You are on page 1of 13

JOURNAL OF THE EXPERIMENTAL ANALYSIS OF BEHAVIOR 2018, 1–13

MELIORATION REVISITED: A SYSTEMATIC REPLICATION OF VAUGHAN (1981)


VIKKI J. BLAND1, SARAH COWIE1, DOUGLAS ELLIFFE1, AND CHRISTOPHER A. PODLESNIK1,2
1
THE UNIVERSITY OF AUCKLAND
2
FLORIDA INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY AND THE SCOTT CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Organisms that behave so as to forfeit a relatively higher overall rate of reinforcement in favor of a rela-
tively lower rate are said to engage in suboptimal choice. Suboptimal choice has been linked with mal-
adaptive behavior in humans. Melioration theory offers one explanatory framework for suboptimal
choice. Melioration theory suggests behavior is controlled by differences in local reinforcer rates
between alternatives. Vaughan (1981) arranged two experimental conditions in which maximizing the
overall rate of reinforcement required behavior that was compatible, or incompatible, with melioration.
Vaughan found pigeons allocated more time to a locally richer alternative even when doing so resulted
in suboptimal choice. However, Vaughan did not show whether these effects could systematically
reverse and did not provide within-session data to show that choice across short time spans remains
under the control of differences in local reinforcer rates. The present study used pigeons to replicate
and extend Vaughan’s findings. We investigated shifts in overall- and within-session choice across
repeated conditions, according to arranged local contingencies. Behavior systematically followed
changes in local contingencies for most pigeons. Within-session data suggests that, providing differences
in local reinforcer rates are discriminated, pigeons will allocate more time to a locally richer alternative,
even if this leads to suboptimal choice. These findings facilitate the more confident use of similar pro-
cedures that investigate how melioration contributes to suboptimal choice.
Key words: melioration theory, suboptimal choice, local reinforcer rates, key peck, pigeons

Evolutionary or economic theories of behav- underlying behavioral processes that contrib-


ior often posit a normative account in which ute to suboptimal choice.
organisms behave so as to achieve the highest The reasons for suboptimal choice in some
possible overall rate of reinforcement. These situations are varied, and perhaps not reducible
are maximizing, or optimizing, accounts to a single mechanism. For example, a subopti-
(e.g., Einhorn & Hogarth, 1981). However, mal alternative response may be preferred
there are many experimental arrangements in when it provides greater certainty of reinforce-
which organisms behave in such a way as to ment even at a lower overall rate (Kendall,
forfeit a higher overall reinforcer rate, and so 1974; Zentall & Laude, 2013; Zentall & Stagner,
may be said to engage in suboptimal choice 2011), if the suboptimal choice provides less
(e.g., Spetch, Belke, Barnet, Dunn, & Pierce, delayed reinforcement, albeit at a lower rate
1990; Zentall, 2014, 2016a, 2016b). (Herrnstein, Prelec, & Vaughan, 1986), if the
Suboptimal choice is an important area to probability of reinforcement depends on
research because of its possible link with mal- behavior on previous trials (Tunney & Shanks,
adaptive behavior in humans. This includes 2002), and even if the response cost of the sub-
behavior described as ‘addictive’ such as illicit optimal alternative is higher than an optimal
drug-taking (Heyman & Dunn, 2002) and alternative (Hinnenkamp, Shahan, & Mad-
gambling resulting in loss (Heyman, 1996; den, 2017).
Molet et al., 2012). If suboptimal choice con- Choice in conventional concurrent variable-
tributes to the development of maladaptive interval (VI) VI schedules can be interpreted
behavior in humans, there is much to be as support for a normative maximization, or
gained for behavioral applications and inter- optimality, account because strict matching
ventions from continued investigation of the (in which the overall proportion of time or
response allocated to an alternative equals the
proportion of reinforcers obtained from that
alternative; Herrnstein, 1961; 1970) produces
Address correspondence to: Vikki J. Bland, School of the highest overall reinforcer rate (Staddon &
Psychology, The University of Auckland, Private Bag
92019, Victoria Street West, Auckland 1142, New Zealand
Motheral, 1978). However, melioration theory
Email: vbla002@aucklanduni.ac.nz (Herrnstein & Vaughan, 1980) offers an alter-
doi: 10.1002/jeab.327 native account of matching that does not

