You are on page 1of 12

J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

DOI 10.1007/s10896-014-9588-3

RESEARCH ON ATTACHMENT STYLES IN RELATION TO ABUSE AND VIOLENCE

The Relationship Between Childhood Physical Abuse and Adult


Attachment Styles
Jo Ann M. Unger & Rayleen V. De Luca

Published online: 23 March 2014


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2014

Abstract The purpose of the study was to identify the inse- Physical abuse is generally defined as the non-accidental
cure adult attachment style associated with childhood physical injury of a child inflicted by a caregiver (Crosson-Tower
abuse. Five hundred fifty-two female and 294 male university 2005). Injuries range from mild bruising to broken bones,
students completed questionnaires on their child abuse history, skull fractures, and even fatalities (Kolko 2002). Some exam-
adult attachment style, and other variables. Regression analy- ples of short-term consequences of physical abuse include
ses were completed. A history of physical abuse was associ- injuries such as fractures sustained from the abuse (Hoskote
ated with attachment avoidance while controlling for other et al. 2003), cognitive or intellectual deficits, social skill
forms of childhood abuse. Some support was also found for an deficits, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and other psy-
association between physical abuse and attachment anxiety. chiatric disorders (Kolko 2002), heightened aggressive and
Social support, as a control variable, was found to be an related externalizing behaviors (Manly et al. 2001), anxiety,
important predictor of attachment avoidance and attachment risk-taking behavior, and suicidal behavior (Finzi et al. 2002).
anxiety. A history of childhood physical abuse has important Examples of long-term consequences of physical abuse in-
and specific impacts on intimate adult relationships with so- clude difficulty trusting others, low self-esteem, anxiety, phys-
cial support acting as a possible protective factor. ical problems, anger, internalization of aggression, depression,
interpersonal difficulties, and substance abuse (Crosson-Tow-
er 2005). Many of the effects of childhood physical abuse
Keywords Childhood physical abuse . Adult attachment
involve a negative impact on later adult relationships. One
style . Social support
way to conceptualize these effects is by using attachment
theory. The current work examines how childhood physical
North American child protection authorities receive millions abuse is related to attachment styles in adulthood.
of reports of child abuse and neglect on a yearly basis
(Runyon et al. 2006). While not all of these reports are
substantiated, there are many other cases left unreported Attachment Theory
(Runyon et al. 2006). It has been estimated that almost three
million children are being abused or neglected today in the Current conceptions of attachment are based on the work of
United States alone (Crosson-Tower 2005). Trocmé et al. John Bowlby and those who have since contributed to his
(2005) found 26,692 incidents of physical abuse and 19,787 attachment theory. Bowlby (1969) advocated that attachment
incidents of multiple forms of abuse over a 3 month sampling is an instinctual system of behaviors or a biological drive
period of cases reported to child welfare agencies in Canada which ensures children remain in close contact with their
(Trocmé et al. 2005). mothers to ensure safety. A secure attachment also allows
for the attachment figure or caregiver to act as a secure base
from which children can explore and learn about their envi-
J. A. M. Unger (*)
New Directions for Children, Youth, Adults and Families, ronment in times of safety (Hart et al. 2002). Bowlby (1969)
400-491 Portage Avenue, Winnipeg, Manitoba R3M 2E4, Canada stated that these early attachment experiences influenced the
e-mail: joannunger75@gmail.com development of internal working models, which would affect
J. A. M. Unger : R. V. De Luca
later interpersonal perceptions, attitudes, and expectations.
University of Manitoba, P404 Duff Roblin Bldg, 190 Dysart Rd, Research over the past 20 years has supported the stability
Winnipeg, MB R3T 2N2, Canada of attachment styles over time (Bartholomew and Shaver
224 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

LOW AVOIDANCE
1998; Shapiro and Levendosky 1999). Children internalize
information gleaned from interactions with their caregivers
which informs the nature of their relationships in adulthood SECURE PREOCCUPIED

(Muller et al. 2000). Children who experience consistent


parenting develop positive working models, secure attach-
ments, and a healthy capacity for intimacy in adulthood
(Swanson and Mallinckrodt 2001). In contrast, children who LOW HIGH
ANXIETY ANXIETY
experience inadequate parenting develop at least one negative
working model, an insecure attachment style, and experience
more interpersonal difficulties in adulthood (Swanson and
Mallinckrodt 2001). These internal working models, while
DISMISSING- FEARFUL-
modifiable, are generally resistant to change (Hazan and AVOIDANT AVOIDANT
Shaver 1994).
Theorists have extended the research with children to hy- HIGH
AVOIDANCE
pothesize that intimate relationships in adulthood are also
Fig. 1 From “Adult romantic attachment: Theoretical developments,
attachment relationships and elicit attachment processes. The emerging controversies, and unanswered questions,” by R.C. Fraley and
biological function of a romantic attachment relationship is to P.R. Shaver, 2000, Review of General Psychology, 4(2), 132–154. Copy-
assure procreation and the long-term commitment of two right 2000 by the American Psychological Association
adults to care for the children (Shaver et al. 1988). Both
childhood and adult attachment relationships are thought to Physical Abuse and Attachment Theory
be governed by the same biological system (Fraley and Shaver
2000). This can be seen in the similarities in behaviors and Research in the area of child maltreatment and adult attach-
interactions in the two sets of relationships. For example, both ment styles began approximately 20 years ago. The focus of
children and romantic partners feel a sense of safety and childhood physical abuse and attachment research has been
security in the presence of their attachment figure (Fraley primarily on child attachment styles as opposed to adult
and Shaver 2000). Research by Brennan et al. (1998) has attachment styles. A number of studies have also been com-
shown that a two-dimensional model best fits the data regard- pleted combining multiple types of child abuse into one group
ing adult romantic attachment styles, and they have proposed and comparing them to a non-abused sample.
that the dimensions be labeled anxiety and avoidance based on As has already been stated, it has been found that those
their extensive review of the empirical literature. The anxiety who have experienced child maltreatment tend to have diffi-
dimension refers to individuals’ sensitivity to detecting and culties in their adult relationships. For example, physical,
perceiving threats to their security or cues of rejection (Fraley sexual, and emotional abuse have been related to negative
and Shaver 2000). The avoidance dimension refers to the functioning in adult romantic relationships (McCarthy and
degree to which one will seek contact and support or withdraw Taylor 1999). Undergraduate student victims of verbal, phys-
from others when faced with a threatening situation (Fraley ical, and sexual abuse were found to use more insults and
and Shaver 2000). physical violence in their relationships with their romantic
The two-dimensional structure creates four quadrants of partners than non-abused participants (Styron and Janoff-
attachment styles, similar to the quadrants described for infant Bulman 1997). These disruptions in relationships point to
attachment (Fig. 1). Low levels of anxiety and avoidance the possible existence of insecure attachment styles in
result in a secure attachment style. These individuals are not maltreated individuals. In fact, this has been supported in a
overly sensitive to threats of rejection and use attachment number of studies comparing abused with non-abused chil-
figures for comfort and support. High levels of anxiety and dren and adult participants. Consistently, participants who
low levels of avoidance result in a preoccupied attachment experienced abuse showed more insecure attachment styles
style. These individuals may be highly sensitive to cues of than participants who had not experienced abuse (e.g., Muller
rejection while at the same time desiring closeness to and et al. 2000, 2001).
dependence on others. Low levels of anxiety and high levels A number of studies have focused on assessing the attach-
of avoidance result in a dismissing-avoidant attachment style. ment style of physically abused children. Consistent with
These individuals are not overly sensitive to fears of rejection previously mentioned research, physically abused children
but avoid using others as sources of comfort and support. High tend to have higher rates of insecure attachment, most often
levels of anxiety and high levels of avoidance result in a anxious/avoidant style, and lower rates of secure attachment
fearful-avoidant attachment style. These individuals experi- than non-abused children (Finzi et al. 2000, 2001, 2002). In
ence both a high sensitivity to cues of rejection and avoidance two reviews, Crittenden (1988; Crittenden and Ainsworth
of others for comfort and support. 1989) outlined two distinct patterns of childhood responses
J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234 225

