You are on page 1of 9

Changes in Posture Control Across the Life Span-

A Systems Approach

m this article, a systems approach to the development of posture control across the Marjorie Hines Woollacott
life span and its integration with voluntary tasks such as walking is described. Anne ShumwapCook
Research shows a clear cephalocaudal gradient in the development of postural
responses. Postural muscle synergies develop appropriate temporal olganization
through e.xperience in each new level of postural skill development. Sensoy inputs
contributing to posture control influence postural responses very early in develop-
ment, with responses being elicited by vision alone, or by somatosensoy and vesti-
bular cues in isolation. Studies of older adults indicate small, but signz3cant, in-
creases in onset latencies and disruptions in the temporal olganization of
postural muscle responses when subjects are given external threats to balance. In
addition, older adults, like young children, use antagonist muscles more open in
coactivation with agonist muscles. Older adults also have more dzficulty balanc-
ing when sensory inputs are reduced experimentally or pathologically. Ankle dor-
szj7exor muscle weakness is also a factor zn balance dysfunction in the older
adult. [W(~ollacott MH, Shumwajl-Cook A. Changes in posture control across the
life span--a gstems approach. Phjls Ther. 1990;70:799407.1

Key Words: Child development; Equilibrium; Pediatrics, development; Posture,


general; Systems theory.

An analysis of the behavioral changes What neural mechanisms underlie the metric tonic neck, symmetric tonic
occurring during the acquisition of predictably changing patterns of be- neck, and tonic labyrinthine reflexes
independent stance shows that infants havior seen in infants learning to being controlled by the next higher
begin to stand with help at about 8 to stand and walk independently? Two level, the brain stem. The righting
9 months of age, begin to walk with models of central nervous system reactions, a group of reactions re-
help at 11 months, pull themselves to (CNS) control-the reflex-hierarchical sponsible for maintaining alignment
a standing position at about 12 model and the systems model-have to gravity and keeping body parts in
months, stand alone at 14 months, been used to describe the neural ba- alignment after rotation, are con-
and walk alone at about 15 months of sis for developing posture and move- trolled at a slightly higher CNS level,
age.l.2 It is widely noted that in the ment control in children. In the the midbrain.314
first few months of walking, the infant reflex-hierarchical model, the CNS is
steps with a wide base and the arms hypothesized to be organized as a Equilibrium reactions, described as
raised. Walking characteristics gradu- strict vertical hierarchy. In this ascend- the body's response to tilting of the
ally change over the next few years, ing hierarchy, primitive reflexes such support surface, are hypothesized to
becoming adultlike by about 5 to 7 as stretch reflexes are controlled at be controlled by the highest level of
years of age. the spinal cord level, with the asym- the CNS, the cortex.5j6Higher-level
equilibrium reactions, as described by
Weiss,5 are purported to be elicited
by stimulation of the labyrinth; how-
ever, this explanation ignores the con-
tribution of visual and somatosensory
M Woollacr)tt, PhD, is Professor, Institute of Neuroscience and the Department of Physical
Education and Human Movement Studies, Gerlinger Hall, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97403
components to equilibrium control.
(USA). Address correspondence to Dr Woollacott.

A Shumway-Cook, PhD, PT, is Director, Balance Disorders Program, Emanuel Rehabilitation Center,
3001 N Gantenbein, Ponland, OR 97227.