© 2018 Society for the Experimental Analysis of Behavior


1
2 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

appeal to maximization. Melioration theory because both predict strict matching. How-
suggests that choice between competing alter- ever, procedures can be devised in which the
natives is controlled by the extent to which two theories’ predictions differ.
local reinforcer rates between those alterna- Vaughan (1981) tested the predictions of
tives differ. Here, ‘local reinforcer rate’ melioration theory by designing an experi-
describes the number of reinforcers obtained ment in which maximizing the overall rate of
at an alternative divided by time spent at that reinforcement required behavior that was
alternative. incompatible with melioration. Three pigeons
When local reinforcer rates are not equal, responded on a two-key concurrent VI sched-
melioration theory suggests that choice favors ule. A response to either the left key or the
the alternative delivering the higher local rate. right key initiated a 2-s timer for that key and
When this happens, more time is spent on that paused the VI-schedule timer for the other
alternative, so that the denominator (time) in key. The timer for any one key stopped when
the local-rate calculation increases, thus reduc- either 2 s had elapsed without a response to
ing the local rate. Eventually, this process of that key, or when the other key was pecked.
spending more time on the alternative that This procedure was used to ensure a precise
currently delivers a higher local reinforcer rate measure of accumulated time on both keys. A
produces an equilibrium at which the local 1-s changeover delay (Herrnstein, 1961) oper-
rates obtained on each alternative are equal: ated for switches between keys. The details of
Vaughan’s scheduling also effectively imposed
R1 R2 a pacing requirement in the form of a mini-
= ð1Þ mum interresponse time before a response
T1 T2
could be reinforced. In the replication
with R1 and R2 representing obtained rein- reported here, we did not include that
forcement for alternatives 1 and 2, respectively, requirement, both because it is unusual and to
and T1 and T2 representing time allocated to confirm that the support for melioration that
alternatives 1 and 2, respectively. Vaughan reported was not a result of an artifi-
For standard concurrent-choice scenarios, cial pacing requirement.
melioration theory accommodates most choice In each 30-min session, Vaughan (1981)
data approximately able to be described by arranged eight successive time periods called
the strict matching law (Herrnstein 1961, ‘preference windows’. These were used to cal-
1970). Therefore, Equation (1) may be rear- culate allocation of time to the left and right
ranged to give strict matching. The time-based keys, and as a result to alter the local rein-
strict matching equation is written as: forcer rates in the next window, according to
the feedback functions shown in Figure 1 rein-
T 1 R1 forcer rates. The left side of Figure 1 shows
= ð2Þ Vaughan’s hourly programmed local rein-
T 2 R2
forcer rates for responses to left and right keys
with terms as in Equation (1). However, for as in Condition A (top panel) and Condition B
long as the local reinforcer rates between con- (bottom panel), as a function of proportion of
currently available alternatives are not equal, time allocated to the right key in the previous
the melioration equation may be written as: preference window. We designated propor-
tional time allocations between 0.125 and 0.25
R1 R2 to the right key the ‘high-rate melioration
Rd = − ð3Þ
T1 T2 zone’, because the local rates of reinforcement
on the two keys are equal (melioration), and
where Rd represents the signed difference the overall reinforcer rate from both keys
(positive or negative) between local reinforcer combined is at a maximum. Similarly, time
rates for alternatives 1 and 2. Once Rd is suffi- allocations between 0.75 and 0.875 to the right
ciently close to zero, melioration theory pre- key are the ‘low-rate melioration zone’,
dicts variability of choice will reduce to because the local reinforcer rates are equal,
maintain a low Rd value. In conventional con- but the overall reinforcer rate is at a mini-
current VI VI schedules, the predictions of mum. Therefore, maximization predicts
maximization and melioration coincide, choice to be between 0.125 and 0.25 in both
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 3

Fig. 1. Diagram of programmed hourly rates of reinforcement (y-axis) in Condition A (top panel) and Condition B
(bottom panel) for left key (dashed black line, L) and right key (solid black line, R) responses as a function of propor-
tion of time on the right key (x-axis) in a previous preference window, for Vaughan’s (1981) procedure (left) and the
procedure for the current study (right). Choice is predicted to shift in a subsequent preference window when proportion
of time allocated to the right key in a current window is between 0.26 to 0.74. Equal rates of reinforcement for left and
right keys occur when proportion of time allocated to the right key is either 0.125 to 0.25 or 0.75 to 0.875.

Conditions A and B, because that produces Vaughan (1981) found that, in the last five
the highest possible overall reinforcer rate. sessions of Condition A, all three pigeons allo-
However, the predictions of melioration cated time so as to remain in the high-rate
require explanation. melioration zone, but shifted to the low-rate
In Condition A, if choice falls at any point zone in the last five sessions of Condition
outside the two melioration zones, the local B. This change led to the pigeons obtaining
reinforcer rates on the two keys are unequal. 17% fewer reinforcers overall in Condition B
Melioration then predicts that more time will relative to Condition A. Vaughan concluded
be spent on the key with the higher local rate. that if maximization of overall reinforcer rates
For time allocation greater than 0.25, the key requires organisms to deviate from arranged
with the higher local rate is the left key. There- local contingencies by allocating more time to
fore, melioration drives choice towards the left a locally leaner alternative, maximization of
key, so that proportional time allocation to the overall reinforcer rates is less likely to occur.
right key decreases. This pushes choice back Instead, pigeons will allocate more time to a
to the high-rate melioration zone. locally richer alternative, in support of meliora-
In Condition B, however, the local rate on tion theory. Therefore, it has been suggested
the right key exceeds that on the left for all melioration may underlie suboptimal, and mal-
intermediate time allocations. Melioration adaptive, choice in humans (Herrnstein & Pre-
therefore predicts spending more time on the lec, 1991; Heyman, 1996; 2013; Rachlin, 1997).
right key, pushing choice towards the low-rate Vaughan’s (1981) study had three main limi-
melioration zone of 0.75 to 0.875, even though tations. First, he only used three pigeons. Of
such choice produces the lowest possible over- these three pigeons, Vaughan increased the
all reinforcer rate. Thus, both melioration and difference in local reinforcer rates between
maximization predict choice in the high-rate competing alternatives for one pigeon during
melioration zone in Condition A, but their Condition B. This change occurred due to the
predictions for Condition B differ. apparent inability of that pigeon to discriminate
4 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

