You are on page 1of 8

European Journal of Sport Science

ISSN: 1746-1391 (Print) 1536-7290 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tejs20

Bilateral practice improves dominant leg


performance in long jump

Anne Focke, Sina Spancken, Christian Stockinger, Benjamin Thürer &


Thorsten Stein

To cite this article: Anne Focke, Sina Spancken, Christian Stockinger, Benjamin Thürer &
Thorsten Stein (2016): Bilateral practice improves dominant leg performance in long jump,
European Journal of Sport Science, DOI: 10.1080/17461391.2016.1141996

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2016.1141996

Published online: 10 Feb 2016.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 77

View related articles

View Crossmark data

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=tejs20

Download by: [University of Coruna] Date: 02 March 2016, At: 03:35


European Journal of Sport Science, 2016
http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17461391.2016.1141996

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Bilateral practice improves dominant leg performance in long jump

ANNE FOCKE, SINA SPANCKEN, CHRISTIAN STOCKINGER, BENJAMIN THÜRER, &


THORSTEN STEIN

BioMotion Center, Institute of Sports and Sports Science, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Karlsruhe, Germany

Abstract
Benefits of bilateral practice both for the non-dominant and for the dominant body side have been shown in several studies.
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

Thereby, most of the studies included movement tasks of the upper extremity or investigated sports games in which the ability
of acting bilaterally is an essential basis for success and, thus, a bilateral practice is reasonable anyway. Individual unilaterally
performed sports including movement tasks of the lower extremity are rarely investigated. Therefore, the aim of our study was
to test if contralateral transfer due to bilateral practice can be found in an unilaterally performed sport including the lower
extremity. We trained and tested 61 adolescent athletes in long jump to compare the jumping performance of the
dominant leg after a 12-week practice period between two groups: a bilateral practice group that practiced specific long
jump exercises with both the dominant and non-dominant leg and an unilateral practice group that practiced specific long
jump exercises only with the dominant leg. Results showed a superior effect of bilateral practice compared to unilateral
practice regarding the jumping performance of the dominant leg. The performance increase at post-test and retention-test
for the dominant limb was significantly higher for the bilateral practice group (pre-to-post: 5.2%, pre-to-retention: 7.4%)
compared to the unilateral practice group (pre-to-post: 3.4%, pre-to-retention: 4.5%). Thus, bilateral practice should be
established in the early practice programmes of track and field athletes to improve the performance of the dominant take-
off leg.

Keywords: Bilateral transfer; lower extremity; contralateral transfer; crosslimb education; bilateral training

Introduction
Silva, & Carvalho, 2003), and therefore produces
Humans show a more or less pronounced motor- an advantage for the player’s ability of situation-
functional laterality that is reflected in a preferred specific actions. Interestingly, bilateral practice can
body side (dominant side). Additionally, humans also be beneficial for skills or sports that are per-
exhibit most often a better performance with the formed unilaterally like throwing (basketball, hand-
dominant side compared to the non-dominant side. ball) or table tennis. Here bilateral practice seems to
Acquisition and optimisation of motor skills is there- be beneficial not only for the non-dominant but
fore mostly performed with that dominant side. also for the dominant side (Maurer, 2005; Stöckel
However, a situation-specific action not only with & Weigelt, 2012). This is based on a positive learning
the dominant but also with the non-dominant side transfer between matched limbs, and is called con-
of the body is crucial for many sports games like bas- tralateral transfer (Magill, 2007).
ketball, football, handball, or volleyball (Grouis, The fact that initial practice of a motor task with
Kiodou, Tsorbatzoudis, & Alexandris, 2004). A one body side leads to an improvement in subsequent
player’s ability of acting bilaterally is an essential con- performance with the other body side was shown in
dition to be successful under time or opponent many studies that investigated upper limb move-
pressure (Stöckel, Weigelt, & Krug, 2011). Thus, ments in the laboratory (Bagesteiro & Sainburg,
an early practice of the non-dominant body side is 2002, 2003; Criscimagna-Hemminger, Donchin,
beneficial to improve the performance of this non- Gazzaniga, & Shadmehr, 2003; Sainburg & Wang,
dominant side (Haaland & Hoff, 2003; Teixeira, 2002; Senff & Weigelt, 2011; Wang & Sainburg,

