You are on page 1of 10

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Pain rather than induced emotions and ICU sound increases


skin conductance variability in healthy volunteers
€rt
€nther1,2, A. R. Schandl1,3, J. Berhardsson4, A. Bja
A. C. Gu a4, M. W € Sundin4, J. Alvarsson5,
allgren4, O.
6 1,3 1,3
M. Bottai , C.-R. Martling and P. V. Sackey
1
Section for Anesthesiology and Intensive Care, Department of Physiology and Pharmacology, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
2
Department of Cardiothoracic Surgery and Anesthesiology, Karolinska University Hospital, Stockholm, Sweden
3
Department of Anesthesiology, Surgical Services and Intensive Care Medicine, Karolinska University Hospital Solna, Stockholm, Sweden
4 €
Division of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology, Mid Sweden University, Ostersund, Sweden
5
Marcus Wallenberg Laboratory, Department of Aeronautical and Vehicle Engineering, School of Engineering Sciences, Royal Institute of
Technology, Stockholm, Sweden
6
Unit of Biostatistics, Department of Environmental Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden

Correspondence Background: Assessing pain in critically ill patients is difficult. Skin


A. C. Gu€nther, Department of Cardiothoracic conductance variability (SCV), induced by the sympathetic response to
Surgery and Anesthesiology, Karolinska pain, has been suggested as a method to identify pain in poorly commu-
University Hospital, 17176 Stockholm, Sweden nicating patients. However, SCV, a derivate of conventional skin conduc-
E-mail: anders.gunther@karolinska.se tance, could potentially also be sensitive to emotional stress. The
purpose of the study was to investigate if pain and emotional stress can
Conflicts of interest
be distinguished with SCV.
The authors have no conflicts of interest.
Methods: In a series of twelve 1-min sessions with SCV recording, 18
Funding
healthy volunteers were exposed to standardized electric pain stimula-
Funding was supplied from by the Department tion during blocks of positive, negative, or neutral emotion, induced
of Anesthesiology, Surgical Services and with pictures from the International Affective Picture System (IAPS).
Intensive Care Medicine, Karolinska University Additionally, authentic intensive care unit (ICU) sound was included in
Hospital Solna. A grant was supplied from half of the sessions. All possible combinations of pain and sound
Vinnova Innovations Agency, Ministry of occurred in each block of emotion, and blocks were presented in ran-
Enterprise, Energy and Communications, domized order.
Stockholm, Sweden. The design of the study, Results: Pain stimulation resulted in increases in the number of skin
data collection, analysis, interpretation, conductance fluctuations (NSCF) in all but one participant. During pain-
writing, and publication of the manuscript free baseline sessions, the median NSCF was 0.068 (interquartile range
were done by the authors without 0.013–0.089) and during pain stimulation median NSCF increased to
participation or influence from any of the 0.225 (interquartile range 0.146–0.3175). Only small increases in NSCF
funding sources.
were found during negative emotions. Pain, assessed with the numeric
rating scale, during the sessions with pain stimulation was not altered
Submitted 22 March 2016; accepted 6 May
significantly by other ongoing sensory input.
2016; submission 5 October 2015.
Conclusion: In healthy volunteers, NSCF appears to reflect ongoing
autonomous reactions mainly to pain and to a lesser extent, reactions to
emotion induced with IAPS pictures or ICU sound.

Editorial comment: what this article tells us


Variation in skin conductance is influenced by pain, emotions, and sound. The authors performed
a volunteer study to evaluate if variability in skin conductance to pain can be used as an analgesia
trigger. This method appears to identify mainly pain and responds less to emotions and ICU
sound. Trends rather than a single absolute number seem to better identify if the subject was in
pain.

