You are on page 1of 5

Neuroscience Letters 685 (2018) 7–11

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Neuroscience Letters
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neulet

Research article

Childhood trauma and emotion regulation: The moderator role of BDNF T


Val66Met
Mirela I. Bîlca, Romana Vulturara,b, Adina Chișa,b, Mădălina Buciumana, Daria Nuţua,

Ioana Buneaa, Aurora Szentágotai-Tătarc, Andrei C. Miua,
a
Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Babeș-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania
b
Department of Molecular Sciences, Iuliu Haţieganu University of Medicine and Pharmacy, Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania
c
Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, Babeș-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Emotion regulation difficulties have been involved in multiple forms of psychopathology and may represent an
Gene-environment interaction important focus for current efforts to understand the biological mechanisms underlying transdiagnostic symp-
Childhood trauma toms. The present study investigated a gene-environment interaction (G × E) in reappraisal, a form of emotion
Emotion regulation regulation that has been extensively linked to psychopathology. In light of recent meta-analytic evidence of its
Psychopathology
consistent role in depression and anxiety disorders, this study focused on the Val66Met (rs6265) single-nu-
cleotide polymorphism in the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) gene and examined its moderator role in
the relation between childhood trauma and reappraisal. A sample of N = 266 participants were genotyped for
BDNF Val66Met, filled in a self-report measure of childhood trauma, and underwent a cognitive task designed to
assess reappraisal ability. The results indicated that, as expected, BDNF Val66Met was a significant moderator in
the relation between childhood trauma and reappraisal. There was a negative relation between the number of
childhood traumatic events and reappraisal ability in BDNF Met carriers, but not Val homozygotes. This finding
suggests that BDNF Val66Met contributes to susceptibility to childhood stress, with long term impact on emotion
regulation.

1. Introduction findings, genetic studies have investigated whether functional poly-


morphisms such as BDNF Val66Met (rs6265) are associated with
Extensive evidence indicates that emotion regulation problems are psychopathology. BDNF Val66Met is a single-nucleotide poly-
implicated in multiple mental disorders [1]. In light of this evidence morphism (SNP) that involves an adenine to guanine substitution in
and considering that recent efforts in psychopathology research have exon 2, and results in a valine (Val) to methionine (Met) replacement
called for an increased focus on transdiagnostic symptoms and their at codon 66 [9]. The Met allele is associated with reduced activity-
underlying biological mechanisms [2], an increasing number of studies dependent secretion of BDNF in neurons [9], and has been associated
have started to investigate gene-environment interactions (G × E) that with multiple neural [10] and cognitive [11] phenotypes. Recent
may contribute to emotion regulation [for review see 3,4]. This work meta-analyses have indicated that there is consistent evidence across
may shed light on the genetic factors moderating the relation between studies on the moderator role of BDNF Val66Met in the relation be-
environmental adversities and emotion regulation difficulties, which tween stressful events and depression [12,13], as well as suicidal
may in turn contribute to risk for psychopathology [5]. behavior [14] and response to antidepressants [15]. These G × E
One of the candidate genes that have been examined in relation to interactions in multiple phenotypes related to psychopathology may
emotion regulation is the brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF). be explained by their underlying effect on a transdiagnostic me-
This gene is widely expressed in the brain and its product molecule chanism such as emotion regulation [5].
plays an important role in neuron survival and neuroplasticity [6]. Several studies have indeed supported the association between
Moreover, the level of serum BDNF is reduced in depression [7] and it BDNF Val66Met and emotion regulation. Using self-report measures of
has been argued that this neurotrophic disturbance may contribute to emotion regulation, it was found that BDNF Met carriers tend to use less
the pathogenesis of stress-related disorders [8]. In light of these efficient, emotion- rather than problem-focused strategies [16], and