Correspondence: Anne Focke, BioMotion Center, Institute of Sports and Sports Science, Karlsruhe Institute of Technology, Engler-Bunte-
Ring 15, 76131 Karlsruhe, Germany. E-mail: anne.focke@kit.edu

© 2016 European College of Sport Science


2 A. Focke et al.

2004a, 2004b, 2006, 2007). In addition to the studies While studies, that included movement tasks with
that included movement tasks of the upper extremity, the upper extremity, investigated both sports games
Stöckel and Wang (2011) also investigated contralat- (e.g. basketball) and sports that are performed unilat-
eral transfer of the lower limbs. They concluded that erally (e.g. table tennis), studies that included move-
the idea of contralateral transfer applies to both upper ment tasks of the lower extremity only investigated
and lower extremity. Even though, most of these lab- sports games in which the ability of acting bilaterally
oratory studies were conducted to investigate the is an essential condition to be successful and thus a
magnitude and direction of transfer to understand bilateral practice is reasonable anyway. We found
the underlying neural mechanisms that are respon- no study that investigated the effect of bilateral prac-
sible for such a learning transfer, the results are very tice in unilaterally performed sports including the
conflicting and thus the mechanisms are still not suf- lower limbs.
ficiently understood (Ruddy & Carson, 2013 for a Therefore, the aim of our study was to test if con-
review). tralateral transfer due to bilateral practice can be
Irrespective of the magnitude, the direction or the found in an unilaterally performed sport including
underlying mechanisms of contralateral transfer, the lower extremity – using the example of long
such a positive learning transfer might have a great jump – for subjects with previous experience in the
practical relevance for planning practice schedules movement task. As the drop, a performance-deter-
in different sports. However, only a few studies inves- mining factor in long jump, is an unilaterally per-
tigated this phenomenon in applied settings. Similar formed motor action we only investigated the
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

to the laboratory studies, most applied studies transfer from the non-dominant to the dominant leg
included movement tasks of the upper limbs (e.g. and compared a commonly used unilateral jumping
Maurer, 2005) and focused on specific questions practice with a bilateral jumping practice to find out
like sequential effects of bilateral practice (Stöckel if one method outmatches the other.
et al., 2011; Stöckel & Weigelt, 2012). In these
studies, only subjects who had no prior experience
with the task were included. Besides the fact that, Methods
similar to the laboratory studies, the magnitude and
direction of transfer was different for different move- Subjects
ment tasks, all studies showed positive learning The sample included 31 girls and 30 boys aged
effects with a bilateral practice for both the dominant between 6.75 and 13.50 years (mean ± SD: 9.66 ±
and non-dominant side. 1.95 years). Subjects were recruited from the athletic
In contrast, only a few studies investigated bilateral sports section of a local sports club. All subjects had
practice of sports skills for the lower extremities or at least one year experience in practicing athletics
included experienced athletes, and the few studies (mean ± SD: 3.6 ± 1.76 years) and mastered the
that were found indicate inconsistent results. Teix- main features of the long jump. Starting with 69 sub-
eira et al. (2003) investigated skilled adolescents in jects, eight subjects dropped out because they either
soccer tasks after a four-month practice period. In did not complete enough practice sessions (at least
one of three tasks (dribbling) the group that practiced 18 of the 24 sessions, n = 5) or did not complete all
with both the preferred and non-preferred limb test sessions (n = 3). No subject dropped out because
showed a superior performance with the non- of injury or pain. The dominant take-off leg in
preferred limb and a similar performance with the jumping was determined using an evaluation of the
preferred limb compared to the control group. jumping distance with each leg during the pre-test.
Thus, the bilateral practice group showed a similar Thereby, the leg yielding the better jumping perform-
performance of the dominant leg, even with ance in the pre-test was defined as dominant leg. By
reduced dominant leg training. In combination with the time of the study none of the children had experi-
the higher performance of the non-dominant leg, a ences in practicing long jump with the non-dominant
bilateral practice leads to a reduction of lateral asym- leg. Guardian consent to participate in the study was
metries. Haaland and Hoff (2003) found for competi- provided for all children and the test protocol was
tive soccer players that the group that practiced an approved by the Institutional Review Board.
individual technical training eight weeks only with All subjects were divided into three age groups
the non-dominant leg improved their soccer perform- (U 10: under 10 years n = 24 , U 12: under 12 years
ance significantly as compared to the control group n = 19, U 14: under 14 years n = 18) and within
for both the non-dominant and dominant leg. each age group subjects were parallelised into two
Thus, a positive learning transfer to the dominant groups (unilateral, bilateral practice group) according
leg was found in the bilateral practice group. to their jumping performance with the dominant leg
Bilateral practice improves dominant leg performance in long jump 3
Table I. Distribution of the subjects among the groups

