You are on page 1of 497

Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor: Johnny L. Matson

Johnny L. Matson Editor

Handbook of
Treatments for
Autism Spectrum
Disorder
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series

Series Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/8665


Johnny L. Matson
Editor

Handbook of
Treatments for Autism
Spectrum Disorder
Editor
Johnny L. Matson
Department of Psychology
Louisiana State University
Baton Rouge, LA, USA

ISSN 2192-922X     ISSN 2192-9238 (electronic)


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series
ISBN 978-3-319-61737-4    ISBN 978-3-319-61738-1 (eBook)
DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1

Library of Congress Control Number: 2017950159

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017


This work is subject to copyright. All rights are reserved by the Publisher, whether the whole or
part of the material is concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of
illustrations, recitation, broadcasting, reproduction on microfilms or in any other physical way,
and transmission or information storage and retrieval, electronic adaptation, computer software,
or by similar or dissimilar methodology now known or hereafter developed.
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, service marks, etc. in this
publication does not imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are
exempt from the relevant protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
The publisher, the authors and the editors are safe to assume that the advice and information in
this book are believed to be true and accurate at the date of publication. Neither the publisher nor
the authors or the editors give a warranty, express or implied, with respect to the material
contained herein or for any errors or omissions that may have been made. The publisher remains
neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Printed on acid-free paper

This Springer imprint is published by Springer Nature


The registered company is Springer International Publishing AG
The registered company address is: Gewerbestrasse 11, 6330 Cham, Switzerland
Contents

1 Historical Development of Treatment........................................   1


Lauren B. Fishbein, Maura L. Rouse, Noha F. Minshawi,
and Jill C. Fodstad
2 Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions
for Individuals with ASD.............................................................   17
Tiffany Kodak and Regina A. Carroll
3 Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment...........................   41
Paige E. Cervantes, Johnny L. Matson, Maya Matheis,
and Claire O. Burns
4 Institutional Review Boards and Standards..............................   59
Claire O. Burns, Esther Hong, and Dennis R. Dixon
5 Informed Consent........................................................................   67
Robert D. Rieske, Stephanie C. Babbitt, Joe H. Neal,
and Julie A. Spencer
6 The History, Pitfalls, and Promise of Licensure
in the Field of Behavior Analysis................................................   85
Julie Kornack
7 Staff Training................................................................................   95
Karola Dillenburger
8 Parent Training for Parents of Individuals Diagnosed
with Autism Spectrum Disorder................................................. 109
Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Sara M. Weinkauf,
Misty L. Oppenheim-Leaf, Mitchell Taubman, and Ronald Leaf
9 Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism
Spectrum Disorder....................................................................... 127
Matthew T. Brodhead, Mandy J. Rispoli, Oliver Wendt,
Jessica S. Akers, Kristina R. Gerencser, and So Yeon Kim
10 Comorbid Challenging Behaviors.............................................. 145
Marc J. Lanovaz, John T. Rapp, Alexie Gendron,
Isabelle Préfontaine, and Stéphanie Turgeon
11 Treatment of Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior............... 171
Brian D. Greer and Wayne W. Fisher

v
vi Contents

12 Philosophy and Common Components of Early Intensive


Behavioral Interventions............................................................. 191
Rebecca MacDonald, Diana Parry-Cruwys,
and Pamela Peterson
13 Additional Treatment Parameters and Issues Requiring
Study: Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention (EIBI)........... 209
Svein Eikeseth
14 Social Skills Training for Children and Adolescents
with Autism Spectrum Disorder................................................. 231
Keith C. Radley, Roderick D. O’Handley,
and Christian V. Sabey
15 Curriculums.................................................................................. 255
Hsu-Min Chiang
16 Augmentative and  Alternative Communication
and Autism.................................................................................... 269
Daphne Hartzheim
17 Vocational Training for Persons with Autism
Spectrum Disorder....................................................................... 289
Matthew J. Konst
18 Supports for Postsecondary Education...................................... 311
Lindsey W. Williams, Hillary H. Bush, and Jennifer N. Shafer
19 Sensory Integration Therapy and DIR/Floortime.................... 331
Jasper A. Estabillo and Johnny L. Matson
20 Socialization Programs for Adults with Autism
Spectrum Disorder....................................................................... 343
Saray Bonete and Clara Molinero
21 Treatment of Addiction in Adults with Autism
Spectrum Disorder....................................................................... 377
Laurence Lalanne, Luisa Weiner, and Gilles Bertschy
22 Diet and Supplementation Targeted for Autism
Spectrum Disorder....................................................................... 397
Mark J. Garcia, Pamela McPherson, Stuti Y. Patel,
and Claire O. Burns
23 The TEACCH Program for People with Autism:
Elements, Outcomes, and Comparison with Competing
Models........................................................................................... 427
Javier Virués-Ortega, Angela Arnold-Saritepe,
Catherine Hird, and Katrina Phillips
24 Positive Behavior Support........................................................... 437
Darlene Magito McLaughlin and Christopher E. Smith
Contents vii

25 Psychotropic Medications as Treatments for People


with Autism Spectrum Disorder................................................. 459
Alan Poling, Kristal Ehrhardt, and Anita Li
26 Current Status and Future Directions....................................... 477
Rachel L. Goldin and Johnny L. Matson
Index...................................................................................................... 485
Contributors

Jessica S. Akers  University of Nebraska Medical Center, Omaha, NE, USA


Angela Arnold-Saritepe The University of Auckland, Auckland, New
Zealand
Stephanie C. Babbitt  Idaho State University, Pocatello, ID, USA
Gilles Bertschy  Department of Psychiatry, Strasbourg University Hospital,
Strasbourg, France
Translational Medicine Federation, Medical School, Strasbourg University,
Strasbourg, France
INSERM 1114, Strasbourg University Hospital, Strasbourg, France
Saray Bonete  Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, Madrid, Spain
Matthew T. Brodhead Department of Educational Studies, Purdue
University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan, USA
Claire O. Burns Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills, CA, USA
Hillary H. Bush, PhD  Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA
Regina A. Carroll Department of Psychology, West Virginia University,
Morgantown, WV, USA
Paige E. Cervantes  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Hsu-Min Chiang, PhD  University of Macau, Taipa, Macau
Joseph H. Cihon  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA
Karola Dillenburger, BCBA-D Centre for Behaviour Analysis, Queens
University Belfast, Belfast, Northern Ireland, UK
Dennis R. Dixon  Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills,
CA, USA
Kristal Ehrhardt  Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA

ix
x Contributors

Svein Eikeseth Department of Behavioral Sciences, Oslo and Akershus


University College of Applied Sciences, Oslo, Norway
Jasper A. Estabillo  Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Lauren B. Fishbein  Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School
of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Wayne W. Fisher Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, University of
Nebraska Medical Center’s Munroe-Meyer Institute, Omaha, NE, USA
Jill C. Fodstad, PhD, HSPP, BCBA-D  Department of Psychiatry, Indiana
University School of Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Mark J. Garcia  Northwest Resource Center, Bossier City, LA, USA
Alexie Gendron  Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
Kristina R. Gerencser  Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA
Rachel L. Goldin  Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Brian D. Greer Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, University of
Nebraska Medical Center’s Munroe-Meyer Institute, Omaha, NE, USA
Daphne Hartzheim  Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Catherine Hird  The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Esther Hong Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills,
CA, USA
So Yeon Kim  Department of Educational Studies, Purdue University, West
Lafayette, IN, USA
Tiffany Kodak Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-
Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, USA
Matthew J. Konst, PhD Kennedy Krieger Institute, Johns Hopkins
University, Baltimore, MD, USA
Department of Psychology, J. Iverson Riddle Developmental Center,
Morganton, NC, USA
Julie Kornack  Center for Autism and Related Disorders, Woodland Hills,
CA, USA
Laurence Lalanne, MD, PhD-INSERM1114 Department of Psychiatry,
Strasbourg University Hospital, Strasbourg, France
Translational Medicine Federation, Medical School, Strasbourg University,
Strasbourg, France
Marc J. Lanovaz  Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
Justin B. Leaf, PhD, BCBA-D  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach,
CA, USA
Contributors xi

Ronald Leaf  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA


Anita Li  Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Rebecca MacDonald  The New England Center for Children, Southborough,
MA, USA
Maya Matheis Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Johnny L. Matson, PhD Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA
Darlene Magito McLaughlin, PhD, BCBA-D  Positive Behavior Support
Consulting & Psychological Resources, P.C, Centerport, NY, USA
Pamela McPherson Northwest Louisiana Human Services District,
Shreveport, LA, USA
Noha F. Minshawi  Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of
Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
Clara Molinero  Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, Madrid, Spain
Joe H. Neal  Idaho State University, Pocatello, ID, USA
Roderick D. O’Handley  University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg,
MS, USA
Misty L. Oppenheim-Leaf  Behavior Therapy and Learning Center, Calgary,
Canada
Diana Parry-Cruwys  The New England Center for Children, Southborough,
MA, USA
Stuti Y. Patel  Northwest Resource Center, Bossier City, LA, USA
Pamela Peterson The New England Center for Children, Southborough,
MA, USA
Katrina Phillips  The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand
Alan Poling  Western Michigan University, Kalamazoo, MI, USA
Isabelle Préfontaine  Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
Keith C. Radley  University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, MS, USA
John T. Rapp  Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA
Robert D. Rieske, PhD  Department of Psychology, Idaho State University,
Pocatello, ID, USA
Mandy J. Rispoli  Department of Educational Studies, Purdue University,
West Lafayette, IN, USA
Maura L. Rouse  Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School of
Medicine, Indianapolis, IN, USA
xii Contributors

Christian V. Sabey  Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA


Jennifer N. Shafer, MS, CRC  University of North Carolina – Chapel Hill,
Chapel Hill, NC, USA
Christopher E. Smith, PhD, BCBA-D Positive Behavior Support
Consulting & Psychological Resources, P.C, Centerport, NY, USA
Julie A. Spencer  Idaho State University, Pocatello, ID, USA
Mitchell Taubman  Autism Partnership Foundation, Seal Beach, CA, USA
Stéphanie Turgeon  Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada
Javier Virués-Ortega  School of Psychology, The University of Auckland,
Auckland, New Zealand
Luisa Weiner Department of Psychiatry, Strasbourg University Hospital,
Strasbourg, France
INSERM 1114, Strasbourg University Hospital, Strasbourg, France
Sara M. Weinkauf  JBA Institute, Torrance, CA, USA
Oliver Wendt  Department of Educational Studies, Purdue University, West
Lafayette, IN, USA
Lindsey W. Williams, PhD, CRC  University of North Carolina – Chapel
Hill, Chapel Hill, NC, USA
About the Editor

Johnny L. Matson, Ph.D.  is professor and distinguished research master in


the Department of Psychology at Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge,
LA, USA. He has also previously held a professorship in psychiatry and clinical
psychology at the University of Pittsburgh. He is the author of more than 800
publications including 41 books. He also served as founding editor in chief of
three journals: Research in Developmental Disabilities (Elsevier), Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders (Elsevier), and Review Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders (Springer).

xiii
Historical Development
of Treatment 1
Lauren B. Fishbein, Maura L. Rouse,
Noha F. Minshawi, and Jill C. Fodstad

Introduction According to psychodynamic theory, autism


was caused by psychogenic factors, particularly
In 1943, Leo Kanner published his seminal paper emotionally cold parenting (e.g., refrigerator
“Autistic Disturbances of Affective Contact.” He mothers). Based on this conceptualization, psy-
identified a condition which he called early infan- chodynamic treatments focused on using play
tile autism or what is referred to today as autism therapy to improve the mother-child bond and
spectrum disorder (ASD). Kanner described 11 help children resolve past conflicts and traumatic
cases in which children displayed a set of symp- events. This initial understanding and attitude
toms including profound social withdrawal, toward the treatment of autism was characterized
obsessive desire for sameness, and absence of by little hope for clinically significant change
language or language that did not serve a social (Lovaas, 1979; Rimland, 1964); as understanding
purpose (Kanner, 1943, 1949). Since Kanner’s of the disorder changed to include biological
identification of autism, there has been a great bases, researchers and clinicians began focusing
deal of focus on better understanding the etiology their attention on identifying more effective,
and treatment of autism. When autism was first alternative treatment approaches.
identified, it was thought to be a form of child- Behavioral treatments or more specifically
hood schizophrenia. Some researchers consid- applied behavior analysis (ABA) emphasized a
ered autism to be caused by psychological and systematic evaluation of specific behaviors and
environmental factors, whereas others argued changing these behaviors through using rein-
that autism was caused by biological factors. forcement and punishment. Behavioral studies
Clinical research and treatment approaches over conducted by researchers such as Ferster
time have evolved from those rooted firmly in a (1961) and Lovaas (1970, 1987) represented a
psychodynamic theoretical orientation to those sharp departure from traditional psychotherapy
based on a behavioral theoretical orientation. approaches and demonstrated clinically signif-
icant changes in the behaviors of individuals
with autism through increasing prosocial
behaviors (e.g., communication, socialization),
L.B. Fishbein • M.L. Rouse • N.F. Minshawi decreasing problem behaviors (e.g., aggression
J.C. Fodstad, PhD, HSPP, BCBA-D (*) and self-­injury), and including parents in the
Department of Psychiatry, Indiana University School
delivery of behavioral treatments to promote
of Medicine, 705 Riley Hospital Dr., Suite 4300,
Indianapolis, IN 46202, USA maintenance and generalization of treatment
e-mail: jfodstad@iupui.edu gains.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 1


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_1
2 L.B. Fishbein et al.

Over the past several decades, there has been child, this led to a less reversible form of autism
a theoretical shift toward more behavioral orien- which they called “chronic autistic disease.”
tations that have changed the overall attitudes Once a child developed chronic autistic disease,
toward the treatment of autism from one of little it was thought that the child’s paranoid attitudes
hope to one which is more optimistic. This chap- were consolidated and more resistant to change
ter provides a review of the historical develop- through psychotherapy.
ments that influenced the way autism has been The belief that refrigerator mothers were
conceptualized and treated since Kanner first responsible for their child’s autism was further
identified early infantile autism. Additionally, the influenced by the work of Bruno Bettelheim. In
implementation of early psychodynamic and 1967, Bettelheim published “The Empty Fortress:
behavioral treatments to improve the symptoms Infantile Autism and the Birth of the Self” assert-
of autism is discussed and evaluated. Furthermore, ing that autism was the result of emotionally cold
we highlight the important events and research parenting and that autism was not caused by bio-
studies that have influenced the identification of logical abnormalities (Bettelheim, 1967). He
effective autism treatments and the growth of illustrated this argument by comparing the home
applied behavior analysis as the gold standard environments of autistic children to concentra-
treatment for autism. tion camps and likening mothers to Nazi prison
guards. Bettelheim expanded on Kanner’s theory
of a psychogenic cause of autism by recommend-
Early Conceptualizations of Autism ing that children be removed from their parent’s
care, which he referred to as parent-ectomies.
Initially, autism was considered to be a form of In the 1960s, clinicians began to disagree
childhood schizophrenia and was conceptualized about how to best conceptualize autism. In con-
within a psychodynamic framework. According trast to Bettelheim’s emphasis on the role of
to psychodynamic theory, autism was caused by emotionally cold parents, lack of parental
psychogenic factors, such as psychological and warmth, and nurturing in early childhood in the
environmental variables (Abbate, Dunaeff, & development of autism, Rimland (1964) con-
Fenichel, 1955; Schopler, 1965). Within this ceptualized autism as a biologically based, neu-
framework, autism was considered a reaction to rological disorder. Rimland was a critic of
an overwhelming inner or outer assault at a vul- purely psychogenic explanations of autism and
nerable developmental stage between 6 and noted that there was a lack of compelling evi-
18 months of age when the child is differentiating dence to support the refrigerator mother theory.
himself from his mother (Garcia & Sarvis, 1964). In his work, “Infantile Autism: The Syndrome
Many authors argued that autism developed as a and Its Implications for Neural Theory of
result of being raised by “refrigerator mothers” Behavior,” he refuted the theory that autism
who were described as emotionally cold. Children could be explained by psychogenic factors
with autism were thought to have emotionally alone. He explained that purely psychogenic
deficient parents, especially mothers, and that causal theories, such as the refrigerator mother
children withdrew to escape their parents’ cold theory, had significant impact on those affected
nature that led to the child developing a paranoid by autism, especially family members who
attitude (Abbate et al., 1955; Bettelheim, 1959; experienced shame, guilt, and martial conflict as
Clancy & McBribe, 1969; Garcia & Sarvis, 1964; a result of being considered the cause of the
Kanner, 1949; Speers & Lansing, 1963). In a child’s symptoms. He suggested the need for
paper emphasizing the individualized application experimental and biological psychologists to
of psychodynamic approaches of the assessment, investigate alternative, biologically based causal
conceptualization, and treatment of four children explanations of autism, citing evidence from
with infantile autism, Garcia and Sarvis (1964) studies of the reticular formation in the brain to
asserted that if the mother counter-rejected the help explain the etiology of autism.
1  Historical Development of Treatment 3

The divide between Bettelheim and Rimland’s based on how fantasy manifests in both condi-
conceptualizations further widened as Rimland tions, where children with autism exhibit deficits
began advocating for parents by providing support in fantasy and children with schizophrenia exhibit
and education. He developed organizations and a excesses in fantasy. Children with schizophrenia
research institute dedicated to determining the frequently exhibited psychotic symptoms such as
causes of autism and developing appropriate treat- delusional thought content, especially thoughts of
ments. In 1969, Kanner delivered an important persecution, as well as auditory and/or visual hal-
speech at the National Society for Autistic Children lucinations. Children with infantile autism rarely
where he drastically shifted his conceptualization exhibited these symptoms of psychosis.
from a purely psychogenic conceptualization and Additionally, autism could be described as a fail-
agreed with Rimland that autism was caused by ure of development, while schizophrenia was bet-
biological factors (Feinstein, 2011). ter described as a loss of the sense of reality after
development was well established. Delusions and
hallucinations were key symptoms of childhood
Autism Versus  Childhood schizophrenia but they were not characteristic of
Schizophrenia autism. There were also differences in intellectual
functioning in both populations with mental retar-
When autism was first identified, the condition dation (MR, now termed intellectual disability
was considered a form of childhood schizophre- [ID]) being more common in autism. Rutter high-
nia (Abbate et al., 1955). In 1965, Schopler lighted differences in the sex distribution of both
(1965) expanded on Rimland’s (1964) conceptu- disorders, where autism was three to four times
alization that infantile autism was a congenital more likely in males than females and rates of
disorder by further indicating that it should be schizophrenia in adults were similar for males and
considered separate from childhood schizophre- females. In summary, Rutter suggested that autism
nia. The work of Sir Michael Rutter (1972) fur- developed on the basis of a disorder of cognitive
ther differentiated childhood schizophrenia from impairment that involved impairment in language
infantile autism. Rutter concluded that the use of comprehension and deficits in utilizing language
the term childhood schizophrenia was no longer and conceptual thinking.
useful for conveying scientific meaning as the Rutter’s (1972) reconceptualization of autism
term had been applied to any array of nonspecific as distinct from schizophrenia was reflected by
childhood problems. In his paper, he reconceptu- several changes in the field. In 1978, the Journal
alized autism as a disorder that presented early in of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia changed
infancy with three main features including defi- its name to the Journal of Autism and
cits in social development, deviant and delayed Developmental Disorders (Feinstein, 2011). In
language development, and ritualistic behaviors. addition, when the Diagnostic and Statistical
Rutter (1972) indicated that there were several Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition
key differences between childhood schizophrenia (DSM-III: American Psychiatric Association,
and autism with respect to differences in symp- 1980), was published, infantile autism was recat-
tomatology, onset, and course of the disorders. In egorized from falling under the heading of
regard to differences in the symptoms of both dis- “childhood schizophrenia” to the heading “per-
orders, Rutter noted that a key characteristic of vasive developmental disorders.” These two
autism was the failure of the child with autism to changes were important in reclassifying autism
develop social relationships, whereas children and showing a shift in the understanding of
with schizophrenia exhibited a loss of a sense of autism as a developmental disorder as opposed
reality after a period of typical social development. to a psychiatric disorder (DeMyer, Hingtgen, &
The two conditions could be further differentiated Jackson, 1981).
4 L.B. Fishbein et al.

Early Psychodynamic Treatments their demands met. The authors reported that
for Autism maternal-child bonds were created within the
first month of treatment, but regressions were
Given that initial conceptualizations of autism seen once the child returned to the natural family
were based on psychogenic factors and the refrig- environment. Of the 53 children treated in the
erator mother theory, early treatment approaches study, the authors reported that 12 were consid-
were rooted in psychodynamic theory. ered to be treated effectively, as measured through
Psychodynamic theory was based on Freud’s improvements in the maternal-child bond, use of
theory of abnormal behavior which emphasized language, and improvements in feeding
the underlying factors that influence human abnormalities.
behavior and that resolution of pathology came Other studies delivered treatment through an
from therapists helping the patient to resolve intensive, nonresidential school program for
underlying sources of psychological conflict children with schizophrenia, including infantile
(e.g., Abbate et al., 1955; Garcia & Sarvis, 1964). autism. Abbate et al. (1955) presented a model
In general, psychodynamic therapy included play of a nonresidential day program called The
therapy approaches that were thought to help League School for children with schizophrenia
reveal past conflicts or traumas and allow the including children with infantile autism. The
therapist to provide a supportive environment to goals of the day school program were to enhance
encourage the individual to reveal more of these ego development and functioning. The authors
conflicts and develop a bond with the therapist. presented a collaborative model of treatment
For example, Garcia and Sarvis (1964) presented that included involvement of social workers,
a psychodynamic-based approach to the evalua- educators, and a psychiatrist to determine
tion and treatment of four children with infantile whether a particular child may benefit from the
autism. The authors described the flexible appli- school program. The program enrolled 12 chil-
cation of a variety of treatment components dren in total, 7 of which were diagnosed with
including any of the following methods: redirec- autism. The school philosophy viewed teachers
tion, limit setting, play therapy, parent counsel- as the important contact for the child and treat-
ing, restarting development at the age of onset, ment focused on child-directed play to facilitate
enrollment in preschool, and/or school the development of relationships. Teachers were
collaboration. also required to deliver treatment based on their
Some authors have suggested that the family intuition about the child’s internal psychic
should be the unit of treatment rather than the events and impose limits to help increase the
individual child in order to help the child develop child’s ego differentiation and object relation-
a family bond and provide the child with a frame- ship development. There was one teacher for
work for normal socialization through the acqui- every two children to allow for individual work
sition of social and language skills. In a study of and attention on the child. The goal was to find
53 children with autism conducted over a 10-year ways to establish contact with the child which
period by Clancy and McBride (1969), children was often started through physical contact com-
and mothers were hospitalized together to pro- bined with rhythmic movements such as cud-
mote the maternal-child bond. The first goal of dling, rocking, or swinging the child. Once the
the therapist was to intrude upon the child. Once teacher established contact with the child, treat-
the child responded consistently to the therapist, ment was centered on child-led play while ther-
treatment delivery was transferred from the ther- apists commented on and described the child’s
apist to the mother. The authors used mealtimes, actions. To evaluate treatment outcomes,
followed by playtime, as a way to enhance the detailed anecdotal records on child progress,
quality of the mother-child interaction. The next problems, needs, and treatment planning meet-
step focused on increasing eye contact by requir- ings were kept by teachers on a daily basis
ing children to make eye contact in order to get (Abbate et al., 1955).
1  Historical Development of Treatment 5

In a longitudinal study of children with infan- ents to receive couple therapy in addition to indi-
tile autism and childhood schizophrenia by Eaton vidual therapy (Eaton & Menoloascino, 1967).
and Menoloascino (1967), children were assigned Several studies also emphasized the importance
to either intensive treatment, moderate treatment, of parent training and/or parent collaboration in
or no treatment conditions. Intensive treatment their child’s treatment (Abbate et al., 1955;
included initial hospitalization followed by day Clancy & McBride, 1969).
treatment or outpatient treatment. The children in Over the years, researchers have demonstrated
the treatment condition received play therapy for that autism is not caused by past trauma and psy-
a minimum of 3 days a week. They also received chodynamic interventions showed little promise
milieu therapy, special education, speech therapy, for change (e.g., Cantwell & Baker, 1984;
and medication management. Parents were Lovaas, 1979). As Lovaas, Freitag, Gold, and
required to participate in couple therapy for a Kassorla (1965) pointed out, psychoanalytic play
minimum of once per week in addition to each therapy provides the most attention and therapeu-
parent receiving individual therapy. Children in tic support to children when they display more
the moderate treatment condition received play severe problems, which potentially reinforced
therapy once per week on an outpatient basis. the problem behaviors and become counter-­
Their parents were seen for parent counseling therapeutic. Furthermore, Lovaas (1979) pub-
and medication management less than once a lished a paper comparing and contrasting
week. Families assigned to the no treatment con- psychodynamic and behavioral treatments for
dition received the same baseline and follow-up autism. In his critique of psychodynamic treat-
evaluations as the patients in the other two condi- ments, Lovaas described psychodynamic treat-
tions. At follow-up, the children with infantile ments as based on an illness model, characterized
autism showed minimal to no improvement with by poorly defined approaches that included varia-
respect to language development, intellectual tions of play therapy and inclusion of parents
functioning, or adaptive behaviors, such as toilet and/or teachers in treatment. The description of
training. these approaches was vague, did not use scientifi-
Psychodynamic treatment components often cally rigorous methods to demonstrate change,
included play therapy activities to promote social and prohibited replication across studies. Given
contact, music activities to facilitate responding, that psychodynamic treatments failed to demon-
as well as water play to help the child increase strate treatment efficacy through both the absence
pleasure and decrease social withdrawal. This of objective data and anecdotal reports of mini-
was thought to renew the privilege of infancy and mal change in patients, the field began to shift
provide the child with a sense of mastery and toward identifying more effective treatments.
control. Art therapy, music therapy, and dramatic Through more rigorous scientific methods, treat-
play were additional components thought to pro- ments based on operant conditioning showed
mote an emotional release and expression of feel- promise with respect to providing a more opti-
ing and needs (Abbate et al., 1955; Garcia & mistic direction in developing and disseminating
Sarvis, 1964; Speers & Lansing, 1963). treatments that fostered clinically significant
Many psychodynamic treatments also changes and improvement in the quality of life of
involved parental participation in a variety of individuals with autism.
ways. In one study, mothers and children were
hospitalized together (Clancy & McBride, 1969).
Other studies have required parents to participate  he Development of Behavior
T
in group and/or individual therapy with the goal Therapy
of helping them to become more aware of their
own narcissistic and dependency needs (Abbate The field of psychology began to experience a
et al., 1955; Eaton & Menoloascino, 1967; Speers shift in the focus of the conceptualization, study,
and Lansing, 1963), and one study required par- and treatment of autism in the beginning to
6 L.B. Fishbein et al.

­ iddle of the 1960s. Whereas the early autism


m current behavioral repertoire and develop new,
pioneers such as Kanner (1943) and Bettelheim more appropriate behaviors. Ferster and DeMyer
(1967) considered the diagnosis of autism as a (1961) conducted a study with three children
core deficit caused by a lack of attachment with a with autism in an inpatient hospital. The experi-
maternal figure, researchers and clinicians in the mental design included many devices that were
latter half of the century began to study autism in operated by a coin or key and provided a rein-
terms of individual behaviors that were exhibited. forcing consequence to the child (i.e., a general-
Early behavioral studies were based on operant ized reinforcer). The generalized reinforcer (i.e.,
conditioning principles characterized by learning coin) could be exchanged for small trinkets,
that took place by the effects of reinforcement packages of food, a music handset, an electric
and punishment (e.g., Ferster & DeMyer, 1961; organ, and a picture viewer. Alone in the observa-
Roos & Oliver, 1969; Hundziak, Maurer, & tion room, children pressed keys to receive their
Watson, 1965). Operant conditioning, first used chosen reinforcer. Reinforcers were then deliv-
to change animal behavior, was later discovered ered contingently to shape children’s behaviors
to be useful to shape children’s behavior. (e.g., point to a target picture, match simple fig-
Behavior modification, characterized by the sys- ures). The results demonstrated that it was possi-
tematic evaluation of behavior, was an approach ble to bring the behavior of these individuals
researchers began to turn to as a way to under- under environmental control through techniques
stand changes in behavior (Keehn & Webster, of operant reinforcement.
1969). This systematic examination of behavior The shift from psychodynamic treatments
began with case studies of objective, specific, and toward behavioral treatments became more evi-
measurable data in an effort to gather information dent through a case study by Jensen and Womack
that properly and accurately defined the experi- (1967). The authors employed traditional psy-
ences of individuals diagnosed with autism. chodynamic treatment during the first year but
The literature has an abundance of case stud- shifted to the use of operant conditioning proce-
ies examining specific behaviors of children with dures after minimal improvements were seen
autism, often measuring increases or decreases in through psychodynamic therapy. The psychody-
behaviors of interest. Several early behavioral namic components of treatment included milieu
studies contributed to the overall growth of therapy, play therapy several times per week, and
behavioral interventions for autism. These stud- psychotherapy for the child’s mother. There were
ies were important because they not only focused improvements seen including improved relation-
on the objective measurement of change in ships with certain individuals, increased fre-
observable behaviors but also because they repre- quency of eye contact, increases in following
sented a sharp departure from traditional psycho- directions, and more appropriate object use.
therapy approaches. Despite these improvements, therapists and staff
The studies conducted by Ferster (e.g., Ferster, remained discouraged from these relatively insig-
1961, Ferster & DeMyer, 1961) were the first nificant results from this intensive, year-long
laboratory studies using experimental analysis of treatment. Therefore, an operant conditioning
behavior to treat behaviors associated with program was used as an adjunct to traditional
autism. They were instrumental in setting the psychodynamic therapy in order to maximize the
groundwork for behavioral treatment studies to child’s progress.
address symptoms of autism. The results from Jensen and Womack’s (1967) operant condi-
these studies demonstrated that behaviors could tioning program included identifying potential
be shaped and maintained by reinforcing conse- positive reinforcers, identifying target behaviors
quences in a laboratory setting. These seminal to increase (e.g., social contact with peers, use of
studies using the experimental analysis of behav- language, cooperative play), and identifying
ior in children with autism provided a basis for undesirable behaviors to decrease (e.g., tan-
using objective techniques to control a child’s trums, aggression, stereotyped behaviors).
1  Historical Development of Treatment 7

This b­ehavioral treatment program lasted ticipated in children’s behavioral treatment,


10 weeks and included reinforcing peer interac- ­family involvement in therapy was viewed as an
tions and verbal behavior and extinguishing tem- important element of children’s overall treatment
per tantrums, aggression, and stereotyped plan (Ward & Hoddinott, 1965).
behaviors. Several areas of improvement were As behavioral treatments were met with suc-
noted including social smiling (previously absent cess in inpatient and residential settings, research-
at baseline), forming novel phrases and sen- ers began to apply these methods to shape
tences (e.g., from only single words at baseline behavior in outpatient settings. Wetzel, Baker,
to flexible use of greetings at the end of treat- Roney, and Martin (1966) conducted a case study
ment), decreased ritualistic behavior, decreased using behavioral principles in an outpatient clinic
aggressive behavior, and decreased temper tan- to treat a 6-year-old child with autism with a
trums. In addition to demonstrating significant focus on changing specific behaviors. The treat-
changes in these target behaviors, the authors ment plan was designed to initially shape this
observed a change in therapist and staff attitudes young patient’s behavior to increase approach to
whereby staff became more encouraged and an object (i.e., bobo doll) by delivering reinforce-
enthusiastic about the changes observed follow- ment of attention when the patient performed the
ing the 10-week behavioral intervention. desired behavior (i.e., closeness to the object). As
Although no formal assessment of staff attitudes the patient gained success with this behavior, the
before, during, or after treatment was reported, researchers expanded the treatment plan to
researchers indicated that virtually all staff held include other social behaviors (e.g., response to
a more positive view of the child after the treat- commands) and verbal behaviors (e.g., label
ment concluded, in part due to decreased prob- objects, initiate verbal interaction). Success in
lem behaviors. This study demonstrated that increasing these more varied social behaviors and
operant conditioning approaches could produce verbal behaviors continued as the participant’s
larger behavioral changes over the course of a parents used shaping procedures outside of the
substantially shorter time period (i.e., 10 weeks) outpatient treatment setting.
as compared to those seen through psychody- The success of the intervention by Wetzel and
namic approaches over a longer period of time colleagues (1966) had implications for different
(i.e., 1 year). ways to apply behavioral principles in an outpa-
Inpatient psychiatric and residential settings tient setting and, again, deviated from traditional
were the first clinical settings in which behavioral psychotherapy approaches. Applying behavioral
modification treatments took place. Residential approaches in the outpatient environment was an
treatment programs supplied a long-term treat- extension of previous studies conducted in more
ment option for children who required intensive, controlled settings such as the laboratory (Ferster,
but less acute, support than would be needed for 1961, Ferster & DeMyer, 1961) and inpatient set-
placement in an inpatient psychiatric unit (see tings (e.g., Lovaas, 1964, Lovaas, Freitag et al.,
Leichtman, 2008 for a review of the history of 1965). With these results, researchers demon-
residential treatment). Residential treatment strated that behavior change using operant condi-
offered children a therapeutic environment with tioning could be extended to outpatient treatment
regular access to counselors. These programs settings with inherently less experimental control
also offered families case management services than would be found in hospital or lab settings.
to aid in encouraging the reunification of children Wetzel et al. (1966) conducted one such study
with their families when the timing was appropri- when they moved from a more controlled setting
ate. Rubin and Simson (1960) reported that resi- to a less controlled setting. This research began in
dential treatment was relatively successful, as the lab and extended to the child’s natural envi-
most facilities at that time indicated that 60% of ronment (e.g., home and school). To maintain
their residents returned to their family home. treatment integrity outside of controlled environ-
Implicitly then, even before parents formally par- ments, emphasis was placed on the importance of
8 L.B. Fishbein et al.

functional relationships, selecting objective, Increasing Prosocial Behavior


­specific, and observable behaviors as targets for
change and reliance on objective data collection Behavior modification techniques used to
to inform treatment progress and outcomes. increase prosocial behavior and adaptive skills
Wetzel and colleagues reported that after 20 ther- utilized reinforcement methods. Lovaas, Koegel,
apy sessions, a child who once engaged in self-­ Simmons, and Long (1973) delineated between
injurious behavior, temper tantrums, little primary and secondary reinforcers, writing that
interaction with others, and few adaptive skills while primary reinforcers (e.g., edibles) can be
had made such behavioral improvements once beneficial, all reinforcers need specific environ-
treatment was completed that the child was mental conditions (i.e., motivation) for them to
judged to be fit to be introduced into a special be meaningful and that individuals with autism
education classroom. benefit most from secondary reinforcers (e.g.,
Additional case studies using operant condi- verbal praise, tokens). Other researchers dis-
tioning techniques in the treatment of children agreed and stated that individuals with autism are
with autism began to emerge and add to the lit- not able to benefit from secondary reinforcers
erature. Risely (1968) described his work with a (Ferster & DeMyer, 1961). A general consensus
6-year-old female patient with problematic existed such that the reinforcer must be durable
behaviors (i.e., climbing on furniture) leading to and potent in a distraction-free environment for it
significant injury. Through treatment focusing on to create meaningful behavioral change (Ferster
specific behaviors, climbing behavior decreased, & DeMyer; Hewett, 1965; Kanfer & Matarazzo,
and consequently, alternative behaviors (i.e., 1959; Skinner, 1953).
maintaining eye contact and sitting in a chair) Reinforcement strategies were used to
increased. Additionally, with Risely’s (1968) increase desirable behaviors. Several studies
emphasis placed on specific behaviors, rather were conducted demonstrating the effectiveness
than “autism” in general, this patient increased of using reinforcement to increase prosocial
her ability to imitate behavior, a skill she (and skills, such as eye contact and compliance
many children with autism) lacked. (Hartung, 1970; Craighead, O’Leary, & Allen,
Risely’s (1968) work points to behavioral 1973). Early success was also experienced in
excesses and deficits characteristic of autism. shaping functional daily living skills (Lovaas
Although individuals with autism frequently et al., 1973). A major skill area in which individu-
have strengths and weaknesses in a variety of als with autism have difficulty is social function-
areas, this population tends to have deficits in ing (e.g., conversation skills, play skills). Perhaps
prosocial and adaptive skills (e.g., verbal and due to the complexity of these skills and social
nonverbal communication, social skills, pre- interactions in general, effectively teaching these
tend play; Rutter, 1978). Alternatively, chil- skills to individuals with autism proved to be dif-
dren with autism tend to have excesses in ficult. In an effort to define and improve these
several areas, notably, maladaptive behaviors skills necessary to function in the social world,
(e.g., aggression, self-stimulatory behaviors; Lovaas, Baer, and Bijou (1965) created a sym-
Margolies, 1977). With this knowledge, the bolic social stimulus that centered on the use of
goal of behavioral therapy became to increase dolls, puppets, and movies. The dolls (inside
children’s prosocial, or desirable, behaviors plexiglass boxes that could be controlled with
and decrease their destructive, or maladaptive, levers by participants to display either aggression
behaviors. As the treatment of children with or affection), puppets (inside plexiglass boxes
autism continued to expand out of the labora- that can be controlled to look at, “talk” to, and
tory and into the environments of children’s offer objects to the participant), and movies
everyday lives, parents became more involved (shown continuously with sound; both the sound
in the execution of behavioral treatment (e.g., and picture were able to be controlled by the par-
Gelfand & Hartman, 1968). ticipant) offered examples of social situations in
1  Historical Development of Treatment 9

which the individual with autism could engage. other disruptive behaviors including aggres-
The social event (controlled by the researcher) sion and tantrums, behavioral researchers
was contingent on the child’s response. By creat- began to use forms of punishment and aversive
ing these social situations, Lovaas et al. taught conditioning (e.g., Lovaas, 1970; Buss, 1961;
these very complex social skills. These research- Deur & Park, 1970). Self-stimulatory behav-
ers emphasized that the utility of the social stim- iors (e.g., “autistic rocking,” Risely, 1968)
uli was to determine if children engaged with the were also a focus of punishment procedures.
objects and how they did so. They noted that a Many forms of punishment have been used to
lack of interaction with the stimuli was also tell- decrease behaviors, from electric shock (e.g.,
ing data related to the child’s social motivation. Lovaas, Schaeffer, & Simmons, 1965) to time
Perhaps one of the most significant social out (e.g., Lovaas, 1970) to verbal and physical
behaviors in which individuals engage is verbal punishments (e.g., slapping, immobilizing
communication. Lovaas, Schreibman, and limbs; Jensen & Womack, 1967; Koegel &
Koegel (1974) wrote of a stepwise language Covert, 1972).
acquisition training program to improve chil- Electric shock, now a controversial form of
dren’s communicative functioning. Their pro- punishment, was accepted as a method to
gram shaped children’s language in four steps of decrease behaviors in the 1960s and 1970s. For
verbal imitation: (1) the child’s vocalizations example, electric shock was used as a contingent
were reinforced by the therapist; (2) the child’s punishment to decrease climbing behavior in a
vocalizations were reinforced contingently (i.e., 6-year-old female (Risely, 1968). In this labora-
only in response to the therapist’s); (3) the child’s tory procedure, electric shock was locally applied
vocalizations were reinforced contingently (until (e.g., to a specific area of the leg), when the
he matched a particular letter sound by the thera- young girl began climbing on furniture. Overall,
pist); and (4) the child was reinforced contin- electric shock, paired with verbal punishment
gently based on his ability to imitate different (“No!”), decreased inappropriate climbing
letter sounds. Once imitative speech was estab- behavior. Lovaas (1970) frequently relied on
lished, the therapist then began working with the electric shock treatment to eliminate self-­
child to create meaningful speech. Based upon destructive behavior from children’s behavioral
their findings, Lovaas et al. concluded that an repertoire, and he found it to be quite effective:
effective program for teaching children with “independently of how badly the child is mutilat-
autism functional language had to include les- ing himself or how long he has been doing so, we
sons on discriminating between expressive (i.e., can essentially remove the self-destructive behav-
verbal) and comprehensive (i.e., nonverbal) ior within the first minute” (p.38). Lovaas indi-
speech, as most communicative situations cated that regardless of the intensity of children’s
included both components. The authors proposed self-destructive behaviors, applying punishment
shaping functional communication first (e.g., procedures could quickly eliminate the behaviors
requesting food) for children with autism, then of concern.
moving on to more abstract concepts (e.g., time) Decreasing self-destructive and disruptive
once the child improved language proficiency. behavior was the goal of the punishment proce-
dures used by researchers such as Lovaas (1970)
and Risely (1968). However, in order to affect
Decreasing Maladaptive Behavior behavior outside of the laboratory, greater general-
ization had to be obtained. One way to improve
Margolies (1977) wrote that before improve- generalization was through the use of overcorrec-
ments in prosocial behavior could be made, tion procedures. Overcorrection, consisting of res-
self-­destructive behavior, such as head banging titution (correcting the effects of the undesirable
and scratching, had to be eliminated. To behavior) and positive practice (repeatedly practic-
decrease these self-destructive behaviors and ing the desirable behavior), was first implemented
10 L.B. Fishbein et al.

in the treatment of a 50-year-old woman with pro- Along with general functioning, a number of
found intellectual disability who engaged in sig- other factors were believed to influence parents’
nificant disruptive and aggressive behavior in an response to children’s behavior and their ability
inpatient unit (Foxx & Azrin, 1972). Overcorrection to implement behavioral treatment in their homes
with children with autism was used as a method to (Ferster, 1989). Parents’ desire to stop the behav-
decrease disruptive and self-destructive behaviors ior from occurring can be a motivating factor in
(e.g., hand mouthing; Foxx & Azrin, 1973). As their responding to their child in a manner that
children engaged in overcorrection, they essen- will increase or decrease the likelihood that the
tially learned new, more appropriate behaviors to behavior will continue. Parent distractedness
replace their existing, less appropriate behaviors. (“prepotency of other performances”; doing
Foxx and Azrin (1973) concluded that overcorrec- something else while the child is engaging in a
tion is often more effective and enduring than pun- behavior) may unintentionally reinforce a behav-
ishment, particularly when shaping self-stimulatory ior (Ferster, p. 6). Additionally, as most behaviors
behaviors. As will be discussed below, parent train- increased in intensity and frequency over time,
ing focused on operant conditioning principles was parents often unknowingly reinforced the child’s
also used as a method to generalize decreases in behavior by gradually changing their own behav-
disruptive behavior (Wetzel et al., 1966). ioral response to accommodate their child (i.e.,
the child’s behavior shaped the parents’ behavior;
Ferster, 1961). Because these factors likely influ-
Parent Training enced implementation of behavioral strategies at
home, pointed parent education was a focus of
As the study of autism treatment continued to some early studies demonstrating the effective-
shift away from a predominately psychody- ness of parent training. For example, the parents
namic approach, a major shift occurred in the of a 3-year-old male with autism attended 21 ses-
delivery of treatment when parent involvement sions during which they learned operant condi-
in behavioral modification arose (Gelfand & tioning techniques (i.e., reinforcement,
Hartman, 1968). Early in the history of behav- punishment), social learning theory, and how to
ior modification, reinforcement and punish- track their child’s behavior (Schell & Adams,
ment were only clinically applied by clinicians 1968). Strategies parents learned during parent
or researchers. However, parents began to be education sessions proved to be successful, as the
viewed as providers of reinforcement or those child’s problematic behaviors continued to
who withdrew reinforcement (Gelfand & remain decreased from baseline at a 4-month
Hartman; Wetzel, 1966). This development pro- follow-up.
vided a major step forward in the field of behav-
ioral treatment for autism in that treatment
could now extend out of the laboratory setting  rowth of Behavioral Treatments
G
and into the home; generalization into real-life for Autism
situations could occur. Several considerations
emerged when including parents in treatment Overtime, behavioral interventions have grown,
that significantly impacted the child. As noted while psychodynamic approaches have failed to
by Jensen and Womack (1967), assessing the demonstrate effectiveness, and less emphasis was
overall functioning (e.g., coping ability) of the placed on an illness model. As the literature on
child’s parents is necessary before implement- behavioral treatments for autism began to grow,
ing behavioral therapy with parent involve- what was previously called behavior modifica-
ment. Parents’ ability to cope with distressing tion became known as applied behavior analysis
situations has clear implications to the effec- (ABA). ABA is “the science in which tactics
tiveness and generalizability of the treatment in derived from the principles of behavior are
the home setting. applied systematically to improve socially
1  Historical Development of Treatment 11

s­ ignificant behavior and experimentation is used and specified so that procedures can be repli-
to identify the variables responsible for behavior cated (e.g., rather than using the broad term
change (Cooper, Heron, & Heward, 2007, p. 20).” “social reinforcement,” ABA must provide a spe-
There were a number of reasons that ABA cific description such as the stimuli used, the
became increasingly popular and more widely contingency, and schedule of reinforcement).
accepted. These reasons included the use of sin- Another dimension was that the intervention
gle subject methodology in behavioral studies, should be conceptually systematic, meaning that
allowing for greater experimental control and the procedures used for change should specify
demonstration of change across many areas of the relevance to the behavioral principles from
functioning. The behavioral approach also which they were derived. ABA interventions
focused on changing specific, observable behav- must also be considered effective and produce
iors. The emphasis on the importance of socially large enough effects for socially valid change.
valid targets for behavior change while planning The final dimension of ABA was generalizability
for maintenance of change over time and general- such that changes in behavior are maintained
ization of behavioral responses across settings over time, across different (nontreatment) set-
and people became a hallmark of ABA (Baer, tings, and across a variety of related behaviors.
Wolf, & Risely, 1968). There were several semi- In the 1960s and 1970s, there was a growth in
nal research studies that demonstrated clinically behavioral approaches to treatment. In the 1980s,
significant changes using objective measures of these behavioral approaches become more spe-
change and experimental control. More emphasis cific and refined. Many studies using ABA dem-
was also placed on identifying the functions of onstrated socially significant changes across
target behaviors and using this data to inform the many areas of functioning including socializa-
implementation of function-based interventions. tion, adaptive behavior, communication, behav-
In addition, state and national agencies per- ior problems, and restricted and repetitive
formed independent evaluations of treatments for behaviors. This standardized behavior analytic
autism and classified ABA as an empirically sup- format included teaching skills using discrete
ported treatment for autism and recommended trial training (DTT), compliance training, and
ABA as the preferred treatment for autism. contingent reinforcement.
Baer et al. (1968) outlined a number of The work of Ivar Lovaas prescribed a frame-
dimensions with which to evaluate whether a work for which to implement a standardized
particular intervention was considered to be treatment that still allowed for individualization.
ABA. First, ABA should be applied, such that In his 1987 study, Lovaas demonstrated that the
the procedures produce socially significant use of behavior modification techniques could
change in an individual’s life (e.g., improving produce significant increases in the cognitive
language, socialization, self-help skills, or lei- functioning of children with autism. When
sure skills). The second criterion was that ABA Lovaas (1987) published his 15-year longitudinal
should have a behavioral focus, meaning that the study describing the improvements children with
intervention should focus on specific behaviors autism can attain through intensive treatment, the
that are both measurable and reliably assessed. psychological community gained some hope that
The analytic component described the impor- it had once lost. In this pivotal study, 19 children
tance of demonstrating functional relationships (prorated mental age of 11 months or more at
between manipulated events and measurable chronological age of 30 months; chronological
change observed in the target behavior (i.e., age less than 40 months or 46 months if nonver-
demonstrating experimental control of the occur- bal) diagnosed with autism received more than
rence of behavior). ABA must also be techno- 40 h of intensive one-to-one treatment per week.
logical, meaning that the techniques used to Two control groups (group 1, 19 children received
change behaviors are fully identified, and all 10 h or less of one-to-one treatment per week;
salient components must be clearly described group 2, 21 children received no treatment) were
12 L.B. Fishbein et al.

