You are on page 1of 7

Psychopharmacology

DOI 10.1007/s00213-014-3525-2

ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION

Smoking improves divided attention in schizophrenia


Eike Ahlers & Eric Hahn & Thi Minh Tam Ta &
Elnaz Goudarzi & Michael Dettling & Andres H. Neuhaus

Received: 22 December 2013 / Accepted: 26 February 2014


# Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2014

Abstract mainly for sustained and selective attention. Gender-specific


Rationale Smoking is highly prevalent in schizophrenia, and effects on cognition need to be further investigated.
there is evidence for beneficial effects on neurocognition.
Smoking is therefore hypothesized a self-medication in Keywords Nicotine . Tobacco . Cigarettes . Divided
schizophrenia. Although much effort is devoted to character- attention . Attention . Dopamine . Cognition . Schizophrenia .
ize those cognitive domains that potentially benefit from Prefrontal cortex . Sex difference
smoking, divided attention has not yet been investigated.
Objectives The aim of this study was to analyze the interac-
tional effects of diagnosis of schizophrenia and smoking his- Introduction
tory on divided attention.
Methods We investigated behavioral measures of divided Smoking prevalence is higher in patients with schizophrenia
attention in a sample of 48 schizophrenic patients and 48 than in the general population and also compared with other
controls (24 current smokers and non-smokers each) carefully psychiatric disorders. Moreover, schizophrenic patients show
matched for age, sex, education, verbal IQ, and smoking heavier smoking patterns compared to the general population
status with general linear models. (Winterer 2010). One explanation, which is supported by a
Results Most important within the scope of this study, significant plethora of findings, is that tobacco smoking is a form of self-
interactions were found for valid reactions and errors of omis- medication, related to schizophrenia symptoms. For instance,
sion: Performance substantially increased in smoking schizo- schizophrenic patients who smoke high-nicotine cigarettes
phrenic patients, but not in controls. Further, these interactions compared to denicotinized cigarettes performed better in a
were modified by sex, driven by female schizophrenic patients verbal memory task and appeared to have transiently reduced
who showed a significant behavioral advantage of smokers over negative symptoms (Smith et al. 2002). Leonard et al. (2007)
non-smokers, other than male schizophrenic patients or healthy could show that variations in the gene coding for nicotinic
controls who did not express this sex-specific pattern. alpha7 receptors were associated with a deficit of P50 auditory
Conclusions Results suggest a positive effect of smoking sensory gating and schizophrenia. Other researchers found that
history on divided attention in schizophrenic patients. This nicotine could temporarily improve a number of neurocognitive
study provides first evidence that the complex attention do- deficits associated with schizophrenia, suggesting that nicotine
main of divided attention is improved by smoking, which intake enhances at least some cognitive domains in schizo-
further substantiates the self-medication hypothesis of phrenic patients (Depatie et al. 2002; Avila et al. 2003; Sacco
smoking in schizophrenia, although this has been shown et al. 2005). Own research supports this line of evidence and
suggests that smoking is related with an improvement of selec-
tive attention in schizophrenia (Hahn et al. 2012).
Eike Ahlers and Eric Hahn contributed equally to this work.
In general, attention deficits form a core cognitive domain
E. Ahlers (*) : E. Hahn : T. M. T. Ta : E. Goudarzi : M. Dettling : in schizophrenia that may be alleviated by nicotine consump-
A. H. Neuhaus
tion. For instance, Jacobsen et al. (2004) reported that nicotine
Department of Psychiatry, Charité University Medicine, Campus
Benjamin Franklin, Eschenallee 3, 14050 Berlin, Germany differentially improved performance of schizophrenic patients
e-mail: eike.ahlers@charite.de during a dichotic two-back task, depending on whether or not
Psychopharmacology