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1111

A. C. GUNTHER ET AL.

Citation
Gu€nther AC, Schandl A, Berhardsson J, Bj€art
a
A, W € Alvarsson J, Bottai
allgren M, Sundin O,
M, Martling C-R, Sackey PV. Pain rather than
induced emotions and ICU sound increases
skin conductance variability in healthy
volunteers. Acta Anaesthesiologica
Scandinavica 2016

doi: 10.1111/aas.12751

Critically ill patients experience significant Skin conductance variability has shown to be
levels of pain and discomfort while in the ICU. a valid method for pain assessment in post-
During the ICU stay, patients may be exposed operative patients.14 Results from a recent study
to disease-related pain, as well as therapy- in ICU patients indicated that pain induced SCV
induced pain. Unrelieved acute pain may lead to changes in these patients.15 However, SCV
negative physiological and psychological conse- increased not only due to painful stimulation
quences.1 Evaluation and treatment of pain in but also appeared to increase due to emotional
ICU patients has shown to be associated with stress as assessed with the Motor and Activity
shorter duration of mechanical ventilation and Assessment Scale.15
ICU stay, as well as reduced adverse events.2–4 Our aim was to, with an experimental design
Assessing pain in ICU patients may, however, in healthy subjects, investigate if SCV could dis-
be difficult as many ICU patients are not able to tinguish between pain and emotional stress.
communicate or self-rate their experience. So Moreover, we hypothesized that pain would be
far, the only method to assess pain in non-com- perceived more intensely during simultaneous
municative patients has been by observing potentially stressful exposures, namely negative
behavioral indicators of pain.5,6 visual stimuli and ICU sound.
There has recently been growing interest in
palmar skin conductance variability (SCV) and
Methods
it has been suggested for detecting autonomous
responses to nociceptive stimulation.7–9 After approval from the regional ethics commit-
Skin conductance variability is derived from tee in Stockholm, Sweden, we conducted the
traditional skin conductance measurement used experiment at the Psychology Laboratory, at the
for monitoring autonomic nervous system acti- Department of Psychology, Mid-Sweden Univer-
vation in basic psychology research, and also in €
sity, Ostersund, Sweden.
research on populations with problems such as
phobias and post-traumatic stress.10,11 In short,
Participants
distress increases sympathetic activation of
palmar and plantar sweat glands, leading to Twenty healthy volunteers (mainly psychology
increased electrical conductivity (i.e., skin con- students) were invited to the study and 18 par-
ductance). Traditional skin conductance moni- ticipated (seven men and 11 women with a
toring measures the change in absolute skin median age of 25 years, ranging from 20 to
conductance in response to a stimulus. In con- 53 years) in the study. One participant revealed
trast, SCV is an online method that identifies afterwards that he had scored pain levels falsely
fluctuations of skin conductance caused by high in the pain titration part of the experiment
ongoing palmar sweat release and reabsorp- and was therefore excluded. One participant did
tion.7,8 Thus, SCV is less sensitive to individual not come for the experiment as planned. Rea-
baseline skin conductance or body temperature sons for exclusion were pregnancy, chronic
than absolute measures of skin conductance pain, heart problems including pacemakers, or
and mirrors current sympathetic neuronal the use of psychotropic drugs. All participants
activity.12,13 signed an informed consent.

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


1112 ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
PAIN RATHER THAN EMOTION TRIGGER SCV