also included. There were no significant differ- Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1994). The methods
ences between any of the groups at baseline. and results of this study offered researchers and
Goals of the first year of Lovaas’ (1987) treat- clinicians guidance on conducting functional
ment program were to decrease aggressive and behavior assessments (FBA) and using the data
self-stimulatory behaviors and increase compli- to inform treatment development specific to a
ance to verbal requests, play behavior, and imita- given individual. This study continued to shift
tion of others. During the second year of the researchers’ and clinicians’ understanding of the
program, researchers sought to increase chil- treatment of autism and associated behaviors
dren’s expressive and abstract language skills and through the treatment of self-injury, often a dan-
social behavior with peers. The third year focused gerous and challenging behavior for families and
on improving emotional expression, pre-­ therapists (Iwata et al., 1994). Self-injury can
academic skills, and observational learning. take many forms (e.g., self-biting, head banging,
Children were enrolled in a participating pre- hand mouthing, eye gouging), and Iwata et al.
school classroom at the appropriate age. (1994) proposed a treatment program that
Children’s diagnosis of autism was not to be dis- decreased a variety of these behaviors.
closed to the school so that they were treated as Researchers introduced four environmental con-
typically as possible. The goal of the treatment ditions (i.e., social disapproval, academic
program was for children to progress into kinder- demand, unstructured play, and child alone) to
garten and then into a mainstream first grade nine participants. The child’s behavior was
classroom, second grade, and so on. Once chil- observed until a stable level of self-injury was
dren were placed in a mainstream grade school observed, unstable levels of self-injury were
classroom, intervention was decreased to 10 h or observed for 5 days, or 12 days of sessions were
less per week. completed.
Lovaas (1987) reported that the children Several within and between participant differ-
enrolled in the treatment program improved in ences were observed; however, Iwata et al. (1994)
many areas of functioning. Most relevant to this suggested five general findings from this study.
discussion are gains in intellectual functioning. They reported that (1) children engaged in rela-
The experimental group made significantly tively low frequencies of self-injury during
higher gains in IQ points than the control groups; unstructured play, (2) self-injury was highest in
notably, this group gained 30 IQ points over con- the alone condition (external simulation was
trol group one, and these gains remained stable at minimal), (3) some subjects had very high fre-
1-year follow-up. Both control groups were quencies during the high demand (i.e., academic)
unchanged from baseline. Lovaas reported that condition, (4) one participant engaged in self-­
by first grade, the experimental group had nine injury most often during the social disapproval
children with IQ scores in the average to above (“Don’t do that, you will get hurt”) condition,
average range (range, 94–120, eight children and (5) two participants demonstrated an undif-
with IQ scores in the extremely low range (IQ ferentiated pattern of self-injury. In sum, Iwata
range, 56–95), and two children with IQ scores et al. suggest that self-injury may be a function of
below 30. Additionally, he reported that nine reinforcement and motivational variables and
children were placed in mainstream first grade provided a technology to be used in research and
classrooms, eight children were placed in special clinical settings in order to identify the function
education classes, and two children were placed of problem behaviors.
in classrooms for children with autism or pro- Applied behavior analysis offered new hope
found intellectual disability. and used clinical research methodology that per-
Another important development in the field of mitted the demonstration of experimental control
ABA occurred in 1994 when Iwata and col- and placed an emphasis on the use of single case
leagues conducted an experimental functional designs to help identify effective treatment
analysis of self-injurious behavior (Iwata, Dorsey, approaches. The growth of ABA was further
1  Historical Development of Treatment 13

influenced by state and national efforts aimed at ising intervention for children with autism (NAC,
evaluating and identifying empirically supported 2009).
treatments for autism (e.g., the New York State Although ABA is the most effective treatment
Department of Health, Early Intervention for behavioral symptoms of autism to date, the
Program and the National Autism Center). The future of autism treatment will likely need to
New York State Department of Health, Early include treatment for comorbid mental health
Intervention Program published clinical practice conditions, such as anxiety or depressive disor-
guidelines regarding treatment of young children ders. ABA treatment programs focus solely on
with autism that concluded ABA demonstrated observable behaviors. However, children also
the most empirical support and recommended experience thoughts and feelings that may not be
ABA as the treatment of choice for young chil- as amendable to ABA treatment protocols as
dren with autism (1999). The National Autism observable behaviors. Treatment of comorbid
Center (NAC) conducted the National Standard diagnoses should be a focus of therapy, as 70% of
Project (NSP) to thoroughly review the current individuals diagnosed with autism also meet
empirical support for various autism treatments diagnostic criteria for one other psychiatric disor-
(National Autism Center, 2009). Based on this der and 40% meet diagnostic criteria for two or
thorough review, the NSP concluded that ABA more disorders (American Psychiatric
demonstrated the strongest evidence base for the Association, 2013). While a behavioral treatment
treatment of individuals with autism. The deter- program, such as ABA, will/should likely play an
mination that ABA was an empirically supported important in the treatment of autism in the future,
treatment for autism by these state and national the strictly behavioral treatment programs will
projects further influenced the growth of ABA likely need to be supplemented with additional
treatments for autism. The extensive research therapeutic approaches to address cognitive and
base and seminal work by researchers such as emotional factors of comorbid conditions. This is
Ferster, Lovaas, and Iwata over the past several of utmost importance, given that the majority of
decades have contributed to the growth of ABA ABA practitioners (i.e., board certified behavior
interventions. Major shifts in the attitudes about analysts [BCBA, BCBA-D], board certified
autism treatment were seen as behavior change assistant behavior analysts [BCaBA], registered
became more apparent, providing more hope for behavior technicians [RBT], etc.) do not receive
socially valid change in the lives of individuals expert-level training in the diagnosis, evaluation,
with autism. and treatment of mental health conditions.
Finally, children with autism benefit from ear-
lier diagnosis. While children can be reliably
Conclusions diagnosed with autism by their second birthday
(as early as 18 months), the median age of diag-
Since Kanner first identified autism as a psycho- nosis in the United States is over 4 years old
logical diagnosis in 1943, the understanding of (Autism and Developmental Disabilities
the disorder has evolved. As the conceptualiza- Monitoring Network [ADDM], 2014; Center for
tion of autism shifted away from a psychoana- Disease Control and Prevention, 2015). This
lytic focus to a behaviorally based approach, so delay in diagnosis indicates that children with
too did treatment. This new emphasis brought autism may be missing a critical time period
optimism to what was once thought to be a rather when they could be receiving intervention.
hopeless prognosis. Parents are now viewed not Similarly, significant differences in identification
as cause of their children’s problems but as the of children in different ethnic groups are a grow-
facilitators of their treatment. The emphasis on ing concern. Because non-Hispanic white chil-
objective and data-driven behavioral treatments dren are more likely to be identified as meeting
gave rise to the popularity of ABA treatment pro- criteria for autism (ADDM, 2014), they are more
grams, which is now supported as the most prom- likely to receive early intervention and therefore
14 L.B. Fishbein et al.

more likely to experience more favorable out- Eaton, L., & Menoloascino, F. J. (1967). Psychotic reac-
tions of childhood: A follow-up study. American
comes in the future. Early identification and diag-
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 37, 521–529.
nosis of all children with autism will lead to Feinstein, A. (2011). A history of autism: Conversations
appropriate treatments and brighter futures. with the pioneers. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley &
Sons.
Ferster, C. B. (1961). Positive reinforcement and behav-
ioral deficits of autistic children. Child Development,
References 32, 437–456.
Ferster, C. B. (1989). Positive reinforcement and behav-
Abbate, G. M., Dunaeff, D., & Fenichel, C. (1955). A ioral deficits of young children. Focus on Autistic
pilot study of schizophrenic children in a nonresiden- Behavior, 4, 1–12.
tial school. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 27, Ferster, C. B., & DeMyer, M. K. (1961). A method for
107–116. the experimental analysis of the behavior of autistic
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic children. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 32,
and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). 89–98.
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Foxx, R. M., & Azrin, N. H. (1972). Restitution: A
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic method of eliminating aggressive-disruptive behavior
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). of retarded and brain damaged patients. Behaviour
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Research and Therapy, 10, 15–27.
Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Foxx, R. M., & Azrin, N. H. (1973). The elimination of
Network. (2014). Community report on autism: A autistic self-stimulatory behavior by overcorrection.
snapshot of autism spectrum disorder among 8-year-­ Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 1–14.
old children in multiple communities across the United Garcia, B., & Sarvis, M. A. (1964). Evaluation and
States in 2010. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ treatment planning for autistic children. Archives of
ncbddd/autism/states/comm_report_autism_2014.pdf General Psychiatry, 10, 530–541.
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). The cur- Gelfand, D. M., & Hartman, D. P. (1968). Behavior
rent dimensions of applied behavior analysis. Journal therapy with children: A review and evaluation of
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97. research methodology. Psychological Bulletin, 69,
Bettelheim, B. (1967). The empty fortress: Infantile autism 204–215.
and the birth of the self. New York, NY: Free Press. Hartung, J. R. (1970). A review of procedures to increase
Bettelheim, B. (1959). Feral children and autistic chil- verbal imitation skills and functional speech in autistic
dren. American Journal of Sociology, 64, 455–467. children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders,
Buss, A. H. (1961). Punishment, displacement, and con- 35, 203–217.
flict. In The psychology of aggression (pp.53–74). Hewett, F. M. (1965). Teaching speech to an autistic child
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. through operant conditioning. American Journal of
Cantwell, D. B., & Baker, L. (1984). Research concern- Orthopsychiatry, 35, 927–936.
ing families of children with autism. In E. Schopler Hundziak, M., Maurer, R. A., & Watson, L. S. (1965).
& G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), The effects of autism on the Operant conditioning in toilet training severely men-
family (pp. 43–68). New York, NY: Plenum Press. tally retarded boys. American Journal of Mental
Center for Disease Control and Prevention. (2015). Deficiency, 70, 120–124.
Screening and diagnosis. Retrieved from http://www. Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., &
cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/screening.html Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis
Clancy, H., & McBride, G. (1969). The autistic process of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis,
and its treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and 27, 197–209.
Psychiatry, 10, 233–244. Jensen, G. D., & Womack, M. G. (1967). Operant con-
Cooper, J. O., Herron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). ditioning techniques applied in the treatment of an
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle autistic child. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
River, NJ: Pearson Education Inc.. 37, 30–34.
Craighead, W. E., O’Leary, K. D., & Allen, J. S. (1973). Kanfer, F. H., & Matarazzo, J. D. (1959). Secondary and
Teaching and generalization of instruction—follow- generalized reinforcement in human learning. Journal
ing in an “autistic” child. Journal of Behavior Therapy of Experimental Psychology, 58, 400–404.
and Experimental Psychiatry, 4, 171–176. Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
DeMyer, M. K., Hingtgen, J. N., & Jackson, R. K. (1981). tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.
Infantile autism reviewed: A decade of research. Kanner, L. (1949). Problems of nosology and psychody-
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 7, 338–451. namics of early infantile autism. American Journal of
Deur, J. L., & Parke, R. D. (1970). Effects of inconsistent Orthopsychiatry, 19, 416–426.
punishment on aggression in children. Developmental Kazdin, A. (2008). Behavior modification in applied set-
Psychology, 2(3), 403. tings (6th ed.). Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press.
1  Historical Development of Treatment 15

Keehn, J. D., & Webster, C. D. (1969). Behavior therapy guidelines for autism spectrum disorders. Randolph,
and behavior modification. Canadian Psychologist/ MA: National Autism Center.
Psychologie Canadienne, 10, 68–73. New York State Department of Health, Early
Koegel, R. L., & Covert, A. (1972). The relationship Intervention Program. (1999). Clinical practice
of self-stimulation to learning in autistic children. guideline: Report of the recommendations, autism/
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5, 381–387. pervasive developmental disorders, assessment
Leichtman, M. (2008). The essence of residential treat- and interventions for young children. Albany,
ment: I. Core concepts. Residential Treatment for NY: New York State Department of Health, Early
Children and Youth, 24, 175–196. Intervention Program.
Lovaas, O. I. (1964). Cue properties of words: The control Rimland, B. (1964). Infantile autism: The syndrome
of operant responding by rate and content of verbal and its implications for a neural theory of behavior.
operants. Child Development, 35, 245–256. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Lovaas, O. I. (1970). Strengths and weaknesses of operant Risely, T. R. (1968). The effects and side effects of pun-
conditioning techniques for the treatment of autism. In ishing the autistic behaviors of a deviant child. Journal
Proceedings of the conference and annual meeting of of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 21–34.
the National Society for Autistic Children (pp.30–41). Roos, P., & Oliver, M. (1969). Evaluation of operant
Lovaas, I. O. (1979). Contrasting the illness and behav- conditioning with institutionalized retarded children.
ioral models for the treatment of autistic children: American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 74, 325–330.
A historical perspective. Journal of Autism and Rubin, E. Z., & Simson, C. B. (1960). A special class pro-
Developmental Disorders, 9, 315–323. gram for the emotionally disturbed child in school: A
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal proposal. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 30,
educational and intellectual functioning in young 144–153.
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Rutter, M. (1972). Childhood schizophrenia reconsidered.
Psychology, 55, 3–9. Journal of Autism and Childhood Schizophrenia, 2,
Lovaas, O. I., Baer, D. M., & Bijou, S. W. (1965). 315–337.
Experimental procedures for analyzing the interaction Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definitions of child-
of symbolic social stimuli and children’s behavior. hood autism. Journal of Autism and Childhood
Child Development, 36, 237–247. Schizophrenia, 8, 139–161.
Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Gold, V. J., & Kassorla, I. C. Schell, R. E., & Adams, W. P. (1968). Training parents of
(1965). Recording apparatus and procedure for a young child with profound behavior deficits to be
observation of behaviors of children in free play set- teacher-therapists. The Journal of Special Education,
tings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2, 2, 439–454.
108–120. Schopler, E. (1965). Early infantile autism and recep-
Lovaas, O. I., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. (1965). tor processes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 13,
Building social behavior in autistic children by use of 327–335.
electric shock. Journal of Experimental Research in Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
Personality, 1, 99–109. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. Speers, R. W., & Lansing, C. (1963). Group psycho-
(1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures therapy with preschool psychotic children and col-
on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of lateral group therapy of their parents: A preliminary
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 131–166. report of the first two years. American Journal of
Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., & Koegel, R. L. (1974). Orthopsychiatry, 34, 659.
A behavior modification approach to the treatment of Ward, T. F., & Hoddinott, B. A. (1965). A study of
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Childhood childhood schizophrenia and early infantile autism:
Schizophrenia, 4, 111–129. I. Description of the sample. The Canadian Psychiatric
Margolies, P. J. (1977). Behavioral approaches to the treat- Association Journal/La Revue De L’association Des
ment of early infantile autism: A review. Psychological Psychiatres Du Canada, 10, 377–382.
Bulletin, 84, 249–264. Wetzel, Baker, Roney, & Martin. (1966). Outpatient treat-
National Autism Center. (2009). National standards proj- ment of autistic behavior. Behavior Research and
ect: Addressing the need for evidence based practice Therapy, 4, 169–177.
Substantiated
and Unsubstantiated 2
Interventions for Individuals
with ASD

Tiffany Kodak and Regina A. Carroll

ferent grade levels, effectiveness ratings, extent


Identifying Substantiated of evidence, delivery method (e.g., small group),
Treatments program type, gender, race, and region. Selecting
choices from each area leads to a more detailed
There are several ways for researchers, practitio- description of relevant interventions.
ners, other professionals, students, and parents to Another website that offers information about
identify whether a treatment is considered evidence-based practices for individuals with
evidence-­based or substantiated. One avenue to autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is the National
search for substantiated interventions is through Professional Development Center on Autism
websites developed by organizations that apply Spectrum Disorder (NCPD) (http://autismpdc.
standards for evaluating the evidence for inter- fpg.unc.edu/). Selecting “Evidence-based
vention and disseminate information about sub- Practices” on the NCPD website leads to a page
stantiated interventions to the public. What that describes how evidence-based practice is
Works Clearinghouse (http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/ defined and a list of these practices. The list of
wwc/) is one website that educators, behavior evidence-based practices is updated on a yearly
analysts, psychologists, and interested parties basis. Interested parties can select an intervention
can use to search for the status of the evidence of to receive a report that (a) summarizes the inter-
an intervention. Under the “Find What Works!” vention, (b) provides step-by-step instructions
link on this website, it is possible to search for regarding how to implement the intervention, and
evidence on treatments under numerous topic/ (c) a list of references that demonstrate the evi-
outcome domains. For example, selecting the dence for the intervention.
topic “Children and Youth with Disabilities” pro- A second way for the public to receive infor-
vides access to information in multiple domains mation regarding substantiated interventions is
(e.g., reading fluency, external behavior), for dif- through the National Autism Center’s National
Standards Project (National Autism Center,
2009). Launched in 2005, the purpose of the
T. Kodak (*) National Standards Project is to identify substan-
Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin- tiated interventions for individuals with
Milwaukee, Milwaukee, WI, USA
e-mail: Kodak@uwm.edu ASD. This project has occurred in two phases.
Phase 1, which was completed in 2009, produced
R.A. Carroll
Department of Psychology, West Virginia University, a report on the status of the evidence for
Morgantown, WV, USA ­interventions from research published between

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 17


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_2
18 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

1959 and fall of 2007. Phase 2 of the project con- Hatton, 2010; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010;
sidered research on interventions for individuals Wong, et al., 2015). Additional reviews are also
with ASD published between 2007 and 2012. A available for interventions used with individuals
full report of the results of Phases 1 and 2 is avail- with intellectual and developmental disabilities
able to download upon free registration through (e.g., Lilienfeld, 2005). As expected, there is
the National Autism Center’s website (http:// overlap in the evidence-based practices identified
www.nationalautismcenter.org/reports/). The within reviews regardless of differences in popu-
reports also list interventions that are described lations. These reviews are particularly beneficial
as emerging or unestablished, based on the evi- for practitioners because the authors described
dence reviewed from published research within criteria for evidence-based practice, how they
the time period of the reports. These sections of identified articles for inclusion, and summarized
the reports may be particularly useful to families the interventions identified as evidence-based
or educators who are unfamiliar with published practices.
literature on interventions and seek to determine Meta-analyses of literature are similar to
whether an intervention has evidence to support reviews, except that the evidence for each study
its use with individuals with ASD. included in the meta-analysis is re-evaluated.
Review articles and meta-analyses are another Thus, the purpose of the meta-analysis is to eval-
way to evaluate the status of evidence for an uate the effectiveness of an intervention by com-
intervention. Review articles often summarize bining data from relevant studies. Data from all
the published literature in a topic area. For exam- studies in a topic area are collected, coded to
ple, Lerman and Vorndran (2002) reviewed the determine effect size, and statistical analyses are
status of basic and applied literature on punish- used to interpret the outcomes of studies that are
ment and suggested areas of additional research grouped together. The results of the meta-­analysis
on punishment. Although not every intervention are used to determine if an intervention has
may be the focus of a review paper, many review sufficient support to characterize the intervention
papers exist for evidence-based practices (e.g., as substantiated, based on the criteria developed
review of functional communication training by by the field in which the intervention is used.
Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek, 2008; review of extinc- For example, Virues-Ortega (2010) conducted a
tion by Lerman & Iwata, 1996; review functional meta-analysis of the literature on comprehensive
analysis by Beavers, Iwata, & Lerman,2013). applied behavior analytic (ABA) intervention for
Thus, educators, practitioners, and other profes- young children with autism. The meta-analysis
sionals can gain useful information about the included 22 studies with 323 participants in
current status of an intervention by reading a intervention groups. The results showed that
review of an area of literature rather than attempt- comprehensive ABA intervention produced
ing to find and read individual studies on a topic positive outcomes in multiple domains (e.g.,
to judge the current evidence for the intervention. language, adaptive behavior, and intellectual
However, review papers do not always include functioning) for children with ASD.
every study on the topic of the review nor do they The results of meta-analyses have been used
describe the quality of the studies included in a by insurance companies to determine the evi-
review. That is, some studies included in a review dence for an intervention to make determina-
might not use empirically sound methodology. tions regarding coverage of treatment for
Several review papers are dedicated to the members. Meta-analyses have also been consid-
identification of evidence-based practices to pro- ered by state and federal organizations to deter-
vide recommendations for practitioners regard- mine public policies. Thus, meta-analyses of
ing substantiated interventions for individuals interventions provide an important contribution
with ASD (e.g., Odom, Boyd, Hall, & Hume, to the literature, practice guidelines, and public
2010; Odom, Collet-Klingenberg, Rogers, & policy.
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 19

I mportance of Identifying Evidence physical or sexual abuse from a family member,


for Interventions and criminal charges were brought against family
members based on the claims. Well-designed
Due to the plethora of available treatments for studies conducted on Facilitated Communication
individuals with ASD, it can be challenging for showed that the facilitator was actually produc-
parents, educators, and professionals to deter- ing the message rather than the individual with
mine which interventions to use. The demand for ASD (e.g., Montee, Miltenberger, & Wittrock,
quick and easy interventions, in combination 1995; Wheeler, Jacobson, Paglieri, & Schwartz,
with the increased prevalence of ASD, has 1993). In addition, the Association for Behavior
resulted in the development of many “fad” or Analysis International (ABAI) issued a position
unsubstantiated interventions. Unsubstantiated statement on Facilitated Communication stating
interventions can be found in internet searches; that there is no direct benefit of this intervention
are recommended by well-meaning friends, fam- and it is a discredited technique (ABAI, 1995).
ily, and treatment team members; and may be Other unsubstantiated interventions have pro-
prevalent in some educational settings. duced physical harm and even death in individu-
Unsubstantiated interventions may be popular als with ASD. For example, chelation therapy is
choices because they report that impressive out- an intervention used to remove heavy metals
comes can occur quickly. Typically, unsubstanti- from the blood. Some parents who believe that
ated interventions rely on testimonials from other their child was exposed to mercury from vaccina-
people to provide evidence for their effective- tions have used this intervention. In 2005, a boy
ness. For example, quotes from families who from Pennsylvania who was diagnosed with
have used the intervention are included in the autism died in the physician’s office shortly after
intervention materials as evidence for the effec- receiving chelation therapy. Furthermore, the
tiveness of intervention. Nevertheless, there may National Institute of Mental Health reported that
be few or no empirically sound studies demon- investigators no longer planned to conduct a
strating the effectiveness of the intervention. funded study of chelation therapy for children
It may be the case that a family or treatment with autism due to little scientific merit and unac-
team would like to use an unsubstantiated inter- ceptable safety risks (Mitka, 2008).
vention, because they do not identify any con- There are other potential costs associated with
cerns with trying the intervention to determine use of unsubstantiated interventions. For exam-
whether it works for the individual with ple, if the intervention takes time away from the
ASD. However, certain unsubstantiated interven- individual’s schedule that could be spent using
tions could be harmful. For example, Facilitated substantiated interventions, the individual has
Communication is an unsubstantiated interven- lost precious treatment time. Many parents seek
tion that became widely used with individuals intervention for their child with ASD upon initial
with ASD in the early 1990s (Green & Shane, diagnosis to help the child gain skills that will
1994). The intervention claims to unlock the indi- bridge the gap between the child’s skills and
vidual’s potential to communicate with others by those of their peers. Thus, time allocated to
assisting the individual to type messages on a unsubstantiated interventions can prevent prog-
keyboard. The intervention purports to resolve ress from occurring.
communication difficulties caused by a motor Using unsubstantiated interventions also may
praxis problem and difficulty identifying the cor- have a long-term effect on behavior and delay
rect words to use during communication. progress once initiated. For example, a child with
Facilitators provided physical assistance to the ASD who has an extremely limited diet and
individual with ASD to type messages; the even- begins a gluten- and casein-free diet may be even
tual goal is to reduce and remove the assistance more resistant to eating novel foods once a sub-
of the facilitator. During the widespread use of stantiated intervention is initiated. In addition,
this intervention, some typed messages claimed the expense for unsubstantiated treatments is not
20 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

typically covered by medical insurance, and the (e.g., problem behavior), may occur at a lower
family usually must pay for the intervention out level of intensity (e.g., 10 h per week), and has
of pocket. Furthermore, families invest time and a shorter treatment duration (e.g., 6 months).
energy in implementing unsubstantiated inter- The review of substantiated interventions will be
ventions, and the disappointing outcomes may divided into these two areas and provide a discus-
discourage parents from seeking other substanti- sion of several evidence-based interventions
ated interventions for their child with within these two categories.
ASD. Therefore, reducing the use of unsubstanti-
ated treatment by disseminating information
about evidence-based practices to families, Comprehensive Treatment Models
practitioners, educators, and other professionals
is an important endeavor. Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention
(EIBI)  Based on the University of California at
Los Angeles Young Autism Project model
Review of Substantiated Treatments (UCLA YAP; Lovaas, 1981, 1987, 2003), EIBI is
the most widely researched and requested com-
Applied behavior analytic (ABA) interventions prehensive treatment model (Green et al., 2006).
are among the most effective interventions for EIBI is grounded in the principles of applied
individuals with ASD (Barbaresi, Kausic, & behavior analysis (ABA) and is an intensive
Voigt, 2006; Lilienfeld, 2005). The evidence for treatment (i.e., up to 40 h a week for 2 or more
ABA interventions has resulted in states mandat- years) that targets the core deficits of ASD (e.g.,
ing ABA services for children with ASD and communication and social deficits, restricted
insurance reform. In addition, many (a) profes- interests, social emotional reciprocity, and inflex-
sional organizations including the American ibility). Effective components of EIBI include (a)
Association on Intellectual and Developmental highly structured one-on-one teaching strategies
Disabilities, American Academy of Child and (i.e., discrete trial training; DTT), (b) an individ-
Adolescent Psychiatry, Association for Science ualized treatment approach focusing on each
in Autism Treatment, and Autism Speaks, among child’s current repertoires and deficits, (c) a func-
others; (b) federal agencies including the Centers tional approach to address challenging behavior
for Disease Control, the Surgeon General of the that interferes with learning, and (d) program-
United States, and the National Institute of ming for generalization and maintenance of
Mental Health, among others; and (c) state task skills. Typically EIBI is supervised by profes-
force committees including the New York State sionals trained in ABA, and there are a number of
Department of Health and Maine Administrators treatment manuals that have been created to
of Services for Children with Disabilities, among guide the sequence of skills targeted for interven-
others, recommend ABA interventions for indi- tion (e.g., Leaf & McEachin, 1999; Lovaas, 2003;
viduals with ASD. Maurice, Green, & Foxx, 2001).
ABA interventions can be categorized as In the first empirical evaluation of the UCLA
comprehensive or focused, and this distinction is YAP, Lovaas (1987) compared pretreatment and
based on the treatment goals of intervention. posttreatment IQ and educational placements for
Comprehensive intervention seeks to address a group of children with ASD, who received EIBI
multiple domains of functioning (i.e., language (40 h a week) for 2 or more years, to a control
skills, social skills, motor skills, adaptive func- group receiving a range of other special educa-
tioning, and cognitive skills) and is recommended tion services. The results showed that 47% of the
to occur at a high level of intensity (e.g., at least group receiving EIBI (n = 19) achieved post-
25 h per week) over a prolonged period of time treatment IQ scores in the normal range and were
(e.g., 3 years). In comparison, focused interven- educated in a general education classroom. These
tion typically seeks to address one targeted area results were compared to the 2% of children from
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 21

the control group (n = 40) who achieved the same routines); (b) teaching children with ASD along-
outcome. Furthermore, these treatment gains side their typically developing, same-age peers;
maintained for the EIBI group for up to 6 years. (c) teaching typically developing peers to facili-
That is, a follow-up evaluation showed that chil- tate the social and communicative behaviors of
dren in the EIBI group continued to have higher peers with ASD; (d) utilizing a range of evidence-­
IQ scores and less restrictive educational place- based focused interventions (e.g., incidental
ments when compared to the control group teaching, pivotal response training, picture-­
(McEachin, Smith, & Lovaas, 1993). exchange communications system); and (e) using
There has been much debate regarding the sig- a structured parent-skill training curriculum
nificance of the findings from the original studies (Strain & Bovey, 2011).
published by Lovaas and colleagues with poten- In the largest-scale study to date, Strain and
tial methodological limitations identified Bovey (2011) conducted a clustered randomized
(Gresham & MacMillan, 1998). However, a control trial of the LEAP model. Of the 56 inclu-
number of comprehensive studies have been pub- sive preschool classrooms that participated in this
lished since, addressing some of the limitations study, 28 classrooms were randomly assigned to
from the original studies and documenting the the full LEAP treatment condition (N = 117) in
effectiveness of EIBI implemented in school which the teachers received 2 years of ongoing
(Eikeseth, Klintwall, Jahr, & Karlsson, 2012; training and consultation on the implementation
Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, 2002), center-­ of LEAP. The other 28 classrooms were assigned
based (Cohen, Amerine-Dickens, & Smith, to a control condition (N = 117) in which the
2006), and home-based (Remington et al, 2007; teachers received the LEAP intervention manual
Sallows & Graunpner, 2005; Smith, Groen, and training presentations but did not receive
Wynn, 2000) settings. Reichow (2012) reviewed ongoing consultation or training. Outcomes of
five meta-analyses of EIBI conducted between this study showed that teachers in the treatment
2009 and 2010 (Eldevik et al., 2009; Makrygianni group achieved a high level of implementation
& Reed, 2010; Reichow & Wolery, 2009; fidelity at the end of 2 years, implementing an
Spreckley & Boyd, 2009; Virués-Ortega, 2010). average of 87% of the treatment components. In
Despite the many methodological differences comparison, classrooms in the control condition
between the meta-analyses, four of the five meta-­ were only implementing an average of 38% of
analyses concluded that EIBI can be an effective the treatment components at the end of 2 years.
intervention, capable of producing gains in IQ Strain and Bovey also found that children in the
and adaptive behavior for many children with treatment group made significantly more gains
ASD. At this time, EIBI is the comprehensive on measures of cognitive, language, autism
treatment model with the most empirical support; symptoms, problem behavior, and social skills
thus, this treatment should be strongly consid- following 2 years of LEAP, as compared to
ered when making treatment decisions for children in the control group.
children with ASD.
Early Start Denver Model (ESDM)  This
Learning Experiences and Alternative model is a comprehensive developmental behav-
Program for Preschoolers and Their Parents ioral intervention for young children with
(LEAP)  The LEAP model uses an inclusive ASD. The ESDM developed out of the Denver
educational approach for teaching preschoolers model (Rogers, Hall, Osaki, Reaven, Herbison,
with ASD (Hoyson, Jamieson, & Strain, 1984). 2000), and it is the only early intervention model
The theoretical and conceptual foundations of that has been validated with a randomized con-
LEAP are largely based on ABA, and the primary trol trial for children with ASD as young as
components of LEAP include (a) using naturalis- 18 months (Dawson et al., 2010). The ESDM
tic teaching strategies (e.g., embedding learning integrates empirically supported ABA techniques
experiences into naturally occurring classroom with relationship-­ based, developmental, and
22 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

p­lay-­based approaches. Training is individual- Focused Intervention


ized for each child, and specific learning objec-
tives are based on the Early Start Denver Model Focused interventions typically target one domain.
Curriculum Checklist, a play-based assessment For example, numerous focused interventions
tool that outlines skills across different develop- effectively treat severe problem behavior.
mental domains, which are sequenced in the Because of the range of ABA-based focused
order they occur during typical development. The interventions that are available to children with
main components of the ESDM include (a) a ASD, a review of each of these is outside of the
well-defined developmental curriculum (Rogers scope of this chapter. We will divide focused
& Dawson, 2010), (b) established teaching strate- interventions into two areas; one area will
gies from ABA (e.g., prompting, prompt fading, describe focused interventions for behavioral
chaining), (c) techniques from pivotal response supports, and the other area will describe focused
training (e.g., following the child’s lead, turn tak- interventions to promote skill acquisition.
ing), (d) activities that promote positive emo-
tional exchanges between children and key adults Behavioral Supports  A proportion of individuals
through play routines (e.g., positive affect and diagnosed with ASD engage in one or more
empathetic response toward the child), and (e) a topographies of problem behavior such as aggres-
focus on parent and family involvement (e.g., sion, disruption, and self-injurious behavior.
parents are taught to incorporate the ESDM strat- Although problem behavior is not part of the
egies throughout the child’s waking hours). diagnostic criteria for ASD, it is a comorbid
At present, there has been one randomized symptom frequently reported by families and
control trial conducted to evaluate the efficacy of educators. The field of ABA has a successful his-
the ESDM (Dawson et al., 2010). Dawson and tory of assessing and treating problem behavior
colleagues randomly assigned 48 children with in individuals with ASD. Unique to the behav-
ASD between 18 and 30 months of age to one of ioral perspective, interventions are based on the
two groups. One group received 2 years of inter- identified function of the individual’s problem
vention using the ESDM, and the other group behavior. That is, rather than treating all aggres-
received 2 years of intervention using interven- sive behavior with a specific intervention, behav-
tions commonly available in the community. ior analysts identify the cause of behavior and
Following 2 years of intervention, the ESDM develop a treatment that is tailored to address the
group showed significant improvements in IQ, cause of behavior. The process of assessing an
adaptive behavior, and autism diagnosis. Children individual’s problem behavior is referred to as a
in the ESDM group showed an increase of an functional behavior assessment.
average of 17 IQ points compared to an average
of seven points in the community intervention Functional Behavior Assessment (FBA)  FBA is
group. Additionally, only 56% of the children used to ascertain the environmental variables
from the ESDM group retained their diagnosis of influencing the occurrence of an individual’s
autistic disorder following 2 years of interven- problem behavior. Due to extensive evidence that
tion, as compared to 71% of children in the com- function-based interventions are more effective
munity intervention group. The original than randomly selected interventions (e.g., Iwata,
randomized control trial on the ESDM was con- Page, Cowdery, & Miltenberger, 1994), the re-­
ducted in a university clinic setting, and more authorization of IDEA in 2004 included a man-
recent studies suggest that this model is also date for FBA in order to collect and analyze data
effective when implemented in community-based about a student’s problem behavior in school
group settings (Eapen et al., 2013; Vivanti et al., ­settings (Individuals with Disabilities Education
2014). Although the evidence available to support Act, 2004).
the use of the ESDM is promising, few compre- FBA is comprised of three types of assess-
hensive studies of this model have been con- ment. The first type of assessment is indirect, and
ducted; thus, additional research is needed. information regarding the individual’s behavior
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 23

is obtained from people who frequently observe from task demands is contingent on problem
the occurrence of problem behavior. Indirect data behavior), and alone/ignore (problem behavior is
are collected through interviews with teachers ignored). A control condition also is included in
and caregivers, questionnaires, ratings scales, which motivation to engage in problem behavior
and other paper assessment methods. Indirect is reduced by removing all demands, having an
assessments do not include observations of the adult provide frequent attention, preferred toys or
individual’s behavior. Indirect assessments alone activities are available, and no consequences are
may not be sufficient to identify the cause of an provided for problem behavior. The conditions
individual’s problem behavior (Smith, Smith, alternate such that participants experience each
Dracobly, & Pace, 2012). Nevertheless, these condition multiple times, and data collectors
data can be used to assist in collecting data record the occurrence of the target problem
through other assessment methods. behavior in each session. The rate of problem
Descriptive assessments are a second type of behavior in test conditions is compared to the rate
FBA which includes direct observation of the of problem behavior in the control condition.
individual’s behavior. Observations of the indi- Any test conditions with elevated and differenti-
vidual in his or her natural setting, collection of ated rates of problem behavior, in comparison to
information about events that occur before and the control condition, are identified as potential
after behavior, and collection of data regarding variables maintaining problem behavior. For
the occurrence of problem behavior occur dur- example, if an individual had elevated rates of
ing the descriptive assessment (Miltenberger, problem behavior in the attention condition in
2013). The information collected within a comparison to the control condition, the outcome
descriptive assessment is analyzed to determine of the assessment suggests that problem behavior
patterns of environmental events that are tem- is maintained by positive reinforcement in the
porally related to problem behavior. However, form of adult attention.
the cause of an individual’s problem behavior is Variations of functional analysis procedures
not identified based on a descriptive assess- were developed to identify other potential func-
ment. Rather, the data provide information tions of problem behavior (e.g., problem behav-
about events that are correlated with behavior; ior maintained by access to tangible items,
one may also identify the probability of certain Northup et al., 1991), decrease the time to con-
events coinciding with the occurrence of prob- duct the FA (e.g., Derby et al., 1992), and increase
lem behavior. the feasibility of FAs conducted in a school set-
The third type of FBA is an experimental anal- ting (e.g., trial-based FAs; Bloom, Iwata, Fritz,
ysis of behavior, or functional analysis, in which Roscoe, & Carreau, 2011).
careful arrangement and experimental manipula- There is an impressive literature base on the
tion of events surrounding behavior lead to con- effectiveness of FA. Two literature reviews on FA
clusions regarding the cause of behavior (e.g., the collectively analyzed 435 peer-reviewed articles
“triggers” or antecedent for behavior and conse- that included a FA of problem behavior (Beavers,
quences that occur following behavior and rein- Iwata, & Lerman, 2013; Hanley, Iwata, &
force problem behavior). Functional analysis was McCord, 2003). In addition, articles that included
described by Skinner (1953, 1957) and others; a large number of participants for whom FAs
however, a methodology for conducting a func- were conducted in various settings provide evi-
tional analysis was developed by Iwata and col- dence that FAs produce conclusive outcomes that
leagues and published in 1982 (Iwata, Dorsey, can be used to develop treatment (Hagopian,
Slifer, Bauman, and Richman, 1982). This func- Rooker, Jessel, & DeLeon, 2013; Iwata et al., 1994;
tional analysis (FA) includes test conditions in Mueller, Nkomi, & Hine, 2011). For example,
which one antecedent and one consequence are Mueller, Nkomi, and Hine (2011) found that FAs
manipulated per condition. Test conditions conducted with 69 participants in their school
include attention (adult attention is provided con- setting produced conclusive outcomes in over
tingent on problem behavior), demand (a break 90% of these cases.
24 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

The collection of research on FBA imple- extinction should be implemented following a


mented with a large number of individuals, the conclusive FBA.
success of these procedures in identifying the Lerman and Iwata (1996) provided a detailed
function(s) of an individual’s problem behavior, analysis of the use of extinction in basic and
and the rigorous experimental control in these applied research. The authors described factors
published studies are why FBA is a substantiated that may influence resistance to extinction such
focused intervention procedure. as the schedule of reinforcement for problem
behavior, the magnitude of reinforcement for
Extinction  Extinction is a highly effective inter- problem behavior, and the effort required to
vention in which behavior that was previously engage in a response. Practitioners considering
reinforced no longer results in reinforcement. the use of extinction as a substantiated interven-
Removal of the reinforcer for behavior leads to tion should read this article to consider how best
the reduction and eventual cessation of behavior. to arrange extinction. In addition, the use of
For example, if adult attention in the form of rep- extinction may lead to a temporary increase in
rimands is the reinforcer that occurs every time a the occurrence of behavior (known as an extinc-
child swears in the classroom, extinction would tion burst). Although extinction bursts may only
involve removing adult attention following occur in 24% of cases (Lerman & Iwata, 1995),
swearing. Thus, every time the child swears, the treatment teams might consider how best to
classroom teacher would ignore the behavior arrange the environment during extinction and
rather than commenting on swearing. In this respond appropriately to problem behavior
example, swearing would decrease over time and should an extinction burst occur.
eventually cease. Although extinction is a substantiated inter-
Extinction is only effective if the reinforcer vention, it is frequently combined with other
for problem behavior is no longer available fol- interventions. The combination of extinction and
lowing behavior. Thus, it is important to identify other interventions may make the use of extinc-
the cause of behavior to determine the tion more effective in rapidly reducing behavior
reinforcer(s) that must be removed following and decrease resistance to extinction (Lerman,
behavior. Iwata and colleagues (1994) demon- Iwata, & Wallace, 1999; Moss, Ruthven, Hawkins
strated the importance of using function-based & Topping, 1983; Rivas, Piazza, Patel, &
extinction (i.e., extinction matching the rein- Bachmeyer, 2010), and the combination of inter-
forcer for behavior) by comparing extinction ventions may be more socially acceptable. For
that matched the function of behavior to at least example, arranging reinforcement for an alterna-
one other type of extinction that did not match tive behavior (e.g., doing classwork) while plac-
the function of behavior. For example, one indi- ing problem behavior (e.g., swearing) on
vidual’s self-injurious behavior was reinforced extinction can produce a shift in response alloca-
by the sensory consequences produced by the tion from inappropriate behavior to appropriate
behavior. Function-based extinction involved fit- behavior (e.g., Vollmer, Roane, Ringdahl, &
ting the individual with a helmet that attenuated Marcus, 1999).
the sensory stimulation produced by self-injuri-
ous head banging. The participant also received Differential Reinforcement  This substantiated
attention extinction (i.e., no attention was provided behavior-change intervention arranges reinforce-
following self-injury) and escape extinction (i.e., ment for one behavior (e.g., an appropriate
no break from demands occurred following self-­ ­alternative behavior) and extinction for problem
injury) without the helmet present. The results behavior. For example, a child who screams to
showed that only sensory extinction reduced get candy in line at the grocery store will receive
self-­injury. The other two types of extinction candy following a polite request (e.g., “May I
that did not match the function of behavior did not have candy, please?”) and will no longer receive
lead to a reduction in self-injury. Therefore, candy for screaming.
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 25

There are several applications of differential Although DRO is an effective intervention to


reinforcement for the treatment of problem treat problem behavior, there are potential disad-
behavior. These applications include (1) differen- vantages to this intervention. First, someone must
tial reinforcement of other behavior (DRO), (2) be available to consistently monitor behavior
differential reinforcement of incompatible behav- during the DRO interval. Tiger et al. (2009)
ior (DRI), (3) differential reinforcement of alter- taught a man with Asperger’s syndrome to self-­
native behavior (DRA), (4) differential monitor behavior during DRO and deliver his
reinforcement of low-rate behavior (DRL), and own reinforcement following therapist-­
(5) differential reinforcement of high-rate behavior implemented DRO. This study provides a fruitful
(DRH). There is considerably more research on avenue of additional research to determine how
DRO, DRA, and DRI than DRL and DRH. Thus, to increase the utility of DRO in settings with
this chapter will focus in the discussion of dif- limited supervision. Second, DRO does not teach
ferential reinforcement procedures on those an individual appropriate behavior to recruit rein-
applications with the most supporting research. forcement. Although some individuals may
DRO involves reinforcing the omission of engage in appropriate behavior during DRO
problem behavior for an established interval of intervals (e.g., compliance with tasks; Kodak
time (Kodak, Miltenberger, Romaniuk, 2003; et al., 2003), it is possible to obtain reinforcement
Tiger, Fisher, Bouxsein, 2009; Vollmer, Iwata, for simply sitting and not engaging in any appro-
Zarcone, Smith, & Mazaleski, 1993). For exam- priate behavior, as long as targeted problem
ple, a boy with ASD may receive a break from his behavior does not occur. Other interventions,
homework if he does not throw his pencil or such as DRA, may be preferable to use if indi-
crumple his paper for 1 min. The interval for the viduals do not engage in appropriate behavior
omission of problem behavior is established during DRO intervals. Finally, some individuals
based on pre-intervention observations of the rate may not access reinforcement frequently if DRO
of problem behavior, and it should be slightly intervals continuously reset. Although this may
shorter than the average length of time between be avoided by carefully arranging DRO intervals,
occurrences of behavior. For example, if a child an individual could intermittently engage in
hits approximately every 2 min, the DRO interval problem behavior and fail to receive reinforce-
may begin at 1 min 30 s. If problem behavior ment for long periods of time, which could
occurs during the DRO interval, the time interval decrease the effectiveness of this intervention.
may be reset. For example, if a child hits at sec- DRA involves the reinforcement of an alterna-
ond 45 in the 1 min 30 s interval, the interval tive behavior and extinction for problem behavior,
resets, and the child must omit problem behavior and it is one of the most common applications of
for another 1 min 30 s to obtain reinforcement. differential reinforcement used by professionals
The DRO interval is gradually increased based on to treat problem behavior. An alternative behavior
reductions in problem behavior. Practitioners is selected either because it is already in the indi-
who use DRO should establish criteria for vidual’s repertoire (Grow, Kelley, Roane, &
increasing and decreasing DRO intervals based Shillingsburg, 2008) or the behavior can be taught
on behavior. For example, a practitioner might to the individual and is likely to reliably produce
decide to increase the DRO interval from 1 min the reinforcer (Schlichenmeyer, Dube, & Vargas-
30 s to 2 min if there is at least an 85% reduction Irwin, 2015). Prompts are used to occasion alter-
in problem behavior across two consecutive native behavior, and prompts are g­ radually faded
intervention sessions. In addition, the practitioner as the individual consistently and independently
might decide to decrease the interval (or return to engages in the appropriate behavior.
the prior DRO interval) if problem behavior During the initial stages of DRA implementa-
increases by more than 50% for three consecutive tion, every instance of the alternative behavior is
sessions. reinforced (i.e., a fixed-ratio 1 [FR 1] schedule of
26 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

reinforcement for appropriate behavior). As tioners. This review included recommendations


treatment progresses, the practitioner gradually based on 21 published studies on FCT conducted
thins the schedule of reinforcement for appropri- with 204 participants. The practice guidelines
ate behavior while maintaining low levels of noted the potential influence of the speed and
problem behavior. For example, Kodak, Lerman, effort of the communicative response on the
Volkert, and Trosclair (2007) provided a choice occurrence of alternative and problem behavior,
between a break and an edible item to children described how and in what settings the communi-
with ASD following increasing intervals of work cative response should be taught, how to arrange
completion. Treatment started with the comple- consequences for problem behavior, and how to
tion of one task (FR-1 schedule) and gradually thin reinforcement for communicative responses
increased to an FR 20 or FR 40 schedule of task to make FCT feasible for long-term use across
completion to obtain the choice of reinforcers. settings. The summary of the literature on FCT
After DRA is thinned to a relatively lean sched- and the thoughtful recommendations within this
ule of reinforcement, it may be feasible for prac- review paper provide an excellent resource for
titioners to maintain low levels of problem professionals interested in using FCT in
behavior for extended periods of time. practice.
DRI is another application of differential rein-
forcement in which incompatible behavior pro-
duces reinforcement rather than problem Skill Acquisition
behavior. Incompatible behavior is typically an
appropriate behavior that cannot occur at the There are numerous focused interventions that
same time as problem behavior. For example, if target specific skill deficits. Children with ASD
the individual engages in self-injurious hand bit- may require intervention to teach language skills,
ing, an incompatible behavior might involve enhance cognitive skills, improve adaptive func-
placing both hands underneath the legs in a seated tioning, develop motor skills, and teach social
position. DRI is considered a specific type of skills. To address the myriad of potential skill
DRA; both differential reinforcement procedures deficits, several interventions that promote skill
arrange reinforcement for appropriate behavior, acquisition received extensive support in the lit-
but DRI involves reinforcing only appropriate erature. Although the list of focused interven-
behavior that is incompatible with problem tions for skill acquisition is lengthy, this chapter
behavior. will describe several substantiated interventions
that are commonly used in practice.
Functional Communication Training  Often con-
ceptualized as a type of DRA, functional com- Discrete Trial Training (DTT)  Training includes
munication training (FCT) involves teaching a highly structured, fast-paced format of instruc-
individuals to engage in an alternative communi- tion in which one adult works directly with one
cative response to obtain reinforcers. The com- child in an environment with minimal distrac-
municative response may occur in many formats tions. Skills are typically broken down into
including a vocalization, picture exchange, sign smaller steps, and each step is repeatedly prac-
language, and card touch, among others. The ticed in trials until mastery is reached. Trials are
alternative response that is taught to the individ- typically delivered during tabletop instruction
ual produces the same reinforcer that maintains and include the arrangement of specific
the individual’s problem behavior. Thus, similar antecedents (e.g., prompts) and consequences
­
to other focused behavioral interventions (e.g., praise and tangible reinforcers). DTT is a
described above, FCT typically occurs following core component of the EIBI comprehensive inter-
a conclusive FBA. vention described earlier in this chapter.
Tiger et al. (2008) published a review on FCT DTT can be used to teach a variety of skills
that also included practice guidelines for practi- including gross motor imitation, labels of common
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 27

objects, receptive identification, matching objects friends to allow for practice requesting the toy
or pictures, vocal imitation, sight words, and play from others.
skills, among others (Leaf & McEachin, 1999; PRT occurs based on the child’s interest in an
Lovaas, 1981). Instructors carefully collect data item or activity. That is, training opportunities are
on child responding during each trial to deter- initiated by the child rather than by an adult. The
mine mastery. Because DTT is highly structured adult follows the child’s lead and identifies learn-
and systematic, inexperienced staff (e.g., ing opportunities based on what the child
Severtson & Carr, 2012; Thompson et al., 2012), approaches and the activities that the child initi-
parents (e.g., Young, Boris, Thompson, Martin, ates. By using child-driven instruction, motiva-
& Yu, 2012), peers (Radley, Dart, Furlow, & tion to learn may be higher than when the adult
Ness, 2015), and adults diagnosed with ASD initiates instruction with an item or object that the
(Lerman, Hawkins, Hillman, Shireman, & child didn’t select or approach (Dufek &
Nissen, 2015) have been trained to implement Schreibman, 2014).
DTT with integrity. PRT can be used to teach individuals with
Due to the highly structured nature of DTT, ASD a variety of skills including requests for
this intervention should be used in combination items, social initiations, imitation, and play
with other substantiated focused interventions to skill, among others. Koegel, Carter, and Koegel
promote generalization of skills across settings (2003) used PRT to target language develop-
and adults and to arrange intervention that also ment with two children with autism. Children
occurs in a less structured setting. were taught to ask “What happened?” when the
adult manipulated items in a pop-up book fea-
Pivotal Response Training (PRT)  This training turing preferred topics. The authors also mea-
targets pivotal behaviors considered to be impor- sured use of regular past tense verbs during
tant behaviors upon which acquisition of other, PRT. Participants rapidly learned to indepen-
untrained skills will occur. For example, lan- dently ask what happened during play, and they
guage is a pivotal behavior because once an indi- also acquired targeted verbs. In addition, train-
vidual can communicate with others, novel and ing generalized to gains in other linguistic
untrained skills may emerge such as play and behaviors such as the number and diversity of
social behavior. PRT is considered a type of natu- verbs emitted by each participant.
ralistic environmental training because learning The National Professional Development
opportunities occur in the individual’s natural Center on Autism Spectrum Disorders catego-
environment during play and everyday rized PRT as an evidence-based intervention
interactions. based on nine studies with single-subject designs
PRT capitalizes on the child’s motivation dur- that showed positive outcomes for individuals
ing instruction. For example, if a child indicates with ASD between ages 2 and 16 (Vismara &
an interest in going into the backyard to play, the Bogin, 2009).
parent takes the opportunity to have the child
practice asking to “go outside.” Multiple cues are Picture Exchange Communication System
used during instructional opportunities so that (PECS)  This alternative communication inter-
skills are practiced in the presence of a variety of vention system is designed for individuals with
relevant antecedents. For example, a child may no or limited vocal verbal behavior. The PECS
ask for a toy that her mother is playing with dur- teaching protocol is based on Skinner’s taxon-
ing free playtime in the home, and her mother omy in Verbal Behavior (1957) and teaches
gives her the toy that she requested. In other ­communicative responses in a specific order to
learning opportunities, the mother may offer a facilitate the development of multiple functions
choice of toys, and the girl can practice asking for of verbal behavior (e.g., mands/requests for items
the toy under this choice arrangement. The same or activities, tacts/labeling items or events in the
toy is present during play with siblings and environment).
28 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

