You are on page 1of 13

LSHSS

Research Article

Examination of Narrative Language


at Microstructural and Macrostructural
Levels in Spanish-Speaking Preschoolers
Lorena Orizaba,a Brenda K. Gorman,a Christine E. Fiestas,b
Gary E. Bingham,c and Nicole Patton Terryd

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to examine Protocol–Spanish and the Narrative Scoring Scheme,
narrative language samples of Spanish-speaking respectively. Participants also completed an assessment
preschoolers to analyze changes in microstructural and of expressive vocabulary in Spanish.
macrostructural skills in their first language (L1) from fall to Results: The results indicated different degrees of change
spring, relationships between narrative and vocabulary in microstructural and macrostructural elements, change
skills in L1, and the extent to which fall skills predict from fall to spring in some but not all elements measured,
spring performance. shifting patterns of association between L1 vocabulary and
Method: Participants included 40 Spanish-speaking narrative skills, and variable prediction of spring scores.
children who were enrolled in community-based preschool Conclusion: Results from this study enhance professionals’
programs. Narrative language retells from the fall and spring understanding of Spanish-speaking preschoolers’ narrative
were examined, and microstructural and macrostructural language skills in L1 and considerations for assessing and
components were analyzed using the Narrative Assessment monitoring progress at different points in the academic year.

A
s evidenced by their emphasis in current academic 1996). Given the relationship between language develop-
standards, strong narrative language skills are ment and academic achievement, assessment and monitoring
critical for successful academic achievement. Easily of young children’s skills are critical elements of effective
visible in academic standards is the expectation that students educational programming and are necessary to individual-
develop progressively robust oral and written narrative ize instruction and support prevention of later difficulties
skills as they advance through academic grade levels. Less (Castro et al., 2011; Justice et al., 2010). Therefore, research
visible in the standards, but apparent from existing research, that enhances professionals’ understanding of valid assess-
is the contribution of language and narrative language skills ment practices and interpretation of young children’s lan-
for reading comprehension (Boulineau et al., 2004; Fazio guage performance is of critical importance, particularly
et al., 1996; Griffin et al., 2004; Justice et al., 2006; Lervåg for children who are learning more than one language.
et al., 2018; Mehta et al., 2005; Montgomery & Kahn, 2003; Narrative language tasks provide a valid means for
Nathanson et al., 2007; Pankratz et al., 2007; Roth et al., children to demonstrate a wide range of linguistic skills
(Justice et al., 2010; Paris & Paris, 2003). Specifically, nar-
rative discourse challenges children to use numerous literate
a
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, language features at sentence and word levels (i.e., micro-
Elmhurst College, IL structure) and higher discourse levels of organization (i.e.,
b
Department of Clinical Health Sciences, Texas A&M macrostructure). Microstructural elements generally in-
University-Kingsville
c clude language forms such as syntactic and morphological
Department of Early Childhood and Elementary Education,
Georgia State University, Atlanta structures, vocabulary types and tokens, and cohesive de-
d
Florida Center for Reading Research, Florida State University, vices. Macrostructure is linked to mental schemas (Stein &
Tallahassee Glenn, 1979) for the organization of the story and is formed
Correspondence to Brenda K. Gorman: brenda.gorman@elmhurst.edu according to the narrator’s exposure to story models. Macro-
Editor-in-Chief: Holly L. Storkel structure is most often measured by story grammar com-
Editor: Kerry Danahy Ebert ponents including, for example, plot elements (problem,
Received November 6, 2019 attempt, and consequence) and setting. In light of research
Revision received November 11, 2019
Accepted November 15, 2019 Disclosure: The authors have declared that no competing interests existed at the time
https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_LSHSS-19-00103 of publication.

428 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020 • Copyright © 2020 American Speech-Language-Hearing Association

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
showing that reading comprehension skills of children who first grade. This predictive nature of L1–L2 language de-
are dual language learners (DLLs) may lag behind their velopment indicates the necessity of monitoring DLLs’ L1
counterparts who are first language (L1) learners (Melby- development.
Lervåg & Lervåg, 2014), early assessment and monitoring
of DLLs’ narrative language including both microstructural Interrelationships Between Narrative and Vocabulary Skills
and macrostructural elements merit particular attention in The association between vocabulary and narrative
early childhood educational programming. language skills appears to be especially important. Narra-
tors must utilize specific vocabulary, including mental state
verbs and descriptive adjectives, to produce macrostruc-
Considerations for DLLs
tural elements such as setting and character descriptions in
Extant research addressing language assessment of their stories. Moreover, there is also evidence of a develop-
young DLLs has largely focused on assessment of children’s mental emergence shared between vocabulary and syntax
proficiency in English rather than in their home language development (Bates & Goodman, 1999), which is indicative
(Hammer et al., 2014), despite theoretical and empirical of the overlap between vocabulary and narrative micro-
evidence suggesting the importance of children’s home lan- structure. Concerning this relationship between vocabulary
guage to their second language (L2) acquisition. In accor- and syntax development, research with young monolingual
dance with Cummins’ Interdependence Hypothesis (1979) Spanish-speaking children suggests that vocabulary growth
that L2 competence is dependent on L1 competence, scholars is associated with growth in mean length of utterance and
have documented that L1 skills as measured in elementary subordination in Spanish (Pérez-Leroux et al., 2012). As
school–age children (e.g., Lindsey et al., 2003; Miller et al., form and function are interrelated, so are narrative macro-
2006) and preschool-age children (Castilla-Earls et al., 2009) and microstructure. Consequently, strategic analysis of
predict literacy and academic achievement in L2. Children children’s narrative language samples can provide a rich
with weaknesses in their L1 are at a higher risk for aca- source of language data as they are in the process of devel-
demic delays as compared to peers who develop competence oping their language skills.
in the home language (Kohnert et al., 2005). Furthermore,
children who are slower to acquire L1 due to having devel-
opmental language disorder will also be impacted in their Narrative Language of Preschool-Age DLLs
learning of the L2 (Castilla-Earls et al., 2009). Together, Narration is a functional task that is both academi-
findings underscore the importance of assessing and moni- cally and culturally relevant for preschool-aged children
toring children’s language skills in L1 and using this infor- from various language backgrounds (Gorman et al., 2016;
mation to individualize instruction to meet children’s needs. Kim et al., 2018; Mehrani, 2018). Preschool-aged children
Recognizing the array of linguistic skills demonstrated are familiar with the idea of stories and storytelling; thus,
through narrative language tasks and the aforementioned narrative language sampling for assessing children’s language
relationship between narrative and reading comprehension skills may introduce less test bias than other assessment
skills, a few key studies have provided insight into narrative formats (Peña et al., 2014). Through a single narrative, chil-
language development of elementary school–age Spanish- dren demonstrate a wide variety of linguistic skills at word,
speaking DLLs. Examining relationships between these sentence, and discourse levels that examiners can strategi-
skills in DLLs, Miller et al. (2006) conducted a large-scale cally analyze. Despite the documented importance of early
study of more than 1,500 Spanish–English bilinguals from narrative skills in L1 and their predictive validity in L2,
kindergarten through third grade. Their results revealed that the literature on assessment and progress monitoring tools
children’s performance on Spanish oral language measures that professionals may use with Spanish-speaking children
elicited in a narrative task predicted reading scores in both is limited.
Spanish and English. Longitudinal studies of Spanish– Two examples of tools that may be used to facilitate
English DLLs have shown that children’s macrostructure analysis of children’s narrative production in Spanish in-
abilities in one language may also predict development of clude the Narrative Assessment Protocol–Spanish (NAP-S;
these skills in the other language (Pearson, 2002; Squires Gorman et al., 2016) and the Narrative Scoring Scheme
et al., 2014; Uccelli & Páez, 2007). Uccelli and Páez (2007) (NSS; Heilmann et al., 2010; Miller & Heilmann, 2004). The
examined school-age DLLs’ narrative language skills; spe- NAP-S is a Spanish version adaptation of Justice et al.’s
cifically, they assessed students’ skills in kindergarten and (2010) NAP. This tool focuses on the microstructural com-
again in first grade. Analyzing narrative productivity (i.e., ponents of narration and was designed specifically for pre-
microstructure) and narrative quality (i.e., macrostructure), school-age children. There is some evidence supporting
they found improvement in both productivity and story the utility of the NAP-S as a tool for assessing the narrative
quality in English, whereas only story quality improved in language skills of Spanish-speaking preschoolers (Gorman
Spanish. Importantly, they found that children’s Spanish et al., 2016). For assessing the macrostructural components
story quality in kindergarten was predictive of their English of narrative language, the NSS was developed with English-
story quality in first grade. Echoing these results, Squires speaking, elementary school–age children in mind. The
et al. (2014) found that kindergarteners’ narrative macro- NSS has been utilized to analyze narrative skills with school-
structure scores in Spanish predicted their English scores in age bilingual children (Lucero, 2018; Miller et al., 2006)

Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool 429


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
and in bilinguals who were fluent in English (Heilmann between microstructural elements of vocabulary and gram-
et al., 2010). However, research on utilization of the NSS mar with their narrative microstructural skills, with vocabu-
with Spanish-speaking preschool-age children is needed. This lary emerging as a major unique predictor of NSS scores.
study extends investigation of the NSS to this population. As mentioned previously, further study of Spanish-speaking
More specifically, the NAP was developed to assess preschoolers’ performance on the NSS is needed. There-
preschool-age children’s expressive language skills in the fore, additional research examining whether or not the NSS
naturalistic and ecologically valid context of narration. The tool is sensitive to their narrative language development
tool measures aspects of English syntactic, morphological, and would benefit assessment practices in early childhood
and vocabulary development in the context of narrating education settings would be beneficial.
a wordless picture book. The Spanish adaptation was devel-
oped to address the inadequacy of appropriate tools for Purpose
measuring and monitoring the progress of Spanish-speaking
children in their native language. Due to the greater mor- The compelling relationships between children’s nar-
phological complexity of Spanish as compared to English, rative language skills and reading comprehension along with
additional grammatical features were added, including article– the increasing number of Spanish-speaking students across
noun agreement (number and gender), noun–modifier the nation requires that professionals understand DLLs’
agreement (number and gender), clitic–noun agreement narrative language development and utilize informative pro-
(number and gender), irregular perfect, regular perfect, sub- tocols to assess and monitor their skills. Various studies
junctive, reflexive verb including clitic pronouns, (e.g., me, have contributed to our understanding of narrative develop-
te, se), transitive verb phrases including clitic pronouns, and ment among older bilingual children (Heilmann et al., 2010;
ditransitive verb phrases including clitics. In their study, Miller et al., 2006; Uccelli & Páez, 2007), but research-
Gorman et al. used the tool to analyze the narrative retells based protocols for assessment and monitoring of Spanish-
of children who were functionally monolingual in Spanish speaking preschoolers’ narrative language are limited. Due
and enrolled in preschool classrooms. Examination of the to the importance of children’s language development in
tool’s construct validity through exploratory factor analysis L1, further study is required to establish recommendations
of all 27 items revealed that 10 items loaded most strongly for examining young DLLs’ narrative microstructural and
on the single narrative factor, resulting in the NAP-S long macrostructural skills and to support interpretation of
form with all 27 items and a NAP-S short form with 10 items performance. In response to these areas of need, this study
(see Table 1). Whereas the long form provides a more com- provides an analysis of preschool-age Spanish speakers’
prehensive and detailed profile of performance, the short narrative language skills in L1 at the beginning and end of
form was also developed as a more feasible tool for use in the school year. Specifically, the investigators posed the
school settings. The investigators found that children’s scores following questions: (a) How much change in narrative
on both long and short forms increased significantly from language microstructural and macrostructural skills from
the fall to the spring, supporting its utility for Spanish- fall to spring do Spanish-speaking preschoolers demonstrate
speaking preschool-age children. Still needed, however, is in L1 (Spanish)? (b) Which specific elements of microstruc-
research that provides further insight into how children’s ture and macrostructure change? (c) How are children’s
microstructural skills in L1 change and how professionals microstructural, macrostructural, and vocabulary skills
may incorporate microstructural analysis in their early child- associated? (d) Do L1 narrative language and vocabulary
hood assessment practices. skills in the fall predict L1 narrative language skills in the
The NSS distinctively evaluates narrative macro- spring? The investigators sought to interpret these findings
structure, including global characteristics of a story and the to provide recommendations for early childhood assessment
cohesion of these elements (see Table 1). The scoring criteria practices with Spanish-speaking preschool-age children.
for the NSS is partly based on the three major categories
of cohesion necessary to tell an effective story: (a) referential Method
cohesion, which allows the narrator to maintain appropriate
reference to the characters, objects, and locations across Participants
utterances using noun phrases and pronouns; (b) conjunctive Narrative transcripts were drawn from a database of
cohesion, whereby the narrator maintains concepts across languages samples of preschool children who participated
phrases and utterances by using conjunctive words and in an earlier study investigating the development and valid-
phrases (e.g., and, but, besides, on the other hand, finally, in ity of the NAP-S (Gorman et al., 2016). Participants were
addition); and (c) lexical cohesion, achieved by the narrator’s enrolled in community-based preschool classrooms that
use of vocabulary to effectively link concepts across utter- were involved in larger language and literacy projects and
ances (Hielmann et al., 2010). The specific categories of were using preschool curricula that included narrative ac-
analysis include the story introduction, character develop- tivities in two large cities; one was located in the midwest
ment, mental states of story characters, referencing to clarify and the other in the southeast region of the United States.
pronouns, story conflicts and their resolutions, story cohe- The primary language of classroom instruction was English,
sion, and the clarity of the conclusion. Further examina- with Spanish support provided by teaching assistants or
tion by Heilmann et al. (2010) also revealed relationships aides. As described in Gorman et al. (2016), parent and

430 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Table 1. Assessment components of the Narrative Assessment Protocol-Spanish (NAP-S) long and short
forms and the Narrative Scoring Scheme (NSS).