conductor of the trial. The Coulbourn device


Procedure
was set at a starting point of 0.6 mA, after
Participants were seated in a chair in a quiet lab which the mA was increased in increments of
room in front of a computer and were given 0.3 mA until 2.3 mA and thereafter a final
headphones. Electrodes for measuring skin con- increase to 4 mA. When the individual rated
ductance were placed on the palmar side of the pain as more than VAS 4 but less than VAS 6,
left hand. A pain stimulator was placed on the this level was used during the exposure session.
index finger of the right hand. Participants Participants received this standardized pain
could not see the investigators but were moni- stimulation during half of the 12 sessions. Elec-
tored by a remote camera. trical stimulation was given at random intervals,
Each participant was exposed to twelve exper- with a total of 24 electrical stimulations over the
imental conditions (sessions), each lasting 60 s. 1-min-long session (0.4/s). The order of the two
Between each session, there was a 1-min resting pain sessions was randomized over the four ses-
period. The 12 sessions consisted of all possible sions within each emotion block (described
combinations of two pain states (pain/no pain), below).
three different emotion-inducing picture batter-
ies (positive, negative, or neutral), and two
Emotion-inducing pictures
sound states (sound/no sound). After each ses-
In order to induce three different emotions (pos-
sion, a message on the screen instructed partici-
itive, neutral, or negative), a total of 36 emotion-
pants to relax until they heard a sound,
ally charged pictures from the International
indicating the start of the next session.
Affective Picture System (IAPS) were used (12
for each emotion).
Materials, apparatus and exposures International affective picture system is based
on large set of pictures used for standardizing
E-prime (Psychology Software Tools Inc,
emotional stimulation. They are calibrated for
Sharpsburg, PA, USA) was used to program the
affective response and characterized primarily
experiment with triggers for electrical stimula-
by normative ratings of valence and arousal.17
tion, pictures, and sound playback, as well as
Valence refers to the attractiveness (high
data collection from online self-reports.
valence) or aversiveness (low valence) of an
object (or event/situation). Arousal refers to
Standardized electrical pain stimulation how excited (high arousal) or calm the subjects
Pain was induced with the Coulbourn Transcu- feel in response to an object/event/situation.
taneous Aversive Finger Stimulator (Coulbourn Both measures range from 1 to 9. Pictures were
Instruments, Whitehall, PA, USA), with remote selected to gain a variety of content and three
triggering via Biopack Systems MP150 (BIO- levels of emotional valence; 12 positive, 12 neu-
PACK Systems INC, Goleta, CA, USA). The tral, and 12 negative pictures (Table 1). The pic-
electrical stimulus was delivered to the distal tures were presented in a 1024 9 768
phalanges of the index and middle finger in resolution, giving them a measurement of
1 ms spikes set to 100 ms duration. To stan- 22 9 18.7 cm on the computer screen.
dardize the stimulation, pain was titrated indi- The positive picture series portrayed for exam-
vidually to a modified visual analog scale (VAS) ple, smiling people and beautiful sceneries (IAPS
of 5 prior to the 30-min experiment.16 VAS 5 pictures: 2091, 2224, 4612, 4622, 4698, 5470,
was chosen as a pain level encountered in ICU 5480, 5626, 5833, 7325, 7472, 7492). The neutral
patients, indicating clear need for analgesia. Par- picture series included innate objects, such as a
ticipants could change the position of the VAS chair or a spoon (IAPS pictures: 7000, 7002, 7004,
scale indicator, rating pain from ‘no pain’, 7006, 7010, 7012, 7025, 7052, 7183, 7185, 7186,
which was the first rating for all participants 7705). The negative picture series contained
prior to electrical stimulation, to ‘worst imagin- images of death, threat, mutilation, and suffering
able pain’. The back of the scale contained a people and were chosen in order to resemble
numbered scale, range 0–10, visible to the unpleasant persecutory hallucinations and fears

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1113

A. C. GUNTHER ET AL.

Table 1 Mean and standard deviation (SD) for valence and arousal of the three series of emotionally charged pictures from international
affective picture system (IAPS). Data from IAPS standardized values of each picture ranging from 1 to 9.

Positive pictures (n 12) Neutral pictures (n 12) Negative pictures (n 12)

Valence Arousal Valence Arousal Valence Arousal

Mean (SD) 7.2 (1.6) 5.1 (2.2) 5.0 (1.1) 2.8 (2.0) 2.4 (1.5) 6.1 (2.3)

consecutive sessions (emotion block). The order


of the three emotion blocks was randomized for
each participant (Table 2).

ICU sound
To simulate the background sound of an ICU
environment, an authentic daytime sound
recording from a fully occupied three-bed room
at the General ICU, Karolinska University
Hospital Solna, Sweden was used. The record-
ing included background sounds (from ventila-
tors and other machines), doctors and nurses
talking, and monitor alarms. The sound presen-
tation lasted for 60 s, with volume peaks at
80 dB. The order of sound/no sound in the four
sessions within each ‘emotion block’ was ran-
domized (Table 2).

