You are on page 1of 18

Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behaviour Research and Therapy


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/brat

Attachment theory and hoarding disorder: A review and theoretical T


integration
Brittany M. Mathesa, Kiara R. Timpanob, Amanda M. Rainesc,d, Norman B. Schmidta,∗
a
Department of Psychology, Florida State University, 1107 West Call St, Tallahassee, FL, USA
b
Department of Psychology, University of Miami, 5665 Ponce de Leon Blvd, Coral Gables, FL, USA
c
Southeast Louisiana Veterans Health Care System, 2400 Canal St, New Orleans, LA, USA
d
Department of Psychiatry, Louisiana State University, 1542 Tulane Ave, New Orleans, LA, USA

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Hoarding disorder (HD) is a severe and persistent mental illness characterized by extreme difficulty parting with
Hoarding disorder possessions and considerable clutter that can result in dangerous living conditions. HD poses a considerable
Attachment public health burden; however, treatment for HD remains relatively limited, as many individuals do not respond
Interpersonal processes to treatment and/or do not maintain treatment gains, suggesting there are important factors not being ade-
Saving behaviors
quately addressed. In particular, one area that is not well-understood nor well-integrated into cognitive beha-
vioral models is the pathological attachment individuals with HD hold to their possessions. The current review
delineates existing work regarding attachment in HD and integrates findings regarding attachment into existing
cognitive behavioral models of HD. We use attachment theory as a foundation by which to examine HD and
better understand the dysfunctional relationships seen in those who hoard. We propose that both maladaptive
cognitions and dysfunctional attachments to people and possessions jointly underlie saving behaviors char-
acteristic of the disorder. The hypotheses put forth in this theory may help to advance our knowledge of HD,
identify potential factors that can be targeted in intervention and prevention efforts, and provide important
future directions for empirical work.

Hoarding behaviors have long been a part of human nature. that is not adequately explained in existing cognitive behavioral models
Archaeologists uncovered evidence of hoarding since as early as the is the pathological attachment to possessions. Therefore, the purpose of
prehistoric era, in which hunter-gatherers collected necessary supplies the current review is to delineate existing work regarding the etiology
in order to ensure protection from potential danger. Historic literary and maintenance of hoarding, and to highlight the extent to which
pieces depict characters who hoard, from Beowulf, published around components of attachment theory may help to fill important gaps in the
1000 AD, to Sherlock Holmes in 1893. In contemporary history, one of current understanding of hoarding behaviors.
the most commonly cited cases of compulsive hoarding is the Collyer Hoarding disorder (HD) is a psychiatric condition in which in-
brothers, two brothers who died in 1947 surrounded by 140 tons of dividuals have significant difficulty discarding possessions, which re-
collected goods in their Manhattan home (Lidz, 2003). Despite these sults in excessive clutter that interferes with their ability to use their
examples of hoarding throughout history, it was not until the 1990s homes (Frost & Gross, 1993). Individuals may also report excessive
that hoarding was systematically examined through an empirical lens. acquisition of items, though this has been established as a specifier for
Frost and Gross (1993) published the first scientific article defining and the diagnosis, rather than a core criteria (American Psychiatric
characterizing pathological hoarding, and since then, over 200 pub- Association, 2013). Epidemiological studies estimate that HD affects
lished manuscripts have further investigated the nature of such beha- approximately 1.5% of the population (Nordsletten et al., 2013), and
vior (Google Scholar, PsycInfo, 3/31/2019). Despite the significant clinically significant hoarding symptoms may impact up to 5.8% of the
increase in empirical work regarding hoarding over the last two dec- population (Timpano, Keough, Traeger, & Schmidt, 2011). Though
ades, treatment for such behaviors remains limited in efficacy (Tolin, symptoms of HD typically develop in adolescence and young adulthood
Frost, Steketee, & Muroff, 2015) and the pathogenesis of the disorder (Tolin, Meunier, Frost, & Steketee, 2010), clinically significant symp-
remains poorly understood. In particular, one component of hoarding toms, particularly in regards to impairing clutter, typically do not


Corresponding author. Department of Psychology, Florida State University, 1107 W. Call St, Tallahassee, FL, 32306-4301, USA.
E-mail address: schmidt@psy.fsu.edu (N.B. Schmidt).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2019.103549
Received 13 June 2019; Received in revised form 19 December 2019; Accepted 29 December 2019
Available online 30 December 2019
0005-7967/ © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

present until middle to late adulthood (Dozier, Porter, & Ayers, 2016; categorization, decision-making, planning, and memory difficulties,
Grisham, Frost, Steketee, Kim, & Hood, 2006). which are hypothesized to interfere with one's ability to efficiently
Hoarding symptoms are notably dimensional in that they occur organize and make decisions about one's belongings, thereby con-
along a continuum, rather than as a dichotomy (Timpano et al., 2013). tributing to a tendency to save, rather than discard, possessions
While general saving tendencies can be normative, at the severe end of (Grisham, Norberg, Williams, Certoma, & Kadib, 2010; Wincze,
the continuum, symptoms of HD can have an enormous impact on Steketee, & Frost, 2007). Though it is clear that HD is characterized by
public health outcomes. HD is associated with high rates of un- overall cognitive dysfunction, research regarding specific deficits has
employment and work impairment, as well as greater financial diffi- been somewhat mixed. For example, some research suggests individuals
culties and impairment in activities of daily living (e.g., cooking; Tolin, who hoard do not exhibit memory deficits, but rather decreased con-
Frost, Steketee, Gray, & Fitch, 2008). Hoarding is also associated with fidence in their memory that contributes to their perceived need to save
greater prevalence of comorbid medical health conditions (e.g., obesity, items to aid memory (Fitch & Cougle, 2013; Hallion, Diefenbach, &
diabetes) and greater utilization of public aid programs and healthcare Tolin, 2015; Hartl et al., 2004). Additionally, trait-level indecisiveness
services (Tolin et al., 2008). Moreover, individuals who hoard often is thought to underlie difficulties making decisions about possessions
report having never married and/or alienating themselves from family (Frost & Gross, 1993; Preston, Muroff, & Wengrovitz, 2009). None-
members who try to help clear their homes, thereby leading to social theless, hoarding is characterized by information processing deficits
isolation (Kim, Steketee, & Frost, 2001; Tolin et al., 2008). Extreme that interfere with one's ability to organize and make decisions about
clutter often results in fire hazards, unsanitary living conditions, high possessions.
rates of evictions and associated legal fees, and in some cases, death Maladaptive cognitions in HD surround beliefs about oneself, other
(Tolin et al., 2008). Little research has investigated specific economic people, and possessions. Individuals with HD describe core beliefs
costs associated with HD. Nonetheless, it has been estimated that re- about being unlovable, helpless, and unworthy (Moulding, Mancuso,
moval of clutter in Florida cost up to $8 million per year (McGuire, Rehm, & Nedeljkovic, 2016, pp. 123–133), and endorse negative views
Kaercher, Park, & Storch, 2013), direct and indirect costs of hoarding in of other people, such as believing that other people cannot be trusted
San Francisco were over $6 million (San Francisco Task Force on and relied upon (Hartl, Duffany, Allen, Steketee, & Frost, 2005;
Compulsive Hoarding, 2009), and the societal cost per individual is Moulding et al., 2016, pp. 123–133). Hoarding symptoms are also po-
greater than that of most other psychiatric disorders, including anxiety sitively associated with negatively perceived emotional coping, such
and depression (Tolin et al., 2008). that individuals who hoard hold negative beliefs about their ability to
Taken together, HD is a severe and debilitating illness that poses a tolerate negative emotions (Ayers, Castriotta, Dozier, Espejo, & Porter,
tremendous burden on the individual, as well as his/her family and 2014; Fernández de la Cruz et al., 2013; Shaw, Timpano, Steketee,
community. Though research regarding HD has significantly pro- Tolin, & Frost, 2015; Timpano, Shaw, Cougle, & Fitch, 2014). Finally,
gressed, many gaps in the literature remain, which may help to explain beliefs about possessions include the perception that possessions are
why treatment is only moderately effective (Tolin et al., 2015). Evi- unique and cannot be replaced, aid in one's memory, and must be
dence-based treatment for HD consists of cognitive behavioral therapy protected and not wasted (Frost, Hartl, Christian, & Williams, 1995;
(CBT), which focuses on challenging maladaptive beliefs about pos- Steketee, Frost, & Kyrios, 2003). Frost and Hartl (1996) also highlight
sessions, developing skills for organization, practicing resisting urges to the importance of beliefs about emotional attachment to possessions,
acquire and/or save, and using behavioral experiments to test as- which they define as encompassing three related beliefs: possessions
sumptions about feared negative outcomes associated with discarding provide comfort and security, have human-like qualities, and represent
and/or not acquiring valued possessions (Steketee & Frost, 2013). a piece of one's self-image. It has further been suggested that insecure
Though CBT results in symptom reduction, treatment response remains attachment style and negative early developmental experiences may
low and most individuals continue to exhibit clinically significant facilitate the development of hoarding behaviors aimed to compensate
symptoms at post-treatment (Tolin et al., 2015). As such, given the for perceived threat or uncertainty from oneself and others (Kyrios,
enormous impact of HD on the community coupled with poor treatment 2014, pp. 206–220; Kyrios et al., 2018). Overall, HD is characterized by
response, it is imperative to identify and investigate underlying factors a myriad of cognitions, and existing treatments largely focus on chan-
of HD that may not currently be accounted for in order to improve our ging maladaptive beliefs about possessions as the primary mechanism
understanding of the disorder and optimize treatment. of change (Levy et al., 2017).
According to cognitive behavioral models, the presence of both in-
1. Cognitive behavioral conceptualizations of hoarding disorder formation processing deficits and maladaptive beliefs contributes to
positive and negative emotional reactions, including pleasure and pride
Cognitive behavioral models of HD posit that several core vulner- regarding the importance of a possession, as well as sadness, anxiety,
abilities contribute to hoarding symptoms (Frost & Hartl, 1996; see and guilt regarding the prospect of losing or having to discard a valued
Fig. 1). These core vulnerabilities include genetic, environmental, possession (Frost & Hartl, 1996). These emotional responses are re-
personality, and neurobiological factors, which may include family inforced through the act of saving and/or acquiring, such that they
history, trauma, stress, impulsivity, and neurobiological dysfunction confirm the individual's beliefs regarding the perceived value of their
(Frost & Hartl, 1996). In regard to biologically-based factors, HD has a possessions. Consequently, saving and acquiring behaviors are devel-
strong genetic factor, as it is more common among first-degree relatives oped and maintained, which, coupled with executive functioning defi-
and genetics account for approximately 50% of the variance in symp- cits that interfere with organization, lead to excessive clutter in one's
toms (Iervolino et al., 2009). Additionally, dysfunction in the ven- home.
tromedial prefrontal cortex and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex are
thought to underlie hoarding symptoms (An et al., 2009; Mataix-Cols 2. Gaps in the literature
et al., 2004). Regarding environmental and personality factors, trauma
and impulsivity are proposed to be particularly salient risk factors for Overall, cognitive behavioral models have provided an important
HD (Cromer, Schmidt, & Murphy, 2007; Timpano et al., 2013). Overall, foundation by which to examine the etiology and maintenance of HD.
these core vulnerabilities are thought to contribute to information However, an important question that has been left unanswered is: why
processing deficits, erroneous beliefs about oneself, other people, and is it that some individuals, but not others, develop intense attachments
possessions, and maladaptive reinforcement cycles that then interact in to possessions that results in them being unable to discard those items?
their influence on the development of HD. Existing cognitive behavioral models of HD conceptualize emotional
Information processing deficits implicated in HD include attention, attachment to possessions as part of a myriad of cognitive distortions

2
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

Fig. 1. Integration of attachment disturbances in cognitive behavioral models of HD.