Age Practice n Gender

U 10 Unilateral 12 Female = 3, male = 9


Bilateral 12 Female = 3, male = 9
U 12 Unilateral 10 Female = 7, male = 3
Bilateral 9 Female = 5, male = 4
U 14 Unilateral 9 Female = 6, male = 3
Bilateral 9 Female = 8, male = 1
Total Unilateral 31 Female = 16, male = 15
Bilateral 30 Female = 16, male = 14

Note: U 10 = under 10 years, U 12 = under 12 years, U 14 = under 14 years.

in the pre-test. Thus, the jumping performance of the new exercises for the specific long jump practice
corresponding two groups was similar. For the prac- were used. Each group (unilateral or bilateral) com-
tice within each age group the first group passed the pleted the same practice workload whereas the unilat-
whole practice period only with the dominant take- eral group performed all exercises of the specific long
off leg (total n = 31, 16 girls and 15 boys) while the jump practice with their dominant leg and the bilat-
second group completed a bilateral jumping practice eral group alternated between the dominant and
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

(total n = 30, 16 girls and 14 boys) (Table I). non-dominant leg in a randomised order. All other
exercises (coordination, stabilisation, sprinting)
were practiced leg independent. To avoid overtrain-
Experimental design and data collection ing, the bilateral practice group did not practice
The investigation extended over a period of 15 weeks 50% with their non-dominant leg from the beginning
(September to January), whereby the jumping prac- of the training period, even though an equal distri-
bution of dominant and non-dominant long jump
tice was organised over a 12-week period. Pre-test
was conducted at the beginning of the practice practice was aspired (Figure 1). Within the first six
period and a post-test was conducted at the end of weeks, only 25% of the practice was performed with
the non-dominant leg. After six weeks, practice with
the 12-week practice period. A retention-test after
another three weeks without practice completed the the non-dominant leg was increased up to 75%
data collection. (Figure 1). Thus, on average the amount of practice
for the dominant and non-dominant leg within the
Each test consisted of six long jumps, three with
each leg. Jumping order between legs was random- 12-week practice period was the same. Additionally,
ised within the tests and all children were instructed all training sessions were planned with the child’s ath-
letic coach to guarantee adequate exercises and to
to jump as long as possible. Thereby, jumping dis-
tance was determined from take-off of the foot to prevent an overtraining of the children. The amount
exclude effects regarding the ability to exactly hit of exercises and jump variations performed within
one training session did not exceed the amount of
the board. For each leg the best jump out of three
was taken for further analysis. Subjects had to com- exercises and jumps performed in the training sessions
plete all three test sessions to be included in the study. prior to the study. Specifically, about 10 jump vari-
ations per session were performed with the youngest
subjects and about 15 jump variations per session
Practice were performed with the oldest subjects.

Practice sessions took place two times a week (24


practice sessions) for 1.5 hours on each day
Statistical analysis
(Tuesday and Friday) and were conducted in a
sports hall to minimise injury risks caused by bad All statistical tests were performed using SPSS 19.0.
weather during the enquiry period. At least, 18 of A multi-factorial ANOVA, 2 (practice group: unilat-
24 practice sessions had to be completed for inclusion eral vs. bilateral) × 3 (age group: U 10 vs. U 12 vs. U
into the study. Subjects separately practiced within 14) × 3 (time: pre vs. post vs. retention), with
each of the three age groups (U 10, U 12, U 14) to repeated measurements on the last factor time was
ensure an adequate level of performance within the conducted for the jumping performance of the domi-
practice sessions. Each practice session consisted of nant leg. To detect differences between the different
a specific long jump practice combined with exercises test times, post-hoc two-way ANOVAs, 2 (practice
of coordination, stabilisation, and sprinting. Only group: unilateral vs. bilateral) × 2 (time: test time a
4 A. Focke et al.