NAP-S long form NAP-S short form NSS

Sentence structure Sentence structure Introduction


Compound sentence Complex sentences Setting
Complex sentences Modifiers Characters
Negative sentences Adverb Character development
Interrogative sentence Nouns Mental states
Phrase structure Noun–modifier agreement Referencing
Elaborated noun phrases Clitic–noun agreement Conflict resolution
Compound noun Verbs Cohesion
Prepositional phrase Auxiliary–main verb Conclusion
Modifiers Copula “be” verb +
Adverb Irregular past tense
Advanced modifier Regular imperfect
Nouns Tier 2 verbs
Pluralized nouns Transitive verb phrases
Posessive forms
Tier 2 nouns
Article–noun agreement
Noun–modifier agreement
Clitic–noun agreement
Verbs
Aux–main
Copula “be” verb +
Irregular past tense
Regular past tense
Irregular imperfect
Regular imperfect
Subjunctive
Tier 2 verbs
Compound verbs
Reflexive verbs
Transitive verb phrases
Ditransitive verb phrases

teacher questionnaires (Gutiérrez-Clellen & Kreiter, 2003; SD = 5.56; 22 boys, 18 girls) that were analyzed in the cur-
Restrepo, 1998) were used to determine language status. rent study.
Participants were selected for the study based on their lan-
guage status as predominantly Spanish speaking based on
all of the following criteria: (a) teacher report that Spanish Procedure
was the child’s dominant language, (b) parent report that The investigation focused on analyses of data obtained
Spanish was the child’s dominant language, (c) parent re- from narrative scoring of language transcripts using the
port that Spanish was the child’s L1, (d) parent report that NAP-S and NSS. Narrative samples were collected in the
the child was hearing only or primarily (≥ 80%) Spanish in fall and spring based on the Mercer Mayer wordless story-
the home, (e) parent report that the child’s systematic expo- book Frog Where Are You? (Mayer, 1969), which is often
sure to English began in preschool, and (f) inability to pro- used in research examining Spanish-speaking children’s nar-
duce a narrative in English during fall testing. Participants rative language development (Bedore et al., 2010; Gutiérrez-
with a history of sensory impairment, neurological condi- Clellen et al., 2012; Petersen et al., 2017; Spencer et al.,
tions, or parent/teacher-reported cognitive concerns were 2019; Uccelli & Páez, 2007). Data collection, transcription,
excluded from the study. The majority (75%) of children and NAP-S scoring procedures are detailed in Gorman et al.
were of Mexican heritage, with the remaining participants (2016). Digital audio recordings of the narratives were sent
of Puerto Rican or multiethnic heritage. According to the to and transcribed by highly trained bilingual (English–
directors of the preschool programs and/or parent question- Spanish) staff at the University of Wisconsin–Madison’s
naires, all children met Head Start criteria for low-income Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts transcription
status. The current study was reviewed and approved by the services laboratory. Once returned from the Systematic
university institutional review board. From the data set of Analysis of Language Transcripts lab, NAP-S scoring was
60 children in the Gorman et al. study, a subsample was completed by two trained bilingual coders. Following Justice
drawn from participants with complete records and task et al.’s (2010) coding procedures, the transcribed narratives
completion at both time points, fall and spring. This resulted were assigned a frequency score of 0, 1, 2, or 3+ occur-
in transcripts from 40 Latino children (M = 52.90 months, rences for their inclusion of the coded NAP-S features

Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool 431


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
(see Table 1). Ten narratives (25%) were randomly selected Regardless of the reason, the investigators determined
for calculation of interrater reliability. Following Justice that further refinement of scoring criteria and examples for
et al. (2010), a “within-one” procedure (La Paro et al., 2004) training were necessary before continuing with NSS scor-
was utilized, such that coders were considered to be in agree- ing. While maintaining expectations of proficiency levels
ment if they were within one point of each other. Percent- for awarding scores of Proficient (5), Emerging (3), and
ages of agreement were 100% for compound sentences, Minimal/Immature (1), the coding team worked together
94% for complex sentences, 100% for negative sentences, to add clarifying information to the scoring rubric. Accord-
100% for interrogative sentences, 94% for elaborated noun ing to the NSS manual, for example, a Conflict Resolution
phrases, 98% for compound nouns, 94% for prepositional score of 1 would be awarded for a narrative with “random
phrases, 90% for adverbs, 97% for advanced adverbs, 100% resolution(s) stated with no mention of cause or conflict”
for pluralized nouns, 100% for possessive forms, 86% for or “conflict mentioned without resolution,” or “many con-
Tier 2 nouns, 99% for article–noun agreement, 100% for flicts and resolutions critical to advancing the plot are not
noun–modifier agreement, 97% for clitic–noun agreement, present.” A Conflict Resolution score of 3 would be awarded
100% for auxiliary–main verb, 100% for copula “be,” 98% for a narrative with “underdeveloped description of con-
for irregular past tense, 100% for regular past tense, 100% flicts and resolutions critical to advancing the plot of the
for irregular imperfect, 100% for regular imperfect, 93% for story” or if “not all conflicts and resolutions critical to ad-
subjunctive, 93% for Tier 2 verbs, 100% for compound vancing the plot are present.” A score of 5 would be awarded
verbs, 100% for reflexive verbs, 95% for transitive verb for a story in which the narrator “clearly states all conflicts
phrases, and 92% for ditransitive verb phrases. and resolutions critical to advancing the plot of the story.”
To conduct a macrostructural analysis of narratives, The coders decided they could enhance their scoring reli-
two coders followed the NSS manual for training. The NSS ability by clarifying how many and which conflicts and res-
manual provides examples of scoring criteria, examples to olutions were considered critical and agreed that at least
illustrate scoring, and rationales. Per the NSS instructions, four stated conflicts and resolutions were appropriate to
coders independently scored the following seven narrative receive a score of 3 in this category. In the Mental States cat-
characteristics: (a) introduction (i.e., the quality of the nar- egory, a score of 1 would be awarded for “no use of mental
rator’s establishment and description of the setting and main state words to develop characters,” a score of 3 for “some
characters), (b) character development (i.e., differentiation use of evidence mental state words to develop characters,”
of main and supporting characters and their significance and 5 if “mental states of main and supporting characters
throughout the narrative), (c) mental states (i.e., reference are expressed when necessary for plot development and ad-
to and description of the character’s thoughts and feelings), vancement” and “a variety of mental state words are used.”
(d) referencing (i.e., use of names, pronouns, and other To clarify “some” and “variety,” a narrative category re-
antecedents and words for clarification), (e) conflict/resolution ceived a score of 3 if the student exhibited “some use of ev-
(i.e., establishment and description of conflicts and their ident mental state words to develop character;” however, the
resolutions throughout the story), (f ) cohesion (i.e., the criteria did not specify a concrete number of mental state
presence and quality of sequencing and transitions), and words that would be adequate to receive a score of 3. After
(g) conclusion (i.e., the clarity and quality of statements to modifications were made, a score of 3 required the use of
wrap up the narrative). two unique emotions or cognitive states (sad, mad, angry,
The NSS rubric provides scoring criteria for scoring etc.). Similar modifications were made to the remaining
each of the narrative characteristics as Proficient (5), Emerg- categories of the NSS. Another example category of the
ing (3), and Minimal/Immature (1). The rubric notes indi- NSS that underwent scoring modifications was the Cohe-
cate that scores in between (e.g., 2, 4) are undefined; raters sion category. Criteria were added to define the possible
are allowed to use their judgment to assign these scores. Af- in-between scores of 2 and 4. For example, a narrative
ter scoring 10 narratives, the coders compared their results. would receive a score of 2 if the student used one transition
The raters again utilized the within-one procedure (La Paro word (e.g., después, luego, todavía, porque, or entonces),
et al., 2004) and considered the scoring to be in agreement a score of 3 if the student used more than one transition
if they were within one point. Despite having studied the word, and a score of 4 if the student used more than two
NSS training manual and practice examples, they did not transition words in addition to the narrative being more or-
achieve 80% interrater reliability. They studied the NSS ganized than a typical emerging narrative. Similar modifi-
training manual and practice items again, and indepen- cations were made to the remaining NSS categories. The
dently rescored the 10 narratives. Again, they were unable purpose of the additional criteria was not to change the
to achieve 80% interrater reliability. The NSS was devel- NSS scoring but to make it more specific in order to in-
oped and tested primarily for school-age, English-speaking crease scoring reliability. The adaptation of the NSS scoring
children; perhaps interpretation of the scoring rubric was chart is available in the Appendix.
more difficult because participants in this study were After this process, the coders independently scored
preschool-age children who produced narratives were in the 10 narratives again and found that they had indeed
Spanish, whereas previous development and focus on the achieved reliability of over 80%. Increasing objectivity within
NSS has been for school-age children who produced nar- the NSS scoring categories resulted in improved scoring
ratives in English. consistency and accuracy, which allowed narratives to