the difference in local reinforcer rates between (2) overall choice systematically changes when
alternatives. Second, Vaughan tested Condition local contingencies change across alternating
A followed by Condition B with no replication conditions, and (3) overall suboptimal choice
of either condition. Therefore, Vaughan did occurs due to choice systematically following
not demonstrate whether these effects could be local contingencies. Therefore, the present
reversed, and relatedly, did not rule out order study replicates and extends the study by
effects of conditions or mere exposure to these Vaughan. In addition, the present study pro-
procedures influencing choice. Third, Vaugh- vides an opportunity to assess the validity of
an’s dependent measure, proportion of time on using Vaughan’s procedure to investigate the
the right key, was reported across an entire ses- role of melioration in overall suboptimal
sion, rather than within each of the preference choice.
windows comprising one session. Therefore, in
testing melioration theory Vaughan’s study did Method
not provide within-session data to show that
choice across short time spans is under the con- Subjects
trol of differences in local reinforcer rates. Subjects were six experimentally experi-
The present study addressed the limitations enced homing pigeons, numbered 61 to 66.
of Vaughan’s (1981) study to assess the reli- The pigeons had previous experience with
ability of his findings. We investigated system- two-alternative concurrent-choice schedules.
atic shifts in overall choice between repeated Pigeons were individually housed within a
alternating conditions, and shifts of within- pigeon colony room and maintained at
session choice according to arranged local 85%  10 g of their free-feeding body weights
contingencies. Six pigeons were trained to using postsession supplementary feeding of
respond on left and right keys on dependent mixed grain. Water and grit were available at
concurrent VI VI schedules. We arranged all times. The colony room lighting was
three alternating cycles of Vaughan’s Condi- switched on at 00:00 and off at 16:00 daily.
tion A and Condition B (i.e., ABABAB). In a Experimental sessions began at 01:00 with a
global choice analysis, the dependent measure 60-min blackout. No personnel entered the
of proportion of time on the right key was cal- room during sessions.
culated across each session, and for each con-
dition and cycle. To discover the extent to
which within-session choice contributes to Apparatus
overall suboptimal choice and supports melio- The pigeons’ home cages served as the
ration theory, we also measured proportion of experimental chambers. The cages measured
time allocated to the right key within each of 380 mm high, 380 mm wide and 380 mm
seven of the eight successive preference win- deep. Three walls were constructed of sheet
dows comprising one session. Window 1 was metal while the floor, ceiling, and front wall
omitted from this analysis because no rein- were metal bars. In each chamber, two
forcement contingencies were arranged in a wooden perches were mounted 60 mm above
previous window within that session. the floor. One perch was parallel to, and
If melioration is used to explain suboptimal 90 mm from, the front wall while the other
choice, it is necessary to demonstrate that was parallel to, and 90 mm from, the right
choosing a locally richer alternative may lead wall. The operant panel was located on the
to relatively lower overall reinforcer rates. right wall and consisted of three 20-mm diam-
Vaughan (1981) used a procedure in which eter translucent keys set 220 mm above the
local contingencies either led to, or were in wooden perches and 85 mm apart, center to
opposition to, the maximization of overall center. Only the left and right keys were used
reinforcer rates. He found overall choice fol- in this experiment. The center key was not
lowed arranged local contingencies whether used. The left and right keys could be transil-
those contingencies lead to suboptimal choice luminated by LEDs located behind each key
or not. The present study extends this finding with red or green lights. Pecks to a lit key
by investigating whether (1) within-session exceeding 0.1 N were registered as responses.
choice systematically follows arranged local The magazine aperture was located 100 mm
contingencies across short time periods, below the center key and measured 50 mm
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 5

high, 50 mm wide, and 40 mm deep. A hop- VI schedule associated with a key advanced
per containing wheat was situated behind the only while the timer was active. If a reinforcer
magazine. During hopper presentations, all was arranged, no further reinforcers could be
key lights were turned off and the hopper was arranged for either response until that rein-
raised and illuminated for 2 s. We excluded forcer had been obtained. Each reinforcer
hopper time from all analyses. All experimen- delivery consisted of 2 s of access to food.
tal events were arranged and recorded by an Table 1 shows VI-schedule reinforcer rates
IBM® PC-compatible computer running MED- for left and right keys in Condition A and Con-
PC IV® software which was located in an dition B. The number of 30-min sessions per
adjacent room. condition varied until the primary dependent
measure of proportion of time allocation was
Procedure stable across five sessions for all six pigeons.
The number of sessions per cycle was 60, 26,
As the pigeons in this study were experi- and 29 for Conditions A1, A2 and A3, respec-
mentally experienced, experimental condi- tively; and 25, 26, and 34 for Conditions B1,
tions were initiated from the outset of the
B2 and B3, respectively. The value of the VI
experiment. We arranged two conditions: A
schedule of reinforcement for each key in any
and B (see Table 1 and description below).
4-min period was determined by the depen-
We assessed Condition A followed by Condi-
tion B three times for all pigeons. Conditions dent measure of the proportion of time allo-
were identified by letter and cycle. Therefore, cated to the right key in a previous 4-min
the conditions were labelled A1, B1, A2, B2, period. Hereafter, these 4-min ‘time windows’
A3, and B3. Both Condition A and Condition (Jones & Davison, 1997; Tonneau, 2005) are
B used a two-key dependent concurrent VI VI referred to as ‘preference windows’. There
schedule (Fleshler & Hoffman, 1962). The left were eight separate and consecutive prefer-
key was lit green and the right key was lit red. ence windows in each session. In keeping with
As in Vaughan’s (1981) procedure, each Vaughan’s (1981) procedure, Windows 1 to
response to a key initiated a timer for that key, 7 were each of 240-s duration; Window 8 was
which ran for 2 s. Time spent on a key accu- 120 s in duration.
mulated only while the timer was running. Figure 1 shows programmed hourly rates of
The timer stopped if 2 s elapsed without reinforcement in Condition A and Condition
another response, or if a reinforcer was deliv- B for left key and right key responses as a
ered, or if a response was made to the other function of proportion of time on the right
key. If a response was made to the other key, a key in a previous preference window. Vaugh-
timer was instead initiated for that key, but no an’s (1981) procedure is shown on the left,
reinforcer could be obtained for 1 s following and the procedure for the current study is
the changeover (a 1-s changeover delay). The shown on the right. Access to the maximum
rate of reinforcement within any one prefer-
ence window (i.e., VI 20 s vs. VI 20 s) required
Table 1
proportion of time on the right key for the
Allocation of VI schedules of reinforcement on left and previous preference window to be within the
right keys for Conditions A and B, as a function of
proportion of time on the right key in the previous 4-min range 0.125 – 0.25. Hereafter, this range is
preference window called the high-rate melioration zone. Access to
the minimum rate of reinforcement (i.e., VI
Relative time on CONDITION A CONDITION B 60 s vs. VI 60 s) required proportion of time
right key in previous
4 min preference Left Right Left Right on the right key to be within the range 0.75 –
window* key key key key 0.875 for the previous preference window.
Hereafter, this range is called the low-rate melio-
0.0 - 0.124 VI 60 VI 20 VI 60 VI 20 ration zone. The zones are called ‘melioration
0.125 - 0.25 VI 20 VI 20 VI 20 VI 20
0.26 - 0.74 VI 20 VI 60 VI 60 VI 20 zones’ because if proportion of time on the
0.75 - 0.875 VI 60 VI 60 VI 60 VI 60 right fell within either of these zones, the pro-
0.876 - 1.0 VI 20 VI 60 VI 20 VI 60 grammed difference in local reinforcer rates
* Window 1 left/right key VI values randomly assigned between left and right keys was zero (Rd = 0
from VI 20 and VI 60. from Eq. (3)).
6 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