Outcome assessment

Skin conductance variability measured with NSCF


The Med-Storm Stress Detector device (MED-
STORM Innovation AS, Oslo, Norway) was
used to measure SCV. In this measurement
application, used in most recent studies as well
Fig. 1. International affective picture system-like pictures with the as our study, SCV is reported as the number of
purpose to induce emotions. Examples of positive, neutral, and skin conductance fluctuations/second (NSCF).7,8
negative emotion-inducing pictures. These pictures were not used in The criterion for the registration of a skin con-
the study. Source: Pixabay.com (Free pictures). ductance fluctuation was an amplitude between
skin conductance trough and peak of > 0.02
described by patients after their ICU stay (IAPS microsiemens (lS), as in previous studies.12,18,19
pictures: 2811, 3063, 3068, 3150, 3168, 6230, The fluctuations registered with this method
6250, 6830, 9000, 9301, 9490, 9599). According to correlate with bursts of sympathetic neuronal
IAPS policy, pictures from the database cannot stimulation of the palmar sweat glands.12
be published. IAPS-like images, from pixabay.- Before the start of the session, three dispos-
com (copyright-free), are included as similar able Ag/AgCl electrodes were attached to the
examples (Fig. 1). palmar surface of the participants hand: thenar
In each 1-min session, the 12 pictures from eminence (current electrode), hypothenar emi-
the same emotion category were shown twice nence (measurement electrode), and just below
(n = 24), for 2.5 s each time. Each emotion- the second and third digits (reference electrode),
inducing picture battery was shown in four respectively. NSCF was measured during the
Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120
1114 ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
PAIN RATHER THAN EMOTION TRIGGER SCV

Table 2 Example of randomized exposures for an individual participant.

Negative emotion block Positive emotion block Neutral emotion block

Pain – – Pain Pain Pain – – Pain – Pain –


Sound – Sound – – Sound Sound – – Sound Sound

entire experiment. Mean NSCF values over each pain/pain), emotion (positive, neutral, or nega-
minute of exposure were used in the analysis. tive), and sound (no sound/sound).
For regression analysis, we used the NSCF
from the session with neutral pictures, no elec-
Pain rating
trical stimulation, and no ICU sound as the
After each 1-min session, participants were
baseline. The same combination was also trea-
instructed to rate pain on the screen in front. To
ted as the NRS 0 baseline condition in the
rate pain during the session, a modified numeric
regression analysis of NRS. Based on the results
rating scale consisting of 11 unnumbered hori-
of the regression models, NSCF and NRS reac-
zontal boxes on the computer screen was used.20
tivity were estimated for the different sessions.
Pain was rated from ‘no pain’ to ‘worst pain
The intra-individual correlation was also esti-
imaginable’ by ticking a box. For analysis,
mated with Spearman0 s rho. Rho indicates the
boxes were later assigned values from 0 to 10
correlation within individuals compared to the
(in concordance with the VAS). The self-ratings
group correlation. Rho 0.5 = substantial intra-
were followed by a 1-min resting phase.
individual correlation, Rho 0 = all individuals
Number of skin conductance fluctuations was
have the same values across sessions. Rho can
measured on the 18 participants across the 12
also be translated to the percentage of inter-
sessions, resulting in a total of 216 observations
individual difference. Thus, a rho value of 0.55
(12 9 18 = 216) (Table 2). Each participant
equals an inter-individual difference of 55%, (a
rated pain six times, for a total of 108 self-
substantial difference, indicating that the vari-
reports of pain. In all subjects, VAS was rated
ability within the group is large).
as zero prior to the experiment sessions. These
Results with P-values of < 0.05 were consid-
values were included in the random-effect linear
ered statistically significant.
regression model.

Results
Statistical analysis
Data for the mean, standard deviation, median,
Power calculation was made for within-subjects interquartile range (IQR p25–p75), and full
analysis with anticipated mean NSCF of 0.07 range of NSCF for the 12 exposures sessions are
(Standard Deviation, SD 0.15) in non-painful presented in Table 3.
exposures and mean NSCF of 0.21 (SD 0.15) dur-
ing painful stimulation. With a sample size of 20,
NSCF and NRS during pain stimulation
with the alpha error < 0.05, power was 97%.
A within-subjects (2 9 3 9 2) analysis for The 18 individuals’ baseline session measure-
dependent measures was used. The dependent ment (neutral pictures, no electrical stimulation,
variables NSCF and self-perceived pain (NRS) and no sound) rendered a median NSCF value
were analyzed in two separate random-effect lin- of 0.068 peaks/s (IQR p25–p75: 0.013–
ear regression models. The participants’ random 0.089 peaks/s, respectively). In the 108 mea-
effects were included to take into account the surements with pain stimulation, median NSCF
potential correlation between the repeated mea- was 0.225 peaks/s (IQR p25–p75: 0.146–
sures in each subject. In each model, the predic- 0.318 peaks/s), respectively. The median NRS
tors of interest were type of pain stimulation (no during pain stimulation (all pain sessions) was