they were smokers. In contrast, healthy controls performed symptoms, and prescription of biperiden. All patients were
worse after a nicotine challenge. In a naturalistic study, ciga- recruited from the inpatient and outpatient units of the
rette smoking first-episode schizophrenic patients had a supe- Department of Psychiatry, Campus Benjamin Franklin,
rior performance compared to non-smoking patients in the Charité University Medicine, Berlin, Germany. All patients
selective and sustained attention measures (Segarra et al. had unrestricted access to cigarettes and a smoker’s room.
2011). Nicotine exposure to non-smoking schizophrenic pa- All patients received typical or atypical antipsychotic medica-
tients (Harris et al. 2004; Barr et al. 2008) and nicotine tion; calculation of chlorpromazine equivalents followed the
application after abstinence (Sacco et al. 2005) also demon- suggestion of Andreasen et al. (2010). Positive and Negative
strated an improvement of attention deficits by nicotine. Syndrome Scale (PANSS) ratings were performed by authors
To the best of our knowledge and in contrast to the well- EH and TMTT within 1 week after neuropsychological testing.
studied domain of sustained attention, investigations focusing on Forty-eight healthy controls (23 females, 25 males) were
divided attention in this context are largely lacking. This is a matched for age and variables related to smoking behavior,
crucial knowledge gap, given that divided attention contributes including smoking status, severity of nicotine dependence,
to cognition-based diagnostic classification of schizophrenia more and lifetime nicotine consumption. No control participant
than most attention domains (Shen et al. 2013). Divided attention had a history of substance abuse other than tobacco smoking,
refers to the ability to process more than one task at a time and can any psychiatric axis I disorder according to DSM-IV, or any
be further conceptualized as within-modal and cross-modal divid- other severe medical or neurological disorder and had never
ed attention, depending on whether received stimuli belong to one received any psychopharmacological treatment. A reported
or more modalities. The dopaminergic system has been identified first-degree family history of psychiatric illness also led to
as one of the key players involved in attention processes and gains exclusion from the study. All control participants were exam-
importance with increasing levels of task complexity, i.e., when ined by a certified psychiatrist prior to inclusion in this study.
“executive aspects as divided attention” are required (Coull Participants were classified as non-smokers (<5 cigarettes
et al. 1997). Nicotine affects cognition via presynaptic dopa- in lifetime) or smokers (daily smoking for at least 24 months);
mine release, preferentially in prefrontal areas; accordingly, former/abstinent smokers were not included in this study.
prefrontal neuronal activity is modulated by nicotine consump- Cross-sectional estimates of severity of nicotine dependence
tion, especially anterior cingulate cortex and prefrontal cortex were provided by the Fagerstroem Test for Nicotine
areas (Rezvani and Levin 2001; Newhouse et al. 2004; Hahn Dependence (Heatherton et al. 1991) as well as the total
et al. 2009). Consistently, functional magnetic resonance im- number of cigarettes smoked per day. Longitudinal measures
aging (fMRI) data on divided attention tasks showed activa- of life-time nicotine consumption were provided by quantify-
tions of ACC and prefrontal cortex areas (Vohn et al. 2007) that ing years of cigarette smoking as well as cigarette pack years
contain a relatively dense array of D1 receptors, as suggested that were calculated as 20 cigarettes per day times the number
by autoradiography studies (Palomero-Gallagher et al. 2009). of years as a smoker (Lu et al. 2011).
Although much effort is devoted to characterize cognitive All participants were right-handed and reported normal or
domains that might potentially benefit from smoking, divided corrected-to-normal vision. As an estimate of verbal IQ, a
attention has not yet been systematically investigated. In this multiple choice vocabulary test (Lehrl et al. 1995) was ap-
study, we analyzed the interactional effects of diagnosis and plied. Clinical and demographic data of patients and controls
smoking on divided attention in a large cohort of schizophren- stratified by smoking status are summarized in Table 1. All
ic patients and carefully matched healthy control participants participants gave written informed consent before participat-
in a cross-sectional design. ing in this study. The study protocol was approved by the
ethics committee of the University Hospital Benjamin
Franklin, Charité University Medicine, Berlin, Germany, and
Materials and methods the study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of
Helsinki and its amendments.
Participants
Cognitive testing
We investigated a sample of 48 clinically stable patients (29
females, 19 males) meeting Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Prior to the experiment, participants were allowed to smoke ad
of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria for libitum. Testing was done after approximately 1 h of nicotine
schizophrenia. Current regular substance intake other than abstinence, thus minimizing both acute nicotine and nicotine
nicotine as well as histories of severe medical disorder, severe withdrawal effects (Stein et al. 1998).
neurological disorder, or electroconvulsive therapy led to ex- The effect of smoking on divided attention was analyzed
clusion from this study. Other exclusion criteria were current using the applicable subtest of the test for attention performance
intake of benzodiazepines, presence of extrapyramidal (Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung, TAP; Zimmermann
Psychopharmacology