PECS includes six phases of training. In Phase of PECS training on other behavior (e.g., problem
I, the individual learns to obtain and hand a behavior, vocalizations; Hart & Banda, 2010;
picture to a communicative partner to access a Preston & Carter, 2009; Tincani & Devis, 2011).
highly preferred item. Only one picture is present For example, Preston and Carter (2009) reviewed
during training. In Phase II, individuals are 27 studies on PECS. The authors concluded that
trained to be more persistent with exchanging the studies provide evidence for the effectiveness
pictures, and the skills developed in Phase I are of PECS to teach nearly all participants some
practiced across communicative partners and set- form of functional communication (e.g., mands).
tings to generalize the skill across people and In addition, Hart and Banda (2010) performed a
locations. In Phase III, two or more pictures are meta-analysis of the literature on PECS. Their
present in a PECS binder, and the individual must meta-analysis included 13 published studies, and
remove a picture to request an item. In the begin- the results showed that all but one participant
ning of this phase, a picture of a non-preferred acquired functional communication with PECS.
item may be placed in the binder along with a Although practitioners and educators may
picture that was previously trained in earlier endorse frequent use of PECS with individuals, it
phases. After the individual learns the discrimina- is unclear whether the picture-exchange program
tion among pictures of preferred and non-preferred that is used aligns with the phases and specific
items, pictures of other preferred items are added procedures described in the PECS manual (Odom,
to the binder. Phase IV involves teaching the stu- Collet-Klingberg, et al. 2010). If practitioners are
dent to request items using a short sentence. A using a picture-exchange program that does not
picture for “I want” is added to the binder, and align with the PECS manual, a distinction should
the individual must place the “I want” picture and be made between implementing PECS versus a
another picture of a preferred item on a sentence generic picture-exchange program. The latter may
strip and hand the sentence strip to the communi- not have the same effectiveness as the PECS pro-
cative partner. This phase also includes pictures gram since the empirical evidence reported in this
for adjectives (e.g., big, bouncy) and prepositions chapter, and used to determine the status of PECS
(e.g., under, in) that are taught and included in the as a substantiated treatment, has only evaluated
short sentences that the individual places on the the PECS training package and not deviations
sentence strip. In Phase V, an adult asks the stu- from these procedures.
dent, “What do you want?” The individual learns
to make a short sentence requesting an item fol- Prompts  Prompts are used during instruction to
lowing this verbal prompt, because this type of rapidly teach individuals with ASD novel skills.
question is frequently asked by caregivers and Prompts are provided in order to ensure that an
educators working with individuals with ASD individual engages in the correct response under
and related disorders. In the last phase, another the correct stimulus conditions. There are two
function of verbal behavior (i.e., tact/labeling) is categories of prompts that can be used during
taught. The individual learns to describe stimuli instruction; they include response prompts and
that she/he hears, sees, smells, and feels. New stimulus prompts.
pictures such as “I see” and “I hear” are added to Response prompts evoke the targeted response
the binder to teach the individual to create a sen- in the presence of the correct antecedents. For
tence that is relevant to the environmental event example, if the child is shown a letter in the
being described by the individual (Frost & alphabet, a response prompt is provided so that
Bondy, 2002). the child says the correct letter name in the pres-
There are more than 100 published studies on ence of the letter. There are several commonly
PECS and several review papers describing the used response prompts in practice including ver-
efficacy of PECS in teaching communicative bal, model, gestural, and physical prompts. A
behavior (Sulzer-Azaroff, Hoffman, Horton, verbal prompt involves telling the person how to
Bondy, & Frost, 2009; Tien, 2008) and the effects engage in the correct response. For example, a
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 29

child with dirty hands who stands in front of a individual performs the correct behavior inde-
sink in the restroom for an extended period of pendently and in the correct situation. Thus,
time may be given the verbal prompt, “Wash your prompt-fading strategies are included during
hands,” by an adult. A model prompt involves instruction to transfer control over the correct
demonstrating how to perform the correct response from the prompt to the relevant stimulus
response while the individual watches the dem- conditions of the task. Prompt fading involves
onstration and has the chance to perform the gradually removing the prompt across repeated
behavior thereafter. For example, an adult might learning opportunities. Common prompt-fading
demonstrate how to wash hands in the sink, while procedures include least-to-most (Cronin &
the child watches. Then, the child might imitate Cuvo, 1979), most-to-­least (Striefel & Wetherby,
the adult by washing his hands in a manner that is 1973), and prompt delay (Touchette, 1971).
identical to the adult’s demonstration. A gestural During least-to-most prompt fading, an
prompt may involve gesturing toward materials instructional opportunity begins with the least
required for a correct response to occur. For intrusive prompt (e.g., a verbal prompt). If the
example, an adult might gesture toward the paper individual does not complete the task within a
towels in a public bathroom if a child has just specified time period (e.g., 5 s), the instructor
washed his hands and is standing with wet hands provides a more intrusive prompt, such as a
by his side for a period of time. Finally, a physi- model prompt. If the individual still does not
cal prompt may be used to physically assist the complete the task correctly within the specific
child to engage in the correct response. For time period, the most intrusive prompt is pro-
example, an adult might provide hand-over-hand vided (e.g., a physical prompt). Prompts are
guidance to the child by placing her hands over faded within this procedure when the individual
top of the child’s hands and guiding the child to correctly completes the task upon presentation of
obtain a paper towel from the dispenser and dry the demand. That is, prompts are faded from
off the fronts and backs of his hands. instruction because the individual responds to the
The second category of prompts is stimulus task prior to the programmed prompt.
prompts. A stimulus prompt involves adding or Most-to-least prompting can include the same
removing stimuli or changing some aspect of a prompts as least-to-most prompting, except that
stimulus so that a correct response is likely to instruction begins with the most intrusive prompt.
occur. Two types of stimulus prompts are within-­ The individual receives physical guidance for
stimulus and extra-stimulus prompts. A within-­ several trials before the prompt is faded to a
stimulus prompt occurs when the stimulus is model prompt. If the individual responds cor-
altered. Alterations to the stimulus can include, rectly at the less intrusive prompting level, the
but are not limited to, manipulating the size, prompt is faded to a verbal prompt. If, at any
intensity, or color of the stimulus. For example, point, the individual is not responding at a lower
an adult might modify the letter “A” to be bold level of prompting (e.g., errors to verbal prompts),
100-point font when teaching a child to point to the subsequent trials would be conducted with a
the letter “A” in an array of two other letters pre- more intrusive prompt (e.g., model prompt).
sented in 30-point font. An extra-stimulus prompt Prompt delay (also referred to as time delay;
involves adding a stimulus to occasion a correct Touchette, 1971) transfers stimulus control from
response. For example, a lawyer may place arrow a prompt to the relevant stimuli for the task by
stickers on a legal document to point out the loca- increasing the amount of time between the
tions in the document that the adult must place a initiation of the task and a prompt. Initially,
­
signature. prompts are provided immediately with the onset
Although prompts are used during instruction of the task (referred to as a 0-s delay). Thereafter,
to occasion a correct response from an individual the amount of time between the onset of the
under the correct stimulus conditions, the goal task and the prompt either gradually increases
of instruction is to fade prompts so that the (e.g., 1 s, 2 s, and so on; referred to as a progressive
30 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

prompt delay) or increases to the terminal delay Unsubstantiated Treatments


for instruction (e.g., 5 s; referred to as a constant for ASD
prompt delay). The prompt delay can also decrease
to a previous delay value (e.g., reduce from a 3-s Gluten-Free and Casein-Free Diet
delay to a 2-s delay in the next trial) if the individual
engages in errors during instruction. The goal of Dietary interventions are frequently used by fami-
the prompt delay is to remove all prompts while lies of children with ASD (Green et al., 2006;
maintaining high levels of correct responding. Owen-Smith et al., 2015; Perrin et al., 2012). One
Seaver and Bourret (2014) compared the effi- of the most commonly used dietary interventions
cacy and efficiency of prompts and prompt-­ is the gluten-free and casein-free (GFCF) diet.
fading strategies for ten individuals with This diet eliminates all food and beverages con-
ASD. First, the authors compared prompt types taining gluten, a protein found in wheat, barley,
to determine the specific prompt that was most rye, and oats (e.g., flours, bread, pasta, pastries),
efficient for teaching a skill. The results showed and casein, a protein found in all dairy products
that the most efficient prompt varied across par- (e.g., milk, yogurt, cheese, butter). The use of a
ticipants, verifying the necessity of an assess- GFCF diet is based on the etiological theory that
ment to determine ideal prompting strategies for the psychological and physiological symptoms of
each individual receiving services. Next, the ASD can be linked to overactivity of the opioid
authors compared least-to-most, prompt delay, system (opioid-excess theory; Shattock, Kennedy,
and most-to-least to identify the prompt-fading Rowell, & Berney, 1990). It has been hypothesized
strategy that was most efficient for participants. that children with ASD do not properly digest glu-
Seven participants with ASD participated in this ten and casein, which causes high levels of opioid
comparison. Although the most efficient peptides. These excessive opioid peptides then
prompt-­ fading strategy varied across partici- leak out of the intestines (i.e., the “leaky gut
pants and was either prompt delay or least-to- hypothesis”; Whiteley, Rodgers, Savery, &
most, the results of the least efficient strategy Shattock, 1999), cross the blood-brain barrier, and
were consistent. The most-to-least prompt-fad- attach to the opioid receptors (Mulloy et al., 2010).
ing strategy was the least efficient strategy for The overactivity of the opioid receptors is believed
all seven participants. to cause behavior problems and other related
Other studies comparing types of prompts and symptoms of ASD.
prompt-fading strategies also found idiosyncratic There have been relatively few studies on the
results; the most efficient strategy varies across impact of the GFCF diet on the symptoms of
participants (Lerman, Vorndran, Addison, & ASD, and even fewer studies have been con-
Kuhn, 2004; Libby, Weis, Bancroft, & Ahearn, ducted with the level of experimental rigor neces-
2008; Walls, Ellis, Zane, and VanderPoel, 1979). sary to support the use of a GFCF diet
Therefore, practitioners working with individuals (Mari-Bauset, Zazpe, Mari-Sanchis, Llopis-­
with ASD can use different types of prompts and Gonzalez, & Morales-Suarez-Varela, 2014;
prompt-fading strategies to teach skills. However, Mulloy et al., 2010). For example, in a systematic
it may be beneficial to conduct an assessment to review of the literature on GFCF diets, Mulloy
compare prompt types and prompt-fading strate- and collogues reviewed 14 studies evaluating the
gies with each individual and across tasks to efficacy of the GFCF diets. Only three of the 14
determine the teaching strategies that will be the studies used controlled experimental designs,
most efficient for each individual. Practitioners none of which supported the use of a GFCF diet
who are interested in conducting these types of (Bird et al., 1977; Elder et al., 2006; Irvin, 2006).
assessments can use the method described by Finally, in one of the best controlled studies to
Seaver and Bourret (2014) and Lerman et al. date, Hyman and collogues (2016) examined
(2004) as models for how to design these the safety and efficacy of the GFCF diet in a
assessments. double-­blind, placebo-controlled study with 14
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 31

young children with ASD. These authors did Activities used during therapy are individualized,
not find evidence of any effect of the GFCF based on each child’s unique sensory profile
diet on measures of physiological functioning, (Dunn, 1999), but typically include wearing
behavioral disturbance (e.g., overactivity), or weighted vests or blankets, massage therapy,
ASD-related behavior. brushing, swinging, therapy balls, and related
In addition to the current lack of evidence sup- activities. Many studies have assessed the efficacy
porting the GFCF diet, there are a number of of SIT for ASD. For example, Lang et al. (2012)
potential limitations and health risks associated reviewed 25 studies using SIT with children with
with this diet. The GFCF diet can be both time ASD. Only 3 of the 25 studies reviewed reported
and resource intensive. Families utilizing this positive effects with SIT, 8 studies reported mixed
intervention must commit to daily meal planning findings, and 14 studies reported no benefits of
and preparation while taking special care to SIT. The three studies that suggested SIT was
ensure their children are still meeting their nutri- effective had serious methodological limitations
tional needs. Additionally, GFCF diets require (e.g., participants were receiving additional inter-
significant financial resources as most foods cost ventions with SIT), which makes discrepancies
almost double the amount of food containing glu- across studies difficult to interpret. In addition, the
ten and casein (Stevens & Rashid, 2008). There is negative findings from 4 of the 14 studies suggest
also the potential for negative social conse- that SIT may actually increase stereotypy and
quences, as special diets may further isolate chil- other problem behaviors (e.g., Devlin, Healy,
dren with ASD from their typically developing Leader, & Hughes, 2011).
peers. GFCF diets also have been linked to a Despite the widespread lack of evidence for
number of adverse side effects including nutri- SIT, it remains one of the most popular treatments
tional deficiencies (Arnold et al., 2003) and sub- for ASD (Green et al., 2006). This finding is
optimal bone development (Hediger et al., 2008). concerning, because implementing SIT takes both
The evidence currently available does not sup- time and resources away from other evidence-­
port the use of a GFCF diet as an intervention for based interventions. Furthermore, forms of SIT
ASD; thus, families should not use this diet may be socially stigmatizing to the individual if
unless recommended by their doctor as treatment implemented within a group of typically develop-
for food allergies. ing children (e.g., an adult brushes a child with
ASD in a social setting). Finally, resources allo-
cated to the materials needed for SIT can be
Sensory Integration Therapy (SIT) expensive and may reduce the resources available
for substantiated interventions.
Children with ASD often present with sensory
abnormalities (Rogers, Hepburn, & Wehner,
2003; Schaaf & Lane, 2015); thus, sensory-based Auditory Integration Therapy
treatments are frequently used as a component of
intervention (Green et al., 2006; Smith & Auditory integration therapy (AIT) started being
Antolovich, 2000; Watling et al., 1999). used as an intervention for ASD in the early
Proponents of SIT hypothesize that many of the 1990s (Berard, 1993). Similar to supporters of
core symptoms of ASD come from deficits in SIT, advocates for AIT suggest that many of the
registering and modulating sensory input from symptoms of ASD are a result of sensory pro-
the environment (Ayres, 1972). Sensory-based cessing abnormalities. Auditory hypersensitivity
treatments focus on helping the individual (i.e., hyperacusis) is a condition in which expo-
respond adaptively to sensory inputs through sure to certain sounds may cause pain and other-
controlled sensory activities, which target the wise impair an individual’s ability to function.
proprioceptive, vestibular, and tactile sensory AIT addresses auditory hypersensitivity by pro-
systems. viding individuals with 10 h of digitally modified
32 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

music or sound across 10 days with two 30-min Academy of Pediatrics, and the Educational
sessions conducted daily. Music is presented Audiology Association agree that AIT should be
through an auditory integration device, which considered experimental, and they do not endorse
alters the sounds by attenuating the frequencies its use for the treatment of communication,
for which the individual is hypersensitive, vary- behavioral, emotional, or learning disorders
ing high and low frequencies and volume on a (American Speech-Language-Hearing
random basis. The aim of AIT is to exercise and Association, 2004).
tone the muscles in the ear to reduce sound sensi-
tivity, which is supposed to decrease individuals’
aberrant reactions to sounds (e.g., aggression, Therapeutic Horseback Riding
self-injurious behavior, rigidity, and stereotypic
behavior). Therapeutic horseback riding (THR) is a type of
At present, very few empirical studies have animal-assisted intervention which has been used
been conducted on AIT, and current support for in the treatment of individuals with ASD. THR
this treatment is based on anecdotal caregiver focuses on developing an awareness of movement,
reports of positive outcomes with AIT. In a weight distribution, hand-eye coordination, and
review of the evidence for AIT, Dawson and improved speech and providing an individual with
Watling (2000) found only five empirical studies a wide range of tactile and auditory experiences
of AIT for individuals with ASD, and of those (All, Loving, & Crane, 1999). THR is typically
five studies, only three included a control condi- implemented by occupational therapists, speech-
tion (Bettison, 1996; Rimland & Edelson, 1995; language pathologists, and physical therapists.
Zollweg, Palm, & Vance, 1997). Of the three The goals of treatment tend to be individualized
studies with a control condition, two studies for each child and will vary depending on which
found improvements in behavioral outcomes for professional is implementing the treatment. For
both the control and AIT group. For example, example, speech-language therapists will focus on
Bettison randomly assigned 40 children with communication goals, whereas physical therapists
ASD or Asperger’s syndrome to an AIT group will focus on goals related to muscle movements,
and 40 to a control treatment. Parents and teach- posture, and balance.
ers were blind to the group assignment. Following The evidence available to support the efficacy
the intervention, both groups showed significant of THR for children with ASD is limited. Davis
improvements on behavioral and cognitive mea- et al. (2015) reviewed the current literature on
sures, and the treatment group did not differ from animal-assisted interventions for children with
the control group. Thus, outcomes for AIT were ASD. These authors identified six studies mea-
not superior to those in the control group that did suring changes in social skills, communication,
not receive AIT. The remaining study with a con- and autism symptomology for children with ASD
trol condition reviewed by Dawson and Watling following THR. Four of the six studies were clas-
found that improvements in the AIT condition sified as having insufficient evidence to support
relative to the control condition were limited by the use of THR. The other two studies were clas-
inadequate pretreatment matching of the control sified as having a preponderance of evidence
and AIT groups. (Bass, Duchowny, & Llabre, 2009; Gabriels
The scientific evidence to support the use of et al., 2012), with a number of limitations identi-
AIT as a treatment for ASD is insufficient. AIT is fied. For example, Bass and colleagues found that
also a costly procedure and puts children at risk children with ASD exposed to 12 weeks of THR
of noise-induced hearing loss if the AIT device is showed significant improvements in standardized
used improperly (Sinha, Silove, Hayen, & measures of social functioning as compared to a
Williams, 2011). At present, the American control group. However, the positive results are
Academy of Audiology, the American Speech-­ difficult to interpret because the authors did not
Language-­ Hearing Association, the American include any direct measures of behavior, the
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 33

raters (i.e., participant’s parents) were not blind communicating the lack of research support for
to the purpose of the study, and several partici- an intervention is not often a socially acceptable
pants received other treatments (e.g., speech ther- explanation for why alternative interventions
apy) simultaneously with THR. should be considered. The remainder of this
Jenkins and DiGennaro Reed (2013) evalu- chapter will describe one example of how a
ated the effects of THR for seven children with behavior analyst could respond to this situation to
ASD using a single-case experimental design. maintain professional relationships with team
These authors extended previous research on members and evaluate the efficacy of the inter-
THR by incorporating direct measures of behav- vention with the client.
ior change following the initiation of
THR. Specifically, the experimenters directly
measured changes in participants’ affect, lan- I dentify and Define the Behavior(s)
guage, off-task behavior, compliance with that the Intervention Purports
demands, and problem behavior during activities to Address
at an afterschool program (e.g., games, snack,
academics), at home, and during THR sessions. A discussion of the behavior(s) that the unsub-
The results showed that THR did not result in stantiated intervention putatively addresses can
clinically significant improvements from base- be helpful. It is possible that the team is unaware
line for any of the dependent variables. Given the of the specific behavior(s) that the intervention
current lack of well-controlled studies demon- may address, particularly if there are no pub-
strating positive outcomes with THR, caregivers lished studies on the intervention. In this case,
should be cautious when considering THR as a the team should identify the behavior(s) that the
component of treatment for children with ASD. intervention will need to change in order to pro-
vide evidence for the benefits and continued use
of the intervention. This discussion should result
 ow to Determine the Evidence
H in the identification of specific behavior(s) and
of Treatment for a Client be accompanied by the development of an opera-
tional definition of each target behavior. Like all
Behavior analysts frequently work as part of a operational definitions, behavior should be
treatment team to provide services to a client. observable, and the definition should facilitate
The behavior analyst may be the only person on objective data collection. In addition, the data
the team who has familiarity with substantiated collection system for the target behavior should
and unsubstantiated treatments for individuals be identified and agreed upon by the team. For
with ASD. Thus, other treatment team members example, the team may agree to collect fre-
may suggest the use of an unsubstantiated inter- quency data on aggressive behavior during
vention (including novel interventions that do not 30-min observation periods at approximately the
yet have research support) with a client. These same three times of day (9 am, 1 pm, and 6 pm).
recommendations can create an ethical dilemma The team also should decide on a time line for
for behavior analysts who have to maintain a pro- evaluating the intervention. If the intervention is
fessional working relationship with members of reported to have immediate effects, the treatment
the treatment team while maintaining interven- time line may be relatively brief. However, inter-
tion practices that are in the best interest of the vention that may take time to change behavior
client (Behavior Analyst Certification Board, (e.g., an SSRI) may require a longer evaluation
2016). One of the most difficult ethical dilemmas period. Determining the potential length of treat-
may occur when a parent is adamant about the ment in advance will decrease the likelihood that
use of an unsubstantiated intervention with his or the intervention is implemented for longer than
her child. If the intervention that the parent asks necessary if the intervention is not having the
to use has not been the subject of any research, intended effect on behavior.
34 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

Data Collection and Analysis but separately. For example, calculations of the


frequency of saliva exiting the mouth should be
The individual(s) who will be responsible for separated from calculations of the frequency of
data collection should be identified and trained to the other target behavior(s).
reliably record behavior. The field of behavior Ongoing meetings could occur to review data
analysis sets reliability standards at a minimum on the progress of intervention as a treatment
of 80%. Thus, two independent data collectors team. During these meetings, the behavior ana-
should agree on the occurrence and nonoccur- lyst will show the treatment team the graphical
rence of behavior during at least 80% of record- depiction of data, explain the graph using lan-
ing opportunities, and comparison data should be guage that everyone on the team can understand,
collected during 30% of intervention intervals, if and describe any treatment effects noted in the
possible. The behavior analyst should volunteer graph. If behavior does not change, that previ-
to conduct training on data collection, participate ously agreed-upon length of time that the team
in data collection (if possible), monitor the will conduct the intervention should be reviewed.
behavior during the evaluation period in a format If the intervention changes behavior in the
that allows for visual inspection of data (e.g., a intended direction (i.e., target behavior improves),
graph), and calculate the reliability of data to the behavior analyst could suggest a brief reversal
ensure that the field standards are upheld. to baseline, if appropriate to the target behavior.
Data on target behavior(s) should be collected It is important that the behavior analyst explain
prior to the implementation of the intervention to that the brief reversal is used to ensure that the
establish a baseline. Collect data on each target observed treatment effect is caused by the inter-
behavior separately in case one behavior is more vention and not some other event that happened
or less affected by the intervention than the other to occur at the same time that the intervention
behavior(s). The behavior analyst should explain began (i.e., an extraneous variable). It may be dif-
the purpose and necessity of baseline data collec- ficult to convince the treatment team to remove
tion, which is (a) used to predict patterns of an effective treatment, even if only for a brief
behavior in the absence of intervention and (b) time. The behavior analyst could remind the team
compared to data obtained during the interven- of the effort in implementing the intervention in
tion to determine an effect. Although it may be the long term and the benefits of ensuring that the
difficult for an eager treatment team to wait to effort dedicated to treatment is worthwhile by
implement the intervention while baseline data confirming that the intervention is the cause of
are collected, a discussion of the benefits of base- the behavior change.
line data in initial team meetings (e.g., when the Discussing potential steps of treatment (i.e.,
intervention is initially discussed) may assuage baseline, treatment, brief withdraw of treatment,
the team’s concerns. The behavior analyst should reinstatement of treatment) in initial team
attempt to collect enough baseline data (a mini- meetings and prior to implementation may make
mum of three points, but more if data are variable it more likely that the team agrees to withdraw
or there is a trend in the direction of the treatment treatment if an effect is observed. If the behavior
effect) to predict behavior in the absence of the analyst anticipates that the team will not agree to
intervention. a reversal to baseline, or if a reversal design is not
When the intervention is implemented, data appropriate for the target behavior (e.g., the inter-
collection should continue to occur in an identical vention seeks to teach a novel skill), other designs
manner to baseline. Occasionally, an unexpected (e.g., multiple baseline across settings or behav-
behavior may emerge during intervention iors) may be another way to demonstrate the
(e.g., excessive saliva secretions from the effects of the intervention on behavior during the
mouth). Rather than changing the data collection treatment evaluation.
for existing target behavior, data on the new If the team agrees to withdraw the interven-
behavior should be collected simultaneously tion, additional baseline data are collected to
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 35

determine whether baseline levels of behavior are Bass, M. M., Duchowny, C. A., & Llabre, M. M. (2009).
The effect of therapeutic horseback riding on social
recaptured. It is possible that target behavior will
functioning in children with autism. Journal of Autism
continue at the same level as when the interven- and Developmental Disorders, 39, 1261–1267.
tion was in place. If this occurs, the team may doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0734-3
decide to continue with baseline data collection Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., & Voigt, R. G. (2006).
Autism: A review of the state of the science for pediat-
to see if behavior change maintains over a long
ric primary health care clinicians. Archives of Pediatrics
time period or eventually returns to the initial and Adolescent Medicine, 160, 1167–1175. doi:10.1001/
baseline level. The behavior analyst should archpedi.160.11.1167
explain that this pattern of behavior indicates that Beavers, G., Iwata, B., & Lerman, D. (2013). Thirty years of
research on the functional analysis of problem behav-
either the treatment effects are maintaining in the
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 1–21.
absence of intervention or the observed effect doi:10.1002/jaba.30
was the result of some extraneous variable and Behavior Analyst Certification Board (2016). Professional
not due to the intervention. If behavior returns to and ethical compliance code for behavior analysts.
http://bacb.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/160120-­
previous baseline levels, the intervention should
compliance-­code-english.pdf
be reinstated and data collection continued. Bettison, S. (1996). The long-term effects of auditory
Although there are other strategies that a behavior training on children with autism. Journal of Autism
analyst and treatment team can use to evaluate the and Developmental Disorders, 26(3), 361–374.
doi:10.1007/BF02172480
efficacy of an intervention, the description in this
Berard, G. (1993). Hearing equals behavior. New Canaan,
chapter is an example of one possible sequence of CT: Keats.
events. Regardless of the course of action determined Bird, B., Russo, D., & Cataldo, M. (1977). Considerations
by the treatment team, evaluating the efficacy of any in the analysis and treatment of dietary effects
on behavior: A case study. Journal of Autism and
non-­substantiated intervention used with the client
Childhood Schizophrenia, 7, 373–382.
should be a consistent goal. The use of substantiated Bloom, S. E., Iwata, B. A., Fritz, J. N., Roscoe, E. M.,
interventions can lead to more efficient skill acquisi- & Carreau, A. B. (2011). Classroom application of
tion and greater reductions in problem behavior. a trial-based functional analysis. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 44(1), 19–31.
Thus, whenever possible, clients should receive sub-
Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T.
stantiated interventions to produce the best interven- (2006). Early intensive behavioral treatment:
tion outcomes for individuals with ASD. Replication of the UCLA model in a com-
munity setting. Journal of Developmental and
Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(Suppl 2), S145–S155.
doi:10.1097/00004703-200604002-00013
References Cronin, K. A., & Cuvo, A. J. (1979). Teaching mend-
ing skills to mentally retarded adolescents. Journal
All, A. C., Loving, G. L., & Crane, L. L. (1999). Animals, of Applied Behavior Analysis, 12(3), 401–406.
horseback riding, and implications for rehabilitation doi:10.1901/jaba.1979.12-401
therapy. Journal of Rehabilitation, 65, 49–57. Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Muson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J.,
American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2004). Greenson, J., …, Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, con-
Auditory integration training [Position Statement]. trolled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism:
Available from www.asha.org/policy. The early start Denver model. Pediatrics, 125(1), e17–
Association for Behavior Analysis International (1995). e23. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-0958
Statement on facilitated communication. https://www. Dawson, G., & Watling, R. (2000). Interventions to facili-
abainternational.org/about-us/policies-and-positions/ tate auditory, visual, and motor integration in autism:
facilitated-communication,-1995.aspx A review of the evidence. Journal of Autism and
Arnold, G. L., Hyman, S. L., Mooney, R. A., & Kirby, Developmental Disorders, 30, 415–421. doi:10.102
R. S. (2003). Plasma amino acids profiles in children 3/A:1005547422749
with autism: Potential risk of nutritional deficiencies. Davis, T. N., Scalzo, R., Butler, E., Stauffer, M., Farah,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, Y. N., Perez, S., & ... Coviello, L. (2015). Animal
449–454. doi:10.1023/A:1025071014191 assisted interventions for children with autism spec-
Ayres, A. (1972). Sensory integration and learning trum disorder: A systematic review. Education and
disabilities. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Training in Autism and Developmental Disabilities,
Services. 50, 316–329.
36 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

Derby, K. M., Wacker, D. P., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly,
Northup, J., Cigrand, K., & Asmus, J. (1992). Brief M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treat-
functional assessment techniques to evaluate aberrant ments used by parents of children with autism.
behavior in an outpatient setting: A summary of 79 Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27(1), 70–84.
cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(3), doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.12.002
713–721. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-713 Gresham, F. M., & MacMillan, D. L. (1998). Early
Devlin, S., Healy, O., Leader, G., & Hughes, B. M. (2011). intervention project: Can its claims be substantiated
Comparison of behavioral intervention and sensory-­ and its effects replicated? Journal of Autism and
integration therapy in the treatment of challeng- Developmental Disorders, 28, 5–13.
ing behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental Grow, L. L., Kelley, M. E., Roane, H. S., & Shillingsburg,
Disorders, 41, 1303–1320. doi:10.1007/s M. A. (2008). Utility of extinction-induced response
10803-010-1149-x variability for the selection of mands. Journal of
Dufek, S., & Schreibman, L. (2014). Natural envi- Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(1), 15–24. doi:10.1901/
ronment training. In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, jaba.2008.41-15
P. Sturmey, J. L. Matson, J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, Hagopian, L., Rooker, G., Jessel, J., & DeLeon, I. (2013).
... J. L. Matson (Eds.) , Handbook of early inter- Initial functional analysis outcomes and modifications
vention for autism spectrum disorders: Research, in pursuit of differentiation: A summary of 176 inpa-
policy, and practice (pp. 325–344). New York, tient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46,
NY, US: Springer Science + Business Media. 88–100.
doi:10.1007/978-1-4939-0401-3_13. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Dunn, W. (1999). The sensory profile; user’s manual. San Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
Antonio, TX: Psychological Cooperation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(2), 147–
Eapen, V., Črnčec, R., & Walter, A. (2013). Clinical out- 185. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147
comes of an early intervention program for preschool Hart, S. L., & Banda, D. R. (2010). Picture exchange
children with autism spectrum disorder in a commu- communication system with individuals with devel-
nity group setting. BMC Pediatrics, 13(1), 3. opmental disabilities: A meta-analysis of single sub-
Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Jahr, E., & Karlsson, P. (2012). ject studies. Remedial and Special Education, 31(6),
Outcome for children with autism receiving early and 476–488.
intensive behavioral intervention in mainstream pre- Hediger, M. L., England, L. J., Molloy, C. A., Yu,
school and kindergarten settings. Research in Autism K. F., Manning-Courtney, P., & Mills, J. L. (2008).
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 829–835. doi:10.1016/j. Reduced bone cortical thickness in boys with autism
rasd.2011.09.002 or autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism
Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2002). and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 848–856.
Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4–7-year-­ doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0453-6
old children with autism: A 1-year comparison con- Hyman, S. L., Stewart, P. A., Foley, J., Cain, U., Peck, R.,
trolled study. Behavior Modification, 2002, 49–68. Morris, D. D., … Smith, T. (2016). The gluten-free/
Elder, J. H., Shankar, M., Shuster, J., Theriaque, D., Burns, casein-free diet: A double-blind challenge trial in chil-
S., & Sherrill, L. (2006). The gluten-free, casein-free dren with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
diet in autism: Results of a preliminary double-blind Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2564-9
clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental Hoyson, M., Jamieson, B., & Strain, P. S. (1984).
Disorders, 36, 413–420. Individualized group instruction of normally and
Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E., autistic-like children: A description and evaluation of
Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analysis the LEAP curriculum model. Journal of the Division of
of early intensive behavioral intervention for Early Childhood, 8, 157–181.
children with autism. Journal of Clinical Child Individuals with Disabilities Education Act, 20 U.S.C. §
and Adolescent Psychology, 38(3), 439–450. 1400 (2004).
doi:10.1080/15374410902851739 Irvin, D. (2006). Using analog assessment procedures for
Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). PECS: The picture determining the effects of a gluten-free and casein-free
exchange communication system. Cherry Hill, NJ: diet on rate of problem behaviors for an adolescent
Pyramid Educational Consultants, Inc. with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 281–286.
Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Holt, K. D., Shoffner, A., Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman,
Zhaoxing, P., Ruzzano, S., & ... Mesibov, G. (2012). K. E., & Richman, G. S. (1982). Toward a func-
Pilot study measuring the effects of therapeutic horse- tional analysis of self-injury. Analysis & Intervention
back riding on school-age children and adolescents in Developmental Disabilities, 2(1), 3–20.
with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism doi:10.1016/0270-4684(82)90003-9
Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 578–588. doi:10.1016/j. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & &
rasd.2011.09.007 Miltenberger, R. G. (1994a). What makes extinction
Green, G., & Shane, H. C. (1994). Science, reason, and work: An analysis of procedural form and function.
facilitated communication. Journal of the Association Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(1), 131–
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 19, 151–172. 144. doi:10.1901/jaba.1994.27-131
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 37

Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R., Lerman, D. C., Vorndran, C., Addison, L., & Kuhn, S. A.
Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., et al. (1994b). The func- (2004). A rapid assessment of skills in young children
tions of self-injurious behavior: An experimental-­ with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior 37(1), 11.
Analysis, 27, 215–240. Libby, M. E., Weiss, J. S., Bancroft, S., & Ahearn, W. H.
Jenkins, S. R., & DiGennaro Reed, F. D. (2013). An (2008). A comparison of most-to-least and least-to-­
experimental analysis of the effects of therapeutic most prompting on the acquisition of solitary play
horseback riding on the behavior of children with skills. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 37–43.
autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, Lilienfeld, S. O. (2005). Scientifically unsupported and
721–740. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.02.008 supported interventions for childhood psychopathol-
Kodak, T., Lerman, D. C., Volkert, V. M., & Trosclair, N. ogy: A summary. Pediatrics, 115, 761–764.
(2007). Further examination of factors that influence Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled
preference for positive versus negative reinforcement. children: The Me book. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 25–44. Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and
doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.151-05 normal educational and intellectual function-
Kodak, T., Miltenberger, R. G., & Romaniuk, C. (2003). ing in young autistic children. Journal of
The effects of differential negative reinforcement of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 3–9.
other behavior and noncontingent escape on compli- doi:10.1037/0022-006X.55.1.3
ance. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36(3), Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop-
379–382. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-379 mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Austin,
Koegel, L. K., Carter, C. M., & Koegel, R. L. (2003). TX: PRO-ED.
Teaching children with autism self-initiations as a Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). A meta-analytic
pivotal response. Topics in Language Disorders, 23, review of the effectiveness of behavioural early inter-
134–145. vention programs for children with autistic spectrum
Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Healy, O., Rispoli, M., Lydon, disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
H., Streusand, W., & ... Giesbers, S. (2012). 4(4), 577–593. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.01.014
Sensory integration therapy for autism spectrum Marí-Bauset, S., Zazpe, I., Mari-Sanchis, A., Llopis-­
disorders: A systematic review. Research in Autism González, A., & Morales-Suárez-Varela, M. (2014).
Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 1004–1018. doi:10.1016/j. Evidence of the gluten-free and casein-free diet in
rasd.2012.01.006 autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review.
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. A. (1999). A work in progress: Journal of Child Neurology, 29(12), 1718–1727.
Behavior management strategies and a curriculum for doi:10.1177/0883073814531330
intensive behavioral treatment of autism. New York: Maurice, C., Green, G., & Foxx, R. M. (2001). Making a
DRL Books. difference: Behavioral intervention for autism. Austin,
Lerman, D. C., Hawkins, L., Hillman, C., Shireman, M., TX: PRO-ED.
& Nissen, M. A. (2015). Adults with autism spectrum McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-­
disorder as behavior technicians for young children term outcome for children with autism who received
with autism: Outcomes of a behavioral skills training early intensive behavioral treatment. American
program. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48(2), Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(4), 359–372.
233–256. doi:10.1002/jaba.196 Mitka, M. (2008). Chelation therapy trials halted. The
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1995). Prevalence of the Journal of the American Medical Association, 300(19),
extinction burst and its attenuation during treatment. 2236.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(1), 93–94. Montee, B. B., Miltenberger, R. G., & Wittrock, D.
doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-93 (1995). An experimental analysis of facilitated com-
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Developing a tech- munication. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
nology for the use of operant extinction in clinical set- 28(2), 189–200. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-189
tings: An examination of basic and applied research. Moss, J. D., Ruthven, A. J., Hawkins, W. M., & Topping, J.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29(3), 345– S. (1983). The relative effects of three responseelimina-
382. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-345 tion procedures in severely/profoundly and trainable
Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999). retarded individuals. Psychological Record, 33, 231–236.
Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting Miltenberger, R. G. (2013). Behavior modification:
and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious Principles and procedures (6th ed.). Belmont, CA:
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), Thomson Wadsworth.
1–8. doi:10.1901/jaba.1999.32-1 Mueller, M., Nkosi, A., & Hine, J. (2011). Functional
Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the sta- analysis in public schools: A summary of 90 func-
tus of knowledge for using punishment: Implications tional analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied 44, 807–818.
Behavior Analysis, 35(4), 431–464. doi:10.1901/ Mulloy, A., Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni,
jaba.2002.35-431 G., & Rispoli, M. (2010). Gluten-free and casein-­
38 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

free diets in the treatment of autism spectrum dis- for children with autism and their parents after two
orders: A systematic review. Research in Autism years. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112,
Spectrum Disorders, 4(3), 328–339. doi:10.1016/j. 418–438. doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[418:EIBI
rasd.2009.10.008 OF]2.0.CO;2
National Autism Center. (2009). National standards Rimland, B., & Edelson, S. M. (1995). Brief report: A
report. Randolph, MA: National Autism Center. pilot study of auditory integration training in autism.
Northup, J., Wacker, D., Sasso, G., Steege, M., Cigrand, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 25,
K., Cook, J., & DeRaad, A. (1991). A brief func- 61–70. doi:10.1007/BF02178168
tional analysis of aggressive and alternative behav- Rivas, K. D., Piazza, C. C., Patel, M. R., & Bachmeyer,
ior in an outclinic setting. Journal of Applied M. H. (2010). Spoon distance fading with and with-
Behavior Analysis, 24(3), 509–522. doi:10.1901/ out escape extinction as treatment for food refusal.
jaba.1991.24-509 Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 673–
Odom, S. L., Boyd, B. A., Hall, L. J., & Hume, K. (2010a). 683. doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-673
Evaluation of comprehensive treatment models for Rogers, S. J., & Dawson, G. (2010). The early Start
individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal Denver model: Promoting language, learning, and
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(4), 425– engagement. New York: Guilford.
436. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0825-1 Rogers, S. J., Hall, T., Osaki, D., Reaven, J., & Herbison,
Odom, S. L., Collet-Klingenberg, L., Rogers, S. J., & J. (2000). A comprehensive, integrated, educational
Hatton, D. D. (2010b). Evidence-based practices in approach to young children with autism and their
interventions for children and youth with autism spec- families. In S. L. Harris & J. S. Handleman (Eds.),
trum disorders. Preventing School Failure, 54, 275– Preschool education programs for children with
282. doi:10.1080/10459881003785506 autism (2nd ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Owen-Smith, A. A., Bent, S., Lynch, F. L., Coleman, K. J., Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). Parent
Yau, V. M., Pearson, K. A., & Croen, L. A. (2015). reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism
Prevalence and predictors of complementary and and those with other developmental disorders. Journal
alternative medicine use in a large insured sample of of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 631–642.
children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Research in doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 17, 40–51. doi:10.1016/j. Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive
rasd.2015.05.002 behavioral treatment for children with autism:
Perrin, J. M., Coury, D. L., Hyman, S. L., Cole, Four-year outcome and predictors. American
L., Reynolds, A. M., & Clemons, T. (2012). Journal on Mental Retardation, 110(6), 417–438.
Complementary and alternative medicine use in a doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2005)110[417:IBTFCW]2.0
large pediatric autism sample. Pediatrics, 130(Suppl .CO;2
2), S77–S82. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-0900E Schaaf, R. C., & Lane, A. E. (2015). Toward a best-practice
Preston, D., & Carter, M. (2009). A review of the effi- protocol for assessment of sensory features in ASD.
cacy of the picture exchange communication system Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental 45(5), 1380–1395. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2299-z
Disorders, 39(10), 1471–1486. Schlichenmeyer, K. J., Dube, W. V., & Vargas-Irwin, M.
Radley, K. C., Dart, E. H., Furlow, C. M., & Ness, E. J. (2015). Stimulus fading and response elaboration in
(2015). Peer-mediated discrete trial training within differential reinforcement for alternative behavior.
a school setting. Research in Autism Spectrum Behavioral Interventions, 30(1), 51–64. doi:10.1002/
Disorders, 9:53–67. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.10.001 bin.1402
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early Seaver, J. L., & Bourret, J. C. (2014). An evaluation
intensive behavioral intervention for young children of response prompts for teaching behavior chains.
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47(4), 777–
Developmental Disorders, 42, 512–520. doi:10.1007/ 792. doi:10.1002/jaba.159
s10803-011-1218-9 Severtson, J. M., & Carr, J. E. (2012). Training nov-
Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills inter- ice instructors to implement errorless discrete-trial
ventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation for evi- teaching: A sequential analysis. Behavior Analysis in
dence-based practices within a best evidence synthesis Practice, 5(2), 13–23.
framework. Journal of Autism and Developmental Sinha, Y., Silove, N., Hayen, A., & Williams, K. (2011).
Disorders, 40, 149–166. Auditory integration training and other sound
Reichow, B., & Wolery, M. (2009). Comprehensive syn- therapies for autism spectrum disorders. The
thesis of early intensive behavioral interventions Cochrane Databases of Systematic Reviews, 12.
for young children with autism based on the UCLA doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003681.pub3
young autism project model. Journal of Autism and Shattock, P., Kennedy, A., Rowell, F., & Berney, T. (1990).
Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 23–41. doi:10.1007/ Role of neuropeptides in autism and their relationships
s10803-008-0596-0 with classical neurotransmitters. Brain Dysfunction,
Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., degli 3(5–6), 328–345.
Espinosa, F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., … Ward, N. (2007). Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior.
Early intensive behavioral intervention: Outcomes New York: The Macmillian Company.
2  Substantiated and Unsubstantiated Interventions 39