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1115

A. C. GUNTHER ET AL.

Table 3 Mean, median, and range data for number of skin conductance fluctuations in the 12 different sessions.

Number of skin conductance fluctuations during the experiment

Session Mean Standard deviation Median Interquartile range (p25–p75) Full range (min–max)

Neutral pictures 0.075 0.084 0.068 0.013–0.089 0–0.318


No sound
No pain
Neutral pictures 0.206 0.125 0.176 0.094–0.348 0.035–0.383
No sound
Pain
Neutral pictures 0.086 0.080 0.069 0.013–0.130 0–0.270
Sound
No pain
Neutral pictures 0.211 0.118 0.321 0.105–0.321 0.035–0.40
Sound
Pain
Positive pictures 0.087 0.090 0.043 0.018–0.137 0–0.283
No sound
No pain
Positive pictures 0.230 0.107 0.260 0.113–0.331 0.05–0.383
No sound
Pain
Positive pictures 0.089 0.068 0.068 0.033–0.154 0–0.203
Sound
No pain
Positive pictures 0.241 0.086 0.233 0.150–0.321 0.133–0.365
Sound
Pain
Negative pictures 0.116 0.146 0.068 0.029–0.163 0–0.6
No sound
No pain
Negative pictures 0.225 0.107 0.233 0.142–0.316 0.018–0.433
No sound
Pain
Negative pictures 0.099 0.095 0.051 0.029–0.156 0–0.318
Sound
No pain
Negative pictures 0.245 0.095 0.226 0.176–0.333 0.068–0.40
Sound
Pain

4, (IQR p25–p75: range: 2–6, respectively). All after adjusting for the effect of ICU sound and
but one subject demonstrated an elevation in pain, (P < 0.05). Positive emotion and ICU
NSCF during pain stimulation (Fig. 2). sounds did not impact NSCF significantly. The
intra-individual correlation rho 0.58 indicates
that 58% of the variability was due to inter-
NSCF in relation to combined pain
individual differences.
stimulation, pictures, and sound
Pain stimulation increased NSCF significantly,
NRS in relation to electrical stimulation,
0.13 peaks/s from baseline, after adjusting for
pictures, and sound
picture-induced emotion and ICU sound
(P < 0.001) (Table 3, Fig. 3). Negative emotion As hypothesized, pain rating was increased,
also increased NSCF significantly, 0.03 peaks/s, with NRS 3.95 during pain stimulation

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


1116 ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
PAIN RATHER THAN EMOTION TRIGGER SCV

Fig. 3. Mean number of skin conductance fluctuation values and 95%


confidence intervals for the 12 sessions from the random-effects
model.

Table 4 Changes in number of skin conductance fluctuation


(NSCF) and pain rating induced by pain stimulation, emotion, and
sound, based on random-effects linear regression.

Change in Change in
Variable N NSCF peaks/s pain NRS

Pain stimulation 108 0.13‡ 3.95‡


Positive emotion 72 0.02 0.10
Negative emotion 72 0.03* 0.36*
ICU sound 108 0.01 0.21
Rho§ 0.58 0.35
Fig. 2. Individual responses in number of skin conductance *P < 0.05; ‡P < 0.001. §Intra-individual correlation. NSCF, num-
fluctuation: effects of negative pictures, ICU sound, and pain ber of skin conductance fluctuations/second; NRS, numeric rating
respectively, compared to baseline session (neutral pictures, no scale.
sound, no pain).