Figure 1. Experimental design for the unilateral and bilateral practice groups over the training and retention period (T1: pre-test, T2: post-
test, T3: retention-test). DL indicates dominant leg and NDL non-dominant leg.

vs. test time b), with repeated measures on the factor Table II(b), but no further analyses are presented on
time were conducted between pre- and post-test as that data in this section. The 2 × 3 × 3 ANOVA for
well as between post- and retention-test. Thereby, the dominant leg data revealed a significant time
all age groups were analysed together because no sig- effect (F2,110 = 156.47, p < .001, h2p = 0.740), a sig-
nificant interaction between time and age group was nificant age group effect (F2,55 = 93.83, p < .001,
found in the overall ANOVA. Finally, post-hoc t- h2p = 0.773) and no significant practice group effect
tests were conducted to reveal significant differences (F1,55 = 0.67, p = .416, h2p = 0.012). All subjects
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

between test times within each practice group. Effect improved their jumping performance from pre- over
sizes were determined using partial eta squared (h2p ). post- to the retention-test and the jumping perform-
According to Cohen (1988), large magnitudes of ance increased with increasing age. The significant
effects were h2p = 0.14, medium-sized effects were time × practice group interaction (F2,110 = 10.97,
h2p = 0.06, and small effects were h2p = 0.01 (Richard- p < .001, h2p = 0.166) showed a higher increase of
son, 2011). The level of significance for all tests was jumping performance for the bilateral (pre-to-post:
set a priori to 0.05. If sphericity was violated Green- 5.2%, pre-to-retention: 7.4%) practice group com-
house–Geisser correction was used. When multiple pared to the unilateral practice group (pre-to-post:
analyses were conducted (post-hoc 2 × 2 ANOVAs, 3.4%, pre-to-retention: 4.5%). The interaction
post-hoc t-tests), Holm–Bonferroni procedure between time and age group was not significant
(sequentially rejective Bonferroni test; Holm, 1979) (F4,110 = 1.63, p = .172, h2p = 0.056) indicating a
was used to adjust the p-values. similar development for all age groups over time.
Thus, for further post-hoc analyses (2 × 2 ANOVAs,
t-tests) no distinction was made between the three
age groups.
Results
The post-hoc 2 × 2 ANOVA between pre- and post-
Jumping distances of the dominant leg for all subject test showed a significant time effect (F1,59 = 159.56,
groups and all tests are shown in Table II(a). p < .001, h2p = 0.730), no significant practice group
Additionally, jumping distances of the non-dominant effect (F1,59 = 0.08, p = .775, h2p = 0.001), and a sig-
leg for both practice groups and all tests are shown in nificant interaction between time and practice

Figure 2. Jumping distance of the dominant leg (mean ± CI95) for all subjects separated for the two practice groups (unilateral and bilateral).
Hash (#) indicates a significant time effect, dagger (†) indicates a significant interaction between time and practice group, asterisk (∗ ) indicates
significant differences between test times within each practice group.
Bilateral practice improves dominant leg performance in long jump 5
Table II. Jumping distance (m) of the (a) dominant leg and (b) non-dominant leg (mean, CI95, and difference between test times) for all three
age groups and both practice groups (unilateral and bilateral)

Pre-test Post-test Retention-test

Δ post/
Mean CI95 Mean CI95 Δ pre/post Mean CI95 retention

(a) Dominant
Age Practice n
U 10 Unilateral 12 2.22 0.20 2.29 0.20 0.08 2.32 0.19 0.02
Bilateral 12 2.23 0.18 2.36 0.22 0.13 2.39 0.20 0.04
U 12 Unilateral 10 2.59 0.16 2.69 0.14 0.10 2.74 0.15 0.05
Bilateral 9 2.61 0.14 2.77 0.16 0.15 2.85 0.15 0.08
U 14 Unilateral 9 3.41 0.16 3.50 0.15 0.09 3.53 0.18 0.02
Bilateral 9 3.42 0.14 3.56 0.15 0.14 3.65 0.15 0.09
Total Unilateral 31 2.68 0.20 2.77 0.20 0.09∗ 2.80 0.20 0.03∗
#† #†
Bilateral 30 2.70 0.20 2.84 0.21 0.14∗ 2.90 0.21 0.06∗
(b) Non-dominant
Age Practice n
U 10 Unilateral 12 2.00 0.19 2.10 0.18 0.10 2.14 0.19 0.04
Bilateral 12 2.03 0.24 2.21 0.26 0.18 2.26 0.26 0.05
U 12 Unilateral 10 2.49 0.23 2.59 0.15 0.10 2.64 0.15 0.05
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