and Fimm 1992). The test was run on a personal computer and mean reaction time, valid reactions, anticipations, errors of
presented on a 17-in. cathode ray tube monitor. Behavioral commission, errors of omission, and lapses of attention with
responses were collected via a response key. Participants per- separate 2×2×2 analyses of covariance (ANCOVAS) that in-
formed a dual task that requiring simultaneous reactions to cluded “Diagnostic group,” “Sex,” and “Smoking status” as
visual and auditory stimuli. During the test, varying numbers fixed factors and “Fagerstroem score” as covariate. For all
of crosses simultaneously appeared on the screen, while at the ANCOVAs, partial eta-squared served as an estimator of effect
same time high and low tones were presented in sequence. size, i.e., the proportion of variance accounted for by the model.
Participants were instructed to press the response key whenever Tests were performed as two-tailed tests with an alpha level set
four crosses formed a square on the screen or when the same at p<0.05.
tone was presented twice in a row. Following a training session
of ten trials, 50 test trials were presented. Outcome measures
were mean reaction time, valid reactions, anticipations, errors Results
of commission, errors of omission, and lapses of attention.
Additionally, all participants were tested with a basic neuropsy- According to the nicotine self-medication hypothesis in
chological test battery (see Table 1) including a multiple-choice schizophrenia and for the sake of clarity in presenting the
vocabulary test to estimate (pre-morbid) verbal intelligence results, we will here focus on those dependent variables that
(Lehrl et al. 1995), the Digit Symbol Test, and the Trail showed an interaction of “Diagnostic group”דSmoking sta-
Making Test. tus,” i.e., valid reactions and errors of omission.

Statistical analyses Valid reactions

Statistical calculations were conducted using IBM SPSS 19.0 ANCOVA indicated a significant main effect of “Diagnostic
(SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Demographic, basic neuropsy- group” (F(1, 95)=18.728; p<0.001; η2 =0.177), where schizo-
chological, and clinical data were analyzed with χ2 test and t phrenic patients had fewer valid reactions than healthy controls
tests for independent samples, as appropriate. We analyzed (26.62±4.9 vs. 30.06±2.1). Another significant main effect

Table 1 Summary of demographic, basic neuropsychological, and clinical data

Schizophrenia Control

Smokers Non-smokers Total Smokers Non-smokers Total

N (female/male) 24 (15/9) 24 (14/10) 48 (29/19) 24 (10/14) 24 (13/11) 48 (23/25)


Age (years) 33.6±10.6 37.8±10.5 35.7±10.6 35.0±6.9 32.6±9.6 33.8±8.3
Education (years) 14.0±1.9 13.6±2.0 13.7±1.9 13.7±1.9 14.8±2.2 14.3±2.1
Verbal IQ 107.9±12.6 108.3±13.2 108.1±12.8 107.1±11.4 113.8±15.0 110.4±13.6
Digit Symbol Test 45.8±8.8 45.8±10.9 45.8±9.8a 56.4±10.1b 64.3±7.1b 60.3±9.5a
Trail Making Test—A 34.3±11.0 35.8±12.5 35.1±11.7a 27.2±6.0 26.0±5.0 26.6±5.5a
Trail Making Test—B 85.9±23.1 85.3±32.0 85.6±27.5a 66.9±17.0b 57.1±13.9 62.0±16.1a
Cigarette pack years 12.6±10.8 – – 10.3±8.2 – –
Years of smoking 10.7±7.0 – – 15.1±9.6 – –
Cigarettes per day 22.5±12.3 – – 16.0±11.5 – –
FTND score 5.8±1.9c – – 3.4±2.2c – –
DOI (months) 78.1±90.7 114.9±90.0 96.5±91.3 – – –
N episodes 3.7±4.0 4.2±3.3 3.9±3.7 – – –
PANSS positive scale 12.3±3.8 14.1±4.5 13.2±4.2 – – –
PANSS negative scale 15.2±5.0 16.5±5.2 15.9±5.1 – – –
PANSS general scale 30.6±7.8 32.9±9.4 31.8±8.6 – – –
CPZ equivalents (mg/day) 475.0±313.1 500.2±322.1 487.6±414.5 – – –