Discussion
(P < 0.001) (Tables 3 and 4, Fig. 4). Negative In our study, pain stimulation titrated to VAS 5
emotion also increased VAS significantly but to prior to the sessions, led to a distinct increase in
a much lesser extent, by 0.36 units (P < 0.05). skin conductance variability, while emotions
Positive pictures and ICU sound did not affect induced by IAPS pictures and authentic ICU
NRS significantly. The intra-individual correla- sound had limited effects on NSCF.
tion rho 0.35 indicates that 35% of the variabil- Our interpretation is that in a controlled envi-
ity was due to inter-individual differences. ronment, NSCF is more specific to pain than to
The random-effect linear regression coeffi- emotion. In a previous study of NSCF in criti-
cients for NSCF as a NRS-dependent variable cally ill ICU patients, many patients were
implied that a 1-unit increase in NRS was asso- unable to self-report ongoing adverse percep-
ciated with a NSCF increase by 0.073 peaks/s tions.15 While non-painful adverse perceptions,
(P < 0.001). The intra-individual correlation rho such as intense fear, hallucinations, or delusions
0.25 indicates that 25% of the variability was may lead to agitation, untreated pain itself may
due to inter-individual differences. also lead to agitation.21 The results of the
Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120
ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1117

A. C. GUNTHER ET AL.

critically ill patients deemed in pain and receiv-


ing analgesics may be of value to confirm its role
and validity in this specific population.
Compared with the effect of pain, there were
only minor NSCF and NRS differences between
the different picture series, with negative pic-
tures increasing NSCF and NRS marginally
(Table 3). Affective modulation of autonomic
reactions to noxious stimulation has been stated
Fig. 4. Mean numeric rating scale values and 95% confidence in earlier studies in which different emotion-
intervals for the six sessions with pain stimulation from the random- inducing picture series were presented during
effects model. nociceptive stimulation.25–27 In our study, the
reference values of valence and arousal of the
present study suggest that agitation noted in chosen IAPS pictures used in the three IAPS
some of these poorly communicating, intubated picture series were sufficiently different to have
patients may have represented an expression of induced the proposed emotions according to the
pain. Poorly treated pain has in fact been associ- IAPS paradigm which should further support
ated with agitated delirium.22 the finding of pain as the main determinant for
Pain, titrated to an individual VAS 5 in each SCV increases.
participant prior to the experiment, led to a med- We evoked pain intermittently with electrical
ian NSCF of 0.27. This is similar to the findings stimulation in otherwise healthy and drug-free
from skin conductance variability studies in volunteers, in order to reduce the heterogeneity
post-operative and ICU patients. In these studies, in exposures and potential drug effects found in
moderate pain, equivalent of VAS 4–5 was asso- critically ill ICU patients. One major difference
ciated with NSCF of approximately 0.25.14,15 between our experimental setting and the real
The rho value for NSCF of 0.58, however, ICU scenario is that the stimulations and inter-
indicates that there are substantial inter-indivi- pretation of these, as well as the autonomic
dual differences. This is depicted with the raw response in the two settings may be vastly dif-
data presented in Fig. 1 and Table 3. Thus, in ferent. The pain exposure in our study may
the clinical setting, NSCF changes in the indi- resemble procedural pain in relatively stable
vidual patient, in combination with clinical and otherwise pain-free patients. Many ICU
assessment may be more relevant to follow than patients, however, may suffer from ongoing
to use a strict NSCF threshold value as an anal- nociceptive inflammatory and in some cases
gesia trigger. neuropathic pain. In parallel, they may be aware
Currently, the most objective measures of pain of their life-threatening situation or be in delu-
in poorly communicating ICU patients are com- sional fear (e.g., of staff trying to kill them).
posite observer’s scales.5,24 These scales are bet- Such fear may be more emotionally distressing
ter than arbitrary assessment, in that inter-rater and perhaps lead to greater autonomous
variability may be reduced. Nonetheless, some responses than what can be mimicked in a
parameters common for the Clinical Pain Obser- study in healthy volunteers. The same applies to
vational Tool (CPOT)5 and Behavioral Pain Scale the connotation of ICU sound. For some ICU
(BPS),24 such as poor ventilator compliance or patients, the alarms from a ventilator or monitor
facial grimacing, might be observed for other rea- are very intimidating, as this may indicate a
sons than pain. Considering that pain was the problem with life-supporting devices or vital
main determinant in NSCF increases and find- functions.28 Thus, while our study assessed
ings from a previous study in ICU patients,15 our standardized stimuli in subjects able to self-
interpretation is that NSCF may potentially be report, a significant limitation may be that these
considered as a complementary pain assessment volunteers are likely not as vulnerable and sen-
method in poorly communicating patients. The sitive to adverse stimuli as are critically ill
potential benefits, however, need large-scale patients in the ICU. Additionally, it is unclear
evaluation. Further study of SCV monitoring in whether some drugs, such as beta-blockers or
Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120
1118 ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd
PAIN RATHER THAN EMOTION TRIGGER SCV