432 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
receive scores that best represented the students’ narrative fall long and short scores, and vocabulary predicted spring
skill level. Scoring of transcripts then continued using the narrative language scores. Means and standard deviations
more detailed scoring rubric. Scores ranging from 0 to are presented in Table 2.
5 were assigned for each of the seven narrative characteris-
tics: introduction, character development, mental states,
referencing, conflict/resolution, cohesion, and conclusion.
The highest possible total score was 35. Results
Interrater reliability for NSS scoring using the up- Magnitude of Change in L1 Microstructural
dated scoring was calculated using 20% of narrative sam- and Microstructural Skills
ples selected randomly from the total number of samples.
To address the first research question, we analyzed
Results from 16 narratives (eight from the fall, eight from
children’s narrative microstructural and macrostructural
the spring) indicated the following percentage agreement:
skills in Spanish, as measured by the NAP-S and modified
100% for the introduction, 100% for character development,
scoring of the NSS, respectively, in the fall (Time 1) and
87.5% for mental states, 93.75% for referencing, 93.75% for
again in the spring (Time 2). Analysis of the NSS indicated
conflict resolution, 100% for cohesion, and 87.5% for the
performance that was significantly higher in the spring,
conclusion.
t(39) = 4.07, p < .001, with an effect size of d = 0.54. Gorman
et al. (2016) reported that performance on the NAP-S long
Vocabulary form and the NAP-S short form was significantly higher
Vocabulary is a valuable contributor to the quality of in the spring; further analysis for the current study indicate
narrative production (Justice et al., 2006). Consequently, the that these effect sizes were d = 0.47 and d = 0.42, respec-
consideration of children’s vocabulary skills in Spanish was tively, which are slightly lower than for the NSS. These
relevant to the current study. Participants completed the results indicate that participants demonstrated a medium
Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test (EOWPVT): degree of change in narrative language microstructural ele-
Spanish–Bilingual Edition (Brownell, 2001) in both the fall ments and a large degree of change in macrostructural
and spring. This measure is a standardized, norm-referenced elements in Spanish from the beginning to the end of the
expressive vocabulary assessment that was normed with school year.
1,150 Spanish-speaking and Spanish–English bilingual chil-
dren between the ages of 4;0 and 12;11 (years;months) living
in the United States. Though not all participants were aged Granular Changes
4;0, this test was selected due to the limited availability of To address the second research question, pairwise
other tests, at the time of the study, intended for Spanish- comparisons using a Bonferroni adjustment to control for
speaking preschoolers living in the United States. The test Type I error within each analysis revealed significant in-
format is appropriate for preschool-age children; for each creases from fall to spring ( p < .01) on three of the five
test item, the examiner shows a colorful picture and asks categories of the NAP-S long form: sentence structure,
the child to name the item or group of items. According to modifiers, and verbs. Probing granular changes from fall
the test manual, items were required to meet specific cri- to spring, results suggest differences in production of com-
teria to support linguistic and cultural appropriateness for pound sentences ( p = .007), complex sentences ( p = .007),
the targeted population. The EOWPVT: Spanish–Bilingual adverbs ( p = .04), advanced modifiers ( p = .03), regular
Edition manual indicated a Cronbach’s coefficient alpha past tense ( p = .04), and transitive verb phrases ( p = .004),
of .95, split-half reliability of .96, and test–retest reliability though none of these met criteria for significance with ap-
of .91. The test was normed based on children’s response plication of a Bonferroni adjustment ( p < .003). Pairwise
in either language; however, to examine associations between comparisons using the Bonferroni adjustment indicated sig-
Spanish vocabulary skills, children were asked to name all nificant increases ( p < .007) on three of the seven NSS
items in Spanish. Given the modification of standardized categories: referencing, conflict resolution, and conclusion.
procedures, raw scores based on Spanish responses were
calculated and analyzed.
Table 2. Means and standard deviations.
Analyses
Fall Spring
To evaluate whether or not the NSS and NAP-S
Assessment tool M (SD) Range M (SD) Range
tools captured significant change over the academic year,
we compared fall (Time 1) and spring (Time 2) scores using NAP-S long form 34.53 (9.16) 11–56 38.75 (8.81) 24–62
paired t tests; pairwise comparisons were then run using a NAP-S short form 16.58 (5.32) 3–26 18.63 (4.46) 11–28
Bonferroni adjustment to control for Type I error within NSS 12.53 (2.87) 8–18 14.25 (3.44) 8–20
each analysis. Correlational analyses were conducted to de- Spanish vocabulary 24.76 (10.6) 12–47 26.75 (7.91) 16–45
termine which narrative assessment tool was more closely Note. NAP-S = Narrative Assessment Protocol–Spanish; NSS =
related to Spanish vocabulary. Linear regression analyses Narrative Scoring Scheme.
were performed to determine how well the NSS and NAP-S

Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool 433


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Associations Between Microstructural, fall vocabulary, r = .232, p = .34; spring vocabulary, r = .101,
Macrostructural, and Vocabulary Skills p = .73); thus, age was not entered as a covariate. Inde-
pendent samples t tests indicated no performance differ-
Correlation analyses between each tool and expressive ences by gender (NAP-S long fall, t(38) = 0.362, p = .72;
vocabulary scores indicated several significant associations. NAP-S long spring, t(38) = .0410, p = .684; NAP-S short fall,
In the fall, Spanish vocabulary scores were significantly t(38) = 0.080, p = .937; NAP-S short spring, t(38) = 0.726,
and highly related to microstructural skills as measured by p = .47; NSS fall, t(38) = 0.268, p = .79; NSS spring, t(38)
the NAP-S long form (r = .71, p = .001) and NAP-S short = 0.046, p = .96; fall vocabulary, t(38) = 1.454, p = .164;
form (r = .69, p = .001) scores, similar to findings from the spring vocabulary, t(38) = 0.329, p = .75); therefore, gen-
larger sample (Gorman et al., 2016). However, results did der was not entered as a control variable. Because efficiency
not reach statistical significance for macrostructural skills and feasibility are important to educators and clinicians as
as measured by the NSS (r = .43, p = .066). Conversely, in they select assessment tools, the investigators used scores
the spring, Spanish vocabulary was significantly and highly from the long and short forms of the NAP-S to analyze
related to macrostructure scores (r = .65, p = .013); how- prediction. Findings demonstrated that children’s NAP-S
ever, results did not reach significance for microstructure fall long scores predicted 43.9% of the variance in NAP-S
as measured by the NAP-S long form (r = .39, p = .17) or spring long scores, and their NAP-S fall short scores pre-
NAP-S short form scores (r = .43, p = .124), unlike results dicted 40.2% of the variance in the NAP-S spring short
from the larger sample. Examining relationships between scores. The NSS fall scores predicted 41.1% of the variance
fall vocabulary scores and spring narrative scores, Spanish in NSS spring scores. By comparison, Spanish expressive
vocabulary scores in the fall were significantly and highly vocabulary scores in the fall were less predictive of spring
related to all narrative scores in the spring: NAP-S long narrative scores, predicting 37% of the variance in NAP-S
form (r = .64, p = .003), NAP-S short form (r = .66, p = .002), long, 40% in NAP-S short, and 30% in NSS spring scores.
and NSS (r = .58, p = .009). Interestingly, further analysis indicated that the NAP-S fall
long scores predicted 50% of the variance in NSS spring
Predicting Spring Performance scores, and the NAP-S fall short scores predicted 43.3% of
the NSS spring scores, thereby showing somewhat higher
Results targeting the fourth research question indi-
predictive capacity than the NSS fall scores. Conversely, NSS
cated that children’s skills in both microstructure and mac-
fall scores did not inform NAP-S spring scores, predicting
rostructure in the fall as measured by the NAP-S and NSS
only 4.4% of the variance in NAP-S spring long and 8.5%
were predictive of their skills in the spring (see Table 3).
of the variance in NAP-S spring short scores.
Before conducting regression analyses, correlations between
these variables with age and gender were explored to deter-
mine if the latter should be included in the analyses. Re- Discussion
sults indicated no significant relationships between age in
The limited corpus of research regarding narrative
months and any of the measures (NAP-S long fall, r = .027,
language development and assessment of young DLLs pre-
p = .874; NAP-S long spring, r = .114, p = .51; NAP-S short
sents unique challenges for the early identification of and
fall, r = .224, p = .19; NAP-S short spring, r = .147, p = .39;
support for children who may experience language difficul-
NSS fall, r = .008, p = .965; NSS spring, r = .001, p = .993;
ties. To address this insufficiency in the research, this study
examined narrative language samples of Spanish-speaking
Table 3. Prediction of spring performance. preschoolers to analyze microstructural and macrostruc-
tural skills in their L1 with the goal of informing early
Outcome Adj R2 B SE B β t p childhood assessment practices for this population.
Our first goal was addressed by examining the mag-
NAP-S long form: Spring
NAP-S long form: Fall .439 0.648 0.115 .673 5.614 < .001 nitude of change over the course of the preschool year in
NAP-S short form: Fall .432 1.107 0.200 .668 5.539 < .001 children’s narrative language skills at microstructural and
NSS: Fall .044 0.802 0.480 .262 1.670 .103 macrostructural levels. As measured using both long and
Spanish vocabulary: Fall .370 0.559 0.165 .636 3.399 .003 short forms of the NAP-S, children demonstrated a signifi-
NAP-S short form: Spring
NAP-S long form: Fall .346 0.293 0.063 .603 4.655 < .001 cant, moderate degree of change in narrative language
NAP-S short form: Fall .402 0.541 0.104 .646 5.217 < .001 microstructural elements from the fall to the spring, with
NSS: Fall .085 0.512 0.238 .330 2.155 .038 similar effect sizes from the long and short versions. Recall
Spanish vocabulary: Fall .398 0.274 0.076 .657 3.592 .002 that the NAP-S long form is used to code 27 linguistic fea-
NSS: Spring
NAP-S long form: Fall .500 0.269 0.043 .716 6.321 < .001 tures and the NAP-S short form is used to code 10 features
NAP-S short form: Fall .433 0.432 0.078 .669 5.544 < .001 within five categories: sentence structure, phrase structure,
NSS: Fall .411 0.782 0.147 .653 5.316 < .001 modifiers, nouns, and verbs. Of these, post hoc analyses
Spanish vocabulary: Fall .298 0.162 0.055 .581 2.940 .009 indicated granular changes from fall to spring in the three
Note. NAP-S = Narrative Assessment Protocol–Spanish; NSS = categories of sentence structure, modifiers, and verbs, al-
Narrative Scoring Scheme. though changes in individual skills within these categories
did not meet criteria for significance with application of the

434 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Bonferroni adjustment. Thus, although the expectation for not previously presented in the original NSS manual. Once
increases in mean length of utterance and sentence complex- the evaluators achieved reliability and completed scoring,
ity in Spanish-speaking preschool-age children has been the NSS did indeed capture significant change in macro-
established (e.g., Gutiérrez et al., 2012; Pérez-Leroux et al., structure from fall to spring, with a marginally higher effect
2012; Uccelli & Páez, 2007), these findings add to the cor- size (d = 0.54) than the NAP-S long (d = 0.47) and NAP-S
pus of information about linguistic development in this short (d = 0.42) forms. Consequently, the NSS appears to
population. As measures using the NSS with scoring refine- provide valuable information about children’s narrative
ments, children demonstrated a significant, large degree of development in preschool and may be a feasible tool for
change in macrostructural elements from fall to spring. clinicians who are willing to transcribe the narrative for
The NSS is used to measure seven areas of macrostructure: coding purposes.
the introduction, character development, mental states, These current research findings indicating continued
referencing, conflict resolution, cohesion, and conclusion. growth in the participants’ L1 are also of interest, given
Of these, post hoc analyses indicated that the specific areas that some research has documented vulnerability of L1 when
in which children demonstrated significant developmental children learn an L2 in early childhood (Leseman, 2000;
change from fall to spring were in referencing, conflict res- Wong-Fillmore, 1991). In contrast, the continued Spanish
olution, and conclusion. These changes in macrostructure support along with English instruction in children’s class-
may reflect not only advances in children’s language skills rooms appears to have contributed to their gains in both
over the year but also growth in their social–emotional and L1 vocabulary and grammar over the course of the school
cognitive skills. From a developmental perspective, children year; this is in line with other research examining bilingual
between the ages of 2 and 6 years tend to display social preschool instruction (e.g., Barnett et al., 2007; Durán
egocentrism (Kegan, 1994). Because young children lack et al., 2013; Farver et al., 2009; Garcia, 2018; Restrepo
perspective-taking skills, it is understandable if their narra- et al., 2013). There are well-documented problems of both
tives lack sufficient macrostructural elements to meet the overidentification and underidentification of English lan-
listener’s needs and interests. Yet, children make significant guage learners in special education (Counts et al., 2018).
strides in social and cognitive development during their Overidentification results, in part, from professionals’ over-
preschool years (Sandy, 2014), which may be reflected in reliance on English measures to interpret bilingual children’s
changes observed across the year in the current study. Al- performance. Underidentification results, in part, from
though all areas in the NSS reflect important features of failure to consider children’s skills in L1 and the use of
macrostructure, the three areas of referencing, conflict reso- a wait-and-see approach, in which professionals wait until
lution, and conclusion showed the most sensitivity to change children have acquired more English to identify and inter-
in this sample, suggesting that they may be the most infor- vene with struggling learners. Consideration of children’s
mative macrostructural areas for monitoring and identifying skills in L1 in a contextually sensitive manner, such as
Spanish-speaking preschool children who are and are not through assessment and progress monitoring of narrative
making expected progress. language skills in Spanish, is one means for addressing these
The current study was motivated by interest in pro- problems.
viding recommendations for early childhood assessment
practices with Spanish-speaking preschoolers. Considering
ease of use and utility, the NAP-S short form is more effi- Associations Between Skills
cient than the long form and provides similar information. Finally, the investigators were interested in examin-
Because the evaluator scores each phrase and sentence for ing associations between children’s expressive vocabulary
microstructural features individually, it may be possible to and narrative language skills in L1. In the fall, Spanish vo-
score accurately the NAP-S from an audio recording with- cabulary was strongly related to scores on both long and
out the need for complete transcription; this format merits short forms of the NAP-S. This finding is consistent with
future investigation. In contrast, scoring of macrostructural earlier research documenting strong associations between
features such as referencing, conflict resolution, and cohe- lexical and grammatical development in early childhood
sion requires that the evaluator attends to and analyzes the for English and Spanish speakers (e.g., Bates & Goodman,
content of multiple sentences simultaneously. This atten- 1997; Dionne et al., 2003; Marchman & Bates, 1994; Pérez-
tional load for the examiner renders NSS scoring without Leroux et al., 2012). In contrast, children’s scores on the
full narrative transcription very difficult. Even with full Spanish vocabulary measure and NSS in the fall were not
transcription, evaluators in the current study experienced related. In the spring, the associations alternated, with a
difficulty scoring the NSS reliably due to their perception significant relationship between vocabulary scores and NSS
of insufficiently defined scoring criteria. Importantly, both performance but no significant relationship between vocab-
for researchers and clinicians who wish to the NSS tool for ulary and NAP-S scores. The established relationship be-
Spanish-speaking preschoolers, the initial difficulties with tween vocabulary and the NAP-S in the fall and between
achieving reliability were remedied through further devel- vocabulary and the NSS in the spring suggests that vocab-
opment of scoring criteria. Specifically, the researchers ulary knowledge has a direct impact on the quality of
added more specific criteria for scoring, including the pos- narratives. A more robust vocabulary inventory allows pre-
sible in-between scores of 2 and 4 for which criteria were schoolers to produce more cohesive narratives. The alternating

Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool 435


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
pattern, however, was unexpected. One might expect sub- the NAP-S spring short scores, and NSS fall scores predicted
stantial associations between vocabulary and both narrative 41.1% of the variance in NSS spring scores. Interestingly,
measures at both times. The transfer of learning within and fall scores on the NAP-S were also predictive of spring per-
across languages is not always straightforward for children formance on the NSS, but the reverse was not true; fall
developing an L2. Observed variation may have also been scores on the NSS were not predictive of spring performance
a function of the types of language that each measure prior- on the NAP-S. This pattern of results may be interpreted
itizes. The EOWPVT predominately features nouns, whereas as logical, since the ability to formulate organized and
the NAP-S also taps modifiers and verbs, and the NSS also cohesive narratives is supported by robust vocabulary and
taps vocabulary related to referencing and cohesion. It may morphosyntactic skills, which typically develop rapidly in
be that the EOWPVT was too broad of a measure of vo- early childhood. Though children also begin to develop
cabulary that was not consistently related to abilities captured decontextualized language skills during early years of life
by the NAPS or NSS at a given point in time but served (Uccelli et al., 2019), the focus on narrative language with
better as a predictor of children’s macro- and microstructural fewer contextual cues increases during preschool and ele-
abilities captured by these instruments. mentary school when classroom instruction emphasizes
Additional variation in children’s performance in the shared storybook reading experiences. Considering limita-
fall and spring and different associations among key vari- tions in professionals’ time, it may not be feasible to analyze
ables may relate to the role of input and language exposure all students’ narratives using both measures. Therefore,
in children’s home and early learning environments. Per- these results suggest that selection of the NAP-S for admin-
haps further examination of children’s home literacy expe- istration at the beginning of the preschool year may be ad-
rience before preschool entry and during preschool would vantageous, not only for the informative data it provides
provide further insight into the development of these skills. about children’s microstructural narrative language skills
Children from low socioeconomic status backgrounds, such at the time of administration but also for the predictive in-
as this participant sample, are less likely than their peers formation it may provide about both microstructural and
to have significant literacy enrichment experiences at home macrostructural skills at the end of the preschool year.
(Lewis et al., 2016). The decontextualized talk that children
are exposed to during early childhood includes their experi-
ences with narratives and has been shown to be predictive Limitations and Future Directions
of children’s academic language proficiency even in early Although the current study offers insight into the
adolescence (Uccelli et al., 2019). Children’s increasing En- narrative language development and assessment of Spanish-
glish exposure and classroom literacy experiences may have speaking preschoolers, further investigation with a larger
influenced the shift in associations among key language and more diversified sample would be beneficial. Partici-
outcomes in the spring of the school year. Although we pants in the current study were predominantly of Mexican
cannot sufficiently explain these results, they do help inform heritage and of low socioeconomic status and enrolled in a
our understanding of typical patterns of language and nar- dual-language Head Start program. Examination of a larger,
rative development in bilingual preschool children. more diverse group of participants that includes children
Given this relationship and limitations in time for from other Spanish-speaking Hispanic/Latino backgrounds
testing, these results may again be interpreted to emphasize is warranted to increase the validity of the assessment tools.
a potential benefit of prioritizing assessment of microstruc- As both coders who scored the NAP-S and NSS in this
tural skills in the fall over the use of a single vocabulary study were bilingual and trained to score the NAP-S and
measure. In this way, the evaluator collects performance NSS, further research is necessary to examine the feasibility
data that relates to vocabulary skill and that also provides of professionals’ use of these tools and the accuracy of
an important view into children’s decontextualized, narra- their scoring.
tive language skills. Additionally, given that skills from L1
are predictive of development and later achievement in L2,
such assessment in Spanish may be particularly useful in Clinical Implications
supporting the identification of and timely remediation for Narrative discourse provides an ecologically valid
children who are truly at risk for language and literacy context for assessment of preschool-age children’s language
difficulties. skills that support their preparation for the academic lan-
guage demands of school. Results from this study enhance
professionals’ understanding of Spanish-speaking pre-
Predicting Spring Performance schoolers’ narrative language development in L1 and offer
Results indicated predictive capabilities of all narra- suggestions for assessing and monitoring progress at differ-
tive measures. Therefore, these findings support the use of ent points in the school year. The NAP-S appears to sup-
these narrative measures at the beginning of preschool in port analysis of children’s language at the sentence and
order to help identify children who are at risk for academic word level (microstructure), whereas the NSS, facilitated
difficulties. Specifically, the NAP-S fall long scores pre- through the use of modified scoring criteria, provides use-
dicted 43.9% of the variance in NAP-S spring long scores, ful information about higher discourse-level organization
NAP-S fall short scores predicted 40.2% of the variance in (macrostructure). Childrens’ performance in the fall was