Physical 'Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990


Researchers who have taken a reflex- ments. A potential explanation for cepts related to the study of balance
hierarchical approach have hypothe- lack of balance control would be the development from a systems perspec-
sized that voluntary movement con- presence of primitive reflexes that tive and present results from research
trol is achieved either through the constrain the emergence of more ma- on developmental changes in compo-
inhibition of the more primitive re- ture higher-level righting and equilib- nents of postural control underlying
flexes by cerebral cortical pathways7 rium reactions. Treatment of a child the emergence of independent ma-
or through reflexes that become the with developmental delays would fo- ture balance control.
substrata for voluntary actions.8 The cus on sensorimotor techniques that
model predicts that prior to attaining progress the child through succes- A fundamental hypothesis in our work
the next developmental milestone, sively higher levels of reflexes and is that the development of indepen-
equilibrium reactions must mature in reactions. dent stance and locomotion emerges
the previous milestone. Thus, before from an interaction among multiple
children can sit, they must first have A more recent model of motor con- neural and mechanical components
developed mature equilibrium reac- trol is the "systems," o r "distributed contributing to balance control. We
tions in the prone position; prior to control," model that evolved from the hypothesize that certain critical com-
standing, equilibrium reactions must work of Bernstein.l5 In systems the- ponents are rate-limiting in the devel-
be present in the sitting and quadru- ory, the body is modeled as a me- opment of balance and gait. Rate-
ped positions.4.6,' chanical system with mass that is sub- limiting components are those aspects
ject to gravity and inertial forces. of the system that limit the rate at
In summary, in the reflex-hierarchical Because these factors change as we which the independent behavior
model, motor development is viewed move, the same motor program gives emerges. Thus, the emergence of in-
as movingfrom reflexive to voluntary different movements depending on dependent stance must await the mat-
control as the child matures. In addi- the position we are in. Bernstein's uration of the slowest critical compo-
tion, the emergence of independent model asks questions about the orga- nent. Experimental studies examining
balance and locomotion is seen as nism as an active agent in a continu- the development of balance control in
dependent on the maturation of se- ously changing environment. He thus healthy children suggest the following
quentially higher levels of the CNS explored the physiology of activity, nervous system and musculoskeletal
hierarchy, with higher levels of behav- not reactions. components may contribute to the
ior, such as the equilibrium reactions, emergence of independent stance and
modifying immature behaviors such It was also Bernstein15who first locomotion: postural muscle response
as tonic reflexes, controlled by lower brought forth the idea of synergies as synergies for controlling balance; vi-
levels within the CNS.10 muscles that are constrained to act as sual, vestibular, and somatosensory
a unit. He viewed the synergy as a systems for detecting loss of balance;
Many therapeutic techniques used in way for the nervous system to solve adaptive systems for modifying sen-
rehabilitation of balance disorders are the control problem of coordinating sory and motor systems to changes in
based on assumptions regarding the many joints as part of a single move- task or environment; muscle strength;
importance of normal reflex matura- ment, and he gave examples of possi- joint range of motion; and body mor-
tion to development. For example, ble synergies such as those found in ph0logy.7.l~~' Current studies are ex-
neurodevelopmental treatment sug- locomotion, postural control, and amining the relative influence of each
gests positions and movements de- breathing. of these components on the emer-
signed to inhibit primitive reflexes, gence of independent stance and
such as bilateral midline activities de- According to the systems model, the lo~ornotion.~~
signed to inhibit the asymmetric tonic nervous system is seen as part of a
neck reflex.I1l2 Pediatric assessment flexible complex of systems and sub- The systems model suggests that mul-
techniques relying on a reflex- systems sharing in the control pro- tiple neural and biomechanical factors
hierarchical model atrempt to deter- cess. Thus, movement is always an interact to achieve the goal of balance.
mine a developmental level using emergent property that comes from The task of balance requires that the
both reflex tests (spinal reflexes, the complex interactions of these center of body mass be maintained
brain-stem reflexes, midbrain re- systems. over the base of support. A cone has
flexes) and tests of voluntary control. been used to represent the task of
For example, developmental proto- Development of upright balance because, for a subject
cols such as those of Chandler et al? Posture Control- standing with feet together on a nor-
Milani-Comparetti and Gidoni,' A Systems Perspective mal flat surface, the area of stability
Fiorentino,'3 and Brazeltonl+were resembles that of a cone-shaped struc-
created to provide a systematic ap- Our experimental approach to study- ture, with its base originating at the
proach to evaluating essential parame- ing the development of balance con- base of support.l"he cone repre-
ters of function in children, including trol in children relies on the systems sents a set of equivalent positions, that
muscle tone, primitive reflexes, auto- model. In the following sections, we is, all the points from which you can
matic reactions, and voluntary move- will introduce some of our basic con- return to a point of origin without