that occur on a continuum from non-clinical to clinical levels of severity caregiver provides sufficient physical and emotional security, the infant
(Frost & Hartl, 1996; Steketee et al., 2003; Timpano, Broman-Fulks is thought to develop a secure attachment with the caregiver. However,
et al., 2013). However, such models fail to explain how or why those when the caregiver fails to do so, the infant develops an insecure at-
distortions reach a clinical level of severity. Indeed, feeling attached to tachment. Importantly, an infant's attachment style (i.e., secure or in-
possessions to some degree is not unique to HD (Grisham et al., 2009), secure) is thought to provide the foundation for social and emotional
and Grisham et al. (2009) found that beliefs about the importance of development. Over time, securely-attached infants go on to develop
possessions were strongly associated with attachment to a novel object, adaptive emotional coping skills, portray others as trustworthy, and
regardless of the presence of hoarding symptoms. Therefore, even those cultivate self-efficacy and confidence. In contrast, insecurely-attach-
without clinical levels of psychopathology may hold inflated beliefs ment infants may have difficulty managing negative emotions, expect
about possessions that can result in increased attachment to objects. others to be unreliable or hurtful, and view themselves negatively.
However, there is a lack of understanding about why only some in- Though attachment theory has its roots in the study of infants, it has
dividuals develop such a significant emotional attachment that causes been extended and applied to the study of adult relationships. Hazan
them to be unable to part with possessions. We suggest that attachment and Shaver (2004) suggest that adults, like children, are securely or
theory, described below, may provide a foundation by which to eluci- insecurely attached, and are characterized by similar features as in-
date the nature of hoarding symptoms. fants. Securely attached individuals are able to effectively express and
cope with negative emotions, and appropriately seek support from
others in times of distress (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012). In contrast,
3. Attachment theory insecurely attached individuals may seek attention due to their per-
ceived inability to independently cope and/or socially isolate and try to
Attachment theory provides a basis for understanding the complex suppress negative emotions (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012). Further, in-
nature of human relationships. The foundation of this theory is that secure attachment is associated with maladaptive coping strategies,
humans have a basic need for social connection, such that they must such as self-blame, avoidance of negative emotion, poor impulse con-
form and maintain interpersonal relationships, and derive a sense of trol, and perceived inability to regulate emotions (Mikulincer, Shaver,
security, acceptance, and belongingness from those relationships & Pereg, 2003). Finally, individuals who are insecurely attached also
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Attachment theory has historically focused report increased self-doubt and unstable self-esteem, which can then
on the importance of the relationship between an infant and his/her exacerbate interpersonal difficulties by increasing sensitivity to per-
primary caregiver, suggesting that infants have an innate need to seek ceived attacks on one's self-image, thereby contributing to avoidance of
safety and security in their caregivers when stressed or threatened or increased hostility toward others (Dickinson & Pincus, 2003; Jones,
(Ainsworth, 1991; Bowlby, 1982). According to Bowlby (1982), when a

3
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

Freemon, & Goswick, 1981; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012; Park, Crocker, beliefs regarding the extent to which possessions provide comfort and
& Mickelson, 2004). safety, possess human-like attributes, and represent one's identity (Frost
Attachment in adults is further conceptualized as falling along two & Hartl, 1996; Frost et al., 1995). However, we suggest that rather than
continua: anxiety and avoidance (Brennan, Clark, & Shaver, 1998). attachment being conceptualized solely as a cognitive distortion, it
High attachment anxiety is characterized by negative self-evaluation should represent the relationship between the individual and his/her
and fears of abandonment that typically result in efforts to seek re- possessions from a cognitive, emotional, and behavioral perspective,
assurance regarding these fears. High attachment avoidance is char- which is in line with attachment theory (Grossmann & Grossmann,
acterized by negative views of others and a subsequent desire to avoid 2006). Further, we propose that attachment should encompass attach-
social connection and intimacy. Whereas individuals high on attach- ment to both possessions and people in order to account for the function
ment anxiety can become overly reliant on attachment figures, thereby of one's relationship with their belongings, which we hypothesize is to
resulting in interpersonal difficulties, individuals high on attachment compensate for unsatisfying interpersonal relationships. Therefore, the
avoidance generally become overly reliant on themselves, thereby proposed model focuses on the extent to which individuals who hoard
leading to social isolation. Further, individuals high on both attachment form and maintain relationships with their possessions and other
anxiety and avoidance are likely to desire close relationships yet fear people, as well as the ways in which those relationships interact to not
rejection, thereby resulting in both interpersonal difficulties and social only reinforce existing dysfunctional beliefs, but to then contribute to
isolation. In general, individuals low on both attachment anxiety and emotional responses to possessions and people that result in saving
avoidance are characterized as being securely attached, whereas in- behaviors.
dividuals high on one or both are considered to be insecurely attached. It should be noted that in a recent review of psychological factors in
It should be noted that there is significant debate regarding the extent HD, Kyrios (2014, pp. 206–220) included poor attachments, attachment
to which attachment should be conceptualized as a categorical or di- style, self-ambivalence, self-construal, and possessions as extension of
mensional construct (Ravitz, Maunder, Hunter, Sthankiya, & Lancee, self as factors that may be implicated in the etiology of HD. However,
2010; Simpson & Rholes, 1998). However, existing work largely sup- this review did not delineate a specific theoretical model, nor clearly
ports the dimensional nature of attachment (Fraley, Hudson, Heffernan, define these terms and the ways in which they may contribute to
& Segal, 2015), and as such, we focus on delineating correlates of in- hoarding symptoms. Therefore, this work provides an important outline
secure attachment more broadly across a continuum. of potential risk factors, but a clear theoretical model that can be em-
Overall, attachment is thought to be a relationship or bond between pirically tested is needed in order to advance our understanding of the
two entities that largely manifests through a behavioral system aimed disorder.
to increase or decrease physical or emotional proximity to the other As with many theoretical models of psychopathology, we suggest
being (Ainsworth, 1991; Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 2015; that the proposed model comprises both specific and non-specific risk
Bowlby, 1979, 1982; Hazan & Shaver, 2004). It is theorized that at- factors for the development of HD. Insecure attachment and beliefs
tachment figures across the lifespan serve three main functions: they about oneself and others are risk factors for a myriad of psychological
should be someone to whom the individual seeks proximity and avoids disorders (Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Ein-Dor & Doron, 2015, pp.
separation, a safe haven in times of need, and a secure base in which the 346–373), whereas beliefs about possessions and object attachment
individual can develop his or her unique identity (Ainsworth et al., appear to be more specific to HD. Indeed, several studies have found
2015; Hazan & Shaver, 2004). Importantly, attachment needs are that individuals with HD report higher dysfunctional beliefs about
thought to be an integral part of human development, such that when possessions, including those related to sentimentality, than do in-
interpersonal attachment needs are not met, individuals may attempt to dividuals with related disorders (e.g., OCD, anxiety disorder;
compensate through other means, such as food and objects (Norris, ds = 1.3–1.4) and healthy controls (ds = 1.2–2.4; Frost, Steketee,
Lambert, Nathan DeWall, & Fincham, 2012). Tolin, Sinopoli, & Ruby, 2015; Grisham et al., 2009; Grisham, Steketee,
& Frost, 2008; Nedelisky & Steele, 2009; Pertusa, Fullana, Singh,
4. The current review Alonso, Menchon, & Mataix-Cols, 2008; Steketee et al., 2003).
Taken together, we suggest that integrating interpersonal attach-
The primary aims of this review are to a) refine cognitive behavioral ment in models of HD may help to explain the extreme attachment to
models in order to highlight the role of interpersonal factors in the possessions often seen in individuals with hoard. We hypothesize that
development of HD, b) consolidate findings regarding attachment and individuals who hoard experience an insecure attachment style, which
HD, and c) provide avenues for future research to further clarify the likely manifests through interpersonal difficulties, as well as dysfunc-
role of attachment in HD. As shown in Fig. 1, we propose that both tional relationships with possessions that serve as efforts to fulfill unmet
maladaptive cognitions and dysfunctional attachments to both people interpersonal needs. Indeed, as noted, insecure attachment is strongly
and possessions jointly contribute to the phenomenological features of associated with interpersonal difficulties, including social isolation,
HD. In the next sections, we provide further background regarding our conflict, and/or hostility towards others (Dickinson & Pincus, 2003;
rationale for the integration of attachment in conceptualizations of Jones, Freemon, & Goswick, 1981; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2012; Park
hoarding, and then review the extant literature regarding attachment et al., 2004). However, because possessions cannot actually provide
and hoarding. reciprocal care, those needs remain unmet, thereby exacerbating in-
terpersonal dysfunction. Overall, we hypothesize that individuals with
5. Attachment theory and hoarding HD view their possessions, rather than people, as primary attachment
figures.
How might attachment theory help to explain HD? We hypothesize
that individuals with HD have thwarted interpersonal needs, and 6. Review of literature regarding hoarding and attachment
therefore compensate by attempting to form secure attachments with
possessions. However, because possessions cannot provide reciprocal In order to evaluate the state of the current literature regarding
care in the way that a human can, they do not actually fulfill one's associations between hoarding and attachment, a literature search was
interpersonal needs, and in fact, only further thwart them by interfering conducted using Google Scholar, PsychINFO, and PubMed through
with the formation and maintenance of interpersonal relationships. March 2019 (Fig. 2). Search terms related to attachment processes
Importantly, our theoretical conceptualization of attachment to consisted of: “attachment,” “interpersonal,” “loneliness,” and “iden-
possessions is closely related to the existing definition of emotional tity.” Search terms related to hoarding disorder consisted of:
attachment within the cognitive behavioral model, which focuses on “hoarding,” “buying,” “possessions,” and “anthropomorphism.” Of

4
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

correlational findings, when compared to healthy controls, individuals


who hoard report greater insecure interpersonal attachment (Grisham,
Martyn, Kerin, Baldwin, & Norberg, 2018; Medard & Kellett, 2014),
though there have been no reported differences in interpersonal at-
tachment when compared to other clinical groups (e.g., OCD, anxiety;
Grisham et al., 2018; Nedelisky & Steele, 2009). Two recent studies
found somewhat contradictory evidence regarding the association be-
tween hoarding and attachment. One study utilized a sample of un-
dergraduates who had elevated scores on the acquiring subscale of a
measure of hoarding symptoms and the second study recruited under-
graduates with elevated scores on the difficulty discarding subscale
(Crone, Kwok, Chau, & Norberg, 2019; Norberg, Crone, Kwok, &
Grisham, 2018). Across both studies, hoarding symptoms were not
significantly associated with interpersonal attachment. The authors
noted one reason for these findings may be range restriction as the
measure used to recruit participants in both studies was also used as the
outcome of interest. Another possible explanation is study differences in
the measure used to assess interpersonal attachment. Crone et al.
(2019) and Norberg et al. (2018) used the Experiences of Close Re-
lationships-Relationship Structures scale (Fraley, Heffernan, Vicary, &
Brumbaugh, 2011), whereas other studies examining associations be-
tween hoarding and interpersonal attachment (Danet & Secouet, 2018;
Keefer et al., 2012; Kehoe & Egan, 2019; Neave et al., 2016) used the
Revised Adult Attachment Scale (Collins & Read, 1990), Adult Attach-
ment Scale (Simpson, Rholes, & Phillips, 1996), and Experiences of
Close Relationships-Revised Scale (Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000).
Though the aforementioned questionnaires were all designed to mea-
Fig. 2. PRISMA flow diagram. sure interpersonal attachment, researchers have noted that many in-
terpersonal attachment questionnaires show inconsistent validity and
note, anthropomorphism was included as a search term given that reliability across studies and/or may be assessing different aspects of
cognitive behavioral models indicate that individuals who hoard hold interpersonal attachment that are differentially associated with per-
beliefs that possessions have human-like qualities, which is consistent sonality traits and psychopathological symptoms (Mikulincer & Shaver,
with definitions of anthropomorphism (Epley, Waytz, & Cacioppo, 2007; Scharfe, 2016). In support of this hypothesis within the hoarding
2007). Search terms from the hoarding and attachment categories were literature, Neave et al. (2016) used both the RAAS and the ECR-RS and
used in all possible combinations to generate the initial literature se- reported significant associations between hoarding symptoms and the
lection (n = 223). Titles and abstracts were then reviewed. RAAS subscales, whereas associations with ECR-RS subscales were ei-
Both quantitative and qualitative studies, but not review nor theo- ther non-significant or the effect was small (rs = 0.17). Therefore, it
retical papers, were included if they described or investigated associa- may be that the ECR-RS has psychometric limitations and/or is mea-
tions between object attachment, interpersonal attachment, or inter- suring some aspect of interpersonal attachment that is less relevant to
personal processes and hoarding, buying, or saving objects. the study of hoarding. In sum, though most work has indicated positive
Additionally, studies were included if they were available in English, associations between insecure attachment and hoarding symptoms,
used adult samples, and focused on object hoarding, as empirical work more work is needed in light of these mixed findings.
suggests other forms of hoarding (e.g., animals, food) are characterized In addition to work directly assessing the connection between
by different clinical characteristics (Frost, Patronek, & Rosenfield, hoarding and attachment processes, there is also indirect evidence for
2011). Using the aforementioned criteria, 52 articles were fully re- this relationship. Individuals with HD report poorer social support and
viewed and included below and in Tables 1–3. The following data were greater interpersonal difficulties than do healthy controls (Grisham
extracted: 1) author and publication year, 2) sample type, 3) mean age et al., 2008; Medard & Kellett, 2014), and often live alone and do not
of sample, 4) sample size, 5) study design, 6) measures of saving be- have romantic partners (Kim et al., 2001). Hoarding symptoms are
haviors, object attachment, and/or interpersonal attachment, and 7) associated with significant burden on one's family and social network
main findings. Results are described qualitatively. Notably, for the re- (Tolin, Frost, Steketee, & Fitch, 2008), as well as increased cognitive
mainder of this manuscript, we use “object attachment” to refer to at- beliefs about being a burden on others (Raines et al., 2016). Hoarding is
tachment to possessions and “interpersonal attachment” to refer to at- negatively associated with maternal care (Chen et al., 2017), which is
tachment to other people. Consistent with attachment theory posited to be an indicator of insecure attachment in childhood (Bowlby,
(Grossmann & Grossmann, 2006), we use “attachment” in both defini- 1979). Moreover, recent work has indicated that hoarding symptoms
tions to refer to the relationship and behavioral system between an may be associated with increased reactivity to and recovery from in-
individual and an attachment figure that serves to facilitate physical terpersonal stress, suggesting that sensitivity to social threat may un-
and emotional proximity between both entities. derlie one's tendency to save (Grisham et al., 2018; Mathes, Kennedy,
Cougle, & Schmidt, 2019). In one study, individuals with HD, as com-
pared to healthy controls, exhibited a slower reduction in negative
7. Interpersonal dysfunction and hoarding symptoms emotions due a task in which they recalled a conflict with an individual
important to the participant (Grisham et al., 2018). Similarly, a sepa-
Empirical work shows that insecure attachment is moderately cor- rate study found that hoarding symptoms were associated with greater
related with saving behaviors and hoarding symptoms across non- hostile feelings in response to a social exclusion lab paradigm (Mathes
clinical and clinical samples (rs = .24 - 0.42, ßs = 0.22 - 0.38; Danet & et al., 2019).
Secouet, 2018; Keefer, Landau, Rothschild, & Sullivan, 2012; Kehoe & A growing body of research also indicates that interpersonal trauma
Egan, 2019; Neave, Tyson, McInnes, & Hamilton, 2016). In addition to and stress are significantly associated with greater hoarding symptoms