436 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
predictive of their skills in the spring, supporting the im- Fazio, B., Naremore, R., & Connell, P. (1996). Tracking children
portance of assessing children’s skills early in the year. The from poverty at risk for specific language impairments: A
coding of both micro- and macrostructural features of 3-year longitudinal study. Journal of Speech and Hearing
Research, 39(3), 611–624. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3903.611
language provides insights into foundational language skills
Garcia, E. B. (2018). The classroom language context and English
that are predictive of later language ability and reading and Spanish vocabulary development among dual language
achievement. learners attending Head Start. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
42, 148–157.
Gorman, B. K., Bingham, G. E., Fiestas, C. E., & Terry, N. P.
Acknowledgments (2016). Assessing the narrative abilities of Spanish-speaking
This research was supported by grants from the U.S. De- preschool children: A Spanish adaptation of the narrative as-
partment of Education (#S359B080083), awarded to the second, sessment protocol. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 36,
fourth, and fifth authors, and the United Way Metro Atlanta. 307–317.
Griffin, T., Hemphill, L., Camp, L., & Wolf, D. (2004). Oral dis-
course in the preschool years and later literacy skills. First
References Language, 24(2), 123–147.
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Kreiter, J. (2003). Understanding child
Barnett, W. S., Yarosz, D., Thomas, J., Jung, K., & Blanco, D.
bilingual acquisition using parent and teacher reports. Applied
(2007). Two-way and monolingual immersion in preschool
Psycholinguistics, 24(2), 267–288.
education: An experimental comparison. Early Childhood
Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., Simon-Cereijido, G., & Sweet, M. (2012).
Research Quarterly, 22(3), 277–293.
Predictors of second language acquisition in Latino children
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar
with specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-
and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, and real-
Language Pathology, 21(1), 64–77. https://doi.org/10.1044/
time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12(5–6),
1058-0360(2011/10-0090)
507–584.
Hammer, C. S., Hoff, E., Uchikoshi, Y., Gillanders, C., Castro, D.,
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar
& Sandilos, L. E. (2014). The language and literacy develop-
from the lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of
ment of young dual language learners: A critical review. Early
language (pp. 29–80). Erlbaum.
Childhood Research Quarterly, 29(4), 715–733.
Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., & Ho, T.-H. (2010).
Language sample measures and language ability in Spanish- Heilmann, J., Miller, J. F., Nockerts, A., & Dunaway, C. (2010).
English bilingual kindergarteners. Journal of Communication Properties of the Narrative Scoring Scheme using narrative re-
Disorders, 43(6), 498–510. tells in young school-age children. American Journal of Speech-
Boulineau, T., Fore, C., Hagan-Burke, S., & Burke, M. D. (2004). Language Pathology, 19(2), 154–166. https://doi.org/10.1044/
Use of story-mapping to increase the story-grammar text com- 1058-0360(2009/08-0024)
prehension of elementary students with learning disabilities. Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., Kaderavek, J. N., Ukrainetz, T. A.,
Learning Disability Quarterly, 27(2), 105–121. Eisenberg, S. L., & Gillam, R. B. (2006). The Index of Narra-
Brownell, R. (2001). Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test: tive Microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age
Spanish-Bilingual Edition. Academic Therapy. children’s narrative performances. American Journal of Speech-
Castilla-Earls, A. P., Restrepo, M. A., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2009). Language Pathology, 15(2), 177–191. https://doi.org/10.1044/
Individual differences and language interdependence: A study 1058-0360(2006/017)
of sequential bilingual development in Spanish–English pre- Justice, L. M., Bowles, R. P., Pence, K., & Gosse, C. (2010). A
school children. International Journal of Bilingual Education scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a
and Bilingualism, 12(5), 565–580. narrative context: The NAP (Narrative Assessment Protocol).
Castro, D. C., Paéz, M., Dickinson, D. K., & Frede, E. (2011). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(2), 218–234.
Promoting language and literacy in young dual language learners: Kegan, R. (1994). In over our heads: The mental demands of mod-
Research, practice and policy. Child Development Perspectives, ern life. Harvard University Press.
5(1), 15–21. Kim, A. Y., Park, A., & Lust, B. (2018). Simultaneous vs. successive
Counts, J., Katsiyannis, A., & Whitford, D. K. (2018). Culturally bilingualism among preschool-aged children: A study of four-
and linguistically diverse learners in special education: English year-old Korean-English bilinguals in the USA. International
learners. NASSP Bulletin, 102(1), 5–21. Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 21(2), 164–178.
Cummins, J. (1979). Linguistic interdependence and the educational Kohnert, K., Yim, D., Nett, K., Kan, P. F., & Duran, L. (2005). In-
development of bilingual children. Review of Educational Re- tervention with linguistically diverse preschool children: A fo-
search, 49(2), 222–251. cus on developing home language(s). Language, Speech, and
Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic Hearing Services in Schools, 36(3), 251–263. https://doi.org/
evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammati- 10.1044/0161-1461(2005/025)
cal development. Child Development, 74(2), 394–412. La Paro, K. M., Pianta, R. C., & Stuhlman, M. (2004). The classroom
Durán, L. K., Roseth, C., Hoffman, P., & Robertshaw, M. B. assessment scoring system: Findings from the pre-kindergarten
(2013). Spanish-speaking preschoolers’ early literacy develop- year. The Elementary School Journal, 104(5), 409–426.
ment: A longitudinal experimental comparison of predomi- Lervåg, A., Hulme, C., & Lervåg, M. (2018). Unpicking the devel-
nantly English and transitional bilingual education. Bilingual opmental relationship between oral language skills and reading
Research Journal, 36(1), 6–34. comprehension: It’s simple, but complex. Child Development,
Farver, J. A., Lonigan, C. J., & Eppe, S. (2009). Effective early 89(5), 1821–1838.
literacy skill developmental for Spanish-speaking English Leseman, P. (2000). Bilingual vocabulary development of Turkish
language learners: An experimental study of two methods. preschoolers in the Netherlands. Journal of Multilingual and
Child Development, 80(3), 703–719. Multicultural Development, 21(2), 93–112.

Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool 437


Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Lewis, K., Hammer, C. S., Sandilos, L., Sawyer, B., & Mendez, L. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2208–2220.
(2016). Relations among the home language and literacy envi- https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0151
ronment and children’s language abilities: A study of Head Petersen, D. B., Chanthongthip, H., Ukrainetz, T. A., Spencer, T. D.,
Start dual language learners and their mothers. Early Educa- & Steeve, R. W. (2017). Dynamic assessment of narratives: Effi-
tion and Development, 27(4), 478–494. cient, accurate identification of language impairment in bilingual
Lindsey, K., Manis, F., & Bailey, C. (2003). Prediction of first- students. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research,
grade reading in Spanish-speaking English-language learners. 60(4), 983–998. https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0426
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(3), 482–494. Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earls, A. P., & Brunner, J. (2012).
Lucero, A. (2018). The development of bilingual narrative retelling General and specific effects of lexicon in grammar: Determiner
among Spanish–English dual language learners over two and object pronoun omissions in child Spanish. Journal of
years. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(3), Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55(2), 313–327. https://
607–621. doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0004)
Marchman, V., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and mor- Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-
phological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis. speaking children with language impairment. Journal of
Journal of Child Language, 21(2), 339–366. Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(6), 1409–1410.
Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? Dial Books for Young https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4106.1398
Readers. Restrepo, M. A., Morgan, G. P., & Thompson, M. S. (2013). The
Mehrani, M. B. (2018). An elicited imitation test for measuring efficacy of a vocabulary intervention for dual-language learners
preschooler’s language development. Psychological Reports, with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and
121(3), 767–786. Hearing Research, 56(2), 748–765. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-
Mehta, P., Foorman, B., Branum-Martin, L., & Taylor, P. (2005). 4388(2012/11-0173)
Literacy as a unidimensional multilevel construct: Validation, Roth, F. R., Speece, D. L., Cooper, D. H., & De La Paz, S. (1996).
sources of influence, and implications in a longitudinal study Unresolved mysteries: How to metalinguistic and narrative
in grades 1–4. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 85–116. skills connect with early reading? The Journal of Special Edu-
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Lervåg, A. (2014). Reading comprehension cation, 30(3), 257–277.
and its underlying components in second-language learners: A Sandy, S. V. (2014). The development of conflict resolution skills:
meta-analysis of studies comparing first- and second-language Preschool to adulthood. In P. T. Coleman, M. Deutsch, &
learners. Psychological Bulletin, 140(2), 409–433. E. C. Marcus (Eds.), The handbook of conflict resolution: Theory
Miller, J., & Heilmann, J. (2004). Bilingual language project up- and practice (pp. 430–463). Wiley.
date. Paper presented at the Department of Communicative Spencer, T. D., Petersen, D. B., Restrepo, M. A., Thompson, M.,
Disorders Colloquium, Madison, WI, United States. & Gutierrez Arvizu, M. N. (2019). The effect of Spanish and
Miller, J. F., Heilmann, J., Nockerts, A., Iglesias, A., Fabiano, L., & English narrative intervention on the language skills of young
Francis, D. J. (2006). Oral language and reading in bilingual dual language learners. Topics in Early Childhood Special Edu-
children. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 21(1), cation, 38(4), 204–219.
30–43. Squires, K. E., Lugo-Neris, M. J., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M.,
Montgomery, J. K., & Kahn, N. L. (2003). You are going to be an Bohman, T. M., & Gillam, R. B. (2014). Story retelling by
author: Adolescent narratives as intervention. Communication bilingual children with language impairments and typically
Disorders Quarterly, 24(3), 143–152. developing controls. International Journal of Language &
Nathanson, R., Crank, J. N., Saywitz, K. J., & Ruegg, E. (2007). Communication Disorders, 49(1), 60–74.
Enhancing the oral narratives of children with learning disabil- Stein, N., & Glenn, C. (1979). An analysis of story comprehension
ities. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 23(4), 315–331. in elementary school children. In R. Freedle (Ed.), New direc-
Pankratz, M. E., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Insalaco, D. M. (2007). tions in discourse processing: Advances in discourse processes
The diagnostic and predictive validity of The Renfrew Bus Story. (Vol. 2, pp. 53–120). Ablex.
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38(4), Uccelli, P., Demir-Lira, Ö. E., Rowe, M. L., Levine, S., &
390–399. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/040) Goldin-Meadow, S. (2019). Children’s early decontextualized
Paris, A. H., & Paris, S. G. (2003). Assessing narrative comprehen- talk predicts academic language proficiency in midadolescence.
sion in young children. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), Child Development, 90(5), 1650–1663.
36–76. Uccelli, P., & Páez, M. M. (2007). Narrative and vocabulary devel-
Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Narrative competence among monolingual opment of bilingual children from kindergarten to first grade:
and bilingual school children in Miami. In D. K. Oller & Developmental changes and associations among English and
R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children Spanish skills. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools,
(pp. 135–174). Multilingual Matters. 38(3), 225–236. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/024)
Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., & Bedore, L. M. (2014). Dynamic as- Wong-Fillmore, L. (1991). When learning a second language
sessment of narrative ability in English accurately identifies means losing the first. Early Childhood Research Quarterly,
language impairment in English language learners. Journal of 6(3), 323–346.