Physical Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990


taking a step o r otherwise moving natural frequency. A typical adult has ment significantly decreased between
your base of s ~ p p o r t . l ~The
, ~ l"stabili- a time constant for a single oscillation 2 to 3 and 4 to 5 months of age.
ty cone" is the domain of stable of 1.92 seconds, with a quarter cycle
movement for a particular postural (the time to go from upright to the Hartbourne et all7 also noted that
task, the edges of which are the outer limits of stability) of 480 milliseconds. children in stage 1 (age 2-3 months),
bounds of equilibrium for the task.16 Because an adult's muscle response who were unable to sit indepen-
The following sections present results times for the ankle strategy are in the dently, showed great variability in the
from research exploring developmen- order of 100 milliseconds, he or she order of muscle activation, both
tal changes in components of postural can easily respond to external threats within and across subjects. Children
control. to balance. According to McCollum in stage 2 (age 4-5 months) showed
and Leen,16 the time constant for bal- an emergence of a distinctive order of
Musculoskeletal and Body ance involving movement at the hip is muscle response organization, with
Morphology Changes shorter than that for the ankle, be- each child showing a preferred pat-
cause a two-link pendulum is being tern o r synergy. The most frequent
In healthy adults, limits of stability are modeled. In this case, a quarter cycle patterns seen were: lumbar
determined by mechanical constraints is 173 milliseconds. However, the on- paraspinal-hamstring muscles, lumbar
that include those from both the indi- set of muscle responses for a hip paraspinal-quadriceps femoris mus-
vidual (eg, height and foot length, strategy is between 73 and 110 milli- cles, and hamstring-quadriceps femo-
strength, and ROM) and the environ- sec0nds.~3Thus, the adult can still use ris muscles, with the lumbar
ment (eg, type and consistency of the this strategy with a reasonable safety paraspinal-hamstring and hamstring-
support surface).Zl How do rapidly margin. quadriceps femoris muscle synergies
changing musculoskeletal and body associated with the least trunk dis-
morphology characteristics affect the However, because of infants' shorter placement. Hartbourne et all7 con-
development of stability in children? height, McCollum and Leen16 predict cluded that the postural synergies that
We hypothesize that, because of dif- that I-year-old infants will have a aid in postural control while sitting
ferences in body morphology (eg, quarter-cycle time of 333 millisec- develop over time (with each child
height, center of mass, foot length), onds. Their ankle muscle responses developing a preferred synergy). Be-
the boundaries of individual stability are typically activated at 100 to 125 cause the study was longitudinal in
cones will vary and that this variability milliseconds, which is still within an nature, it also gives a window for ob-
will affect the selection of motor strat- effective range for regaining balance, serving the gradual emergence in
egies appropriate to maintaining the if it is perturbed. An infant's hip each child of a preferred muscle re-
body within the boundaries of the movement, however, would have a sponse synergy.
cone.16 For example, previous re- quarter-cycle time of 114 milliseconds
search"-2" has identified three pos- and thus would complete a quarter Recent studies in our own laboratory
tural movement strategies that are cycle too quickly to be corrected. As a on the development of neuromuscu-
typically used by healthy adults for result, they predict that a hip strategy lar response organization underlying
controlling balance: (1) the ankle would not be seen in young children. postural control in seated infants 4 to
strategy, in which balance adjustments Thus, the changes in height that occur 14 months of age have attempted to
are made at the ankle joint and the during an infant's or a toddler's devel- determine the time course of the de-
individual sways as an inverted pen- opment are examples of musculoskel- velopment of postural muscle re-
dulum; (2) the hip strategy, in which etal changes that may contribute to sponse synergies used in response to
adjustments are made predominantly the emergence of stability. external threats to balance.18In these
at the hip; and (3) the suspensory experiments, the infant was placed in
strategy, in which the subject flexes at Changes in Muscle an infant seat o r seated independently
the ankle, knee, and hip to lower the Response Synergies on a support surface (a hydraulically
center of gravity toward the base of activated platform) that could be
support. Hartbourne et all7 performed a longi- moved forward o r backward. Surface
tudinal study on the development of EMGs were used to monitor the re-
The efficic:ncy of a particular strategy, muscle activation sequences associ- sponses of the neck extensors and
however, depends on a number of ated with independent sitting in chil- flexors, the trunk extensors, and the
characteristics. For example, McCol- dren aged 2 to 5 months. Children abdominal muscles.
lum and ken16 have indicated that were first supported around the trunk
the movement of an inverted pendu- by the experimenter, then released to We found that infants aged 5 to 6
lum is characterized by a time con- sit on their own, during which time months, who could not yet sit inde-
stant that indicates the rate of fall they slumped forward. Electromyo- pendently, showed responses only in
from an upright position. The time graphic (EMG) data were collected the muscles of the neck. When the
constant is the inverse of the natural from muscles of the back and hip. platform was moved backward and
frequency of a pendulum, and the The authors noted that trunk displace- the child swayed forward, the neck
shorter the pendulum, the faster its extensor muscles were activated to

