You are on page 1of 15

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/7859518

Parenting stress in mothers of children with an intellectual disability: The


effects of parental cognition in relation to child characteristics and family
support

Article  in  Journal of Intellectual Disability Research · July 2005


DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00673.x · Source: PubMed

CITATIONS READS

408 4,786

3 authors, including:

Richard Hassall John Rose


The University of Sheffield University of Birmingham
14 PUBLICATIONS   425 CITATIONS    162 PUBLICATIONS   3,639 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Developing Qualitative research with prople with Intellectual Disabilities View project

Working with Staff View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Richard Hassall on 25 April 2019.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Journal of Intellectual Disability Research doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00673.x
405
    pp  –   

Blackwell Science, LtdOxford, UKJIRJournal of Intellectual Disability Research-Blackwell Publishing Ltd, 6405418Original ArticleParental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities R. Hassall et al.

Parenting stress in mothers of children with an


intellectual disability: the effects of parental cognitions in
relation to child characteristics and family support
R. Hassall,1 J. Rose2 & J. McDonald1
1 Oxfordshire Learning Disability NHS Trust, Oxford, UK
2 School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Birmingham, UK

Abstract analysis. The results indicated that most of the vari-


ance in parenting stress was explained by parental
Background Recent theories of stress and coping in
locus of control, parenting satisfaction and child
parents of children with intellectual disabilities (ID)
behaviour difficulties. Whilst there was also a strong
emphasize the importance of cognitive appraisals in
correlation between family support and parenting
influencing parents’ levels of stress and their adapta-
stress, this was mediated by parental locus of control.
tions to difficulties presented by the children. This
Conclusions The results demonstrate the potential
study investigated the relationships between parental
importance of parental cognitions in influencing
cognitions, child characteristics, family support and
parental stress levels. It is argued that these results
parenting stress. The aspects of cognitions studied
have implications for clinical interventions for pro-
were: parenting self-esteem (including efficacy and
moting parents’ coping strategies in managing chil-
satisfaction) and parental locus of control.
dren with ID and behavioural difficulties.
Methods The group studied consisted of  mothers
of children with ID. The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Keywords behaviour problems, family support,
Scales and Maladaptive Behavior Domain were intellectual disability, parental locus of control,
administered by interview. Mothers also completed parenting satisfaction, stress
four questionnaires: the Family Support Scale, the
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale, a shortened
form of the Parental Locus of Control Scale and the Introduction
Parenting Stress Index (Short Form).
Many studies have demonstrated that parents of a
Results Data were analysed using Pearson’s correla-
child with an intellectual disability (ID) are likely to
tion coefficients, partial correlations and a regression
experience significantly higher levels of parenting
stress than are parents of nondisabled children (e.g.
Rodrigue et al. ; Dyson , ; Roach et al.
Correspondence: Richard Hassall, Psychology Department,
Oxfordshire Learning Disability NHS Trust, Slade House,
). However despite these broad findings, it is also
Horspath Driftway, Headington, Oxford OX JH, UK acknowledged that parents of children with a disabil-
(e-mail: Richard.Hassall@oldt.nhs.uk). ity vary considerably in the levels of stress they expe-
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
406
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

rience and that their stress levels are associated with tions are associated with parental stress, affect and
a wide range of variables (e.g. Frey et al. ; Quine behaviour, and how these outcomes might be related
& Pahl ; Baxter et al. ). A few studies have to their children’s behaviour. Research in this area
found that parenting stress is associated with the suggests there may be some potential for addressing
severity of the child’s ID (e.g. Minnes ), these cognitions as an adjunct to clinical interven-
although others have failed to find any similar asso- tions involving behavioural parent training for chil-
ciations (e.g. Beckman ; Walker et al. ). dren’s behaviour problems (Johnston ; Coleman
Generally, it seems that specific characteristics asso- & Karraker ; Bugental & Johnston ).
ciated with the disability are more important corre- Mash & Johnston () proposed a model which
lates of parental stress (Minnes ). These include includes three categories of variables, conceptualized
the child’s communication skills (Frey et al. ) as ‘child characteristics’, ‘parent characteristics’ and
and particularly the level of the child’s behavioural ‘environmental characteristics’. These may interact in
difficulties (e.g. Friedrich et al. ; Konstantareas various ways to influence parent–child interactive
& Homatidis ; Quine & Pahl ). stress. Similarly these variables may be influenced
In addition, social support in the form of marital reciprocally by parenting stress. Applying this model
happiness and family social climate has been found to parents of hyperactive children, Mash & Johnston
to be associated with more effective coping strategies () suggest that parent–child interactive stress
(Friedrich et al. ). Other forms of social support, may arise from a combination of difficult child char-
including social networks, are also associated with acteristics and parental cognitions. Although they do
reduced stress levels in parents (e.g. Frey et al. ; not explicitly apply the model to parents of children
Krauss ). with an ID, the main features of it correspond closely
Researchers have increasingly adopted the view with other multidimensional approaches to the study
that families with a disabled child are not generally of stress in parents of children with disabilities (e.g.
characterized by high levels of pathology and that Frey et al. ).
families may employ various strategies to adapt suc- Parental cognitive processes play a key role in the
cessfully to the demands posed by the child (Hodapp Mash and Johnston model, as well as in other models
; Stoneman ). The cognitive model of stress of parenting stress. Recent reviews of research (e.g.
and coping (Lazarus & Folkman ) has frequently Coleman & Karraker ; Bugental & Johnston
served as the basis for research into parental stress ) have also highlighted various forms of parental
and coping in families of children with disabilities cognition as being potentially capable of predicting
(Frey et al. ; Quine & Pahl ; Sloper et al. parent and child outcomes, although relatively few
; Miller et al. ; Sloper & Turner ; Grant such studies have looked at parents of children with
& Whittell ). A distinctive feature of this model disabilities. Some studies have focused on parental
is the importance assigned to the individual’s self-esteem (Johnston & Mash ; Coleman & Kar-
appraisal of the stressor in affecting her or his adap- raker ), whilst others have focused on parental
tation to a stressful situation. For example, Quine & locus of control (e.g. Roberts et al. ; Hagekull
Pahl () in a study of  mothers reported that et al. ).
the mothers’ cognitive appraisals, particularly their
adjustment to and acceptance of the child, acted as
Parental self-esteem
variables mediating the influence of the child’s behav-
iour problems on consequent parenting stress. The concept of parental self-esteem or efficacy rep-
In parallel with this interest in the effects of cogni- resents the degree to which parents view themselves
tive processes on the adaptation of parents of children as effective in their parenting role. This is frequently
with disabilities, there has been increasing recogni- measured using the Parenting Sense of Competence
tion of the relationships between parental cognitions Scale (PSOC) (Johnston & Mash ), which com-
and various parent and child outcomes for children prises two subscales measuring ‘parenting satisfac-
in general (Grusec & Mammone ; Coleman & tion’ and ‘parenting efficacy’, respectively, and which
Karraker ; Bugental & Johnston ). Typi- provides a composite score referred to as ‘parental
cally, researchers have studied how parental cogni- self-esteem’. In a study using the PSOC with 
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
407
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