438 Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
Appendix ( p. 1 of 2)
Narrative Scoring Scheme Preschool Spanish Adaptation
Characteristic Proficient (5) (4) Emerging (3) (2) Minimal/immature (1)

Introduction (A) Setting: -Plus one detail. (A) Setting: -States general -Just one aspect, either -Launches into story
-States general place and setting but provides no setting or the two or with no attempt to
provides some detail about detail. three characters (boy/ provide the setting.
the setting (e.g., reference -Description or dog/frog). -Mentions only one
to the time of the setting, elements of setting -Character(s) are mentioned character.
daytime, bedtime, season). are given intermittently after Page 6.
-Setting elements are throughout story. -Character(s) are
stated at an appropriate -May provide description mentioned after Page 6.
place in story. of specific element of
(B) Characters: setting (e.g., the frog is
-Main characters are in the jar).
introduced with some -Dia/noche or habitación/
description or detail cuarto and the three
provided. characters; boy/dog/frog
-Plus more than 2 details. but with no details.
-Dia or bosque and the
3 characters but with
no details.
(B) Characters:
-Characters of the story
are mentioned with no
detail or description.
Character -Main character(s) and all -Two adjectives/ -Both main and active -One instance of -Inconsistent mention of
development supporting character(s) characteristics supporting characters narration. Narrative involved or active
are mentioned. and two instances are mentioned. makes sense to some characters.
-Throughout story it of narration. -Main characters are not degree. -Character(s) necessary
Orizaba et al.: Narrative Language Spanish Preschool

is clear that child can clearly distinguished from for advancing the plot
discriminate between supporting characters. are not present.
main and supporting -Minimal narration in first -No narration in first
characters (e.g., more person. person.
description of or emphasis -One specific adjective/ -Confusing, not clear,
upon main character[s]). characteristic or more than reader does not
-Child narrates in first one instance of narration understand the
person using character (e.g., “ranita” not sufficiently characters.
voice (e.g., “You get out an adjective).
of my tree,” said the owl.).
Mental states -Mental states of main and -Three unique emotions -Some use of evident mental -One unique emotion or -No use of mental state
supporting characters are and/or cognitive state words to develop cognitive state. words to develop
expressed when necessary states. character(s). characters.
for plot development and -Two unique emotions and/or
advancement. cognitive states (e.g., enojar,
-A variety of mental state pensar, querer, gustar).
words are used.
Referencing -Provides necessary -Referents are clear -Inconsistent use of referents/ -Referents are clear 25% -Excessive use of pronouns.
antecedents to pronouns. 75% of the time. antecedents. of the time. -No verbal clarifiers used.
-References are clear -Referents are clear 50% -Child is unaware that
throughout the story. of the time. the listener is confused.
-Significant confusion,
439

almost always unclear.


(table continues)

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions
440

Appendix ( p. 2 of 2)
. (Continued).
Narrative Scoring Scheme Preschool Spanish Adaptation
Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools • Vol. 51 • 428–440 • April 2020

Characteristic Proficient (5) (4) Emerging (3) (2) Minimal/immature (1)


Conflict -Clearly states all conflicts -At least five conflicts. -Underdeveloped description -Two or three conflicts -Mentions only 1 conflict.
resolution and resolutions that are of conflicts and resolutions (e.g., Gone-looked -Random
critical to advancing the critical to advancing the Head stuck-breaks resolution(s) stated with
plot of the story (for a plot of the story. Bites-leaves, yells no mention of cause or
detailed list of conflicts/ or Bees chase-run away conflict.
resolutions, see addendum). -Not all conflicts and resolutions Wants frog-gets one). or
critical to advancing the plot -Conflict mentioned without
are present. resolution.
-At least four conflicts. or
-Many conflicts
and resolutions critical to
advancing the plot are
not present.
Cohesion -Language helps the events -More organized than -Events follow a logical order. -Either porque, entonces, -No use of smooth devices.
follow a logical order. “emerging.” -Excessive detail or emphasis después, luego, y ahora,
-Critical events are -Transitions are provided on minor events or todaví a (consider siempre
included while less relatively clear. leading the listener astray. if facilitates cohesion).
emphasis is placed or
on minor events. - Transitions to the next event
-Smooth transitions unclear.
are provided between or
events. -Minimal details given for critical
events.
or
-Equal emphasis on all events.
-More than one unique word
and occurrence of cohesive
devices.
Conclusion -Story is clearly wrapped -One sentence about -Specific event is concluded, -Sentence about finding -Child abruptly ends story
up using general finding the frog, one but no general statement the frog, or an expression (e.g., stops narrating and
concluding statements about leaving with made as to the conclusion indicating the end (“el fin,” listener may need to
such as “and they were the frog, and one of the whole story. “se acabó,” “bye”). confirm that retell has
together again happy expression indicating -Some indication the story -One out of three sentences ended).
as could be.” the end of the story is complete. required for a score of 4. -The examiner indicates the
(e.g., “el fin,” “se acabó”). -Sentence about finding the ending of the story.
-One sentence about frog and an expression
following the boy’s indicating the end of the
footprints home, one story (e.g., “el fin,” “se
about finding the boy acabó,” “bye”).
in the bathtub taking -Two out of the three sentences
a bath, and one about required for a score of 4.
the frog jumping in and
jumping on top of the
dog’s head.

Note. Adapted from Heilmann et al., 2010. Details that the authors added to enhance scoring reliability are in italics.

Downloaded from: https://pubs.asha.org 1996 Maria Reyes on 01/13/2021, Terms of Use: https://pubs.asha.org/pubs/rights_and_permissions

You might also like