According to these contingencies, in the right reached 0.50, the right key paid off
Condition A, choice could be within, or transi- at 60 reinforcers per hour, as in the present
tion toward, the high-rate melioration zone study. The decision to remove the curve func-
across successive preference windows while tion from programming for the present study
maintaining behavior supporting melioration was made to simplify programming of the con-
theory. In Condition B, by comparison, choice tingencies. Because the arranged contingen-
remaining within, or transitioning toward the cies operate under the same maximization
high-rate melioration zone required a depar- and local contingencies arranged by Vaughan,
ture from behavior supporting melioration. we did not expect that arranging a more
However, in Condition B, choice remaining abrupt change in rates of reinforcement
within or transitioning toward the low-rate between alternatives would impact the primary
melioration zone did not require a departure findings.
from melioration. For example, if the propor-
tion of time on the right key was between 0.26
– 0.74 in any given preference window in Results
Condition B, in the next preference window Response and Reinforcer Rates
the right key would pay off at 180 reinforcers Table 2 shows mean overall response rates
per hour, and the left key would pay off at per minute across the last five sessions of indi-
60 reinforcers per hour. These contingencies vidual A and B conditions, for individual
were reversed for the range 0.26 – 0.74 in Con- pigeons. The far right-hand column presents
dition A. Therefore, if choice fell within this
these rates averaged across all conditions.
range in Condition B, it was not possible to
Mean overall response rates for Pigeons
return to the high-rate melioration zone unless
61 and 64 were consistently lower across condi-
the pigeons consistently allocated more time to
the locally leaner left key. Doing so required a tions compared with the other four pigeons.
temporary departure from melioration. This finding is examined further in the
Note that left- and right-key reinforcer rates Discussion.
for the range 0.26 – 0.74 in the present study Figure 2 shows mean overall reinforcer rates
(Fig. 1, right graph) differed slightly from from the last five sessions of Conditions A1, A2
those arranged by Vaughan (1981) (Fig. 1, left and A3, and then averaged for individual
graph). In Vaughan’s study, reinforcer rates pigeons. We used the same method to calcu-
within the range of 0.26 – 0.74 gradually late mean overall reinforcer rates across repli-
decreased from 180 to 60 reinforcers per cations of Condition B. We found no reliable
hour. For example, if proportion of time on difference in overall reinforcer rates between
the right in a previous preference window was conditions. However, overall reinforcer rates
0.30 or 0.40 in Condition A, while the left key were higher for Condition A than Condition B
then paid off at 180 reinforcers per hour, the for four of six pigeons, with exceptions being
right key paid off at 90 and 70 reinforcers per Pigeons 61 and 64. As Vaughan (1981)
hour, respectively. If proportion of time on reported greater overall reinforcer rates in

Table 2
Mean overall response rates per minute across the last five sessions of individual A and B conditions, for individual
pigeons

CONDITION
PIGEON A1 B1 A2 B2 A3 B3 MEAN

61 15.53 44.27 47.47 49.15 59.86 57.14 45.57


62 69.10 63.61 54.93 62.00 63.63 60.15 62.24
63 70.64 84.95 76.41 86.09 97.74 85.04 83.48
64 47.59 49.69 46.13 60.82 48.41 46.41 49.84
65 96.77 66.15 101.97 76.27 89.60 60.64 81.90
66 83.96 65.46 73.31 74.62 70.55 79.35 74.54
Note. The furthermost right-hand column presents the same rates averaged across all conditions.
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 7

Condition A Condition B arranged in a previous window within that ses-


2.0 sion. The arranged contingencies (see Fig. 1)
predict that during A conditions, the propor-
Mean reinforcers per min