mothers and  fathers of school-age children, Ohan Some studies (e.g. Friedrich et al. ; Frey et al.
et al. () found negative correlations for both ; Wiggs & Stores ) have used general locus
mothers and fathers between parenting satisfaction of control measures, such as Rotter’s () scale,
scores and their children’s externalizing and internal- with parents of children with ID. Generally, the
izing behaviour problems. For parenting efficacy, no results of these show that low personal control is
association with child problems was found for moth- associated with higher parenting stress and psycho-
ers and only a weak association was evident for logical distress. In one study, successful behavioural
fathers. treatment for sleep disorders in their children was
Other studies of parenting efficacy and self-esteem associated with a greater sense of internal control in
have been conducted with mothers of infants in the the mothers (Wiggs & Stores ).
first year of life (Teti & Gelfand ), parents of
hyperactive children (Mash & Johnston ;
The present study
Johnston & Patenaude ) and parents of children
with diabetes (Rodrigue et al. ). There have been The main aim of the present study was to test the
some studies of self-efficacy in parents of children Mash & Johnston () model of parenting stress
with ID which indicate that this may be predictive of with mothers of children with an ID, and specifically
parental stress (e.g. Friedrich et al. ; Frey et al. to examine the effects of parental cognitions on
), although this is typically measured by individ- parenting stress. The particular aspects of parental
ually tailored rating scales with little supporting psy- cognitions to be examined were parental self-esteem
chometric data. One recent study found that self- and locus of control. A further aim was to assess the
efficacy mediated the effects of child behaviour prob- relative associations between these variables and
lems on anxiety and depression in mothers of chil- parenting stress, compared with the effects of other
dren with autism (Hastings & Brown ). There variables reflecting child and environmental charac-
appear to be few published studies of parenting effi- teristics as predicted by the Mash and Johnston
cacy in parents of children with disabilities using the model. In view of the range of variables which could
PSOC. be considered, particularly in relation to environmen-
tal characteristics (e.g. Frey et al. ), this was con-
sidered to be an exploratory study of the possible
Parental locus of control
contributions to parenting stress of different types of
The Parental Locus of Control Scale (PLOC) was variable. A measure of child behaviour difficulties was
developed by Campis et al. (), based on the con- therefore selected to reflect child characteristics, and
cept of generalized locus of control (Rotter ). a measure of family and social support was chosen to
They found that parents of children with behaviour reflect environmental characteristics.
difficulties had a more external locus of parenting Several hypotheses were formulated for this study.
control on the PLOC, compared with parents of chil- The first two were intended to replicate findings from
dren without identified problems. Subsequent previous research in relation to the effects of child
research with the PLOC has shown that mothers of and environmental characteristics, whilst the remain-
children with behavioural difficulties who success- ing hypotheses were concerned with the effects of
fully completed a parent training programme demon- parental cognitions on parenting stress. The study
strated a more internal locus of control following the also allowed for an exploration of possible relation-
programme (Roberts et al. ). In a longitudinal ships between the severity of the child’s disability and
study, using the Parental Control subscale of the parenting stress within the group of mothers studied,
PLOC, Hagekull et al. () found that parents although no specific hypotheses were formed because
reporting more external control tended to have chil- of contrasting results in previous studies of this
dren with more behavioural difficulties. They also relationship.
found that the Parental Control assessments for par- The following specific hypotheses were therefore
ents when their children were  months old pre- examined:
dicted externalizing behaviour problems in the  Mothers of children with an ID and higher lev-
children at  years of age. els of behavioural difficulties would experience
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
408
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

higher levels of parenting stress compared with abilities ranged from  to  years old, with a mean
those of disabled children with fewer behavioural of . and standard deviation of .. Of these chil-
difficulties. dren,  were boys and  were girls. The adaptive
 Mothers who reported higher levels of social sup- behaviours of the children were measured using the
port would experience lower levels of parenting Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Survey Form)
stress. and the Adaptive Behavior Composite scores for the
 Levels of parenting stress in mothers would be children ranged from  to , with a mean of .
inversely related to their sense of parenting effective- and standard deviation of ..
ness and satisfaction. Specifically, higher levels of Nearly all the mothers spoke English as their first
perceived effectiveness and satisfaction would be language and were of white European ethnic back-
associated with lower stress levels. ground, reflecting the predominant ethnic mix in
 Mothers with a more external locus of control for the area. The only exception was one woman origi-
parenting would display higher levels of parenting nating from northern Europe who spoke English
stress. fluently. Mothers of other ethnic groups were sent
In addition, it was intended to explore relationships details about the research, but none volunteered to
between these parental cognitive variables, and to take part. Of the  in the final sample,  were
examine the extent of any measurement overlap married, engaged or cohabiting with a long-term
between them. The study was also designed to permit partner. The remaining seven were divorced, sepa-
the exploration of any mediating effects shown by rated or single.
these variables, although specific hypotheses were dif- The scores for total stress on the Parenting Stress
ficult to formulate due to the limited number of pub- Index (PSI) (Short Form) of the sample group
lished studies in this area. ranged from  to , with a mean of . and a
standard deviation of .. The normative sample for
the PSI (Short Form) reported by Abidin ()
Method consisted of  participants obtaining scores with a
mean of  and standard deviation of .. A t-test
Participants
showed that the mean for the present sample was
The participants were mothers of children with an significantly higher than for the normative sample
ID who were recruited through six special schools (t = .; d.f. = ; P < .), suggesting that these
serving a predominantly rural middle-class area in parents were on average experiencing higher stress
southeast England. Head-teachers at the schools levels than were the normative sample.
contacted a total of  mothers and sent each of
them a leaflet about the research, a letter inviting
them to participate, a consent form and a stamped Measures
addressed envelope. Of these,  returned the con-
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales (Survey Form)
sent form agreeing to take part. Full data sets could
(Sparrow et al. )
not be obtained for six of the mothers for various
reasons. Two could not participate because of per- This was used to measure the ability levels of the
sonal or family health problems, one was never children in relation to their adaptive behaviour skills.
present at the prearranged times when the For children of  years and older, three domains of
researcher visited, two could not be contacted to skill, labelled, respectively, Communication, Daily
arrange interviews and one participated in the inter- Living Skills and Socialization, are assessed and stan-
views but did not return the other questionnaires dard scores are calculated for each domain. The over-
despite several requests. This left  participants for all level of adaptive skills of each child is measured
whom full data sets were obtained and on whom the by the Adaptive Behavior Composite, derived from
subsequent analysis is based. the sum of the standard scores on the three domains.
The ages of mothers in the sample ranged from  Evidence regarding the reliability and validity of the
to  years old, with a mean of . and standard scales is summarized in the Vineland manual (Spar-
deviation of .. The ages of the children with dis- row et al. ).
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
409
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