5
Table 1
Interpersonal dysfunction and hoarding symptoms.
Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of hoarding behaviors Measures of interpersonal Main findings
attachment processes
B.M. Mathes, et al.

Burgess et al. Undergraduate students 20.00 224 Correlational Anthropomorphism Questionnaire; UCLA Loneliness Scale Loneliness was significantly associated
(2018) and community adults Graves Anthropomorphism Task with hoarding symptoms and
Scale; Saving Inventory Revised; cognitions, and was a moderator of
Saving Cognitions Inventory anthropomorphism of objects
Chen, Bienvenu, Individuals with OCD with 40.3 OCD with hoarding: Comparative None Parental Bonding Instrument Women with hoarding reported lower
et al. (2017) and without hoarding 334; OCD without maternal care and higher maternal
hoarding: 560 protection and control than did women
without hoarding.
Claes et al. (2016) Community adults 39.37 254 Correlational Compulsive Buying Scale; Saving Erikson Psychosocial Stage The association between identity
Inventory Revised Inventory confusion and compulsive buying was
mediated by materialistic values
Crone et al. (2019) Undergraduate students 21.96 156 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Frost Experiences in Close There was an indirect effect of anxious,
and community adults Indecisiveness Scale Relationships-Relationship but not avoidant, attachment on
Structures Questionnaire indecisiveness via emotional reactivity
Danet and Secouet Community adults 30.58 197 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Relationship Scale Questionnaire When accounting for age and education,
(2018) anxious attachment, but not fearful,
avoidant, nor secure, was a significant
predictor of hoarding symptoms
Frost et al. (2007) Community adults Not reported, age 127 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Self-Ambivalence Measure; Early Self-ambivalence and uncertainty were
range = 18-22 Compulsive Acquisition Scale Developmental Influences significantly and positively associated
Inventory with hoarding symptoms, even when
accounting for indecisiveness and
depression.
Grisham et al. Individuals with Hoarding group: 55.0; 30 in each group Comparative Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Inventory of Interpersonal Individuals with hoarding reported

6
(2008) compulsive hoarding, Anxiety or depression Cognitions Inventory Problems-Circumplex Version greater interpersonal problems than did
individuals with anxiety group: 51.5; Control community controls, but not individuals
or depression, community group: 52.1 with anxiety or depression. Across both
controls clinical groups, beliefs about
possessions was a marginally significant
predictor of interpersonal problems,
when accounting for depression and
anxiety.
Grisham et al. Individuals with hoarding Clinical group: 40.38; Clinical group: 24; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Modified experiences in close Individuals with HD reported
(2018) disorder, clinical controls, Clinical controls: 41.00; Clinical controls: 22; relationships-Relationship significantly greater attachment anxiety
healthy controls Community controls: Community controls: Structures Questionnaire; self- and avoidance than did clinical, but not
48.31 26 reported and physiological healthy, controls. Additionally,
reactivity to recollections of individuals with HD reported more
interpersonal conflict intense negative emotions and slower
recovery during the stressor task than
did clinical and health controls.
Hartl et al. (2005) Individuals with hoarding Clinical group: 54.27; Clinical group: 36; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Traumatic Events Scale-Lifetime Individuals with hoarding reported
and healthy controls Control group: 50.17 Control group: 26 significantly greater interpersonal
traumas than did healthy controls.
Keefer et al. (2012) Community adults Not reported Study 1: 99; Study 2: Experimental Reciprocal Attachment Experiences in Close In studies 1 and 2, participants
(studies 1 and 2) and 47; Study 3: 51 Questionnaire-Adapted Relationships-Revised scale instructed to write about times in which
undergraduate students a close friend or family member was
(study 3) unreliable reported greater attachment
to possessions than did individuals
instructed to write about strangers' or
self unreliability. Additionally, there
was an indirect effect of condition on
object attachment through attachment
anxiety, but not avoidance. In study 3,
(continued on next page)
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
Table 1 (continued)

Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of hoarding behaviors Measures of interpersonal Main findings
attachment processes
B.M. Mathes, et al.

participants instructed to write about


uncertainty about their relationships
reported greater separation anxiety
from mobile phones than did those
instructed to write about self-
uncertainty.
Kehoe and Egan Community adults Not reported; age 463 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Adult Attachment Questionnaire Attachment anxiety and avoidance were
(2019) range = 18-75 Cognitions Inventory significantly and positively correlated
with hoarding symptoms and
attachment to possessions. A chained
mediation model indicated there was an
indirect effect of childhood trauma on
hoarding symptoms via attachment
insecurity and attachment to
possessions.
Kim et al. (2001) Older adults with 76.9 62 Correlational Semi-structured interview created Semi-structured interview Of the sample, 55% were unmarried and
compulsive hoarding and qualitative for the study created for the study 82% lived alone. Having never been
married was associated with greater
hoarding symptoms and impairment
due to symptoms.
Kyrios et al. (2018) Individuals with hoarding 47.4 Clinical group: 89; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Hoarding-Related Early As compared to healthy controls and
disorder, OCD, social Control group: 20 Cognitions Inventory Developmental Influences individuals without HD, individuals
anxiety and/or panic Inventory with HD reported significantly greater
disorder, and healthy uncertainty about self and others and
controls significantly less emotional warmth in

7
one's family. However, there were no
differences in uncertainty and warmth
between individuals with HD with OCD
and without OCD.
Mathes et al. Community adults 37.30 258 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Buss-Perry Aggression There was an indirect effect of
(2017) Questionnaire; Posttraumatic interpersonal trauma, but not other
Diagnostic Scale types of trauma, on hoarding symptoms
via aggression, even when accounting
for negative affect.
Mathes et al. Undergraduate students Study 1: 19.14; Study 2: Study 1: 195; Study 2: Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Word Sentence Association Hoarding symptoms were significantly
(2019) 18.70 117 and Paradigm-Hostility and self- and positively associated with perceived
experimental reported state-based hostility hostility. Additionally, hoarding
symptoms moderated hostile reactivity
to a social exclusion manipulation.
Medard and Kellett Individuals with hoarding Clinical group: 36.66; Clinical group: 380; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Relationship Questionnaire; Individuals with hoarding symptoms
(2014) symptoms, undergraduate Student group: 22.93; Student group: 670; Social Provisions Questionnaire reported significantly greater
students, and community Community group: 40.18 Community group: 379 attachment anxiety and avoidance and
controls lower social support than did students
and controls. Across the entire sample,
was a significant interaction between
attachment anxiety, but not avoidance,
and social support in the effect of
hoarding symptoms.
Neave et al. (2016) Community adults Females: 22.41; Males: 283 (210 females, 73 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Revised Adult Attachment Scale; Hoarding behaviors and cognitions were
27.86 males) Cognitions Inventory; Reciprocal Experiences of Close significantly and positively correlated
Attachment Questionnaire- Relationships-Relationship with attachment anxiety, attachment
Adapted; Anthropomorphism Structures avoidance, object attachment, and
Questionnaire anthropomorphism. When accounting
for age, sex, and other variables of
(continued on next page)
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
Table 1 (continued)

Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of hoarding behaviors Measures of interpersonal Main findings
attachment processes
B.M. Mathes, et al.

interest, only attachment anxiety and


object attachment remained
significantly associated with hoarding
behaviors, and only object attachment
remained associated with hoarding
cognitions.
Nedelisky and Individuals with OCD with 46.09 Hoarding group: 14; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Reciprocal Attachment Individuals with hoarding reported
Steele (2009) and without hoarding Non-hoarding group: Questionnaire; Five Minute significantly less emotional
16 Speech Statement overinvolvement with family members
or friends than did individuals without
hoarding.
Norberg et al. Individuals with acquiring 22.79 361 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Experiences of Close There was an indirect effect of anxious
(2018) behaviors Compulsive Acquisition Scale; Relationships-Relationship attachment on acquiring behaviors
Graves Anthropomorphic Task Structures through distress intolerance and
Scale-Comfort anthropomorphism of comforting items.
Raines et al. (2016) Community adults with 31.77 125 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Interpersonal Needs Hoarding symptoms were significantly
elevated hoarding Questionnaire and positively associated with the
symptoms perception that one is a burden on
others.
Roster (2015) Professional organizers Not reported 28 Qualitative Narrative case descriptions of Narrative case descriptions of Individuals with hoarding described
who work with hoarding behavior attachment styles attachment to possessions that provide
individuals with hoarding both affiliative and self-identity
motivations.
Timpano et al. Undergraduate students 18.97 465 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Negative Life Events Stress related to friends, school, and
(2011) Questionnaire family was uniquely and positively

8
associated with hoarding symptoms.
Tolin et al. (2010) Adults with self-identified 49.16 751 Correlational Hoarding Rating Scale Self-reported distress, Periods of symptom onset and increases
hoarding symptoms and descriptive impairment, and history of were more likely to be associated with
traumatic life events periods of relationship changes and/or
interpersonal violence.
Tolin, Frost, Friends or family Family of individuals Family of individuals Correlational Hoarding Rating Scale; Clutter Patient Rejection Scale and self- Living with an individual who hoards
Steketee, and members of individuals with hoarding: 45.36; with hoarding: 571; and descriptive Image Rating reported distress regarding was associated with significant familial
Fitch (2008) with hoarding or family of individuals with family of individuals family member's hoarding stress, and increased family frustration
subthreshold hoarding subthreshold hoarding: with subthreshold symptoms and hostility.
53.23 hoarding: 94
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
Table 2
Interpersonal attachment and object attachment.
Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of saving behaviors Measures of object attachment Main findings
B.M. Mathes, et al.