Correspondence to: 37 Republicii Street, 400015 Cluj-Napoca, Cluj, Romania.
E-mail address: andreimiu@psychology.ro (A.C. Miu).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.018
Received 29 January 2018; Received in revised form 9 July 2018; Accepted 10 July 2018
Available online 11 July 2018
0304-3940/ © 2018 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
M.I. Bîlc et al. Neuroscience Letters 685 (2018) 7–11

show, for instance, enhanced anxious preoccupation [17] and avoid- experienced each type of event and if they had, the approximate age at
ance [18] in stressful situations. These individuals also display an in- which the event occurred, and they also rated its severity on a 7-point
creased tendency to ruminate [19,20], a maladaptive emotion regula- Likert scale (1 = not at all traumatic, to 7 = extremely traumatic).
tion strategy that has been linked to depression. Importantly, the When other traumatic events were reported, participants described the
highest levels of habitual rumination have been found in BDNF Met event and also rated its traumatic intensity. This measure of childhood
carriers with a history of more stressful events [19], which underscores trauma has been previously related to depressive and anxiety symptoms
the moderator role of this polymorphism in the relation between stress [e.g., 29] and showed sensitivity to G × E interactions relevant for
and emotion regulation. psychopathology [30].
Recent developments in the emotion regulation field have tried to
overcome the limits of self-report measures, which may be increasingly 2.2.3. Reappraisal ability
prone to recall or reporting biases [21]. For instance, cognitive tasks Reappraisal ability was assessed using a computerized cognitive
have been designed in order to assess the ability of participants to task similar to the one used in our previous study [25], and which has
implement a specific emotion regulation strategy such as reappraisal, been extensively employed in previous research [e.g., 31,32]. Briefly,
which involves changing emotional responses by reformulating the participants were presented with emotionally neutral and negative
meaning of a situation [22]. Lower use of reappraisal is known to images (10 s), preceded by one of two instructions (4 s): “Look”, which
characterize psychopathology [23] and it is this emotion regulation cued participants to attend naturally to the subsequent image; and
strategy that is targeted in cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy in de- “Decrease”, which cued them to use reappraisal in order to decrease the
pression and anxiety disorders [24]. Using a cognitive task, Miu et al. emotional impact of the image. Before the task, reappraisal was ex-
[25] have recently reported for the first time that reappraisal ability is plained and illustrated using examples such as finding a positive
reduced in BDNF Met carriers with a history of child maltreatment. meaning (e.g., “happy-end story”), adopting an objective perspective
Provided that it is replicated, this result may have implications for (e.g., “you are a doctor or firefighter”) or detaching from emotional
understanding G × E interactions that influence emotion regulation content (e.g., “the image is not real”). Participants then practiced re-
and may contribute both to vulnerability for psychopathology, and appraisal during 6 learning trials, in which a trained experimenter of-
response to psychotherapy. fered feedback and prompted them with alternative interpretations. The
The present study was designed to replicate and extend, in an in- experimenter conducting the reappraisal task was blind to participants’
dependent sample, the previous BDNF Val66Met × childhood stressful genotype and childhood trauma measures. The actual task included 48
events in emotion regulation. In addition to child maltreatment, other trials: 16 neutral images and 16 negative images preceded by the
types of childhood trauma were also assessed in order to explore the “Look” instruction; and another 16 negative images preceded by the
extent of this effect. As previously [25], a reappraisal ability task was “Decrease” instruction. Order of trials was randomized at the beginning
used to evaluate emotion regulation, but the images used in this study of the experiment and remained the same for all participants. After each
had higher resolution and were chosen to be more realistic. image, participants rated their emotional arousal using a 9-point Likert
scale (1 = low, to 9 = high). Images were selected from the Nencki
2. Material and methods Affective Picture System [33], based on valence and arousal norms.
Neutral stimuli had significantly lower emotional arousal (t[45.72] =
2.1. Participants −20.86, p < .001, d = 5.68) and higher (i.e., midrange) emotional
valence (t[44.77] = 37.28, p < .001, d = 10.5) than negative stimuli.
Two hundred and sixty-six volunteers (218 women), aged 18–44 Emotional arousal and valence scores of negative emotional stimuli in
(M = 20.57, SD = 3.76 years), participated in this study. Participants the Look and Decrease conditions were not significantly different (both
were recruited through campus and online advertisements and they ps > 0.960). Reappraisal ability was estimated as the mean decrease in
were all students at Babeș-Bolyai University. None of the participants negative affect when using reappraisal relative to passive looking (Look
included in this replication sample were part of the prior study [25]. All Negative – Decrease Negative) [32], such that higher scores indicated
participants were Caucasians of European descent and Romanian was greater reappraisal ability or decreases in negative affect due to re-
their first language. The study protocol followed the recommendations appraisal.
of the Declaration of Helsinki regarding participant safety and was
approved by the Ethics Committee of Babeș-Bolyai University. 2.3. Statistical analysis