to physical abuse found in her research. One typical response does the avoidant style identified by Finzi et al. (2000), 2002,
involved the physically abused child becoming excessively 2001). Therefore, it was hypothesized that adults who were
compliant to parental requests. This behavior likely reduced physically abused as children would score highly on the
the probability of receiving abuse from parents but also may avoidance scale, that is, they would show higher rates of the
have led the children to view their worth as dependent on the fearful-avoidant and dismissing-avoidant styles of attachment.
expectations of others and a vulnerability to self-doubt when
others were displeased. The second type of response to phys-
ical abuse was becoming angry and acting out against parents. Control Variables
This behavior likely increased the probability of abuse and
relational problems in the future, as an expectation of harm Research in the area of childhood abuse has shown high
from others was learned and acted upon. However, children correlations between the various types of abuse including
who acted out were likely able to develop and maintain a physical abuse, sexual abuse, psychological maltreatment or
sense of self which the compliant children were not able to emotional abuse, and neglect (e.g., McCarthy and Taylor
find (Crittenden 1988; Crittenden and Ainsworth 1989). 1999; Styron and Janoff-Bulman 1997; Unger 2004). There-
Additionally, a number of studies have used older children fore, it is difficult to study the unique effect of physical abuse.
as participants in this area of research. In one longitudinal In the current study, an attempt was made to identify cases of
study by Sternberg et al. (2005), adolescents who were victims pure physical abuse. However, not enough participants were
of parental abuse had significantly lower levels of attachment found to complete the relevant analyses. Therefore, the vari-
to their parents than non-abused adolescents or adolescents ables of sexual abuse, psychological maltreatment, and ne-
who had witnessed physical abuse between their parents. A glect were included as control variables in the analyses.
series of studies by Finzi and associates analyzed the attach- Social support is an important variable in the area of abuse
ment styles of abused older children in Israel, ages 6 to and attachment. Social support is often associated with more
13 years (Finzi et al. 2000, 2001, 2002). In two separate positive outcomes for abuse victims (Fleming et al. 1997;
samples, researchers found that 85 % of physically abused Unger 2004). For example, social support has been found to
children had an avoidant style of attachment using a three- have a direct positive effect on physical abuse outcomes
category model of attachment (avoidant, anxious/ambivalent, (McLewin and Muller 2006). More specifically, studies with
and secure). This high rate of avoidant attachment in the women who were physically or sexually abused as children
physically abused group was significantly higher than the have found an association between increased social support
rates found in the neglected and non-abused groups. It was and lower levels of depressive symptoms (Banyard 1999;
hypothesized that the avoidant strategy allowed children to Hobfoll et al. 2002). In addition, social support would likely
deactivate their proximity seeking behaviors in order to avoid influence adult attachment style as it qualifies as a corrective
possible dangerous consequences (Finzi et al. 2002). social experience. Positive social experiences later in life are
thought to influence the early negative effect of abuse on
attachment styles (Hazan and Shaver 1994). Therefore, it
Hypotheses was important that social support be controlled in order to
discover the hypothesized effects, independent of the influ-
Physical abuse is consistently related to an insecure attach- ence of social support.
ment style. In children, there is evidence of an increased rate Gender is another variable that required inclusion in the
of an avoidant attachment style (Finzi et al. 2000, 2001, 2002) proposed analyses. First, while some studies have found no
with two distinct types of responding: compliant and aggres- gender differences in attachment style (Lapsley et al. 2000;
sive (Crittenden 1988; Crittenden and Ainsworth 1989). Shi 2003), others have found females to be more secure than
While few published studies have analyzed the relationship males (Brennan et al. 1991; Matsuoka et al. 2006) and males
between adult attachment and physical abuse, no specific more likely than females to be dismissing. However, females
insecure attachment styles had been found for adults physi- are more likely than males to be fearful (Brennan et al. 1991)
cally abused as children at the initiation of this study. Consis- when using the Bartholomew adult attachment model.
tency has been found between childhood and adult attachment Second, while some studies have shown that abuse out-
styles (Bartholomew and Shaver 1998; Shapiro and comes do not differ depending on gender, others have found
Levendosky 1999) and was expected due to the theorized significant gender differences in outcomes and abusive expe-
development of internal working models of self and other riences. Studies using the 8,000 male and 8,000 female re-
(Bowlby 1969). Therefore, hypotheses about adult attachment sponses to the National Violence Against Women Survey
styles were made based on evidence found in childhood. The have found both similarities and differences in their experi-
compliant and aggressive response patterns outlined by ences of childhood abuse. Men were more likely than women
Crittenden (1988) both involve a negative view of others as to have experienced physical abuse (Pimlott-Kubiak and
226 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