Physical Therapy /Volume 70, Number


to stand, from no experience in inde- aged 1 to 3 years were slower and
pendent stance at 8 months, through more variable than those of adults
minimal experience at 10 months, to and showed more antagonist muscle
6 weeks' experience in stance and coactivation. As in the model of
walking at 14 months. The 8-month- McCollum and Leen,l6 slower EMG
old infant (lightly supported at the responses and faster rates of sway
waist, to maintain stability) showed no acceleration were seen in young chil-
muscle responses to platform move- dren as a result of their shorter stat-
ments. These results could lead to the ure. These outcomes caused larger
assumption that lack of nervous sys- and more oscillatory sway amplitudes
tem maturity or experience caused than Forssberg and Nashnerz4 ob-
this lack of response; however, it is served in older children and adults.
also possible that the support given
by the mother reduced the effect of Shumway-Cook and Woollacott25
the platform movement or the need showed that responses of 15- to 31-
for a postural response. month-old children were consistently
large in amplitude and longer in du-
For the 10-month-old infant, who was ration when compared with older
standing but not walking indepen- children and adults (Fig. 2). An un-
dently, directionally specific responses predicted change in response charac-
were observed in the distal muscle of teristics in the 4- to 6-year-old age
the leg (gastrocnemius) during 40% group was also noted. A regression
of the platform movements causing was apparent in the postural response
anterior sway. Hamstring muscle re- organization in the 4- to 6-year-old
sponses were observed in only one children in that their synergies were
Figure 1. Drawing of a child stand- trial. more variable and longer in latency
ing on the platform The platform could than in the 15-month- to 3-year-old
be moved in an anterior or posterior di- For the 14-month-old infant, who was children, the 7- to 10-year-oldchil-
rection or rotated upward or downward walking independently, directionally dren, or the adults (Fig. 2). The 4- to
about the axis of the ankle joints. Electro-
myographic profiles were recorded from appropriate leg muscle responses 6-year-old group was more variable in
the gastrocnemius, tibialis anterior, ham- with adultlike response organization muscle response characteristics than
string, and quadricepsfemoris muscles. were observed consistently. When the the 15-month- to 3-year-old children;
(Reprinted with permission fiom Woolla- platform was moved backward, the however, behavioral data indicated
cott MH, Sbumway-Cook A, eds. Detlelop- infant showed forward sway, and the that they swayed less in response to
ment of Posture and Gait Across the
Lifepan. Columbia, SC: IJniuersity of gastrocnemius (distal) and hamstring platform movements.25 By the age of
South Carolina Press; 19133:8.?.) (proximal) muscles responded in an 7 to 10 years, this variability was
ascending sequence in which the ten- greatly reduced, and this age group
compensate for the sway. Postural ter of mass returned to its normal exhibited postural responses that
response synergies of independently range. Gastrocnemius muscle re- were essentially like those seen in the
seated 8- to 14-month-old children sponses in the 14-month-old infant adult synergy (Figs. 2C, 2D). Thus,
included muscles of both the neck were at latencies close to those seen children go through a transition pe-
and trunk and were directionally spe- in adults (109+24 milliseconds). riod at 4 to 6 years of age in which
cific to compensate for the platform- Thus, this gradual emergence of pos- their responses become slower and
induced sway. Thus, when the plat- tural response organization in chil- more variable, followed by maturation
form moved backward, causing dren learning to stand is similar to of the responses at about 7 to 10
forward sway, the trunk extensor and that seen by Hartbourne et all7 and years of age.
neck extensor muscles were activated, Woollacott et allHin children learning
and, when the platform moved for- to sit. In each study, an increase in Developmental Changes
ward, causing backward sway, the neuromuscular response organization in Sensory Inputs for
trunk flexor and neck flexor muscles was observed with age and experi- Posture Control
were activated. ence with the new skill.
In addition to these motor strategies
A cross-sectional study18 using the Although adultlike response organiza- for posture control, a number of sen-
same platform system to study pos- tion in the infant has been observed sory strategies are available to aid in
ture control in the developmental within 6 weeks of experience walking, balancing. Thus, a child or adult may
transition period leading to indepen- there are still many response charac- create different rules for combining
dent stance (Fig. 1) has examined teristics that are immature. For exam- the use of the available sensory inputs
infants in a range of stages in learning ple, Forssberg and Nashner24 ob- depending on the environmental cir-
served that responses of children cumstances. Normally, three classes of

Physical Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990


sensory inputs are available for bal-
ance control: (1) somatosensory in-

:&
puts, (2) visual inputs, and (3) vestibu-
B lar inputs. It has previously been

~QF& \H
shown that healthy adults rely primar-
ily on somatosensory inputs under
normal sensory conditions in which
all sensory inputs are available.26
However, they can be made to rely
on visual inputs by giving them a
novel stance condition o r by making
T
h\&
Q
support-surface inputs unreliable (eg,
by standing on a narrow beam).27

2~@ ' -1 Lee and Aronsonl9 have shown that


children learning to stand initially are
more influenced by visual cues than

QB
are adults. Owen and Lee28 hypothe-
size that this initial high susceptibility
T'
to incongruent visual information is
due to the infant having poorer infor-
mation from the ankles and feet than
27 months the adult, because the infant has not
yet had the opportunity to calibrate o r
5 years fine-tune this information for use in
balance control. With practice in inde-
D pendent stance and walking, this cali-

1
Q
T\ L
% bration takes place and the infant re-
lies less on visual cues.

Another experimental paradigm29 that


has been used to test the ability of
children to use different sensory in-
puts and to adapt to altered sensory
conditions requires the child to stand

:Qlzzzzzl
fx- & T

24\r*--_J
H
quietly for 5 seconds under condi-
tions in which the redundancy of sen-
sory inputs relevant for balance con-
trol is gradually reduced until only
vestibular inputs remain. The condi-
tions include (1) somatosensory ankle
joint, visual, and vestibular inputs nor-