1.5
tion of time allocated to the right key should
decrease across successive 4-min preference
windows toward the high-rate melioration
1.0 zone. During B conditions, the proportion of
time allocated to the right key should increase
0.5 across successive preference windows toward
the low-rate melioration zone.
In Condition A1, choice fell within the high-
0.0 rate melioration zone from preference window
61 62 63 64 65 66 2 and generally remained toward the high-rate
Pigeon melioration zone across successive preference
windows for Pigeons 63 and 66. This behavior
Fig. 2. Overall obtained reinforcer rate as an average was in accordance with A condition local con-
of the last five sessions of Conditions A1, A2 and A3 (dark
grey bars); and an average of the last five sessions of Con- tingencies. For the other four pigeons, choice
ditions B1, B2 and B3 (light grey bars), for individual mostly fell within the 0.26 to 0.74 range across
pigeons. windows 2 to 7, producing a 3:1 difference in
local reinforcer rates between left and right
alternatives. However, by window 8, choice
Condition A than B for three subjects, this had shifted toward the high-rate melioration
finding is examined further in the Discussion. zone for Pigeons 62 and 65 in accordance with
A condition local contingencies. Therefore,
Global Choice Analysis the results of Condition A1 suggest choice was
sensitive to the arranged local contingencies
Figure 3 shows proportion of time on the
for four of six pigeons. In contrast, Pigeons
right key for individual pigeons as a mean of
61 and 64 variably allocated a greater propor-
the last five sessions from each condition.
tion of time to either the left or right key
Despite differences in levels of time allocation
across successive preference windows, despite
across pigeons, choice tended to be further
toward the high-rate melioration zone in A the 3:1 difference in local reinforcer rates
conditions, and further toward the low-rate between left and right alternatives that
melioration zone in B conditions for all occurred when choice fell within the 0.26 to
pigeons. However, there were individual differ- 0.74 range. In addition, compared with the
ences in the degree to which choice shifted other pigeons, 61 and 64 allocated a greater
between the high- and low-rate melioration proportion of time to the locally leaner right
zones between conditions. For example, choice key in all A conditions. Therefore, the behav-
by Pigeons 61 and 64 consistently remained ior of Pigeons 61 and 64 did not follow the
closer to the low-rate melioration zone across arranged local contingencies of A conditions.
all conditions compared with the other Choice in Condition B1 fell within the low-
pigeons, while choice by Pigeon 63 remained rate melioration zone from preference window
relatively closer to the high-rate melioration 3 and remained generally toward the low-rate
zone across all conditions compared with other melioration zone across successive preference
pigeons. windows for five of six pigeons. This behavior,
which was in accordance with B condition
local contingencies, was maintained in subse-
Within-Session Choice Analysis quent B condition reversals for the same
Figure 4 shows mean proportion of time on pigeons. The exception was Pigeon 63, which
the right key across successive preference win- allocated a greater proportion of time to the
dows 2-8 for individual pigeons as a mean of locally leaner left key in Condition B1, and to
the last five sessions in A conditions (left a lesser extent, in Condition B3. Therefore,
column) and B conditions (right column). the behavior of Pigeon 63 did not reliably fol-
Window 1 was omitted from this analysis low the arranged local contingencies of these
because no reinforcement contingencies were B conditions.
8 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

1
Low rate melioration zone
0.875
61
Proportion time on right

0.75
62
0.625
63
0.5
64
0.375
65
0.25
66
0.125
Mean
High rate melioration zone
0
A1 B1 A2 B2 A3 B3
Condition

Fig. 3. Proportion of time on the right key (y-axis) for individual pigeons, averaged across the last five sessions of suc-
cessive conditions A1, B1, A2, B2, A3 and B3 (x-axis). Horizontal rectangular boxes indicate high-rate and low-rate melio-
ration zones.

Overall, the results of this analysis suggest toward the low-rate melioration zone and away
most pigeons discriminated differences in from the high-rate melioration zone in B con-
local reinforcer rates between alternatives. ditions. Changes in allocation between Condi-
This led to (1) choice remaining toward the tions A and B in these analyses may be
high-rate melioration zone across preference expected given that we calculated average pro-
windows in A conditions, and (2) choice portion of time on the right across the last five
remaining toward the low-rate melioration sessions of each condition. Therefore, shifts in
zone across preference windows in B condi- choice direction between A and B conditions
tions. Therefore, choice followed the arranged suggest the pigeons had learned the local
local contingencies for repeated cycles of Con- contingencies of each condition.
ditions A and B for most pigeons. Although choice changed systematically
across conditions for all pigeons (see Fig. 3),
Discussion global choice fell exactly within high- or low-
rate melioration zones in respective A and B
We tested melioration theory by investigat- conditions only for two of six pigeons in A
ing whether within-session and across-session conditions (Pigeon 63 in A1 and A2, and
choice systematically followed arranged local Pigeon 62 in A3) and three of six pigeons in B
contingencies. Specifically, this study sought conditions (Pigeon 65 in B1 and B3, and
to replicate and extend the findings of Pigeons 61 and 64 in all B conditions). This
Vaughan (1981) using six pigeons by (1) ana- may be compared with all three pigeons in A
lyzing both across-session (global analyses) conditions, and two of three pigeons in B con-
and within-session dependent measures ditions, for Vaughan’s (1981) study. There-
(within-session analyses), and (2) repeating fore, the global-choice results of the present
cycles of alternating conditions in which max- study generally support those of Vaughan.
imized reinforcer rates favored melioration However, questions remain as to why all
(Condition A) or did not (Condition B). pigeons’ behavior was not precisely controlled
Overall, the results of this study partially by the prevailing contingencies.
support melioration theory (Herrnstein & The extent to which choice fell exactly
Vaughan, 1980) and extend the suboptimal within a melioration zone likely depended on
choice findings of Vaughan (1981). the extent to which the pigeons could main-
Global-choice analyses (see Fig. 3) show that tain learned local contingencies. For exam-
all six pigeons shifted choice toward the high- ple, the within-session analyses of the present
rate melioration zone and away from the low- study (see Fig. 4) show that when choice in
rate melioration zone in A conditions, and any one preference window fell within either
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 9

Fig. 4. Mean proportion of time on the right key (y-axis) within successive preference windows 2-8 (x-axis) for individ-
ual pigeons as an average of the last five sessions in A conditions (left panel) and B conditions (right panel). Horizontal
rectangular boxes indicate high-rate and low-rate melioration zones.