Cortina 2003; Thompson et al. 2004). However, Pimlott- deep down.” An example of an anxiety item is, “I worry about
Kubiak and Cortina (2003) found that the experience of being abandoned.”
exposure to aggressive situations was related to psychological The ECR was developed through a factor analysis of 323
and physical distress in both genders at comparable levels. attachment related constructs found in a detailed literature
Similar findings were reported by Shack and associates in a review (Brennan et al. 1998). This analysis resulted in two
smaller sample of psychiatric patients (Shack et al. 2004). independent factors corresponding to the avoidance and anx-
The preceding evidence points to the complexity and im- iety dimensions. The scale originators found that the avoid-
portance of gender in the area of physical abuse and attach- ance scale correlated highly with other scales measuring
ment. While the literature base does not allow for specific avoidance and discomfort with closeness, and the anxiety
hypotheses to be made, it does support the inclusion of the scale correlated highly with scales measuring anxiety, preoc-
variable within the current study. Therefore, gender was con- cupation with attachment, jealousy, and fear of rejection.
trolled for in the main analyses. Brennan et al. (1998) also reported high internal consistency
for both scales (avoidance alpha = 0.94 and anxiety alpha =
0.91).
Method In the current study, as recommended by the scale creators,
a total score for each dimension was created by calculating the
Participants mean score for the questions designed to assess that dimension
after reversal of the negatively worded questions. This result-
Participants for this study included 552 female and 294 male ed in a range of one to seven for each dimension score. A
(two participants declined to identify their sex) university measure of internal consistency was calculated resulting in a
students recruited from the Introductory Psychology classes Cronbach’s alpha of 0.91 for the entire scale, 0.92 for the
at the University of Manitoba. They received course credit for avoidance dimension, and 0.90 for the anxiety dimension.
their voluntary participation. Participants were limited to those
who were fluent in English and were 18 years of age or older. Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support The
Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support
(MSPSS; Zimet et al. 1988) was selected as the measure of
Procedure social support due to its strong psychometric properties. Fac-
tor analysis was used to create this 12-item scale, which
Participants completed a number of questionnaires designed assesses social support from family, friends, and significant
to tap the variables of interest. The questionnaires were ad- others. Using a seven-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very
ministered in group settings of approximately 10 to 300 par- strongly disagree) to 7 (very strongly agree), participants
ticipants per session. Five forms of the questionnaire booklet ranked their level of agreement to statements like “My family
were created in order to counterbalance the order effect of the really tries to help me,” “I can count on my friends when
questionnaires. For most participants, answering the question- things go wrong,” and “I have a special person who is a real
naires took approximately 35 min. source of comfort to me”. Initial internal reliability was found
to be strong with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from 0.85 to 0.91
Measures for the three factors and the total scale. Test-retest reliabilities
were also high, ranging from 0.72 to 0.85 (Zimet et al. 1988).
Demographics Questionnaire Participants completed a demo- In the current study, only the total score was used. It was
graphics questionnaire before completing the questionnaires created by calculating the mean of all of the items in the scale.
of interest. This provided basic information about the sample This resulted in a range of possible scores from 1 to 7. The
being studied and allowed for future analyses with other Cronbach’s alpha for the current study was found to be 0.94.
variables of interest. Examples of demographic questions
include age, gender, and ethnicity. Comprehensive Child Maltreatment Scale The Comprehen-
sive Child Maltreatment Scale (CCMS) assesses history of
Experiences in Close Relationships The Experiences in Close physical abuse, psychological maltreatment, witnessing vio-
Relationships Scale (ECR; Brennan et al. 1998) was selected lence, sexual abuse, and neglect resulting in a continuous
to assess the anxiety and avoidance dimensions of attachment score of abuse severity for total abuse and for each of the
in adulthood. Participants were asked to rate their level of trauma subtypes (Higgins and McCabe 2001). The use of a
agreement with 36 statements regarding their experiences in continuous measure allows for the measure of abuse severity
close relationships on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from (Higgins and McCabe 2001) and reduces random error, sys-
1 (disagree strongly) to 7 (agree strongly). An example of an tematic error, and response error (Hulme 2004). The CCMS
avoidance item is, “I prefer not to show a partner how I feel asks participants to rate how frequently they have experienced
J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234 227