Vineland Maladaptive Behavior Domain (Sparrow comprises  items, Johnston and Mash () found
et al. ) that one item (‘Being a good mother/father is a
reward in itself’) did not load strongly on either of
The Vineland Maladaptive Behavior Domain (VINE-
the two identified factors. This item was therefore
MAL) was used as a measure of each child’s level of
omitted from the scale in this study.
behavioural difficulties. Simple raw scores on this
checklist are used in the analysis of the data. Evidence Parental Locus of Control Scale (Short Form Revised)
for the reliability and validity of the Maladaptive
A shortened form of the PLOC (Campis et al. )
Behavior Domain is provided by Sparrow et al. ().
was used for this study. The original scale used by
Family Support Scale (Dunst et al. ) Campis et al. contained  items which parents were
asked to rate on a five-point Likert scale ranging from
This scale was used as a measure of the social support ‘strongly disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’. Their factor
available from various sources to the mothers. It pro- analysis on these items produced five factors within
vides an overall score for total level of social support, the scale, which they labelled Parental Efficacy (PE),
as well as five weighted subscale scores covering par- Parental Responsibility (PR), Child Control of Par-
ents’ perceptions of helpfulness of partner/spouse ents’ Life (CC), Fate/Chance and Parental Control
support, informal kinship support, formal kinship of Child’s Behaviour (PC), respectively. These sub-
support, social organizations and professional ser- scales contained an unequal number of items, rang-
vices. In addition, the Family Support Scale (FSS) ing from  to . Campis et al. () reported alpha
provides a measure of the total number of sources of coefficients for internal consistency ranging from
support available to parents. Dunst et al. () . to . for the five subscales and . for the
report an alpha coefficient for internal consistency of total scale. Test–retest reliability for the whole scale
., and coefficients for test–retest reliability over a has been demonstrated with a correlation of .
-month interval of . for the whole scale and . over an interval of between  and  days (Roberts
for the average of the separate items. Discriminant et al. ). Good construct and discriminant validity
and construct validity for the FSS has been demon- for the scale has been demonstrated with parents of
strated in several studies (e.g. Frey et al. ). children with no reported difficulties and parents of
children with behavioural problems (Campis et al.
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale
; Roberts et al. ).
This is a self-report scale, which parents are asked to To reduce the burden on participants in this study,
rate on a six-point Likert scale ranging from ‘strongly it was felt necessary to shorten the original PLOC.
disagree’ to ‘strongly agree’, and which was originally This was carried out by removing one of the original
constructed by Gibaud-Wallston & Wandersman subscales (Fate/Chance) which was found to lack dis-
(). The total score (PSOC-TOT) is used as a criminant validity in the study by Campis et al.
measure of parenting self-esteem. The PSOC also (), who recommended that it should be omitted
provides measures of parenting satisfaction (PSOC- by future researchers. One item (‘My child usually
S) and efficacy (PSOC-E), the two principal factors ends up getting his/her way, so why try’) from the
identified by Johnston & Mash () who confirmed original PE subscale was also omitted, again follow-
the factor structure of the initial test construction. ing a recommendation by Campis et al. (), as this
Johnston and Mash also reported alpha scores for item reduced the alpha coefficient for that subscale
internal consistency of . for the satisfaction factor and was felt to have ambiguous meanings. The
and . for the efficacy factor. Test–retest correla- remaining four subscales (and the remaining items
tions over a -week interval ranging from . to . on the Parental Efficacy subscale) were shortened by
were reported by Gibaud-Wallston & Wandersman selecting the six items with the highest factor loadings
(). Convergent and discriminant validity for the on each subscale in the Campis et al. () study.
PSOC has been reported in a range of studies with This followed a similar procedure adopted by Love-
parents of children with differing disorders (e.g. joy et al. (), who produced a shortened version
Johnston & Mash ; Rodrigue et al. ; Lovejoy of the five-factor PLOC, which they labelled ‘PLOC-
et al. ; Ohan et al. ). Although the full scale SF’. The resultant scale for this study, which used
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
410
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