the high- or low-rate melioration zone, choice analyses show that four of six pigeons (62, 63,
remained toward that zone across subsequent 65 and 66) obtained a lower overall rate of
windows for most pigeons in accordance with reinforcement in B conditions compared with
the local contingencies. This suggests most A conditions (see Fig. 2). The within-session
pigeons could discriminate local reinforcer- analyses (see Fig. 4) show this occurred
rate differences. However, as observed in the because most pigeons (except Pigeon 63) allo-
global-choice analyses, choice across succes- cated a greater proportion of time to the
sive preference windows did not always locally richer right key across successive prefer-
remain exactly within the melioration zone. ence windows in B conditions, despite the fact
This suggests choice did not always follow the that this reduced the overall rate of reinforce-
local contingencies that led to equal local ment in B conditions. Although this reduction
reinforcer rates. was small, it was likely not larger due to the
The extent to which choice followed melioration zones being narrow or con-
arranged local contingencies for high- or strained. For example, in Condition A, to
low-rate melioration zones had an impact on access VI 20 VI 20 schedules of reinforcement,
overall obtained reinforcer rates in A and B proportion of time on the right must fall pre-
conditions. For example, the global-choice cisely between 0.125 and 0.25 within any 4-min
10 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

window. If the proportion of choice is instead provisional, it offers one explanation for why
between 0.26 and 0.74, the pigeons are choice remained close to the low-rate meliora-
exposed to VI 20 VI 60 schedules of reinforce- tion zone in both A and B conditions for these
ment. The same applies in Condition B: if pro- pigeons.
portion of choice is not exactly within 0.75 to It is also possible that a right-key bias over-
0.875, the schedule of reinforcement will be rode the arranged local contingencies in A
VI 60 VI 20, not VI 60 VI 60. Therefore, conditions for Pigeons 61 and 64. The within-
although the difference in overall obtained session analyses of Figure 4 show relative time
reinforcer rates between A and B conditions is on the right key across successive 4-min prefer-
small, the lower overall obtained reinforcer ence windows. In A conditions, the arranged
rates in B conditions is evidence of a subopti- local contingencies required the pigeons to
mal choice outcome. allocate more time to the locally richer left key
Although most of the pigeons obtained a across preference windows to maximize overall
higher overall reinforcer-rate in A conditions reinforcer rates. Pigeons 61 and 64 allocated
compared with B conditions, this did not more time to the locally leaner right key in A
occur for Pigeons 61 and 64 (see Fig. 2). conditions than the other pigeons. However,
Therefore, these pigeons obtained a lower choice was only consistently toward the right
overall rate of reinforcement across both con- key across preference windows for both
ditions compared to the other pigeons. In pigeons in Condition A2. In Condition A1,
addition, the global choice analysis (Fig. 3) choice across preference windows varied for
shows that reversals in choice direction Pigeon 61, and did not shift toward the right
between A and B conditions were less pro- key until preference window 6 for Pigeon 64.
nounced for Pigeons 61 and 64 compared with In Condition A3, choice was more consistently
the other pigeons. The within-session analysis toward the right key across preference win-
(Fig. 4) shows this occurred due to Pigeons dows for Pigeon 64. However, for Pigeon
61 and 64 allocating a greater proportion of 61 this did not occur until preference window
time to the locally leaner right key across pref- 5, after which proportion of time on the right
erence windows in A conditions compared remained below 0.60. Although the evidence
with other pigeons, and subsequently to the of a right key bias is inconclusive, it is possible.
locally richer right key in B conditions, in This raises the question of whether the melio-
accordance with other pigeons. Therefore, a ration equation could be improved by the
greater proportion of time was allocated to inclusion of a bias parameter.
the right key for these two pigeons, irrespec- There is research to support the possibility
tive either of condition or whether the right that Pigeons 61 and 64 had difficulty discrimi-
key was locally richer or leaner across succes- nating unequal differences in local reinforcer
sive preference windows. As this behavior is rates between alternatives in both A and B
not in accordance with the local contingencies conditions. Davison (1990) investigated the
of A conditions, the overall suboptimal choice role of discriminated local reinforcer rates
observed for Pigeons 61 and 64 cannot be within the framework of melioration theory.
directly attributed to melioration. Davison proposed ‘melioration threshold the-
It is possible that choice by Pigeons 61 and ory’ to explain studies in which pigeons’
64 followed contingencies other than the choice did not closely follow a locally richer
arranged local contingencies when local rein- alternative (Baum, 1979; Myers & Myers, 1977;
forcer rates were not equal, but followed Taylor & Davison, 1983; Wearden & Burgess,
arranged local contingencies when local rein- 1982). Davison modeled exponential sched-
forcer rates were equal. For example, these ules of concurrent VI VI reinforcement that
pigeons may have learned the more general ranged from VI 56 s VI 56 s to VI 29 s VI
rule that ‘choosing right more often’ eventu- 556 s. He found that as the difference in local
ally resulted in equal local reinforcer rates in reinforcer rates between alternatives became
any condition. If so, it could be argued that progressively smaller, feedback functions
behavior remained under the control of dis- became flatter. That is, simulated choice ratios
criminated equal local reinforcer rates, neither progressively failed to follow modeled rein-
supporting nor fully opposing melioration forcer ratios. In referring to data from studies
theory. While this interpretation remains with pigeons, Davison showed these flat
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 11