Bilateral 9 2.50 0.23 2.70 0.20 0.20 2.75 0.19 0.05


U 14 Unilateral 9 3.07 0.20 3.24 0.16 0.18 3.31 0.17 0.07
Bilateral 9 3.09 0.16 3.39 0.10 0.31 3.46 0.11 0.07
Total Unilateral 31 2.47 0.19 2.59 0.19 0.12∗ 2.64 0.20 0.05∗
#† #
Bilateral 30 2.49 0.20 2.71 0.21 0.22∗ 2.77 0.22 0.06∗

Note: U 10 = under 10 years, U 12 = under 12 years, U 14 = under 14 years. Results of post-hoc 2 × 2 ANOVAs for pooled age groups (Total):
hash (#) indicates a significant time effect; dagger (†) indicates a significant interaction between time and practice group. Results of the post-
hoc t-tests: asterisk (∗ ) indicates significant differences between pre- and post-test and between post- and retention-test.

group (F1,59 = 8.87, p = .008, h2p = 0.131). Similar jumping practice to find out if one method out-
results were found for the 2 × 2 ANOVA between matches the other. Our results show a superior
post- and retention-test (time: F1,59 = 41.94, effect of bilateral practice compared to unilateral
p < .001, h2p = 0.415, practice group: F1,59 = 0.33, practice after a 12-week practice programme. The
p > .999, h2p = 0.005, time × practice group: F1,59 = performance increase at post-test and retention-test
4.51, p = .038, h2p = 0.071) (Figure 2). Thus, subjects for the dominant limb was significantly higher for
increased their jumping performance from pre- to the group that practiced equivalent with the domi-
post-test as well as from post- to retention-test. nant and non-dominant limb compared to the
This increase of performance was higher between group that practiced only with the dominant limb.
pre- and post-test than between post- and reten- Thereby, the higher performance increase of the
tion-test (unilateral: t30 = 3.80, p = .001; bilateral: bilateral practice group was more pronounced
t29 = 3.87, p = .001). The bilateral practice group between pre- and post-test than between post- and
increased its performance more than the unilateral retention-test.
practice group. Post-hoc t-tests showed a significant To our knowledge, this is the first study that inves-
improvement of jumping performance for both tigated the effect of bilateral practice of an unilaterally
groups between pre- and post-, as well as between performed sport including the lower extremity. Our
post- and retention-test (pre-test to post-test unilat- findings support the results of Haaland and Hoff
eral: t30 = −7.11, p < .001; pre-test to post-test bilat- (2003) who found a positive effect of non-dominant
eral: t29 = −10.61, p < .001; post-test to retention- limb practice on the performance of the dominant
test unilateral: t30 = −3.63, p = .001; post-test to limb in soccer-specific dribbling, shooting, and
retention-test bilateral: t29 = −5.33, p < .001). passing tasks. In contrast, Teixeira et al. (2003) ana-
lysed the effect of bilateral practice on soccer-specific
dribbling and kicking tasks after a four-month prac-
tice programme and found a significant benefit of
Discussion bilateral practice only for the non-dominant but not
The aim of our study was to compare a commonly for the dominant limb. As they included only subjects
used unilateral jumping practice with a bilateral that had extensive previous soccer experience
6 A. Focke et al.