2.2. Measures In a preliminary analysis, chi-square tests were used to investigate


whether the genotype distribution departed from the Hardy-Weinberg
2.2.1. Genotyping equilibrium [34]. In addition, we checked whether reappraisal was
DNA was extracted from buccal epithelial cells using the MasterPure associated with reduced emotional responses to negative images in the
Complete DNA & RNA Purification Kit (Epicenter, Madison, USA) and task, using repeated measures ANOVA with condition (Look Neutral vs.
kept at −20 °C. BDNF Val66Met genotyping was performed using tetra Look Negative vs. Decrease Negative) as within-subject factor. Where
primer amplified refractory mutation system (ARMS)-PCR method [26], Mauchly’s test indicated that the assumption of sphericity was violated,
as previously described [25]. Considering that the BDNF Met allele is Greenhouse-Geisser corrected degrees of freedom were used.
relatively rare (i.e., approximately 19%) in European samples [27], we The main analysis investigated the interaction between BDNF
aimed at comparing Met carriers (i.e., Val/Met, Met/Met) and Val Val66Met and the number of childhood traumatic events on reappraisal
homozygotes (i.e., Val/Val). ability. We focused on the number rather than perceived severity of
traumatic events in order to avoid any potential overlap between the
2.2.2. Childhood trauma latter and the emotional measures in the reappraisal task [35]. In ad-
The Childhood Traumatic Events Scale [28] was used to investigate dition, we aimed to investigate the global effect of childhood trauma,
the history of traumatic events before age 17: (1) violent events such as rather than that of specific types of traumatic events. In a preliminary
physical abuse, mugging or assault; (2) sexual abuse, such as rape or step, we checked the relation between genotypes and childhood
sexual molestation; (3) major parental conflicts; (4) death of a family trauma, to exclude a potential gene-environment correlation. Then, the
member or a very close person; (5) severe illness or injury; and (6) other G × E interaction was tested using multiple regression analysis. To
traumatic events, which were perceived as a major influence on per- control for the potential confounding effect of sex, both sex and its
sonality and life trajectory. Participants reported whether or not they interactions with genotype and childhood trauma were entered in the

8
M.I. Bîlc et al. Neuroscience Letters 685 (2018) 7–11

regression model [36]. All analyses were run in SPSS (IBM, Armonk, childhood trauma had a significant effect, their interaction was a sig-
NY, USA). nificant predictor of reappraisal ability.
A follow-up analysis, in which the mean severity of traumatic events
3. Results was used as covariate, confirmed that the BDNF Val66Met × childhood
trauma interaction remained significant: B = −0.410; SE(B) = 0.16;
3.1. Genotype distribution p = 0.028; CI: −0.73, −0.08.
Slope analysis on the relation between childhood trauma and re-
Frequencies of the BDNF Val66Met genotypes in the present sample appraisal ability in each genotype group indicated a significant negative
were as follows: 162 Val homozygotes (Val/Val); 90 heterozygotes association between childhood trauma and reappraisal ability in BDNF
(Val/Met); and 14 Met homozygotes (Met/Met). These genotypes were Met carriers (B = −0.304; SE(B) = 0.12; p = 0.019; CI: −0.56,
in Hardy Weinberg equilibrium (χ2 = 0.11, p > .05). Considering the −0.04), but not Val homozygotes (B = 0.124; SE(B) = 0.09;
small frequency of the BDNF Met allele (minor allele frequency p = 0.205; CI: −0.06, 0.31) (Supplementary Fig. 1).
q = 0.22), Met carriers (i.e., Val/Met and Met/Met) were compared to
Val homozygotes in all subsequent analyses. 4. Discussion