a variety of abusive experiences by their mother, father, or The ECR, MSPSS, and CES measures involved calculating
others. For questions on physical abuse, psychological mal- a composite score or scores based on responses to multiple
treatment, and neglect a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 questions. If participants had fewer than 20 % of their re-
(never or almost never) to 5 (very frequently) was used. For sponses missing for the ECR, MSPSS and SRS, the missing
questions regarding sexual abuse, a six-point Likert scale data was replaced with the mean of the answered questions.
ranging from 1 (never to almost never) to 6 (more than 20 Totals were not calculated if participants had more than 20 %
times) was used. The questions regarding witnessing violence of responses missing for a questionnaire. If participants had
were not used in the current study. Some examples of ques- fewer than 20 % of their responses missing for the CES, the
tions include “Severely hurt you (requiring medical atten- missing data was replaced with a score of one, indicating no
tion)” and “Shut you in a room alone for an extended period abuse. One participant had more than 20 % of her responses
of time”. The title of the scale was changed in order to reduce missing on the CES. Given the importance of that question-
response bias by participants. Also, the format of the ques- naire to the analyses, all of that participant’s responses were
tionnaire was adapted to allow for ease of data collection. removed from the study.
While the CCMS has not received much attention in re-
search, the initial study by Higgins and McCabe (2001) Data Screening Procedures Multivariate outliers were ana-
showed promising psychometric results. Their Cronbach’s lyzed by using the Mahalanobis distance method
alphas ranged from 0.66 for the physical abuse subscale to (Tabachnick and Fidell 2007). A total of 34 cases were re-
0.93 for the total score. Test-retest reliability scores, after a 6- moved. Observations with z-scores larger than |3.29| were
to 8-week period, ranged from 0.62 for the neglect subscale to identified as univariate outliers. Outliers in the abuse variables
0.95 on the sexual abuse subscale. Concurrent and criterion were kept due to the high level of expected skew on those
related validity was assessed comparing the CCMS to the variables. Physical abuse had 12 outliers, psychological mal-
Child Abuse and Trauma Scale. The total scores correlated treatment had 8 outliers, neglect had 19 outliers, sexual
significantly at 0.86. While more research is needed, the abuse had 8 outliers, and total abuse had 7 outliers.
ability of the CCMS to measure multiple types of abuse, using For the other variables, the univariate outliers were
a continuous score, and its promising psychometric properties Windsorized. This was done by replacing the outlying data
made it an ideal choice for this study. with non-outlying values while retaining the order of the
A number of scores were calculated from the items of the outliers (Sexton et al. 2003). The avoidance variable had one
CCMS. Physical abuse, sexual abuse, psychological maltreat- outlier and the MSPSS variable had 13 outliers.
ment, and neglect scores were created by summing the rele-
vant items of the CCMS. Each score had a different possible Descriptive Statistics In order to better understand the sample
range because each abuse type had a different number of used in this study, a number of descriptive statistics were
relevant questions. The physical abuse score could range from collected. In terms of demographics of the sample, the major-
9 to 45, sexual abuse from 31 to 155, psychological maltreat- ity of the participants were female (65.6 %), 18 to 19 years old
ment from 9 to 45, neglect from 6 to 30, and total abuse from (61.1 %), single (76.6 %), and in their first year of university
57 to 285. Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for all scores in (73.3 %). Their families of origin tended to earn between
the current study. The Cronbach’s alpha for the physical abuse $40,000 and $100,000 annually (52.2 %), with both fathers
and neglect scores were only moderately strong at 0.75 and (64.9 %) and mothers (67.1 %) having some post-secondary
0.71, respectively. The sexual abuse, psychological maltreat- education.
ment, and total scores showed stronger internal consistency Descriptive statistics for the variables of interest can be
with Cronbach’s alphas of 0.92, 0.84, and 0.90 respectively. found in Table 1. They indicate that the participants rated
themselves as having low to moderate levels of attachment
avoidance and attachment anxiety, moderately high levels of
social support and self-esteem, and few psychological symp-
Results toms. In terms of abuse, this sample experienced low levels of
physical abuse, neglect, and sexual abuse and a low to mod-
Preliminary Analyses erate level of psychological maltreatment.
In order to check the relationship between the variables
Data Cleaning Procedures Appropriate data cleaning proce- and assess the level of multicollinearity a correlation matrix
dures were conducted in order to check for data errors. The was constructed (Table 2). Many correlations were signifi-
data was also checked for response sets or patterns of cant at the 0.05 level of significance, however there were no
responding, such as selecting the same answer for a whole signs of multicollinearity, evidenced by an absence of large r
questionnaire. Those with significant response sets on vital values. The abuse variables were all significantly correlated
scales were removed (six participants). with each other, with r’s ranging from 0.201 to 0.530
228 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for the variables of interest

Variable n Min Max M SD Skew

Avoidance 800 1.00 6.30 2.91 1.05 0.28


Anxiety 800 1.00 6.17 3.12 1.08 0.04
Social support 804 1.53 7.00 5.55 1.22 −1.33
Physical abuse 806 9.00 34.00 10.91 2.79 2.52
Psychological maltreatment 806 9.00 39.00 15.66 5.22 1.21
Neglect 804 6.00 26.00 7.21 2.28 2.74
Sexual abuse 805 31.00 134.00 31.84 4.49 15.76
Total abuse 803 57.00 235.00 68.87 11.73 4.53

The following are the values for each variable which indicate no endorsement of that variable: Avoidance = 1; Anxiety = 1; Social Support = 1; Physical
Abuse = 9; Psychological Maltreatment = 9; Neglect = 6; Sexual Abuse = 31; Total Abuse = 55

(p<0.001), indicating their need to be included as control correlation, physical abuse was not significantly corre-
variables. lated with attachment anxiety, r=0.160, p<0.001.
The correlations (Table 2) allow for a preliminary
review of the hypotheses. It was hypothesized that Type of Abuse and Attachment Style
physical abuse would significantly predict the level of
attachment avoidance. At the 0.05 level of significance, Regression Analyses In order to test the relationship between
physical abuse, r = 0.137, p < 0.001, was significantly physical abuse and adult attachment styles, while controlling
correlated with attachment avoidance. It was hypothe- for other types of abuse, sex, and social support, two regres-
sized that physical abuse would not significantly predict sion analyses were completed. Both were conducted using the
the level of attachment anxiety. At the level of simple 0.05 level of significance.

Table 2 Correlations of variables of interest

Variable Anxiety Sex Social support Physical abuse Psych. Mal. Neglect Sexual abuse Total abuse

Avoid. r=0.211 r=0.083 r=−0.292 r=0.137 r=0.072 r=0.066 r=0.086 r=0.108


p<0.001 p=0.019 p<0.001 p<0.001 p=0.043 p=0.062 p=0.015 p=0.002
n=800 n=798 n=796 n=798 n=798 n=796 n=797 n=795
Anxiety – r=−0.055 r=−0.112 r=0.160 r=0.273 r=0.107 r=0.003 r=0.205
p=0.124 p=0.002 p<0.001 p<0.001 p=0.003 p=0.937 p<0.001
n=798 n=796 n=798 n=798 n=796 n=797 n=795
Sex – r=−0.185 r=0.041 r=−0.018 r=0.086 r=-0.049 r=−0.002
p<0.001 p=0.247 p=0.605 p=0.014 p=0.164 p=0.960
n=802 n=804 n=804 n=802 n=803 n=801
Social support – r=−0.205 r=−0.196 r=−0.183 r=−0.142 r=−0.243
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001
n=802 n=802 n=800 n=801 n=799
Physical abuse – r=0.530 r=0.391 r=0.387 r=0.766
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001
n=806 n=804 n=805 n=803
Psych. Mal. – r=0.430 r=0.201 r=0.805
p<0.001 p<0.001 p<0.001
n=804 n=805 n=803
Neglect – r=0.330 r=0.648
p<0.001 p<0.001
n=803 n=803
Sexual abuse – r=0.655
p<0.001
n=803