L
Tk
mal; (2) somatosensory ankle joint
and vestibular inputs normal, eyes
Q
\ closed; (3) ankle joint inputs mini-
Q mized by rotating the platform in di-
3
rect relationship to body sway, but
7 years \ ADULT i visual and vestibular inputs normal;
and (4) ankle joint inputs minimized
(as above), eyes closed, vestibular
system normal.
Figure 2. Electromyographic activation pattemsfrom three successizle trials of
platform translations causing posterior sway. The adult (D) and the 7-year-old child (C)
showed directionally specijc, short-duration responses, with the stretched tibialis anterior In studies by Forssburg and Nashner25
(7J and qtradricepsfemons (Q) muscles being activated The 27-month-old child (A) and Shumway-Cook and Woollacott,25
showed longer-duration responses, with additional activation of the antagonist gastroc- the performance of children under
nemius (G) and hamstring (H) muscles. The 5-year-old child (B) showedgreat trial-to- these conditions was measured by
trial z~artability.(Reprinted with permission from the Helen Dwight Reid Educational determining body sway as a percent-
Foundation.-?5Published 611 Heldref Publications. 4000 Albemarle St h W )Washington,
DC 20016 CopyrightQ1985,) age of theoretical maximum sway,
with 100% indicating loss of balance.

Physical 'Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990


sway-related inputs, the youngest age
group showed long delays between
the beginning of forward sway and
the activation of the appropriate gas-
trocnemius muscle. They noted that
the children then began to sway back-
ward, but this time activated the ap-
propriate tibialis anterior muscle re-
sponse only when beyond the limits
of stability. They noted that postural
responses were activated much more L.
quickly in 7- to 10-year-old children
and that oscillations remained within
normal limits.

These studies suggest that children


under 7 years of age are unable to
balance efficiently when both soma-
tosensory and visual cues are re-
moved, leaving only vestibular cues to
control stability. Shumway-Cook and
Wo~llacott*~ found that 4- to 6-year-
old children showed progressively
decreasing stability as they lost redun-
dant sensory inputs for postural con-
trol. This age group also was less effi-
cient than older children at shifting
from the use of ankle joint soma-
tosensory cues to visual cues when
ankle joint inputs were made incon-
gruent with body sway. This finding
may indicate the inability of 4- to
6-year-old children to resolve in-
tersensory conflict dunng postural
Figure 3. Histograms shouling stabili~index Gercentage of theoretical maximum control.
swaJ')for 4- to 6year-olds, 7- to 10-uvear-olds,and adults under four dgerent sensoly
conditions: (from left to right) eyes open, normal support sulface; eyes closed, normal Integration of Posture Control
support surface; eyes open, support surface rotating to eliminate sway-related ankle
joint inputs; eyes closed, support surface rotating. Into the Gait Cycle

This procedure allows the compari- 6-year-old children were greatly desta- Berger et aBOstudied children who
son of balance abilities in children of bilized and one lost balance (Fig. 3). walked on a treadmill and examined
d ~ e r e n heights.
t Shumway-Cook and The last sensory condition, with ankle their ability to integrate postural re-
W o ~ l l a c o t indicated
t~~ that even under joint inputs unrelated to sway and sponses into the step cycle. Responses
normal stance conditions, 4- to 6-year- with eyes closed, leaving primarily were evoked by momentarily acceler-
old children swayed significantly vestibular cues to aid in balance, was ating or decelerating the treadmill
more (Fig. 3, far left) than older chil- the most difficult. Four of the five speed during the step cycle.
dren o r adults (the youngest children children in this age group needed
could not tolerate the altered condi- assistance to maintain stability, Results indicated that monosynaptic
tions without crying). With eyes whereas none of the older children reflexes were present in the youngest
closed, the stability of the 4- to 6-year- o r adults lost balance (Fig. 3, far children (1 year old), diminished in
old children decreased further, yet all right). amplitude in 2.5-year-old children,
of them were able to remain within and absent in 4-year-old children and
their limits of stability (Fig. 3). How- An interesting observation by Forss- adults. Postural responses also be-
ever, in the condition in which the bcrg and Nashnerzqs that when the came shorter in duration and showed
support surface was rotated with body youngest children balanced under less antagonist muscle coactivation as
sway, thus keeping thc ankle joint at conditions in which the support sur- the children developed. The results of
90 degrees and eliminating sway- face and the visual environment were this study were similar to those re-
related ankle joint inputs, the 4- to rotated with body sway to remove ported previously (ie, both a shorten-
ing in the duration of postural re-