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Action, MA: Tincani, M., & Devis, K. (2011). Quantitative synthesis
Copley Publishing Group. and component analysis of single-participant stud-
Smith, T., & Antolovich, M. (2000). Parental perceptions ies on the picture exchange communication system.
of supplemental interventions received by young chil- Remedial and Special Education, 32(6), 458–470.
dren with autism in intensive behavior analytic treat- doi:10.1177/0741932510362494
ment. Behavioral Interventions, 15(2), 83–97. Touchette, P. E. (1971). Transfer of stimulus control:
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized Measuring the moment of transfer. Journal of the
trial of intensive early intervention for children with Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 15(3), 347–354.
pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal doi:10.1901/jeab.1971.15-347
on Mental Retardation, 105(4), 269–285. Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic inter-
Smith, C. M., Smith, R. G., Dracobly, J. D., & Pace, A. P. vention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis,
(2012). Multiple-respondent anecdotal assessments: meta-regression and dose–response meta-analysis
An analysis of interrater agreement and correspon- of multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review,
dence with analogue assessment outcomes. Journal of 30(4), 387399. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.008
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(4), 779–795. Vismara, L. A., & Bogin, J. (2009). Steps for implementa-
Spreckley, M., & Boyd, R. (2009). Efficacy of applied tion: Pivotal response training. Sacramento, CA: The
behavioral intervention in preschool children with National Professional Development Center on Autism
autism for improving cognitive, language, and adaptive Spectrum Disorders, The M.I.N.D. Institute, The
behavior: A systematic review and meta-­analysis. The University of California at Davis School of Medicine.
Journal of Pediatrics, 154(3), 338–344. doi:10.1016/j. Vivanti, G., Paynter, J., Duncan, E., Fothergill, H.,
jpeds.2008.09.012 Dissanayake, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2014). Effectiveness
Stevens, L., & Rashid, M. (2008). Gluten-free and regu- and feasibility of the Early Start Denver Model imple-
lar foods: A cost comparison. Canadian Journal of mented in a group-based community childcare set-
Dietetic Practice and Research, 69, 147–150. ting. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Strain, P. S., & Bovey, E. I. (2011). Randomized, con- 44(12), 3140–3153. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2168-9
trolled trial of the LEAP model of early intervention Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G.,
for young children with autism spectrum disorders. & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in
Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 31(3), the treatment of attention-maintained self-injurious
133–154. doi:10.1177/0271121411408740 behavior: Noncontingent reinforcement and differ-
Striefel, S., & Wetherby, B. (1973). Instruction-following ential reinforcement of other behavior. Journal of
behavior of a retarded child and its controlling stimuli. Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 9–21. ­doi:10.1901/
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(4), 663–670. jaba.1993.26-9
doi:10.1901/jaba.1973.6-663 Vollmer, T. R., Roane, H. S., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus,
Sulzer-Azaroff, B., Hoffman, A. O., Horton, C. B., Bondy, B. A. (1999). Evaluating treatment challenges with
A., & Frost, L. (2009). The picture exchange commu- differential reinforcement of alternative behavior.
nication system (PECS): What do the data say? Focus Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32(1), 9–23.
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, doi:10.1901/jaba.1999.32-9
24(2), 89–103. doi:10.1177/1088357609332743 Walls, R. T., Ellis, W. D., Zane, T., & VanderPoel, S. J.
Thomson, K. M., Martin, G. L., Fazzio, D., Salem, S., (1979). Tactile, auditory, and visual prompting in
Young, K., & Yu, C. T. (2012). Evaluation of a self-­ teaching complex assembly tasks. Education and
instructional package for teaching tutors to conduct Training of the Mentally Retarded, 14, 120–130.
discrete-trials teaching with children with autism. Watling, R., Deitz, J., Kanny, E. M., & McLaughlin,
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(3), 1073– J. F. (1999). Current practice of occupational ther-
1082. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.02.005 apy for children with autism. American Journal of
Tien, K. (2008). Effectiveness of the picture exchange Occupational Therapy, 53(5), 498–505. doi:10.5014/
communication system as a functional communication ajot.53.5.498
intervention for individuals with autism spectrum dis- Wheeler, D. L., Jacobson, J. W., Paglieri, R. A., &
orders: A practice-based research synthesis. Education Schwartz, A. A. (1993). An experimental assessment
and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 43(1), of facilitated communication. Mental Retardation,
61–76. 31(1), 49–59.
Tiger, J. H., Fisher, W. W., & Bouxsein, K. J. (2009). Whiteley, P., Rodgers, J., Savery, D., & Shattock,
Therapist- and self-monitored DRO contingencies as a P. (1999). A gluten-free diet as an interven-
treatment for the self-injurious skin picking of a young tion for autism and associated spectrum disor-
man with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Applied ders: Preliminary findings. Autism, 3(1), 45–65.
Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 315–319. doi:10.1901/ doi:10.1177/1362361399003001005
jaba.2009.42-315 Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K. A., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional A., Kucharczyk, S., ... Schultz, T. R. (2015). Evidence-­
communication training: A review and practical guide. based practices for children, youth, and young adults
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 16–23. with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
40 T. Kodak and R.A. Carroll

sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental parents of children with autism. Research in Autism
Disorders, 45(7), 1951–1966. doi:10.1007/ Spectrum Disorders, 6(4), 1321–1330. doi:10.1016/j.
s10803-014-2351-z rasd.2012.05.001
Young, K. L., Boris, A. L., Thomson, K. M., Martin, Zollweg, W., Palm, D., & Vance, V. (1997). The efficacy
G. L., & Yu, C. T. (2012). Evaluation of a self-­ of auditory integration training: A double blind study.
instructional package on discrete-trials teaching to American Journal of Audiology, 6, 39–47.
Ethical Considerations Regarding
Treatment 3
Paige E. Cervantes, Johnny L. Matson,
Maya Matheis, and Claire O. Burns

of these formal ethical codes; one of the most well


Ethical Considerations known was the Nuremberg trials following uneth-
Regarding Treatment ical medical experiments conducted on prisoners
of war during World War II. These trials led to the
All decisions regarding treatment for autism establishment of the Nuremberg Code, which
spectrum disorder (ASD) can be regarded as ethi- highlights the necessity of voluntary, informed
cal issues, as treatment has direct and lasting consent for human participants in research; addi-
impact on the functioning of an individual and tional emphasis was placed on preserving partici-
their family members. To maximize outcomes pant safety (The Nuremberg Code, 1947). The
while minimizing harm, professionals in the Declaration of Helsinki was later developed to
ASD field must carefully consider many factors expand the Nuremberg code and further address
related to the ratio between benefit and risk when clinical research. A key component of this decla-
selecting intervention components and in the ration is the principle that “it is the duty of the
course of treatment implementation. The purpose physician to promote and safeguard the health,
of this chapter is to highlight and discuss several well-being and rights of patients” (World Medical
ethical considerations in the context of common Association, 1964). In response to the unethical
ASD treatments. research practices used in the Tuskegee syphilis
Ethical codes for professional practice have experiment, the Belmont Report was created in
been established for specific disciplines by orga- 1974 and outlined three central ethical principles
nizations, such as the American Psychological that continue to be emphasized both in research
Association (APA), which outline general princi- and in practice today:
ples and provide an overview of conduct gover-
nance. These formal guidelines help to provide a 1. Respect for persons (i.e., that individuals be
framework for making ethical decisions when able to make their own decisions regarding
working as a clinician and a researcher. Several participation and that those with diminished
historical events contributed to the development ability to make their own decisions are enti-
tled to extra protections)
2. Beneficence (i.e., to protect the safety and
P.E. Cervantes (*) • J.L. Matson • M. Matheis well-being of the participant)
C.O. Burns
3. Justice (i.e., analysis of the distribution of

Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA risks and benefits; Department of Health,
e-mail: pcerva2@lsu.edu Education, and Welfare, 1978)

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 41


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_3
42 P.E. Cervantes et al.

These origins for ethical practice influenced cally responsibility to help caregivers make these
the basis for the future of psychological work. informed decisions.
The APA’s “Ethical Principles of Psychologists As intervention programming guided by the
and Code of Conduct” focuses on five fundamen- principles of applied behavior analysis (ABA) is
tal principles for effective and ethical profes- considered the gold standard of autism treatment,
sional practice: beneficence and nonmaleficence, a majority of this chapter will focus on ethical
fidelity and responsibility, integrity, justice, and considerations specifically related to
respect for people’s rights and dignity. The APA ABA. However, topics related to psychopharma-
also emphasizes the importance of competence, cology and alternative treatment options also
education and training, privacy and confidential- warrant attention and will be discussed below.
ity, and human relations (e.g., conflict of interest, The chapter will conclude with discussion of the
multiple relationships). Specific guidelines for concept of informed choice.
assessment and therapy are also outlined (e.g.,
obtaining informed consent for testing and treat-
ment decisions, planning for termination of ther- Applied Behavior Analysis
apy, maintaining confidentiality, avoiding
multiple relationships in therapy; APA, 2010). Treatment using ABA strategies is currently the
Intervention for individuals with ASD can be only evidence-based option for children with
particularly complex due to the variability in ASD and has been shown to produce, on average,
symptom presentation across individuals, mak- comprehensive and lasting effects (Eldevik et al.,
ing careful ethical considerations imperative for 2009; Foxx, 2008). ABA involves applying meth-
effective practice. As such, focus is needed on ods derived directly from the scientific principles
ethical issues related to ASD treatment recom- of learning and behavior (e.g., operant condition-
mendations and implementation included and ing) in order to encourage socially significant
beyond what is detailed in relevant ethical guide- behavior change. Methods commonly used to
lines. First and foremost, treatment recommenda- teach skills are discrete trial training and natural
tions made by clinicians should be evidence-based. environment teaching; procedures like positive
This is especially relevant to the ASD population, reinforcement, shaping, fading, and prompting
as there are many unsubstantiated treatments that are often used within these teaching procedures
have emerged in recent years. Clinicians are obli- (Foxx, 2008). ABA programming, especially
gated to be informed on the efficacy of different when applied to younger populations within
treatments and to consider the impact of individ- early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI), is
ual client characteristics when determining both intensive (e.g., 20–40 h/week) and long term
appropriate intervention approaches. Beyond (e.g., for 2 or more years). Treatment is compre-
empirical support, there are several other impor- hensive and individualized in that all skill deficits
tant considerations for treatment planning. These and behavioral excesses present in a child will be
include, but are not limited to, intrusiveness, cost, operationally defined and systematically targeted
time commitment, and negative side effects. (Green, Brennan, & Fein, 2002). In addition,
Many treatments can be expensive and intensive, intervention often occurs in small groups or in a
requiring a great deal of time and effort from par- one-on-one adult-to-child setting to encourage
ents as well as professionals. These factors can skill acquisition. Other factors stressed within
impact parental preference for treatments and ABA programming include thorough and objec-
choices related to intervention planning. tive progress monitoring and goal setting as well
However, despite potential inconveniences asso- as planning for maintenance and generalization
ciated with more intensive treatments, the possi- of skills (Foxx, 2008).
ble benefits for the individual may well outweigh Beginning in the 1980s, evidence for the
these drawbacks. Therefore, clinicians are ethi- effectiveness of ABA strategies with individuals
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 43

with ASD has grown exponentially (Foxx, 2008; ment procedure (Klintwall, Gillberg, Bölte, &
Virués-Ortega, 2010). ABA has been shown to Fernell, 2012). Ethical considerations related to
produce large gains in intellectual functioning, these factors will be discussed in the following
language, adaptive behavior, and social skills and sections.
has led to improvements in autism symptoms and
challenging behaviors (Darrou et al., 2010;
Eldevik et al., 2010; Foxx, 2008; MacDonald, Client Characteristics
Parry-Cruwys, Dupere, & Ahearn, 2014;
Reichow, 2012; Virués-Ortega, 2010). Though Client characteristics that predict responsiveness
there is an abundance of evidence supporting the to ABA treatment components have not been
use of ABA as the primary treatment for children fully identified which makes providing recom-
with autism, there are several ethical consider- mendations of best treatment options for a given
ations in the realm of ABA treatment that warrant individual with ASD difficult (Kamio, Haraguchi,
attention. The Behavior Analyst Certification Miyake, & Hiraiwa, 2015; Smith, Klorman, &
Board (BACB) does a thorough job outlining Mruzek, 2015). Although research is inconsis-
guidelines to ensure Board Certified Behavior tent, the factors that have been most notably
Analysts (BCBAs) act ethically and responsibly implicated in ABA and EIBI outcomes are ASD
in their professional activity (BACB, 2014). severity, intellectual functioning, and age (Kamio
Some ethical obligations defined in the BACB et al., 2015). In regard to autism symptomology,
codes mirror that of the APA ethical guidelines individuals with milder presentations of ASD at
(e.g., boundaries of competence, obtaining con- the start of treatment demonstrate greater
sent, client right to effective treatment, remain improvements through treatment. This is particu-
up-to-date on scientific knowledge and make larly true for individuals with less severe social
treatment decisions based upon this knowledge, and language impairments (Sallows, Graupner, &
reduce conflict with other professions); however, MacLean, 2005; Smith et al., 2015). IQ is also a
some are specific to behavior analysts (e.g., large predictor in treatment outcomes; children
appraise effects of any treatment that may impact with ASD and comorbid intellectual impairments
the goals of behavior change, objectively define are less likely to show large gains compared to
goals of treatment and conduct risk-benefit anal- children with ASD and typical intellectual func-
ysis on the procedures to be implemented, uphold tioning (Sallows et al., 2005).
and advance the values, ethics, and principles of Lastly, there has been a substantial amount of
behavior analysis; APA, 2010; BACB, 2014; research indicating the earlier a child is enrolled
Schreck & Miller, 2010). in treatment, the better the outcomes will be
(Granpeesheh, Dixon, Tarbox, Kaplan, & Wilke,
2009; MacDonald et al., 2014; Smith et al.,
Ethical Considerations 2015). For example, children who begin treat-
Regarding Effectiveness ment at younger ages have been found to make
larger gains in IQ, adaptive functioning, and, to a
According to both the APA and the BACB guide- lesser extent, social interaction and social com-
lines, we are professionally and ethically obli- munication abilities and ASD symptomology
gated to provide our clients treatment that works. (Smith et al., 2015). Of note, there is limited data
However, there are several factors that must be available demonstrating treatment effectiveness
considered within that. Though ABA has been for children with ASD under 3 years old (Vismara,
shown to produce large gains on a group level, Colombi, & Rogers, 2009). Also in need of more
researchers have found that improvements in a research is the application of ABA principles to
given individual can vary widely. This differen- issues relevant to adult autism populations.
tial response may be explained by a variety of Research and policy currently focus more atten-
factors related to the client as well as to the treat- tion on child populations, and though gains in
44 P.E. Cervantes et al.

childhood could prevent poorer prognosis in larly true for children with more severe ASD
adulthood, there are many more adults with symptoms, low intellectual functioning, and who
autism than there are children (Jang et al., 2014; start ABA treatment at later ages. The concept of
Matson, Turygin, et al., 2012). While race, eth- recovery may also change parental perceptions.
nicity, socioeconomic status, and area of resi- When the only caregiver goal is to have their
dence have never been linked to treatment child no longer meet criteria for ASD, significant
outcome, there is also a scarcity of research gains in symptomology may be ignored if they do
examining the effectiveness of ABA program- not translate to normal functioning. Although the
ming on underrepresented populations (Lord majority of children will not recover, progress
et al., 2005). Therefore, clinicians should be can be made toward improved quality of life for
aware that direct evidence for the effectiveness of individuals with ASD and their families.
different treatment protocols is not available for Therefore, other optimal outcomes need to be
many ethnic minorities, non-English speaking discussed with caregivers, and discussion of
children, and individuals living in rural areas recovery should be avoided (Ozonoff, 2013;
when recommending interventions or interven- Warren et al., 2011).
tion planning (Lord et al., 2005).
Within the variability in responsiveness to
ABA across individuals with ASD, there appears Treatment Characteristics
to be a small but significant subset of children
who achieve a level of functioning that is indis- Within the realm of ABA programming for indi-
tinguishable from typically developing peers viduals with autism, there is also a wide variety
(Green et al., 2002; Matson, Tureck, Turygin, in how intervention is planned and implemented;
Beighley, & Rieske, 2012; Ozonoff, 2013; Smith there are many different intervention agents and
et al., 2015). Lovaas (1987) was the first to label supervisory models, treatment settings, and treat-
a group of children with ASD who achieved typi- ment intensities (Romanczyk, Callahan, Turner,
cal education and intellectual functioning post- & Cavalari, 2014). Strict guidelines for appropri-
treatment as “recovered” (Ozonoff, 2013). Since ate treatment intensity and duration, treatment
then, the concept of a cure or recovery from setting, therapist training and supervision, and
autism has grown; though, an objective and con- treatment components for a given individual do
sistent definition of what recovery entails has yet not exist (Reichow, 2012). Therefore, clinicians
to be provided (Bölte, 2014; Ozonoff, 2013). need to consider individual characteristics and
Evidence is available demonstrating that some research support in making these intervention
children with ASD who undergo intensive ABA decisions when practicing ethically.
treatment no longer meet criteria for ASD post-
treatment and that EIBI can alter brain develop- Treatment Intensity and Duration  For
ment (Ozonoff, 2013); however, this occurs for decades, researchers have stressed the impor-
only some children. Many children will not expe- tance of treatment intensity and duration in the
rience these large gains in functioning. Further, effective delivery of ABA services. Findings gen-
the children that do show dramatic gains in cer- erally indicate that higher intensity (i.e., h/week
tain areas may continue to experience significant of therapy) and longer duration (i.e., months/
impairments in other domains of functioning years that therapy is provided) interventions pro-
(Warren et al., 2011). duce greater treatment effects (Romanczyk et al.,
Given this variability in individual outcome, 2014; Virués-Ortega, 2010). Some researchers
use of the term “recovery” or “cure” in the mar- suggest that there is a point of diminished returns
keting of ABA programming would be ethically when treatment intensity becomes too high
problematic. Doing so may instill false hope in (Reed, Osborne, & Corness, 2007; Virués-­
many families affected by ASD, as many chil- Ortega, 2010). For example, Reed and colleagues
dren do not reach this outcome. This is particu- (2007) found that although children receiving
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 45

high-intensity treatment (M = 30 h/week) had Treatment Setting and Intervention


better outcomes than children receiving low-­ Agent  ABA programs can differ in the primary
intensity treatment (M = 12 h/week), further setting of treatment (e.g., one-on-one or group
increase of hours of therapy per week within the therapy in home- or clinic-based sessions) and
high-intensity group was not related to further the primary intervention agent (e.g., parents or
gains. The authors suggested that this may reflect behavior therapists). In regard to differential
an exhaustion of treatment effects after a certain effectiveness of home-based versus clinic-based
level and that 40 h/week of therapy may not be programs as well as parent-directed versus
optimal for all individuals with autism (Reed therapist-­
directed treatment, research evidence
et al., 2007). However, this point of diminished has been mixed. Some researchers have found no
returns has not been found consistently in the differences in outcomes related to treatment set-
research literature. For example, Granpeesheh ting and intervention agent; though, others have
and colleagues (2009) found only an increasing shown that significantly more improvement
trend where the rate of treatment gains rose as a occurs in clinic-based, therapist-directed pro-
function of the number of treatment hours for grams (Reed et al., 2007; Virués-Ortega, 2010).
children under 7 years old. In regard to treatment Because of the inconsistency in research find-
duration, most ABA programming lasts for 2 or ings, it is important that clinicians use clinical
more years. However, complete termination of judgment and consider client and family vari-
clients following treatment is not recommended. ables (e.g., preferences, feasibility, client symp-
Instead, encouraging clients to seek out compre- tom presentation) when deciding on treatment
hensive assessments at certain timepoints over format. Clinic-based, therapist-directed, one-on-­
the lifespan and providing booster sessions as one treatment offers greater environmental con-
needed would be more appropriate. This would trol and thus encourages faster skill acquisition,
help to prevent regression in skills and allow for while home-based, parent-directed treatment and
swift intervention following any new behavioral group therapy offer a greater opportunity for skill
concerns (Matson, Tureck, et al., 2012). generalization to more naturalistic settings and
Given the variability in the research regard- across individuals. Therefore, many ABA pro-
ing optimal treatment intensity and duration, grams use a combination of treatment formats
treatment decisions should be informed by indi- (i.e., a mixture of parent- and therapist-directed
vidual client characteristics and family factors treatment within home- and clinic-based ses-
(Romanczyk et al., 2014). One client variable sions) to take advantage of the benefits of each
that should be considered is age. Granpeesheh approach (Fava & Strauss, 2011).
and colleagues (2009) found a differential
response to varying levels of treatment intensi- Training and Supervision  Most ABA services
ties by age. While children under 7 years old are provided within a tiered framework where a
showed greater levels of skill mastery with BCBA designs a treatment protocol and behavior
increased treatment hours, there was no relation technicians implement the protocol; this aids in
between treatment intensity and number of cost-effectiveness as BCBAs can then manage
objectives mastered in clients over 7 years of several cases simultaneously and behavior tech-
age (Granpeesheh et al., 2009). Further, due to nicians can provide a majority of direct services
the limited data available for very young chil- at lower costs. However, this model brings addi-
dren with ASD, there is no clear start point for tional ethical considerations such as ensuring
when to begin therapy or for how much therapy sufficient training and supervision of technicians
young children should receive. Therefore, as and tracking treatment fidelity in addition to
the average age of ASD diagnosis continues to treatment effectiveness (Fisher et al., 2014;
decrease, more research is warranted focusing Romanczyk et al., 2014). Though the field has
on infants and toddlers in ABA programs historically lacked consensus and formal guide-
(Matson & Konst, 2014). lines regarding necessary skill development for
46 P.E. Cervantes et al.

behavior technicians, the BACB recently intro- analyst (Romanczyk et al., 2014). In regard to
duced the registered behavior technician (RBT) quantity, researchers have shown that supervision
credential in attempts to standardize training of intensity is significantly related to client out-
staff providing these direct services (Fisher et al., comes (Romanczyk et al., 2014). Behavior tech-
2014). Within the RBT credential, the BACB nicians who noted receiving high levels of
requires technicians be trained and assessed in supervisor support also reported less emotional
their knowledge and performance related to mea- exhaustion and a greater sense of accomplish-
surement, skill acquisition and behavior reduc- ment and therapeutic self-efficacy in their work
tion procedures, documentation and reporting, (Gibson, Grey, & Hastings, 2009). However,
and professional conduct (BACB, 2013). This supervisors must be qualified to design treatment
new credential is encouraging. However, like any plans and provide feedback on their implementa-
system-wide change, the RBT certification may tion for supervision intensity to be meaningful. In
take time to be fully adopted by ABA providers practice, supervisors are frequently BCBAs. As
to the point where comprehensive evaluation of previously mentioned, the BCBA is a certificate
improvement in staffing can take place. available through the BACB. This credential is
Additionally, the training required for the beneficial in that it ensures all practicing behav-
RBT credential is not provided directly by the ior analysts are trained in the same content and
BACB; instead, ABA agencies and BCBA super- thus have a more uniform and comprehensive
visors design and carry out their own training skillset when graduated. Individuals seeking the
programs (BACB, n.d.). Though, there is limited BCBA credential must also pass a certification
research available related to best practice for examination assessing an extensive collection of
training intervention agents (e.g., behavior tech- important competencies. Once an individual
nicians, parents) to provide ABA treatment for earns a BCBA, continuing education require-
individuals with ASD (Fisher et al., 2014). At ments exist to ensure the maintenance of profi-
current, a combination of didactic training on the ciency over time. Though the BCBA certification
conceptual bases of ABA treatment and in vivo is useful in providing standardization in training
training on the implementation of treatment plans and practice, a BCBA is not adequate to super-
appears optimal. Understanding the conceptual vise any given case (Shook, 2005). Clinicians are
foundations of ABA strategies is important for ethically required to be aware of their boundaries
problem-solving within intervention sessions of competence; if a client presents with a prob-
when immediate supervision is not available lem the supervisor has little experience in
(Granpeesheh et al., 2010); and, fidelity in con- addressing, the behavior analyst is responsible
ducting intervention plans is imperative for treat- for referring the client to appropriately qualified
ment effectiveness (Fisher et al., 2014; Klintwall professionals and/or seeking supervision from
et al., 2012). In regard to training modalities, evi- qualified individuals on the case (Shook, 2005).
dence exists supporting the use of virtual training Of note, the BACB also offers a Board Certified
programs in improving knowledge of ABA prin- Assistant Behavior Analyst (BCaBA) certifica-
ciples in behavior technicians and parents as well tion that requires an individual hold a bachelor’s
as enhancing the accuracy of treatment delivery degree as opposed to the BCBA’s master’s degree
in behavior technicians (Fisher et al., 2014; requirement. Individuals who earn BCaBAs
Granpeesheh et al., 2010; Jang et al., 2012). practice under the supervision of BCBAs and are
Virtual training appears optimal because it is not responsible for upholding the same ethical stan-
only an effective method of training, but it is also dards of practice.
convenient and accessible (Fisher et al., 2014).
The quantity (i.e., amount and frequency) and Intervention Components  There are several
quality (i.e., supervisor credentials and experi- strategies used within ABA that warrant attention
ence) of supervision are also big factors to con- in regard to ethical practice. The first relates to
sider when practicing ethically as a behavior the functional analysis of potentially harmful
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 47

behaviors (e.g., self-injury, aggression). [DTT]; Poling & Edwards, 2014). In addition, for
Functional analysis is an important assessment decades, researchers have shown that punishment
tool that allows for the experimental determina- procedures are effective in reducing problem
tion of the cause of behavior and involves sys- behavior. Though concerns have been raised
tematically exposing clients to various controlled regarding difficulties with maintenance and gen-
conditions to measure changes in rates of behav- eralization of treatment gains and a potential for
ior. When the function of behavior is able to be negative side effects when using punishment and
determined, controlling variables can then be negative reinforcement procedures, the same
manipulated within an intervention plan to reduce concerns again have been noted for many other
or eliminate problem behavior more effectively; behavior change strategies (Gerhardt, Holmes,
therefore, there are substantial benefits to con- Alessandri, & Goodman, 1991; Poling &
ducting functional analyses. However, the pro- Edwards, 2014).
cess involves temporarily exposing clients to The substantial problem resulting from strict
conditions that will make potentially dangerous opposition to punishment and negative reinforce-
behaviors more likely to occur. Therefore, func- ment procedures relates to the possible failure to
tional analyses should be conducted by compe- provide the most effective treatment available for
tent clinicians when determined necessary (e.g., clients. For example, researchers have found that
when indirect measures fail to produce clear punishment leads to a faster cessation or reduc-
results), and specified termination criteria and tion of problem behavior in comparison to
safeguards should be in place to protect both cli- reinforcement-­ based techniques and therefore
ents and assessors (Poling, Austin, Peterson, may be a better treatment option for intense and
Mahoney, & Weeden, 2012; Poling & Edwards, dangerous self-injurious behavior or aggression
2014). For in depth discussion regarding ethical (Gerhardt et al., 1991). Withholding this treat-
considerations specific to functional analysis, ment option would then be considered unethical.
refer to Poling et al. (2012). On the contrary, the implementation of punish-
The use of punishment in ABA programming ment and negative reinforcement strategies by
has been a center of controversy for some time as untrained professionals holds potential for abuse
well. According to the BACB ethical guidelines, of clients (Gerhardt et al., 1991). Therefore, per-
reinforcement procedures should be employed haps “aversive procedures” need not be restricted
above punishment procedures and, when punish- in practice but better controlled through compre-
ment procedures are implemented, reinforcement-­ hensive training and monitoring of behavior ana-
based procedures should be used concurrently lysts. In sum, clinicians agree that ethical
(BACB, 2014). Further, the implementation of treatment involves special consideration of what
punishment-based strategies in schools and clini- procedures work best for a particular client.
cal settings is restricted, and many advocacy Sometimes, punishment or negative reinforce-
groups strongly oppose the use of punishment. ment procedures may present as the best option
However, many behavior analysts have conflict- available for a given presenting problem (Poling
ing opinions regarding the ethics of punishment; & Edwards, 2014). In these cases, Gerhardt et al.
and, much of this conflict comes from how pun- (1991) recommend reflecting on several points.
ishment is defined (Poling & Edwards, 2014). First, the intent of imposing the discomfort asso-
Punishment and negative reinforcement strate- ciated with the use of punishment and negative
gies are often categorized as “aversive” proce- reinforcement strategies should be considered.
dures because of their potentially unpleasant Second, the risks and benefits of the application
effects to clients. However, researchers and clini- of these procedures should be measured. Lastly,
cians in the field do not agree with this label par- clinicians should ensure appropriate safeguards
ticularly because many behavior change strategies are in place to protect the client.
may produce discomfort or unpleasantness but The last issue that will be discussed related to
clearly benefit clients (e.g., discrete trial training ethical considerations in intervention plan
48 P.E. Cervantes et al.

c­ omponents involves the incorporation of empiri- year; Chasson, Harris, & Neely, 2007; Kornack,
cally unsupported treatments within ABA pro- Persicke, Cervantes, Jang, & Dixon, 2014).
gramming. Although the BACB ethical guidelines While funding sources exist and policies regard-
clearly state that BCBAs must use scientifically ing autism treatment funding are growing in
validated treatments, researchers have shown that prevalence, the financial responsibility is often
a small but concerning percentage of profession- placed on state and federal government bodies,
als reported using unsupported treatments as well private insurance providers, and families of indi-
(BACB, 2014; Schreck & Mazur, 2008; Schreck viduals with ASD. However, acquiring appropri-
& Miller, 2010). Given the increasing number of ate and sufficient funding is a complex task that
individuals seeking BCBA credentials and the often requires great persistence on the part of the
growing number and popularity of unsupported individual’s caregivers (Kornack et al., 2014).
treatments available for autism, Schreck and Clinicians should be cognizant of these difficul-
Mazur (2008) call for the need to improve educa- ties and provide assistance when able. According
tion of BCBAs regarding unsupported interven- to the BACB ethical guidelines, clinicians are
tions to encourage more ethical clinical practice. even ethically responsible for advocating for the
necessary level of services needed to meet inter-
vention goals. However, when unable to achieve
Additional Factors to Consider complete funding, the ethics of providing a treat-
ment intensity that matches the availability of
Family strain is an important variable to consider financial resources rather than the individual’s
when providing treatment to clients with need should be considered.
ASD. Families raising children with ASD report Beyond the financial cost, barriers such as
elevated levels of internalizing symptoms, and long waitlists and a lack of providers in a given
level of parental stress has been shown to effect geographical region are important to consider.
behavioral treatment outcomes (Fava & Strauss, Optimal treatment may not always be accessible,
2011; Schwichtenberg & Poehlmann, 2007). so clinicians are often required to make alterna-
Therefore, the incorporation of family-level tive recommendations. To address the waitlists
intervention components may be important for associated with ABA programs, professionals
effective and ethical treatment delivery. Further, have highlighted the importance of parent train-
fewer depressive symptoms have been reported ing programs that could support caregivers in act-
by mothers of children with ASD who receive ing as intervention agents while waiting for
more hours of ABA therapy per week indicating program enrollment (Vismara et al., 2009).
that ABA programs serve as a resource for fami- Further, we hope that individuals living in rural
lies. However, mothers reported more personal areas will experience improved access to behav-
strain when they spent more hours per week ioral interventions given the growth in virtual
directly involved in their child’s ABA therapy. training opportunities for parents and caregivers
Therefore, parental involvement in therapy as well as the increase in individuals seeking the
should be individualized, and an open line of BCBA certification.
communication should exist between behavior
analysts and parents to ensure productive and
willing caregiver participation in treatment Psychopharmacology
(Schwichtenberg & Poehlmann, 2007).
The financial expense involved in providing Although there are no approved pharmacological
quality, optimal intensity ABA services should treatments specifically targeting the core symp-
also be considered. Though ABA has proven toms of ASD (Mohiuddin & Ghaziuddin, 2013;
cost-effective in the long term for children who Murray et al., 2013; Steckler, Spooren, &
receive early and intensive ABA intervention, Murphy, 2014), pharmacotherapy among indi-
initial costs are substantial ($40,000–100,000 per viduals with ASD is widespread. Studies of
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 49

insurance claim databases have revealed that psy- cations are generally used for their sedative
chotropic drugs are prescribed to the majority of effects rather than their therapeutic effects
children, adolescents, and adults with ASD (Gualtieri & Hawk, 1980; Matson & Mahan,
(Esbensen, Greenberg, Seltzer, & Aman, 2009; 2010; Sturmey, 2015).
Mandell et al., 2008; Williams et al., 2012). The pro re nata (PRN; as needed) use of psy-
Given the high prevalence of psychotherapeutic chotropic medications to calm and sedate indi-
drug use, there is a pressing need for practitioners viduals with developmental disorders is common;
to be aware of the research base, related ethical however, these medications are also used contin-
issues, and practice guidelines for psychophar- uously and as the main form of treatment for
macology among this population. behavioral concerns (Sturmey, 2015). The use of
psychotropic medications has been considered a
form of restraint, as the intention is to control an
Research Base individual’s behavior or movements (Sturmey,
2015); therefore, thoughtful ethical consider-
Psychotropic medications have been found to be ations should be made in the decision-making
the most commonly prescribed class of medica- process of prescribing professionals. The ratio-
tions to individuals with ASD (Esbensen et al., nale behind PRN and routine use of psychotropic
2009; Rosenberg et al., 2009), with rates of pre- drugs to treat challenging behaviors is to increase
scription increasing over time (Aman, Lam, & the safety of the individual and others. However,
Van Bourgondien, 2005). Older ages, co-­ there is limited research to support this justifica-
occurring psychiatric diagnoses, and greater use tion as well as emerging contradictory evidence.
of ASD-related services were found to increase A study found that eliminating the use of PRN in
the likelihood of the prescription of psychotropic a psychiatric hospital over a 15-month period
medication (Mandell et al., 2008). The prescrip- resulted in a reduction in injuries to patients and
tion of psychotropic drugs to very young children staff, rather than an increase (Smith et al., 2008).
is also common. A study of 2008 Medicaid Additionally, longitudinal analysis of prescrip-
claims in the state of Kentucky revealed that psy- tion patterns over 4.5 years revealed that once an
chotropic medications were prescribed to 79% of individual with ASD is prescribed a medication,
children with ASD between 1 and 5 years, 92% it is very unlikely that the prescription will be dis-
between 6 and 12 years, and 95% between 13 and continued (Esbensen et al., 2009). This suggests
18 years (Williams et al., 2012). Non-­psychotropic that pharmacotherapy is seldom used as a tempo-
medications (e.g., anticonvulsants) have also rary treatment option among this population and
been found to be prescribed at high rates among that the initial decision to treat an individual with
this population (Witwer & Lecavalier, 2005). medication has lasting effects.
Psychotropic medications, such as antipsy- Adverse side effects related to the use of psy-
chotics, are commonly used to treat challenging chotropic medication have been widely noted.
behaviors such as aggression and self-injurious These include short-term effects such as irritabil-
behavior among individuals with ASD and other ity and weight gain, as well as long-term side
developmental disorders (de Kuijper et al., effects, such as tardive dyskinesia (Matson &
2010; Matson & Dempsey, 2008; Mohiuddin & Hess, 2011). Risperidone, one of the most com-
Ghaziuddin, 2013). However, many researchers monly prescribed medications in this population,
in the field have noted concerns about the lack has been linked to significant weight gain, drows-
of evidence supporting pharmacological treat- iness, dizziness, and tardive dyskinesia in chil-
ment for challenging behaviors (Deb, Sohanpal, dren with ASD (Lemmon, Gregas, & Jeste, 2011;
Soni, Lentre, & Unwin, 2007; Edelsohn, McCracken et al., 2002). Further, it should be
Schuster, Castelnovo, Terhorst, & Parthasarathy, noted that the long-term effects of psychotropic
2014; Matson & Mahan, 2010; Tsiouris, Kim, medication use begun at young ages and contin-
Brown, Pettinger, & Cohen, 2012). These medi- ued through development are still unknown.
50 P.E. Cervantes et al.

Research on the effects of psychotropic medi- ages and level of functioning. Their recommen-
cation has several major methodological limita- dations are summarized below as well as in
tions that must be mentioned. Most notably, as Table 3.1:
mentioned, the long-term effects of psychotropic
medication are still unknown, especially among 1. Challenging behaviors should be clearly
individuals with ASD. This is particularly con- identified and functional assessment con-
cerning given the young ages at which these med- ducted prior to beginning pharmacological
ications are commonly prescribed. Additionally, treatment. Causes and consequences of the
very little research has been conducted examin- behavior should be determined through a
ing the effects of multiple medications being functional assessment in order to consider all
administered simultaneously. Similarly, there is behavior management options. The benefits
limited research on the use of pharmacological and risks of a behavior management interven-
treatment among individuals with comorbid dis- tion should be considered.
orders, which is problematic given the high rates 2. Medication-based treatments should be con-
at which ASD co-occurs with other disorders and sidered if there is an obvious physical or psy-
medical conditions (Matson & Dempsey, 2008). chiatric cause to a behavior or if a
As many studies on pharmacological treatment non-medication-based intervention poses harm
are funded by pharmaceutical companies, there is or has been unsuccessful. Deb et al. (2009) dis-
also the potential for bias to influence research cuss several situations in which medication
findings (Matson & Konst, 2015). might be considered over non-­pharmacological
treatments, including when a behavior poses a
risk of harm to an individual or others; if the
Guidelines behavior occurs at high severity or frequency;
if an individual is at risk of losing an educa-
Although no professional organizations have for- tional, vocational, or treatment placement due
mal guidelines regarding pharmacological treat- to the behavior; to help increase responsiveness
ment for individuals with developmental to another intervention; or if there is evidence
disabilities, several researchers have put forth that an individual previously responded well to
recommendations. Deb et al. (2009) proposed a medication. The use of medication should
set of guidelines for the use of psychotropic med- always be in the best interest of the individual.
ication specifically in relation to managing chal- 3. The effects of medication should be monitored
lenging behaviors in adults with intellectual at regular intervals. Data on both the effective-
disabilities; however, we believe that they are ness of a medication and its possible negative
useful in relation to individuals with ASD of all effects should be collected regularly and moni-
tored. Further, Deb et al. (2009) recommend
Table 3.1  Guidelines for use of psychotropic medica-
that medications should be prescribed at the
tions to treat challenging behaviors, as adapted from Deb lowest effective dosage within the standard rec-
et al. (2009) ommended dosage range, that doses should be
1. Challenging behaviors should be clearly identified started low and titrated up, that medication
and functional assessment conducted prior to should be used only for the minimum amount of
beginning pharmacological treatment time necessary, and that non-­pharmacological
2. Medication-based treatments should be considered treatment options should be considered through-
if there is an obvious physical or psychiatric cause
to a behavior or if a non-medication-based out the medication management process.
intervention poses harm or has been unsuccessful 4. Communication about the pharmacological
3. The effects of medication should be monitored at treatment should be clear. Caregivers and
regular intervals individuals, to the greatest extent possible,
4. Communication about the pharmacological should be provided information about the
treatment should be clear
pharmacological treatment and the plan for
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 51

medication management. Potential side 2010). For example, holding therapy, secretin
effects should be discussed and appropriate injections, and chelation therapy have all been
actions in response to adverse events presented as potential cures for autism but also
reviewed. Other professionals working with have no empirical evidence for effectiveness and
the ­individual should receive communica- have been linked to serious and in some cases
tions related to the treatment on a “need-to- lethal physical consequences (Metz, Mulick, &
know” basis. Butter, 2005). Obviously, providing these poten-
tially harmful therapies would be considered
Given the range and seriousness of potential unethical, as does failing to inform caregivers of
side effects and the gaps in the literature, it is the risks of these treatment approaches as a pro-
important that clinicians carefully consider the fessional working with clients with autism.
risk/benefit ratio when considering pharmaco- However, an ethical dilemma still exists when
logical treatment with individuals with individuals with ASD are seeking out treatments
ASD. Clinicians and caregivers should be famil- that are not harmful but are also not effective.
iar with the research on specific medications and Such is the case for many fad treatments now
be aware of the potential risks to ensure informed available in the ASD field that are growing in pop-
choice. It is recognized that medication manage- ularity despite having inadequate empirical evi-
ment is appropriate and necessary in the treat- dence (e.g., sensory integration training, Floortime;
ment of certain presentations of ASD (e.g., when Metz et al., 2005; Poling & Edwards, 2014). When
safety is at risk, when challenging behaviors are individuals choose to enroll in programs deliver-
chronic, severe, and unresponsive to prior treat- ing unproven interventions, both time and money
ment; Matson & Dempsey, 2008). Therefore, are poured into approaches that will likely lead to
when pharmacotherapy is deemed an appropriate little improvement. Because time and financial
treatment choice, identifying and continuing to resources are finite, these treatments can be per-
assess the dosage where benefits are maximized ceived as detrimental as well (Shabani & Lam,
while adverse side effects are largely avoided is 2013). This is particularly true given the research
imperative. Further, a plan for future medication indicating that the largest gains are made in ABA
management should be devised proactively. therapy when children are enrolled at younger
Ultimately, as with all treatment, the aim should ages (Smith et al., 2015). Beyond time and finan-
be to maintain benefits while minimizing harm to cial costs to pursuing scientifically unproven but
the greatest extent possible. benign treatments, some treatment methods may
hold other potentially negative side effects such as
social stigmatization (Poling & Edwards, 2014;
Popular Treatments with Minimal Shabani & Lam, 2013). For example, Poling and
Empirical Support Edwards (2014) illustrate the use of weighted
vests as treatment for autism. Though wearing a
Because there are a variety of alternative treat- weighted vest is not necessarily physically damag-
ments available, each with varying levels of ing, it is socially aberrant and will likely affect
empirical support, the ethics surrounding the use peer interactions. Given these issues, professionals
of these interventions in autism treatment are are ethically responsible to inform caregivers
more complicated. Though all interventions that seeking these treatments of the likelihood for
depart from ABA should not be rejected (e.g., improvement as well as the financial and opportu-
speech and language pathology, physical ther- nity costs involved (Poling & Edwards, 2014).
apy), many popular treatments are scientifically Another issue is that a majority of caregivers
unsupported and have been shown to have little-­ choose an eclectic approach to therapy (i.e.,
to-­no efficacy. Some have even caused grave and incorporating components from many different
dangerous side effects for clients with ASD intervention models into one treatment program)
(Poling & Edwards, 2014; Schreck & Miller, and/or use a variety of treatments simultaneously
52 P.E. Cervantes et al.

for their children with ASD (Foxx, 2008; Goin-­ guideline and the ethical obligation to practice
Kochel, Mackintosh, & Myers, 2009). In fact, within one’s boundaries of competence. With the
researchers have shown children with ASD ever-increasing amount of alternative treatments
are receiving on average between four and six developed from a variety of different fields (e.g.,
different interventions simultaneously and have psychopharmacology, medicine, occupational
tried between seven and nine treatments in the therapy), an ethical risk exists for guiding parents
past (Goin-Kochel et al., 2009). The popularity of on and appraising the effects of therapies for
this approach is most likely due to caregiver which the behavior analyst or psychologist has
desire to provide the best for their child com- no training (Poling & Edwards, 2014). This high-
bined with an inaccurate perception that there is lights the importance of being both a competent
utility in every intervention available. However, practitioner and a competent scientist. Effective
there are many drawbacks involved in this clinicians must be able to accurately evaluate rel-
approach. First, the more treatments employed evant research for quality of methodology and
by families of children with ASD, the more likely strength of findings and then successfully inform
an ineffective and potentially harmful interven- caregivers of key conclusions.
tion will be incorporated. Further, receiving While this can be a daunting task for a given
numerous treatments simultaneously may pre- professional, several organizations have sought to
vent or diminish improvement from an effective promote the use of empirically supported autism
intervention because it cannot be provided at the treatments by publishing comprehensive assess-
intensity needed to produce the best outcomes. ments of the strength of evidence for various inter-
Last, separate intervention approaches may vention strategies. For example, the National
restrict or counteract each other’s potential effec- Autism Center has completed two phases of the
tiveness. For example, Floortime and ABA may National Standards Project (NSP) that present the
conflict with one another as Floortime empha- level of research supporting an extensive range of
sizes an unstructured therapeutic environment available ASD interventions. Within the NSP,
and certain components of ABA programming empirical support is evaluated systematically by
value structure in treatment (e.g., visual sched- an expert panel of professionals in the autism field.
ules, DTT; Foxx, 2008). Interventions are classified into three categories
Given these issues with the implementation of and separated by age of clientele targeted
unsupported treatments, it is imperative that pro- (<22 years old and ≥22 years old). The categories
fessionals in the field are able to assist families in are established interventions (i.e., those treatments
treatment choices and equip caregivers with the that have been thoroughly researched and have
skills needed to evaluate intervention options for sufficient evidence for effectiveness), emerging
their children. In fact, both the APA and BACB interventions (i.e., those treatments that have one
ethical standards help to guide professionals or more studies suggesting favorable outcomes but
against unsupported treatments. Both sets of additional high quality studies are necessary to
guidelines state that practitioners should remain indicate effectiveness), and unestablished inter-
aware of scientific knowledge regarding treat- ventions (i.e., treatments that have little to no
ment options, choose treatments based upon sci- research evidence to draw conclusions upon
entific knowledge, and recommend empirically regarding effectiveness; National Autism Center,
supported and effective treatment approaches; 2015). These comprehensive appraisals published
the BACB guidelines go even further to state that by expert groups, like the NSP, help practitioners
behavior analysts should review and appraise in the task of evaluating treatments most likely to
likely effects of all alternative treatments that benefit their clients.
may influence behavior change programs (APA, When families of children with ASD report
2010; BACB, 2014; Schreck & Miller, 2010). already using an unsupported treatment, the best
However, an interesting point raised by Poling practice would again be to inform caregivers of
and Edwards (2014) is the conflict between this level of research evidence for effectiveness.
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 53