Bartz, Tchalova, & Community adults 39.50 178 Experimental Self-report measure of Experiences in Close Reminding people of a close relationship reduces
Fenerci, 2016 anthropomorphism Relationships Scale; UCLA anthropomorphism of gadgets and pets
Loneliness Scale; Need to Belong
Scale
Chen, Wan, and Undergraduate students and Not reported Study 1: 118; Experimental Self-reported attitudes towards Self-reported need for social Socially excluded participants demonstrated a
Levy (2017) community adults Study 2: 342; products affiliation greater preference for products with human
Study 3: 378 characteristics, in part, due to an increased need
for social affiliation
Ferraro et al. (2011) Study 1: undergraduate Not reported Study 1: 137; Experimental Self-reported rating of the importance Self-reported rating of the The extent to which an individual views a
students; Study 2: Study 2: 361 of possessions and reactions to the loss perceived link between self and possession as part of their self-worth, but not
community adults of possessions possessions, and the importance monetary value of a possession, is significantly
of domains of self-worth associated with increased object attachment,
which in turn, is associated with increased
negative emotions when asked to consider losing
possessions
Keefer et al. (2012) Community adults (studies Not reported Study 1: 99; Study Experimental Reciprocal Attachment Questionnaire- Experiences in Close In studies 1 and 2, participants instructed to
1 and 2) and undergraduate 2: 47; Study 3: 51 Adapted Relationships-Revised scale write about times in which a close friend or
students (study 3) family member was unreliable reported greater
attachment to possessions than did individuals
instructed to write about strangers' or self
unreliability. Additionally, there was an indirect
effect of condition on object attachment through
attachment anxiety, but not avoidance. In study
3, participants instructed to write about
uncertainty about their relationships reported

9
greater separation anxiety from mobile phones
than did those instructed to write about self-
uncertainty.
Kwok et al. (2018) Community adults and 22.88 331 Experimental Saving Inventory Revised; Social exclusion manipulation Individuals who were socially excluded did not
undergraduate students Anthropomorphic Mental State ratings; (Cyberball) report greater anthropomorphism than did
with elevated acquiring self-reported object attachment and socially included individuals. However,
behaviors perceived value of an object regardless of condition, there was an indirect
effect of anthropomorphism on object
attachment via perceived sentimental value.
Lastovicka and Community adults Not reported Study 1: 11; Study Correlational Measure created for the study that UCLA Loneliness Scale Loneliness significantly mediated the association
Sirianni (2011) 2: 127; Study 3: assesses possession love between interpersonal deficits and possession
110; Study 4: 142; love.
Study 5: 41
Loveland et al. Studies 1,2,3,4: Study 1: 21.7; Study 1: 136; Experimental Decisions regarding nostalgic and non- Need to Belong Scale; self- Individuals who were socially excluded, but not
(2010) undergraduate students; Study 1b: 42.6; Study 1b: 63; nostalgic products reported social support; implicit included, demonstrated greater preference for
Study 1b: community adults Study 2: 24.1; Study 2: 43; Study word task comprising words nostalgic products, and this effect was
Study 3: 21.6; 3: 94; Study 4: 72 related to belongingness particularly pronounced for individuals with a
Study 4: 21.2 high need to belong. Additionally, the
consumption, but not selection, of a nostalgic
product subsequently reduced the need to
belong.
Mead et al. (2011) Undergraduate students Not reported Study 1: 30; Study Experimental Preference for and decision to buy Experimental manipulation of As compared to control participants, individuals
2: 149; Study 3: products social exclusion and subsequent who were socially excluded were more likely to
151; Study 4: 116 ratings of affect spend money on products that represented group
membership, report product preferences
consistent with those of peers, and be willing to
try foods and substances they did not like to
increase social connection.
Undergraduate students Not reported Experimental
(continued on next page)
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

(Hartl et al., 2005; Mathes et al., 2017; Timpano et al., 2011; Tolin

Participants who were randomized to conditions

are present when individuals are reminded that


Specifically, they reported a decreased need to
engaged with products that contained human

behaviors. However, compensatory behaviors


et al., 2010). Individuals with clinically significant hoarding symptoms

characteristics exhibited fewer compensatory

engage with close others and/or in prosocial


behaviors than did individuals who engaged
possessions as more self-expressive than did

Individuals who were socially excluded and

connections, as well as a decreased need to


inducing self-uncertainty reported viewing
endorse a greater lifetime incidence of interpersonal trauma, including

inflate the number of one's social media


with non-anthropomorphized products.
sexual and physical assault, than do healthy controls (Hartl et al.,
those who received a self-certainty 2005), and describe interpersonal violence and/or a significant change

products are not actually human.


in a relationship preceding the worsening of symptoms (Tolin et al.,
2010). Further, many individuals who hoard report experiencing a
significant interpersonal loss that precipitated the onset or exacerbation
of their symptoms (Landau et al., 2011; Tolin et al., 2010). Interest-
ingly, a recent study indicated that insecure interpersonal attachment
manipulation.
Main findings

and increased attachment to objects partially mediated the association


between childhood trauma and hoarding symptoms among a commu-
nity samples of adults (Kehoe & Egan, 2019). The authors suggested
that adverse life experiences result in negative relationships with self
and others that then interact with or contribute to maladaptive re-
Self-reported number of friends

spend on the phone with close


Measures of object attachment

and self-reported sense of self-

others; Social Assurance Scale


Experimental manipulation of

lationships with possessions, which, in turn, results in hoarding symp-


on Facebook, desired time to

toms.
There is also evidence that anthropomorphism may play a role in
the connection between processes related to interpersonal attachment
and hoarding symptoms. Recently, Burgess, Graves, and Frost (2018)
found that loneliness moderated the association between anthro-
certainty

pomorphism and hoarding symptoms in a large sample of under-


graduate students, and the authors suggested that saving and anthro-
pomorphizing possessions may serve to compensate for a lack of social
Experimental manipulation of products
with or without human characteristics

connection. Additionally, in a study of individuals with increased ac-


and subsequent questions regarding
Self-reported view of possessions as

quiring behavior, Norberg et al. (2018) found that anthropomorphism


and distress intolerance partially mediated the associated between an-
Measures of saving behaviors

xious attachment and compulsive buying. The authors concluded that


individuals with maladaptive interpersonal styles may engage in ex-
being self-expressive

cessive acquisition partially due to their difficulties with emotion reg-


ulation and tendency to humanize objects. Therefore, the tendency to
desire to save

imbue human characteristics on non-human beings may be an im-


portant indicator of attachment difficulties in individuals who hoard.
Finally, insecure attachment styles are associated with negative or
uncertain self-evaluation, and hoarding symptoms are significantly as-
sociated with self-ambivalence (Frost, Kyrios, McCarthy, & Matthews,
Experimental
Study design

2007) and identity confusion (Claes, Müller, & Luyckx, 2016). Further,
a recent study found that individuals with HD reported greater un-
certainty about self and others as compared to healthy controls (Kyrios
et al., 2018). Qualitative work has also indicated that individuals who
Study 1: 96; Study

hoard often describe saving possessions to feel connected to others and


2: 51; Study 3:

118;
327;
162;
184

to maintain a sense of identity (Roster, 2015).


Sample size

Taken together, research regarding interpersonal attachment pro-


1:
2:
3:
4:
Study
Study
Study
Study

cesses and hoarding symptoms is relatively nascent, and most, though


124

not all, of the extant literature indicates a positive association between


hoarding symptoms, attachment difficulties, and maladaptive inter-
personal functioning.
20.3;
33.0;
29.5;
36.0
Mean age

8. Interpersonal dysfunction and object attachment


1:
2:
3:
4:
Study
Study
Study
Study

Connections between interpersonal processes and object attach-


ment, more broadly, can largely be found in social psychology and
Undergraduate students and

consumer marketing research. Empirical work indicates that percep-


tions of other people as being unreliable is associated with increased
community adults

object attachment (Keefer et al., 2012). Additionally, loneliness is as-


sociated with increased liking of possessions (Lastovicka & Sirianni,
Sample type

2011) and preference for products that are associated with nostalgia or
affiliation with a group (Loveland, Smeesters, & Mandel, 2010; Mead,
Baumeister, Stillman, Rawn, & Vohs, 2011). In a separate, but related,
line of research, the study of anthropomorphism may also be relevant to
Johnson (2011)
Table 2 (continued)

understanding the link between interpersonal relationships and object


attachment. Anthropomorphism is thought to be a reaction to real or
Morrison and

Mourey et al.
Author/year

perceived social isolation (Chen, Wan, & Levy, 2017), and one study
(2017)

found that reminding people of close relationships reduces anthro-


pomorphism of both pets and objects (Bartz, Tchalova, & Fenerci,
2016). Anthropomorphism may also mitigate the impact of social

10
Table 3
Object attachment and hoarding symptoms.
Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of saving behaviors Measures of object attachment Main findings
B.M. Mathes, et al.

Burgess et al. Undergraduate students and 20.00 224 Correlational Anthropomorphism Saving Questionnaire; Graves Anthropomorphism was significantly
(2018) community adults Inventory Revised; Saving Anthropomorphism Task Scale and positively associated with
Cognitions Inventory hoarding behaviors and cognitions.
Cherrier and Individuals who classify Not reported; age 8 Qualitative Discussion of hoarding symptoms Discussion of attachment to Participants described saving
Ponnor themselves as “hoarders” range = 35-57 in a documentary video of each possessions in a documentary possessions to maintain emotional
(2010) participant video of each participant connection to the past, feel safe and
prepared for the future, and maintain
joy in the present
Dozier et al. Individuals with hoarding Clinical group: 61.83; Clinical group: 77; control Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Clutter Relationship Between Self and Object interconnectedness was
(2017) disorder and community control group: 42.80 group: 30 Inventory Revised; Saving Items Measure significantly and positively associated
controls Cognitions Inventory with overall hoarding symptoms and
difficulty discarding possessions, but
not acquiring behaviors, clutter in one's
home, saving cognitions, nor
depression and anxiety symptoms
Frost et al. Undergraduate students and Control group: not Control group: 101; Comparative and Hoarding Scale Control Over Possessions Scale; Emotional attachment to possessions
(1995) individuals with self- reported; Clinical group: Clinical group: 52 correlational Attachment to Possessions Scale; and perceived need to protect
identified hoarding 43 Environmental Consciousness possessions was significantly correlated
Scale; Responsibility for Harm with hoarding symptoms in both the
control group and clinical group.
Frost et al. Individuals with HD and HD group: 52.72; Control HD group: 103; Control Experimental Hoarding Rating Scale; Saving Possessions List-Discarding; Compared to community controls,
(2016) community controls group: 53.85 group: 66 Inventory Revised Saving Cognitions Inventory individuals with HD reported stronger
beliefs related to emotional
attachment, responsibility, memory,
utility, and aesthetic appeal about a

11
personal item and a lab item.
Frost et al. Individuals with hoarding HD group: 52.63; OCD HD group: 217; OCD Comparative Hoarding Rating Scale; Saving Hoarding Interview Individuals with HD reported
(2015) disorder, individuals with group: 34.54; Control group: 96; Control group: Inventory Revised significantly higher motives (aesthetic,
OCD, and community controls group: 52.63 130 avoid waste, emotion, information) for
saving and acquiring than did clinical
and healthy controls. Among
individuals with HD, avoiding waste
and aesthetic reasons were the
strongest predictors of acquiring
behaviors, whereas avoiding waste and
saving information were the strongest
predictors of saving behaviors.
Emotional attachment to possessions
was not a significant predictor of
behaviors in the HD group.
Gordon et al. Individuals with hoarding Hoarding - OCD: 59; Hoarding - OCD: 24; Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Clutter Beliefs about Hoarding Individuals with hoarding without
(2013) without OCD, individuals Hoarding + OCD: 50.6; Hoarding + OCD: 21; Inventory Revised; Compulsive Questionnaire OCD reported greater object
with hoarding with OCD, OCD – Hoarding: 32.5; OCD – Hoarding: 22; Acquisition Scale attachment disturbances than
individuals with OCD without Control: 51.4 Control: 21 individuals with OCD without hoarding
hoarding, and clinical and clinical controls; however, there
controls were no differences in attachment as
compared to individuals with hoarding
with OCD
Grisham et al. Individuals with OCD 43.42 62 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Object Attachment Hoarding behaviors and cognitions
(2009) Cognitions Inventory Questionnaire were significant predictors of
attachment to a novel item at baseline.
Attachment at baseline was a
significant predictor of attachment to
(continued on next page)
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
Table 3 (continued)

Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of saving behaviors Measures of object attachment Main findings

the item one week later, regardless of


B.M. Mathes, et al.

hoarding symptom severity.