alpha-2-agonists, or severe critical illness might unit: a randomized clinical trial. J Crit Care 2013;
interfere with sympathetic tone and responses 28: 918–22.
to stimulation. A future study in ICU survivors, 4. Arbour C, Gelinas C, Michaud C. Impact of the
recently exposed to intensive care, could evalu- implementation of the critical-care pain observation
ate if there are potentially larger SCV increases tool (CPOT) on pain management and clinical
to non-painful, adverse stimuli and as stated, outcomes in mechanically ventilated trauma
studies of critically ill patients with validated intensive care unit patients: a pilot study. J Trauma
Nurs 2011; 18: 52–60.
pain assessment tools during stimulation and
5. Gelinas C, Fillion L, Puntillo KA, Viens C, Fortier M.
analgesia are warranted.
Validation of the critical-care pain observation tool in
In summary, skin conductance variability
adult patients. Am J Crit Care 2006; 15: 420–7.
increased mainly in response to pain and only
6. Sessler CN, Gosnell MS, Grap MJ, Brophy GM,
marginally in response to induced emotion or
O’Neal PV, Keane KA, Tesoro EP, Elswick RK.
ICU sound in healthy volunteers. Individual The Richmond Agitation-Sedation Scale: validity
trends rather than absolute thresholds appeared and reliability in adult intensive care unit
to be relevant. In conclusion, skin conductance patients. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2002; 166:
variability, as defined in our study, reacts more to 1338–44.
pain (rated as VAS > 4 and < 6) than strongly 7. Storm H. Changes in skin conductance as a tool to
negative emotionally charged pictures in healthy monitor nociceptive stimulation and pain. Curr
volunteers. There is a substantial degree of inter- Opin Anaesthesiol 2008; 21: 796–804.
individual differences in skin conductance vari- 8. Storm H, Fremming A, Odegaard S, Martinsen OG,
ability reactions. Skin conductance variability Morkrid L. The development of a software program
can potentially be a supplement in assessing pain for analyzing spontaneous and externally elicited
in the critically ill, poorly communicating patient skin conductance changes in infants and adults.
but needs evaluation as a complement in the ICU Clin Neurophysiol 2000; 111: 1889–98.
setting. Studies of skin conductance variability as 9. Storm H, Støen R, Klepstad P, Skorpen F, Qvigstad
an adjunct to observational scales in the assess- E, Raeder J. Nociceptive stimuli responses at
ment and treatment of pain in poorly communi- different levels of general anaesthesia and genetic
cating ICU patients are warranted. variability. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2013; 57:
89–99.
10. De Pascalis V, Magurano MR, Bellusci A. Pain
Acknowledgements perception somatosensory event-related potentials
and skin conductance responses to painful stimuli
The authors thank Dr Ulrik Sartipy at the in high mid and low hypnotizable subjects: effects
Department of Cardiothoracic Surgery and of differential pain reduction strategies. Pain 1999;
Anesthesiology, Karolinska University Hospital, 83: 499–508.
for great support during the manuscript phase 11. Lader MH. Palmar skin conductance measures in
of this study. anxiety and phobic states. J Psychosom Res 1967;
11: 271–81.
12. Wallin BG. Sympathetic nerve activity underlying
References electrodermal and cardiovascular reactions in man.
1. Lindenbaum L, Milia DJ. Pain management in the Psychophysiology 1981; 18: 470–6.
ICU. Surg Clin North Am 2012; 92: 1621–36. 13. Lidberg L, Wallin BG. Sympathetic skin nerve
2. Payen J-F, Bosson J-L, Chanques G, Mantz J, Labarere discharges in relation to amplitude of skin
J. Pain assessment is associated with decreased resistance responses. Psychophysiology 1981; 18:
duration of mechanical ventilation in the intensive 268–70.
care unit: a post Hoc analysis of the DOLOREA study. 14. Ledowski T, Bromilow J, Wu J, Paech MJ, Storm
Anesthesiology 2009; 111: 1308–16. H, Schug SA. The assessment of postoperative pain
3. Mansouri P, Javadpour S, Zand F, Ghodsbin F, by monitoring skin conductance: results of a
Sabetian G, Masjedi M, Tabatabaee HR. prospective study. Anaesthesia 2007; 62: 989–93.
Implementation of a protocol for integrated 15. G€unther AC, Bottai M, Schandl AR, Storm H, Rossi
management of pain, agitation, and delirium can P, Sackey PV. Palmar skin conductance variability
improve clinical outcomes in the intensive care and the relation to stimulation, pain and the motor