feedback functions would present as increas- the other pigeons across all conditions is of
ingly indifferent or random choice. Davison interest and might play some role in their pat-
concluded that when small differences in local terns of behavior.
reinforcer rates become difficult to discrimi- At present, melioration theory does not pro-
nate, the ‘threshold’ required for melioration vide a parameter that measures the extent to
is breached, increasing the likelihood of indif- which differences in local reinforcer rates
ferent choice. between alternatives are discriminated. This
Supporting the findings of Davison (1990), may be addressed in development of quantita-
Vaughan (1981) attributed the random choice tive analyses designed to integrate melioration
demonstrated by one pigeon in his study to a theory and contingency discriminability theory
failure to discriminate differences in local (Davison & Jenkins, 1985). Taken together,
reinforcer rates. Specifically, while mean the findings of Davison (1990) and Vaughan
global choice ultimately fell exclusively within (1981) suggest that if differences between
high- and low- rate melioration zones in those local reinforcer rates had been increased in
conditions, this effect initially was observed for the present study, Pigeons 61 and 64 may have
only two of three pigeons in Condition B of allocated progressively more time to the locally
Vaughan’s study. Shifting choice to the low- richer left key during A conditions in accor-
rate melioration zone in Condition B for the dance with the local contingencies.
third pigeon required increasing the differ- Although we did not increase the size of
ence in local reinforcer rates between alterna- local reinforcer rate differences for Pigeons
tives. As a result, that pigeon began to allocate 61 and 64, the findings of Vaughan (1981)
a greater proportion of time to the right key and Davison (1990) suggest that, for some
during Condition B in accordance with the individuals, discriminating local contingencies
local contingencies. This finding suggests may require larger differences in local rein-
Vaughan made the arranged local contingen- forcer rates. Moreover, if melioration does
cies more discriminable for this one pigeon. contribute to overall suboptimal choice there
As suggested by Davison, the extent to which is benefit in investigating whether progres-
local reinforcer rate differences are discrimi- sively smaller local reinforcer-rate differences
nated likely depends upon the extent to which disrupt choice already following local contin-
the melioration threshold is maintained and gencies. Future studies might also investigate
not breached. Vaughan’s results suggest there the extent to which suboptimal choice may be
are likely to be individual differences in where subverted if local reinforcer rates become diffi-
that threshold lies. Similarly, the findings of cult to discriminate, and overall reinforcer
the present study suggest individual differ- rates are simultaneously signaled. For exam-
ences in the ability of Pigeons 61 and 64 to dis- ple, in a study with pigeons Heyman and Tanz
criminate local reinforcer-rate differences (1995) made greater overall reinforcer rates
compared with the other four pigeons. contingent on choice not following local rein-
The attenuated control by arranged local forcer rates. In Experiment 1, if choice closely
contingencies for Pigeons 61 and 64 relative to followed local reinforcer rates, responses were
the other pigeons might have resulted in lower not eligible for reinforcement. Therefore,
overall response rates for these pigeons com- overall reinforcer rate decreased, resulting in
pared with the other four pigeons (see suboptimal choice. In Experiment 2, a blue
Table 2). For example, relatively lower overall light was presented whenever responses were
response rates for Pigeons 61 and 64 may be not immediately eligible for reinforcement
due to a right-key bias, or due to difficulty in and a white light was presented whenever
discriminating unequal local reinforcer rates. responses were eligible for reinforcement.
It is also possible that lower overall response Heyman and Tanz found when the lights were
rate impacted the ability of these pigeons to absent, choice followed local-reinforcer rates,
discriminate local contingencies. We are not leading to suboptimal choice. When the lights
aware of any research that suggests difficulty were present, the extent to which choice fol-
with contingency discriminability impacts over- lowed local reinforcer rates decreased as a
all response rates, or vice versa. Nevertheless, function of increased overall reinforcer rate.
the finding that overall response rate was con- Therefore, the results of the studies by Davi-
sistently lower for Pigeons 61 and 64 than for son (1990) and Heyman and Tanz suggest that
12 VIKKI J. BLAND et al.