3.2. Childhood traumatic events The results of the present study indicate that BDNF Val66Met is a
moderator in the relation between childhood trauma and reappraisal
Participants reported between zero and six traumatic events. While ability. As expected based on previous evidence, BDNF Met carriers, but
19.18% of participants reported no childhood trauma, the rest of par- not Val homozygotes showed a negative association between the
ticipants reported one (38.72%), two (21.80%), three (12.79%), four number of childhood traumatic events and efficiency of reappraisal.
(5.64%), five (1.50%) or six (0.37%) traumatic events. Given the lower This study replicates our previous findings [25], which have shown
number of reports of more than three traumatic events, they were for the first time the interactive involvement of BDNF Val66Met and
collapsed in the same category. Therefore, participants with zero, one, childhood maltreatment in reappraisal. In both studies, reappraisal was
two, and three or more traumatic events were compared in all sub- assessed using a cognitive task that has shown sensitivity to depressive
sequent analyses. Details on the specific types of traumatic events that symptoms [37]. In the present study, as well as in previous research
were reported, their perceived traumatic intensity and the self-reported [25,38], childhood trauma was not directly linked to reappraisal
mean age at the time of trauma are described in the Supplementary ability. That is, only BDNF Met carriers with a history of childhood
Material. trauma showed poor reappraisal efficiency. This may be explained by
the distal influence of childhood trauma, which may sensitize to sub-
3.3. Reappraisal ability sequent stressful events in the long term and may contribute to emo-
tional problems when acting in tandem with genetic factors [39,40]. To
A repeated measures ANOVA indicated significant differences in date, the BDNF Val66Met × childhood stress in reappraisal ability has
emotional arousal between experimental conditions (F[1.95, been replicated in two independent samples [25; the present study] and
515.78] = 537.73, p < .001, ƞP2 = .67). Bonferroni post-hoc tests showed considerable statistical robustness given that different measures
showed that emotional arousal was significantly reduced in the of childhood stressful events and different sets of affective stimuli were
Decrease Negative (M = 3.98, SD = 1.61) compared to the Look employed in the two studies. Notably, the stimuli used in the present
Negative (M = 4.80, SD = 1.61) condition. This confirmed that re- study have higher resolution compared to those used in our previous
appraisal of negative images reduced their emotional impact. As ex- work, and may have been perceived as more realistic [33]. The present
pected, emotional arousal was lowest in the Look Neutral condition findings on reappraisal are also in line with previous evidence on BDNF
(M = 2.24, SD = 0.94). Val66Met × stress in rumination [19] and less efficient emotion-fo-
cused coping [17,18].
3.4. Gene-environment interaction Having assessed a diverse array of stressful events, this study ex-
tends the previous evidence by showing that childhood trauma in-
Table 1 shows the distribution of BDNF Val66Met genotypes by cluding sexual and physical abuse, but also more common events such
number of childhood traumatic events. The correlation between geno- as loss of a close person, parental conflicts, and severe illness or injury
types and childhood trauma was not significant (r = 0.053, p = .387), in childhood, interact with BDNF Val66Met to influence emotion reg-
ruling out a potential gene-environment correlation. ulation. This is in line with previous work on psychopathology [e.g.,
The genotype × childhood trauma events in reappraisal ability was 29,41], which has suggested that both interpersonal (i.e., involving
investigated in a multiple regression model, in which BDNF Val66Met, intentional harm, such as childhood abuse and neglect) and non-in-
the number of childhood traumatic events and sex (women coded with terpersonal (e.g., death of someone close, disease) trauma are asso-
0; men coded with 1) were entered as predictors in the first step, and ciated with internalizing symptoms.
their interactions were added in the second step (Table 2). Results in- The finding that BDNF Met carriers may be more sensitive to early
dicated a negative relation between sex and reappraisal ability, with stressful events, with long term effects on emotion regulation, could be
higher performance in women. While neither BDNF Val66Met, nor explained by the role of the BDNF molecule in neural development and
neuroplasticity [6]. The Met allele is associated with reduced activity-
dependent secretion of BDNF [9], which may enhance the stress vul-
Table 1 nerability of developing neural structures involved in emotion regula-
BDNF Val66Met genotypes by number of childhood traumatic events. tion, including emotional brain hubs such as the hippocampus, amyg-
Number of childhood traumatic events dala and prefrontal cortex [42]. Reappraisal ability has been
consistently associated with activity in these brain areas [31,32] and
0 1 2 3+ the present focus on this measure may have contributed to the success
of capturing the interaction between BDNF Val66Met and childhood
BDNF Genotypes Met carriers 15 44 23 22
Val homozygotes 36 59 35 32 trauma. Indeed, reappraisal ability may represent an intermediate
phenotype that is more sensitive to G × E interactions than more global
Note: 3+ indicates three or more childhood traumatic events. symptoms or psychiatric diagnostic categories [43,44]. In addition, this
intermediate phenotype may show transdiagnostic value considering

