remains able on request to the first author.
a limitation. Furthermore, in selecting brand
personality dimensions, this research
employed the previously developed dimen-
REFERENCES
Aaker, D.A. (1991) Managing Brand Equity. New York,
sions, rather than developing brand-specific NY: Free Press.
personality dimensions by conducting a Aaker, D.A. (1996a) Building Strong Brands. New York,
pretest, such as a free-association task. NY: Free Press.
Aaker, D.A. (1996b) Measuring brand equity across
We constitute the before-after experi- products and markets. California Management Review
mental design. Respondents were exposed 38(3): 102–120.
to the brands and then we checked whether Aaker, J.L. (1997) Dimensions of brand personality.
Journal of Marketing Research 34(3): 347–356.
a change occurs in the measures. The time Aaker, J.L., Benet-Martinez, V. and Garolera, J. (2001)
interval between the measures is short. Consumption symbols as carriers of culture: A study
Although a previous study showed that the of Japanese and Spanish brand personality con-
results of an experiment that provided a structs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
81(3): 492–508.
5-day delay between the exposure and the Alba, J.W. and Hutchinson, J.W. (1987) Dimensions
measurements do not differ from those of consumer expertise. Journal of Consumer Research
without a 5-day delay (for example, Pullig 13(4): 411–454.
Anderson, J.R. (1983) A spreading activation theory of
et al, 2006), such a short time between memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal
measures in this research can be an exper- Behavior 22(3): 261–295.
imental limitation. Consumers are exposed Anderson, M.C., Green, C. and McCulloch, K.C.
(2000) Similarity and inhibition in long-term
to original and second comer brands in memory: Evidence for a two-factor theory. Journal
various ways, thus future research can of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
constitute experimental design to reflect Cognition 26(5): 1141–1159.
real-life settings better. Baron, R.M. and Kenny, D.A. (1986) The moderator-
mediator variable distinction in social psychological
The findings have shown that if the research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical con-
reputation of or familiarity with the leader siderations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
brand was formed and clearly established at 51(6): 1173–1182.
Biel, A. (1992) How brand image drives brand equity.
the time of the copycat’s entry into the Journal of Advertising Research 32(6): 6–12.
market, the dilution of the personality of Burke, R.R. and Srull, T.K. (1988) Competitive inter-
the leader brand does not occur. However, ference and consumer memory for advertising.
Journal of Consumer Research 15(1): 55–68.
even for a highly familiar leader brand, a Campbell, M.C. and Keller, K.L. (2003) Brand famil-
number of factors, such as distinctive iarity and advertising repetition effects. Journal of
brand elements other than brand name and Consumer Research 30(2): 292–304.