Unsupported treatments are most often chosen Table 3.2  Questions to ask regarding specific treatment,
as adapted from Freeman (1997)
due to cost-effectiveness, ease of ­implementation,
and perceived benefits (Schreck & Mazur, 2008; 1.  Will the treatment cause harm?
Shabani & Lam, 2013). Further, caregivers and 2.  Is the treatment developmentally appropriate?
clients may already have strong rapport with cur- 3. How will failure of the treatment affect the
individual and the family?
rent providers; so, recommendations to stop
4.  Has the treatment been scientifically validated?
alternative treatments may be met with some
5. How will the treatment be integrated into the
resistance. In this case, professionals suggest individual’s current program?
either offering to take data or creating a data
recording system for caregivers to systematically
monitor whether the current treatment is leading families and professionals should be actively
to improvements (Goin-Kochel et al., 2009; engaging throughout the course of treatment.
Poling & Edwards, 2014). Concrete evidence When considering treatment approaches, fam-
from their own child’s performance may be more ilies and professionals must consider potential
substantial for parents than evidence from pub- risks, potential benefits, scientific support, and
lished research literature. To further promote the the needs of the individual. With so many factors
use of evidence-based treatments, researchers to assess, this process can be daunting to even
must also continue to compare the effectiveness those who are well-informed. To aid in this pro-
and characteristics of established interventions cess, Freeman (1997) outlined five questions to
versus emerging and unsupported interventions, guide the evaluation a specific treatment
and journals must publish treatment studies with (Table 3.2). First: Will the treatment cause harm?
null results (Schreck & Mazur, 2008; Shabani & Potential physical risks should be carefully con-
Lam, 2013). sidered, as well as potential risks of emotional
distress or social stigmatization. Beyond these
more obvious risks, “harm” can also be concep-
Informed Choice tualized as the failure to improve outcomes. As
discussed earlier, a particular treatment approach
Given the large number of treatments marketed that results in failure to provide an individual
for ASD, and the mass of information and misin- exposure to another treatment that would be more
formation regarding their outcomes, the evalua- beneficial can be considered harmful. The poten-
tion of treatment options can be a complex task tial risks should be carefully weighed against any
for parents and caregivers. As previously potential benefits.
addressed, professionals can help families with The second question proposed by Freeman
this process by providing information about evi- (1997) concerns the fit between the treatment and
dence supporting treatment efficacy, equipping the individual: Is the treatment developmentally
families with the skills necessary to critically appropriate? This necessitates consideration of
evaluate options, and guiding families in the what is appropriate at various points in the lifes-
decision-making process to reach an informed pan as well as what is appropriate for an individ-
choice. Informed choice describes a decision ual given their social context. For example, a
made after full consideration of available infor- treatment approach may be appropriate for a tod-
mation about treatment options along with the dler but inappropriate for a teenager and vice
family’s values (Marteau, Dormandy, & Michie, versa. This question is also important to ask
2001). This is not only important when selecting throughout the course of treatment as an individ-
treatment approaches and methodologies but also ual matures.
in relation to provider selection and determining Thirdly: How will failure of the treatment
treatment goals. Treatment decisions should be affect the individual and the family? ASD is a
continually reassessed as an individual develops; disorder that has lifelong implications for indi-
as such, informed choice is a process in which viduals. If family resources, whether emotional
54 P.E. Cervantes et al.

or financial, are exhausted on a specific treatment treatments that can provide support for the current
that does not deliver the expected results, there needs of an individual.
will be effects on both family functioning and the Treatment decisions should also take into con-
future treatment of the individual. As considering sideration the needs of a family. Interventions
treatment choices within this larger framework of are commonly evaluated based on therapeutic
family functioning and long-term care may not outcomes for the individual with ASD while
be the first inclination of many parents, especially neglecting the overall family context. Each
after first receiving an ASD diagnosis, clinicians family has different strengths, barriers, values,
and treatment providers should help families and resources. Parent and family functioning is
understand this broader context and the accom- often impacted by having a child with ASD, and
panying implications. improvements in these domains have been linked
The fourth question proposed by Freeman to better therapeutic outcomes (Karst & Hecke,
(1997) addresses the quality of the evidence sup- 2012). Decisions about treatment approaches and
porting a treatment: Has the treatment been sci- treatment goals for an individual should be made
entifically validated? As discussed previously, within the context of family functioning and rela-
professionals have a responsibility to inform tionships. Factors to consider include the role of
families about the scientific validity of specific family members within interventions, the effect
treatments. Practitioners should be intentional on allocation of time and resources, the effect on
about helping families navigate and understand parenting stress and mental health, and the priori-
the quality and meaning of available evidence. ties and values of a family. Professionals can help
Often, pseudoscientific claims are made about with this process by assessing family strengths
treatment approaches that can be difficult to dis- and preferences, by discussing the role of the
tinguish from valid scientific evidence. Some family within treatment approaches, and by mon-
“red flags” that may indicate lack of empirical itoring the impact of intervention on family func-
support include treatment efficacy research that tioning during the course of treatment.
is conducted by the same person who founded the Choices about treatment methodologies and
treatment approach, research that is not published goals should be made based on careful consider-
in peer-reviewed journals, and treatments that ation of the empirical evidence and the appropri-
promise quick results (Romanczyk & Gillis, ateness for the individual and family. This is a
2005). If families are interested in experimental complex process, as it involves both subjective
treatments, it is the responsibility of profession- evaluations (e.g., does the treatment approach
als to inform them about potential risks and avail- align with the family’s values?) and objective
able evidence, as well as to manage expectations evaluations (e.g., is the treatment empirically
regarding results. supported?). As such, it may be helpful for both
The fifth question proposed by Freeman (1997) professionals and families to approach informed
for use in evaluating a specific treatment concerns choice in a methodical manner by systematically
the potential effects on overall programming: How identifying the specific needs of an individual,
will the treatment be integrated into the individu- the priorities and preferences of the family, and
al’s current program? Families and professionals the state of the evidence supporting a treatment.
should consider how a new treatment approach
will affect distribution of time and resources for
current and future treatment approaches. Freeman Conclusion
(1997) warns against “infatuation” with a specific
treatment at the expense of treatments that target Treatment decisions for individuals with ASD
functional skills relevant to an individual’s devel- should aim to maximize long-term outcomes
opmental level of functioning. Long-term goals of while minimizing harm. Professionals have an
improving outcomes should be balanced against ethical responsibility to provide empirically sup-
ported treatments, as emphasized by both the
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 55

APA and BACB ethical codes. Currently, ABA adults with intellectual disabilities. World Psychiatry,
8(3), 181–186.
is the only evidence-based treatment option
Deb, S., Sohanpal, S. K., Soni, R., Lentre, L., & Unwin, G.
for children with ASD. A range of ethical con- (2007). The effectiveness of antipsychotic medication
siderations were discussed, including factors in the management of behaviour problems in adults
impacting treatment effectiveness, use of psycho- with intellectual disabilities. Journal of Intellectual
Disability Research, 51(10), 766–777.
pharmacotherapy, and treatment approaches
Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. (1978).
with minimal empirical support. Professionals The Belmont Report [Text]. Retrieved 26 Nov 2016,
should stay abreast of research literature, provide from http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/regulations-and-­
caregivers with information regarding research policy/belmont-report/index.html
Edelsohn, G. A., Schuster, J. M., Castelnovo, K., Terhorst,
evidence for treatment effectiveness as well as
L., & Parthasarathy, M. (2014). Psychotropic prescrib-
potential negative effects, and endeavor to ing for persons with intellectual disabilities and other
support families through the process of informed psychiatric disorders. Psychiatric Services, 65(2),
choice. 201–207.
Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analysis of
early intensive behavioral intervention for children
References with autism. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology, 38(3), 439–450.
Aman, M. G., Lam, K. S. L., & Van Bourgondien, Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
M. E. (2005). Medication patterns in patients with Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2010). Using partici-
autism: Temporal, regional, and demographic pant data to extend the evidence base for intensive
influences. Journal of Child and Adolescent behavioral intervention for children with autism.
Psychopharmacology, 15(1), 116–126. https://doi. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
org/10.1089/cap.2005.15.116. Disabilities, 115(5), 381–405.
American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical Esbensen, A. J., Greenberg, J. S., Seltzer, M. M., & Aman,
principles of psychologists and code of conduct. M. G. (2009). A longitudinal investigation of psycho-
Retrieved 23 May 2016 from http://www.apa.org/eth- tropic and non-psychotropic medication use among
ics/code/principles.pdf adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disor-
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2013). Registered ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
behavior technician (RBT) task list. Retrieved 39(9), 1339–1349.
25 May 2016 from http://bacb.com/wp-content/ Fava, L., & Strauss, K. (2011). Cross-setting complemen-
uploads/2016/03/160321-RBT-task-list.pdf. tary staff- and parent-mediated early intensive behav-
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2014). Professional ioral intervention for young children with autism: A
and ethical compliance code for behavior analysts. research-based comprehensive approach. Research in
Retrieved 23 May 2016 from http://bacb.com/wp- Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 512–522.
content/uploads/2016/03/160321-­compliance-code-­ Fisher, W. W., Luczynski, K. C., Hood, S. A., Lesser,
english.pdf A. D., Machado, M. A., & Piazza, C. C. (2014)
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (n.d.). Registered Preliminary findings of a randomized clinical trial of a
behavior technician (RBT) eligibility requirements. virtual training program for applied behavior analysis
Retrieved 25 May 2016 from http://bacb.com/ technicians. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
rbt-requirements/ 8(9), 1044–1054.
Bölte, S. (2014). Is autism curable? Developmental Foxx, R. M. (2008). Applied behavior analysis treatment
Medicine & Child Neurology, 56(10), 927–911. of autism: The state of the art. Child and Adolescent
Chasson, G. S., Harris, G. E., & Neely, W. J. (2007). Cost Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(4), 821–834.
comparison of early intensive behavioral interven- Freeman, B. J. (1997). Guidelines for evaluating inter-
tion and special education for children with autism. vention programs for children with autism. Journal of
Journal of Child and Family Studies, 16(3), 401–413. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(6), 641–651.
Darrou, C., Pry, R., Pernon, E., Michelon, C., Aussilloux, Gerhardt, P., Holmes, D. L., Alessandri, M., & Goodman,
C., & Baghdadli, A. (2010). Outcome of young children M. (1991). Social policy on the use of aversive inter-
with autism: Does the amount of intervention influence ventions: Empirical, ethical, and legal considerations.
developmental trajectories? Autism, 14(6), 663–677. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Deb, S., Kwok, H., Bertelli, M., Salvador-Carulla, 21(3), 265–277.
L., Bradley, E., Torr, J., & Barnhill, J. (2009). Gibson, J. A., Grey, I. M., & Hastings, R. P. (2009).
International guide to prescribing psychotropic medi- Supervisor support as a predictor of burnout and
cation for the management of problem behaviours in therapeutic self-efficacy in therapists working in
56 P.E. Cervantes et al.

ABA schools. Journal of Autism and Developmental for prescription: Use of antipsychotic drugs in ID in
Disorders, 39(7), 1024–1030. the Netherlands. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Goin-Kochel, R. P., Mackintosh, V. H., & Myers, B. J. Research, 54(7), 659–667.
(2009). Parental reports on the efficacy of treatments Lemmon, M. E., Gregas, M., & Jeste, S. S. (2011).
and therapies for their children with autism spectrum Risperidone use in autism spectrum disorders: A retro-
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, spective review of a clinic-referred patient population.
3(2), 528–537. Journal of Child Neurology, 26(4), 428–432.
Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., Lord, C., Wagner, A., Rogers, S., Szatmari, P., Aman, M.,
& Wilke, A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treat- Charman, T., … Yoder, P. (2005). Challenges in evalu-
ment intensity on behavioral intervention outcomes ating psychosocial interventions for autistic spectrum
for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(4), 1014–1022. Disorders, 35(6), 695–708.
Granpeesheh, D., Tarbox, J., Dixon, D. R., Peters, C. A., Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal
Thompson, K., & Kenzer, A. (2010). Evaluation of educational and intellectual functioning in young
an eLearning tool for training behavioral therapists autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
in academic knowledge of applied behavior analysis. Psychology, 55(1), 3–9.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(1), 11–17. MacDonald, R., Parry-Cruwys, D., Dupere, S., & Ahearn,
Green, G., Brennan, L. C., & Fein, D. (2002). Intensive W. (2014). Assessing progress and outcome of early
behavioral treatment for a toddler at high risk for intensive behavioral intervention for toddlers with
autism. Behavior Modification, 26(1), 69–102. autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Gualtieri, C. T., & Hawk, B. (1980). Tardive dyskinesia 35(12), 3632–3644.
and other drug-induced movement disorders among Mandell, D. S., Morales, K. H., Marcus, S. C., Stahmer,
handicapped children and youth. Applied Research in A. C., Doshi, J., & Polsky, D. E. (2008). Psychotropic
Mental Retardation, 1(1), 55–69. medication use among Medicaid-enrolled children
Jang, J., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Granpeesheh, D., with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 121(3),
Kornack, J., & de Nocker, Y. (2012). Randomized trial e441–e448.
of an eLearning program for training family members Marteau, T. M., Dormandy, E., & Michie, S. (2001). A
of children with autism in the principles and proce- measure of informed choice. Health Expectations:
dures of applied behavior analysis. Research in Autism An International Journal of Public Participation in
Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 852–856. Health Care and Health Policy, 4(2), 99–108.
Jang, J., Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Konst, M. J., Matson, J. L., & Dempsey, T. (2008). Autism spectrum
Cervantes, P. E., & Goldin, R. L. (2014). What are the disorders: Pharmacotherapy for challenging behaviors.
ages of persons studied in autism research: A 20-year Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 20(2), 175–191.
8(12), 1756–1760. Matson, J. L., & Hess, J. A. (2011). Psychotropic drug
Kamio, Y., Haraguchi, H., Miyake, A., Hiraiwa, M. efficacy and side effects for persons with autism
(2015). Brief report: Large individual variation in spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum
outcomes of autistic children receiving low-intensity Disorders, 5(1), 230–236.
behavioral interventions in community settings. Child Matson, J. L., & Konst, M. J. (2014). Early intervention
and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 9(6). for autism: Who provides treatment and in what set-
Karst, J. S., & Hecke, A. V. V. (2012). Parent and fam- tings. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(11),
ily impact of autism spectrum disorders: A review and 1585–1590.
proposed model for intervention evaluation. Clinical Matson, J. L., & Konst, M. J. (2015). Why pharmacother-
Child and Family Psychology Review, 15(3), 247–277. apy is overused among persons with autism spectrum
Klintwall, L., Gillberg, C., Bölte, S., & Fernell, E. (2012). disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 9,
The efficacy of intensive behavioral intervention for 34–37.
children with autism: A matter of allegiance? Journal of Matson, J. L., & Mahan, S. (2010). Antipsychotic drug
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(1), 139–140. side effects for persons with intellectual disabil-
Kornack, J., Persicke, A., Cervantes, P., Jang, J., & Dixon, ity. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6),
D. (2014). Economics of autism spectrum disorders: 1570–1576.
An overview of treatment and research funding. In Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., Turygin, N., Beighley, J., & Rieske,
J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & J. L. Matson R. (2012). Trends and topics in early intensive behav-
(Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for autism ioral interventions for toddlers with autism. Research in
Spectrum disorders (pp. 165–178). New York, NY: Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(4), 1412–1417.
Springer. Matson, J. L., Turygin, N. C., Beighley, J., Rieske, R.,
de Kuijper, G., Hoekstra, P., Visser, F., Scholte, F. A., Tureck, K., & Matson, M. L. (2012). Applied behavior
Penning, C., & Evenhuis, H. (2010). Use of antipsy- analysis in autism spectrum disorders: Recent devel-
chotic drugs in individuals with intellectual disabil- opments, strengths, and pitfalls. Research in Autism
ity (ID) in the Netherlands: Prevalence and reasons Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 144–150.
3  Ethical Considerations Regarding Treatment 57

McCracken, J. T., McGough, J., Shah, B., Cronin, P., disorders enrolled in a national registry, 2007–2008.
Hong, D., Aman, M. G., … McMahon, D. (2002). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Risperidone in children with autism and serious 40(3), 342–351.
behavioral problems. The New England Journal of Sallows, G. O., Graupner, T. D., & MacLean, W. E., Jr.
Medicine, 347(5), 314–321. (2005). Intensive behavioral treatment for children with
Metz, B., Mulick, J. A., & Butter, E. M. (2005). Autism: autism: Four-year outcome and predictors. American
A late-20th-century fad magnet. In J. W. Jacobson, Journal on Mental Retardation, 110(6), 417–438.
R. M. Foxx, & J. A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial Schreck, K. A., & Mazur, A. (2008). Behavior analyst use
therapies for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion of and beliefs in treatments for people with autism.
and science in professional practice (pp. 237–263). Behavioral Interventions, 23(3), 201–212.
New York, NY: CRC Press. Schreck, K. A., & Miller, V. A. (2010). How to behave
Mohiuddin, S., & Ghaziuddin, M. (2013). ethically in a world of fads. Behavioral Interventions,
Psychopharmacology of autism spectrum disorders: A 25(4), 307–324.
selective review. Autism, 17(6), 645–654. Schwichtenberg, A., & Poehlmann, J. (2007). Applied
Murray, M. L., Hsia, Y., Glaser, K., Simonoff, E., Murphy, behaviour analysis: Does intervention intensity
D. G. M., Asherson, P. J., … Wong, I. C. K. (2013). relate to family stressors and maternal well-being?
Pharmacological treatments prescribed to people with Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 51(8),
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in primary health 598–605.
care. Psychopharmacology, 231(6), 1011–1021. Shabani, D. B., & Lam, W. Y. (2013). A review of compar-
National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu- ison studies in applied behavior analysis. Behavioral
sions: National standards project, phase 2. Randolph, Interventions, 28(2), 158–183.
MA: National Autism Center. Shook, G. L. (2005). An examination of the integrity and
Ozonoff, S. (2013). Editorial: Recovery from autism future of the behavior analyst certification board cre-
spectrum disorder (ASD) and the science of hope: dentials. Behavior Modification, 29(3), 562–574.
Editorial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, Smith, G. M., Davis, R. H., Altenor, A., Tran, D. P., Wolfe,
54(2), 113–114. K. L., Deegan, J. A., & Bradley, J. (2008). Psychiatric
Poling, A., Austin, J. L., Peterson, S. M., Mahoney, A., use of unscheduled medications in the Pennsylvania
& Weeden, M. (2012). Ethical issues and consider- state hospital system: Effects of discontinuing the use
ations. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Functional assessment of PRN orders. Community Mental Health Journal,
for challenging behaviors (pp. 213–233). New York, 44(4), 261–270.
NY: Springer. Smith, T., Klorman, R., & Mruzek, D. W. (2015). Predicting
Poling, A., & Edwards, T. L. (2014). Ethical issues in early outcome of community-based early intensive behav-
intervention. In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, ioral intervention for children with autism. Journal of
& J. L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early interven- Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(7), 1271–1282.
tion for autism spectrum disorders (pp. 141–164). Steckler, T., Spooren, W., & Murphy, D. (2014). Autism
New York, NY: Springer. spectrum disorders – an emerging area in psychophar-
Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Corness, M. (2007). Brief macology. Psychopharmacology, 231(6), 977–978.
report: Relative effectiveness of different home-based Sturmey, P. (2015). Reducing restraint and restrictive
behavioral approaches to early teaching intervention. behavior management practices. Cham, Switzerland:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Springer International Publishing.
37(9), 1815–1821. The Nuremberg Code. (1947). British Medical Journal,
Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early 313(7070), 1448.
intensive behavioral intervention for young children Tsiouris, J. A., Kim, S.-Y., Brown, W. T., Pettinger, J., &
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Cohen, I. L. (2012). Prevalence of psychotropic drug use
Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 512–520. in adults with intellectual disability: Positive and nega-
Romanczyk, R. G., Callahan, E. H., Turner, L. B., & tive findings from a large scale study. Journal of Autism
Cavalari, R. N. S. (2014). Efficacy of behavioral inter- and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 719–731.
ventions for young children with autism spectrum Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic inter-
disorders: Public policy, the evidence base, and imple- vention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis,
mentation parameters. Review Journal of Autism and meta-regression and dose–response meta-analysis
Developmental Disorders, 1(4), 276–326. of multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review,
Romanczyk, R. G., & Gillis, J. M. (2005). Treatment 30(4), 387–399.
approaches for autism: Evaluating options and mak- Vismara, L. A., Colombi, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2009). Can
ing informed choices. In D. Zager & D. Zager (Eds.), one hour per week of therapy lead to lasting changes
Autism spectrum disorders: Identification, education, in young children with autism? Autism, 13(1), 93–115.
and treatment (3rd ed.pp. 515–535). Mahwah, NJ: Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-Feig,
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. J. H., Glasser, A., & Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2011).
Rosenberg, R. E., Mandell, D. S., Farmer, J. E., Law, J. K., A systematic review of early intensive intervention
Marvin, A. R., & Law, P. A. (2009). Psychotropic for autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 127(5),
medication use among children with autism spectrum e1303–e1311.
58 P.E. Cervantes et al.

Williams, P. G., Woods, C., Stevenson, M., Davis, D. W., spectrum disorders. Journal of Child and Adolescent
Radmacher, P., & Smith, M. (2012). Psychotropic Psychopharmacology, 15(4), 671–681.
medication use in children with autism in the World Medical Association. (1964). Declaration
Kentucky Medicaid population. Clinical Pediatrics, of Helsinki – ethical principles for medical
51(10), 923–927. research involving human subjects. Helsinki,
Witwer, A., & Lecavalier, L. (2005). Treatment incidence Finland. Retrieved from ­http://www.wma.net/
and patterns in children and adolescents with autism en/30publications/10policies/b3/
Institutional Review Boards
and Standards 4
Claire O. Burns, Esther Hong, and Dennis R. Dixon

disseminating findings. Prior to the twentieth cen-


History of IRB tury, there were no established ethical guidelines or
governing bodies to regulate research ethics.
Ethics are norms of conduct for distinguishing Consequently, the history of research with human
between right and wrong and between what is subjects has been fraught with injustice and mis-
acceptable and unacceptable behavior. Given that conduct. This history has contributed to the devel-
there are different norms across disciplines, profes- opment of several guidelines for the protection of
sions, and institutions, it is no surprise that there human subject research and the establishment of
have been many ethical disputes and issues institutional review boards (IRBs).
throughout the history of research. Ethical guide- The need for ethical guidelines was formally
lines are necessary to ensure that researchers and recognized following the Nuremberg trials, in
practitioners adhere to the same standards of 1945–1946, in which German physicians were
behavior and that the rights of human subjects are indicted for conducting medical experiments on
protected. Further, governing bodies are required to prisoners of war (“Nürnberg trials,” 2015). As
enforce ethical guidelines, so that researchers are documented in the trials, prisoners were sub-
held accountable for their actions. Without such jected to egregious abuses of medical experimen-
guidelines, there would be no way to regulate how tation, without prior consent or free choice to
researchers are designing experiments, recruiting withdraw from experimentation. In 1949, The
participants, determining risks and benefits, and Nuremberg Code was created to provide a clear
standard of ethical principles for research on
human subjects (U.S. Department of Health and
Human Services [DHHS], 2005). The first point
of the Nuremberg Code states that “the voluntary
C.O. Burns consent of the human subject is absolutely essen-
Center for Autism and Related Disorders,
21600 Oxnard Street, Suite 1800, Woodland Hills, tial” and describes the capacity an individual
CA 91367, USA must possess to consent to research. The other
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State points demand the protection of human subjects,
University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA identification of potential benefits and risks,
E. Hong • D.R. Dixon (*) sound experimental design, qualified scientific
Center for Autism and Related Disorders, personnel, and the right of the subject to with-
21600 Oxnard Street, Suite 1800, Woodland Hills, draw from experiment. The Nuremberg Code
CA 91367, USA provided researchers with a comprehensive
e-mail: d.dixon@centerforautism.com

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 59


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_4
60 C.O. Burns et al.

guideline and laid the foundation for ethical e­ xperimental drugs was not strictly controlled or
experimentation with human subjects. thoroughly tested for potential side effects. In
The Nuremberg Code became an accepted response to this tragedy, the United States passed
guideline for ethical research practices, but the the Kefauver-Harris Amendment in 1962 (Clarke,
application of the guidelines was not enforced. 2012). The Kefauver-Harris Amendment led to
The duty and responsibility of implementing the stronger US Food and Drug Administration
provisions were placed upon the individual(s) (FDA) regulations and changed the way drugs are
engaged with the experiment. That is to say, the approved and regulated.
researchers were expected to follow the code as a In 1963, studies were conducted at the Jewish
matter of honor. Without an independent govern- Chronic Disease Hospital to understand the
ing body to oversee the application of the body’s ability to reject cancer cells. Hospitalized
Nuremberg Code, there was no way to ensure patients, with various chronic debilitating dis-
that researchers were adhering to ethical research eases, were injected with live cancer cells.
practices. Multiple studies over the following Researchers claimed to have obtained oral con-
20 years demonstrated the need for additional sent from the patients, but some critics argue that
oversight and specific protections. it would not be possible to get proper informed
An example of how the system of self-­regulated consent from a “senile” population (Hornblum,
research ethics failed is seen in the Willowbrook 2013). Though researchers received consent from
Hepatitis Experiments. The experiments began in study participants, they did not notify the sub-
1954 and involved more than 700 mentally dis- jects of the injection of cancer cells. The ramifi-
abled children housed in the Willowbrook State cations of these studies were a subsequent review
School (National Institutes of Health [NIH], 2009; proceeding by the Board of Regents of the State
Robinson & Unruh, 2008). Researchers injected University of New York, in which the researchers
subjects with the hepatitis virus to test the effects of were found guilty of fraud, deceit, and personal
the disease and immunity. This study raised several conduct (Freeze, 2014).
concerns, including the infection of a vulnerable As public awareness of the unethical treat-
population (i.e., mentally disabled children) and ment of human subjects increased, so did the
recruitment methods. It was not necessary to study pressure on the medical community to strengthen
hepatitis in children before studying it in adults. human research regulations. In 1964, the 18th
Indeed, researchers could have injected the 1,000 World Medical Association (WMA) General
adult staff members of the Willowbrook School, Assembly met in Helsinki, Finland, and issued
but no adults were included in the study. Instead, it the Declaration of Helsinki (1964). The
is likely that children were the focus of the study Declaration of Helsinki expanded upon the
due simply to the ease of access. In addition, par- Nuremberg Code by providing specific ethical
ents were unduly influenced to consent for their guidelines for physicians conducting medical
child to participate in the study, in exchange for research involving human subjects. The
admission to the overcrowded school. Though par- Declaration of Helsinki states that it is the physi-
ents gave consent, their willingness to give consent cians’ mission to “safeguard the health of the
was called into question, for they had no choice but people” and to protect the well-being and best
to participate if they wanted care for their child. interest of patients (1964). The document also
Another instance in which a vulnerable popu- addresses the risks, burdens, benefits, scientific
lation was affected occurred in the late 1950s to requirements and research protocols, vulnerable
early 1960s. Thousands of pregnant women were populations, patient rights, privacy, informed
given an experimental medication called thalido- consent, and more. The Declaration of Helsinki
mide (Kim & Scialli, 2011). Thalidomide was has undergone several revisions since its
used to control sleep and nausea throughout preg- inception and continues to be modified today
­
nancy, but the drug was later found to cause birth (WMA, 2013).
defects in approximately 10,000 babies (Kim & As reforms were being made to research eth-
Scialli, 2011). During this time, the use of ics, the public became aware of the most ­notorious
4  Institutional Review Boards 61

example of medical misconduct and human populations. Just ways distribute benefits and
rights, which prompted the US Congress to take burdens (1) to each person an equal share, (2) to
further action. The Tuskegee Syphilis Study, each person according to individual need, (3) to
which occurred between 1932 and 1972, was each person according to individual effort, (4) to
conducted by the US Public Health Service and each person according to society contributions,
studied the natural progression of untreated syph- and (5) to each person according to merit. This
ilis in 600 low-income, African-American men principle ensures that the burdens of research are
(Centers for Disease Control and Prevention not placed on vulnerable, disadvantaged popula-
[CDC], 2016). By 1947, penicillin had become a tions, while the benefits are disseminated to priv-
proven cure for syphilis, but for the sake of the ileged populations.
research study, treatment was withheld from the The National Research Act and the release of
study participants and their families. Rather than the Belmont Report prompted the establishment
minimizing risks to human subjects, this study of IRBs and required IRB review and approval of
placed the burden of risk on one disadvantaged, all federally funded research involving human
vulnerable population. subjects.
In response to the Tuskegee tragedy, the US
Congress passed the National Research Act of
1974 and formed the National Commission for Current IRB
the Protection of Human Subject of Biomedical
and Behavioral Research (DHHS, 1979). In Institutional review boards are review commit-
1979, the commission published the Belmont tees whose purpose is to protect the rights and
Report, a statement of ethical guidelines and welfare of human participants in research. IRBs
principles for research involving human subjects. are federally mandated, locally administered
It is a comprehensive set of rules established to groups from different institutions, so the specific
protect the rights and welfare of human subjects. guidelines may differ. The guidelines outlined in
The Belmont Report has been widely accepted this chapter are based on the US Department of
by federal department and agencies and is the Health and Human Services (DHHS) Regulations
cornerstone reference for IRBs. The Belmont and are specified in Title 45 Code of Federal
Report established three principles essential to Regulations (CFR) Part 46 (DHHS, 2009).
the ethical conduct of research with human sub- IRB approval is required for all research that
jects: (1) respect for persons, (2) beneficence, involves human subjects. Human subjects
and (3) justice. research encompasses all systematic investiga-
Respect for persons is divided into two separate tions designed to expand or contribute to general-
requirements: that individuals be treated as auton- izable knowledge which involves obtaining
omous agents and individuals with diminished identifiable private information or data through
autonomy are entitled to protection. Respect for intervention or interaction with an individual
persons demands that human subjects must enter (§46.101 DHHS, 2009). However, research does
research voluntarily (i.e., informed consent), only not qualify if the information collected is not pri-
after being provided with adequate information vate or individually identifiable. The DHHS
(e.g., risk and benefits). guidelines apply to all human subjects research
Beneficence maximizes the benefits to human that is funded, conducted, or regulated by the
subjects, society, and science while reducing government. Research that does not fall under the
potential risk and harm. This principle states CFR is regulated by federal, state, or local laws
that researchers have an obligation to consider and guidelines (see http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/
the benefits for the individual subject as well as policy/checklists/decisioncharts.html; §46.101
the societal benefits that might be gained from DHHS, 2009) and must be approved and moni-
the research. tored by the institution’s IRB. Given that IRBs
Justice demands the fair distribution of are locally administered, the requirements for
research benefits and burdens among various research at different institutions may vary.
62 C.O. Burns et al.

The primary goals of the IRB process are to when participants could be susceptible to coer-
ensure that risks to subjects are minimized; these cion or undue influence. Researchers must pro-
risks are reasonable compared to anticipated ben- vide procedures and proper documentation for
efits; participant selection is equitable; each par- informed consent, as well as assent if the study
ticipant gives informed consent; this consent is involves minors or cognitively impaired indi-
documented properly; when necessary data is viduals (§46.111 DHHS, 2009). The application
monitored to protect the safety of the participant; should also include a plan for collection, stor-
and there are sufficient plans to protect the privacy age, analysis, and disclosure of data that ensures
and confidentiality of the participants (§46.111 that the individuals’ safety, privacy, and confi-
DHHS, 2009). IRBs have the authority to approve, dentiality are protected (American Psychological
require modifications in order to approve, disap- Association [APA], 2002). Privacy refers to
prove, conduct continuing reviews, suspend or issues related to methods used to obtain infor-
terminate approval, verify changes, and observe mation about participants, while confidentiality
the consent process and research procedures of all refers to methods used to ensure that the
research activities that fall under the IRB. Each obtained information is not improperly divulged.
research project is continually reviewed at certain IRBs should ensure that proper precautions are
intervals, which are determined based on the taken to avoid invading the privacy or breaching
degree of risk, but must occur at least once per the confidentially of the participant (OHRP,
year (§46.109 DHHS, 2009). 1993).

IRB Composition Types of IRB Review

The composition of the IRB depends on the type of Full/Convened Committee Review
review necessary for the research. The IRB is com-
prised of a total of five members, and there must be A full committee review is the standard type of
members of both sexes that come from varied pro- IRB review and must be used for the initial review
fessions. These professions should include at least of all studies that are not eligible for expedited
one whose profession is in a scientific field, one review or exemptions. The research must receive
whose profession is not in a scientific field, and at approval from the majority of the members pres-
least one member who is not affiliated with the ent at the meeting (§46.108 DHHS, 2009). These
institution besides serving on the IRB. The IRB types of reviews are used for studies that pose
may also invite experts to help review projects more than minimal risk to the participants, with
when appropriate (§46.107 DHHS, 2009). minimal risk is defined as the “probability and
magnitude of harm or discomfort anticipated in
the research are not greater in and of themselves
IRB Requirements than those ordinarily encouraged in daily life or
during the performance of routine physical or
The IRB requires that researchers identify and psychological examinations or tests” (§46.303
assess any risks and anticipated benefits associ- DHHS, 2009).
ated with the study. Risk to research participants
is classified as physical, psychological, social,
or economic and must be minimized and shown Expedited Review
to be reasonable when compared to potential
benefits (Office for Human Research Protections Expedited review is used for either research that
[OHRP], 1993). Selection of participants must involves no more than minimal risk or for minor
be equitable and take into account any vulnera- changes in previously approved research. Rather
ble populations involved in the research. than convening a full committee for review, the
Additional safeguard should be implemented IRB chairperson can complete the expedited
4  Institutional Review Boards 63

review, or the chairperson can designate one or 1.


When done in established educational
more experienced reviewers to complete the contexts;
review. The reviewer has the authority to approve, 2. Use surveys, educational tests, interviews, or
require modifications, or refer the research to a observation of behavior in a public setting.
convened IRB meeting. However, a research 3. Subjects are public officials or candidates who
study cannot be disapproved during an expedited have been elected.
review; it must be referred to a full/convened IRB 4. Includes openly available de-identified infor-
meeting review before the study can be disap- mation such as records, specimens, data, or
proved (§46.110 DHHS, 2009). documents.
There are nine categories of research that can 5. Conducted by officials in government depart-
be reviewed by the IRB through expedited review ments or agencies that are involved in the
(from website; OHRP, 1998): evaluation of public programs.
6. Evaluation of food and consumer reception.
1. Clinical studies.
2. Collection of blood samples. Research involving at-risk populations such
3. Collection of specimens through noninvasive as prisoners and children is usually not qualified
procedures. for exemption.
4. Data collection obtained by commonly used
noninvasive procedures.
5.
Collection information for non-research Reports
purposes.
6. Collection of recordings (e.g., voice, video) Once a research study has received IRB approval,
for research purposes. additional reviews may be necessary. The pri-
7. Involving individual or group characteristics mary investigator (PI) is accountable for the
or behavior or “research employing survey, research and for reporting any changes to the
interview, oral history, focus group, program approved protocol or unanticipated problems to
evaluation, human factors evaluation, or qual- the IRB. The PI is responsible for understanding
ity assurance methods” (OHRP, 1998). IRB requirements for reporting, occurrences in
8. Continuing review or research previously
which it is necessary, and the process for submit-
approved by IRB, certain provisions are met, ting the report. The PI must also set up a system
such as when the study is permanently closed that guarantees that any events that result in a
to new enrollment, no participants have been deviation from the approved protocol are
enrolled, and the only remaining tasks is data ­identified and submitted to the IRB in a timeline
entry. manner. Reports are often required if there is an
9. Continuing review of research that has already occurrence of adverse events or unanticipated
been deemed to be no more than minimal risk. issues involving risk to the participants or others,
incidents of noncompliance, complaints, devia-
tions from the approved protocol, and violations
Exemption from Review of the terms of approval. The IRB may also
require reports summarizing data safety and
Some human participant research may be monitoring (§46.103 DHHS, 2009).
exempt from IRB monitoring when it involves
no more than minimal risk and falls into one of
the six categories described below. The research  dditional Protections for Vulnerable
A
must be submitted to the IRB in order to receive Subjects
exempt status, but following exemption is no
longer subject to IRB review. There are six cat- Although standard regulations are sufficient for
egories of research that are eligible for exemp- most research, some studies involve participants
tion status (OHRP, 1998): who are at a higher risk of harm or have
64 C.O. Burns et al.

q­ uestionable ability to consent. In addition to the or whereabouts are unknown, if he is not reason-
basic federal regulations under the Common ably available (§46.204 DHHS, 2009), if he does
Rule (DHHS regulations, Subpart A), certain not acknowledge that he is the father of the fetus,
populations qualify for additional protections or if he does not accept responsibility for the
that aim to ensure that all research is conducted fetus (OHRP; 1993). Additionally, if the preg-
in an ethical manner. Many of these populations nancy was the result of rape or incest, paternal
have been exploited in research in the past, such consent is not required (§46.204 DHHS, 2009).
as the studies mentioned previously in this chap- A neonate is a newborn fetus, and nonviable
ter, so these protections are in place to guard neonates and those of uncertain viability may be
against future unfair treatment. There are participants in research under Subpart B (§46.205
several categories of vulnerable subjects that are DHHS, 2009). Viability is the ability of the fetus
given special consideration by IRBs, including to survive and maintain heartbeat and respiration
fetuses, pregnant women, children and minors, independently. Neonates of uncertain viability
cognitively impaired persons, and prisoners. can participate in research if there is no addi-
Additional categories include traumatized and tional risk or if the research may enhance the pos-
comatose patients, terminally ill patients, elderly sibility of survival to the point of viability.
persons, minorities, and students, employees, and Nonviable fetuses can be involved in research if
normal volunteers, among others (OHRP, 1993). the procedures will not cause the vital functions
to be artificially maintained or cause the termina-
tion of the heartbeat or respiration, if there will
 regnant Women, Human Fetuses,
P not be an additional risk to the neonate, and if the
and Neonates research will add to biomedical knowledge which
cannot be attained by other means. If the fetus is
Subpart B of the DHHS regulations set forth addi- viable, research requirements fall under the cate-
tional protections for human fetuses, pregnant gory of additional protections for children
women, and neonates involved in research (DHHS, 2009).
(§46.201–46.207 DHHS, 2009). It is usually Research on fetuses that are not living and
required that appropriate studies have been previ- fetal material (§46.206 DHHS, 2009) is not regu-
ously conducted on animals and nonpregnant indi- lated by Subpart B and is conducted based on
viduals. The risk to the fetus must be minimal if federal, state, or local laws. This usually falls
there is no benefit to the health of the fetus or under the Uniform Anatomical Gift Act and
mother and if the purpose of the study is to advance requires parental consent.
biomedical knowledge. If there is greater than
minimal risk, the study may be allowed if there is
a potential benefit to the mother or fetus and if Prisoners
there is the least amount of risk to fetus as possi-
ble. Additionally, all research that involves in vitro According to Subpart C (§46.301–46.306 DHHS,
fertilization (IVF) must be reviewed by a national 2009) of the DHHS regulations, a prisoner is
Ethics Advisory Board (§46.204 DHHS, 2009). defined as any individual involuntarily confined
The consent process is also slightly different or detained in a penal institution. There are addi-
for research involving pregnant women and tional regulations in place to restrict prisoners
fetuses. Only the mother’s consent is necessary if from participating in research, since they are
the research poses minimal risk to the fetus or under constraints that could affect their ability to
direct benefit to the mother. However, if the make decisions to consent to participation
research has the potential to benefit only the (§46.305 DHHS, 2009). The goal of these limita-
fetus, the consent of both the mother and father is tions is to restrict research with prisoners to
required. The only circumstances in which the research that is related to the prisoner’s lives.
father’s consent is not required are if his identity Therefore researchers cannot recruit prisoners
4  Institutional Review Boards 65

simply because they are more convenient to All research with children must include
access; instead, the study must be designed to appropriate procedures to obtain permission
answer questions about prisoners. from parents and assent from the child (§46.404
Prisoners can be involved in four different DHHS, 2009). Research that poses more than
types of research. The first is research that studies minimal risk may be approved if it presents a
the possible causes, effects, and processes of direct benefit to the participant. The potential
incarceration and criminal behavior. The second benefit must be great enough that it justifies any
is research that studies prisons as institutions or potential risks, and the benefit must be at least
prisoners as incarcerated individuals. Research in equal to those presented by alternative treat-
both the first and second category must not involve ments or approaches. If the research does not
more than minimal risk and disturbance to the present a direct benefit to the participant, it may
participants. The third is research on conditions still qualify for approval if the procedure may
that impact prisoners as a group that exhibits cer- result in a generalizable knowledge about the
tain characteristics more than the general popula- participant’s disorder or condition. Research in
tion (i.e., research on alcoholism, drug addiction, this category must also only pose a minor
and sexual assault). Lastly, prisoners can partici- increase over minimal risk, and the experiences
pate in research if the aim is to investigate prac- involved must be relatively comparable to expe-
tices that could improve the health or well-being riences encountered in their everyday life (i.e.,
of the participants (§46.306 DHHS, 2009). medical, dental, psychological, social, or educa-
tional situations; §46.406 DHHS, 2009). Lastly,
research that does not qualify under the previ-
Children ously stated conditions may be considered for
approval if the research demonstrates an oppor-
Subpart D (§46.401–46.409 DHHS, 2009) pro- tunity to further the understanding, prevention,
vides additional provisions for children in or treatment of a condition or problem that
research. These include restrictions of criteria affects children (§46.407 DHHS, 2009).
for exemption when children are participants,
classification of four levels of risks/benefits,
specifications for parental permission and the Cognitively Impaired Individuals
child’s assent (based on the level), and criteria
for waivers of consent and assent. Children are Although there are no additional DHHS regula-
defined as individuals who are not yet of legal tions for individuals who are cognitively
age of consent under the applicable laws, which impaired, researchers should address additional
differ by state (§46.402 DHHS, 2009). In most considerations when conducting research that
states, the age of majority is 18, so anyone 17 involves these individuals as participants. Since
and under is considered a child in regard to there is no clear consensus on the acceptable
research on human subjects. amount of risk for this population, IRBs should
The consent process is also different for chil- consider consulting experts in the appropriate
dren participating in research. Since children are field (OHRP, 1993). A primary concern for indi-
unable to give informed consent, parents give viduals with psychiatric, cognitive, or develop-
permission for their children to participate and mental disorders is that they may not be able to
children provide their assent, which is an agree- fully understand the information given about the
ment to participate. While children are not legally study and make an informed decision about
able to give consent, they can assent to participa- whether or not to participate. Individuals, who
tion, especially if the research does not involve have been determined incompetent or unable to
treatment that may benefit the participant or if the manage their own affairs and make major deci-
child can understand what they are agreeing to sions for themselves, have a court-appointed
(§46.408 DHHS, 2009). guardian who is responsible for making all of
66 C.O. Burns et al.

their legal decisions, including participation in Clarke, P. (2012). 50 Years: The Kefauver-Harris
Amendments. Retrieved from http://www.fda.gov/
research (OHRP, 1993).
Drugs/NewsEvents/ucm320924.htm
Freeze, S. (2014). Jewish chronic disease hospi-
tal [Prezi presentation]. Retrieved from https://
Final Thoughts prezi.com/3datqmxu0gms/jewish-chronic-
disease-hospital/
Hornblum, A. M. (2013, December 28). NYC’s
Though IRBs oversee studies in an attempt to forgotten cancer scandal. New York Post.
ensure ethical research practices, researchers also Retrieved from http://nypost.com/2013/12/28/
have a moral obligation to protect the rights and nycs-forgotten-cancer-scandal/
Kim, J. H., & Scialli, A. R. (2011). Thalidomide: The
safety of participants. Studies that involve indi-
tragedy of birth defects and the effective treatment of
viduals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) disease. Toxicological Sciences, 122(1), 1–6.
require particular consideration as these individ- Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Williams, L. W., & Rieske,
uals generally fall under the category of vulnera- R. D. (2013). Why are there so many unsubstantiated
treatments in autism? Research in Autism Spectrum
ble populations.
Disorders, 7(3), 466–474.
Children with ASD are typically classified as National Institutes of Health (NIH). (2009). Willowbrook
members of vulnerable populations on multiple hepatitis experiments. In Exploring bioethics
levels, which make additional protections neces- (pp. 294–297). Retrieved from https://science.educa-
tion.nih.gov/supplements/nih9/bioethics/guide/pdf/
sary to safeguard the rights of these individuals.
teachers_guide.pdf
Previous violations of ethical rights of cogni- Nürnberg trials. (2015). In Encyclopedia Britannica
tively impaired children, such as the Willowbrook online. Retrieved from http://www.brittanica.com/
State School studies (Robinson & Unruh, 2008), event//Nurnberg-trials
Office for Human Research Protections (OHRP). (1993).
suggest that parents of children with develop-
Institutional Review Board Guidebook. Retrieved
mental disabilities may be particularly suscepti- from http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/archive/irb/irb_guide-
ble to coercion or undue influence. This is a book.htm
noteworthy concern as there are so many unsub- Office for Human Rights Protection (OHRP). (1998).
Federal Register, Volume 63, Number 216. Retrieved
stantiated treatments for ASD (Matson, Adams,
from http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/policy/63fr60364.html#
Williams, & Rieske, 2013), and parents’ decision Robinson, W. M., & Unruh, B. T. (2008). The hepatitis
regarding participation in research may be influ- experiments at the Willowbrook state school. In E. J.
enced by their desire for their child to receive Emanuel, R. A. Crouch, C. Grady, R. K. Lie, F. G.
Miller, & D. Wendler (Eds.), The Oxford textbook of
treatment. Researchers therefore have a particu-
clinical research ethics (pp. 80–85). New York, NY:
lar responsibility to conduct research that Oxford University Press.
involves children with ASD with the upmost con- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS).
sideration for the well-being of the participants, (1979). The Belmont Report. Retrieved from http://www.
hhs.gov/ohrp/humansubjects/guidance/belmont.html
as well as avoid undue influence over the deci-
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS).
sion to participate. (2005). The Nuremberg code. Retrieved from http://
www.hhs.gov/ohrp/archive/nurcode.html
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (DHHS).
(2009). Code of Federal Regulations, Title 45 Part 46.
References Retrieved from http://www.hhs.gov/ohrp/humansub-
jects/guidance/45cfr46.html#46.101
American Psychological Association. (2002). Ethical World Medical Association (WMA). (1964). Deklaration
principles of psychologists and code of conduct. von Helsinki 1964. Retrieved from https://www.aix-­
American Psychologist, 57(12), 1060–1073. scientifics.com/en/_helsinki64.html
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). World Medical Association (WMA). (2013). World
(2016). U.S. Public Health Service Syphilis Study at Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical
Tuskegee. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/tuske- principles for medical research involving human sub-
gee/timeline.htm jects. JAMA, 310(2), 2191–2194.
Informed Consent
5
Robert D. Rieske, Stephanie C. Babbitt,
Joe H. Neal, and Julie A. Spencer

Introduction dents struggled to identify their ability to refuse


participation when parents gave consent, as well
Within the field of psychology, it is a well-­ as their right to receive information regarding
founded practice to obtain informed consent procedures and to be protected from harm. Fourth
from all individuals who may participate in grade students struggled to comprehend even the
research or receive therapeutic services. This more basic components of the informed consent
requires individuals to agree to the terms and process and evinced impaired judgments regard-
conditions through written and/or verbal consent. ing the consent process (Bruzzese & Fisher,
However, this procedure is rooted in the underly- 2003). Other examples include impaired judg-
ing assumption that individuals clearly compre- ment in individuals with serious medical condi-
hend all aspects of the study/treatment such as tions due to physical and/or emotional distress
the associated risks and benefits, rights of partici- (Casarett, Karlawish, & Hirschman, 2003) and
pation (e.g., permitted to withdraw at any time), impaired understanding of the right to refuse par-
and the purpose of the study/treatment. ticipation for minority members with a history of
While it stands uncontended that this is a fixed societal oppression and increased susceptibility
and essential (i.e., legal, ethical) aspect of the for compliance to authoritative entities (Fisher
process, what has received less attention are ways et al., 2002).
in which informed consent may differ for vulner- Apropos of this literature, individuals with
able populations (e.g., intellectually disabled). developmental disabilities represent a population
Research has demonstrated impaired decision-­ that may also present with unique challenges in
making in relation to informed consent in several regard to informed consent due to impaired cog-
vulnerable populations. For example, Bruzzese nition, thus warranting further attention and
and Fisher (2003) found that seventh grade stu- greater amounts of recognition and protection. In
order to fully assess the benefits, shortcomings,
and special considerations of informed consent
as it is currently implemented, it is prudent to first
understand the development, specifically the
R.D. Rieske, PhD (*) long-standing history of maltreatment and
Department of Psychology, Idaho State University, exploitation of underprivileged populations.
Pocatello, ID 83209, USA The evolution of informed consent can be traced
e-mail: riesrobe@isu.edu
back to a series of historical events that evidence
S.C. Babbitt • J.H. Neal • J.A. Spencer disregard and maltreatment of i­ ndividuals involved
Idaho State University, Pocatello, ID, USA