58 / 804 Physical Therapy / Volume 70, Number 12 / December 1990


A C
L TIB-
L QUAD - L QUAD LQUAD

L TORQUE

SWAY SWAY

YOUNG ADULT 200 ms AGING 1 200 ms AGING 2 200 ms


NORMAL TIMING TEMPORAL DELAYS TEMPORAL REVERSAL

Figure 4. Comparison of electromyographic activation pattern of young and older adults from single trials of platform pertur-
bations cawing posterior sumy: (AJ response of a young adult; (B) delayed onset response of an older adulr; (C) reversal in the re-
sponse pattern for a second older adult. (L TIB = left tibialis anterior mwcle; L Q U L = left quadriceps femoris muscle.) (Reprinted
with permission..^*)

sponses during development and a the automatic postural responses of adults coactivated antagonist mus-
reduction in the coactivation of antag- the older adult group showed the cles with the agonist muscle signifi-
onist muscles along with the agonist following changes in both timing and cantly more than did young adults
muscles). The monosynaptic reflexes amplitude characteristics when com- when responding to platform
were observed in addition to the pared with the young adults: translations.
longer-latency automatic postural re-
sponses. I t is of interest that the chil- 1. Significant increases in the abso- Sensory Inputs Contributing to
dren showed a gradual reduction in lute latency of distal (ie, tibialis Posture Control
amplitude and disappearance between anterior) muscle responses in re-
the ages of 1 and 4 years, as the pos- sponse to platform translations Woollacott et al3l also tested the abil-
tural responses began to show more causing posterior sway (onset la- ity of older adults to retain stability
mature characteristics. tencies: young adults, 102+6 milli- under conditions of reduced o r con-
seconds; older adults, 109+9 milli- flicting infonnation from the visual,
Aging and Balance Control seconds). Figure 4 shows the EMG vestibular, and somatosensory sys-
responses of young and older tems. The protocol required that the
Studies in our own laboratory have adults to a platform perturbation, older and younger adult groups bal-
used the systems approach to expand giving examples of the response ance for a 10-second period under six
the study of balance control to the delays seen in the older adults. different sensory conditions: (1) nor-
entire life span in order to determine mal vision, normal base of support;
the specific changes in the different 2. Intermittent reversals in the nor- (2) eyes closed, normal base of sup-
nervous system and musculoskeletal mal distal-to-proximal sequence of port; (3) visual environment rotated to
components contributing to balance leg muscle contractions so that the follow body sway, normal base of
control. The following sections will proximal quadriceps femoris m u s support; (4) normal vision, base of
summarize changes in the different cle was activated before the distal support rotated to follow body sway;
subsystems in the elderly. tibialis anterior muscle. Five of the (5) eyes closed, base of support ro-
12 older subjects showed these tated to follow body sway; and (6) vi-
Muscle Response Synergies intermittent reversals (Fig. 4C). sion and base of support rotated to
follow body sway. The last two condi-
Woollacott et a13l and Manchester et 3. A larger incidence of short-latency tions reduced both relevant visual and
a132 have investigated whether there spinal monosynaptic reflexes, when somatosensory cues for posture con-
are age-related changes in the ability subjected to platform rotations. trol, so that primarily vestibular cues
to appropriately activate and organize Seven of the 12 older adults remained. They found that the sway
postural ~nusclesynergies when ex- showed an activation of monosyn- measurements of the older adults
posed to threats to balance. Woolla- aptic reflexes, whereas none of the were not significantly greater than
cott et a121 compared the muscle re- younger subjects showed this acti- those of the young adults for the first
sponse cl~aracteristicsof 12 older vation. However, the incidence of four sensory conditions. However, for
adults (6:l-78 years of age) with those these reflexes was small (18% of the last two conditions, the older
of 14 younger adults (19-38 years of the trials), even in those subjects in adults had significantly more sway
age), using the platform translations which they were elicited. Manches- than the young adults, and many of
described previously. They noted that ter et als2 also found that older the older adults lost stability, requir-