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 605
Choy and Kim

Celsi, R.L. and Olson, J.C. (1988) The role of involve- purchase intention: An empirical test in a multiple
ment in attention and comprehension processes. brand context. Journal of Business Research 37(2):
Journal of Consumer Research 15(2): 210–224. 115–120.
Cowley, E. and Mitchell, A.A. (2003) The moderating Lau, K.C. and Phau, I. (2007) Extending symbolic
effect of product knowledge on the learning brands using their personality: Examining anteced-
and organization of product information. Journal of ents and implications towards brand image fit
Consumer Research 30(3): 443–454. and brand dilution. Psychology and Marketing 24(5):
Crocker, J., Fiske, S.T. and Tayler, S.E. (1984) 421–444.
Schematic bases of belief change. In: J.R. Eiser (ed.) Loken, B., Ross, I. and Hinkle, R.L. (1986) Consumer
Attitudinal Judgment. New York: Springer-Verlag, ‘confusion’ of origin and brand similarity perceptions.
pp. 197–226. Journal of Public Policy and Marketing 5: 195–211.
Esch, F.R., Langner, T., Schmitt, B.H. and Geus, P. Mackenzie, S.B., Lutz, R. and Belch, G.E. (1986) The
(2006) Are brands forever? How brand knowledge role of attitude toward the ad as a mediator of adver-
and relationships affect current and future purchase. tising effectiveness: A test of competing explanations.
Journal of Product and Brand Management 15(2): Journal of Marketing Research 23(2): 130–143.
98–105. Mishra, S., Umesh, U.N. and Stem Jr, D.E. (1993)
Fazio, R.H. (1986) How do attitudes guide behavior?. In: Antecedents of the attraction effect: An informa-
R.M. Sorrentino and E.T. Higgins (eds.) The Handbook tion-processing approach. Journal of Marketing
of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior. Research 30(3): 331–349.
New York, NY: Guilford, pp. 204–243. Morrin, M. and Jacoby, J. (2000) Trademark dilution:
Freling, T.H. and Forbes, L.P. (2005) An empirical Empirical measures for an elusive concept. Journal
analysis of the brand personality effect. Journal of of Public Policy and Marketing 19(2): 265–276.
Product and Brand Management 14(7): 404–413. Morrin, M., Lee, J. and Allenby, G.M. (2006) Deter-
Horen, F. and Pieters, R. (2012a) When high-similarity minants of trademark dilution. Journal of Consumer
copycats lose and moderate-similarity copycats gain: Research 33(2): 248–257.
The impact of comparative evaluation. Journal of Murphy, G.L. and Wright, J.C. (1984) Changes in
Marketing Research 49(1): 83–91. conceptual structure with expertise: Differences
Horen, F. and Pieters, R. (2012b) Consumer evaluation between real-world experts and novices. Journal of
of copycat brands: The effect of imitation type. Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cog-
International Journal of Research in Marketing 29(3): nition 10(1): 144–155.
246–255. Onkvisit, S. and Shaw, J. (1987) Self-concept and image
Howard, D.J., Kerin, R.A. and Gengler, C. (2000) The congruence: Some research and managerial implica-
effects of brand name similarity on brand source con- tions. Journal of Consumer Marketing 4(1): 13–23.
fusion: Implications for trademark infringement. Pan, Y. and Lehmann, D.R. (1993) The influence of
Journal of Public Policy and Marketing 19(2): 250–264. new brand entry on subjective brand judgments.
Humphreys, M.S., Tehan, G., O’Shea, A. and Bolland, Journal of Consumer Research 20(2): 76–86.
S.W. (2000) Target similarity effects: Support for Pattishall, B.W. (1984) Dawning acceptance of the
the parallel distributed processing assumptions. dilution rationale for trademark-trade identity pro-
Memory and Cognition 28(5): 798–821. tection. The Trademark Reporter 74(4): 289–310.
Jacoby, J. (2001) The psychological foundations of Plummer, J.T. (2000) How personality makes a differ-
trademark law: Secondary meaning, genericism, ence? Journal of Advertising Research 40(6): 79–84.
fame, confusion, and dilution. The Trademark Reporter Pullig, C., Simmons, C.J. and Netemeyer, R.G. (2006)
91(5): 1013–1071. Brand dilution: When do new brands hurts existing
Keller, K.L. (1993) Conceptualizing, measuring, and brands? Journal of Marketing 70(2): 52–66.
managing customer-based brand equity. Journal of Ratchford, B. (1987) New insights about the FCB grid.
Marketing 57(1): 1–22. Journal of Advertising Research 27(4): 24–38.
Keller, K.L. (eds.) (2003) Strategic Brand Management: Ruth, J.A. (2001) Promoting a brand’s emotion ben-
Building, Measuring, and Managing Brand Equity. efits: The influence of emotion categorization proc-
Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education. esses on consumer evaluations. Journal of Consumer
Kent, R.J. and Allen, C.T. (1994) Competitive inter- Psychology 11(2): 99–113.
ference effects in consumer memory for advertising: Rutherford, D.G., Perkins, A.W. and Spangenberg,
The role of brand familiarity. Journal of Marketing E.R. (2000) Trade dress and consumer perception
58(3): 97–105. of product similarity. Journal of Hospitality and
Kim, C.K. and Ahn, Y.H. (2000) The role of brand Tourism Research 24(2): 163–179.
personality based on the FCB grid model. The Simonson, A. (1993) How and when do trademarks
Korean Journal of Advertising 11(4): 65–85. dilute: A behavioral framework to judge ‘likelihood’
Kotler, P. (eds.) (2002) Marketing Management: Analysis, of dilution. The Trademark Reporter 83(2): 149–174.
Planning, Implementation, and Control. Engelwood Simonson, I. (1994) Trademark infringement from the
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. buyer perspective: Conceptual analysis and meas-
Laroche, M., Kim, C. and Zhou, L. (1996) Brand urement implications. Journal of Public Policy and
familiarity and confidence as determinants of Marketing 13(2): 181–199.

606 © 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607
New brands diluting the personality of existing brands

Sirgy, J. (1982) Self-concept in consumer behavior: a Warlop, L. and Alba, J.W. (2004) Sincere flattery:
critical review. Journal of Consumer Research 9(3): Trade-dress imitation and consumer choice. Journal
287–300. of Consumer Psychology 14(1&2): 21–27.
Srinivasan, N. and Ratchford, B.I. (1991) An Zauberman, G., Ratner, R.K. and Kim, B.K. (2009)
empirical test of an external search for auto- Memories as assets: Strategic memory protection in
mobiles. Journal of Consumer Research 18(9): choice over time. Journal of Consumer Research 35(5):
233–242. 715–728.

© 2013 Macmillan Publishers Ltd. 1350-23IX Journal of Brand Management Vol. 20, 7, 590–607 607

You might also like