four subscales and consisted of  items, was labelled Behavior Composite and either PSI-TOT or two
‘PLOC-SFR’. of its subscales, PSI-PD and PSI-DC. There was,
Half of all items in the full scale were reverse however, a small inverse correlation (r = -.,
scored. High scores on the scale indicate an external P = .) between the Adaptive Behavior Composite
locus of parenting control and low scores indicate an and one subscale, PSI-P/CDI. There were similar
internal locus. inverse correlations between PSI-P/CDI and the
Vineland subscales (Communication: r = -.,
Parenting Stress Index (Short Form) P = .; Daily Living Skills: r = -., P = .;
Socialization: r = -., P = .).
The Short Form of the PSI (rd edition) (Abidin
For all the remaining results, the pattern of
) was used as the dependent variable. The Short
correlations of variables with PSI-TOT was similar
Form yields a total score for parenting stress (PSI-
to the pattern of correlations with the individual
TOT) and three subscale scores labelled, respec-
PSI subscales. Therefore, only the correlations
tively, Parental Distress (PD), Parent–Child Dys-
with PSI-TOT will be reported for the following
functional Interaction (P/CDI) and Difficult Child
results.
(DC). Detailed reliability and validity data for the
PSI are reported in the PSI manual (Abidin ).
Hypothesis 1
Procedure The results provided confirmation of an association
For each mother who agreed to participate, the Vine- between children’s behavioural difficulties and
land Adaptive Behavior Scales (including the Mal- parenting stress. There was a significant correlation
adaptive Behavior Domain) were completed by between the VINE-MAL and the PSI-TOT
interview. The other four questionnaires (FSS, (r = ., P = .).
PSOC, PLOC-SFR and PSI) were left with her to fill
in independently and were collected by the inter- Hypothesis 2
viewer subsequently. In a few cases, administration of
the Vineland proved to be very lengthy and an extra In confirmation of the hypothesis that social support
visit a short time later had to be arranged to allow is inversely related to parenting stress in parents, a
this to be completed. number of significant inverse correlations were
obtained between PSI-TOT and the FSS and its sub-
Results scales measuring informal kinship, formal kinship
and social organization supports. These results are
Analysis shown in Table .
The correlation of PSI-TOT with the total scale
The scores on each of the variables in the study were
score of the FSS was stronger than with any of the
found to approximate to a normal distribution on a
FSS subscales. Consequently, all subsequent presen-
one-sample Kolmogorov-Smirnoff test. The hypoth-
tations of results with the FSS will just refer to the
eses were therefore all tested by a correlational anal-
total scale score.
ysis using Pearson’s correlational coefficients, with
the assumption that associations were linear. The
variables that were found to be strongly associated Hypothesis 3
with the total scale score on the PSI were then
entered into a step-wise regression analysis to deter- In confirmation of the hypothesis that parenting
mine which of these accounted for most of the vari- self-esteem would be inversely related to parenting
ance in the dependent variable (PSI-TOT). stress, there were significant inverse correlations
between parenting stress (PSI-TOT) and the PSOC
(see Table ). Of the two subscales of the PSOC,
Parenting stress and children’s adaptive skills
the satisfaction subscale (S) of the PSOC showed
Examining Pearson’s correlation coefficients, there the strongest inverse correlation with parenting
were no significant correlations between the Adaptive stress.
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
411
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

Table 1 Pearson’s correlation coefficients for the FSS and its sub- Table 3 Pearson’s correlation coefficients for the PLOC-SFR and
scales with PSI-TOT its subscales with PSI-TOT

Correlations Correlations

FSS – total -0.485; P = 0.001** PLOC-SFR 0.673; P < 0.001**


FSS – partner/spouse -0.144; NS PE 0.210; NS
FSS – informal kinship -0.397; P = 0.006** PR -0.106; NS
FSS – formal kinship -0.292; P = 0.049* CC 0.713; P < 0.001**
FSS – social organizations -0.411; P = 0.005** PC 0.692; P < 0.001**
FSS – professional services -0.249; NS
FSS – No. of support sources 0.136; NS **P < ..
NS, not significant; PLOC-SFR, Parental Locus of Control Scale
*P < .; **P < .. (Short Form Revised); PE, Parental Efficacy; PR, Parental
NS, not significant; FSS, Family Support Scale; PSI-TOT, total Responsibility; CC, Child Control of Parents’ Life; PC, Parental
score for Parenting Stress Index. Control of Child’s Behaviour; PSI-TOT, total score for Parenting
Stress Index.

Table 2 Pearson’s correlation coefficients for PSOC with PSI-TOT


tions between these variables were examined. Table 
gives the correlation coefficients between these and
Correlations
their respective subscales.
There was a strong inverse correlation between
PSOC-S -0.636; P < 0.001** PLOC-SFR and PSOC-TOT scores, implying that
PSOC-E -0.383; P = 0.009**
PSOC-TOT -0.624; P < 0.001**
mothers with higher levels of parenting self-esteem
were likely to have a more internal locus of parenting
**P < .. control. Similar patterns of correlations were
PSOC, Parenting Sense of Competence Scale; PSOC-S, parenting observed for three of the PLOC-SFR subscales (PE,
satisfaction subscale of PSOC; PSOC-E, parenting efficacy subscale CC and PC), but there was no such association
of PSOC; PSOC-TOT, total score for PSOC; PSI-TOT, total score between the PR subscale and any of the PSOC scales.
for Parenting Stress Index.
As the two subscales of the PSOC are regarded as
being conceptually distinct dimensions of parenting
self-esteem (Johnston & Mash ), it is possible
Hypothesis 4
that they may have different relationships with
This hypothesis stated that mothers with a more parenting stress (PSI-TOT) and may interact differ-
external locus of parenting control would display ently with PLOC-SFR in accounting for the variance
higher levels of parenting stress. The internal consis- in parenting stress. In particular, at least part of the
tency of the PLOC-SFR, which was used to test the effect of the PSOC on parenting stress could be
hypothesis, was checked and an alpha coefficient of accounted for by correlations with PLOC-SFR. To
. was obtained. The results (see Table ) sup- test these possibilities, partial correlations were car-
ported the hypothesis, showing that mothers with an ried out, in which parenting stress was correlated
external locus of control were more likely to experi- with the subscales and total score of the PSOC whilst
ence higher stress levels. controlling for PLOC-SFR.
When PLOC-SFR was controlled, there was no
significant correlation between the efficacy (E) sub-
Relationships between PLOC-SFR, PSOC and
scale of the PSOC and parenting stress (PSI-TOT),
other variables
suggesting that the zero-order correlation between
As both parenting locus of control and parenting self- PSOC-E and PSI-TOT is wholly attributable to the
esteem (including efficacy and satisfaction) are correlation between PSOC-E and PLOC-SFR. How-
strongly correlated with parenting stress, and both ever, there was a strong first-order correlation
represent parental cognitive variables, the correla- between the satisfaction (S) subscale of the PSOC
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
412
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

Table 4 Pearson’s correlation coefficients


PSOC-TOT PSOC-S PSOC-E between PSOC and PLOC-SFR scores

PLOC-SFR -0.674; P < 0.001** -0.529; P < 0.001** -0.602; P < 0.001**
PE -0.492; P = 0.001** -0.335; P = 0.023* -0.500; P < 0.001**
PR -0.026; NS 0.121; NS -0.192; NS
CC -0.514; P < 0.001** -0.567; P < 0.001** -0.267; NS
PC -0.626; P < 0.001** -0.473; P = 0.001** -0.580; P < 0.001**

*P < .; **P < ..