FTND Fagerstroem Test for Nicotine Dependence, DOI duration of illness, PANSS Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale, CPZ chlorpromazine
a
Significant between-group differences: schizophrenia vs. control total
b
Significant between-group differences: smokers vs. non-smokers controls
c
Significant between-group differences: schizophrenia vs. control smokers
Psychopharmacology

was found for “Smoking status” (F(1, 95)=5.175; p<0.05; smokers vs. smokers 3.48±4.8 vs. 1.54±2.0; F(1, 95)=
η2 =0.056), where non-smokers had fewer valid reactions than 9.135; p<0.005; η2 =0.095). The interaction of “Diagnostic
smokers (27.40±5.2 vs. 29.28±2.5). When following the in- group”דSmoking status” (F(1, 95)=6.129; p<0.05; η2 =
teraction of “Diagnostic group”דSmoking status” (F(1, 95)= 0.066) was based on a superior performance of smoking vs.
5.508; p<0.05; η2 =0.060), we found that smoking schizo- non-smoking schizophrenic patients (2.21±2.2 vs. 5.79±5.8;
phrenic patients performed significantly better than non- T846)=−2.816; p<0.01); there was no difference between
smoking patients (28.44±2.4; T(46)=2.769; p<0.01) while smokers and non-smokers in healthy controls (0.88±1.6 vs.
there was no difference between smokers and non-smokers in 1.17±1.4; see Fig. 2a).
healthy controls (30.13±2.3 vs. 30.00±2.0; see Fig. 1a). This Again, the interaction of “Diagnostic group”דSmoking
interaction was further modified, thus resulting in a tripartite status” was modified by the factor “Sex” (F(1, 95)=4.478;
interaction of “Diagnostic group”דSmoking status”דSex” p<0.05; η2 =0.049) that was driven by female schizophrenic
(F(1, 95)=7.114; p<0.01; η2 =0.076), which was driven by patients who showed a clear behavioral advantage of smokers
female schizophrenic patients who showed a significant be- over non-smokers (2.13±2.2 vs. 7.36±7.0). This difference
havioral advantage of smokers over non-smokers (28.53±2.4 was not present in male schizophrenic patients (2.33±2.4 vs.
vs. 22.86±6.7; T(27)=3.081; p<0.01), other than male schizo- 3.60±2.7) or in healthy controls (female 1.50±2.3 vs. 0.85±
phrenic patients (28.30±2.4 vs. 27.50±3.7) or healthy controls 1.1; male 0.43±0.6 vs. 1.55±1.7; see Fig. 2b).
(female 29.20±3.1 vs. 30.54±1.9; male 30.79±1.3 vs. 29.36±
2.0; see Fig. 1b) who did not express this sex-specific pattern.
Discussion
Errors of omission
This is the first study aiming at investigating the effects of
When examining errors of omission, we observed a similar smoking history on performance in a cross-modal divided
pattern with significant main effects of “Diagnostic group” attention task in schizophrenia compared with matched
(schizophrenia vs. control 4.00±4.7 vs. 1.02±1.5; F(1, 95)= healthy controls. We conducted a cross-sectional study to
15.647; p< 001; η2 = 0.152) and “Smoking status” (non- address the question, whether divided attention is affected

a b
35

30
N valid reactions

25
non-smokers
20
smokers

15

10

0
female male
Controls

35

30
*
N valid reactions

25

20 non-smokers
smokers
15

10

0
female male
Schizophrenia

Fig. 1 a Combined scatter plot of lifetime nicotine consumption in pack status. Differences in number of valid reactions in this task are driven by
years (abscissa) vs. number of valid reactions in a cross-modal divided smoking status in female schizophrenic patients. Error bars denote stan-
attention task for healthy controls and schizophrenic patients. b Controls dard error of the mean
(top) and schizophrenic patients (bottom) stratified for sex and smoking
Psychopharmacology

a b
8

N errors of omission
6

5
non-smokers

4 smokers

0
female male
Controls

10
9 *
8

N errors of omission
7
6 non-smokers
smokers
5
4
3
2
1
0
female male
Schizophrenia