9
M.I. Bîlc et al. Neuroscience Letters 685 (2018) 7–11

Table 2
Regression coefficients for the effects of BNDF Val66Met and childhood trauma on reappraisal ability.
Model B SE (B) Beta Sigma (p) R2-change

Step 1 BDNF Val66met 0.264 0.163 0.098 0.106 0.041


Number of childhood traumatic events −0.007 0.079 −0.005 0.933
Sex (0 = women; 1 = men) −0.620 0.208 −0.182 0.003
Step 2 BDNF Val66Met × Number of childhood traumatic events −0.454 0.181 −0.213 0.013 0.034
BDNF Val66Met × Sex −0.381 0.425 −0.078 0.371
Number of childhood traumatic events × Sex 0.177 0.238 0.064 0.457
BDNF Val66Met × Number of childhood traumatic events × Sex 0.263 0.401 0.058 0.512

Note: B = unstandardized regression coefficient; SE (B) = standard error ; Beta = standardized regression coefficient; R2 = proportion of variance explained.

that difficulties with reappraisal characterize multiple mental disorders [5] K.C. Fernandez, H. Jazaieri, J.J. Gross, Emotion regulation: a transdiagnostic per-
[1,23]. In light of the present findings, BDNF Met carriers may carry the spective on a new RDoC domain, Cogn. Ther. Res. 40 (2016) 426–440.
[6] B. Lu, P.T. Pang, N.H. Woo, The yin and yang of neurotrophin action, Nat. Rev.
highest risk of emotion regulation difficulties following early stress, but Neurosci. 6 (2005) 603–614.
they may also show the highest sensitivity to interventions targeting [7] S. Sen, R.S. Duman, G. Sanacora, Meta-analysis of serum brain-derived neuro-
reappraisal, such as cognitive-behavioral psychotherapy [24]. Indeed, trophic factor (BDNF) levels in depression and antidepressant treatment, Biol.
Psychiatry 63 (2008) 527–532.
our previous study has suggested that BDNF Met carriers can show both [8] R.S. Duman, L.M. Monteggia, A neurotrophic model for stress-related mood dis-
the lowest and highest level of reappraisal ability, depending on whe- orders, Biol. Psychiatry 59 (2006) 1116–1127.
ther they were exposed to major stressful events in childhood. Further [9] M.F. Egan, M. Kojima, J.H. Callicott, T.E. Goldberg, B.S. Kolachana, A. Bertolino,
E. Zaitsev, B. Gold, D. Goldman, M. Dean, B. Lu, D.R. Weinberger, The BDNF
research on BDNF Val66Met as a “plasticity gene” [45], which could be val66met polymorphism affects activity-dependent secretion of BDNF and human
involved in the response to psychotherapeutic interventions [46], is memory and hippocampal function, Cell 112 (2003) 257–269.
warranted in the future. [10] K.G. Bath, F.S. Lee, Variant BDNF (VAl66Met) impact on brain structure and
function, Cogn. Affect Behav. Neurosci. 6 (2006) 79–85.
We acknowledge that the sample size in this study is not ideal for
[11] I. Dincheva, C.E. Glatt, F.S. Lee, Impact of the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism on
detecting the typically small effects of genetic polymorphisms. cognition: implications for behavioral genetics, Neuroscientist 18 (2012) 439–451.