Physical 'Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990 805 / 59


ing assistance to regain their balance. through experience in each new level the different musculoskeletal and ner-
Two of the older adults lost balance of postural skill development. Muscle vous subsystems can be explained by
under condition 5, and 6 of the 12 strength changes may also contribute developmental changes in functional
older adults lost balance under condi- to the development of postural con- status of each system independently.
tion 6. trol, but few data are available on this There is no need to invoke the exis-
aspect of postural development. We tence of a strict vertical hierarchy. For
According to the systems model, these believe sensory inputs contributing to example, the similarities in use of
results indicate that postural control is posture control may be able to influ- antagonist muscles along with ago-
an emergent property that involves ence postural responses very early in nists in posture control in the two age
the interactions of a number of sen- development, with postural responses groups (children versus older adults)
sory systems. The results show that as being evident if influenced by vision simply imply that each may use the
long as two sensory inputs are avail- alone, o r by somatosensory and vesti- agonist-antagonist coactivation to
able, both young and older adults can bular cues in isolation. stiffen the ankle joint and thus limit
easily shift from the use of one sen- the degrees of freedom needed for
sory input to another. However, when Studies on the older adult indicate postural control. This is a typical strat-
only one sensory input-the vestibu- small, but significant, increases in the egy found in any motor skill when
lar system-remains, the sway of the onset latencies and disruptions in the function is not optimal; it is not an
older adult is sufficiently impaired to temporal organization of postural indication of a "lower level" of the
cause loss of balance in many in- muscle responses when subjects are vertical hierarchy reemerging in
stances. given external threats to balance. In dominance.
addition, older adults, like young chil-
Musculoskeletal Changes dren, use antagonist muscles more The systems model can be used to
often in coactivation with agonist evaluate changes in the different sys-
An additional body system that con- muscles when balan~ing.3~ tems contributing to balance control
tributes to balance control is the mus- across the life span by asking ques-
culoskeletal system, and one charac- Older adults also have more difficulty tions such as: When the function of
teristic of the musculoskeletal balancing when sensory inputs con- one system contributing to balance
system-muscle strengthaecreases tributing to balance control are re- control is unavailable, what other sys-
significantly with age.33-" Whipple et duced, so that they have less redun- tems can compensate? Are there spe-
a135 found that elderly nursing home dancy of sensory information. Thus, cific environmental conditions that
residents with a history of falls had when both somatosensory and visual threaten balance control when spe-
severe impairments in overall ankle inputs are made incongruent with cific systems are impaired, and can
muscle strength when compared with postural sway, the older adult shows these conditions be avoided? and Can
age-marched controls. They noted that significantly increased sway compared balance strategies be modified to im-
ankle dorsiflexion strength was most with the young adult, and many older prove balance function when a spe-
severely impaired in these nursing adults lose balance completely. This cific system is no longer functioning
home residents with a history of falls. characteristic is also similar to that at optimal levels? Thus, this model
These data are similar to those re- seen in young children. Muscle (ie, has great flexibility and great potential
ported by Woollacott et al,." which ankle dorsiflexor) weakness may also in contributing not only to our under-
showed a significant slowing in onset be a factor in balance dysfunction in standing of balance changes across
latency for the tibialis anterior mus- the older adult. the life span, but to therapeutic inter-
cles in response to external threats to ventions in the child or the older
balance. Given the many similarities in func- adult with balance dysfunction. How-
tional capabilities of the different sys- ever, our understanding of the clinical
Summary and Conclusions tems contributing to balance control implications of many of the experi-
in the child and the older adult when mental findings has only recently
These studies on the development of compared with the young adult, d o been e x p l ~ r e d . j ~As
. ' ~a result, effec-
posture control across the life span these results support the strict vertical tive approaches to assessment and
and its integration with voluntary tasks hierarchy hypothesis [hat as children treatment of some types of postural
such as walking show a number of mature, higher nervous system ten- problems identified through systems
interesting principles of developmen- ters take over function from more research are still limited.
tal progression. First, infants show a primitive reflex systems, and that as
clear cephalocaudal gradient in the adults age and higher centers deterio-
development of postural responses, rate, lower-level systems begin to References
with control first appearing in the show functions that reemerge? A- 1 Shirley M. The First Two Years: A Study of
muscles of the neck, then the trunk, though there are limited data to show Twenfy-Five Babies. Minneapolis, Minn: Univer-
and finally the legs. Data show that that there is some reemergence of sity of Minnesota Prcss; 1931:l.
postural muscle synergies develop spinal reflexes in the older adult,36 all 2 McGraw MB. Ncuromuscular development
of the human infant as exemplified in the
appropriate temporal organization other similarities in function between