Psych. Mal. Psychological Maltreatment; Avoid. Avoidance


J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234 229

Testing Regression Assumptions The regression assumptions Table 3 Regression model for attachment avoidance with control
variables
requiring assessment included normality, linearity, homosce-
dasticity of the variables, independence of errors, Model Predictor Standardized beta t p
multicollinearity, and singularity. The assumption of normality
was analyzed by calculating the skew for each variable in- 1 Constant 19.466 <0.001
volved in the subsequent regression analyses. Due to the nature Sex 0.030 0.859 0.391
of the abuse variables, a high level of skew was detected. Social support −0.280 −8.082 <0.001
Attempts made to transform the variables appropriately were 2 Constant 13.539 <0.001
unsuccessful. Regression analyses were completed with both Sex 0.029 0.847 0.397
the transformed variables and the non-transformed variables Social support −0.264 −7.473 <0.001
with minimal differences between them. The central limit Physical abuse 0.077 2.219 0.027
theorem states that the sampling distribution of means rapidly
n=792
approaches a normal distribution as sample size increases,
regardless of the shape of the parent population (Glass and
Hopkins 1996). Given the large sample size of approximately
789, the failure to meet this assumption should have little 10.443, p<0.001, R2 =0.038. The additional variance explained
impact on the results. Robustness studies have found that by physical abuse, added after the control variables, was also
violations to the normality assumption have inconsequential significant, F(1, 788) = 15.186, p<0.001, R2change = 0.019.
effects on the accuracy of the probability statements of the tests Social support, ra(b.c) = −0.097, p=0.005, sex, ra(b.c) = −0.080,
being used (Glass and Hopkins 1996). Considering the large p=0.022, and physical abuse, ra(b.c) = 0.136, p<0.001 were
sample size and that no meaningful differences were found significant individual predictors of attachment anxiety.
between the transformed and non-transformed variables, only
the analyses using the non-transformed variables are reported. Regression Analysis Including Control Variables and Other
The assumption of linearity was tested by creating bivariate Abuse Types Due to the high level of correlation between the
scatterplots between each combination of continuous indepen- abuse types and the low number of pure abuse type cases, the
dent and dependent variables. The desired oval shape of the previous regression analyses were completed including the
distributions was not found, as most of the variables were not other abuse types as control variables. For these analyses,
normal. However, there was no evidence of curvilinear rela- the control variables of sex and social support were entered
tionships between the variables. The assumptions of homo- first, the control abuse types of sexual abuse, neglect, and
scedasticity, independence of errors, multicollinearlity, and psychological maltreatment were entered second, and physi-
singularity were met. cal abuse was entered in the final step.
It was hypothesized that physical abuse would be able to
Regression Analysis Including Control Variables. In order to significantly predict attachment avoidance above and beyond
test the main hypotheses, regression analyses were completed the variance predicted by the control variables and other abuse
using attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety as depen- types. The final regression model (Table 5) was statistically
dent variables. In the first step of each analysis the control significant, F(6, 782) = 12.773, p<0.001, R2 =0.089. The
variables of sex and social support were entered. In the second added variance of physical abuse in the final step of the model
step of each analysis physical abuse was entered. contributed significantly to the model, F(1, 782) = 4.746, p=
It was hypothesized that physical abuse would be able to 0.030, R2 change = 0.006. In this model, only social support,
significantly predict attachment avoidance above and beyond ra(b.c) = −0.252, p<0.001, and physical abuse, ra(b.c) = 0.074,
the variance predicted by the control variables. The final p=0.030 were significant predictors of attachment avoidance.
regression model (Table 3) was statistically significant, F(3, It was hypothesized that physical abuse would not be able
788) = 25.360, p<0.001, R2 =0.088. The additional variance to significantly predict attachment anxiety above and beyond
explained by physical abuse, added after the control variables, the variance predicted by the control variables and other abuse
was also significant, F(1, 788) = 4.926, p=0.027, R2change = types. The final model for the regression analysis (Table 6) was
0.006. Both social support, ra(b.c) = −0.254, p<0.001, and statistically significant, F(6, 782) = 12.170, p<0.001, R2 =
physical abuse, ra(b.c) = 0.076, p=0.027, were significant 0.085. As expected, the additional variance added by physical
individual predictors of attachment avoidance. abuse, F(1, 782) = 0.864, p=0.353, was not significant when
It was hypothesized that physical abuse would not be able to added as the third step in the model. The variables of social
significantly predict attachment anxiety above and beyond the support, ra(b.c) = −0.076, p=0.027, sex, ra(b.c) = −0.068, p=
variance predicted by the control variables, as severity of abuse 0.047, psychological maltreatment, ra(b.c) = 0.196, p<0.001,
was not controlled by this analysis. However, the final regres- and sexual abuse, ra(b.c) = −0.069, p=0.043, were significant
sion model (Table 4) was statistically significant, F(3, 788) = predictors of attachment anxiety.
230 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

Table 4 Regression model for attachment anxiety with control variables Table 6 Regression model for attachment anxiety with control variables
and other abuse types
Model Predictor Standardized beta t p
Model Predictor Standardized beta t p
1 Constant 18.044 <0.001
Sex −0.080 −2.246 0.025 1 Constant 17.932 <0.001
Social support −0.130 −3.637 <0.001 Sex −0.078 −2.172 0.030
2 Constant 11.459 <0.001 Social support −0.127 −3.529 <0.001
Sex −0.081 −2.292 0.022 2 Constant 8.954 <0.001
Social support −0.101 −2.790 0.005 Sex −0.068 −1.937 0.053
Physical abuse −0.139 3.897 <0.001 Social support −0.082 −2.295 0.022
Sexual abuse −0.067 −1.829 0.068
n=792 Psych. Mal. 0.264 6.864 <0.001
Neglect 0.005 0.124 0.901
Discussion 3 Constant 8.868 <0.001
Sex −0.070 −1.988 0.047
Based on a thorough analysis of the child abuse and adult Social support −0.080 −2.218 0.027
attachment style literature, it was hypothesized that physical Sexual abuse −0.078 −2.025 0.043
abuse would be significantly associated with attachment Psych. Mal. 0.247 5.733 <0.001
avoidance but not attachment anxiety. Support was found for Neglect 0.001 0.024 0.981
an association between physical abuse and attachment Physical abuse 0.041 0.929 0.353
avoidance. The relationship between physical abuse and
attachment avoidance, though not large, was found even n=789; Psych. Mal. = Psychological Maltreatment
when controlling for sex, social support, and all other
forms of child abuse. This is consistent with other victim’s view of others or leads to a tendency toward an
research in this general area. In a sample of Israeli avoidant attachment in significant adult relationships.
children and adolescents, physical abuse has been asso- The association between physical abuse and attachment
ciated with, predominantly, an avoidant attachment style anxiety was unexpected. However, in a second review of the
(Finzi et al. 2000, 2001, 2002). The experience of literature base, two additional studies were found with signif-
physical abuse appears to have a negative impact on a icant relationships between a history of physical abuse and a
negative view of self or attachment anxiety in adulthood.
Muller et al. (2008) examined the mediating role of attach-
Table 5 Regression model for attachment avoidance with control vari- ment in the relationship between childhood physical abuse
ables and other abuse types and perceptions of social supports in adulthood and found
physical abuse to be significantly associated with a negative
Model Predictor Standardized beta t p
view of both self and other. McLewin and Muller (2006)
1 Constant 19.389 <0.001 found that greater attachment security, in adults who had been
Sex 0.030 0.869 0.385 physically abused as children, was a strong predictor of lower
Social support −0.278 −8.013 <0.001 levels of psychopathology regardless of abuse status. They
2 Constant 9.891 <0.001 also found physical abuse to be negatively associated with a
Sex 0.035 0.988 0.323 positive view of self and a positive view of other, and a
Social support −0.271 −7.550 <0.001 positive view of self was found to be a larger predictor of
Sexual abuse 0.048 1.317 0.188 reduced psychopathology than view of other and social
Psych. Mal. 0.008 0.202 0.840 support.
Neglect −0.007 −0.186 0.853 In the current study, the disappearance of the relationship
3 Constant 9.740 <0.001 between physical abuse and attachment anxiety when control-
Sex 0.030 0.861 0.390 ling for other abuse variables may be explained by the strong
Social support −0.265 −7.382 < 0.001 association between physical abuse and psychological mal-
Sexual abuse 0.023 0.592 0.554 treatment. When psychological maltreatment alone is exclud-
Psych. Mal. −0.034 −0.788 0.431
ed, physical abuse remains a predictor of attachment anxiety.
Neglect −0.017 −0.418 0.676
Once psychological maltreatment is added physical abuse is
Physical abuse 0.096 2.179 0.030
no longer included as a predictor. Therefore, both physical
abuse and psychological maltreatment may have important,
n=789; Psych. Mal. = Psychological Maltreatment though overlapping, associations with attachment anxiety.
J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234 231