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd 1119

A. C. GUNTHER ET AL.

activity assessment scale in intensive care unit 22. Meier DE. Pain as a cause of agitated delirium.
patients. Crit Care 2013; 17: R51. Arch Intern Med 2012; 172: 1130.
16. Price DD, McGrath PA, Rafii A, Buckingham B. 23. Robinson S, Vollmer C. Undermedication for pain
The validation of visual analogue scales as ratio and precipitation of delirium. Medsurg Nurs 2010;
scale measures for chronic and experimental pain. 19: 79–83.
Pain 1983; 17: 45–56. 24. Payen J, Bru O, Bosson J, Lagrasta A, Novel E,
17. Lang PJ, Bradley MM, Cuthbert BN. International Deschaux I, Lavagne P, Jacquot C. Assessing pain
affective picture system (IAPS): affective ratings of in critically ill sedated patients by using a
pictures and instruction manual. Technical Report behavioural pain scale. Crit Care Med 2001; 29:
A-8. Gainesville, FL: University of Florida, 2008. 2258–63.
18. Storm H, Shafiei M, Myre K, Raeder J. Palmar skin 25. Rhudy JL, Williams AE, McCabe KM, Russell JL,
conductance compared to a developed stress score Maynard LJ. Emotional control of nociceptive
and to noxious and awakening stimuli on patients reactions (ECON): do affective valence and arousal
in anaesthesia. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand 2005; 49: play a role? Pain 2008; 136: 250–61.
798–803. 26. Rhudy JL, Bartley EJ, Williams AE. Habituation,
19. Gjerstad AC, Wagner K, Henrichsen T, Storm H. sensitization, and emotional valence modulation of
Skin conductance versus the modified COMFORT pain responses. Pain 2010; 148: 320–7.
sedation score as a measure of discomfort in 27. Godinho F, Magnin M, Frot M, Perchet C, Garcia-
artificially ventilated children. Pediatrics 2008; 122: Larrea L. Emotional modulation of pain: is it the
848–53. sensation or what we recall? J Neurosci 2006; 26:
20. Hullett B, Chambers N, Preuss J, Zambudio I, 11454–61.
Lange J, Pascoe E, Ledowski T. Monitoring 28. Johansson L, Bergbom I, Persson Waye K, Ryherd
electrical skin conductance: a tool for the EBL. The sound environment in an ICU patient
assassment of postoperative pain in children?. room- a content analysis of sound levels and
Anaesthesiology 2009; 111: 513–7. patient experiences. Intensive Crit Care Nurs 2012;
21. Downie WW, Leatham PA, Rhind VM, Wright V, 28: 269–79.
Branco JA, Anderson JA. Studies with pain rating
scales. Ann Rheum Dis 1978; 37: 378–81.

Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica 60 (2016) 1111–1120


1120 ª 2016 The Acta Anaesthesiologica Scandinavica Foundation. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd

You might also like