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 67


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_5
68 R.D. Rieske et al.

in empirical investigations. During World War II, the intent to advance innovative science. The injuri-
experimental science was conducted on susceptible ous effects and lack of concern for general human
populations. Throughout this significant epoch, welfare highlight notable flaws of the scientific
atrocious acts were committed on individuals of approach and necessitated greater acknowledgment
vulnerable populations (e.g., individuals with men- of the importance of informed consent.
tal illness and intellectual disabilities) in the guise Consequently, the Helsinki Declaration (1964) was
of “studies” led by Nazi officials. Emanating from implemented in order to modify and extend the
this maltreatment was the Nuremberg Code; estab- principles put forth in the Nuremberg Code. The
lished in 1949, this doctrine identified ten princi- Helsinki Declaration heavily focused on clinical
ples of acceptable experimentation. While a research, and one of the most notable additions of
noteworthy beginning of creating guidelines, the the Helsinki Declaration was the acknowledgment
Nuremberg Code was criticized for its firm asser- that individuals with limited capacity to consent
tion of “voluntary consent” as a core tenant. should be provided the assistance of a proxy deci-
Unfortunately, this principle failed to account for sion-maker. Additionally, it averred the requirement
individuals with intellectual disabilities who may that all study participants be fully informed of diag-
not have the capacity to (a) fully understand and nostic and/or therapeutic methods (Weiss-Roberts
thus (b) provide voluntary consent as a truly & Roberts, 1999). By 1979, the momentum of pro-
informed decision (Brody, 1998; Sturman, 2005). tecting human subjects reached its pinnacle with the
Following the Nuremberg Code, ethical guide- seminal document known as the Belmont Report.
lines were seemingly halted in the nascent stage, as The report defined three imperative principles to
disquieting experiments were regularly conducted in ethically guide any and all research endeavors. The
the United States. One such example is the numer- overall impact of the Belmont Report was a distinct
ous human radiation experiments that were con- assertion that informed consent was a foundational
ducted during the Cold War, the aim of which was to component of conducting ethical and sound
examine the effects of radiation exposure (e.g., research on human subjects.
atomic radiation, radioactive contamination). In Taken together, the historical components of
these experiments, thousands of individuals were informed consent clearly demonstrate a long-­
exposed to the aforementioned hazardous chemicals standing pattern of disregard, maltreatment, and
in attempts to understand their influence on the exploitation of many individuals, specifically
human body. The majority of the subjects were those with cognitive deficits. More so, it is evident
pooled from underprivileged populations, such as that informed consent is indeed a crucial aspect of
individuals that were ill or impoverished (Loue, conducting ethically sound research and/or thera-
2000). More so, children with intellectual disabilities peutic practices. It is implemented with intention
were also used as test subjects in radiation studies, of protecting the legal rights of participants as well
one example involving these children being fed with as serving as an ethical guideline of fair and just
items laden with radioactive chemicals. Additional treatment (Hall, Prochazka, & Fink, 2012).
ethical disregard is exemplified by research that spe- Informed consent is a process by which an indi-
cifically exploited individuals with cognitive dis- vidual is made aware of the purpose, procedures,
abilities. One infamous example is the Willowbrook and potential risks and benefits associated with
study, in which intellectually disabled children were involvement. This provides clients with the neces-
injected with hepatitis (Katz, 1972) to understand the sary knowledge for making an informed decision
course of the disease. Other research has utilized a regarding their participation, including their right
similar approach with the injection of experimental to decline or withdraw at any point. Given that an
vaccinations (Rothman & Michels, 1994). individual may not always possess the cognitive
These obsolete practices demonstrate the histori- capacity to make an informed judgment, assessing
cal precedency of scientific inquiry, with little the said capacity is an essential aspect of proper
regard for the deleterious effects subsequently informed consent (Iacono & Murray, 2003).
imposed upon participants; ethical concerns were Obtaining informed consent requires an array of
often of secondary thought, and paramount was cognitive abilities, such as the retention and com-
5  Informed Consent 69

prehension of the information provided, under- maintain the balance between the rights of the
standing the information in relation to personal client, ethical principles, and legal requirements.
context, efficiently evaluating multiple pieces of
information, and effectively communicating a final
decision. These cognitive faculties have a high like- Client Rights and Ethical Principles
lihood of being impaired in individuals diagnosed
with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD), given One important principle to consider when work-
that a significant amount of individuals with ASD ing with individuals with ASD is the level of men-
are nonverbal and/or intellectually disabled tal capacity required for the individual to
(Jerskey, Correira, & Morrow, 2014). For consent competently make treatment decisions (Fields &
to be considered valid, it is expected that it be given Calvert, 2015). The ability of individuals with
of one’s own volition. Individuals with autism do ASD to make decisions is related to the level of
not always possess the communication skills nec- autonomy the individual possesses (Buchanan,
essary for completing this action, given that diffi- 2004). Autonomy is defined as independent action
culties with expression and communication are a that one takes after deliberating and reflecting on
common feature in ASD (Mitchell et al., 2006). a given situation. When it has been determined
Furthermore, these individuals may lack the neces- that an individual with ASD has a higher level of
sary insight for accurately assessing ways in which capacity, others are more likely to endorse that
they may be personally impacted. These special individual’s right to determine what type of care
considerations for individuals with ASD, taken in they should receive (Rich, 2002; Ryan, 2005).
tandem with the long-standing history of unethi- Shortly before and in the aftermath of World
cally treatment of cognitively impaired individuals, War II, psychologists began exploring ways that
emphasize the importance of discussing informed they could increase the standards of care in which
consent in the context of ASD and competency. they operated as a result of increased public
Informed consent is a topic that has acquired awareness of the activities entailed in the field of
increased attention over time. It is now well estab- psychology. The advancement of psychology led
lished that clients have a right to consent to partici- to growing concern about how to resolve the
pate in, or withdraw from, treatment. For individuals moral dilemmas that practitioners face on a daily
with autism spectrum disorders, many of whom basis. This concern led to the development of the
also present with intellectual disabilities, additional American Psychological Association’s (APA)
unique challenges exist. Many of these individuals Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of
may not be able to completely comprehend the Ethics (APA Ethics Code).
terms of agreement for which they are expected to The 2010 APA Ethics Code contains a set of
consent to. How these special challenges can be ten Ethical Standards. The purpose of the Ethical
attended to, current professional and legal stan- Standards is to set forth enforceable rules that
dards, and standard procedures will be addressed. guide the work of practicing psychologists.
Standards 3.10, 8.02, 9.03, and 10.01 are related
to providing informed consent to individual
Ethical and Legal Considerations receiving psychological services.
Standard 3.10 specifies that an individual
While it is ethically appropriate to allow individ- receiving psychological services in person and
uals with ASD to actively participate in the treat- through electronic or other forms of communica-
ment process, it is also important to consider the tion must consent to services provided by a prac-
individual’s unique needs and capacity to make ticing psychologist. Furthermore, the individual
informed decisions about treatment modalities providing consent must also have the nature of the
that would be most effective. When considering services being provided conveyed to them in lan-
an individual’s ability to provide informed con- guage that is understandable to them (with the
sent, numerous factors must be taken into account exception being when legislation or the APA
for psychologists and intervention providers to Ethics Code otherwise states that consent of the
70 R.D. Rieske et al.

individual receiving services is not required). Table 5.1  APA Ethics Code: General principles
Examples of when psychologists are not required Principle guidelines
to obtain consent include when the client is a Principle A ∙ Do no harm
minor or services are court mandated. When indi- Beneficence and ∙ Promote welfare of others
viduals are deemed incapable of providing con- nonmaleficence ∙ Uphold professional
sent due to minor status or as a result of reduced standards of conduct
cognitive capacity, psychologists are nevertheless Principle B ∙ Establish trust in
professional relationships
required to provide an explanation of the services
Fidelity and ∙ Clarify professional roles
that are being provided. Psychologists must also responsibility ∙ Concerned about ethical
attempt to obtain assent of the individual partici- compliance
pating in the services, take into account actions in Principle C ∙ Promote honesty in practice
the best interest of the individual receiving ser- Integrity ∙ Maximize benefits and
vices, and obtain permission from legal guardians minimize harm
or representatives of the individual. ∙ Correct harmful effect
Unique Standards also exist when considering Principle D ∙ Concern with client equality
an individual’s right to consent to research Justice ∙ Consider reasonable
(Standard 8.02), assessment (Standard 9.03), and precautions
treatment (Standard 10.01). Regardless of the ∙ Cognizant of personal
biases and limitations
services being offered, psychologists are bound
Principle E ∙ Respect for diverse
to describe the nature of the therapeutic relation- clientele
ship, the likely course of treatment, anticipated Respect for people’s ∙ Considerations for
fees, potential involvement of third parties for the rights and dignity vulnerable populations
purposes of consultation and additional services, ∙ Work to eliminate effects of
and limitations to confidentiality and to allow the personal biases
client or their legal representatives the opportu- Adapted from APA Ethics code (2010)
nity to ask questions and receive answers that
adequately address their concerns. When the
methods being utilized by the clinician are not The core tenants of principle A (beneficence
well established, psychologists must disclose this and nonmaleficence) involve psychologists pro-
information to the client as well as describe the tecting the rights and welfare of individuals
anticipated outcomes, risks, and benefits and any with whom they interact with in a professional
alternative methods that may be available to capacity. Arguably, one of the most well-known
address the client’s presenting concerns. The cli- principles of practicing psychologists is the
ent must also be informed when a practicing ther- principle of “do no harm.” While many con-
apist is a trainee and should be provided the name sider harm to result from actions taken, it is
of the trainee’s immediate supervisor. equally relevant to consider the risks associated
The APA Ethics Code also consists of five gen- with the decision to decline treatment. This
eral principles (APA, 2010). While the general being the case, psychologists strive to protect
principles do not represent legal obligations to against the potential misuse of their influence
which psychologists must adhere, they represent that may lead to personal, organizational, or
aspirational ethical ideals to which psychologists financial gain.
should aspire that would aid them in reaching the Principle B (fidelity and responsibility) is
highest standards of professional practice (Hobbs, included to clarify professional relationships
1948). The general principles (Table 5.1) include and responsibilities toward others. Primarily
beneficence and nonmaleficence (principle A), related to informed consent, psychologists have
fidelity and responsibility (principle B), integrity a responsibility to maintain the best interest of
(principle C), justice (principle D), and respect for individuals with whom they work. More specifi-
people’s rights and dignity (principle E). cally, psychologists attempt to maintain ethical
5  Informed Consent 71

compliance and hold themselves and their col- ment (Canterbury v. Spence, 1972). The purpose
leagues to the highest standards of professional of consent, therefore, is to educate the client about
conduct. options available for treatment and to protect indi-
Principle C (integrity) is primarily concerned viduals from being forced to undergo therapeutic
with psychologists maintaining a standard of care interventions that they may otherwise decline.
that does not mislead the clients with whom they One major complication with informed consent
work. Should psychologists discover that the procedures and requirements is that they vary from
methods they are using have the potential to harm one jurisdiction to the next. Even more problem-
the client, they have an ethical obligation to atic is that individual interpretation of the require-
reveal this information and consider alternatives ments within a jurisdiction often varies from one
that may better benefit the client. clinician to the next. Fortunately for psychologists,
The capability of the clinician to meaningfully the standard of proof for individuals providing
contribute toward the advancement of clinical informed consent typically relies on evidence that
care is covered by principle D (justice). The prin- the clinicians took reasonable steps to explain
ciple of justice requires that psychologists should therapeutic processes to the individual receiving
maintain awareness of their limitations and biases services and, when the situation requires, the cli-
and share their level of competence with the ent’s legal custodians (Iacono & Murray, 2003).
methods they are utilizing with their clients. This While the amount of time spent reviewing the
principle is especially important for the informed therapeutic interventions and alternatives that the
consent process, given that psychologist are clinician may offer varies from one provider to the
required to disclose information relevant for the next, rigorous guidelines do not exist that require a
treatment and outcomes that the client should specific amount of time be spent on informed con-
reasonably expect based on unique client factors sent procedures (Hall, Prochazka, & Fink, 2012).
(Siegal, Bonnie, & Appelbaum, 2012). While the guidelines of informed consent may
Finally, principle E (respect for people’s rights vary by clinician, empirical studies have been
and dignity) is intended to encapsulate the respect conducted to determine which factors are most
psychology should have for clinical diversity. relevant to determine when considering whether
Psychologists should be aware of vulnerable an individual has been adequately informed
populations and take reasonable steps to provide (DeRenzo, Conley, & Love, 1998). In the devel-
the highest level of professional care regardless opment of a competence screening tool intended
of a given client’s age, religious affiliation, sex- to measure an individual’s “capacity to consent”
ual orientation, potential disability, or other fac- in a psychiatric research study, Zayas, Cabassa,
tors that may place a client at risk of experiencing and Perez (2005) noted four legal standards
difficulty comprehending information regarding (Table  5.2) necessary for demonstrating such
the onset, course, or duration of treatment that is capacity including understanding, appreciation,
presented by the practicing psychologist (Roberts reasoning, and choice (also see Appelbaum,
& Roberts, 1999). Grisso, Frank, O’Donnell, & Kupfer, 1999; Cea
& Fisher, 2003; Grisso & Appelbaum, 1995).
Understanding refers to an individual’s ability
Legal Obligations to comprehend information that is presented to
them that describes a given treatment method and
The concept of consent was written into law in the procedures associated with that method. This
1914 and was determined to refer to an individu- standard requires clinicians to consider the men-
al’s “right to determine what shall be done with tal capacity of clients to determine whether the
his body” (Schloendorff v. Society of New York client understands the nature and purpose of
Hospital, 1914). By 1975, the American judicial treatment, the risks, and benefits associated with
system established that medical professionals are the proposed treatment modalities and whether
required to reveal information that a “reasonable they understand the procedures that will occur
person” would want to know when seeking treat- throughout treatment.
72 R.D. Rieske et al.

Table 5.2  Four legal standards for demonstrating competence in informed consent for treatment
Definition Question
Understanding Ability to comprehend information Does the client show ability to
that is given about the treatment and comprehend the information given
its procedures. Consider mental about the nature and purpose of the
capacity treatment and the procedures
involved including risks and
benefits?
Appreciation Ability to recognize the value or Does the client demonstrate the
significance of the treatment as well ability to appreciate the significance
as potential consequences of treatment?
Reasoning Ability to manipulate information Does the client show the ability to
given about the treatment and the reason about the risks and benefits
consequences of disclosing of participating versus not
information about themselves participating?
Choice Ability to decide to participate or Does the client show the ability to
not and then communicate that volunteer freely to participate in
choice to their provider without treatment or not and without fear or
fearing loss of services/rights or sense of coercion?
disappointing others
Adapted from Grisso and Appelbaum (1995) and Zayas et al. (2005)

The standard of appreciation refers to a client’s An alternative method of determining


ability to recognize the value or significance of the whether the risk of treatment is outweighed by
treatment, as well as the actions the client will be the benefits was outlined by Terry (1915).
required to take throughout its duration. The clini- According to Terry (1915), the risk of a given
cian must consider whether the client understands treatment will depend on (1) the likelihood that
the significance of the treatment method and the the individual may be subjected to harm, (2) the
potential consequences of receiving treatment. magnitude of harm to which they may be sub-
Third, the standard of reasoning refers to the jected, (3) the likelihood that the goals the indi-
capability of the client to consider and manipulate vidual is trying to achieve will be reached, (4)
information regarding recommended treatments. the perceived value of achieving the desired
Mental health professionals must consider whether goals, and (5) why the specific treatment is nec-
the client demonstrates the ability to weigh the risks essary, even when alternative treatments may be
and benefits of participating in treatment versus available.
abstaining from treatment. Given that reasoning As described by Applebaum (2007) and
requires a higher level of cognitive functioning in Karlawish (2003), if every suspected case of
comparison to the other standard, it is often consid- impaired cognitive capacity resulted in judicial
ered the most difficult standard to achieve and assess. review, the legal system would be unable to keep
Finally, choice is involved when the client is up with the number of cases that presented for
able to actively communicate preferred options review. Hence, it is imperative that practicing
based off of provider expertise and guidance, professionals incorporate sound methods for
without fear of disappointing others that may be assessing cognitive functioning in their consent
involved in the treatment process. Clinicians processes.
must demonstrate that the client made decisions Special considerations must be made when
of their own free will and that they were not the individual receiving treatment is a minor,
coerced into selecting a treatment modality based given that children lack legal power to provide
off of the expectations of others. Choice is con- consent for psychological or medical treatment.
sidered the easiest standard for clients of all Therefore, the responsibility to provide consent
levels of cognitive functioning to understand
­ becomes the responsibility of the parents or legal
(Zayas et al., 2005). guardians of a minor (Bernat, 2001).
5  Informed Consent 73

Conclusions to make an informed decision regarding a spe-


cific treatment including psychotherapy and
Understanding the ethical and legal obligations behavioral treatments. Basic components of the
that psychologists must undertake when seeking informed consent process should include a dis-
informed consent from individuals with ASD is a cussion of risks/benefits, clear description of the
delicate process under which a variety of factors proposed treatment including empirical support
must be considered. As outlined by Hall et al. and limitations, alternative treatments as well as
(2012), while the balance between legal, ethical, prognosis without treatment, and confidentiality
and administrative responsibilities remains a including limits to confidentiality. The process of
complex procedure that involves consideration of informed consent should answer any and all
clinician competence, unique client factors, and questions that clients have regarding the treat-
understanding local, state, federal, and organiza- ment options. In the development of informed
tional statutes and guidelines, the primary ele- consent for treatment, clinicians should address
ments that must be considered when seeking the following questions (see Table 5.3).
client consent include the capacity of the
decision-­maker to understand the elements of
and course of treatment, the steps taken by the Risks and Benefits
psychologist to provide information about the
risks and benefits and alternatives to the treat- Within the field of psychology, the clinician’s
ment modality that is selected, the primary first obligation is to consider the risks for the
decision-­maker demonstrating that they compre- individual client in the context of the proposed
hend information presented by the psychologist treatment. Risks and benefits of treatment should
and are able to make an informed decisions, and be communicated to the client/family including
that the decision-maker selects a modality based discussion of monetary costs, expected time obli-
on their own will that is not unduly influenced by gations, known risks related to the treatment,
coercive tactics on the part of the psychologist. direct benefits to the individual and/or family,
Individuals with ASD are a particularly vul- and any other pertinent information. As discussed
nerable population given the wide array of cogni- by Ahern (2012), discussion of the risks and ben-
tive differences among individuals with the efits to be included in informed consent must be
disorder. This being the case, special care must based on evidence to support those assumptions.
be taken when considering the capability of indi- Few studies have completed a post hoc evalua-
viduals with ASD to consent to treatment. tion of perceived harm and benefits, and often
Clinicians must be aware of the ethical and legal those included in informed consents are based on
considerations involved in the informed consent clinician/researcher subjective thoughts.
process as well as research regarding the compo- Clinicians and researchers alike should include
nents of informed consent and the need to assess measurement of perceived risks and benefits
for competency. Additional concerns, as they from the client or participant’s perspective (client
relate to these ethical and legal considerations, subjective ratings) as well as objective and out-
are also discussed below. come measures. Such research is important given
findings that including possible risks, although
ethically necessary, can cause what some
Informed Consent for Treatment researchers have termed a “nocebo effect” which
has been shown to cause an exacerbation of
There are basic components that should be symptoms after discussion of risks during the
included in all consents for treatment, and informed consent process (Cohen, 2014).
although the content of the consents will vary, the Although research regarding the nocebo effect
components should not. The goal of informed during informed consent is limited, it is an impor-
consent for treatment is to provide clients and tant dilemma to consider in our ethical charge to
their families with all of the information needed “do no harm.”
74 R.D. Rieske et al.

Table 5.3  Questions to be addressed through informed provider of treatment. The treatment description
consent
also includes some general psychoeducation
Questions regarding the modality of the treatment options
Risks/benefits ∙ What are the risks or (e.g., behavior therapy, exposure based, skills
discomforts associated with
building) as well as the evidence base supporting
this treatment?
the given treatment. In a treatment capacity, clini-
∙ What are the personal benefits
of this treatment? cians should only be providing treatment options
∙ Can I stop treatment at any which have an empirical basis, and, as consumers
time? of treatment, clients and their families have the
Treatment ∙ What are the procedures right to know what evidence is available to sup-
description involved in this treatment? port a given treatment. For example, the
∙ What results should I expect University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
from treatment?
recently released a report of 27 evidenced-based
∙ What are the limitations of this
treatment? practices for children, youth, and young adults
∙ What is the evidence base for with ASD covering several aspects of treatment
the effectiveness of this consideration (Wong et al., 2014). This free pub-
treatment? lication (available at http://autismpdc.fpg.unc.
∙ What is the cost of treatment? edu/sites/autismpdc.fpg.unc.edu/files/2014-
∙ What is the time commitment EBP-Report.pdf) includes the evidence base for a
for treatment?
wide variety of interventions ranging from
∙ Who will be involved in
Picture Exchange Communication Systems and
treatment?
∙ What are the qualifications of
Functional Behavior Assessment to Discrete
the provider? Trial and Pivotal Response Training. Use of such
Alternative ∙ What alternative treatments are resources is important when describing interven-
treatments available? (Include treatment tions and the evidence base supporting their use.
description for each) In addition to the discussion of the treatment
∙ Is not seeking treatment an procedures and protocols, the intended effect that
option?
the intervention will have and the results that cli-
∙ What is the prognosis without
treatment? ents and their families should expect during the
Confidentiality ∙ How will my privacy be course of treatment should be provided. Along the
protected? same lines, clients should also be informed of the
∙ What information will you limitations of the treatment, and all information
share and with whom? provided should be based on empirical evidence
∙ What circumstances would you when available. When such information is not
share that information?
available (i.e., lack of empirical evidence for a spe-
cific treatment) clinicians must carefully describe
the justifications for the given treatment, what evi-
Treatment Description dence is available suggesting its effectiveness, as
well as any alternative treatments available.
In addition to discussing the risks and benefits of
a given treatment, clients need to be made aware
of the actual procedures that will be involved Alternative Treatments
when engaging in treatment. This component of
the informed consent process includes a detailed During the informed consent process, clients
description of what treatment sessions will look should also be presented with what alternative
like, who will be involved in treatment (e.g., indi- treatments are available following many of the
vidual, family, group), who will be providing same guidelines presented above. The purpose of
specific components, and the qualifications of the informed consent for treatment is to provide
5  Informed Consent 75

clients and their families the information needed safety measures that will be utilized to protect all
to make an informed decision for treatment, and information. It is also important to discuss with
that process cannot be completed without provid- the client the limits of confidentiality before any
ing them with alternative forms of treatment. interview or other procedures take place. This
Clinicians must be careful to not let their per- includes informing clients about local laws
sonal beliefs or opinions about a given alternative regarding mandated reporting (e.g., abuse/
treatment affect the decision of their client as this neglect, threats of harm to self or others) as well
may be seen as coercive in nature. Instead, clini- as issues related to guardianship. When working
cians should provide the same level of informa- with children and individuals with intellectual
tion based on empirical evidence and the expected and developmental disabilities (such as ASD), it
outcomes for the alternative treatment. is often the case that these individuals may not
Researchers have found that when making deci- fully comprehend the limits to confidentiality. It
sions regarding treatment, and when provided is important for clinicians to provide this infor-
with the appropriate information, caregivers mation on a level that is developmentally appro-
place the most weight upon current scientific priate for the individual, especially for cases that
research followed by the clinicians’ experience involve custody disputes (Condie & Koocher,
(Allen & Varela, 2015). The goal is to provide the 2008). Clinicians should also be especially sensi-
client with multiple options for treatment and tive and carefully explain limits of confidential-
enough information for them to be able to weigh ity, when working with children or adolescents
the risks and benefits for each within their own who may disclose sexual behavior, substance
personal context. use, illegal behavior, or suicidal ideation (Duncan,
Lastly, clients should also be made aware that Williams, & Knowles, 2013; Knowles, Duncan,
not seeking treatment is an option and that clients & Hall, 2015). Clinicians should be aware of the
and their families are not obliged to participate in varying regulations and statutes which govern
any treatment if they decide against it. There are such disclosure within their local area, and the
several reasons that individuals and families may same sensitivity should be given when working
chose not to engage in treatment including cost, with adult clients who have a custodial guardian.
time commitment, cultural, and religious consid-
erations. It is not the place of the clinician to put
undue pressure on the family to engage in treat- Additional Considerations
ment; however, it is the responsibility of the clini-
cian to provide individuals and families with the A further issue that has developed in the process
information needed to make that decision and to of informed consent for treatment (for both medi-
make sure the client understands their decision. cal and psychological treatments) is the balance
This would include providing clients with an between providing the right amount of informa-
expected prognosis if no treatment was provided tion for the client in order for them to make an
as well as any additional risks or benefits for not informed decision regarding treatment as well as
engaging in treatment. protecting clinicians legally. Additionally, clini-
cians struggle with the task of delivering infor-
mation that is appropriate for the developmental
Confidentiality level of the individual and their families while
balancing the client’s need or desire for informa-
Finally, as a part of the informed consent process, tion. Many states have vague laws regarding the
individuals and their families should be provided amount of information that should be disclosed
with information regarding how their personal as part of the informed consent process leaving
information is used and protected as a part of the clinicians with the burden of determining what is
treatment. This includes the standard procedures appropriate for each given individual and
that are used within the clinic or practice and the treatment.
76 R.D. Rieske et al.

One option that has been presented to address also have comorbid intellectual disabilities
this issue has been termed “information on demand” should be of concern to various practitioners and
and provides the client with the ability to control the professionals providing such services. In order to
amount of information that they wish to know about provide consent to services, an individual must
the given treatment (Siegal et al. 2012). In the initial demonstrate the ability to comprehend the infor-
implementation stage, this process involves provid- mation that is being provided, understand the
ing all clients with a basic level of information (e.g., risks and benefits of a given treatment, and be
nature of the treatment, justification, prognosis, able to communicate their decision effectively to
etc.) and then providing them with the option to their provider. Given that many individuals with
learn more about alternative treatments and risk fac- ASD are also nonverbal, assessing their ability to
tors (intermediate information) followed by exten- engage in the informed consent process is impor-
sive technical information regarding the treatment, tant. Much of the research evaluating the ability
alternatives, and risks (extensive information). This to engage in the informed consent process has
process was proposed as an intermediate step been completed in the context of research partici-
toward fully individualized informed consent in pation; however, important findings and tools
which the client controls the flow of information. that have been developed are germane to informed
This strategy transfers the burden and decision- consent for treatment.
making of the amount of information to be dis-
closed to the client which also shifts the legal
burdens away from clinicians. This strategy, how- Rationale for Capacity Assessments
ever, does not come without further questions and
disadvantages for individuals and families with In some treatment centers, the process of obtaining
intellectual and developmental disabilities and does informed consent has, unfortunately, become more
not adjust the content of the information based on of a formality than a thoughtful practice to protect
developmental level. A combination of the tradi- client welfare. Often, informed consent becomes an
tional informed consent process along with the pro- informal process where the client is simply asked to
posed process with consideration of the individual sign on the appropriate line, with little knowledge as
needs of the client and their developmental level to what they are agreeing to. Similarly, clinicians
will likely prove to be an improvement over the cur- are not in the habit of assessing capacity to consent,
rently used process. and therefore, capacity is assumed. Generally, this
The recurring issue regarding the informed assumption is sound as many clients are more than
consent process that occurs at every level when capable of providing legal consent to treatment.
working with individuals with ASD, intellectual, However, valid informed consent requires capacity
or other developmental disabilities is the determi- (see four legal standards for informed consent), and
nation of competency. One cannot ethically com- a subset of clients are incapable of consenting to
plete informed consent if the client does not have treatment in a meaningful way.
the ability to comprehend the information being There are many factors that may inhibit a cli-
provided. Discussion and assessment of compe- ent’s capacity to consent (Holzer, Gansier,
tency is an important issue to consider within the Moczynski, & Folstein, 1997). Disease and other
field of ASD intervention given the host of treat- serious health conditions, emotional distur-
ments available to individuals and families. bances, and cognitive impairments have all been
demonstrated to negatively impact one’s ability
to provide consent. Therefore, it is imperative
Competency that clinicians screen for capacity when working
with individuals within these populations. For
Individuals with ASD are often engaged in vari- example, individuals with intellectual disabilities
ous treatments; however, their ability to consent are more likely to feel coerced by caregivers
to such treatments given that many individual (Irvine, 2010) or comply with authority figures
5  Informed Consent 77

(Zayas et al., 2005), such as clinicians who sug- aspects of the study. Further, each participant was
gest the client participates in treatment. It is rec- required to pass a “capacity-to-consent” screener
ommended that when working with intellectually prior to admission into the study (i.e., participant
disabled individuals, clinicians inquire about the must correctly answer 8/10 questions about the
client’s rationale for involvement in treatment. study). The researchers were particularly inter-
This small act can serve as an added protection ested in how many attempts were required to pass
for a potentially vulnerable client and is in line the capacity-to-consent screening measure, as
with the principle of true informed consent. well as factors that impacted capacity to consent.
Informed consent requires capacity. When Less than 5% of the sample was unable to pass the
done properly, informed consent requires that the capacity-to-consent screener after three attempts,
client understands all relevant information about suggesting that the majority of this population
the treatment, including risks and benefits. possess the cognitive abilities to provide informed
Additionally, the client must be able to compre- consent. Moreover, over half of the sample was
hend that consent is voluntary and can be with- successful in passing the screening tool on the
drawn at any time. Therefore, the client must be first attempt, while roughly 40% required two or
able to engage in the decision-making process three attempts to meet the criterion. Interestingly,
and effectively communicate his/her decision. the consent brochure was not associated with
However, it is not always clear if an individual is improved performance on the capacity-to-consent
capable of providing consent, and thus, it is measure. It is unclear why the brochure failed to
important to have adequate methods to evaluate improve the capacity to consent, although Morton
cognitive capacity. and Cunningham-­ Williams (2009) hypothesize
that boredom and inattention prevented partici-
pants from benefitting from this supplemental
Research on Capacity to Consent material. Overall, the results of this study indicate
that cognitively impaired individuals were able to
Very little research has been conducted regarding provide informed consent; however, these indi-
capacity to provide consent with individuals who viduals may require multiple attempts to meet the
meet criteria for an autism spectrum disorder. criterion and may benefit from a more compre-
However, a larger literature exists involving capac- hensive overview of the treatment (i.e., adequate
ity to consent for individuals with cognitive impair- time for question/answer session about the inter-
ments, such as the intellectually disabled. vention; Morton & Cunningham-Williams, 2009).
Therefore, this review and subsequent recommen- Similarly, Zayas and his colleagues (2005)
dations are based primarily on literature involving were interested in evaluating capacity to consent
cognitively impaired clients and may or may not be with psychiatric outpatients in a research context.
relevant for all individuals with an autism spectrum The authors developed a capacity-to-consent
disorder. Nonetheless, any client who consents to a screening device for use within an urban commu-
psychosocial intervention must be deemed capable nity mental health clinic and provided an initial
prior to their entry into treatment. test of their instrument. Sixty-eight adults (aged
Morton and Cunningham-Williams (2009) 21 years and older) with no psychiatric treatment
conducted a cross-sectional study with develop- history in the past year agreed to participate in
mentally disabled homeless individuals to deter- the study. In order to be included in the study,
mine factors that impede one’s ability to provide research participants were required to correctly
informed consent. In this study, 62 homeless indi- answer at least eight out of ten (score of 80%)
viduals with a self-reported history of special edu- questions on the capacity-to-consent screening
cation completed semi-structured interviews. In tool. The 80% criterion was selected by the
order to aid comprehension of the consent docu- researchers based on their intent to protect pro-
ment, every odd-numbered participant received a spective research participants and not on previ-
consent brochure, which highlighted important ous literature. As literature in this area is scarce,
78 R.D. Rieske et al.

no conclusive cutoff for capacity determination recommended that informed consent documents
exists. The vast majority (92.6%, n = 63) of the should not exceed the reading level of an average
68 interested participants were successful in eighth grader (Fields & Calvert, 2015; Iacono &
demonstrating capacity to consent. Moreover, 59 Murray, 2003). Similarly, Iacono and Murray
of the 63 participants passed the capacity-to-­ (2003) offer additional methods to facilitate the
consent screener on their first attempt, while the consent process when working with individuals
remaining participants were successful on their with an intellectual disability, such as utilizing
second attempt. Importantly, the authors state augmentative and alternative communication
that the individuals who were unable to meet cri- (AAC) devices, developing videotapes and pic-
terion on the capacity-to-consent screener had tures, and training clinicians/researchers to assess
limited education and suspected to be intellectu- the individual’s understanding of the material
ally disabled or cognitively impaired (e.g., mem- covered in the consent form. Taken together, these
ory impairments). Therefore, this research recommended strategies may increase a client’s
provides additional support for the regular assess- abilityInformed consent:capacity to consent
ment of capacity to consent. Even with a sample to consent to treatment and should be put into
of adult outpatients, nearly 10% were unable to practice when appropriate.
provide valid informed consent based on the
researcher’s criteria (Zayas et al., 2005).
Clinicians are encouraged to engage in the fol- Assessment of Capacity to Consent
lowing “best practices” offered by researchers
within the field of informed consent. Several There are a number of competency scales avail-
strategies exist for enhancing an individual’s able for assessing client capacity to consent to
capacity to consent to treatment. One such strat- treatment. Fields and Calvert (2015) recommend
egy involves direct training. According to Zayas using measures such as the Mini-Mental State
et al. (2005), capacity to consent can be improved Examination (MMSE), MacArthur Competence
through instruction. For example, many clients Assessment Tool for Treatment (MacCAT-T),
may benefit from the professional reading the Hopemont Capacity Assessment Interview
consent document aloud and explaining the more (HCAI), or Capacity to Consent to Treatment
complex or difficult aspects of the document Instrument (CCTI). While several instruments
(Irvine, 2010). Likewise, previous research has exist, many have been developed for use in cogni-
demonstrated that increased capacity to consent is tively impaired populations (i.e., severe mental
associated with the professional revisiting com- illness, Alzheimer’s/Parkinson’s disease, and
ponents of the consent document that the client intellectually disabled), and no instruments have
has struggled with (presumably based on a capac- been specifically designed for estimating capacity
ity-to-consent measure). It may be necessary to with individuals with ASD. The following list of
review the consent document multiple times (see assessment tools is not exhaustive, but provides a
Morton & Cunningham-Williams, 2009), allow- good starting place for interested clinicians.
ing the client to ask questions and the clinician to
assess for adequate client comprehension. Hopkins Competency Assessment Test (HCAT;
Individuals with cognitive impairments may have Janofsky, McCarthy, & Folstein, 1992)  The
more difficulty navigating a written consent form HCAT is a six-item tool that requires the client to
(Iacono & Murray, 2003). Therefore, it is impor- answer questions in true/false and sentence com-
tant that the consent be available in other medi- pletion format. The client is provided with a short
ums (e.g., read aloud to the participant or document that outlines the informed consent pro-
supplemented with visual aids). This may enhance cess. After reading the document, the client is
the client’s ability to comprehend the consent asked to respond to the six questions. Lower scores
document. Further, the consent document must be (i.e., a score of 3 or less out of a possible 10) on
written in clear language. As a general rule, it is the HCAT indicate impaired u­nderstanding,
5  Informed Consent 79

which suggests the client is incapable of providing arguably the most empirical support of any com-
informed consent. According to Sturman (2005), petency measure, demonstrating good reliability
using this threshold the HCAT has perfect (100%) and validity across multiple samples. Further,
sensitivity and specificity in determining incompe- The MacCAT-T is one of few capacity instru-
tence. This test has also been shown to have high ments with an accompanying training manual
interobserver reliability (Sturman, 2005) and crite- and training video.
rion validity (Holzer et al. 1997). The HCAT takes
approximately 10 min to administer. Structured Interview for Competency/
Incompetency Assessment Testing and
Competency Interview Schedule (CIS; Bean, Ranking Inventory (SICIATRI; Tomoda et al.,
Nishisato, Rector, & Glancy, 1994)  The CIS is 1997)  The SICIATRI was developed by Tomoda
a structured interview developed for assessing and colleagues to assess capacity to provide
competency in psychiatric inpatients. Client informed consent to treatment. This measure was
responses are rated on a seven-point Likert scale. initially tested with both psychiatric and medical
Lower scores on the CIS suggest competency, inpatients. The SICIATRI is a 12-item structured
while higher item scores indicate impairment. interview. Clients are rated on a 1-to-3 scale for
The CIS is said to measure all four legal princi- each item with lower scores (i.e., score = 1) evi-
ples of capacity (choice, understanding, appreci- dencing poor performance and higher scores (i.e.,
ation, and reasoning) and has adequate score = 3) indicating adequate performance.
psychometric properties. The CIS has good inter- Scoring for this measure includes a “Ranking
nal consistency (Chronbach’s α = 0.96), interra- Inventory for Competency” where the evaluator
ter reliability, and criterion validity. Notable must rank the client’s competency into one of five
limitations include mixed test-retest reliability levels (lower levels indicate impairment). During
data over a period of 24 h, no specified cutoff cri- its initial test, the SICIATRI displayed good inter-
teria, and unpredictable administration time. As rater reliability and concurrent validity. According
the CIS is a structured interview, administration to Tomoda et al. (1997), the SICIATRI possesses
time may significantly differ across clients good sensitivity (0.83) and specificity (0.67).
(Sturman, 2005).
Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument
MacArthur Competency Assessment Tool for (CCTI; Marson, Ingram, Cody, & Harrell,
Treatment (MacCAT-T)  The MacCAT-T was 1995)  The CCTI was developed for use with cog-
developed by Appelbaum, Grisso, and Hill-­ nitively impaired clients (individuals with
Fotouhi (1997). Like the CIS, the MacCAT-T Alzheimer’s disease). The format of the CCTI is an
measures each of the four legal standards of interactive interview where clients are presented
informed consent in a structured interview for- with vignettes that require hypothetical medical
mat (Applebaum, 2007). The MacCAT-T is decisions. This measure assesses all four legal
thought to be more comprehensive than compara- competency standards and also examines the cli-
tive tools (e.g., HCAT or CIS) as it yields scores ent’s ability to reason about two comparative treat-
for understanding, reasoning, and appreciation. ments. The CCTI is well validated and requires
However, there are no recommended cutoff roughly 25 min to administer (Fields & Calvert,
scores available. The MacCAT-T interview con- 2015; Marson et al., 1995; Sturman, 2005).
sists of 21 items which requires the test adminis-
trator must make “inadequate,” “partial,” or The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE;
“adequate” ratings. According to Applebaum Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975)  The
(2007), the MacCAT-T requires about 20 min to MMSE is a brief screening tool (11 items)
administer and should be supplemented by exam- designed to assess mental status. While not origi-
ining the client’s file/medical chart (Sturman, nally developed for estimating a client’s capacity
2005). Psychometrically, the MacCAT-T has to consent to treatment, it is often used as a proxy
80 R.D. Rieske et al.

measure of competency. Scores on the MMSE this individual should be considered (e.g., spouse,
range from 0 to 30 with higher scores (i.e., scores adult child, etc.), it is also important to consider
= 23–26+) indicating increased capacity to con- the most ethically responsible person for that
sent (Applebaum, 2007). Like many measures of specific individual (Karlawish, 2003). When
its kind, the MMSE does not have recommended states do not have legally designated representa-
cutoffs for capacity determinations; however, cli- tives, the responsibility often falls on various
ents who score below 19 merit further assessment family members; however, this practice assumes
and additional safeguards. Finally, the MMSE has that family members will always act in the indi-
been known to correlate with clinician judgments vidual’s best interest, which is not always the
of capacity (Applebaum, 2007; Sturman, 2005). case (Iacono & Murray, 2003).
Little research or regulations have addressed
Other Competency Measures  Additional the issue of designating a legally authorized rep-
capacity measures include the Evaluation to Sign resentative to provide informed consent for those
Consent (ESC, Moser et al., 2002) and the individuals found to not be capable. Some states
Informed Consent Survey (ICS; Wirshing, have adopted the following in helping clinicians
Wirshing, Marder, Liberman, & Mintz, 1998). decide who can give informed consent in order of
preference: guardian, spouse, adult son/daughter,
parent, adult sibling, adult grandchild or other
 imitations of Capacity to Consent
L close relative, close friend, and guardian of estate.
to Treatment Instruments Although not always consistent, it has been
argued that medical professionals, providers, or
All of the abovementioned scales were devel- employees of a provider should never serve as a
oped for geriatric psychiatry populations (e.g., legally authorized representative due to conflicts
Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease, demen- of interest. In locales where no regulations exist,
tia) or severe mental illness populations (e.g., clinicians are urged to determine who is the most
schizophrenia, depression); however, few tools ethically appropriate person to provide informed
have been studied or validated for use with ASD consent for each individual client based on their
or intellectual disability populations. Moreover, own personal situation within the context of the
many standardized capacity measures lack ade- treatment being offered.
quate testing, and what little empirical support
exists for these measures is based on relatively
small sample sizes (Sturman, 2005). Conclusions

Informed consent has been demonstrated as an


Legally Authorized Representatives essential component in both research and thera-
peutic settings. Research has sufficiently demon-
When a client has been deemed to lack the capac- strated several examples of how the process of
ity to consent to treatment, the clinician then is informed consent may be impacted by impaired
tasked with determining who is the most appro- decision-making in a subset of populations, such
priate person to provide a proxy decision on as children (Bruzzesse & Fisher, 2003), those with
behalf of the client. Different locales have differ- medical/physical conditions (Casarett et al., 2003),
ing laws or regulations regarding what is often and minority members (Fisher et al., 2002). Such
termed as a “legally authorized representative,” findings elucidate rudimentary differences across
and many areas have vague or nonexistent guide- varying populations of how individuals may form
lines for determining who is best equipped to decisions in response to informed consent.
serve in this role unless the client has a legal Furthermore, the importance of this process has
guardian. While some states have laws or regula- been demonstrated by many historical events in
tions which define the relational order in which which vulnerable individuals were exploited and
5  Informed Consent 81

mistreated for the sake of empirical investigations. to most accurately obtain informed consent in indi-
These infamous accounts of maltreatment are rep- viduals with ASD.
resentative of a plethora of accounts in which indi- Additionally, legal obligations also inform
viduals with cognitive deficits were not only consent procedures. The American judicial sys-
denied the opportunity to provide informed con- tem has written into law that individuals have the
sent but were significantly exploited. Interspersed right to choose how they are treated and are enti-
between these studies was the evolution of tled access to any relevant information regarding
informed consent. The Nuremberg Code first iden- a given treatment. This led to an understanding of
tified ten core principles for acceptable treatments informed consent as the process of educating cli-
(Brody, 1998); this led to the Helsinki Declaration, ents about treatment options and protecting cli-
which focused on clinical research and identified ents from engaging in therapeutic interventions as
that persons with impaired cognitive abilities be a result of coercion. Worth noting, legal standards
allowed a proxy decision-maker (Roberts & associated with the process of informed consent
Roberts, 1999). Eventually, the Belmont report may vary by jurisdictions and are further con-
recognized informed consent as a core component founded by individual interpretation. Currently,
for conducting ethically sound investigations on there are no specific criteria that guide the inter-
human subjects. pretation and implementation of informed con-
The historical context illustrates the importance sent (Hall et al. 2012). However, research has
of understanding informed consent in regard to examined factors most pertinent for determining
specific populations, such ASD. Persons diag- whether or not a client has been sufficiently
nosed with ASD may present with unique chal- informed. Research by Zayas et al. (2005) identi-
lenges that impact their ability to give proper fied understanding, appreciation, reasoning, and
informed consent due to cognitive deficits and/or choice as the four legal standards for demonstrat-
impaired communication skills (Mitchell et al., ing capacity to consent. While it has been demon-
2006). Thus, rights of client, professional and legal strated that legal obligations engender clinician’s
standards, and standard procedures were reviewed thoughtfulness of client rights, the amount of
to foster a more detailed understanding of the cases that could be presented for review would
informed consent process in persons with ASD. present an insurmountable task for the judicial
The APA Ethics Code is founded on five general system (Appelbaum, 2007; Karlawish, 2003).
principles, which are intended to guide ethical Therefore, while legality offers many safeguards,
decision-making of the provider. These principles the onus should remain on the clinician to utilize
are applicable to all potential clients, and include empirically validated measures for evaluating
doing no harm (beneficence and nonmaleficence), competency during the consent process.
maintaining the best interest of the client (fidelity These ethical and legal obligations create an
and responsibility), maintaining a standard of care even more complex procedure when taken into
that does not misinform the client (integrity), being consideration with ASD. To help control for this
aware of personal limitations (justice), and embrac- complexity, four primary elements should be
ing a diverse array of clients (respect for people’s considered when requesting client consent. For
rights and dignity). As mentioned, these principles review, these are (1) the capacity of the decision-­
guide the ethical practice of all mental health pro- maker, (2) sufficient explanation of risks and
fessionals, but notably, principle E highlights an benefits, (3) established competency of the
ethical responsibility to be thoughtful about indi- decision-­maker, and (4) decision-maker’s choice
vidual ­ differences (e.g., vulnerable populations, as a result of their own free will (Hall et al. 2012).
ASD). More specifically, for persons with ASD, Overall, it is expected that consideration of com-
the capacity to consent is largely related to the indi- petency for individuals with ASD is done cau-
vidual’s level of autonomy. This must be taken into tiously and with care. Clinicians are expected to
account in conjunction with the ethical guidelines be familiar with, and find the balance between,
82 R.D. Rieske et al.