However, having used a performance-based measure of emotion reg- [12] G.M. Hosang, C. Shiles, K.E. Tansey, P. McGuffin, R. Uher, Interaction between
ulation, which is more time consuming than self-report questionnaires, stress and the BDNF Val66Met polymorphism in depression: a systematic review
and meta-analysis, BMC Med. 12 (2014).
precluded us from of recruiting a larger sample in this study. In defense [13] M. Zhao, L. Chen, J. Yang, D. Han, D. Fang, X. Qiu, X. Yang, Z. Qiao, J. Ma, L. Wang,
of the present results, we argue that the BDNF Val66Met × childhood S. Jiang, X. Song, J. Zhou, J. Zhang, M. Chen, D. Qi, Y. Yang, H. Pan, BDNF
trauma was replicated in two independent samples [25; the present Val66Met polymorphism, life stress and depression: a meta-analysis of gene-en-
vironment interaction, J. Affect. Disord. 227 (2017) 226–235.
study]. Moreover, this G × E interaction has been consistently linked to [14] T.B. Gonzalez-Castro, M. Salas-Magana, I.E. Juarez-Rojop, M.L. Lopez-Narvaez,
depression in recent meta-analyses [12,13]. Finally, given that the C.A. Tovilla-Zarate, Y. Hernandez-Diaz, Exploring the association between BDNF
present measure of reappraisal ability was based on self-report ratings Val66Met polymorphism and suicidal behavior: meta-analysis and systematic re-
view, J. Psychiatr. Res. 94 (2017) 208–217.
of emotional arousal, which could be susceptible to desirability biases, [15] Y.F. Zou, D.Q. Ye, X.L. Feng, H. Su, F.M. Pan, F.F. Liao, Meta-analysis of BDNF
future studies could include a physiological correlate of emotional Val66Met polymorphism association with treatment response in patients with
arousal [37]. major depressive disorder, Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 20 (2010) 535–544.
[16] S. Aizawa, Y. Ishitobi, K. Masuda, A. Inoue, H. Oshita, H. Hirakawa, T. Ninomiya,
Y. Maruyama, Y. Tanaka, K. Okamoto, C. Kawashima, M. Nakanishi, H. Higuma,
5. Conclusion M. Kanehisa, J. Akiyoshi, Genetic association of the transcription of neuroplasticity-
related genes and variation in stress-coping style, Brain Behav. 5 (2015).
[17] M.J. Koh, H.C. Jeung, K. Namkoong, H.C. Chung, J.I. Kang, Influence of the BDNF
In conclusion, the present results suggest that BDNF Met carriers
Val66Met polymorphism on coping response to stress in patients with advanced
may be particularly vulnerable to childhood trauma, showing a dose- gastric cancer, J. Psychosom. Res. 77 (2014) 76–80.
response effect of the number of early stressful events on emotion [18] L. Ambrus, L. Traskman-Bendz, A. Westrin, C. Sunnqvist, A. Ekman, P. Suchankova,
regulation ability. Associations between avoidant focused coping strategies and polymorphisms in
genes coding for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and vascular endothelial growth
factor in suicide attempters: a preliminary study, Psychiatry Res. 220 (2014)
Acknowledgements 732–733.