NS, not significant; PSOC, Parenting Sense of Competence Scale; PSOC-TOT, total score
for PSOC; PSOC-S, parenting satisfaction subscale of PSOC; PSOC-E, parenting efficacy
subscale of PSOC; PLOC-SFR, Parental Locus of Control Scale (Short Form Revised); PE,
Parental Efficacy; PR, Parental Responsibility; CC, Child Control of Parents’ Life; PC,
Parental Control of Child’s Behaviour.

Table 5 Model summary for regression analysis with PSI-TOT as subscales and the Vineland Adaptive Behavior scores.
dependent variable Therefore, there was no relationship between the
mothers’ locus of parenting control and the level of
Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 ability of their children. There was a small, but sig-
nificant correlation for the VINE-MAL with the
R 0.673* 0.749† 0.787‡ PLOC-SFR (r = ., P = .), and a stronger
R square 0.452 0.561 0.619 correlation with one of the PLOC-SFR subscales,
Adjusted R square 0.440 0.541 0.592 PC, representing parental control over the child’s
R square change 0.452 0.109 0.058
behaviour (r = ., P = .). This suggests that
Significance <0.001 0.002 0.015
mothers of children with higher levels of maladaptive
*Predictors: (Constant), PLOC-SFR.
behaviour were more likely to have an external locus

Predictors: (Constant), PLOC-SFR, PSOC-S. of parenting control. There were no significant corre-

Predictors: (Constant), PLOC-SFR, PSOC-S, VINE-MAL. lations with the other PLOC-SFR subscales.
PSI-TOT, total score for Parenting Stress Index; PLOC-SFR, For the relationship of the PLOC-SFR and the
Parental Locus of Control Scale (Short Form Revised); PSOC,
FSS, there was a significant inverse correlation
Parenting Sense of Competence Scale; PSOC-S, parenting
satisfaction subscale of PSOC; VINE-MAL, Vineland Maladaptive
between these measures (r = -.; P = .), indi-
Behavior Domain. cating that mothers with greater levels of social sup-
port tended also to have a more internal locus of
parenting control. This association was particularly
strong between FSS and the PC subscale of the
and PSI-TOT (r = -.; P = .), suggesting that PLOC-SFR (-.; P < .).
this subscale fully accounted for the correlation
between parenting self-esteem and parenting stress,
Regression analysis
and might therefore contribute some unique variance
in the present model. There was also a significant From the above results, four main variables
first-order correlation between PSOC-TOT and PSI- emerged which were strongly associated with total
TOT (r = -.; P = .). These results suggest parenting stress (PSI-TOT), these being: the VINE-
that there may be some measurement overlap MAL score, the FSS score, the PSOC-S and the
between the efficacy (E) subscale of the PSOC and PLOC-SFR. The criterion for selecting these was
PLOC-SFR. that they all displayed strong correlations with PSI-
Examining the relationship between the PLOC- TOT at a significance level of at least .. The
SFR scores and the Vineland Scales revealed no sig- PSOC-S was selected in preference to the PSOC-
nificant correlations between the PLOC-SFR or its TOT as this accounted for all of the association
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
413
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

between PSOC-TOT and PSI-TOT. These four Table 6 First-order correlations between family support (FSS) and
variables were entered into a step-wise regression parenting stress (PSI-TOT) with other variables controlled

analysis with PSI-TOT as the dependent variable,


with the order of entry being determined by the First-order correlations – Variable
amount of additional variance for which each vari- FSS vs. PSI-TOT controlled
able accounts. The PLOC-SFR was entered first,
followed by PSOC-S and finally VINE-MAL. No R = -0. 251; NS PLOC-SFR
variables were removed at any stage. The model R = -0.453; P = 0.002** PSOC-S
R = -0.381; P = 0.010* VINE-MAL
summary is given in Table . Using the figures for
‘adjusted R square’, parental locus of control
***P < .; P < ..
(PLOC-SFR) accounted for % of the variance in NS, not significant; FSS, Family Support Scale; PSI-TOT, total
parenting stress (PSI-TOT) in model . When score for Parenting Stress Index; PLOC-SFR, Parental Locus of
parenting satisfaction (PSOC-S) was added in Control Scale (Short Form Revised); PSOC, Parenting Sense of
model , % of the variance was accounted for by Competence Scale; PSOC-S, parenting satisfaction subscale of
PSOC; VINE-MAL, Vineland Maladaptive Behavior Domain.
these two variables. Adding child behaviour prob-
lems (VINE-MAL) to these variables accounted for
% of the variance in parenting stress in model .
The results of the regression analysis indicated that parents of children with ID. With regard to child
parental cognitive variables (locus of control and characteristics, there was clear evidence of an associ-
parenting satisfaction) were both significant predic- ation between child behaviour difficulties and parent-
tors of parenting stress and that child behaviour dif- ing stress, which is consistent with the results of
ficulties also made a significant contribution to the previous research (e.g. Quine & Pahl ). However,
variance in parenting stress. However, family support there was little association between the child’s level
did not contribute significantly to explaining any of of skills and parenting stress in the mothers, which is
this variance. Therefore, it was inferred that the pre- consistent with other research in this area (e.g.
viously noted correlation between family support and Walker et al. ).
parenting stress (see Table ) appeared to be medi- The association of environmental characteristics
ated by another intervening variable. To test this with parenting stress is also demonstrated by the
inference, partial correlations were calculated present results showing a strong inverse correlation
between family support (FSS) and parenting stress between the FSS and the PSI. This indicates that
(PSI-TOT) controlling for parental locus of control mothers with greater levels of social support experi-
(PLOC-SFR), parenting satisfaction (PSOC-S) and ence lower levels of parenting stress, and this finding
child behaviour difficulties (VINE-MAL), respec- is consistent with much previous research (e.g. Frey
tively (see Table ). These are compared with the et al. ). This was the only measure of environ-
zero-order correlation of r = -. (P = .) mental characteristics included in the present study,
between the FSS and PSI-TOT. These first-order and it is possible that it includes an element of paren-
correlations indicate that the association between tal characteristics in that it measures parents’ percep-
FSS and PSI-TOT is largely unaffected by PSOC-S tions of the support they receive. Interestingly, the
or by VINE-MAL. However, the correlation between subscale of the FSS measuring the number of sources
FSS and PSI-TOT is greatly reduced, and no longer of support available for mothers showed no correla-
significant, when PLOC-SFR is controlled. tion with parenting stress (nor with any other variable
in this study). The critical variable, therefore, seems
to be the perceived helpfulness of the support
received by mothers rather than the range of supports
Discussion
available.
In accordance with the Mash & Johnston () The present study provided clear evidence of strong
model of parenting stress, the results of this study associations between mothers’ cognitive states and
appeared to demonstrate the importance of all three their parenting stress, in confirmation of the role of
sets of characteristics described by the model for parental characteristics in the Mash & Johnston
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
414
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