ethical obligations, legal standards, and special clinical judgment to continually assess the cli-
considerations (e.g., competency) in individuals ent’s comprehension. The presentation of the
with ASD. document should be considered as well, possibly
While the content included in informed con- including the use of visual aids or, alternatively,
sent documents may vary by clinician, setting, ensuring the document is constructed in clear,
and purpose, the components remain the same. In comprehensible language. Overall, these strate-
order to provide clients with necessary informa- gies outline many feasible options for improving
tion, five basic areas should be covered: commu- a client’s capacity for consenting to treatment. It
nication of risks and benefits (e.g., monetary is recommended that these strategies be imple-
costs, known outcomes), treatment description mented at all appropriate opportunities. When it
(procedures entailed), alternative treatment is clear that clients lack the needed competency
options, rights and limitations of confidentiality, to engage in the informed consent process, clini-
and additional considerations (delivering the cians should utilize local regulations or guide-
appropriate amount of information). lines for determining who is the most ethically
Ultimately, the discussion of ASD and appropriate individual to serve as a legally autho-
informed consents revolves around the issue of rized representative for proxy decisions.
competency. The communication deficits and Historical components, ethical and legal obli-
high rates of comorbid intellectual disabilities in gations, and competency all lend valuable insight
clients with ASD confound the capacity to con- toward understanding informed consent in
sent. Potential complications include feeling ASD. While an issue of great complexity, these
coerced by caregivers (Irvine, 2010) or reluc- core components provide structure that can
tantly complying with authority figures (Zayas inform ethical and effective practice of the
et al., 2005). Therefore, clinicians should query informed consent process for mental health pro-
the client’s rationale for pursuing treatment to fessionals working with ASD clients. Given the
provide an additional safeguard for a potentially ethical mandate for psychologists to consider
vulnerable population. While little research has unique characteristics of any client, it is of
been done in regard to capacity to consent for upmost importance that the field continues to
individuals with ASD, extant literature has refine and improve the approaches that are rou-
closely examined capacity to consent for indi- tinely utilized.
viduals with cognitive deficits. This research has Unfortunately, interventions in ASD have
demonstrated that, for this population, the major- become a hotbed for predatory treatments with a
ity of individuals do possess some capacity to lack of empirical support or evidence of possible
give informed consent; however, they may need harm (e.g., facilitated communication, chelation
to have materials reviewed with them multiple therapy, specialized diets). Because of such invali-
times to ensure they understand the presented dated and harmful treatments, the informed con-
content (Morton & Cunningham-Williams, sent process is even more crucial to the ASD
2009). When considering proper informed con- population. Providing clients and their families
sent, certain strategies should be utilized for with the necessary information to make informed
improving individuals’ capacity to consent to decisions regarding treatments and interventions is
treatment. Strategies supported by research paramount. Given the heterogeneous nature of the
include having the document read aloud, detailed ASD spectrum, special considerations need to be
explanation of more complex components, and made regarding the informed consent process and
spending additional time on areas that were more assessment of competency. Due to the paucity of
challenging for the client (Irvine, 2010). More so, research in this area as it relates to individuals with
the document may need to be reviewed more than ASD or other developmental disabilities in the
once (Morton & Cunningham-Williams, 2009), context of treatment, researchers and clinicians are
and the client should be encouraged to ask ques- urged to further the field in this area to help sup-
tions. All the while, clinicians should employ port and protect those clients which we serve.
5  Informed Consent 83

References ality to disclose information to parents? Australian


Psychologist, 48, 408–419.
Fields, L. M., & Calvert, J. D. (2015). Informed con-
Ahern, K. (2012). Informed consent: Are researchers accu-
sent procedures with cognitively impaired patients:
rately representing risks and benefits? Scandinavian
A review of ethics and best practices. Psychiatry and
Journal of Caring Sciences, 26, 671–678.
Clinical Neurosciences, 69(8), 462–471.
Allen, B., & Varela, A. (2015). Caregiver perceptions of
Fisher, C. B., Hoagwood, K., Boyce, C., Duster, T.,
clinical decision-making for the treatment of children.
Frank, D. A., Grisso, T., et al. (2002). Research eth-
Community Mental Health Journal, 51, 828–832.
ics for mental health science involving ethnic minor-
American Psychological Association. (2010). Ethical
ity children and youth. American Psychologist, 57,
principles of psychologists and code of conduct.
1024–1040.
Retrieved from http://apa.org/ethics/code/index.aspx
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975).
Appelbaum, P. S. (2007). Assessment of patients’ compe-
“Mini-mental state”: A practical method for grading
tence to consent to treatment. New England Journal of
the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal
Medicine, 357(18), 1834–1840.
of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.
Appelbaum, P., Grisso, P., & Hill-Fotouhi, C. (1997). The
Grisso, T., & Appelbaum, P. S. (1995). Comparison of
MacCAT-T: A clinical tool to assess patients’ capaci-
standards for assessing patients’ capacity to make
ties to make treatment decisions. Psychiatric Services,
treatment choices. American Journal of Psychiatry,
48, 1415–1419.
152, 1033–1037.
Appelbaum, P. S., Grisso, T., Frank, E., O’Donnell, S.,
Hall, D. E., Prochazka, A. V., & Fink, A. S. (2012).
& Kupfer, D. J. (1999). Competence of depressed
Informed consent for clinical treatment. Canadian
patients for consent to research. American Journal of
Medical Association Journal, 184(5), 533–540.
Psychiatry, 156, 1380–1384.
Hobbs, N. (1948). The development of a code of ethical
Bean, G., Nishisato, S., Rector, N. A., & Glancy,
standards for psychology. American Psychologist,
G. (1994). The psychometric properties of the
3(3), 80–84.
Competency Interview Schedule. The Canadian
Holzer, J. C., Gansler, D. A., Moczynski, N. P., & Folstein,
Journal of Psychiatry/La Revue canadienne de psy-
M. F. (1997). Cognitive functions in the informed con-
chiatrie, 39, 368–376.
sent evaluation process: A pilot study. Journal of the
Bernat, J. L. (2001). Informed consent. Muscle & Nerve,
American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online,
24(5), 614–621.
25(4), 531–540.
Brody, B. A. (1998). The ethics of biomedical research:
Iacono, T., & Murray, V. (2003). Issues of informed
An international perspective. New York, NY: Oxford
consent in conducting medical research involving
University Press.
people with intellectual disability. Journal of Applied
Bruzzese, J. M., & Fisher, C. B. (2003). Assessing and
Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 16(1), 41–51.
enhancing the research consent capacity of children
Irvine, A. (2010). Conducting qualitative research
and youth. Applied Developmental Science, 7, 13–26.
with individuals with developmental disabili-
Buchanan, A. (2004). Mental capacity, legal competence
ties: Methodological and ethical considerations.
and consent to treatment. Journal of the Royal Society
Developmental Disabilities Bulletin, 38(1), 21.
of Medicine, 97(9), 415–420.
Janofsky, J. S., McCarthy, R. J., & Foistein, M. F.
Canterbury v. Spence. (1972). 464 F.2d 772.
(1992). The Hopkins competency assessment test:
Casarett, D. J., Karlawish, J. H. T., & Hirschman, K. B.
A brief method for evaluating patients’ capacity to
(2003). Identifying ambulatory cancer patients at risk
give informed consent. Psychiatric Services, 43(2),
of impaired capacity-to-consent to research. Journal
132–136.
of Pain and Symptom Management, 26, 615–624.
Jerskey, B. A., Correira, E. D., & Morrow, E. M. (2014).
Cea, C. D., & Fisher, C. B. (2003). Health care decision-­
Informed consent in adults with Autism: Ethical and
making by adults with mental retardation. Mental
legal considerations in the United States. Atlanta, GA:
Retardation, 41, 78–87.
Poster session presented at the International Meeting
Cohen, S. (2014). The nocebo effect of informed consent.
for Autism Research.
Bioethics, 28, 147–154.
Karlawish, J. H. (2003). Research involving cognitively
Condie, L. O., & Koocher, G. P. (2008). Clinical man-
impaired adults. New England Journal of Medicine,
agement of children’s incomplete comprehension of
348(14), 1389–1392.
confidentiality limits. Journal of Child Custody, 5,
Katz, J. (1972). Experimentation with human beings.
161–191.
New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation Press.
DeRenzo, E. G., Conley, R. R., & Love, R. (1998).
Knowles, A., Duncan, R. E., & Hall, A. C. (2015). Ethical
Assessment of capacity to give consent to research
dilemmas of confidentiality with adolescent clients:
participation: State-of-the-art and beyond. J. Health
Case studies from psychologists. Ethics and Behavior,
Care L & Pol’y, 1, 66.
25, 197–221.
Duncan, R. E., Williams, B. J., & Knowles, A. (2013).
Loue, S. (2000). Textbook of research ethics: Theory and
Adolescents, risk behavior and confidentiality: When
practice. New York, NY: Kluwer Academic/Plenum
would Australian psychologists breach confidenti-
Publishers.
84 R.D. Rieske et al.

Marson, D. C., Ingram, K. K., Cody, H. A., & Harrell, Schloendorff v. Society of New York Hospital. (1914).
L. E. (1995). Assessing the competency of patients 105 N.E. 92 (N.Y.).
with Alzheimer’s disease under different legal stan- Siegal, G., Bonnie, R. J., & Appelbaum, P. S. (2012).
dards: A prototype instrument. Archives of Neurology, Personalized disclosure by information-on-demand:
52(10), 949–954. Attending to patient’s needs in the informed con-
Mitchell, S., Brian, J., Zwaigenbaum, L., Roberts, W., sent process. Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 40,
Szatmari, P., Smith, I., & Bryson, S. (2006). Early 359–367.
language and communication development of infants Sturman, E. D. (2005). The capacity to consent to treat-
later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal ment and research: A review of standardized assess-
of Developmental Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 69–78. ment tools. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 954–974.
Morton, L. G., II, & Cunningham-Williams, R. M. Terry, H. T. (1915). Negligence. Harvard Law Review,
(2009). The capacity to give informed consent in 29(1), 40–54.
a homeless population with developmental dis- Tomoda, A., Yasumiya, R., Sumiyama, T., Tsukada, K.,
abilities. Community Mental Health Journal, 45(5), Hayakawa, T., Matsubara, K., … Kitamura, T. (1997).
341–348. Validity and reliability of structured interview for
Moser, D. J., Schultz, S. K., Arndt, S., Benjamin, M. L., competency incompetency assessment testing and
Fleming, F. W., Brems, C. S., … Andreasen, N. C. ranking inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology,
(2002). Capacity to provide informed consent for 53(5), 443–450.
participation in schizophrenia and HIV research. Wirshing, D. A., Wirshing, W. C., Marder, S. R.,
American Journal of Psychiatry. Liberman, R. P., & Mintz, J. (1998). Informed con-
Rich, B. A. (2002). The ethics of surrogate decision mak- sent: assessment of comprehension. American Journal
ing. Western Journal of Medicine, 176(2), 127. of Psychiatry, 155, 1508–1511.
Roberts, L. W., & Roberts, B. (1999). Psychiatric research Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K., Cox, A. W., Fettig, A.,
ethics: An overview of evolving guidelines and cur- Kucharcyzk, S., … Schultz, T. R. (2014). Evidence-­
rent ethical dilemmas in the study of mental illness. based practices for children, youth, and young adults
Biological Psychiatry, 46(8), 1025–1038. with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: The
Rothman, K. J., & Michels, K. B. (1994). The continuing University of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham
unethical use of placebo controls. The New England Child Development Institute, Autism Evidence-Based
Journal of Medication, 331, 394–398. Practice Review Group.
Ryan, R. M. (2005). The developmental line of auton- Zayas, L. H., Cabassa, L. J., & Perez, M. C. (2005).
omy in the etiology, dynamics, and treatment of Capacity-to-consent in psychiatric research:
borderline personality disorders. Development and Development and preliminary testing of a screening
Psychopathology, 17(04), 987–1006. tool. Research on Social Work Practice, 15, 545–556.
The History, Pitfalls, and Promise
of Licensure in the Field 6
of Behavior Analysis

Julie Kornack

A confluence of laws, public policies, professional


societies, and scholarly research has propelled History of Occupational Licensing
the field of behavior analysis to the forefront of
the effort to treat the deficits and behaviors asso- The history of occupational licensing is – perhaps
ciated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). surprisingly – fraught with drama arising from pro-
Although behavior analysts often emphasize the fessional and ideological partisanship. Along with
broad utility of behavior analysis in addressing a the professionals targeted for regulation, econo-
variety of conditions, this chapter addresses the mists have strong opinions about the purpose of
licensure of behavior analysts, which, for now, is regulation and the effect of regulation on the econ-
inextricably tied to the wealth of research that omy. As one might imagine, many elements of
demonstrates the effectiveness of applied behav- occupational regulation are either great or horrible,
ior analysis (ABA) in treating ASD (Granpeesheh, depending on whom you ask, and a few variables
Tarbox, & Dixon, 2009; Matson & Smith, 2008). are not quite so simply black or white.
As the field of behavior analysis has grown and Occupational regulation arose in the late nine-
ABA has gained acceptance as a health-care ser- teenth century as the United States transitioned
vice, the licensing of behavior analysts has fromaservice-orientedeconomytoamanufacturing-­
gained momentum at a time when licensure laws based economy, and legislators, consumers, and
have come under fire for the potential barriers professionals sought to establish mechanisms that
they may create, both for the consumers they are would ensure quality and consumer safety
meant to protect and the professionals they aim (Kleiner & Krueger, 2010). In its earliest form,
to regulate. As the field of behavior analysis joins licensure of an occupation acted as a resource for
the regulatory fray of state licensure, this chapter consumers who sought to identify a professional’s
examines the impetus of such laws, the elements minimal qualifications. Qualifications – or stan-
of an effective law, the features of a disruptive dards – for a given occupation are typically devel-
law, and when and whether licensing of behavior oped by members of that occupation, who then
analysts makes sense. often act as the gatekeepers to new members of
the field in the form of a regulatory or licensure
board. For this reason, some would argue that
occupational licensing is not solely intended to
J. Kornack (*) ensure consumer protection and act as a mechanism
Center for Autism and Related Disorders,
Woodland Hills, CA, USA by which to set and preserve standards. Renowned
e-mail: J.Kornack@centerforautism.com economist Milton Friedman characterized

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 85


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_6
86 J. Kornack

occupational regulation as an effort to impose the Forms of Occupational Regulation


monopoly that is anathema to capitalist econo-
mies, asserting that regulation of professions was Occupational regulation may take the form of reg-
intended to limit those who could join the profes- istration, certification, or licensure. Registration
sion and thereby drive up the cost for consumers is the least restrictive form of regulation, with
of the professionals’ services (Friedman, 1962). states typically requiring minimal information,
The number of licensing laws across the United such as an individual’s name, address, and quali-
States has grown considerably, with 4.5% of the fications. Certification may require the same
workforce holding at least one occupational basic information but likely incorporates an exam
license in the 1950s and approximately 29% of the or some other applicant assessment in order for
workforce holding some sort of occupational the government to certify an applicant’s qualifica-
license in 2009 (Kleiner & Park, 2014). As recently tions. Licensure imposes the greatest amount of
as July of 2015, President Obama’s administration regulation and – barring exclusions – makes it
weighed in on the practice of occupational licens- illegal to practice the profession without a license
ing, acknowledging potential benefits to consumer (Kleiner & Park, 2014).
health and safety but cautioning states to weigh the
costs and benefits of licensing to both the profes-
sion and its consumers and urging state regulators  mergence of Licensing of Behavior
E
to identify best practices and evaluate whether Analysts
their state licensing practices warrant reform
(Department of the Treasury Office of Economic Licensure of behavior analysts has arisen for dif-
Policy, the Council of Economic Advisers, and the ferent reasons in different states. As a wealth of
Department of Labor, 2015). research studies have demonstrated the effective-
ness of ABA in treating autism, ensuing legisla-
tion and regulatory guidance have increased
Authority of States to License access to ABA (Granpeesheh et al., 2009; Matson
& Smith, 2008). Insurance reform (i.e., autism
While federal labor laws typically supersede mandates), the Affordable Care Act (ACA), clari-
state law, this is not true for occupational licens- fication that autism treatment is a covered benefit
ing. In the late 1800s, the US Supreme Court under Medicaid for beneficiaries under 21 years
issued a decision in Dent v. West Virginia (1988) of age (Centers for Medicare and Medicaid
that “took away the federal right of preemption in Services, 2014), and a stronger federal mental
the arena of occupational licensing and gave it to health parity law (Paul Wellstone and Pete
the states” (Kleiner, 2006, p. 21). That is, Dent v. Domenici Mental Health Parity and Addiction
West (1988) empowered states to enact licensing Equity Act of 2008) have collectively paved the
laws without federal oversight. As a result, occu- way for reimbursement of ABA in the health-care
pational licensing varies widely from state to field. As a result, the field of behavior analysis has
state, both in terms of the occupations that are grown considerably since the first study demon-
regulated and the regulatory framework that strating the effectiveness of ABA in treating ASD
underpins those licensing laws. Additionally, (Lovaas, 1987).
because professional licenses are granted at the States have responded to this growth in many
state level, professionals who practice in more instances by legislating standards, enacting licen-
than one state are often required to attain and sure laws, and/or creating registries in an effort to
maintain multiple licenses. This aspect of licen- regulate behavior analysts, safeguard consumers,
sure is becoming more relevant as health-care and – in some instances – comply with a state’s
systems increasingly rely on telehealth to deliver legal or regulatory framework for insurance
health care to underserved and rural areas reimbursement. In states where licensure is
(Thomas & Capistrant, 2016). required for delivery of health-care services or a
6  Behavior Analysis Licensure 87

perception exists that licensure is required, either process “perhaps the greatest challenge” in the
for reimbursement by a third-party payer (e.g., effort to license behavior analysts and stated that
insurance carrier, health plan, state agency, etc.) “Some professions are well-equipped to partici-
or as a general requirement of state insurance pate in the political process. Behavior analysis is
laws and regulations, passage of a bill to license not one of them at present” (p. 61).
behavior analysts has often accompanied or
shortly followed the passage of the state’s autism Professional Consensus  For better or worse,
mandate (i.e., a law requiring some or all state-­ professional organizations typically play a criti-
regulated insurance policies to include coverage cal role in developing a state’s licensure frame-
of autism treatment) or implementation of an work, from engendering the political will to pass
autism treatment benefit under Medicaid. a licensure law and drafting the text of that law to
Some states have sought licensure in response influencing the selection of the first members of
to successful campaigns for licensure by promi- the licensing board. As the prevalence of ASD
nent members of the field of behavior analysis has increased and the framework for autism treat-
who view licensure of behavior analysts as an ment has evolved, professional consensus on
opportunity to protect both consumers and the whether to license behavior analysts has been
field of behavior analysis from unqualified prac- elusive. As recently as 2009, dueling articles
titioners; codify educational, training, and expe- appeared in Behavior Analysis in Practice offer-
riential standards; and ensure that behavior ing two different perspectives on the licensure of
analysts have the right to practice ABA indepen- behavior analysts. In their article Licensing
dently without the supervision of another licensed Behavior Analysts: Risks and Alternatives, Green
professional, such as a psychologist (Dorsey, and Johnston (2009) assert that pursuit of licen-
Weinberg, Zane, & Guidi, 2009; Hassert, Kelly, sure for behavior analysts is premature and that
Pritchard, & Cautilli, 2008). Whereas some states the role of the Behavior Analyst Certification
have enacted licensure laws without much con- Board (BACB) as a certifying entity is sufficient,
troversy, other states have encountered opposi- whereas Dorsey et al. (2009) make the case that
tion, ranging in intensity from mild to fierce. In licensure is overdue and that “continued depen-
states with active resistance to licensure, the dence on a board certification process will not be
effort to license behavior analysts likely requires adequate to protect consumers” (p. 53).
a combination of political will, professional con- Green and Johnston (2009) ask a critical ques-
sensus, and consumer support. tion that may foreshadow the problems that arise
as licensing of behavior analysts begins to prolif-
Political Will  Political will plays an important erate: “Are there enough practitioners eligible for
role in efforts to expand licensure of behavior ana- licensure to provide easy access to services for
lysts. Beyond the fundamental need for a legisla- consumers?” This question lies at the heart of the
tor to be motivated to sponsor a licensing bill, the struggle to find professional consensus in the
legislative committees through which a bill passes effort to enact licensure laws. On the one hand,
scrutinize a wide-ranging variety of elements, legislators and consumers are loathe to support a
including potential revenue from license fees, licensing bill that could hinder access to ABA by
costs associated with a new licensing board, the prohibiting individuals who currently provide
support or opposition of special interest groups, ABA services from practicing. On the other hand,
and the impact of licensure on constituents. In many behavior analysts worry that the quality of
general, political will arises when a problem ABA services will be diminished if the scope of a
exists that has produced widespread concern licensure act encompasses other licensed
which, in turn, engenders widespread support for ­professionals, making the point that “competence
potential solutions. Political will is fragile, in behavior analysis cannot be assumed” of psy-
though, and fades quickly amidst controversy. chologists and other licensed professionals
Green and Johnston (2009) called the political (Shook, 1993). Consequently, as some behavior
88 J. Kornack

analysts seek to limit licensure to BACB certifi- requirements that make an individual eligible to
cants, others work to ensure the ability of other sit for the BCBA or BCaBA exam. As of 2016,
licensed professionals (e.g., psychologists, mar- 20,000 professionals had secured the BCBA or
riage and family therapists, etc.) to practice ABA, Board Certified Behavior Analyst-Doctoral
either by exempting them from the licensure act or (BCBA-D) credential, and 2,315 professionals
by allowing such professionals to qualify for licen- held a BCaBA (Behavior Analyst Certification
sure as behavior analysts. Wrongly or rightly, the Board [BACB], 2016b). Through its certification
effort to strike a balance between expanding access programs, the BACB has created a valuable
to treatment without diluting treatment quality is framework for practitioners of behavior analysis.
inevitably influenced by the insufficient number of In fact, Dixon et al. (2016) found that “supervi-
BACB certificants in light of the rate of ASD. sors with BCBA certifications produce 73.7%
greater mastery of learning objective per hour as
Consumer Support  Although consumer pro- compared to supervisors without a BCBA.”
tection is a primary impetus for most state licens- The Model Act for Licensing/Regulating
ing laws, consumers in the autism community Behavior Analysts, Revised September 2012
may be wary of the potential for licensure to limit (BACB, 2012), offered by the BACB to states
access to treatment by imposing requirements contemplating licensure of behavior analysts,
that proscribe some providers from practicing seeks to codify the BACB’s BCBA and BCaBA
ABA. Consumers who are accustomed to ABA credentials as the primary paths to licensure. As a
may be confident in their ability to choose a pro- result of the effective dissemination of the
vider and hesitant to have that choice limited by a BACB’s Model Act, many state licensure require-
licensure requirement. Consumers for whom ments mirror the BACB’s certification require-
ABA is uncharted territory may, in turn, be more ments. Given the effectiveness of BCBAs in
supportive of a licensure law that gives ABA producing a higher rate of skill mastery in chil-
treatment the regulatory structure of most other dren with ASD, the BACB’s Model Act contains
health-care services. Certainly, consumer sup- important education, training, and experience
port – or, at a minimum, lack of vocal consumer requirements that have demonstrated their effec-
opposition – plays a role in the effort to pass any tiveness (Dixon et al., 2016). The drawbacks cre-
licensure bill, including those that would license ated by relying solely on the BACB Model Act,
behavior analysts (Kleiner, 2006). however, echo the challenges experienced in the
effort to reach professional consensus. One recur-
ring issue in licensure initiatives is that not all
To BACB or Not to BACB behavior analysts have pursued BACB certifica-
tion; most often, the careers of these behavior
In 2007, the credentialing programs of the analysts predate the establishment of the BACB
Behavior Analyst Certification Board (BACB), in and its credentials. That is, prominent behavior
use since the 1990s (Kazemi & Shapiro, 2013), analysts have chosen not to add the BCBA cre-
were accredited by the National Commission for dential to their existing degrees, having worked
Certifying Agencies (NCCA), demonstrating that for decades without any such credential. While
the credentialing programs for the Board Certified the BACB Model Act exempts some profession-
Behavior Analyst (BCBA) and the Board als from the license requirement, it precludes all
Certified Assistant Behavior Analyst (BCaBA) but psychologists from calling themselves behav-
met the rigorous standards of the NCCA and, ior analysts.
therefore, effectively assessed professional com- Notably, the BACB is careful to ensure that
petency. With its credentialing process, the the BCBA and BCaBA credentials are not autism
BACB has established a certification for behavior specific but, rather, pertain to the entire field of
analysts and assistant behavior analysts that iden- behavior analysis as a whole. Therefore, it is rel-
tifies the education, training, and experience evant to note that an individual can complete the
6  Behavior Analysis Licensure 89

extensive education, training, and experience Standards and the BACB Guidelines for
requirements and pass the BCBA or BCaBA Responsible Conduct for Behavior Analysts”
exam without having any knowledge of or expe- (BACB, 2012, p.4). States may be reluctant to link
rience with people affected by ASD. In that con- a state license to an ethical code whose content is
text, behavior analysts whose education, training, not controlled by the state and whose causes for
and careers predate the founding of the BACB disciplinary action may include proprietary mat-
may be hard-pressed to understand why they find ters that do not reflect the state’s interests.
themselves struggling to preserve their right to
practice when a licensure law is implemented
that gives the only path to licensure to BCBAs Licensure Boards
and BCaBAs.
Oregon’s recent experience with its effort to When a licensure law is enacted, oversight of the
license behavior analysts is illustrative of the license may fall to a state agency or may be dele-
controversy that may arise between BCBAs and gated to a licensing board. These boards typically
non-BCBAs. In many states, such as Oregon, promulgate rules to implement the licensure law.
coverage of autism treatment by a third-party Behavior analysts are regulated by their own
payer is relatively new, and the number of BCBAs board in just under one-third of the states that
with clinical practices specializing in autism is require behavior analysts to be licensed
quite small. When Oregonians first had access to (Association of Professional Behavior Analysts,
autism treatment through health insurance, 2015). Depending on the language in the licen-
Oregon likely had an autism population number- sure act, an existing board (e.g., psychology) may
ing over 12,0001 but fewer than 50 BCBAs be directed to incorporate oversight of behavior
(BACB, 2016a), and only about half of those analysts. The composition of a board varies but
were autism treatment providers. Despite the typically includes members of the profession,
daunting gulf between demand and supply, prom- members of related professions, and consumers
inent behavior analysts led the charge to impose a who are served by the profession. The BACB
licensing structure that would limit licensure to Model Act recommends that “An overwhelming
BACB certificants. Although other professionals majority of the members of the Regulatory
may have been able to continue practicing ABA, Authority should be Board Certified Behavior
they would likely have been unsuccessful in any Analysts with additional membership of at least
effort to be reimbursed by insurance entities. one Board Certified Assistant Behavior Analyst
Another controversial component of the and at least one Consumer/Public Member”
BACB Model Act may be that it contains lan- (BACB, 2012, p. 2–3). A recent decision by the
guage that technically makes it illegal for family US Supreme Court in North Carolina State Board
members to use ABA outside of the home, only of Dental Examiners v. Federal Trade Commission
exempting family members from licensure (2015) may cause state licensing boards, includ-
“within the home” as long as they are acting ing those that regulate behavior analysts, to
“under the extended authority and direction of a rethink their board composition and licensure
Licensed Behavior Analyst or a Licensed Assistant regulations. In its decision, the Supreme Court
Behavior Analyst” (BACB, 2012, p. 7). This held that “State licensing boards are not automati-
restrictive language has prompted consumers to cally exempted from antitrust scrutiny…if a con-
oppose licensing bills in the past. The BACB trolling number of board members are themselves
Model Act also incorporates “compliance with ‘active market participants’” (Department of the
the BACB Professional Disciplinary and Ethical Treasury Office of Economic Policy, the Council
of Economic Advisers, and the Department of
1  Labor, 2015). That is, if a majority of the mem-
Based on US Census Bureau Population Estimate for
2013 of Individuals Under 18 and CDC Prevalence Rate bers of a licensing board that regulates behavior
of 1:68. analysts earn income as practitioners of behavior
90 J. Kornack

analysis, then behavior analysts whose market to practice up to 30 days annually without
participation (i.e., income) is adversely affected obtaining a California license. Guest licensure
by the rules promulgated by that board may have provisions are uncommon in licensing acts for
cause to pursue antitrust litigation. This decision behavior analysts, however, and this missing
seems to be in harmony with Milton Friedman’s element in the licensure of behavior analysts is
view that occupational regulation can produce likely to exacerbate delays and provider
monopolies (Friedman, 1962). State boards are shortages, especially if additional states decide to
less vulnerable to antitrust allegations when states license behavior analysts.
play a greater role in the supervision of their regu- Often in licensure laws, states grant reciproc-
latory boards and if the majority of board mem- ity or license by endorsement to a person who is
bers are not “active market participants” licensed in another state that “imposes compara-
(Department of the Treasury Office of Economic ble licensure requirements” (BACB, 2012, p. 9).
Policy, the Council of Economic Advisers, and Unlike guest licensure provisions, reciprocity
the Department of Labor, 2015, p. 52). provisions offer temporary or permanent licen-
sure in the state granting the reciprocity. Although
the BACB includes a provision for reciprocity in
 uest Licensure and Reciprocity
G its Model Act (BACB 2012, p. 9), it does not
Provisions appear to be a provision that has been adopted
frequently, possibly because reciprocity in behav-
Since licensure laws are enacted at the state level, ior analyst licensure is less relevant when states
guest licensure provisions are common. Without a rely on the BCBA and BCaBA certifications,
guest licensure provision, a licensed professional which do not vary from one state to the next.
in one state is not allowed to practice in another
state that requires licensure until s/he secures that
state’s license. Guest licensure enables a behavior Conclusion
analyst who is licensed in State “A” to practice in
State “B” for a specified period of time before As the field of behavior analysis continues to
being subject to the licensure requirements of grow and ABA is increasingly recognized as a
State “B.” Guest licensure provisions are impor- medically necessary treatment, licensure seems
tant for a number of reasons. Such provisions act to be a natural next-step, especially in states that
as de facto grace periods when a behavior analyst require health-care providers to be licensed.
moves from one state to another, so the behavior While occupational regulation has the potential
analyst can work as a behavior analyst on his/her to legitimize a field, elevate its standards, and
first day in a new state. Guest licensure provisions protect consumers, it also has the potential to act
also facilitate the use of telehealth, so a behavior as an impediment to growth and access to medi-
analyst living in State “A” can occasionally or cally necessary treatment.
temporarily provide services in State “B.” This is Despite the significant growth of the field of
especially useful in bridging gaps created by pro- behavior analysis, the field has not been able to
vider shortages, which are systemic in the field of keep pace with the extraordinary demand for its
autism treatment. services. As long as the number of behavior ana-
Nearly all states include a guest licensure lysts is insufficient to meet the demand for behav-
provision in their licensure laws for psychologists. ior analytic services, efforts to exempt other
For example, Arizona allows psychologists who licensed professionals from a license act that
are licensed in another state to practice in Arizona would otherwise proscribe them from practicing
without an Arizona license up to 20 days per ABA are likely to be regarded as in the best inter-
year. California allows out-of-state psychologists ests of the public. To this point, consider that 1:68
6  Behavior Analysis Licensure 91

children in the United States are diagnosed with When consumers, behavior analysts, and legis-
ASD (Christensen et al., 2016) and that the num- lators agree on the need to license behavior ana-
ber of BCBAs and BCaBAs in the United States lysts, the details of the licensing bill may be divisive
totaled under 25,000 in 2016 (BACB, 2016a). If as legislators consider the educational, training,
we relied solely on BCBAs and BCaBAs to treat and experience requirements, as well as which pro-
the autism population under 18 [US CENSUS fessionals to exempt from the license law. In addi-
Bureau Population Estimate for 2013 of individ- tion to exacerbating a pervasive shortage of autism
uals under 18], every BACB certificant in the treatment providers, license laws that limit the
United States would need to maintain a caseload practice of ABA to BCBAs draw the ire of psy-
of nearly 50 children for supply to meet demand. chologists, social workers, and other licensed pro-
Then, consider that this scenario omits the num- fessionals for whom ABA may be in their scope of
ber of adults who need ABA and overlooks the practice. On the other hand, licensure efforts that
many BACB certificants who do not work as place oversight of behavior analysts under a board
autism treatment providers, and any proliferation of psychology, such as the license bill that failed in
of licensure laws that hinders access to ABA may California,2 are viewed by some behavior analysts
very well be the source of a public health emer- as diluting the effort to assert behavior analysis as
gency, not only depriving individuals with ASD its own profession, worthy of its own regulatory
of the treatment they need but, also, shifting the board. Often, a regulatory board promulgates the
cost of caring for these individuals from insur- rules that have the greatest impact on access to
ance entities to state and local governments ABA, so the composition of the board is critical.
whose budgets grow more strained each year. Additionally, board composition that creates a
Common justification for licensure is the majority of active market participants may be vul-
assertion that it preserves or increases the quality nerable to antitrust allegations.
of service, thus protecting consumers from the Currently, licensure of behavior analysts is in its
harm of receiving services from a less qualified early days, so we can only hypothesize about effec-
or unqualified person. Such consumer protection tive elements of licensure laws governing behavior
is in a state’s interests to ensure the well-being of analysts. (See Table 6.1 for Considerations in
its citizens and insulate the state from the likely Evaluating Effectiveness of Licensure Laws &
financial consequences of a consumer’s poor Regulations for Behavior Analysts.) Going for-
decision, i.e., providing long-term services and ward, states should solicit and provide data to dem-
supports to consumers who may not have onstrate the effectiveness of these laws.
required them had they been prevented – or pro- Additionally, states – or professional organizations
tected – from receiving services from an unqual- acting on behalf of the states – should survey prac-
ified person. If licensure substantially narrows titioners and consumers of behavior analysis to
the field of available behavior analysts, though, identify challenges that may have been inadver-
family members may be relegated to implement- tently created by licensure laws, recognizing, in the
ing “do-it-­yourself remedies,” the consequences face of the prevalence of ASD, that it is in the best
of which are unlikely to be captured in any interests of the state to facilitate liberal access to
assessment of a license law’s effectiveness behavior analysis to ensure that consumers do not
(Svorny, 2000, p. 297). Recent guidance from encounter unnecessary barriers to critical treatment.
the federal government suggests that additional
scrutiny of all licensure laws is warranted to 2 
California Assembly Bill (2016) is an act to amend
ensure that the benefits do, in fact, outweigh the Sections 27 and 2920 of; to amend, repeal, and add Sections
2922, 2923, and 2927 of; to add Chapter 6.7 (commencing
cost and that the laws function effectively for the
with Section 2999.10) to Division 2 of; and to repeal
consumers they seek to protect and the profes- Sections 2999.20, 2999.26, 2999.31, and 2999.33 of the
sionals they seek to regulate. Business and Professions Code, relating to healing arts.
92 J. Kornack

Table 6.1  Considerations in evaluating effectiveness of licensure laws and regulations for behavior analysts
Inquiry Consideration(s)
Will current practitioners of ABA be able Given the current rate of ASD and the limited number of BACB
to continue practicing once the law takes certificants, states should examine whether it is helpful to ensure that
effect? the licensure law allows non-BACB certificants to practice.
Are behavior analysts able to practice Master’s and doctoral-level behavior analysts should be allowed to
independently? practice without supervision from another licensed professional as
long as they are acting within the scope of their competency.
Do education, training, and experience Setting aside the BACB certification, licensure acts should
requirements reflect the standard of care? incorporate education, training, and experience requirements that
reflect the standard of care.
Does the licensure act support the Licensure acts should incorporate all three levels of ABA treatment
tiered-­delivery model of ABA? delivery: (1) a master’s or doctoral-level supervisor, (2) a bachelor’s
level assistant supervisor, and (3) a behavior technician who meets
minimal education and training requirements.
Do the behavior technician requirements, if The position of behavior technician is an entry-level position, and
addressed, reflect the standard of care? requirements should be minimal (i.e., a high school diploma or
equivalent or higher, 40 h of training, and 15 h of practicum).
Are family members, teachers, and other Outcomes are likely to be optimized when caregivers have the
caregivers able to implement ABA across opportunity to support treatment by implementing ABA to the best
all environments? of their ability. While training caregivers is important, a licensure act
should not prohibit family members and others from implementing
ABA as long as they do not call themselves behavior analysts or seek
reimbursement.
Does the composition of the board To avoid scrutiny for potential antitrust violations, a majority of
adequately represent all stakeholders while board members should not be active market participants.
protecting the interests of the state? Consideration should be given to individuals who do not earn
income as practitioners of behavior analysis. All stakeholders should
be represented.
Is consumer safety adequately addressed? Consumer safety is greatest when every member of the treatment
team is required to submit proof of an active (ongoing) background
check. Ideally, the state should offer public access to a registry
through which credentials and active background checks can be
confirmed. A mechanism should be in place to receive and evaluate
complaints and, when necessary, impose disciplinary action.
Does the licensure act include a guest To avoid unnecessary barriers to ABA, a licensure act should include
licensure provision? a guest licensure provision that allows behavior analysts who are
licensed in another state to practice a specified number of days each
year without a license.
Is the ethics code culturally sensitive? Ethics codes should accommodate efforts of licensees to be
culturally sensitive; e.g., a rule that prohibits the licensee from
accepting gifts from the patient should incorporate professional
discretion that allows a licensee to accept, for example, a plate of
cookies from a parent who may be offended if the offering is
declined.
6  Behavior Analysis Licensure 93

References with autism: A description and review of treatment


research. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 21, 162–173.
Green, G., & Johnston, J. M. (2009). Licensing behavior
Association of Professional Behavior Analysts. (2015).
analysts: Risks and alternatives. Behavior Analysis in
Overview of state laws to license or otherwise
Practice, 2, 59–64.
regulate practitioners of applied behavior analy-
Hassert, D. L., Kelly, A. N., Pritchard, J. K., & Cautilli,
sis. Retrieved from http://www.apbahome.net/pdf/
J. D. (2008). The licensing of behavior analysts:
StateRegulationofBA080615.pdf
Protecting the profession and the public. Journal of
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2012). Model act
Early and Intensive Behavioral Intervention, 5, 8–19.
for licensing/regulating behavior analysts. Retrieved
Kazemi, E., & Shapiro, M. (2013). A review of board
from http://bacb.com/wp-content/uploads/2015/05/
standards across behavioral health professions: Where
BACB_Model_Act.pdf
does the BCBA credential stand? Behavior Analysis is
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2016a). Certificant
Practice, 6, 28–29.
registry. Retrieved from http://info.bacb.com/o.
Kleiner, M. M. (2006). Licensing occupations: Ensuring
php?page=100155
quality or restricting competition? Kalamazoo, MI:
Behavior Analyst Certification Board. (2016b). Data on
W. E. Upjohn Institute for Employment Research.
credential holders. Retrieved from http://bacb.com/
Kleiner, M. M., & Krueger, A. B. (2010). The preva-
about-the-bacb/
lence and effects of occupational licensing. British
Centers for Medicare and Medicaid Services. (2014,
Journal of Industrial Relations, 48, 676–687.
July 7). CMCS Informational Bulletin, Clarification
doi;10.1111/j.1467-8543.2010.00807.x
of medicaid coverage of services to children with
Kleiner, M. M., & Park, K. W. (2014). Life, limbs, and
autism. Retrieved from http://www.medicaid.gov/
licensing: Occupational regulation, wages, and work-
Federal-­Policy-­Guidance/Downloads/CIB-07-07-14.
place safety of electricians, 1992–2007. Monthly
pdf
Labor Review. Retrieved from Bureau of Labor
Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V., Blider, D.,
Statistics website: http://www.bls.gov/mlr/2014/arti-
Charles, J., Constantino, J. N., … Yeargin-Allsopp, M.
cle/life-limbs-and-licensing.htm
(2016). Prevalence and characteristics of autism spec-
Lovaas, I. O. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal
trum disorder among children aged 8 Years – Autism
educational and intellectual functioning in young
and developmental disabilities monitoring network,
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
11 Sites, United States, 2012. Morbidity and Mortality
Psychology, 55, 3–9.
Weekly Report Surveillance Summaries, 65, 1–23.
Matson, J. L., & Smith, K. R. M. (2008). Current status of
Dent v. West Virginia. (1988). 129 U.S. 114.
intensive behavioral interventions for young children
Department of the Treasury Office of Economic Policy, the
with autism and PDD-NOS. Research in Autism Spectrum
Council of Economic Advisers, and the Department of
Disorders, 2, 60–74. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.03.003
Labor. (2015). Occupational licensing: A framework
North Carolina State Board of Dental Examiners v.
for policymakers. Retrieved from https://www.white-
Federal Trade Commission, 135 S. Ct. 1101. (2015).
house.gov/sites/default/files/docs/licensing_report_
Paul Wellstone and Pete Domenici Mental Health Parity
final_nonembargo.pdf
and Addiction Equity Act of 2008, H.R. 6983, 110th
Dixon, D. R., Linstead, E., Granpeesheh, D., Novack,
Cong. (2008).
M. N., French, R., Stevens, E., … Powell, A.. (2016).
Shook, G. L. (1993). The professional credential in
An evaluation of the impact of supervision intensity,
behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 87–101.
supervisor qualifications, and caseload on outcomes in
Svorny, S. (2000). Licensing, market entry regulation.
the treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Behavior
In B. Bouckaert & G. De Geest (Eds.), Encyclopedia
Analysis in Practice. Advance online publication.
of law and economics (vol. III, pp. 296–328).
doi:10.1007/s40617-016-0132-1
Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.
Dorsey, M. F., Weinberg, M., Zane, T., & Guidi, M. M.
Thomas, L., & Capistrant, G. (2016). State telehealth gaps
(2009). The case for licensure of applied behavior ana-
analysis: Coverage and reimbursement, American
lysts. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 21, 53–58.
telehealth association. Retrieved from American
Friedman, M. (1962). Capitalism and freedom. Chicago,
Telemedicine website: http://www.americantelemed.
IL: University of Chicago Press.
org/docs/default-source/policy/2016_50-state-
Granpeesheh, D., Tarbox, J., & Dixon, D. R. (2009).
telehealth-­gaps-analysis--coverage-and-reimbursement.
Applied behavior analytic interventions for children
pdf?sfvrsn=2
Staff Training
7
Karola Dillenburger

obviously play a key role in the delivery of good


Introduction and consistent services.
As with other professionals, behaviour analysts
Staff training is important to ensure competent undergo extensive qualifying level training, with
delivery of services, regardless of the industry or both theory and practice components. Those who
the target audience. In addition, staff training has aim to become Board Certified Behavior Analysts
been identified as the most important factor in the (BCBA) undertake an approved Masters-level
reduction of the ‘revolving door’ of unwanted staff course sequence at a university, as well as 1500 h
turnover; other factors include supervision, pay of supervised practice, after which they have to
and job satisfaction (Kazemi, Shapiro, & Kavner, pass the Behavior Analyst Certification Board’s
2015). A comprehensive behavioural systems (BACB) exam. Subsequently, they have to engage
analysis (BSA) of workplace practices goes in substantive approved continuous education to
beyond this chapter (cf. Diener, McGee, & Miguel, maintain their certification. Those who take their
2009; Malott, Vunovich, Boettcher, & Groeger, training to doctoral levels can be designated as
1995; Strouse, Carroll-Hernandez, Sherman, & Board Certified Behavior Analysts-Doctoral
Sheldon, 2004), although it is important to note (BCBA-D). The process required to achieving and
that satisfaction with pay does not depend on the maintaining these certifications and designations
amount paid but rather on the control over income is described in more detail in Chap. 7 of this book.
(Abernathy, 2011). BCBAs and BCBA-Ds commonly supervise
In monetary terms, staff training is more eco- programmes that are delivered by Board Certified
nomic than staff turnover, which can cost between Assistant Behavior Analysts (BCaBA) and/or
15% and 200% the annual salary of the member Registered Behavior Technicians (RBT) or other
of staff who leaves (Sundberg, 2016). In human social or health-care professionals or paraprofes-
terms, staff training is even more valuable, not sionals. Staff training and supervision for all of
only for staff career prospects but, more impor- these professionals are central to their practice.
tantly, for the service user, because the skill and The Behavior Analyst Certification Board speci-
performance and the retention of good staff fies clear requirements for the minimum addi-
tional training of BCBA who engage in
supervision of trainee behaviour analysts (Sellers,
K. Dillenburger, BCBA-D (*)
Valentino, & LeBlanc, 2016); however, there are
Centre for Behaviour Analysis, Queens University
Belfast, Belfast, BT71HL, Northern Ireland, UK few guidelines as to evidence-based behaviour
e-mail: k.dillenburger@qub.ac.uk analytic methods to train staff.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 95


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_7
96 K. Dillenburger

In an overview of staff training, Reid, O’Kane analysis. Clearly there is a need for post-­
and Macurik, (2011) note the following: qualifying training.
Staff training and management represent a long-­ Most multidisciplinary teams also include
standing area of focus in applied behavior analysis. service users, and although some service users,
Soon after initial demonstrations of the efficacy of especially some adults with autism, have spo-
behavior analysis for improving the behavior of ken or written about ABA, usually they are not
people with special needs in the 1960s, attention
was directed to disseminating the emerging tech- trained in behaviour analysis, and therefore
nology among human service personnel. Concern they are unlikely to be au fait with the history
first centered on training service providers in basic and the accurate application of the science (e.g.
behavioral procedures to apply with people who Milton, 2012).
had developmental and related disabilities. Shortly
thereafter it became apparent that the same princi- Despite the identified need for post-qualifying
ples underlying behavior change procedures for training, most health, social care, medical and edu-
people with developmental disabilities were appli- cation professionals receive very little training in
cable with staff work performance. Concern over autism or behaviour analysis after they qualify.
training human service staff to use behavioral pro-
cedures and applying behavioral strategies to man- Subsequently, their understanding and knowledge
age staff performance continues today. (p. 281) in these areas remain very limited, unless they
have personally invested in further training. In
Effective high-quality staff training and supervi- fact, frequently, self-reported knowledge of autism
sion are crucial to the quality of services avail- and behaviour analysis overestimates actual
able to vulnerable service users. The current knowledge significantly (Dillenburger, McKerr, &
chapter focusses on training staff who work Jordan, 2016; Fennell & Dillenburger, 2016).
mainly in the areas of education, social and health Parents are often better informed about behaviour
care, and/or residential settings with individuals analysis and autism and more willing to learn than
with various kinds of disabilities or psychiatric professionals (Dillenburger, Keenan, Doherty,
diagnosis. Byrne, & Gallagher, 2012). Consequently, much
First up, it is important to note that behaviour depends on post-qualifying and in-service training
analysts do not work in isolation. More often for staff.
than not, they work in, or lead, multidisciplinary The need to focus on staff training methodolo-
teams or teams of paraprofessionals. Most multi- gies became apparent in the early 1960s, soon
disciplinary professionals working with children after evidence emerged of the effectiveness of
with autism, for example, will have received very behaviour analysis-based interventions, espe-
little training in autism and virtually no training cially in terms of improving the quality of lives of
in behaviour analysis during their qualifying people with intellectual disabilities and their fam-
training. This is true for teachers, educational ilies. It became obvious that the behaviour ana-
psychologists and other education staff. It is also lytic technology was not only effective in helping
the case for social workers, family therapists, clients or service users but that the same princi-
counsellors, cognitive behaviour therapists and ples also were relevant for training staff to deliver
other social care staff and for medical profession- these interventions. Obvious to a behaviour ana-
als, including paediatricians and psychiatrists. lyst is that client outcomes are a function of a
Equally, allied health professionals, such as number of contingencies, including staff training,
occupational therapists, clinical psychologists maintenance of staff skills, and transfer of staff
and speech and language pathologists, receive skills across settings, clients, and programmes
very limited training in autism or behaviour anal- (Jahr, 1998). Despite this realisation, staff training
ysis (Dillenburger et al., 2014b). Therefore, these has not received the same attention in behaviour
professionals cannot be expected to fully grasp analytic literature, as have i­nterventions for users
the complex concepts and basic scientific under- of behaviour analytic services (Macurik, O’Kane,
pinnings that inform the practice of behaviour Malanga, & Reid, 2008).
7  Staff Training 97