[19] P.C. Clasen, T.T. Wells, V.S. Knopik, J.E. McGeary, C.G. Beevers, 5-HTTLPR and
BDNF Val66Met polymorphisms moderate effects of stress on rumination, Genes
This work was supported by the Romanian National Authority for Brain Behav. 10 (2011) 740–746.
Scientific Research, CNCS – UEFISCDI (grant number PN-III- ID-PCE- [20] C.A. Van Hulle, S. Clifford, M.N. Moore, K. Lemery-Chalfant, H.H. Goldsmith,
2016-4- 0864). Partial replication of two rumination-related candidate gene studies, Cogn. Emot.
31 (2017) 963–971.
[21] J.E. Arndt, W.L.G. Hoglund, E. Fujiwara, Desirable responding mediates the re-
Appendix A. Supplementary data lationship between emotion regulation and anxiety, Pers. Individ. Differ. 55 (2013)
147–151.
[22] J.J. Gross, Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations, in: J.J. Gross
Supplementary material related to this article can be found, in the (Ed.), Handbook of Emotion Regulation, Guilford Press, New York, NY, 2014, pp.
online version, at doi:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.018. 3–20.
[23] A. Aldao, S. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Schweizer, Emotion-regulation strategies across
psychopathology: a meta-analytic review, Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30 (2010) 217–237.
References
[24] P.R. Goldin, M. Ziv, H. Jazaieri, K. Werner, H. Kraemer, R.G. Heimberg, J.J. Gross,
Cognitive reappraisal self-efficacy mediates the effects of individual cognitive-be-
[1] J.J. Gross, H. Jazaieri, Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: an af- havioral therapy for social anxiety disorder, J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 80 (2012)
fective science perspective, Clin. Psychol. Sci. 2 (2014) 387–401. 1034–1040.
[2] T. Insel, B. Cuthbert, M. Garvey, R. Heinssen, D.S. Pine, K. Quinn, C. Sanislow, [25] A.C. Miu, M. Carnuta, R. Vulturar, R.D. Szekely-Copindean, M.I. Bilc, A. Chis,
P. Wang, research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework M. Cioara, K.C. Fernandez, A. Szentagotai-Tatar, J.J. Gross, BDNF Val66Met poly-
for research on mental disorders, Am. J. Psychiatry 167 (2010) 748–751. morphism moderates the link between child maltreatment and reappraisal ability,
[3] T. Canli, J. Ferri, E.A. Duman, Genetics of emotion regulation, Neuroscience 164 Genes Brain Behav. 16 (2017) 419–426.
(2009) 43–54. [26] H.I. Sheikh, E.P. Hayden, K.R. Kryski, H.J. Smith, S.M. Singh, Genotyping the BDNF
[4] A.C. Miu, M.I. Bilc, Genetics of emotion regulation: a systematic review, in: A.C. rs6265 (val66met) polymorphism by one-step amplified refractory mutation system
Miu, J.R. Homberg, K.P. Lesch (Eds.), Genes, Brain and Emotions: Interdisciplinary PCR, Psychiatr. Genet. 20 (2010) 109–112.
and Translational Perspectives, Oxford University Press, Oxford, in press. [27] R. Vulturar, A. Chis, M. Hambrich, B. Kelemen, L. Ungureanu, A.C. Miu, Allelic