Physical Therapy / Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990


achievement of erect locomotion. J Pediatr. 16 McCollum G, Leen TK. Form and explora- 28 Owen BM, Lee DN. Establishing a frame.of
1940,17,747-771 tion of mechanical stability limits in erect reference for action. In: Wade MG, Whiting
3 Chandler LS, Andrews MS, Swanson MW. stance.Journal oJMotor Behat~ior. HTA, eds. Motor Detslopmenr in Children.
Movement Arsessmenr oJ fnjants: A Manual. 1989;21:225-244. Aspects o j Coordinatio7z and Control. Dor-
Rolling Bay, Wash: Rolling Bay Press; 1980. 17 Hartbourne R, Giuliani CA, NacNeela J. Ki- drecht, the Netherlands: Martinus NijhoffDr W
4 EBgen SK. Integration of the plantar grasp nematic and electromyographic analysis of the Junk Publishers; 1986.
reflex as an indicator of amhulation potential development of sitting posture in infants. Dev 29 Nashner LM, McCollum G. The organiza-
in developmentally disabled infants. Phj~sTher. Med Child Neurol. 1987;29:31-32. tion of human postural movements: a formal
1982;62:433-435. 18 Woollacott MH, Debu B, Mowatt M. Neuro- basis and experimental synthesis. Rehav Brain
5 Weiss S. Studies in equilibrium reaction. muscular control of posture in the infant and Sci. 1985;8:135-172.
/ Nerv Ment Dis. 1938;88:153-162. child: is vision dominant?Journal oJiMotor 30 Berger W, Quintern J, Dietz V. Develop-
6 Milani-Comparetti A, Gidoni EA. Routine Behavior. 1987;19:167-186. ment of bilateral coordination of stance and
developmental examination in normal and 19 Lee DN, Aronson E. Visual proprioceptive gait in children. In: Amblard B, Berthoz A,
retarded children. Dev Med Child Neurol. control of standing in human infants. Percept Clarac F, eds. Posture and Gait Developmenr,
1967;9:631-638. Ps)'chophys. 1974;15:529-532. Adaptation, and Modulation. Amsterdam, the
20 Woollacott MH, Sveistrup H. Changes in Netherlands: Elsevier; 1988:67-74.
7 Wyke B. The neurological basis of move-
ment: a developmental review. In: Holt KS, ed. sequencing and timing of muscle response 31 Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook A, Nashner
Movement and Child Development. Philadel- coordination associated with developmental LM. Aging and posture control: changes in sen-
phia, Pa: JB Lippincott Co; 1975:19-33. transitions in balance abilities. Human Mozle- sory organization and muscular coordination.
ment Science. In press. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 1986;23:97-114.
8 Twitchell TE. The automatic grasping re-
sponses of infants. Neuroprychologia. 21 Nashner LM, McCollum G. The organiza- 32 Manchester D, Woollacott MH, Zederbauer-
1965;3:247-2 59. tion of human postural movements: a formal Hylton N, Marin 0 . Visual, vestibular and so-
basis and experimental synthesis. Behav Brain matosensory contributions to balance control
9 Haley S. Sequential analyses of postural re-
Sci. 1985;9:135-172. in the older adult. J Gerontol. 1989;44:11%
actions in nonhandicapped infants. Phys Ther.
22 Nashner L, Woollacott MH, Tuma G. Orga- 127.
1986;66:531--536.
nization of rapid responses to postural and 33 Aniansson A, Hedberg M. Henning G, et al.
10 Gilfoyle EM, Grady AP, Moore KC. Children
locomotor-like perturbations of standing man. Muscle morphology, enzymatic activity, and
Adapt. Thorofare, NJ: Slack Inc; 1986.
Exp Brain Res. 1979;36:463-476. muscle strength in elderly men: a follow-up
11 Bobath B.Bobath K. The neuro- study. Muscle Nerve. 1986;9:585-591
developmer~taltreatment. In: Scrutton D, ed. 23 Horak FB, Nashner LM. Central program-
ming of postural movements: adaptation to 34 Larsson L, Grirnby G, Karlsson J. Muscle
Manageme~zroJ the Motor Disorders oJ Chil-
altered support-surface configurations./ Neu- strength and speed of movement in relation to
dren with Cerebral Palsy. Inndon, England:
rophysiol 1986;55:1369-1381, age and muscle morphology. J Appl Phjsiol.
Spastics International Medical Publications;
24 Forssberg H, Nashner LM. Ontogenetic de- 1979;46:451456.
1984.
velopment of postural control in man: adapta- 35 Whipple RH. Wolfson LI, Amerman PM.
12 Wat L. Mobility module counteracting the
tion to altered support and visual conditions The relationship of knee and ankle weakness
tonic labyrinthine reflex in a cerebral palsied
during stance. J Neurosci. 1982;2:545-552. to falls in nursing home residents: an isoki-
child: suggestion from the field. Phys Ther,
25 Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott MH. The netic study. J A m Genatr Soc. 1987;35:13-20.
1978;58:880-881.
growth of stability: postural control from a 36 Paulson G, Gottlieb G . Developmental re-
13 Fiorentino MR. ReJex Testing Methods Jor
developmental perspective. Journal ojMotor flexes: the reappearance of foetal and neonatal
Evaluating CNS Development. Springfield, Ill:
Behavior 1985;17:131-147. reflexes in aged patients. Brain. 1968;91:37-52.
Charles C Thomas, Publisher; 1973.
26 Nashner LM, Berthoz A. Visual contribution 37 Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB. Assessing the
14 Brazeltc~nTB. Neonatal Behavioral Asses-
to rapid motor responses during posture con- influence of sensor). interaction on balance:
ment Scale. 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa: JB Lippin-
trol. Brain Kes 1978;150:403-407, suggestion from the field. Plrys Ther 1986;
cott Co; 1984.
27 Lishman JR, I.ee DN. The autonomy of vi- 66:1548-1550.
15 Bernstein N. Co-ordination and Regula-
sual kinaesthesis. Perception. 1973;2:287-294. 38 Shumway-Cook A, Horak FB. Rehabilitation
tion oJMovements. New York, NY: Pergamon
strategies for patients with vestibular deficits.
Press Inc; 1967.
Neurol Clin. 1990;8:441457.

Physical Therapy /Volume 70, Number 12 /December 1990

You might also like