between four and six years, the practice might have available via callosal transfer. This enables access to
been too unspecific to reach a performance progress the motor engrams during movements with the
also for the dominant limb. However, this remains unpractised limb (Ruddy & Carson, 2013). If and
speculative as Haaland and Hoff (2003) also included how these theoretical models can explain the benefit
experienced soccer players and found positive effects of bilateral practice in long jump remains unclear as
of non-dominant leg practice for both legs after an these theories are mainly based on studies including
even shorter practice programme of eight weeks. simple movement tasks of the upper extremity like
Our results are also in line with several previous adaptation of reaching and pointing movements to
studies including movement tasks involving the visuo-motor perturbations (Sainburg & Wang,
upper extremity (for a summary, see Maurer, 2002; Wang & Sainburg, 2004a, 2006, 2007) or
2005). Thus, a bilateral practice seems to be ben- dynamic perturbations (Bagesteiro & Sainburg,
eficial for sports including both the upper and the 2002; Criscimagna-Hemminger et al., 2003; Stockin-
lower extremity. ger, Thürer, Focke, & Stein, 2015; Wang & Sain-
Bilateral practice thereby seems to be beneficial for burg, 2004b), and are most likely not transferable
several reasons. The transfer of learning caused by to complex sports movements including the lower
bilateral practice can prevent an increment of lateral extremity (Wulf & Shea, 2002).
asymmetries and thus compensate for performance Due to our study design no assumptions can be
differences between the two body sides (Teixeira made regarding the optimal bilateral practice order
et al., 2003). This can be crucial especially for or the optimal time at which a bilateral practice
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

sports games in which the inclusion of the non-domi- should be started. Previous studies showed a larger
nant body side offers an extension of possible moves learning gain for subjects that started learning a new
in a match. As practice of the non-dominant side can task with the non-dominant side before changing to
also improve the performance of the dominant side, the dominant side compared to subjects that started
practice with the non-dominant side is useful learning with the dominant side before changing to
during an injury-break, a blockade due to technical the non-dominant side (Stöckel et al., 2011). In
difficulties, or to avoid overtraining. Above all, the addition, an early beginning of bilateral practice
current and most of the previous studies showed seems to be beneficial compared to a delayed start
that with similar practice effort the performance of bilateral practice (Maurer, 2005). However, these
increase is higher for bilateral compared to unilateral previous studies only investigated movement tasks
practice. Thus, a bilateral practice is not only ben- including the upper limbs. Thus, further research is
eficial during practice periods in which the dominant needed to confirm findings of these previous studies
side cannot be practiced but also within the scope of (Maurer, 2005; Stöckel et al., 2011) and transfer
regular practice sessions. Additionally, this positive them to different movement tasks also of the lower
practice effect seems to be relatively robust in our extremity.
study as the performance increase continued even Subjects of our study were aged between 6 and 13
after the 12-week practice period. Nevertheless, the years and were experienced in track and field training
benefit seems to be largest directly after the practice for 3.6 years on average. Thus, our results are not
period as the performance increase was less pro- necessarily valid for adults or subjects with less or
nounced between post- and retention-test. more experience in the tested movement task.
Although, possible explanations for the benefit of Although previous studies showed bilateral practice
bilateral practice are discussed in the literature, the was beneficial for adolescent and adult novices in
underlying mechanisms have still not been fully the upper extremity (Maurer, 2005; Stöckel et al.,
clarified. The cross-activation model (Parlow & 2011; Stöckel & Weigelt, 2012), we can only
Kinsbourne, 1989) states that “bilateral cortical assume that bilateral practice in the long jump
activity generated during unilateral training drives would be beneficial for novices of different ages.
concurrent neural adaptations in both cerebral hemi- However, since basic sport skills are taught at young
spheres” (Ruddy & Carson, 2013, p. 2). Thus, unilat- ages, the tested subject group of children and adoles-
eral practice leads to a weakened copy of movement cents seems to be an interesting group of learners and
representation in the hemisphere that normally con- thus the results of the current study have a high prac-
trols the muscles of the opposite limb (Ruddy & tical relevance.
Carson, 2013). In contrast, the bilateral access model In summary, our study showed superiority of bilat-
originally proposed by Taylor and Heilmann (1980) eral compared to unilateral practice in long jump for
states that motor engrams formed during unilateral children and adolescents of medium proficiency
practice are either established in central brain level. Irrespective of the theoretical approaches
regions that are responsible for both sides or are explaining the phenomenon of bilateral transfer, the
established in the responsible hemisphere but results do have a practical relevance. Bilateral
Bilateral practice improves dominant leg performance in long jump 7