the extent to which melioration occurs may Davison, M. (1990). Undermatching, melioration and the
depend on the ability of organisms to both dis- discrimination of local reinforcer rates. Behavioral
Processes, 21, 189-195. https://doi.org/10.1016/0376-
criminate local reinforcer rates and not discrim- 6357(90)90024-A
inate overall reinforcer rates. Davison, M., & Jenkins, P. E. (1985). Stimulus discrimina-
It has been suggested that melioration may bility, contingency discriminability, and schedule per-
underlie suboptimal, and maladaptive, choice formance. Learning & Behavior, 13, 77-84. https://doi.
in humans (Herrnstein & Prelec, 1991; Hey- org/10.3758/bf03213368
Einhorn, H. J., & Hogarth, R. M. (1981). Behavioral deci-
man, 1996; 2013; Rachlin, 1997). However, sion theory: Processes of judgment and choice. Journal
although these studies support the overall of Accounting Research, 19, 1-31. https://doi.org/10.
predictions of melioration theory, they do 2307/2490959
not demonstrate the extent to which choice Fleshler, M., & Hoffman, H.S. (1962). A progression for
remains systematically under the control of generating variable-interval schedules. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 5, 529-530. https://
local contingencies. Vaughan’s (1981) semi- doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1962.5-529
nal study attempted to do just that, but his Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of
results are not fully compelling due to a lack response as a function of frequency of reinforcement.
of demonstrated experimental control across Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 267-
repeated condition changes. The present 272. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267
study addresses these limitations and extends Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the Law of Effect. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 13, 243-266.
Vaughan’s study by showing overall choice https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1970.13-243
shifts systematically with repeated changes in Herrnstein, R. J., & Prelec, D. (1991). Melioration: A
local contingencies. The results of the pre- theory of distributed choice. The Journal of Economic
sent study also suggest that proportioning Perspectives, 5, 137-156. https://doi.org/10.1257/
choice across successive preference windows jep.5.3.137
will be maintained if the proportion of Herrnstein, R. J., Prelec, D., & Vaughan, W. (1986). An
intra-personal prisoners’ dilemma. Paper presented at the
choice results in equal local reinforcer rates. IX Symposium on Quantitative Analysis of Behavior:
These proportions will be maintained even if Behavioral Economics, Harvard University.
local choice leads to overall suboptimal Herrnstein, R. J., & Vaughan, W. (1980). Melioration and
choice outcomes. behavioral allocation. Limits to action: The allocation of
This study facilitates the more confident individual behavior (pp. 143-176). New York, NY: Aca-
demic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-
use of this and similar procedures in future 662650-6.50011-8
studies designed to further investigate how Heyman, G.M. (1996). Resolving the contradictions of
melioration contributes to suboptimal choice. addiction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 193-203.
For example, if melioration contributes to https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x00042990
suboptimal choice, there is much to be Heyman, G. M. (2013). Addiction and choice: theory and
gained from a follow-up investigation of the new data. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 4 (31). https://doi.
org/10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00031
extent to which increased differences in local Heyman, G. M., & Dunn, B. (2002). Decision biases and
reinforcer rates lead to better discrimination persistent illicit drug use: an experimental study of
of those rates, thereby facilitating meliora- distributed choice and addiction. Drug and Alcohol
tion. If larger differences in local reinforcer Dependence, 67, 193-203. https://doi.org/10.1016/
rates result in choice closely following s0376-8716(02)00071-6
Heyman, G. M., & Tanz, L. (1995). How to teach a pigeon
arranged local contingencies across both to maximize overall reinforcement rate. Journal of the
shorter and longer time spans, this offers an Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 64, 277-297. https://
opportunity to investigate ways in which a sta- doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1995.64-277
ble melioration process may be able to be Hinnenkamp, J.E., Shahan, T.A., & Madden, G.J. (2017).
subverted, resulting in more optimal choice How suboptimal is suboptimal choice? Journal of the
outcomes. Such findings could provide Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 107, 136-150. https://
doi.org/10.1002/jeab.239
insight into processes related to understand- Jones, B., & Davison, M. (1997). The control of choice by
ing and treating suboptimal choice as it its consequences. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
relates to addiction. Behavior, 68, 329-348. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.
1997.68-329
References Kendall, S.B. (1974). Preference for intermittent reinforce-
ment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21,
Baum, W.M. (1979). Matching, overmatching and under- 463-473. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1974.21-463
matching in studies of choice. Journal of the Experimen- Molet, M., Miller, H.C., Laude, J.R., Kirk, C., Manning, B., &
tal Analysis of Behavior, 32, 269-281. https://doi. Zentall, T.R. (2012). Decision making by humans in a
org/10.1901/jeab.1979.32-269 behavioral task: Do humans, like pigeons, show
MELIORATION REVISITED: REPLICATING VAUGHAN (1981) 13

suboptimal choice? Learning and Behavior, 40, 439-447. Vaughan, W. (1981). Melioration, matching, and maximiza-
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-012-0065-7 tion. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36,
Myers, D.L., & Myers, L.E. (1977). Undermatching: a reap- 141-149. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1981.36-141
praisal of performance on concurrent variable- Wearden, J.H., & Burgess, I.S. (1982). Matching since
interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Baum (1979). Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 203-214. https:// Behavior, 38, 339-348. https://doi.org/10.1901/
doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1977.27-203 jeab.1982.38-339
Zentall, T. R. (2014). Suboptimal choice by pigeons: An
Rachlin, H. (1997). Four teleological theories of addiction. analog of human gambling behavior. Behavioral Pro-
Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 4, 462-473. https:// cesses, 103, 156-164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.
doi.org/10.3758/bf03214335 2014.03.001
Spetch, M.L., Belke, T.W., Barnet, R.C., Dunn, R., & Zentall, T.R. (2016a). When humans and other animals
Pierce, W.D. (1990). Suboptimal choice in a behave irrationally. Comparative Cognition & Behavior
percentage-reinforcement procedure: Effects of signal Reviews, 11, 25-48. https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2016.
condition and terminal link length. Journal of the 110002
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 53, 219-234. https:// Zentall, T.R. (2016b). Resolving the paradox of subopti-
doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1990.53-219 mal choice. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal
Staddon, J.E., & Motheral, S. (1978). On matching and Learning & Cognition, 42, 1-14. https://doi.org/
maximizing in operant choice experiments. Psychologi- 10.1037/xan0000085
cal Review, 85, 436-444. https://doi.org/10.1037// Zentall, T.R., & Laude, J.R. (2013). Do pigeons gamble? I
0033-295x.85.5.436 wouldn’t bet against it. Current Directions in Psychologi-
Taylor, R., & Davison, M. (1983). Sensitivity to reinforce- cal Science, 22, 271-277. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963
ment in concurrent arithmetic and exponential 721413480173
schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Zentall, T. R., & Stagner, J. (2011). Maladaptive choice
Behavior, 39, 191-198. https://doi.org/10.1901/ behavior by pigeons: an animal analogue and possible
jeab.1983.39-191 mechanism for gambling (suboptimal human
Tonneau, F. (2005). Windows. Behavioral Processes, 69, 237- decision-making behavior). Proceedings of the Royal Soci-
247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.007 ety of London B: Biological Sciences, 278, 1203-1208.
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1607
Tunney, R. J., & Shanks, D. R. (2002). A re-examination of
melioration and rational choice. Journal of Behavioral
Decision Making, 15, 291-311. https://doi.org/ Received: June 18, 2017
10.1002/bdm.415 Final Acceptance: March 20, 2018

You might also like