() model. This was especially evident for the tion and parental locus of control are conceptually
PSOC and the PLOC-SFR.There were strong inverse more distinct.
correlations between the PSOC and the PSI indicat- The results of the regression analysis and the par-
ing that mothers with higher levels of parenting self- tial correlations between FSS and PSI-TOT further
esteem experienced lower parenting stress. The clarify the manner in which parental cognitive vari-
PSOC dimension for which this was strongest was ables are associated with parenting stress in this
parenting satisfaction (PSOC-S), which is considered group of mothers. The regression analysis showed
to reflect an affective dimension of parenting that parental locus of control and parenting satisfac-
(Johnston & Mash ). The other PSOC dimension tion, together with child behaviour difficulties,
(PSOC-E), reflecting parents’ sense of their efficacy accounted for more than half the variance in parent-
in parenting, demonstrated significant, but weaker, ing stress, and that family support was not a signifi-
inverse correlations with the PSI and its subscales. cant predictor in this model. However, parental locus
There were strong positive correlations between of control and parenting satisfaction appeared to be
the PLOC-SFR and the PSI and its subscales. These associated in different ways with the other variables
indicated that mothers with a more internal locus of in the model. The first-order correlation between FSS
parenting control tended to experience lower levels and PSI-TOT, controlling for parental locus of con-
of parenting stress. This result reflects other findings trol, was small and nonsignificant, implying that
using general locus of control measures which have parental locus of control accounted for most of the
shown that similar groups of parents with more inter- zero-order correlation between these two variables. It
nal control experience lower levels of stress and psy- appears therefore that parental locus of control acts
chological distress (e.g. Friedrich et al. ; Frey as an intervening variable mediating the effect of FSS
et al. ). on PSI-TOT, and hence accounting for the failure of
Two subscales of the PLOC-SFR showed high cor- FSS to contribute significantly to the variance in PSI-
relations with PSI scores, these being CC and PC. TOT in the regression analysis. However, when
The correlations between the remaining two sub- parenting satisfaction was controlled, a significant
scales PE and PR and the PSI subscales were all first-order correlation remained between FSS and
either insignificant or very low. The CC and PC sub- PSI-TOT, indicating that the variance in parenting
scales therefore were those with the greatest value in satisfaction had little effect on this relationship. The
predicting parenting stress in this study, reflecting first order correlations between child behaviour prob-
similar findings regarding the validity of these sub- lems and PSI-TOT also remained significant, when
scales reported by Campis et al. () and Hagekull either parental locus of control or parenting satisfac-
et al. (). tion was controlled, thus confirming the finding of
From the correlational analysis, parental locus of the regression analysis that child behaviour problems
control (PLOC-SFR) and parental self-esteem had a direct association with parenting stress.
(PSOC) emerged as strong predictors of parenting In summary, this study provides evidence that
stress. There was also a strong correlation between parenting stress in mothers of children with ID is
these two variables and their subscales, raising the associated with the level of behavioural difficulties of
possibility of some conceptual overlap between them. their children, their locus of parenting control and
Examining the partial correlations of the PSOC sub- their sense of satisfaction with parenting. Whilst their
scales with PSI-TOT showed that the efficacy sub- stress levels are also inversely correlated with family
scale had no association with parenting stress support, this association appears to be mediated by
independently of the effect of parental locus of con- their locus of parenting control. This result would
trol. This finding is consistent with previous research suggest that the direct effect of family support
reporting some convergent validity between parental appears mainly to be in contributing to a more inter-
locus of control and parenting efficacy (Lovejoy et al. nal locus of control in the mothers. Further research
). In contrast, the satisfaction dimension of the with a larger and more representative sample would
PSOC continued to demonstrate a significant associ- be required to clarify how far this can be generalized.
ation with parenting stress independently of parental However, Beresford () cites earlier research sug-
locus of control, suggesting that parenting satisfac- gesting that social support is viewed as an effective

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
415
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

coping resource by individuals with a more internal perceptions are the principal subject of study. One
locus of control compared with those with an external alternative would be use to a measure of children’s
locus of control. The direction of causality in the behaviour problems based on teacher observations,
present result is therefore unclear. Furthermore, it although not all children who display behavioural
appears that locus of control beliefs may interact with difficulties at home have similar problems at school.
other coping resources and with previous coping Another alternative to relying on self-report for
experiences to influence various outcomes for the parenting stress would be to measure spouses’ per-
parents (Beresford ), and this also would be a ceptions of their partners’ stress levels. However,
fruitful area for further research. this would involve an assumption that spouses have
There may also be more general theoretical an accurate perception of their partners’ experi-
grounds supporting the importance of parental locus ences, and would also be inapplicable for single
of control in this model. Locus of control can be seen parents.
as one expression of Bandura’s () concept of
‘self-efficacy’, which according to Bandura is a cen-
Clinical implications
tral motivating influence in human behaviour, with
effects on other cognitive processes, emotional reac- An area for future research with potential clinical
tions and the behaviour of the individual. implications would be the links between the parental
cognitions analysed above and parents’ coping styles,
following the Lazarus & Folkman () model of
Methodological limitations
cognitive appraisals and coping styles. Previous
There are some methodological constraints in this research with parents of children with disabilities has
study which limit the conclusions that can be drawn. shown that the use of emotion-focused coping styles
First, the absence of a longitudinal design prevented is associated with higher levels of distress and parent-
clear conclusions being reached about the directions ing stress, whereas problem-focused styles are asso-
of the observed associations. Second, the mothers ciated with lower stress levels (Miller et al. ;
were drawn from a largely rural middle-class area and Hastings & Johnson ). It would therefore be
therefore do not adequately reflect the ethnic and valuable to study the relationships between parental
socio-economic diversity of parents in the UK. Addi- locus of control, parenting satisfaction and adaptive
tionally, fathers were not included in the study and coping styles, and to clarify further any mediating
one cannot infer that similar results would be effects between these variables and parental stress
obtained for them. and distress. This might have particular implications
Third, the number of participants was too small to for cognitive interventions with parents aimed at
allow any meaningful analysis of subgroups of moth- ameliorating the stress associated with raising a child
ers, where possible differences in results could with a disability (Weiss ).
emerge. For example, mothers of an only child with Further research could also examine the potential
a disability may experience lower parenting satisfac- buffering effects of positive experiences of caring for
tion and a more external locus of control than do a disabled child on the parents’ emotional state.
mothers with other children without disabilities. There is some recent evidence that mothers’ posi-
However, there were only seven mothers of only chil- tive perceptions of caring for a child with a disability
dren in this study, which is insufficient to establish are associated with reframing coping strategies and
any reliable group differences. Similarly, it was not that the use of social support resources contributes
possible to test for the effects of different syndromes to some aspects of these positive perceptions (Hast-
or disabilities which could account for different levels ings et al. ). These positive aspects of parenting
of parenting stress and differential effects on parental may therefore have significant implications for sup-
cognitions (Hodapp ). porting families of children with ID (Hastings et al.
Finally, all the measures used here depend on ).
parental report and some of the associations found Furthermore, the evidence that adaptive coping
might therefore result from source variance. Never- styles and beliefs in the efficacy of behavioural inter-
theless, this may be difficult to avoid when parents’ ventions are associated with lower parenting stress
©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
416
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