practice should be established in the early practice Ruddy, K. L., & Carson, R. G. (2013). Neural pathways mediating
programmes of track and field athletes not only to cross education of motor function. Frontiers in Human
Neuroscience, 7, 1–22.
improve the performance of the non-dominant leg Sainburg, R. L., & Wang, J. (2002). Interlimb transfer of visuo-
and reduce asymmetries but also to improve the per- motor rotations: Independence of direct and final end position
formance of the dominant take-off leg. information. Experimental Brain Research, 145, 437–447.
Senff, O., & Weigelt, M. (2011). Sequential effects after
practice with the dominant and non-dominant hand on the
Disclosure statement acquisition of a sliding task in schoolchildren. Laterality, 16,
227–239.
No potential conflict of interest was reported by the Stöckel, T., & Wang, J. (2011). Transfer of short-term motor
authors. learning across the lower limbs as a function of task conception
and practice order. Brain and cognition, 77, 271–279.
Stöckel, T., & Weigelt, M. (2012). Brain lateralisation and motor
References learning: Selective effects of dominant and non-dominant
Bagesteiro, L. B., & Sainburg, R. L. (2002). Handedness: hand practice on the early acquisition of throwing skills.
Dominant arm advantages in control of limb dynamics. Laterality, 17, 18–37.
Journal of Neurophysiology, 88, 2408–2421. Stöckel, T., Weigelt, M., & Krug, J. (2011). Acquisition of a
Bagesteiro, L. B., & Sainburg, R. L. (2003). Non-dominant arm complex basketball-dribbling task in school children as a func-
advantages in load compensation during rapid elbow joint tion of bilateral practice order. Research Quarterly for Exercise
movements. Journal of Neurophysiology, 90, 1503–1513. and Sport, 82, 188–197.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Stockinger, C., Thürer, B., Focke, A., & Stein, T. (2015).
Intermanual transfer characteristics of dynamic learning: direc-
Downloaded by [University of Coruna] at 03:35 02 March 2016

(2nd ed.). New York, NY: Academic Press.


Criscimagna-Hemminger, S. E., Donchin, O., Gazzaniga, M. S., & tion, coordinate frame, and consolidation of interlimb generaliz-
Shadmehr, R. (2003). Learned dynamics of reaching move- ation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 114(6), 3166–3176. doi:10.
ments generalize from dominant to nondominant arm. Journal 1152/jn.00727.2015.
of Neurophysiology, 89, 168–176. Taylor, G. H., & Heilmann, K. M. (1980). Left hemisphere motor
Grouis, G., Kiodou, I., Tsorbatzoudis, H., & Alexandris, K. dominance in right handers. Cortex, 16, 587–603.
(2004). Handedness in sport. Journal of Human Movement Teixeira, L. A., Silva, M. V., & Carvalho, M. A. (2003). Reduction
Studies, 43, 347–361. of lateral asymmetries in dribbling: The role of bilateral practice.
Haaland, E., & Hoff, J. (2003). Non-dominant leg training Laterality, 8, 53–65.
improves the bilateral motor performance in soccer players. Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2004a). Limitations in interlimb
Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 9, 179–184. transfer of visuomotor rotations. Experimental Brain Research,
Holm, S. (1979). A simple sequentially rejective multiple test pro- 155, 1–8.
cedure. Scandinavian Journal of Statistics, 6, 65–70. Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2004b). Interlimb transfer of novel
Magill, R. A. (2007). Motor learning and control – concepts and appli- inertial dynamics is asymmetrical. Journal of Neurophysiology,
cations. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. 92, 349–360.
Maurer, H. (2005). Beidseitiges Übensportmotorischer Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2006). Interlimb transfer of visuo-
Fertigkeiten [Bilateral practice of motor skills]. Zeitschrift für motor rotations depends on handedness. Experimental Brain
Sportpsychologie, 12, 93–99. Research, 175, 223–230.
Parlow, S. E., & Kinsbourne, M. (1989). Asymmetrical transfer of Wang, J., & Sainburg, R. L. (2007). The dominant and nondomi-
training between hands: Implications for interhemispheric com- nant arms are specialized for stabilizing different features of task
munication in normal brain. Brain and Cognition, 11, 98–113. performance. Experimental Brain Research, 178, 565–570.
Richardson, J. T. E. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as Wulf, G., & Shea, C. H. (2002). Principles derived from the study
measures of effect size in educational research. Educational of simple motor skills do not generalize to complex skill learn-
Research Review, 6, 135–147. ing. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 185–211.

You might also like