Hartl et al. Individuals with hoarding and Clinical group: 54.27; Clinical group: 36; Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Possessions Comfort Scale Individuals with hoarding reported
(2005) healthy controls Control group: 50.17 Control group: 26 significantly greater comfort derived
from possessions than did healthy
controls.
Haws et al. Study 1: Community adults; Study 1: 39.00; Study 2: Study 1: 313; Study 2: Correlational Product Retention Tendency Attachment to Possessions Scale; Product retention tendency is
(2012) Studies 2,3,4,5: 21.00; Study 3: 21.00; 186; Study 3: 167; Study and Scale; Saving Inventory Revised; Self-reported attachment to significantly and positively associated
undergraduate students Study 4: 21.00; Study 5: 4: 211; Study 5: 278 experimental Self-reported likelihood of specific possessions with attachment to possessions.
21.00 discarding possessions However, compulsive hoarding
symptoms are more strongly associated
with attachment than is product
retention tendency.
Kellett et al. Individual with hoarding 55 1 Case study Self-reported number of objects Self-reported attachment to Cognitive behavioral therapy with a
(2010) disorder discarded each day and perceived possessions focus on object-affect fusion was
ease of ability to discard associated with significant reductions
in hoarding symptoms.
Kellett et al. Individuals with compulsive 38.41 11 Descriptive Semi-structured interview Semi-structured interview Participants described four common
(2010) hoarding themes that underlie their hoarding
behaviors: childhood factors,
relationship with possessions,
avoidance, and relationship with self
and others.
Kwok et al. Community adults and 22.88 331 Experimental Saving Inventory Revised; Social exclusion manipulation Individuals who were socially excluded
(2018) undergraduate students with Anthropomorphic Mental State (Cyberball) did not report greater
elevated acquiring behaviors ratings; self-reported object anthropomorphism than did socially

12
attachment and perceived value included individuals. However,
of an object regardless of condition, there was an
indirect effect of anthropomorphism on
object attachment via perceived
sentimental value.
Levy et al. Individuals with hoarding 56.55 62 Naturalistic Saving Inventory Revised Saving Cognitions Inventory Changes in saving cognitions, including
(2017) disorder treatment emotional attachment to possessions,
significantly mediated reductions in
hoarding behaviors following
individual or group cognitive
behavioral therapy for hoarding.
Moulding et al. Individuals with hoarding Treatment completers: Treatment completers: 41; Naturalistic Saving Inventory Revised Saving Cognitions Inventory There were large reductions in
(2017) disorder 53.5; treatment non- treatment non- treatment hoarding symptoms and moderate
completers: 55.1 completers: 36 reductions in beliefs about emotional
attachment to possessions.
Neave et al. Community adults 31.3 93 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Anthropomorphism Anthropomorphism was a significant
(2015) Cognitions Inventory Questionnaire predictor of hoarding behaviors and
cognitions, and women showed
significantly higher levels of
anthropomorphism than did men.
Nedelisky and Individuals with OCD with 46.09 Hoarding group: 14; Non- Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Reciprocal Attachment Individuals with hoarding reported
Steele and without hoarding hoarding group: 16 Questionnaire-Adapted significantly greater emotional
(2009) overinvolvement with possessions than
did individuals without hoarding.
Attachment to possessions was a
significant predictor of hoarding
symptoms.
Norberg et al. Individuals with acquiring 22.79 361 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Graves Anthropomorphic Task
(2018) behaviors Compulsive Acquisition Scale Scale-Comfort
(continued on next page)
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
Table 3 (continued)

Author/year Sample type Mean age Sample size Study design Measures of saving behaviors Measures of object attachment Main findings

Anthropomorphism was significantly


B.M. Mathes, et al.

and moderately associated with


hoarding symptoms.
Pertusa et al. Individuals with compulsive Hoarding with OCD Hoarding with OCD Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Self-reported descriptions of Individuals with hoarding reported
(2008) hoarding with OCD and group:; Hoarding without group: 25; Hoarding items saved saving items primarily due to
individuals with compulsive OCD group: without OCD group: 27 emotional or intrinsic value.
hoarding without OCD
Phung et al. Community adults 29.11 150 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised Saving Cognitions Inventory There was an indirect effect of
(2015) measures of emotion regulation
(anxiety sensitivity and negative
urgency) on hoarding symptoms via
beliefs about attachment to
possessions, when accounting for
anxiety and depression.
Roster (2015) Professional organizers who Not reported 28 Qualitative Narrative case descriptions of Narrative case descriptions of Individuals with hoarding described
work with individuals with hoarding behavior attachment styles attachment to possessions that provide
hoarding both affiliative and self-identity
motivations.
Shafran and Individuals with hoarding Not reported 3 Qualitative Narrative case descriptions of Narrative case descriptions of Participants described significant loss
Tallis hoarding behavior hoarding behavior and fear of criticism that they
(1996) attributed to early childhood
experiences.
Steketee et al. Individuals with hoarding, Hoarding group: 52.0; Hoarding group: 95; OCD Comparative Saving Inventory Saving Cognitions Inventory Individuals with hoarding reported
(2003) individuals with OCD, OCD group: 36.7; Control group: 21; Control group: significantly greater cognitions,
community controls group: 42.0 40 including beliefs about attachment to
possessions, than did individuals

13
without hoarding.
Steketee et al. Individuals with and without Hoarding group: 71.7; Hoarding group: 25; Non- Comparative Saving Inventory Revised Saving Cognitions Inventory Individuals with hoarding reported
(2012) hoarding Non-hoarding group: 77.2 hoarding group: 28 significantly greater beliefs about
attachment to possessions and
responsibility for possessions than did
individuals without hoarding.
Timpano and Undergraduate students 19.23 72 Correlational Saving Inventory Revised; Saving Individual Differences in Hoarding symptoms were significantly
Shaw Cognitions Inventory; Anthropomorphism and positively associated with
(2013) Compulsive Acquisition Scale Questionnaire anthropomorphism, and
anthropomorphism moderated the
impact of saving cognitions on
acquiring behaviors and attachment to
a novel item.
Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

exclusion (Mourey, Olson, & Yoon, 2017), though a recent study found and acquiring, not object attachment, and the measure used to assess
that individuals who were socially included and excluded did not differ reasons for saving was not validated and included only one item as-
in their self-reported anthropomorphism of objects after the social ex- sessing emotional attachment. Therefore, consistent with con-
clusion manipulation (Kwok, Grisham, & Norberg, 2018). The authors ceptualizations of hoarding, it may be that individuals who hoard save
of this recent study noted this may be due to the nature of the lab belongings for a variety of reasons, but those reasons for saving may
manipulation used (Cyberball), but more work is needed to assess the nonetheless contribute to increased attachment to possessions. Overall,
impact of acute versus chronic social stress on the tendency to imbue individuals who hoard describe extreme attachment to their possessions
human characteristics on non-human beings. Finally, possessions may that is consistent with conceptualizations of interpersonal attachment
be used an attempt to compensate for one's uncertain sense of self by figures.
creating a sense of connection to others (Ferraro, Escalas, & Bettman,
2011; Morrison & Johnson, 2011), providing further evidence for the 10. Discussion and future directions
connection between interpersonal factors and object attachment.
Nonetheless, there is evidence that objects, in general, may be used to Taken together, existing work largely supports our hypothesis that
compensate for negative beliefs about oneself and others. HD is characterized by dysfunctional relationships with both people
and possessions, thereby supporting our theoretical model. However,
9. Object attachment and hoarding symptoms considerable research is needed to elucidate the nature of the re-
lationship between hoarding and interpersonal factors. To start, future
It is well-established that individuals who hoard hold an extreme research should further examine the relationship between object at-
attachment to possessions. The behaviors characteristic of HD largely tachment and interpersonal attachment in HD. Though some studies
serve to maintain proximity to and avoid separation from possessions, suggest that individuals who hoard hold an insecure attachment with
which consistent with attachment theory conceptualizations of a secure people (Grisham et al., 2018; Medard & Kellett, 2014), there is limited
base. Indeed, a core symptom of HD is the inability to part with pos- empirical support for the connection between object and interpersonal
sessions due to the perceived distress that would arise from discarding attachment within the area of HD. There is also a dearth of work ex-
(APA, 2013). Further, when compared to healthy and clinical controls, amining the extent to which insecure attachment is associated with
individuals who hoard report greater attachment to possessions and interpersonal dysfunction specifically among individuals with HD. As
tendency to seek comfort in possessions (Frost et al., 2015; Frost, Ong, such, more work is needed to delineate the specific interpersonal dif-
Steketee, & Tolin, 2016; Gordon, Salkovskis, & Oldfield, 2013; Hartl ficulties that may arise as a result of insecure attachment within HD.
et al., 2005; Nedelisky & Steele, 2009; Pertusa et al., 2008; Steketee Additionally, research regarding object attachment has focused on at-
et al., 2003; Steketee, Schmalisch, Dierberger, DeNobel, & Frost, 2012). tachments to objects that are owned; however, future work should
Hoarding symptoms are also correlated with increased emotional explore the extent to which attachments might generalize to objects
comfort derived from possessions and greater beliefs related to one's regardless of ownership status. Finally, given that HD is commonly
attachment to possessions (Frost et al., 1995; Grisham et al., 2009; comorbid with disorders characterized by social difficulties (e.g., social
Haws, Naylor, Coulter, & Bearden, 2012; Phung, Moulding, Taylor, & anxiety, depression; Frost et al., 2011), future work should examine the
Nedeljkovic, 2015). Moreover, individuals who hoard report viewing extent to which attachment disturbances are unique to HD or represent
their possessions as extensions of themselves and their individuality overlap with comorbid conditions.
(Dozier, Taylor, Castriotta, Mayes, & Ayers, 2017; Roster, 2015), and Future research should also further explore potential subgroups
feel as though parting with possessions is like losing a part of who they within HD to which attachment disturbances most apply. There is
are (Steketee et al., 2003). In treatment, there are moderate reductions evidence to suggest that sentimental beliefs about possessions may not
in beliefs about emotional attachment to possessions (Moulding, be a primary reason for saving, which may suggest object attachment is
Nedeljkovic, Kyrios, Osborne, & Mogan, 2017), changes in beliefs about not an integral piece of the disorder for all individuals. Indeed, in-
attachment to possessions are a significant mediator of treatment out- dividuals who hoard often report saving objects for many reasons, in-
come (Levy et al., 2017), and a case study indicated that a focus on cluding sentimentality, avoidance of waste, aesthetic reasons, feelings
object-affect fusion was associated with reductions in hoarding symp- of responsibility for the item, remembering information, and main-
toms (Kellett et al., 2010). Finally, qualitative studies have further taining control of their environment (Frost et al., 2015; Gordon et al.,
described dysfunctional attachments to possessions characterized by 2013; Steketee et al., 2003). Additionally, in one study comparing
use of possessions for comfort in those who hoard (Cherrier & Ponnor, hoarding beliefs and symptoms in undergraduates in China and the
2010; Kellett, Greenhalgh, Beail, & Ridgway, 2010; Shafran & Tallis, United States, concerns about usefulness and wastefulness, but not
1996). For example, Frost and Hartl (1996) described a client who memory, attachment, control, responsibility, and aesthetics, were sig-
explained that after a stressful day, he/she wanted to “go home and nificantly associated with hoarding symptoms in China, whereas all
gather my treasures (things) around me. seven beliefs about possessions were associated with hoarding symp-
Additionally, anthropomorphism is thought to be a component of toms in the U.S. (Timpano et al., 2015). Overall, the aforementioned
one's attachment to possessions (Frost & Hartl, 1996) and is moderately findings regarding reasons for saving may suggest the presence of
associated with hoarding symptoms (Burgess et al., 2018; Neave, subgroups that may be differentially associated with attachment dis-
Jackson, Saxton, & Hönekopp, 2015; Norberg et al., 2018; Timpano & turbances. However, it may also be that, consistent with the proposed
Shaw, 2013). A recent study also found an indirect effect of anthro- model, reasons for saving interact with attachment disturbances. For
pomorphism on object attachment via perceived sentimental value, example, an individual might save their belongings due to their per-
indicating an association between imbuing human characteristics on ceived use, but then be unable to part with those items due to the be-
items and attachment to those items (Kwok et al., 2018). Further, many havioral attachment related to unmet interpersonal needs. Indeed, in-
individuals with HD report believing that discarding a possession would itial support for this hypothesis can be found in a recent study that
be like losing a friend or a part of themselves, and often describe grief found an indirect effect of anthropomorphism on object attachment via
reactions when discarding (Frost et al., 1995; Steketee et al., 2003). both instrumental and sentimental value (Kwok et al., 2018), indicating
It should be noted that a recent study found that beliefs about at- that both sentimental and non-sentimental beliefs likely underlie saving
tachment to possessions was not a significant predictor of saving be- behavior. Nonetheless, further empirical work is needed to assess this
haviors when considered with other hoarding-related beliefs, such as hypothesis.
saving for aesthetic reasons or to retain information (Frost et al., 2015). There is also a dearth of work regarding the underlying mechanisms
However, this study was importantly focused on motivations for saving of attachment relevant to HD. Bowlby (1982) suggested that