Contrary to the hypothesis, although the relationship is not couple’s therapy, a more effective treatment approach could be
strong, those who have experienced physical abuse may also tailored for the couple. For example, Gordon and Christman
have higher levels of attachment anxiety or a negative view of (2008) recommend incorporating attachment theory into the
self than in the general population. Their abuse experience more traditional skills-based behavioral approaches to cou-
may have led to a more negative view of themselves resulting ple’s therapy. Knowing clients’ attachment styles provides
in experiencing more anxiety in their significant adult rela- information regarding the clients’ likely skill deficits and
tionships. Therefore, integrating the physical abuse results underlying motivations and goals in interpersonal interac-
together, those who have a history of physical abuse may be tions. This allows for a “more comprehensive assessment of
more likely to have a fearful-avoidant attachment style (high the couple’s interaction patterns and is likely to yield more
attachment avoidance and attachment anxiety). effective treatment” (Gordon and Christman 2008, p. 134).
Sue Johnson (2007) believes attachment theory to be the
Implications of Findings underlying guiding theory or “map” to understanding couple
functioning and providing effective Emotionally Focused
The current results have implications for individual treatment Therapy for couples. According to Johnson (2007), the basic
approaches for child abuse victims. The relationship between underlying difficulty for dysfunctional or distressed couples is
a therapist and client can be an intimate one. Those clients an issue of emotional connection and responsiveness or inse-
with a child abuse history may re-enact their insecure attach- cure attachment. Understanding the abuse history of a couple
ment behaviors with their therapist. It is important for thera- could help a therapist, using Johnson’s approach, ascertain
pists to understand this possible effect on their therapeutic their attachment style tendency quickly and help them develop
alliance as it may have significant implications for the thera- the skills to improve their methods for seeking and providing
peutic relationship, treatment behaviors, and treatment out- emotional responsiveness, a more secure attachment style, or
come. For example, in one study Sauer and associates found effective dependency.
that secure attachment to the therapist was significantly asso- The significance of social support as a predictor of both
ciated with greater reductions in client distress over time attachment dimensions speaks to the use of family and friend
(Sauer et al. 2010). The current study implies that those with social support as a possible means to improving adult attach-
a history of physical abuse may develop a dismissing-avoidant ment style and related mental health outcomes in abuse vic-
or fearful-avoidant attachment style in adulthood. According tims. For example, in a study of women with a history of
to Slade (2000), those with a dismissing attachment style may multiple types of child maltreatment Vranceanu et al. (2007)
tend to defend against all types of intense affect, minimize or found that social support partially mediated the relationship
deny their emotional experience, have difficulty trusting between child maltreatment and PTSD symptoms. Social
others, and attempt to keep emotional distance from the ther- support is linked to a variety of positive outcomes and may
apist. Daly and Mallinckrodt (2009) studied the therapeutic have its impact through positively affecting attachment di-
strategies of 12 experienced therapists when faced with at- mensions. In a literature review on social support and disso-
tachment avoidant and attachment anxious clients. They ciative identity disorder (DID), a disorder highly linked to a
found that these therapists began therapy by generally meeting history of child maltreatment, Korol (2008) found that familial
the client’s preferred social proximity or distance but then, and social support are important protective factors against the
over the course of therapy, created opportunities for their development of DID and that social interventions may be able
clients to move toward a more healthy interpersonal distance to mitigate the psychological consequences of insecure attach-
and relatedness. Therefore, for physical abuse victims, a ther- ment and a history of child abuse. In the current study, the
apist may need to maintain a certain interpersonal and emo- relationship between social support and adult attachment di-
tional distance initially but then slowly and empathically mensions are consistent with Korol’s (2008) conclusions.
move toward increased emotional awareness and interperson- Enhancing abuse victims’ and their families’ abilities to use
al connectedness. existing or to establish new social supports may be an impor-
Given that the current study measures attachment to sig- tant and effective part of an intervention strategy.
nificant others, the results certainly have implications for the
development of healthy, supportive, and enjoyable adult rela- Limitations
tionships and the treatment of adult couple difficulties. Know-
ing the abuse history of the partners in therapy may provide While the current study provides preliminary evidence for
important information regarding the attachment behaviors physical abuse to be associated with attachment avoidance
used by the couple. Those with a history of physical abuse and, to a lesser extent, attachment anxiety, the results must be
may be more dismissing and avoidant in their intimate rela- set within the context of the study limitations. First, the sample
tionships. By understanding a couple’s individual abuse his- was university-based and, therefore, the results can only be
tory and incorporating the relevant attachment strategies in generalized to the larger population with caution. Second, the
232 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