Fig. 2 a Combined scatter plot of lifetime nicotine consumption in pack status. Differences in number of omission errors in this task are driven by
years (abscissa) vs. number of errors of omission in a cross-modal divided smoking status in female schizophrenic patients. Error bars denote stan-
attention task for healthy controls and schizophrenic patients. b Controls dard error of the mean
(top) and schizophrenic patients (bottom) stratified for sex and smoking

by chronic nicotine consumption, similarly to existing studies cognitive functions: While schizophrenic patients move to-
within the sustained or the selective attention domains. We ward the curve maximum, controls may either show very little
found significant improvements of behavioral performance effect or move toward an unfavorable D1 receptor activation
regarding valid reactions and errors of omission in schizo- state. This mechanism is thought to underlie the growing body
phrenia, but not in healthy controls. Moreover, this dissocia- of findings in schizophrenia, e.g., in working memory
tion was mainly driven by female schizophrenic patients. (George et al. 2002) or selective attention (Hahn et al. 2012),
In general, the pattern of results is consistent with studies and may also explain unaffected behavioral measures (Hahn
investigating the effect of smoking history on attention and et al. 2009) as well as performance decrements in healthy
working memory that found beneficial effects in schizophren- controls in corresponding studies (Ernst et al. 2001;
ic patients and no or detrimental effects in healthy controls Lawrence et al. 2002; Jacobsen et al. 2004, 2005).
(George et al. 2002; Harris et al. 2004; Jacobsen et al. 2004; Our results were mainly driven by female patients, which
Barr et al. 2008; Segarra et al. 2011; Hahn et al. 2012). This may be indicative of an undetected population stratification
dissociation may be explained by nicotine’s agonistic action effect in our sample other than those demographic variables
on dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area that that were controlled for across groups (smoking vs. non-
project to the prefrontal cortex (Imperato et al. 1986) and the smoking; schizophrenia vs. control). In line with this thought,
inverted U-shaped function of prefrontal dopamine receptors Goldstein et al. (1998) studied cognition in schizophrenia and
(Vijayraghavan et al. 2007). As a consequence of the func- found sex-related differences in various cognitive domains with
tional prefrontal D1 deficit, schizophrenic patients are thought better performance of female compared with male schizophren-
to be located on the ascending left part of the inverted U ic patients. They discussed the heterogeneity of previously
function; in contrast, healthy controls are thought to be located reported data as being due to different sampling strategies; here,
around the top of the curve, corresponding to an optimal D1 differences in the clinical course of the disorder between males
receptor activation. Nicotine mediates the release of dopa- and females are likely to contribute to this gender effect.
mine, which in turn leads to a rightward shift on that curve. On the other hand, this result may serve as a starting point
Given the different starting points of schizophrenic patients for gender-specific effects of nicotine, similar to gender-
and healthy controls, this shift differentially affects D1 recep- specific effects on cognition (Neuhaus et al. 2009). Data on
tor activation and, as a consequence, dopamine-mediated sex effects on prefrontal cognition in schizophrenia are still
Psychopharmacology