14
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

attachment represents a broader construct characterized by cognitive avoid waste were the strongest cross-cultural predictors of hoarding
biases about oneself and others. In particular, individuals may view symptoms (Timpano et al., 2015). Therefore, culture may be an im-
other people as being unreliable, threatening, and/or harmful, thereby portant consideration when assessing this relationship. Additionally,
contributing to their decreased attachment to others. Initial work sug- future work should further assess the impact of information processing
gests that hoarding symptoms are associated with greater aggression deficits on attachment and hoarding symptoms. Though research is
and hostility (Mathes et al., 2019, 2017) and greater emotional reac- mixed (see Woody, Kellman-McFarlane, & Welsted, 2014 for a review),
tions in response to interpersonal stress (Grisham et al., 2018; Mathes individuals with HD are thought to have memory deficits that con-
et al., 2019), though further work is needed to identify and elucidate tribute to or exacerbate their tendency to save as they keep items as an
specific mechanisms. Moreover, exploration of genetic and biological attempt to aid memory. Therefore, a further exploration regarding the
mechanisms implicated in attachment is sorely needed. For example, extent to which these deficits may relate to attachment processes is
oxytocin and vasopressin have been implicated as integral in the de- warranted.
velopment of interpersonal attachment (e.g., Carter, 1998; Feldman, Finally, and perhaps most importantly, there is a need for standar-
2012), though no study to date has explored these mechanisms in HD. dization of measurement as there is significant variation in the mea-
Additionally, given that approximately 50% of variance in HD is ac- sures used to assess hoarding and attachment across studies. To start,
counted for genetics (Iervolino et al., 2009), an investigation of po- the hoarding literature continues to be clouded by historical overlap
tential genetic traits that may confer risk for attachment disturbances with OCD, as many studies of hoarding utilize samples of individuals
may prove promising. with OCD. Additionally, the attachment literature, in general, is hin-
Additionally, longitudinal studies are needed to examine the tem- dered by inconsistency across measures (Fraley et al., 2015), and this is
poral precedence of attachment processes and hoarding symptoms. further exacerbated in HD research by the lack of standardized measure
Importantly, though our proposed model posits that attachment dis- of attachment to possessions. Indeed, many studies use the Saving
turbances lead to hoarding symptoms, it is quite possible that hoarding Cognitions Inventory, which assess emotional attachment to posses-
symptoms actually contribute to attachment disturbances. As noted, sions as part of a myriad of distorted cognitions (Steketee et al., 2003).
individuals with HD experience considerable interpersonal difficulties, More recently, some researchers have started using an adapted version
often due to conflict with loved ones and community members re- of the Reciprocal Attachment Questionnaire, which is a measure of
garding belongings and clutter (Kim et al., 2001; Tolin et al., 2008). interpersonal attachment based on attachment theory (Nedelisky &
Further, individuals with HD are prone to self-criticism and shame Steele, 2009). However, the adapted RAQ lacks validity, thereby lim-
(Chou et al., 2018), all of which may arise in response to the symptoms iting the meaningfulness of its use. Further, behavioral assessments of
and associated consequences. Therefore, it may be that HD is actually a attachment in HD would likely be fruitful for exploring in vivo re-
risk factor for attachment disturbances and interpersonal difficulties, sponses to objects. Though there are a handful of behavioral tasks often
such as interpersonal conflict, social isolation, and loneliness. As such, used in research regarding hoarding (Shaw & Timpano, 2016; Tolin,
future work is needed to identify the direction of the relationship be- Kiehl, Worhunsky, Book, & Maltby, 2009), such tasks are not con-
tween attachment and hoarding in order to enhance our understanding ceptualized as measuring attachment per say. The development of a
of these phenomena and inform the development of prevention and task that assesses behavioral components of attachment, such as
treatment efforts. showing preference for an attachment figure, would elucidate the
Similarly, it would be interesting to examine the longitudinal re- nature of hoarding behaviors.
lationship between object and interpersonal attachment in hoarding.
We suggest that these factors concurrently interact, but there may be 11. Clinical implications
temporal precedence. Further, investigations of associations between
hoarding symptoms, cognitive beliefs, and attachment disturbances Our model may have implications for the assessment and treatment
over the lifespan are needed to determine temporal precedence. It may of HD, as the evaluation of interpersonal factors may provide im-
be that attachment disturbances are present in childhood and worsen portant, yet previously overlooked, information about the nature of the
over time, or alternatively, life stressors, such as trauma, may result in disorder. Though it is standard practice to assess attachment to pos-
the development of attachment disturbances, which, in turn, contribute sessions in the evaluation of HD, the assessment of interpersonal at-
to hoarding symptoms. Similarly, more work is needed to determine the tachment and biases are not always included. Therefore, understanding
factors that contribute to the development of object attachment, and to one's views of others may help to clarify the nature and severity of one's
the tendency to use possessions to attempt to compensate for unmet attachment to possessions. Moreover, interpersonal factors may need to
interpersonal needs. Though the core vulnerabilities identified in the be more directly addressed in treatment. In particular, it may be that
model (e.g., genetics, family history), likely play an important role, interpersonal treatments or a combination of CBT for HD and inter-
research is needed to determine the ways in which patterns of attach- personal treatments may prove promising. Several evidence-based
ment to possessions develop. treatments directly target interpersonal difficulties and interpretation
Another line of related research that may prove promising is that of biases, including Cognitive Behavioral Analysis System of
prolonged grief. As noted, many individuals who hoard report experi- Psychotherapy (McCullough, 2003) and Interpersonal Psychotherapy
encing a significant interpersonal loss prior to the onset or exacerbation (IPT; Klerman & Weissman, 1994). Support for the hypothesis that
of their symptoms (Landau et al., 2011; Tolin et al., 2010). Therefore, it targeting interpersonal problems may prove promising can be found in
may be that the loss of an attachment figure results in a need to replace the depression and eating disorder literature, in which IPT has been
that void, but instead of replacing it with other people, it becomes re- shown to be as effective as or more effective than CBT (Ekeblad,
placed with possessions. As such, it may not necessarily be that in- Falkenström, Andersson, Vestberg, & Holmqvist, 2016; Fairburn et al.,
dividuals who hoard have had insecure attachments with all people, but 2015). As stated above, HD is commonly comorbid with depression
rather they have lost their primary source of attachment and do not feel (Frost, Steketee, & Tolin, 2011). Moreover, HD and binge eating dis-
they can turn to others, which exacerbates their biological predisposi- order are both driven by similar processes, including compulsive be-
tion to HD. haviors, emotion regulation, and insecure attachment (Grisham et al.,
Research should also explore potential moderators of the relation- 2018; Raines, Boffa, Allan, Short, & Schmidt, 2014; Tasca & Balfour,
ship between attachment and hoarding symptoms. Some research has 2014). Importantly, evidence-based treatments for depression and
suggested that emotional attachment to possessions may be most re- binge eating disorder directly addresses negative views of oneself and
levant in Euro-American culture (Kings, Moulding, & Knight, 2017), as have shown to be effective in reducing symptoms (Cuijpers et al., 2013;
beliefs about the perceived usefulness of possessions and/or a desire to Rieger et al., 2010). In addition, given the integral role of both emotion

15
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

regulation and interpersonal difficulties in HD, dialectical behavior American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental
therapy (DBT) may prove promising. DBT is an evidence-based treat- disorders (5th ed.). American Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.
books.9780890425596.744053.
ment that improves mindfulness, distress tolerance, emotion regulation, An, S. K., Mataix-Cols, D., Lawrence, N. S., Wooderson, S., Giampietro, V., Speckens, A.,
and interpersonal effectiveness (Linehan, 1993) and has been shown to et al. (2009). To discard or not to discard: The neural basis of hoarding symptoms in
be effective for a variety of conditions (Neacsiu, Eberle, Kramer, obsessive-compulsive disorder. Molecular Psychiatry, 14(3), 318–331. https://doi.
org/10.1038/sj.mp.4002129.
Wiesmann, & Linehan, 2014; Robins & Chapman, 2004) Therefore, DBT Ayers, C. R., Castriotta, N., Dozier, M. E., Espejo, E. P., & Porter, B. (2014). Behavioral and
may be a promising treatment strategy that targets all aspects of the experiential avoidance in patients with hoarding disorder. Journal of Behavior Therapy
proposed model. and Experimental Psychiatry, 45(3), 408–414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.
04.005.
Should future work support our hypotheses regarding the integral Bartz, J. A., Tchalova, K., & Fenerci, C. (2016). Reminders of social connection can at-
role of interpersonal attachment in the development and maintenance tenuate anthropomorphism: A replication and extension of Epley, Akalis, Waytz, and
of HD, it may help to explain why treatment to date has been shown to Cacioppo (2008). Psychological Science, 27(12), 1644-1650.
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal
be only moderately effective. Specifically, given that the need for se-
attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),
curity is a basic human need, it may be that until interpersonal at- 497–529. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497.
tachment difficulties are resolved, possessions will continue to remain Bowlby, J. (1979). The Bowlby-Ainsworth attachment theory. Behavioral and Brain
the “secure base” for the individual. As such, even if cognitive beha- Sciences, 2(4), 637–638. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00064955.
Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Retrospect and prospect. American Journal of
vioral strategies focused on reducing the importance of possessions is Orthopsychiatry, 52(4), 664–678. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.
effective, the strong emotional attachment will remain until the inter- tb01456.x.
personal difficulties are addressed. Brennan, K. A., Clark, C. L., & Shaver, P. R. (1998). Self-report measurement of adult
attachment: An integrative overview. Attachment theory and close relationshipshttps://
doi.org/10.1097/00005768-199606000-00014.
12. Conclusions Burgess, A. M., Graves, L. M., & Frost, R. O. (2018). My possessions need me:
Anthropomorphism and hoarding. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 59(3),
340–348. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12441.
HD is a severe and persistent mental illness characterized by ex- Carter, S. (1998). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love.
treme difficulty parting with possessions and considerable clutter that Psychoneuroendocrinology. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00055-9.
can result in dangerous living conditions. HD poses a considerable Chen, D., Bienvenu, O. J., Krasnow, J., Wang, Y., Grados, M. A., Cullen, B., et al. (2017a).
Parental bonding and hoarding in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Comprehensive
public burden, as clean-up costs are substantial and it is estimated the Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.11.004.
cost per individual may be greater than that of related disorders, in- Chen, R. P., Wan, E. W., & Levy, E. (2017b). The effect of social exclusion on consumer
cluding depression and anxiety. Though research regarding HD has preference for anthropomorphized brands. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 27(1),
23–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2016.05.004.
advanced in recent years, there remain important gaps in the literature,
Cherrier, H., & Ponnor, T. (2010). A study of hoarding behavior and attachment to ma-
particularly regarding the pathological attachment individuals with HD terial possessions. Qualitative Market Research: An International Journal, 13(1), 8–23.
hold to their possessions. Moreover, treatment for HD remains rela- https://doi.org/10.1108/13522751011013945.
tively limited, as many individuals do not respond to treatment and/or Chou, C. Y., Tsoh, J., Vigil, O., Bain, D., Uhm, S. Y., Howell, G., et al. (2018).
Contributions of self-criticism and shame to hoarding. Psychiatry Research, 262,
do not maintain treatment gains, suggesting there are important factors 488–493.
not being adequately addressed. Claes, L., Müller, A., & Luyckx, K. (2016). Compulsive buying and hoarding as identity
Therefore, we propose that attachment theory may provide an im- substitutes: The role of materialistic value endorsement and depression.
Comprehensive Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.04.005.
portant foundation by which to examine HD. Specifically, we propose Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models, and relationship
that individuals who hoard view possessions as their primary attach- quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(4), 644.
ment figure in order to compensate for an insecure attachment with Cromer, K. R., Schmidt, N. B., & Murphy, D. L. (2007). Do traumatic events influence the
clinical expression of compulsive hoarding? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(11),
people. The hypotheses put forth in this theory may help to advance our 2581–2592. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.06.005.
knowledge of HD and to identify potential factors that can be targeted Crone, C., Kwok, C., Chau, V., & Norberg, M. M. (2019). Applying attachment theory to
in intervention and prevention efforts. Indeed, underlying interpersonal indecisiveness in hoarding disorder. Psychiatry Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
psychres.2019.01.055.
factors may play a larger role in the etiology and maintenance of the
Cuijpers, P., Berking, M., Andersson, G., Quigley, L., Kleiboer, A., & Dobson, K. S. (2013).
disorder than previously thought. Nonetheless, future research should A meta-analysis of cognitive-behavioural therapy for adult depression, alone and in
test the hypotheses put forth in this manuscript, and should further comparison with other treatments. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. https://doi.org/
10.1177/070674371305800702.
explore potential mechanisms that may help to explain the relationship
Danet, M., & Secouet, D. (2018). Insecure attachment as a factor in hoarding behaviors in
between hoarding and attachment. a non-clinical sample of women. Psychiatry Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
psychres.2018.09.053.
Author disclosure Dickinson, K. A., & Pincus, A. L. (2003). Interpersonal analysis of grandiose and vul-
nerable narcissism. Journal of personality disorders, 17(3), 188–207.
Dozier, M. E., Porter, B., & Ayers, C. R. (2016). Age of onset and progression of hoarding
The authors of this manuscript declare no conflict of interest. The symptoms in older adults with hoarding disorder. Aging & Mental Health, 20(7),
authors did not receive funding from an external source for this work. 736–742. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2015.1033684.
Dozier, M. E., Taylor, C. T., Castriotta, N., Mayes, T. L., & Ayers, C. R. (2017). A pre-
liminary investigation of the measurement of object interconnectedness in hoarding
CRediT authorship contribution statement disorder. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 41(5), 799–805. https://doi.org/10.1007/
s10608-017-9845-x.
Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic
Brittany M. Mathes: Conceptualization, Methodology, process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1(3), 192–205. https://doi.org/10.
Investigation, Writing - original draft. Kiara R. Timpano: 1680/ijct.2008.1.3.192.
Conceptualization, Methodology, Writing - review & editing. Amanda Ein-Dor, T., & Doron, G. (2015). Psychopathology and attachment. Attachment theory and
research: New directions and emerging themes. 2015.
M. Raines: Conceptualization, Writing - review & editing. Norman B.
Ekeblad, A., Falkenström, F., Andersson, G., Vestberg, R., & Holmqvist, R. (2016).
Schmidt: Conceptualization, Writing - review & editing. Randomized trial of interpersonal psychotherapy and cognitive behavioral therapy
for major depressive disorder in a community-based psychiatric outpatient clinic.
Depression and Anxiety. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.22495.
References
Epley, N., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). On seeing human: A three-factor theory of
anthropomorphism. Psychological Review, 114(4), 864–886. https://doi.org/10.1037/
Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1991). Attachments and other affectional bonds across the life cycle. 0033-295X.114.4.864.
In C. M. Parkes, J. Stevenson-Hinde, & P. Marris (Eds.). Attachment across the life cycle Fairburn, C. G., Bailey-Straebler, S., Basden, S., Doll, H. A., Jones, R., Murphy, R., et al.
(pp. 33–51). New York, NY, US: Tavistock/Routledge. (2015). A transdiagnostic comparison of enhanced cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT-
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. N. (2015). Patterns of attach- E) and interpersonal psychotherapy in the treatment of eating disorders. Behaviour
ment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Patterns of attachment: A psy- Research and Therapy. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2015.04.010.
chological study of the strange situation. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203758045. Feldman, R. (2012). Oxytocin and social affiliation in humans. Hormones And Behavior.