severity of this sample’s abuse experiences was low resulting be beneficial to develop and test treatment programs based on
in low variability in the data. It is possible that with a more these associations. Future research could also examine wheth-
community-based or clinical sample, resulting in more vari- er adults with a history of physical abuse benefit more from a
ability in the data, stronger and a larger number of relation- treatment approach addressing their avoidant tendencies than
ships could be found. Third, the scale used to measure abuse other treatment approaches.
history, while useful in its ability to provide a continuous Childhood physical abuse is a serious problem in society
measure, does not allow for the calculation of abuse preva- with the potential for many negative short- and long-term
lence rates. This and the fact that few studies to date have used consequences for its victims and their families. Attachment
this measure make it difficult to compare the current sample theory provides a way to understand the impact of childhood
with samples used in previous studies. Fourth, there were very physical abuse on adult intimate relationships. While the
few pure cases of abuse identified within this sample. While current study requires replication and expansion, it provides
other abuse types were controlled statistically, it is important support for the association between physical abuse and attach-
to remember that the relationships identified were completed ment avoidance while controlling for sex, social support, and
in a sample where the majority of child abuse victims had other types of child abuse histories. Social support was also
experienced more than one type of abuse. Last, the use of found to be a significant predictor of attachment avoidance
social support as a control variable may have reduced the and attachment anxiety, providing support for the possibility
ability of this study to find important significant relationships. of corrective social experiences having a positive impact on
Since social support is highly related to both attachment and adult attachment styles. It is hoped that future research will
child abuse, justifying its use as a control variable, this high continue in this area, as it has important implications for the
level of relatedness may have accounted for shared variance support and treatment of child abuse victims and their ability
that might have been explained by the relationship between to improve their intimate relationships.
attachment dimensions and child abuse types. Mikulincer and
Shaver (2009) summarized research indicating that one’s at-
tachment style affects one’s ability to seek out and use social
support from others. A securely attached individual would be
more likely to seek out and provide support to others. Theo- References
retically, adult attachment styles and social support are very
similar concepts and may, in fact, overlap so much to possibly Banyard, V. L. (1999). Childhood maltreatment and the mental health of
consider them redundant variables. low-income women. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 69(2),
161–170.
Bartholomew, K., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Methods of assessing adult
Future Directions and Conclusion attachment: Do they converge? In J. A. Simpson & S. Rholes (Eds.),
Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 25–45). New York:
Replicating the study using community and clinical samples Guilford Press.
would allow for further generalization of the findings. In Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). London: Hogarth Press.
Brennan, K. A., Shaver, P. R., & Tobey, A. E. (1991). Attachment styles,
addition, the variability of the abuse experiences would likely gender and parental problem drinking. Journal of Social and
increase allowing for more power within the analyses. A Personal Relationships, 8, 451–466.
university sample has, most likely, experienced less child Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report mea-
abuse than community and clinical samples, evidenced by surement of adult attachment. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes
(Eds.), Attachment theory and close relationships (pp. 46–76). New
their ability to attend university. The increased variability on York: The Guilford Press.
the abuse measures would certainly allow for stronger relation- Crittenden, P. (1988). Family and dyadic patterns of functioning in
ships to be found. It would also be beneficial to analyze the maltreating families. In K. Browne, C. Davies, & P. Stratton
relationship between physical abuse and adult attachment style (Eds.), Early prediction and prevention of child abuse (pp. 161–
189). London: John Wiley and Sons.
using individuals who have experienced only physical abuse. Crittenden, P. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Child maltreatment and
While these individuals might be rare, an analysis like this attachment theory. In D. Cicchetti & V. Carlson (Eds.), Child mal-
would add considerable strength to the relationships identified. treatment: Theory and research on the causes and consequences of
The power of the analyses would also increase as the other child abuse and neglect (pp. 432–463). New York: Cambridge
University Press.
abuse types would not need to be included as control variables. Crosson-Tower, C. (2005). Understanding child abuse and neglect (6th
Future research could also expand on the current study in a ed.). Toronto: Pearson Education.
number of additional ways. For example, it might be interest- Daly, K. D., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2009). Experienced therapists’ approach
ing to see if there are any differences in the relationship to psychotherapy for adults with attachment avoidance or attach-
ment anxiety. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56(4), 549–563.
between abuse type and attachment style based on gender. Finzi, R., Cohen, O., Sapir, Y., & Weizman, A. (2000). Attachment styles
Given the treatment implications of certain abuse types being in maltreated children: a comparative study. Child Psychiatry and
associated with certain adult attachment styles, it would also Human Development, 31(2), 113–128.
J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234 233