10
M.I. Bîlc et al. Neuroscience Letters 685 (2018) 7–11

distribution of BDNF Val66Met polymorphism in healthy Romanian volunteers, cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and de-
Transl. Neurosci. 7 (2016) 31–34. pressive symptoms, Emotion 10 (2010) 783–795.
[28] J.W. Pennebaker, J.R. Susman, Disclosure of traumas and psychosomatic processes, [38] K.A. McLaughlin, M. Peverill, A.L. Gold, S. Alves, M.A. Sheridan, Child maltreat-
Soc. Sci. Med. 26 (1988) 327–332. ment and neural systems underlying emotion regulation, J. Am. Acad. Child
[29] A.C. Miu, M.I. Bilc, L. Bunea, A. Szentagotai-Tatar, Childhood trauma and sensi- Adolesc. Psychiatry 54 (2015) 753–762.
tivity to reward and punishment: implications for depressive and anxiety symptoms, [39] C. Hammen, R. Henry, S.E. Daley, Depression and sensitization to stressors among
Pers. Individ. Differ. 119 (2017) 134–140. young women as a function of childhood adversity, J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 68
[30] A. Szentagotai-Tatar, A. Chis, R. Vulturar, A. Dobrean, D.M. Candea, A.C. Miu, (2000) 782–787.
Shame and guilt-proneness in adolescents: gene-environment interactions, PLoS [40] K.A. McLaughlin, K.J. Conron, K.C. Koenen, S.E. Gilman, Childhood adversity, adult
One 10 (2015). stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: a test of the stress
[31] P.R. Goldin, T. Manber-Ball, K. Werner, R. Heimberg, J.J. Gross, Neural mechan- sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults, Psychol. Med. 40
isms of cognitive reappraisal of negative self-beliefs in social anxiety disorder, Biol. (2010) 1647–1658.
Psychiatry 66 (2009) 1091–1099. [41] E. Alisic, A.K. Zalta, F. van Wesel, S.E. Larsen, G.S. Hafstad, K. Hassanpour,
[32] K.N. Ochsner, R.D. Ray, J.C. Cooper, E.R. Robertson, S. Chopra, J.D. Gabrieli, G.E. Smid, Rates of post-traumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed children and
J.J. Gross, For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- adolescents: meta-analysis, Br. J. Psychiatry 204 (2014) 335–340.
and up-regulation of negative emotion, Neuroimage 23 (2004) 483–499. [42] S.J. Lupien, B.S. McEwen, M.R. Gunnar, C. Heim, Effects of stress throughout the
[33] A. Marchewka, L. Zurawski, K. Jednorog, A. Grabowska, The Nencki Affective lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10 (2009)
Picture System (NAPS): introduction to a novel, standardized, wide-range, high- 434–445.
quality, realistic Picture database, Behav. Res. Methods 46 (2014) 596–610. [43] A. Caspi, A.R. Hariri, A. Holmes, R. Uher, T.E. Moffitt, Genetic sensitivity to the
[34] S. Rodriguez, T.R. Gaunt, I.N. Day, Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium testing of biolo- environment: the case of the serotonin transporter gene and its implications for
gical ascertainment for Mendelian randomization studies, Am. J. Epidemiol. 169 studying complex diseases and traits, Am. J. Psychiatry 167 (2010) 509–527.
(2009) 505–514. [44] I.I. Gottesman, T.D. Gould, The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology
[35] D. Cicchetti, S.L. Toth, Child Maltreatment, M.E. Lamb, R.M. Lerner (Eds.), and strategic intentions, Am. J. Psychiatry 160 (2003) 636–645.
Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science, vol. 3, Wiley, New [45] J. Belsky, C. Jonassaint, M. Pluess, M. Stanton, B. Brummett, R. Williams,
Jersey, 2015, pp. 1–51. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol Psychiatry 14 (2009) 746–754.
[36] M.C. Keller, Gene x environment interaction studies have not properly controlled [46] K.J. Lester, T.C. Eley, Therapygenetics: using genetic markers to predict response to
for potential confounders: the problem and the (simple) solution, Biol. Psychiatry psychological treatment for mood and anxiety disorders, Biol. Mood Anxiety
75 (2014) 18–24. Disord. 3 (2013) 4.
[37] A.S. Troy, F.H. Wilhelm, A.J. Shallcross, I.B. Mauss, Seeing the silver lining:

11

You might also like