could have implications for how such interventions Health Service (NHS) trusts who allowed time for
are delivered and the nature of the support that par- the preparation of the project and for the research to
ents need when implementing them (Hastings & be undertaken.
Johnson ). Generally it would seem that if par-
ents are to implement interventions for children with
behavioural difficulties, then they need to view these
References
interventions as acceptable (Reimers et al. ) and
to have some beliefs in their potential efficacy and Abidin R. R. () Parenting Stress Index, rd edn. PAR,
appropriateness to the problem (Cross Calvert & Odessa, FL.
Johnston ). Therefore, behavioural interventions Bandura A. () Self-efficacy mechanism in human
for children with challenging behaviours need also to agency. American Psychologist , –.
include components that address the parents’ coping Baxter C., Cummins R. A. & Yiolitis L. () Parental
stress attributed to family members with and without
styles and beliefs about their own parenting capaci-
disability: a longitudinal study. Journal of Intellectual and
ties, when faced with the demands of a child whose Developmental Disability , –.
problems may be attributed to causes viewed as Beckman P. J. () Influence of selected child character-
largely outside parental control. It seems likely that istics on stress in families of handicapped infants. Amer-
the success of such interventions could be enhanced ican Journal of Mental Deficiency , –.
by attention to these issues. Beresford B. A. () Resources and strategies: how par-
Finally, other aspects of parental cognitions and ents cope with the care of a disabled child. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry , –.
beliefs also appear to be important in influencing
parental reactions. There is accumulating evidence of Bugental D. B. & Johnston C. () Parental and child
cognitions in the context of the family. Annual Review of
the importance of parents’ attributions for their chil- Psychology , –.
dren’s behaviour in affecting parental outcomes
Bugental D. B., Johnston C., New M. & Silvester J. ()
(Miller ; Bugental et al. ). Specifically, Measuring parental attributions: conceptual and meth-
mothers who attribute more control to their children odological issues. Journal of Family Psychology , –
for their negative behaviour are more likely to expe- .
rience angry feelings towards their children and to Campis L. K., Lyman R. D. & Prentice-Dunn S. ()
use over-assertive disciplinary practices (e.g. Dix The Parental Locus of Control Scale: development and
validation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology , –.
et al. ; Smith Slep & O’Leary ). Whilst few
Chavira V., Lopez S. R., Blacher J. & Shapiro J. ()
studies have been performed with parents of children
Latina mothers’ attributions, emotions, and reactions to
with ID, one such study found that mothers who the problem behaviors of their children with developmen-
attribute responsibility to their children for behaviour tal disabilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry
problems are more likely to experience negative emo- , –.
tional reactions and to employ harsh disciplinary Coleman P. K. & Karraker K. H. () Self-efficacy and
methods (Chavira et al. ). Further studies of parenting quality: findings and future applications. Devel-
parental attributions with this group of parents could opmental Review , –.
therefore contribute an additional perspective to the Cross Calvert S. & Johnston C. () Acceptability of
treatments for child behavior problems: issues and impli-
understanding of the role of parental cognitions and
cations for future research. Journal of Clinical Child Psy-
to the design of therapeutic interventions for child chology , –.
behaviour problems. Dix T., Ruble D. N. & Zambarano R. J. () Mothers’
implicit theories of discipline: child effects, parent effects,
and the attribution process. Child Development , –
.
Acknowledgements Dunst C. J., Jenkins V. & Trivette C. M. () Family
Support Scale: reliability and validity. Journal of Individ-
The authors would like to thank the parents who
ual, Family, and Community Wellness , –.
agreed to take part in this study. We would also like
Dunst C. J., Trivette C. M. & Hamby D. W. () Mea-
to thank the staff at the schools in Oxfordshire who suring social support in families with young children with
assisted in recruiting participants. Thanks are also disabilities. In: Supporting and Strengthening Families,
due to managers in the two employing National Vol. L: Methods, Strategies, and Practices (eds C. J. Dunst,

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
417
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