16
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.01.008. Journal of Psychiatry, 166(10), 1156–1161. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.


Fernández de la Cruz, L., Landau, D., Iervolino, A. C., Santo, S., Pertusa, A., Singh, S., 08121789.
et al. (2013). Experiential avoidance and emotion regulation difficulties in hoarding Jones, W. H., Freemon, J. E., & Goswick, R. A. (1981). The persistence of loneliness: Self
disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(2), 204–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. and other determinants 1. Journal of personality, 49(1), 27–48.
janxdis.2013.01.004. Keefer, L. A., Landau, M. J., Rothschild, Z. K., & Sullivan, D. (2012). Attachment to objects
Ferraro, R., Escalas, J. E., & Bettman, J. R. (2011). Our possessions, our selves: Domains of as compensation for close others' perceived unreliability. Journal of Experimental
self-worth and the possession-self link. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 21(2), Social Psychology, 48(4), 912–917. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2012.02.007.
169–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2010.08.007. Kehoe, E., & Egan, J. (2019). Interpersonal attachment insecurity and emotional at-
Fitch, K. E., & Cougle, J. R. (2013). Perceived and actual information processing deficits tachment to possessions partly mediate the relationship between childhood trauma
in nonclinical hoarding. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 2(2), and hoarding symptoms in a non-clinical sample. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and
192–199. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2013.02.006. Related Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2018.12.001.
Fraley, R. C., Heffernan, M. E., Vicary, A. M., & Brumbaugh, C. C. (2011). The experiences Kellett, S., Greenhalgh, R., Beail, N., & Ridgway, N. (2010). Compulsive hoarding: An
in close relationships—relationship Structures questionnaire: A method for assessing interpretative phenomenological analysis. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy,
attachment orientations across relationships. Psychological Assessment, 23(3), 615. 38(2), 141–155. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1352465809990622.
Fraley, C. R., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., & Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment Kim, H.-J., Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2001). Hoarding by elderly people. Health & Social
styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship- Work, 26(3), 176–184. https://doi.org/10.1093/hsw/26.3.176.
specific attachment orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. https:// Kings, C. A., Moulding, R., & Knight, T. (2017). You are what you own: Reviewing the link
doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000027. between possessions, emotional attachment, and the self-concept in hoarding dis-
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item response theory analysis of order. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1016/
self- report measures of adult attachment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, j.jocrd.2017.05.005.
78, 350–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.2.350. Klerman, G. L., & Weissman, M. M. (1994). Interpersonal psychotherapy of depression: A
Frost, R. O., & Gross, R. C. (1993). The hoarding of possessions. Behaviour Research and brief, focused, specific strategy. Jason Aronson, Incorporated.
Therapy, 31(4), 367–381. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(93)90094-B. Kwok, C., Grisham, J. R., & Norberg, M. M. (2018). Object attachment: Humanness in-
Frost, R. O., & Hartl, T. L. (1996). A cognitive-behavioral model of compulsive hoarding. creases sentimental and instrumental values. Journal of behavioral addictions, 7(4),
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 34(4), 341–350. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005- 1132–1142. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.98.
7967(95)00071-2. Kyrios, M. (2014). Psychological models of hoarding. The Oxford handbook of hoarding and
Frost, R. O., Hartl, T. L., Christian, R., & Williams, N. (1995). The value of possessions in acquiring. 206–220.
compulsive hoarding: Patterns of use and attachment. Behaviour Research and Kyrios, M., Mogan, C., Moulding, R., Frost, R. O., Yap, K., & Fassnacht, D. B. (2018). The
Therapy, 33(8), 897–902. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(95)00043-W. cognitive–behavioural model of hoarding disorder: Evidence from clinical and non-
Frost, R. O., Kyrios, M., McCarthy, K. D., & Matthews, Y. (2007). Self-ambivalence and clinical cohorts. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 25(2), 311–321. https://doi.org/
attachment to possessions. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 21(3), 232–242. 10.1002/cpp.2164.
https://doi.org/10.1891/088983907781494582. Landau, D., Iervolino, A. C., Pertusa, A., Santo, S., Singh, S., & Mataix-Cols, D. (2011).
Frost, R. O., Ong, C., Steketee, G., & Tolin, D. F. (2016). Behavioral and emotional con- Stressful life events and material deprivation in hoarding disorder. Journal of Anxiety
sequences of thought listing versus cognitive restructuring during discarding deci- Disorders, 25(2), 192–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.09.002.
sions in hoarding disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 85, 13–22. https://doi. Lastovicka, J. L., & Sirianni, N. J. (2011). Truly, madly, deeply: Consumers in the throes
org/10.1016/j.brat.2016.08.003. of material possession love. Journal of Consumer Research. https://doi.org/10.1086/
Frost, R. O., Patronek, G., & Rosenfield, E. (2011a). Comparison of object and animal 658338.
hoarding. Depression And Anxiety. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20826. Levy, H. C., Worden, B. L., Gilliam, C. M., D'Urso, C., Steketee, G., Frost, R. O., et al.
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Tolin, D. F. (2011b). Comorbidity in hoarding disorder. (2017). Changes in saving cognitions mediate hoarding symptom change in cogni-
Depression and Anxiety, 28(10), 876–884. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20861. tive-behavioral therapy for hoarding disorder. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and
Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., Tolin, D. F., Sinopoli, N., & Ruby, D. (2015). Motives for ac- Related Disorders, 14, 112–118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017.06.008.
quiring and saving in hoarding disorder, OCD, and community controls. Journal of Lidz, F. (2003). Ghosty Men: The Strange But True Story of the Collyer Brothers, New York’s
Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 4, 54–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd. Greatest Hoarders: an Urban Historical. Bloomsbury Publishing USA.
2014.12.006. Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality dis-
Gordon, O. M., Salkovskis, P. M., & Oldfield, V. B. (2013). Beliefs and experiences in order. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. https://doi.
hoarding. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 27(3), 328–339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. org/10.1017/CBO9781107415324.004.
janxdis.2013.02.009. Loveland, K. E., Smeesters, D., & Mandel, N. (2010). Still preoccupied with 1995: The
Grisham, J. R., Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., Kim, H. J., & Hood, S. (2006). Age of onset of need to belong and preference for nostalgic products. Journal of Consumer Research,
compulsive hoarding. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 20(5), 675–686. https://doi.org/ 37(3), 393–408. https://doi.org/10.1086/653043.
10.1016/j.janxdis.2005.07.004. Mataix-Cols, D., Wooderson, S., Lawrence, N., Brammer, M. J., Speckens, A., & Phillips,
Grisham, J. R., Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., Kim, H. J., Tarkoff, A., & Hood, S. (2009). M. L. (2004). Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom
Formation of attachment to possessions in compulsive hoarding. Journal of Anxiety dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61(6),
Disorders, 23(3), 357–361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.12.006. 564–576. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.564.
Grisham, J. R., Martyn, C., Kerin, F., Baldwin, P. A., & Norberg, M. M. (2018). Mathes, B. M., Kennedy, G. A., Cougle, J. R., & Schmidt, N. B. (2019). An examination of
Interpersonal functioning in Hoarding Disorder: An examination of attachment styles the relationship between hoarding symptoms and hostility. Journal of Psychiatric
and emotion regulation in response to interpersonal stress. Journal of Obsessive- Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2019.01.025.
Compulsive and Related Disorders, 16, 43–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2017. Mathes, B. M., Portero, A. K., Gibby, B. A., King, S. L., Raines, A. M., & Schmidt, N. B.
12.001. (2017). Interpersonal trauma and hoarding: The mediating role of aggression. Journal
Grisham, J. R., Norberg, M. M., Williams, A. D., Certoma, S. P., & Kadib, R. (2010). of Affective Disorders, 227, 512–516. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.062.
Categorization and cognitive deficits in compulsive hoarding. Behaviour Research and McCullough, J. P. (2003). Treatment for chronic depression: Cognitive behavioral ana-
Therapy, 48(9), 866–872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2010.05.011. lysis system of psychotherapy (CBASP). Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. https://
Grisham, J. R., Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2008). Interpersonal problems and emotional doi.org/10.1037/1053-0479.13.3-4.241.
intelligence in compulsive hoarding. Depression and Anxiety, 25(9), E63–E71. https:// McGuire, J. F., Kaercher, L., Park, J. M., & Storch, E. A. (2013). Hoarding in the com-
doi.org/10.1002/da.20327. munity: A code enforcement and social service perspective. Journal of Social Service
Grossmann, K. E., & Grossmann, K. (2006). Attachment quality as an organizer of emo- Research, 39(3), 335–344. https://doi.org/10.1080/01488376.2013.770813.
tional and behavioral responses in a longitudinal perspective. Attachment across the Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F., Rawn, C. D., & Vohs, K. D. (2011). Social
life cycle (pp. 101–122). Routledge. exclusion causes people to spend and consume strategically in the service of affilia-
Hallion, L. S., Diefenbach, G. J., & Tolin, D. F. (2015). Poor memory confidence mediates tion. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(5), 902–919. https://doi.org/10.1086/
the association between inattention symptoms and hoarding severity and impair- 656667.
ment. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 7, 43–48. https://doi.org/ Medard, E., & Kellett, S. (2014). The role of adult attachment and social support in
10.1016/j.jocrd.2015.10.003. hoarding disorder. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 42(5), 629–633. https://
Hartl, T. L., Duffany, S. R., Allen, G. J., Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2005). Relationships doi.org/10.1017/S1352465813000659.
among compulsive hoarding, trauma, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and
Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(2), 269–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat. change. Guilford Press.
2004.02.002. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2012). An attachment perspective on psychopathology.
Hartl, T. L., Frost, R. O., Allen, G. J., Deckersbach, T., Steketee, G., Duffany, S. R., et al. World Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wpsyc.2012.01.003.
(2004). Actual and perceived memory deficits in individuals with compulsive Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., & Pereg, D. (2003). Attachment theory and affect regula-
hoarding. Depression and Anxiety, 20(2), 59–69. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20010. tion: The dynamics, development, and cognitive consequences of attachment-related
Haws, K. L., Naylor, R. W., Coulter, R. A., & Bearden, W. O. (2012). Keeping it all without strategies. Motivation and Emotion. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024515519160.
being buried alive: Understanding product retention tendency. Journal of Consumer Morrison, K. R., & Johnson, C. S. (2011). When what you have is who you are: Self-
Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2011.05.003. uncertainty leads individualists to see themselves in their possessions. Personality and
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. R. (2004). Attachment as an organizational framework for re- Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(5), 639–651. https://doi.org/10.1177/
search on close relationships. Close relationships: Key readings (pp. 186–214). . https:// 0146167211403158.
doi.org/10.4324/9780203311851. Moulding, R., Mancuso, S. G., Rehm, I., & Nedeljkovic, M. (2016). The self in the obsessive-
Iervolino, A. C., Perroud, N., Fullana, M. A., Guipponi, M., Cherkas, L., Collier, D. A., et al. compulsive-related disorders: Hoarding disorder, body dysmorphic disorder, and tricho-
(2009). Prevalence and heritability of compulsive hoarding: A twin study. American tillomania. The Self in Understanding and Treating Psychological Disorders123–133.