controversial (Roesch-Ely et al. 2009), and studies investigat- Avila MT, Sherr JD, Hong E, Myers CS, Thaker GK (2003) Effects
of nicotine on leading saccades during smooth pursuit eye
ing divided attention in this context are currently missing.
movements in smokers and nonsmokers with schizophrenia.
There is evidence, however, for an impact of sex hormones Neuropsychopharmacology 28(12):2184–2191
on dopamine-dependent cognitive processes as a potential Barr RS, Culhane MA, Jubelt LE, Mufti RS, Dyer MA, Weiss AP,
mechanism for differences in cognitive performance. Deckersbach T, Kelly JF, Freudenreich O, Goff DC, Evins AE
(2008) The effects of transdermal nicotine on cognition in non-
Estradiol has been found to be associated with decision making
smokers with schizophrenia and nonpsychiatric controls.
in a reward paradigm in rats (Uban et al. 2012). In humans, Neuropsychopharmacology 33(3):480–490
estrogen seems to modulate working memory performance in Chambers RA, Krystal JH, Self DW (2001) A neurobiological basis for
healthy women as a function of baseline dopamine in the substance abuse comorbidity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry
50(2):71–83
prefrontal cortex (Jacobs and D'Esposito 2011). In a recent
Coull JT, Frith CD, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM (1997) The
review, Sacher et al. (2013) conclude that there is “preliminary neural correlates of the noradrenergic modulation of human atten-
evidence for neuroplastic changes across the menstrual cycle” tion, arousal and learning. Eur J Neurosci 9(3):589–598
in various brain areas. Hence, controlling for the phase of the Depatie L, O'Driscoll GA, Holahan AL, Atkinson V, Thavundayil JX,
Kin NN, Lal S (2002) Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for
menstrual cycle in female participants is warranted in the future,
schizophrenia: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study.
and the lack of doing so most probably constitutes a method- Neuropsychopharmacology 27(6):1056–1070
ological limitation, which is also true for the current study. Ernst M, Heishman SJ, Spurgeon L, London ED (2001) Smoking
Two other limitations have to be acknowledged. First, no history and nicotine effects on cognitive performance.
Neuropsychopharmacology 25(3):313–319
objective control of nicotine dose is available for this study.
George TP, Vessicchio JC, Termine A, Sahady DM, Head CA, Pepper
Although we sought to minimize both acute nicotine and WT, Kosten TR, Wexler BE (2002) Effects of smoking abstinence
nicotine withdrawal effects by testing after approximately on visuospatial working memory function in schizophrenia.
1 h of nicotine abstinence, it cannot be ruled out that the Neuropsychopharmacology 26(1):75–85
resulting data are heterogeneous due to individual smoking Goldstein JM, Seidman LJ, Goodman JM, Koren D, Lee H, Weintraub S,
Tsuang MT (1998) Are there sex differences in neuropsychological
habits. A control via measurement of serum cotinine or of functions among patients with schizophrenia? Am J Psychiatry
expired carbon monoxide levels would have been a more 155(10):1358–1364
favorable measure of nicotine dose than the history of Hahn B, Ross TJ, Wolkenberg FA, Shakleya DM, Huestis MA, Stein EA
smoking behavior. Second, the cross-sectional nature of our (2009) Performance effects of nicotine during selective attention,
divided attention, and simple stimulus detection: an fMRI study.
study does not permit to conclude whether the observed Cereb Cortex 19(9):1990–2000
associations between chronic smoking patterns and divided Hahn C, Hahn E, Dettling M, Gunturkun O, Ta TM, Neuhaus AH (2012)
attention in schizophrenia were caused by smoking or whether Effects of smoking history on selective attention in schizophrenia.
they preceded its initiation. Pre-existing differences between Neuropharmacology 62(4):1897–1902
Harris JG, Kongs S, Allensworth D, Martin L, Tregellas J, Sullivan B,
smokers and non-smokers with schizophrenia might consti- Zerbe G, Freedman R (2004) Effects of nicotine on cognitive deficits
tute unmeasured confounding variables, and other explana- in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 29(7):1378–1385
tions like the addiction vulnerability hypothesis (Chambers Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerstrom KO (1991) The
et al. 2001) cannot be ruled out by the current study design. Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the
Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict 86(9):1119–1127
In sum, however, first evidence is presented that divided Imperato A, Mulas A, Di Chiara G (1986) Nicotine preferentially stim-
attention is responsive to smoking history in schizophrenia. ulates dopamine release in the limbic system of freely moving rats.
This study thus adds to the growing body of evidence in favor Eur J Pharmacol 132(2–3):337–338
of the self-medication hypothesis of nicotine in schizophrenia. Jacobs E, D'Esposito M (2011) Estrogen shapes dopamine-dependent
cognitive processes: implications for women's health. J Neurosci
31(14):5286–5293
Jacobsen LK, D'Souza DC, Mencl WE, Pugh KR, Skudlarski P, Krystal
Acknowledgments We would like to thank all participants of this study JH (2004) Nicotine effects on brain function and functional connec-
for contributing their time and effort. This study is part of the doctoral tivity in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 55(8):850–858
thesis of Elnaz Goudarzi. Jacobsen LK, Krystal JH, Mencl WE, Westerveld M, Frost SJ, Pugh KR
(2005) Effects of smoking and smoking abstinence on cognition in
adolescent tobacco smokers. Biol Psychiatry 57(1):56–66
Conflict of interest There is no conflict of interest for any of the Lawrence NS, Ross TJ, Stein EA (2002) Cognitive mechanisms of
authors. nicotine on visual attention. Neuron 36(3):539–548
Lehrl S, Triebig G, Fischer B (1995) Multiple choice vocabulary test
MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence.
References Acta Neurol Scand 91(5):335–345
Leonard S, Mexal S, Freedman R (2007) Smoking, genetics and schizo-
phrenia: evidence for self medication. J Dual Diagn 3(3–4):43–59
Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Miller D, Ho BC (2010) Lu Y, Wang SS, Reynolds P, Chang ET, Ma H, Sullivan-Halley J, Clarke
Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized CA, Bernstein L (2011) Cigarette smoking, passive smoking, and
method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiatry non-Hodgkin lymphoma risk: evidence from the California teachers
67(3):255–262 study. Am J Epidemiol 174(5):563–573
Psychopharmacology