Finzi, R., Ram, A., Har-Even, D., Shnit, D., & Weizman, A. (2001). without a history of physical maltreatment. Child Abuse and
Attachment styles and aggression in physically abused and Neglect, 30, 171–191.
neglected children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 30(6), 769– Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2009). An attachment and behavioral
785. systems perspective on social support. Journal of Social and
Finzi, R., Har-Even, D., Shnit, D., & Weizman, A. (2002). Psychosocial Personal Relationships, 26(7), 7–19.
characterization of physically abused children from low socioeco- Muller, R. T., Sicoli, L. A., & Lemieux, K. E. (2000). Relationship
nomic households in comparison to neglected and nonmaltreated between attachment style and posttraumatic stress symptomatology
children. Journal of Child and Family Studies, 11, 441–453. among adults who report the experience of childhood abuse. Journal
Fleming, J., Mullen, P., & Bammer, G. (1997). A study of potential risk of Traumatic Stress, 13(2), 321–332.
factors for sexual abuse in childhood. Child Abuse and Neglect, Muller, R. T., Lemieux, K. E., & Sicoli, L. A. (2001). Attachment and
21(1), 49–58. psychopathology among formerly maltreated adults. Journal of
Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment: theo- Family Violence, 16(2), 151–169.
retical developments, emerging controversies, and unanswered Muller, R. T., Gragtmans, K., & Baker, R. (2008). Childhood phys-
questions. Review of General Psychology, 4(2), 132–154. ical abuse, attachment, and adult social support: test of a
Glass, G. V., & Hopkins, K. D. (1996). Statistical methods in education meditational model. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science,
and psychology (3rd ed.). Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon. 40(2), 80–89.
Gordon, K. C., & Christman, J. A. (2008). Integrating social information Pimlott-Kubiak, S., & Cortina, L. M. (2003). Gender, victimization, and
processing and attachment style research with cognitive-behavioral outcomes: reconceptualizing risk. Journal of Consulting and
couple therapy. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 38, 129– Clinical Psychology, 71(3), 528–539.
138. Runyon, M. K., Kenny, M. C., Berry, E. J., Deblinger, E., & Brown, E. J.
Hart, S. N., Brassard, M. R., Binggeli, N. J., & Davidson, H. A. (2002). (2006). Etiology and surveillance in child maltreatment. In J. R.
Psychological maltreatment. In J. E. B. Myers, L. Berliner, J. Briere, Lutzker (Ed.), Preventing violence: research and evidence-based
C. T. Hendrix, C. Jenny, & T. A. Reid (Eds.), The APSAC handbook intervention strategies (pp. 23–47). Washington: American
on child maltreatment (2nd ed., pp. 79–103). Thousand Oaks: Sage. Psychological Association.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (1994). Attachment as an organizational Sauer, E. M., Anderson, M. Z., Gormley, B., Richmond, C. J., & Preacco,
framework for research on close relationships. Psychological L. (2010). Client attachment orientations, working alliances, and
Inquiry, 5(1), 1–22. responses to therapy: a psychology training clinic study.
Higgins, D. J., & McCabe, M. P. (2001). The development of the Psychotherapy Research, 20(6), 702–711.
comprehensive child maltreatment scale. Journal of Family Sexton, K. A., Norton, P. J., Walker, J. R., & Norton, G. R. (2003).
Studies, 7(1), 7–28. Retrieved from http://www.jfs.e- Hierarchical model of generalized and specific vulnerabilities in
contentmanagement.com. anxiety. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 32(2), 82–94.
Hobfoll, S. E., Bansal, A., Schurg, R., Young, S., Pierce, C. A., Hobfoll, Shack, A. V., Averill, P. M., Kopecky, C., Krajewski, K., & Gummattira,
I., et al. (2002). The impact of perceived child physical and sexual P. (2004). Prior history of physical and sexual abuse among the
abuse history on Native American women’s psychological well- psychiatric inpatient population: a comparison of males and females.
being and AIDS risk. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychiatric Quarterly, 75(4), 343–358.
Psychology, 70(1), 252–257. Shapiro, D. L., & Levendosky, A. A. (1999). Adolescent survivors of
Hoskote, A. U., Martin, K., Hormbrey, P., & Burns, E. C. (2003). childhood sexual abuse: the mediating role of attachment style and
Fractures in infants: one in four is non-accidental. Child Abuse coping in psychological and interpersonal functioning. Child Abuse
Review, 12, 384–391. and Neglect, 23(11), 1175–1191.
Hulme, P. A. (2004). Retrospective measure of childhood sexual abuse: a Shaver, P., Hazan, C., & Bradshaw, D. (1988). Love as attachment: The
review of instruments. Child Maltreatment, 9(2), 201–217. integration of three behavioural systems. In R. J. Sternberg & M. L.
Johnson, S. (2007). A new era for couple therapy: theory, research, and Barnes (Eds.), The psychology of love (pp. 68–99). New Haven:
practice in concert. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 26(4), 5–16. Yale University Press.
Kolko, D. J. (2002). Child physical abuse. In J. E. B. Myers, L. Berliner, Shi, L. (2003). The association between adult attachment styles and
J. Briere, C. T. Hendrix, C. Jenny, & T. A. Reid (Eds.), The APSAC conflict resolution in romantic relationships. The American
handbook on child maltreatment (2nd ed., pp. 21–54). Thousand Journal of Family Therapy, 31(3), 143–157.
Oaks: Sage. Slade, A. (2000). The development and organization of attachment:
Korol, S. (2008). Familial and social support as protective factors against implications for psychoanalysis. Journal of the American
the development of dissociative identity disorder. Journal of Trauma Psychoanalytic Association, 48, 1147–1174.
and Dissociation, 9(2), 249–267. Sternberg, K. J., Lamb, M. E., Guterman, E., Abbott, C. B., & Dawd-
Lapsley, D. K., Varshney, N. M., & Aalmsa, M. C. (2000). Pathological Noursi, S. (2005). Adolescents’ perceptions of attachments to
attachment and attachment style in late adolescence. Journal of their mothers and fathers in families with histories of domestic
Adolescence, 23, 137–155. violence: a longitudinal perspective. Child Abuse and Neglect,
Manly, J. T., Kim, J. E., Rogosch, F. A., & Cicchetti, D. (2001). 29, 853–869.
Dimensions of child maltreatment and children’s adjustment: con- Styron, T., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (1997). Childhood attachment and
tributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and abuse: long-term effects on adult attachment, depression and conflict
Psychopathology, 13, 759–782. resolution. Child Abuse and Neglect, 21, 1015–1023.
Matsuoka, N., Uji, M., Hiramura, H., Chen, Z., Shikai, N., Kishida, Y., Swanson, B., & Mallinckrodt, B. (2001). Family environment, love
et al. (2006). Adolescents’ attachment style and early experiences: a withdrawal, childhood sexual abuse, and adult attachment.
gender difference. Archives of Women's Mental Health, 9, 23–29. Psychotherapy Research, 11(4), 455–472.
McCarthy, G., & Taylor, A. (1999). Avoidant/ambivalent attachment Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics
style as a mediator between abusive childhood experiences and adult (5th ed.). Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.
relationship difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Thompson, M. P., Kingree, J. B., & Desai, S. (2004). Gender differences
Psychiatry, 40(3), 465–477. in long-term health consequences of physical abuse of children: data
McLewin, L. A., & Muller, R. T. (2006). Attachment and social support from a nationally representative survey. American Journal of Public
in the prediction of psychopatholoty among young adults with and Health, 94(4), 599–604.
234 J Fam Viol (2014) 29:223–234

Trocmé, N., Fallon, B., MacLaurin, B., Daciuk, J., Felstiner, C., Black, T., Vranceanu, A., Hobfoll, S. E., & Johnson, R. J. (2007). Child multi-type
et al. (2005). Canadian incidence study of reported child abuse and maltreatment and associated depression and PTSD symptoms: the
neglect – 2003: major findings. Ottawa: Public Health Agency of role of social support and stress. Child Abuse and Neglect, 31, 71–
Canada. 84.
Unger, J. A. M. (2004). The relationship between students’ and percep- Zimet, G. D., Dahlem, N. W., Zimet, S. G., & Farley, G. K. (1988). The
tion of parents’ attitudes and child sexual abuse. Unpublished multidimensional scale of perceived social support. Journal of
master’s thesis. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba. Personality Assessment, 52(1), 30–41.

You might also like