17
B.M. Mathes, et al. Behaviour Research and Therapy 125 (2020) 103549

https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139941297.014. Shafran, R., & Tallis, F. (1996). Obsessive-compulsive hoarding: A cognitive-behavioural


Moulding, R., Nedeljkovic, M., Kyrios, M., Osborne, D., & Mogan, C. (2017). Short-term approach. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 24(3), 209–221.
cognitive–behavioural group treatment for hoarding disorder: A naturalistic treat- Shaw, A. M., & Timpano, K. R. (2016). An experimental investigation of the effect of stress
ment outcome study. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 24(1), 235–244. https:// on saving and acquiring behavioral tendencies: The role of distress tolerance and
doi.org/10.1002/cpp.2001. negative urgency. Behavior Therapy, 47(1), 116–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.
Mourey, J. A., Olson, J. G., & Yoon, C. (2017). Products as pals: Engaging with anthro- 2015.10.003.
pomorphic products mitigates the effects of social exclusion. Journal of Consumer Shaw, A. M., Timpano, K. R., Steketee, G., Tolin, D. F., & Frost, R. O. (2015). Hoarding
Research, 44(2), 414–431. https://doi.org/10.1093/jcr/ucx038. and emotional reactivity: The link between negative emotional reactions and
Neacsiu, A. D., Eberle, J. W., Kramer, R., Wiesmann, T., & Linehan, M. M. (2014). hoarding symptomatology. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 63, 84–90. https://doi.
Dialectical behavior therapy skills for transdiagnostic emotion dysregulation: A pilot org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.02.009.
randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 59, 40–51. https://doi. Simpson, J. A., & Rholes, W. S. (1998). Adult attachment patterns: A test of the typolo-
org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.05.005. gical model. Attachment theory and close relationships.
Neave, N., Jackson, R., Saxton, T., & Hönekopp, J. (2015). The influence of anthro- Simpson, J. A., Rholes, W. S., & Phillips, D. (1996). Conflict in close relationships: An
pomorphic tendencies on human hoarding behaviours. Personality and Individual attachment perspective. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology, 71(5), 899–914.
Differences, 72, 214–219. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.041. Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2013). Treatment for hoarding disorder: Workbook. Oxford
Neave, N., Tyson, H., McInnes, L., & Hamilton, C. (2016). The role of attachment style and University Press.
anthropomorphism in predicting hoarding behaviours in a non-clinical sample. Steketee, G., Frost, R. O., & Kyrios, M. (2003). Cognitive aspects of compulsive hoarding.
Personality and Individual Differences, 99, 33–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27(4), 463–479. https://doi.org/10.1023/
2016.04.067. A:1025428631552.
Nedelisky, A., & Steele, M. (2009). Attachment to people and to objects in obsessive- Steketee, G., Schmalisch, C. S., Dierberger, A., DeNobel, D., & Frost, R. O. (2012).
compulsive disorder: An exploratory comparison of hoarders and non-hoarders. Symptoms and history of hoarding in older adults. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and
Attachment & Human Development, 11(4), 365–383. https://doi.org/10.1080/ Related Disorders. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jocrd.2011.10.001.
14616730903016987. Tasca, G. A., & Balfour, L. (2014). Attachment and eating disorders: A review of current
Norberg, M. M., Crone, C., Kwok, C., & Grisham, J. R. (2018). Anxious attachment and research. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 47(7), 710–717. https://doi.org/10.
excessive acquisition: The mediating roles of anthropomorphism and distress intol- 1002/eat.22302.
erance. Journal of Behavioral Addictions. https://doi.org/10.1556/2006.7.2018.08. Timpano, K. R., Broman-Fulks, J. J., Glaesmer, H., Exner, C., Rief, W., Olatunji, B. O.,
Nordsletten, A. E., Reichenberg, A., Hatch, S. L., Fernández De La Cruz, L., Pertusa, A., et al. (2013a). A taxometric exploration of the latent structure of hoarding.
Hotopf, M., et al. (2013). Epidemiology of hoarding disorder. British Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25(1), 194–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029966.
Psychiatry, 203(6), 445–452. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.130195. Timpano, K. R., Çek, D., Fu, Z. F., Tang, T., Wang, J. P., & Chasson, G. S. (2015). A
Norris, J. I., Lambert, N. M., Nathan DeWall, C., & Fincham, F. D. (2012). Can't buy me consideration of hoarding disorder symptoms in China. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 57,
love?: Anxious attachment and materialistic values. Personality and Individual 36–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.11.006.
Differences, 53(5), 666–669. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.05.009. Timpano, K. R., Exner, C., Glaesmer, H., Rief, W., Keshaviah, A., Brähler, E., et al.
Park, L. E., Crocker, J., & Mickelson, K. D. (2004). Attachment styles and contingencies of (2011a). The epidemiology of the proposed DSM-5 hoarding disorder: Exploration of
self-worth. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. https://doi.org/10.1177/ the acquisition specifier, associated features, and distress. Journal of Clinical
0146167204264000. Psychiatry, 72(6), 780–786. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.10m06380.
Pertusa, A., Fullana, M. A., Singh, S., Alonso, P., Menchón, J. M., & Mataix-Cols, D. Timpano, K. R., Keough, M. E., Traeger, L., & Schmidt, N. B. (2011b). General life stress
(2008). Compulsive hoarding: OCD symptom, distinct clinical syndrome, or both? and hoarding: Examining the role of emotional tolerance. International Journal of
American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(10), 1289–1298. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi. Cognitive Therapy, 4(3), 263–279. https://doi.org/10.1521/ijct.2011.4.3.263.
ajp.2008.07111730. Timpano, K. R., Rasmussen, J., Exner, C., Rief, W., Schmidt, N. B., & Wilhelm, S. (2013b).
Phung, P. J., Moulding, R., Taylor, J. K., & Nedeljkovic, M. (2015). Emotional regulation, Hoarding and the multi-faceted construct of impulsivity: A cross-cultural investiga-
attachment to possessions and hoarding symptoms. Scandinavian Journal of tion. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(3), 363–370. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Psychology, 56(5), 573–581. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12239. jpsychires.2012.10.017.
Preston, S. D., Muroff, J. R., & Wengrovitz, S. M. (2009). Investigating the mechanisms of Timpano, K. R., & Shaw, A. M. (2013). Conferring humanness: The role of anthro-
hoarding from an experimental perspective. Depression and Anxiety, 26(5), 425–437. pomorphism in hoarding. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(3), 383–388.
https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.10.007.
Raines, A. M., Boffa, J. W., Allan, N. P., Short, N. A., & Schmidt, N. B. (2014). Hoarding Timpano, K. R., Shaw, A. M., Cougle, J. R., & Fitch, K. E. (2014). A multifaceted as-
and eating pathology: The mediating role of emotion regulation. Comprehensive sessment of emotional tolerance and intensity in hoarding. Behavior Therapy, 45(5),
Psychiatry. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.11.005. 690–699. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beth.2014.04.002.
Raines, A. M., Short, N. A., Fuller, K. L., Allan, N. P., Oglesby, M. E., & Schmidt, N. B. Tolin, D. F., Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Fitch, K. E. (2008a). Family burden of compulsive
(2016). Hoarding and depression: The mediating role of perceived burdensomeness. hoarding: Results of an internet survey. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(3),
Journal of Psychiatric Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2016.08.003. 334–344. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.12.008.
Ravitz, P., Maunder, R., Hunter, J., Sthankiya, B., & Lancee, W. (2010). Adult attachment Tolin, D. F., Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., Gray, K. D., & Fitch, K. E. (2008b). The economic
measures: A 25-year review. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. https://doi.org/10. and social burden of compulsive hoarding. Psychiatry Research, 160(2), 200–211.
1016/j.jpsychores.2009.08.006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2007.08.008.
Rieger, E., Van Buren, D. J., Bishop, M., Tanofsky-Kraff, M., Welch, R., & Wilfley, D. E. Tolin, D. F., Frost, R. O., Steketee, G., & Muroff, J. (2015). Cognitive behavioral therapy
(2010). An eating disorder-specific model of interpersonal psychotherapy (IPT-ED): for hoarding disorder: A meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety, 32(3), https://doi.
Causal pathways and treatment implications. Clinical Psychology Review. https://doi. org/10.1002/da.22327.
org/10.1016/j.cpr.2010.02.001. Tolin, D. F., Kiehl, K. A., Worhunsky, P., Book, G. A., & Maltby, N. (2009). An exploratory
Robins, C. J., & Chapman, A. L. (2004). Dialectical behavior therapy: Current status, study of the neural mechanisms of decision making in compulsive hoarding.
recent developments, and future directions. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(1), Psychological Medicine. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291708003371.
73–89. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.18.1.73.32771. Tolin, D. F., Meunier, S. A., Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (2010). Course of compulsive
Roster, C. A. (2015). “Help, I have too much stuff!”: Extreme possession attachment and hoarding and its relationship to life events. Depression and Anxiety, 27(9), 829–838.
professional organizers. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 49(2), 303–327. https://doi.org/ https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20684.
10.1111/joca.12052. Wincze, J. P., Steketee, G., & Frost, R. O. (2007). Categorization in compulsive hoarding.
San Francisco Task Force on Compulsive and Hoarding (2009). Beyond overwhelmed: The Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45(1), 63–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2006.
impact of compulsive hoarding and cluttering in san Francisco and recommendations to 01.012.
reduce negative impacts and improve care. San Francisco, CA: San Francisco Task Force Woody, S. R., Kellman-McFarlane, K., & Welsted, A. (2014). Review of cognitive per-
on Compulsive Hoarding. formance in hoarding disorder. Clinical Psychology Review. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.
Scharfe, E. (2016). Measuring what counts: Development of a new four‐category measure cpr.2014.04.002.
of adult attachment. Personal Relationships, 23(1), 4–22.

18

You might also like