C. M. Trivette & A. G. Deal), pp. –. Brookline in Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy (eds K. S. Dobson &
Books, Cambridge. K. D. Craig), pp. –. Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Dyson L. L. () Response to the presence of a child with Johnston C. & Mash E. J. () A measure of parenting
disabilities: parental stress and family functioning over satisfaction and efficacy. Journal of Clinical Child Psychol-
time. American Journal on Mental Retardation , –. ogy , –.
Dyson L. L. () Fathers and mothers of school-age Johnston C. & Patenaude R. () Parent attributions for
children with developmental disabilities: parental stress, inattentive-overactive and oppositional-defiant behaviors.
family functioning, and social support. American Journal Cognitive Therapy and Research , –.
on Mental Retardation , –. Konstantareas M. M. & Homatidis S. () Parental per-
Frey K. S., Greenberg M. T. & Fewell R. R. () Stress ception of learning-disabled children’s adjustment prob-
and coping among parents of handicapped children: a lems and related stress. Journal of Abnormal Child
multi-dimensional approach. American Journal on Mental Psychology , –.
Retardation , –.
Krauss M. W. () Child-related and parenting stress:
Friedrich W. N., Wilturner L. T. & Cohen D. S. () similarities and differences between mothers and fathers
Coping resources and parenting mentally retarded chil- of children with disabilities. American Journal on Mental
dren. American Journal of Mental Deficiency , –. Retardation , –.
Gibaud-Wallston J. & Wandersman L. P. () Develop- Lazarus R. S. & Folkman S. () Stress, Appraisal and
ment and Utility of the Parenting Sense of Competence Scale. Coping. Springer, New York.
Paper presented at the meeting of the American Psycho-
Lovejoy M. C., Verda M. R. & Hays C. E. () Conver-
logical Association, Toronto.
gent and discriminant validity of measures of parenting
Grant G. & Whittell. B. () Differentiated coping strat- efficacy and control. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology
egies in families with children or adults with intellectual , –.
disabilities: the relevance of gender, family composition,
Mash E. J. & Johnston C. () Parental perceptions of
and the life span. Journal of Applied Research in Intellectual
child behavior problems, parenting self-esteem, and
Disabilities , –.
mothers’ reported stress in younger and older hyperactive
Grusec J. E. & Mammone N. () Features and sources and normal children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
of parents’ attributions about themselves and their chil- Psychology , –.
dren. In: Review of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol.
 (ed. N. Eisenberg), pp. –. Sage, Thousand Oaks, Mash E. J. & Johnston C. () Determinants of parenting
CA. stress: illustrations from families of hyperactive children
and physically abused children. Journal of Clinical Child
Hagekull B., Bohlin G. & Hammarberg A. () The role Psychology , –.
of parental perceived control in child development: a
longitudinal study. International Journal of Behavioral Miller S. A. () Parents’ attributions for their children’s
Development , –. behavior. Child Development , –.
Hastings R. P., Allen R., McDermott K. & Still D. () Miller A. C., Gordon R. M., Daniele R. J. & Diller L.
Factors related to positive perceptions in mothers of chil- () Stress, appraisal, and coping in mothers of dis-
dren with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Applied abled and nondisabled children. Journal of Pediatric Psy-
Research in Intellectual Disabilities , –. chology , –.
Hastings R. P. & Brown T. () Behavior problems of Minnes P. M. () Family resources and stress associated
children with autism, parental self-efficacy, and mental with having a mentally retarded child. American Journal
health. American Journal on Mental Retardation , – on Mental Retardation , –.
. Minnes P. () Mental retardation: the impact upon the
Hastings R. P. & Johnson E. () Stress in UK families family. In: Handbook of Mental Retardation and Develop-
conducting intensive home-based behavioral intervention ment (eds J. A. Burack, R. M. Hodapp & E. Zigler), pp.
for their young child with autism. Journal of Autism and –. Cambridge University Press, New York.
Developmental Disorders , –. Ohan J. L., Leung D. W. & Johnston C. () The Parent-
Hodapp R. M. () Parenting children with Down syn- ing Sense of Competence Scale: evidence of a stable
drome and other types of mental retardation. In: Hand- factor structure and validity. Canadian Journal of Behav-
book of Parenting, Vol.  (ed. M. H. Bornstein), pp. – ioural Science , –.
. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ. Quine L. & Pahl J. () Stress and coping in mothers
Hodapp R. M. () Direct and indirect behavioral effects caring for a child with severe learning difficulties: a test
of different genetic disorders of mental retardation. Amer- of Lazarus’ transactional model of coping. Journal of
ican Journal on Mental Retardation , –. Community and Applied Social Psychology , –.
Johnston C. () Addressing parent cognitions in inter- Reimers T. M., Wacker D. P., Derby K. M. & Cooper
ventions with families of disruptive children. In: Advances L. J. () Relation between parental attributions and

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research      
418
R. Hassall et al. • Parental cognitions and stress in mothers of children with disabilities

the acceptability of behavioral treatments for their Smith Slep A. M. & O’Leary S. G. () The effects of
child’s behavior problems. Behavioral Disorders , maternal attributions on parenting: an experimental anal-
–. ysis. Journal of Family Psychology , –.
Roach M. A., Orsmond G. I. & Barratt M. S. () Moth- Sparrow S. S., Balla D. A. & Cicchetti D. V. () Vine-
ers and fathers of children with Down syndrome: parental land Adaptive Behavior Scales. American Guidance Ser-
stress and involvement in childcare. American Journal on vice, Circle Pines, MN.
Mental Retardation , –. Stoneman Z. () Mental retardation and family adapta-
Roberts M. W., Joe V. C. & Rowe-Hallbert A. () tion. In: Ellis’ Handbook of Mental Deficiency: Psychological
Oppositional child behavior and parental locus of control. Theory and Research (ed. W. E. MacLean), pp. –.
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology , –. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ.
Rodrigue J. R., Geffken G. R., Clark J. E., Hunt F. & Fishel Teti D. M. & Gelfand D. M. () Behavioral competence
P. () Parenting satisfaction and efficacy among car- among mothers of infants in the first year: the mediational
egivers of children with diabetes. Children’s Health Care role of maternal self-efficacy. Child Development , –
, –. .

Rodrigue J. R., Morgan S. B. & Geffken G. () Families Walker L. S., Van Slyke D. A. & Newbrough J. R. ()
of autistic children: psychological functioning of mothers. Family resources and stress: a comparison of families of
Journal of Clinical Child Psychology , –. children with cystic fibrosis, diabetes, and mental retar-
dation. Journal of Pediatric Psychology , –.
Rotter J. B. () Generalised expectancies for internal vs.
Weiss M. J. () Hardiness and social support as predic-
external control of reinforcement. Psychological Mono-
tors of stress in mothers of typical children, children with
graphs , .
autism, and children with mental retardation. Autism ,
Sloper P., Knussen C., Turner S. & Cunningham C. () –.
Factors related to stress and satisfaction with life in fam-
Wiggs L. & Stores G. () Behavioural treatment for
ilies of children with Down’s Syndrome. Journal of Child
sleep problems in children with severe intellectual disabil-
Psychology and Psychiatry , –.
ities and daytime challenging behaviour: effect on moth-
Sloper P. & Turner S. () Risk and resistance factors in ers and fathers. British Journal of Health Psychology ,
the adaptation of parents of children with severe physical –.
disability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry ,
–. Accepted  August 

©  Blackwell Publishing Ltd, Journal of Intellectual Disability Research , –

View publication stats

You might also like