Neuhaus AH, Opgen-Rhein C, Urbanek C, Gross M, Hahn E, Ta TM, smoking in first-episode psychosis: the self-medication hypothesis.
Koehler S, Dettling M (2009) Spatiotemporal mapping of sex dif- Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 261(4):241–250
ferences during attentional processing. Hum Brain Mapp 30(9): Shen C, Popescu FC, Hahn E, Ta TT, Dettling M, Neuhaus AH (2013)
2997–3008 Neurocognitive pattern analysis reveals classificatory hierarchy of
Newhouse PA, Potter A, Singh A (2004) Effects of nicotinic attention deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull
stimulation on cognitive performance. Curr Opin Pharmacol Smith RC, Singh A, Infante M, Khandat A, Kloos A (2002) Effects of
4(1):36–46 cigarette smoking and nicotine nasal spray on psychiatric symptoms
Palomero-Gallagher N, Vogt BA, Schleicher A, Mayberg HS, Zilles K and cognition in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 27(3):
(2009) Receptor architecture of human cingulate cortex: evaluation 479–497
of the four-region neurobiological model. Hum Brain Mapp 30(8): Stein EA, Pankiewicz J, Harsch HH, Cho JK, Fuller SA, Hoffmann RG,
2336–2355 Hawkins M, Rao SM, Bandettini PA, Bloom AS (1998) Nicotine-
Rezvani AH, Levin ED (2001) Cognitive effects of nicotine. Biol induced limbic cortical activation in the human brain: a functional
Psychiatry 49(3):258–267 MRI study. Am J Psychiatry 155(8):1009–1015
Roesch-Ely D, Hornberger E, Weiland S, Hornstein C, Parzer P, Thomas Uban KA, Rummel J, Floresco SB, Galea LA (2012) Estradiol
C, Weisbrod M (2009) Do sex differences affect prefrontal cortex modulates effort-based decision making in female rats.
associated cognition in schizophrenia? Schizophr Res 107(2–3): Neuropsychopharmacology 37(2):390–401
255–261 Vijayraghavan S, Wang M, Birnbaum SG, Williams GV, Arnsten AF
Sacco KA, Termine A, Seyal A, Dudas MM, Vessicchio JC, Krishnan- (2007) Inverted-U dopamine D1 receptor actions on prefrontal
Sarin S, Jatlow PI, Wexler BE, George TP (2005) Effects of cigarette neurons engaged in working memory. Nat Neurosci 10(3):376–384
smoking on spatial working memory and attentional deficits in Vohn R, Fimm B, Weber J, Schnitker R, Thron A, Spijkers W, Willmes K,
schizophrenia: involvement of nicotinic receptor mechanisms. Sturm W (2007) Management of attentional resources in within-
Arch Gen Psychiatry 62(6):649–659 modal and cross-modal divided attention tasks: An fMRI study.
Sacher J, Okon-Singer H, Villringer A (2013) Evidence from neuroim- Hum Brain Mapp 28(12):1267–1275
aging for the role of the menstrual cycle in the interplay of emotion Winterer G (2010) Why do patients with schizophrenia smoke? Curr
and cognition. Front Hum Neurosci 7:374 Opin Psychiatry 23(2):112–119
Segarra R, Zabala A, Eguiluz JI, Ojeda N, Elizagarate E, Sanchez P, Zimmermann P, Fimm B (1992) Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprüfung
Ballesteros J, Gutierrez M (2011) Cognitive performance and (tap). Psytest, Freiburg

You might also like