There is much more research in the area of evidence-­based behaviour analytic services are
parent training in behaviour analysis (Bearss now considered gold standard across most of
et al., 2015; Dillenburger et al., 2004; Green North America (Autism Speaks, 2014; Perry &
et al., 2010). This is relevant here, because while Condillac, 2003). Typically, the eclectic
there are important differences between staff approach resembles traditional special educa-
training and parent training, for example, for par- tion, where various intervention procedures are
ents of young children with developmental dis- implemented concurrently or consecutively
abilities including autism, both are linked by the without a common theoretical framework. This
common focus on the use of behaviour analytic kind of approach harbours a number of prob-
principles to teach behaviour analytic skills sets. lems, not least because it can become very dis-
Given that traditional didactic methods of jointed. For many reasons, true eclecticism is
staff training have shown to be not particularly impossible, i.e. staff cannot be trained in all
effective, behaviour analysts who are charged possible procedures, and as such selection of
with staff training have developed new and inno- intervention procedures necessarily remains
vative training methods (Parsons, Rollyson, & Reid, limited. In addition, there is ample evidence
2012; Reid et al., 2011). This research has identified that the eclectic approach is less effective than
a number of factors that are important, including: procedures that are based on behaviour analy-
sis (Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, &
1. Working collaboratively Stanislaw, 2005; Howard et al., 2014). In
2. Identifying necessary competences effect, eclecticism has been exposed as pseu-
3. Establishing performance measures doscience (Gardner, 1957), yet it remains sup-
4.
Designing and applying staff training ported in some countries (Dillenburger,
procedures McKerr, & Jordan, 2014a; Dillenburger, 2011),
5. Using technology in staff training for reasons defined by Tavis (2003):
6.
Implementing performance management, Pseudoscience is particularly attractive because,
generalisation and maintenance by definition, [it] promises certainty, whereas sci-
ence gives us probability and doubt. Pseudoscience
is popular because it confirms what we believe;
science is unpopular because it makes us question
Working Collaboratively what we believe. (pp. xv–xvi)

Regardless of where they work, behaviour ana- As a result, the eclectic approach has been
lysts are unlikely to work in isolation. It is much considered a ‘postcode lottery’ (Unumb, 2014)
more likely that they are collaborating with other and likened to a ‘haphazard pick and mix’
professionals, not only in the area of autism but approach (Cumine, Dunlop, & Stevenson,
also in education and other areas of application. 2009; Dillenburger, 2011; Howard et al., 2005,
Therefore, behaviour analysts need to be pre- 2014; Stanton, 2000).
pared to work and train in multidisciplinary It is important, however, to distinguish
teams, and, in turn, other professionals may between the eclectic approach and multidisci-
require some behaviour analytic training. plinary work. Multidisciplinary work is usually
It is important to note here the distinction highly valued by professionals and service users.
between multidisciplinary work and specific In fact, there are various ways in which profes-
methods of interventions, such as those used in sionals trained in different disciplines can work
an ‘eclectic approach’ (Dillenburger, 2011; together. Generally, when collaborating with dif-
Howard, Stanislaw, Green, Sparkman, & ferent disciplines in health, social care or educa-
Cohen, 2014). The eclectic approach is popular tion, each profession aims to address different
in autism services across Europe, despite the aspects of the same situation or diagnosis.
lack of ­evidence in its favour and the fact that Given different training histories, at times, it is
98 K. Dillenburger

not easy to explain the concepts of one discipline r­egularly reviewed in the task lists for BCBAs,
in terms that are comprehensible to professionals BCaBAs and RBTs (BACB, 2015), staff compe-
from other disciplines. Nevertheless, collabora- tences, performance and outcome measures are
tions can be valuable as they may have the poten- less clear for paraprofessionals or other staff who
tial to expand the scope of an existing discipline. therefore require training or supervision by
Ultimately, a range of professionals from differ- behaviour analysts. The BACB autism compe-
ent disciplines working together to the benefit of tency list (BACB, 2015) focusses mainly on ethi-
the service user can take various forms: cal practice, current best evidence-based
interventions, and staff accountability. However,
• Multidisciplinary work, where different disci- the autism competency list is only useful for staff
plines deal with different aspects of the same working in autism services and has limited appli-
problem which has the potential to address cation to other service areas. Other competency
more complex issues and realise efficiencies. frameworks generally refer to specific disci-
• Interdisciplinary work intends to address a plines, such as nursing or social work (BASW,
problem which requires knowledge from var- 2016), or specific methodologies, such as posi-
ied and multiple sources. Thus, interdisciplin- tive behaviour support (PBS Coalition UK,
ary work has the potentially to lead to the 2015).
development of an entirely new discipline. Employers commonly specify their expecta-
• Cross-disciplinary work means that aspects of tions of newly appointed staff in job specifica-
one discipline are explained in terms of the tions and internal policies and procedures
concepts and language of another. Cross-­ outlining how staff are to behave in relation to
disciplinary work thus has the potential to service users and co-workers, e.g. physical
expand the scope of existing disciplines. restraint procedures for persons who engage in
• Transdisciplinary work occurs when different challenging or injurious behaviours. Employers
disciplines retain their individual knowledge expect that, subsequently, staff are fluent in these
base but have a common theoretical basis. management procedures. However, oftentimes,
The key advantage of transdisciplinary work these training courses are brief and do not allow
is that professionals use a common language, for enough practice time to properly assess, gen-
and it therefore allows for a consistent and eralise and maintain fluency.
joint-up approach to the problem (Dillenburger Didactic teaching per se is not sufficient and,
et al., 2014b). given the frequent overuse of basic technology,
may lead to ‘death by PowerPoint’ (Taylor,
The discipline of behaviour analysis can have 2007), and ultimately, of course, staff training
beneficial impact in all of these settings, espe- and competence lists are only as useful as their
cially with regard to developing clear and trans- application in practice.
parent procedures for identifying target
behaviours (including the necessary staff compe-
tences) and for developing and implementing Establishing Performance Measures
behavioural measurement systems, effective
interventions, as well as generalisation and main- Detailed training and assessments are needed
tenance procedures. before staff acquire the necessary competencies.
As with any behavioural interventions, staff skills
require repeated measurement, through direct
Identifying Necessary Competences observation, video analysis, and/or written exam-
ination or testing. For example, the York Measure
While there are clear and relatively well-defined of Quality of Intensive Behavioural Intervention
staff competencies in relation to autism and (YMQI) was used by Denne, Thomas, Hastings
behaviour analysis, which are identified and and Hughes (2015) to assess competence according
7  Staff Training 99

to the UK Society of Behaviour Analysis A number of studies focussed on shorter staff


(UK-SBA) Autism Education Competence training courses in residential or other care set-
Framework (Level 1) for experienced and inex- tings. For example, McDonnell et al. (2008) ran a
perienced staff working with children on the short 3-day staff training course that focussed on
autism spectrum. They found that, while the the management of aggressive behaviour of ser-
instrument was able to differentiate between vice users on the autism spectrum. The training
these two groups, ‘[t]here were few associations for the intervention group was followed up over
between the different methods of assessing com- 10 months, while the control group received
petence’ (p. 67). Therefore, they concluded that training before this study but was not followed
one measure, when used in isolation, could not up. While the staff training itself increased staff
reliably assess all necessary competencies. confidence, with regard to staff coping, support
Thus, traditional questionnaire evaluations are or perceived control of challenging behaviours,
not sufficient for measuring acceptability of staff no training effects were observed, although
training systems. When staff were asked about reports of difficulties in the management of chal-
their preference between familiar and unfamiliar lenging behaviour reduced in both target groups.
staff training systems, they did not report any Others have applied staff training procedures
specific preference; however, when they were in the classroom. For example, Schmidt, Urban,
given a choice, they generally chose the familiar Luiselli, White and Harrington (2013) trained
format (Reid & Parsons, 1995). educational staff to implement behaviour
analysis-­based interventions, especially those
related to antecedent manipulations, e.g. appear-
Designing Staff Training Procedures ance, organisation and safety in the classrooms.
They used task directives, daily supervision and
There are a variety of training procedures, includ- graphic performance feedback and showed that
ing didactic teaching, in vivo and video model- favourable classroom environments were main-
ling, programmed instruction, peer tutoring, tained at a school for children with intellectual
written instruction, and on-the-job feedback. In and developmental disabilities over a two-week
most cases, staff training includes a combination period after completion of the intervention.
of these procedures, but there are only a few stud- Few studies have focussed on staff training for
ies who offer a component analysis. those who are working with high-functioning
Delamater et al. (1984) explored three differ- adolescents on the spectrum autism in naturalis-
ent staff management procedures to determine tic settings. A study by Palmen, Didden and
which would be more effective: (a) in-service Korzilius (2010) describes behavioural skills
training, (b) direct feedback of actual staff per- training that was conducted via group instruction
formance, and (c) role playing. Eight members of and supervisory feedback. The focus of this study
staff of an inpatient psychiatric unit took part, was on staff (a) providing positive reinforcement,
including nurses and aides, and their interactions (b) providing error correction and (c) initiating
with children were observed directly in naturalis- opportunities for students to show the target
tic settings for 21 weeks. They found that the in-­ response (i.e. asking for help). Data on student
service training was not very effective in terms of target behaviours showed that the intervention
changing staff behaviour, while direct feedback effect was limited; however, staff performance
led to temporary increases in appropriate staff specifically in relation to accurate use of error
responding. However, these changes were not correction procedures improved significantly,
maintained across time. The largest effect on and improvements were maintained across time.
staff behaviour was observed subsequent to the Generalisation of staff skills was limited, although
use of role playing that involved instruction, the intervention was considered effective in staff
modelling, behavioural rehearsal, feedback, and self-reported evaluations. Similar staff reports of
reinforcement of appropriate staff responses. high social validity were found in studies that
100 K. Dillenburger

focussed on teaching staff-specific procedures, social validity (Durgin, Mahoney, Cox, Weetjens,
e.g. discrete-trial teaching (DTT) (Sarokoff & & Poling, 2014).
Sturmey, 2008). Feedback from peers was effective as a staff
Staff skills are particularly important when it training method in a vocational programme for
comes to dealing with aberrant and challenging adults with intellectual disabilities. Working in
behaviours, such as self-injurious behaviour pairs, peers were trained to monitor, record and
(SIB). Courtemanche et al. (2014) evaluated the graph data, provide feedback and set goals with
effectiveness of staff training that included role the other staff member (Fleming & Sulzer-­
playing, in vivo training, feedback paired with Azaroff, 1992). Public verbal feedback deliv-
contingent money and an escape contingency on ered at staff meetings was effective in increasing
treatment fidelity of three frontline care staff, staff performance of training clients in self-help
who were monitored, both remotely and in per- skills; however, improvements were limited as
son. They found that intervention fidelity was the only behaviours that increased were those
high, both in role play and in vivo situations con- for which staff received feedback (Wilson, Reid,
tingent on ongoing feedback and money. Nigro-­ & Korabek-Pinkowski, 1991).
Bruzzi and Sturmey (2010) confirmed the Self-management procedures have also been
importance of instructions, modelling, rehearsal, used in staff training. A self-management
and feedback, for staff training resulting in Acceptance and Commitment Therapy-based
increases in staff performance across a range of training intervention (ACTr) was compared with
settings. psychoeducation training (PETr) in terms of staff
While much of the staff training literature attitudes towards patients diagnosed with person-
reports research conducted in the context of intel- ality disorder (PD), staff-patient relations, and
lectual disability services, less is known about staff well-being. While attitudes and staff-patient
staff training in dementia care settings despite the relations improved up to 6 months after training,
evidence of the effectiveness of staff training in staff well-being did not improve for either group
these settings (Spector, Orrell, & Goyder, 2013). (Clarke, Taylor, Lancaster, & Remington, 2015).
Here, on-the-job feedback is a popular staff train- Acceptability of immediate versus delayed
ing procedure especially when used after conven- verbal (spoken) feedback was assessed for staff
tional analogue staff training. Arco and du Toit working with people with severe disabilities, fol-
(2006) explored the effectiveness of staff feed- lowing classroom-based instruction (Reid &
back in nursing staff subsequent to conventional Parsons, 1995). Similarly, abbreviated perfor-
staff training group workshops and showed that mance feedback was assessed as a training strat-
workshops alone did not increase staff perfor- egy for paraprofessional staff, including verbal
mance sufficiently; however, with on-the-job praise for accurate skills and clarification/redi-
feedback, all staff participants achieved and rection for incorrect performance. Staff skills
maintained competency and the procedures were improved rapidly and were maintained post-­
considered socially valid. When verbal feedback training. The procedures showed high social
alone and verbal feedback with approval state- validity and acceptance (Leblanc, Ricciardi, &
ments were compared, findings showed that, not Luiselli, 2005).
surprisingly, the former was less effective than The relative importance of performance feed-
the latter in decreasing off-task and increasing back in the acquisition and maintenance of skills
on-task staff behaviour (Brown, Willis, & Reid, is related to discriminative and/or reinforcing
1981). In another study, a multipurpose job aid functions. Roscoe, Fisher, Glover and Volkert
and feedback training package were used to (2006) evaluated the relative contributions of
improve skills of supervisors and animal trainers these two functions and found that performance-­
in a nongovernmental organisation in resource-­ specific instructions were more important to
poor area of East Africa, showing high levels of skill acquisition than contingent reinforcement
skills maintenance and generalisation as well as (i.e. money).
7  Staff Training 101

However, these findings are in contrast to Using Technology in Staff Training


Brackett, Reid and Green (2007), who explored
reactivity of staff behaviour to observations of A key development in staff training relates to
their work performance. They found that interven- the use of technology, most frequently video-
tion fidelity was higher when performance obser- based procedures. For example, Macurik et al.
vations were inconspicuous rather than when (2008) compared video versus live staff train-
observations were conspicuous, even after staff (in ing procedures and found that, according to
this case job coaches) were taught to record their knowledge quizzes and on-the-job observa-
own job performances. These findings are in line tions, both training methods were effective,
with those from a 7-day intensive Mindfulness- although video training had the edge in terms of
Based Positive Behaviour Support (MBPBS) direct-contact time with staff, if the time to
training that was provided for group home staff make the videos was not included in the calcu-
who were regularly exposed to severely challeng- lations, while live training had slightly better
ing service user behaviours. Results showed sig- social validity.
nificant reductions of verbal redirection, disuse of With regard to specific staff skills, for exam-
physical restraints, cessation of injuries and reduc- ple, functional behavioural assessment (FBA),
tion in staff stress and turnover, as well as substan- training is frequently conducted using video
tial financial savings (Singh et al., 2015). Thus, modelling, lectures, feedback, and written proto-
self-generated feedback can be effective and cols (NcCahill, Healy, & Ramey, 2014). Williams
socially valid (Arco, 2008). and Gallinat (2011) compared the use of video-
In other contexts, group instruction and super- taped feedback and video modelling, while
visory feedback was effective in behavioural Huskens, Reijers and Didden (2012) developed a
skills training on providing positive reinforce- training package regarding Pivotal Response
ment and error correction and initiating opportu- Treatment (PRT) for young children with autism
nities for adolescents with autism to ask for help that was delivered across a 2-day training work-
in naturalistic training settings. Skills generalisa- shop, followed up with live feedback and video
tion and maintenance were achieved for staff per- feedback for 3 months after training workshop.
formance (Palmen et al., 2010). Use of In addition to staff training, they also included
most-to-least prompting within teaching proce- parent training across eight group sessions and
dures and use of manual signs have also been two individual sessions (see also Huskens &
used in staff training (Parsons et al., 2012). Verburg, 2011). In the case of training for the use
Very brief staff training seems to achieve of a Picture Exchange and Communication
mixed outcomes. A one-session staff training System (PECS), verbal instructions in addition to
procedure was assessed in Hong Kong regarding an instructional video did not result in much
its effectiveness in reducing challenging behav- improvement in staff skill (Barnes, Dunning, &
iour in children with autism. A large group of Rehfeldt, 2011).
frontline staff (n = 311) either received psycho- Some basic applied behaviour analysis-based
education (PE), training about functional behav- procedures can be trained very quickly and effec-
iour analysis (FBA) and about emotional tively. For example, brief instruction, a video
management (EM), or were allocated to the con- model, and rehearsal with verbal feedback were
trol group. While training workshop appeared to effective in training staff to conduct stimulus
increase knowledge of autism, it actually preference assessments using a paired stimulus
decreased behavioural intention (Ling & Mak, and other formats (Lavie & Sturmey, 2002;
2012). On the other hand, classroom-based Roscoe & Fisher, 2008). Equally, procedural
instruction, role playing, feedback and brief on-­ integrity in discrete trial has been successfully
the-­job training lead to enhanced intervention increased following staff training using video
fidelity and improved child behaviours (Schepis, modelling (Catania, Almeida, Liu-Constant, &
Reid, Ownbey, & Parsons, 2001). DiGennaro Reed, 2009).
102 K. Dillenburger

More general training in basic teaching skills staff training (i.e. video and graphic feedback) in
was conducted in a 1-day, classroom-based train- a residential setting for children with intellectual
ing event using verbal and video instruction, fol- disabilities and attention deficit hyperactivity dis-
lowed by practice and on-the-job feedback to order (ADHD). Staff training occurred on an
train undergraduate interns, teacher aides, and individual basis; feedback to staff was presented
residential staff. Results showed social validity during routine staff meetings, yet generalisation
and improved skill levels (Parsons, Reid, & and maintenance was not achieved. Other proce-
Green, 1996). Video feedback used in addition to dures that did not lead to generalised or main-
instruction has been shown to lead to substantial tained gains in staff skills include, for example,
increases in correct trainer behaviour as well as communication training, which if used alone was
staff’s correct response prompting and child’s not sufficient to maintain staff skill of fostering
correct responses (van Vonderen, de Swart, & appropriate communicative interactions with
Didden, 2010). adults with challenging behaviour (Schmidt
While direct staff training (i.e. in vivo) is the et al., 2013). Equally, while daily feedback led to
most often used staff training setting, virtual improved staff implementation of instructions,
training (e.g. videoconferencing) is utilised prompts and consequences, and staff knowledge
increasingly. Hay-Hansson and Eldevik (2013) in certain content areas (communication and
compared brief (3 × 15 min) in vivo and video-­ gross motor skills), this learning did not gener-
based training in matching, receptive and expres- alise across content areas, e.g. training in com-
sive labelling and found no significant differences munication skills development did not generalise
between the groups. However, it is important to to gross motor skills (Page, Iwata, & Reid, 1982).
note that behaviour skills training (BST) that When attention is paid to the importance of
included instructions, modelling, rehearsal, and generalisation and maintenance as part of a train-
feedback has been found to be more effective ing package, through instructions, feedback,
than computer-based training in staff skills devel- rehearsal, and modelling, this can lead to rapid
opment of implementing DTT (Nosik, Williams, and large improvements in treatment fidelity of
Garrido, & Lee, 2013). teachers, for example, in using discrete-trial
More advanced use of technology, for exam- teaching (Sarokoff & Sturmey, 2004). Similarly,
ple, data collection via Bluetooth®, in combina- improvements in appearance, organisation, and
tion with immediate feedback, self-monitoring safety of classrooms for children with disabilities
and delayed positive feedback using video clips were achieved and maintained by combining task
and graphs, has been used effectively for staff directives, daily supervision, and graphic perfor-
training, for example, in a setting for young mance feedback (Schmidt et al., 2013). In-service
adults with autism (Nepo, 2010). and in-service plus feedback training also lead to
improved data collection accuracy that gener-
alised to other service users and times (Jerome,
Implementing Performance Kaplan, & Sturmey, 2014).
Management, Generalisation Even more complex service user behaviours,
and Maintenance such as prompted voiding to gain improvements
in continence, can be achieved and maintained
In order to achieve generalisation and mainte- but may require more elaborate staff training pro-
nance of staff behaviour change, performance cedures. Hawkins, Burgio, Langford and Engel
management procedures need to be developed (1993) successfully used periodic supervisory
and implemented. Without good performance monitoring and verbal and graphic feedback,
management, Embregts (2002) showed that biweekly letters of praise or disapproval and
although appropriate staff responses may be three monthly letters summarising performance
increased, the behaviour of the residents may not during this period, which were used in annual
improve exponentially. They included direct-care performance evaluations.
7  Staff Training 103

In order to assess methods to achieve generali- zures. As a welcome collateral, the tiered-peer
sation of staff skills, Ducharme and Feldman training procedures can be effective in ensuring
(1992) trained residential care staff in (a) the pro- maintenance of skills in the trainers as well as
vision of written instructions, (b) performance-­ showing high social validity (Pol, Reid, & Fuqua,
based training using a single client programme 1983). Such tiered training procedures are now
exemplar and simulated clients (single case train- widely used in settings that employ behaviour
ing), (c) performance-based training using devel- analysts (Ducharme, Williams, Cummings,
opmentally delayed clients as trainees (common Murray, & Spencer, 2001; Fleming, Oliver, &
stimuli training), and (d) performance-based Bolton, 1996).
training using multiple client programme exem- However, while there is much research using a
plars with simulated clients (general case train- combination of staff training procedures, often-
ing). They found that not all generalisation times it remains unclear exactly which of the com-
criteria were met until general case training was ponents are effective. A notable meta-­ analysis
provided, even when they controlled for potential established the active ingredients in staff training
sequence effects of the training procedure. (i.e. goals, format, and techniques) over a 20-year
Prompting and self-monitoring have been period. The 55 studies that were included reported
used with persons with disabilities to teach many 502 single-subject designs and 13 studies with
skills; however, these procedures also have been larger samples and provided evidence that the
successful in maintaining staff skills, for exam- combination of in-service training with coaching
ple, in the application of token economies on the job was most effective in terms of staff
(Cullen, 1988). Accuracy feedback adds to the skills development and retention. The findings
effectiveness of these methods (Petscher & also showed that multiple techniques can be used
Bailey, 2006). effectively but that verbal feedback, in the form of
Untargeted, collateral behaviour change is a praise and correction, was recommended. The
common occurrence when behaviour change identification of training goals, training format and
plans are implemented. The same is true for staff training techniques was key to successful staff
training. King, Lange and Errickson (1982) training programmes (van Oorsouw, Embregts,
tested the effect of public and individualised Bosman, & Jahoda, 2009).
feedback on staff behaviour in terms of giving While most staff training research evidences
verbal praise to hospital residents with intellec- the effectiveness of certain procedures in training
tual disabilities. Along with increases in targeted staff to carry out specific tasks or procedures, less
behaviours, positive changes occurred in verbal is known about teaching staff in more general
instructions and manual guidance instructions knowledge in applied behaviour analysis.
and resident on-task behaviour. Luiselli, Bass and Whitcomb (2010) looked at
three distinct content areas, including measure-
ment, behaviour support, and skill acquisition.
Conclusion They used basic group format didactic teaching
procedures using PowerPoint® presentations,
Training staff may not only improve staff and practice exercises, and video demonstrations.
service user well-being, it may also prove to be Knowledge tests before and after training showed
cost-­efficient for service providers (Test, Flowers, consistent improvement between pre- and post-­
Hewitt, & Solow, 2004). Most commonly, a training. However, Fennell and Dillenburger
tiered training approach is used, where experi- (2016) found that didactic group training deliv-
enced members of staff train less experienced ered by statutory training agencies (staff a­ ttending
staff. This has shown to be effective in achieving half-day or full-day workshops) was not effective
and maintaining basic as well as more complex in achieving knowledge in content areas such as
staff skills, such as safety-related skills and man- autism, behaviour analysis, functional assess-
aging aggressive behaviours and convulsive sei- ment, and challenging behaviours. They found
104 K. Dillenburger

that, post-training, self-perceived knowledge in Bearss, K., Johnson, C., Smith, T., Lecavalier, L., Swiezy,
N., Aman, M., … Scahill, L.. (2015). Effect of parent
these areas far exceeded actual knowledge. Clearly,
training vs parent education on behavioral problems in
a didactic lecture that lasts a few hours is not the children with autism spectrum disorder A randomized
same as properly supervised on-­the-­job staff train- clinical trial. Journal of American Medical Association,
ing (Dillenburger, Kerr, & Jordan, 2016). Trainers 313(15), 1524–1533. doi:10.1001/jama.2015.3150.
BPS. (2015). Code of ethics and conduct. British
and employers should heed these data, as overcon-
Psychological Society. Retrieved 27 Feb 2015, from
fident, poorly trained staff can fall pray to the http://www.bps.org.uk/
Dunning-Kruger effect and thus constitute a liabil- Brackett, L., Reid, D. H., & Green, C. W. (2007). Effects
ity to employers and service users (BPS, 2015). of reactivity to observations on staff performance.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40(1), 191–
In sum, most key staff in multidisciplinary
195. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.112-05.
teams receive little or no training in applied behav- Brown, K. M., Willis, B. S., & Reid, D. H. (1981).
iour analysis during their qualifying training Differential effects of supervisor verbal feed-
(Dillenburger et al., 2014b), and didactic post- back and feedback plus approval on institutional
staff performance. Journal of Organizational
qualifying staff training shows very mixed results.
Behavior Management, 3(1), 57–68. doi:10.1300/
However, when in-­ service staff training proce- J075v03n01_05.
dures are based on applied behaviour analytic Catania, C. N., Almeida, D., Liu-Constant, B., &
principles, which include clear target setting, DiGennaro Reed, F. D. (2009). Video modeling to
train staff to implement discrete-trial instruction.
in vivo modelling or video modelling, and consis-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(2), 387–
tent positive performance feedback, staff skills 392. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-387.
improvements can be generalised and maintained, Clarke, S., Taylor, G., Lancaster, J., & Remington, B.
even in quite complex areas of interventions. (2015). Acceptance and commitment therapy–based
self-management versus psychoeducation train-
Tiered staff training and performance manage-
ing for staff caring for clients with a personality
ment procedures can lead to improvements in staff disorder: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of
as well as trainer skills. Personality Disorders, 29(2), 163–176. doi:10.1521/
pedi_2014_28_149.
Courtemanche, A., Sheldon, J., Sherman, J., Schroeder,
S., Bell, A., & House, R. (2014). Assessing the effects
References of a staff training package on the treatment integrity
of an intervention for self-injurious behavior. Journal
Abernathy, W. (2011). Pay for profit: Designing an of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 26(4),
organization-wide performance-based compensa- 371–389. doi:10.1007/s10882-014-9372-6.
tion system. Atlanta, GA: Performance Management Cullen, C. (1988). A review of staff training: The emper-
Publications Inc. or’s old clothes. Irish Journal of Psychology, 9(2),
Arco, L. (2008). Feedback for improving staff training 309., 1988, Ireland. doi:10.1080/03033910.1988.105
and performance in behavioral treatment programs. 57723.
Behavioral Interventions. doi:10.1002/bin.247. Cumine, V., Dunlop, J., & Stevenson, G. (2009). Autism
Arco, L., & du Toit, E. (2006). Effects of adding on-the-­ in the early years: A practical guide. Abington, UK:
job feedback to conventional analog staff training in Routledge.
a nursing home. Behavior Modification, 30(5), 713– Delamater, A. M., Conners, C. K., Wells, K. C.,
735. doi:10.1177/0145445505281058. Delamater, A. M., Conners, C. K., & Wells, K. C.
Autism Speaks. (2014). State initiatives. Autism speaks. (1984). A comparison of staff training procedures.
Retrieved 21 Jul 2014, from http://www.autismspeaks. Behavioral applications in the child psychiatric
org/advocacy/states ­inpatient setting. Behavior Modification, 8(1), 39–58.
BACB. (2015). Behavior analyst certification board. doi:10.1177/01454455840081003.
BACB. Retrieved 3 May 2015, from www.bacb.com Denne, L. D., Thomas, E., Hastings, R. P., & Hughes,
Barnes, C. S., Dunning, J. L., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2011). J. C. (2015). Assessing competencies in applied
An evaluation of strategies for training staff to imple- behavior analysis for tutors working with children
ment the picture exchange communication system. with autism in a school-based setting. Research in
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(4), 1574– Autism Spectrum Disorders, 20, 67–77. doi:10.1016/j.
1583. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.03.003. rasd.2015.08.008.
BASW. (2016). Guidance for practitioners from social Diener, L. H., McGee, H. M., & Miguel, C. F. (2009).
care and health services in developing culturally com- An integrated approach for conducting a behav-
petent practice. British Association for Social Work. ioral systems analysis. Journal of Organizational
Retrieved 26 Mar 2016, from https://www.basw.co.uk/ Behavior Management, 29(2), 108–135.
resource/?id=1706 doi:10.1080/01608060902874534.
7  Staff Training 105

Dillenburger, K. (2011). The emperor’s new clothes: Organizational Behavior Management, 16, 3–25.
Eclecticism in autism treatment. Research in Autism doi:10.1300/J075v16n01_02.
Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1119–1128. doi:10.1016/j. Gardner, M. (1957). Fads and fallacies in the name of sci-
rasd.2010.12.008. ence. Mineola, NY: Dover Publications.
Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., Gallagher, S., & Green, J., Charman, T., McConachie, H., Aldred, C.,
McElhinney, M. (2004). Parent education and home-­ Slonims, V., Howlin, P., … Pickles, A.. (2010). Parent-­
based behaviour analytic intervention: An examina- mediated communication-focused treatment in chil-
tion of parents’ perceptions of outcome. Journal of dren with autism (PACT): A randomised controlled
Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 29(2), 119– trial. Lancet, 375(9732), 2152–2160. doi:10.1016/
130. doi:10.1080/13668250410001709476. S0140-6736(10)60587-9.
Dillenburger, K., Keenan, M., Doherty, A., Byrne, T., Hawkins, A. M., Burgio, L. D., Langford, A., & Engel,
& Gallagher, S. (2012). ABA-based programs for B. T. (1993). The effects of verbal and written super-
children diagnosed with autism spectrum disor- visory feedback on staff compliance with assigned
der: Parental and professional experiences at school prompted voiding in a nursing home. Journal of
and at home. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, Organizational Behavior Management, 13(1), 137–
34(November), 111–129. doi:10.1080/07317107.201 150. doi:10.1300/J075v13n01_09.
2.684645. Hay-Hansson, A. W., & Eldevik, S. (2013). Training
Dillenburger, K., McKerr, L., & Jordan, J.-A. (2014a). discrete trials teaching skills using videoconference.
Lost in translation: Public policies, evidence-based Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(11), 1300–
practice, and autism spectrum disorder. International 1309. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2013.07.022.
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G., &
61(2), 134–151. doi:10.1080/1034912X.2014.905059. Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behav-
Dillenburger, K., Röttgers, H.-R., Dounavi, K., Sparkman, ior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children
C., Keenan, M., Thyer, B., & Nikopoulos, C. (2014b). with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Multidisciplinary teamwork in autism: Can one size 26(4), 359–383. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.09.005.
fit all? The Australian Educational and Developmental Howard, J. S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman, C. R.,
Psychologist, 30, 1–16. doi:10.1017/edp.2014.13. & Cohen, H. G. (2014). Comparison of behavior
Dillenburger, K., McKerr, L., & Jordan, J.-A. (2016). analytic and eclectic early interventions for young
Staff training in autism: The one-eyed man…. children with autism after three years. Research in
International Journal of Environmental Research and Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 3326–3344.
Public Health, under review. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.021.
Ducharme, J. M., & Feldman, M. A. (1992). Comparison Huskens, B., & Verburg, M. (2011). An applied behavior
of staff training strategies to promote generalized analysis training package in a child psychiatric clinic:
teaching skills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, A chance to empower staff and parents? European
25(1), 165–179. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-165. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Retrieved from
Ducharme, J. M., Williams, L., Cummings, A., Murray, P., & http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi?T=JS&PAGE=re
Spencer, T. (2001). General case quasi-pyramidal staff ference&D=emed10&NEWS=N&AN=70489227
training to promote generalization of teaching skills in Huskens, B., Reijers, H., & Didden, R. (2012). Staff train-
supervisory and direct-­care staff. Behavior Modification, ing effective in increasing learning opportunities for
25(2), 233–254. doi:10.1177/0145445501252004. school-aged children with autism spectrum disorders.
Durgin, A., Mahoney, A., Cox, C., Weetjens, B. J., & Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 15(6), 435–447.
Poling, A. (2014). Using task clarification and feed- doi:10.3109/17518423.2012.705910.
back training to improve staff performance in an East Jahr, E. (1998). Current issues in staff training. Research
African nongovernmental organization. Journal of in Developmental Disabilities, 19(1), 73–87.
Organizational Behavior Management, 34(2), 122– doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(97)00030-9.
143. doi:10.1080/01608061.2014.914007. Jerome, J., Kaplan, H., & Sturmey, P. (2014). The effects
Embregts, P. J. C. M. (2002). Effect of resident and direct-­ of in-service training alone and in-service training
care staff training on responding during social interac- with feedback on data collection accuracy for direct-­
tions. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23(5), care staff working with individuals with intellectual
353–366. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(02)00134-8. disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Fennell, B., & Dillenburger, K. (2016). Applied behaviour 35(2), 529–536. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.11.009.
analysis: What do teachers of students with autism Kazemi, E., Shapiro, M., & Kavner, A. (2015). Predictors
spectrum disorder know. International Journal of of intention to turnover in behavior technicians work-
Educational Research, under review. ing with individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
Fleming, R., & Sulzer-Azaroff, B. (1992). Reciprocal peer Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 17, 106–115.
management: Improving staff instruction in a vocational doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2015.06.012.
training program. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, King, R. P., Lange, M. J., & Errickson, E. A. (1982).
25(3), 611–620. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-611. Public posting and individualized feedback: An
Fleming, R. K., Oliver, J. R., & Bolton, D. M. (1996). effective procedure to improve direct care staff per-
Training supervisors to train staff. Journal of formances. Behavioral Engineering, 8(1), 25–33.
106 K. Dillenburger

Retrieved from http://ovidsp.ovid.com/ovidweb.cgi Nosik, M. R., Williams, W. L., Garrido, N., & Lee, S.
?T=JS&PAGE=reference&D=psyc2&NEWS=N (2013). Comparison of computer based instruction
&AN=1983-21927-001. to behavior skills training for teaching staff imple-
Lavie, T., & Sturmey, P. (2002). Training staff to conduct mentation of discrete-trial instruction with an adult
a paired-stimulus preference assessment. Journal with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35(2), 209–211. 34(1), 461–468. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.011.
doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-209. van Oorsouw, W. M. W. J., Embregts, P. J. C. M., Bosman,
Leblanc, M., Ricciardi, J. N., & Luiselli, J. K. (2005). A. M. T., & Jahoda, A. (2009). Training staff serving
Improving discrete trial instruction by paraprofes- clients with intellectual disabilities: A meta-­analysis
sional staff through an abbreviated performance of aspects determining effectiveness. Research
feedback intervention. Education and Treatment of in Developmental Disabilities, 30(3), 503–511.
Children, 28(1), 76–82. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.07.011.
Ling, C. Y. M., & Mak, W. W. S. (2012). Coping with Page, T. J., Iwata, B. A., & Reid, D. H. (1982). Pyramidal
challenging behaviours of children with autism: training: A large-scale application with institutional
Effectiveness of brief training workshop for front- staff. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15(3),
line staff in special education settings. Journal of 335–351. doi:10.1901/jaba.1982.15-335.
Intellectual Disability Research, 56(3), 258–269. Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Korzilius, H. (2010).
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2011.01469.x. Effectiveness of behavioral skills training on staff per-
Luiselli, J. K., Bass, J. D., & Whitcomb, S. A. (2010). formance in a job training setting for high-functioning
Teaching applied behavior analysis knowledge com- adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Research
petencies to direct-care service providers: Outcome in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4(4), 731–740.
assessment and social validation of a training pro- doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.01.012.
gram. Behavior Modification, 34(5), 403–414. Parsons, M. B., Reid, D. H., & Green, C. W. (1996).
doi:10.1177/0145445510383526. Training basic teaching skills to community and insti-
Macurik, K. M., O’Kane, N. P., Malanga, P., & Reid, tutional support staff for people with severe disabili-
D. H. (2008). Video training of support staff in inter- ties: A one-day program. Research in Developmental
vention plans for challenging behavior: Comparison Disabilities, 17(6), 467–485. doi:10.1016/
with live training. Behavioral Interventions, 23(3), S0891-4222(96)00031-5.
143–163. doi:10.1002/bin.261. Parsons, M. B., Rollyson, J. H., & Reid, D. H. (2012).
Malott, R. W., Vunovich, P. L., Boettcher, W., & Groeger, Evidence-based staff training: A guide for practitio-
C. (1995). Saving the world by teaching behavior anal- ners. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 5(2), 2–11.
ysis: A behavioral systems approach. The Behavior PBS Coalition UK (2015). Positive Behavioural Support: A
Analyst/MABA, 18(2), 341–354. Retrieved from http:// competence framework. Positive Behavioural Support
www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid= (PBS) Coalition UK. Retrieved 26 Mar 2016, from
2733719&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract. http://www.skillsforcare.org.uk/Document-library/
McDonnell, A., Sturmey, P., Oliver, C., Cunningham, J., Skills/People-whose-behaviour-challenges/Positive-­
Hayes, S., Galvin, M., … Cunningham, C. (2008). Behavioural-­Support-Competence-Framework.pdf
The effects of staff training on staff confidence and Perry, A., & Condillac, R. (2003). Evidence-based
challenging behavior in services for people with practices for children and adolescents with autism
autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum disorders: Review of the literature and prac-
Spectrum Disorders, 2(2), 311–319. doi:10.1016/j. tice guide. Toronto, ON: Children’s Mental Health.
rasd.2007.08.001. Petscher, E. S., & Bailey, J. S. (2006). Effects of train-
Milton, D. E. M. (2012). The normalisation agenda and ing, prompting, and self-monitoring on staff behavior
the psycho-emotional disablement of autistic people. in a classroom for students with disabilities. Journal
Autonomy, the Critical Journal of Interdisciplinary of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39(2), 215–226.
Autism Studies, 1(1), 1. doi:10.1901/jaba.2006.02-05.
NcCahill, J., Healy, O., & Ramey, S. (2014). Training van den Pol, R. A., Reid, D. H., & Fuqua, R. W. (1983).
educational staff in functional behavioral assessment: Peer training of safety-related skills to institutional
A systematic review. Journal of Developmental and staff: Benefits for trainers and trainees. Journal
Physical Disabilities, 26(4), 479–505. Retrieved from of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16(2), 139–156.
http://www.tara.tcd.ie/handle/2262/73401. doi:10.1901/jaba.1983.16-139.
Nepo, K. G. (2010). The use of technology to improve Reid, D. H., & Parsons, M. B. (1995). Comparing choice
staff performance. International Journal of Behavioral and questionnaire measures of the acceptability of a
Consultation & Therapy, 6(2), 134–141. doi:10.1037/ staff training procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior
h0100901. Analysis, 28(1), 95–96. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-95.
Nigro-Bruzzi, D., & Sturmey, P. (2010). The effects of Reid, D.H., O’Kane, N.P., Macurik, K.M. (2011). Staff
behavioral skills training on mand training by staff training and management. Handbook of Applied
and unprompted vocal mands by children. Journal Behavior Analysis, 281–294.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(4), 757–761. Roscoe, E. M., & Fisher, W. W. (2008). Evaluation of
doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-757. an efficient method for training staff to implement
7  Staff Training 107

stimulus preference assessments. Journal of Applied Stanton, M. (2000). Learning to live with high functioning
Behavior Analysis, 41(2), 249–254. doi:10.1901/ autism: A parent’s guide for professionals. London,
jaba.2008.41-249. UK: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Roscoe, E. M., Fisher, W. W., Glover, A. C., & Volkert, Strouse, M. C., Carroll-Hernandez, T. A., Sherman, J. A.,
V. M. (2006). Evaluating the relative effects of feed- & Sheldon, J. B. (2004). Turning over turnover: The
back and contingent money for staff training of stimulus evaluation of a staff scheduling system in a community-
preference assessments. Journal of Applied Behavior based program for adults with developmental disabili-
Analysis, 39(1), 63–77. doi:10.1901/jaba.2006.7-05. ties. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management,
Sarokoff, R., & Sturmey, P. (2004). The effects of behav- 23(2–3), 45–63. doi:10.1300/J075v23n02.
ioral skills training on staff implementation of discrete-­ Sundberg, D. (2016). Why people quit your company and
trial teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, how behavior analysis can slow the revolving door at
37(4), 535–538. doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-535. aba service providers. Behavioral Science in the 21
Sarokoff, R. A., & Sturmey, P. (2008). The effects of Century. Retrieved 17 Apr 2016, from bSc21.org
instructions, rehearsal, modeling, and feedback on Tavis, C. (2003). The widening scientist-practitioner
acquisition and generalization of staff use of discrete gap: A view from the bridge. In S. O. Lilienfeld, S. J.
trial teaching and student correct responses. Research Lynn, & J. Lohr (Eds.), Science and pseudoscience in
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(1), 125–136. clinical psychology (pp. ix–xviii). New York, NY: The
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.04.002. Guilford Press.
Schepis, M. M., Reid, D. H., Ownbey, J., & Parsons, Taylor, D. (2007). Death by PowerPoint. Developmental
M. B. (2001). Training support staff to embed teach- Medicine and Child Neurology, 49(5), 395.
ing within natural routines of young children with dis- Test, D. W., Flowers, C., Hewitt,A., & Solow, J. (2004).Training
abilities in an inclusive preschool. Journal of Applied needs of direct support staff. Mental Retardation, 42(5),
Behavior Analysis, 34(3), 313–327. doi:10.1901/ 327–337. doi:10.1352/0047-6765(2004)42<327:TNOD
jaba.2001.34-313. SS>2.0.CO;2.
Schmidt, J. D., Urban, K. D., Luiselli, J. K., White, C., Unumb, L.. (2014). BCBS must provide ABA coverage
& Harrington, C. (2013). Improving appearance, orga- for autism. Letter to the Editor. Newsobserver.com.
nization, and safety of special education classrooms: Retrieved Jul 17 2014, from http://www.newsob-
Effects of staff training in a human services setting. server.com/2014/07/17/4012585/lorri-unumb-bcbs-­
Education and Treatment of Children, 36(2), 1–13. must-provide.html
doi:10.1353/etc.2013.0012. van Vonderen, A., de Swart, C., & Didden, R. (2010).
Sellers, T. P., Valentino, A. L., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2016). Effectiveness of instruction and video feedback
Recommended practices for individual supervision on staff’s use of prompts and children’s adaptive
of aspiring behavior analysts. Behavior Analysis in responses during one-to-one training in children with
Practice. doi:10.1007/s40617-016-0110-7. severe to profound intellectual disability. Research
Singh, N. N., Lancioni, G. E., Karazsia, B. T., Myers, in Developmental Disabilities, 31(3), 829–838.
R. E., Winton, A. S. W., Latham, L. L., & Nugent, K. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.02.008.
(2015). Effects of training staff in MBPBS on the use Williams, W. L., & Gallinat, J. (2011). The effects of
of physical restraints, staff stress and turnover, staff evaluating video examples of staffs’ own versus oth-
and peer injuries, and cost effectiveness in devel- ers’ performance on discrete-trial training skills in a
opmental disabilities. Mindfulness, 6(4), 926–937. human service setting. Journal of Organizational
doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0369-0. Behavior Management, 31(2), 97–116. doi:10.1080/0
Spector, A., Orrell, M., & Goyder, J. (2013). A system- 1608061.2011.570099.
atic review of staff training interventions to reduce Wilson, P. G., Reid, D. H., & Korabek-Pinkowski, C. A.
the behavioural and psychological symptoms of (1991). Analysis of public verbal feedback as a staff
dementia. Ageing Research Reviews. doi:10.1016/j. management procedure. Behavioral Interventions,
arr.2012.06.005. 6(4), 263–277. doi:10.1002/bin.2360060404.
Parent Training for Parents
of Individuals Diagnosed 8
with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Justin B. Leaf, Joseph H. Cihon, Sara M. Weinkauf,


Misty L. Oppenheim-Leaf, Mitchell Taubman,
and Ronald Leaf

Stewart, & Scahill, 2015) with a number of cor-


I ntroduction to Parent Training responding labels. “Parent support” often consists
and ASD of several parents gathering together to discuss
ideas, stories, experiences, and information about
Per the Center for Disease Control and Prevention ASD and intervention, usually with the facilita-
(2012), it is now reported that 1 out of every 68 tion of professionals (Bearss et al., 2015a).
children living in the United States are diagnosed “Parent education” is a form of parent training in
with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The high which a professional provides didactic or manual
prevalence has also been reported globally instruction on concepts related to ASD and/or
(Christensen, Baio, & Braun, 2012). For individ- intervention (Bearss et al., 2015a). Parent training
uals diagnosed with ASD to make meaningful can also consist of counseling sessions, during
progress, they require early, intensive, and com- which a therapist works with parents on issues
prehensive intervention (Smith, Groen, & Wynn, related to stress, family functioning, and daily liv-
2000), with general consensus that interventions ing. Another approach to parent training includes
should be based upon the principles of applied parent-mediated intervention, which consists of
behavior analysis (Smith & Iadarola, 2015). One hands-on training during which parents are taught
recommended and empirically validated compo- specific techniques that can be used to develop
nent of comprehensive intervention is parent and improve their own child’s skills (Kasari,
training (National Autism Center, 2009, 2015). Gulsrud, Paparella, Hellemann, & Berry, 2015).
There are many different varieties of parent The goal of parent-mediated intervention is “…
training within the literature (Bearss, Burrell, that parents may become co-­facilitators in the
intervention process” (Radley, Jeson, Clark, &
O’Neill, 2014, p. 241). It is common for the afore-
J.B. Leaf, PhD, BCBA-D (*) • J.H. Cihon mentioned varieties of parent training to occur in
M. Taubman • R. Leaf isolation or as a combination with other formats.
Autism Partnership Foundation,
Although the term “parent training” can represent
200 Marina Drive, Seal Beach, CA 90808, USA
e-mail: Jblautpar@aol.com multiple forms, the primary focus of this chapter
will be parent-­mediated interventions, parent sup-
S.M. Weinkauf
JBA Institute, Torrance, CA, USA port groups, and parent education.
The purpose of this chapter is to (1) discuss the
M.L. Oppenheim-Leaf
Behavior Therapy and Learning Center, importance of parent training, (2) provide a histori-
Calgary, Canada cal perspective of parent training within the field of

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 109


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_8
110 J.B. Leaf et al.

applied behavior analysis (ABA) as it relates to 2009, 2015). Therefore, given the many benefits
autism intervention, (3) provide a general overview parent training can have for the family and the
of the research on parent training, and (4) discuss individual diagnosed with ASD, training for par-
future research and clinical implications. ents and the family as a whole should be included
as part of a comprehensive intervention program.

Benefits of Parent Training


 eminal and Early Research
S
There are many reasons why parent training on Parent Training
should be included as part of a comprehensive
intervention model. First, given the high preva- In one of the first empirical investigations of
lence of individuals receiving an ASD diagnosis, behavioral intervention for individuals diagnosed
it may often be difficult for families to access the with ASD, Wolf, Risley, and Mees (1963) imple-
intensity of intervention required for best out- mented operant conditioning procedures (e.g.,
comes (Symon, 2005). Research has demon- extinction and shaping) to decrease the frequency
strated that training parents to implement some of tantrums, improve bedtime behavior, and
or all of the intervention can help optimize the increase the duration of wearing glasses for a
intensity of intervention (Wainer & Ingersoll, 3.5-year-old boy named Dickey. The results of this
2013a). Second, research has demonstrated that study demonstrated that operant conditioning pro-
parent training can improve the quality of parent-­ cedures were responsible for improved behavior
child interactions (Koegel, Bimbela, & and provided the first empirical demonstration of
Schreibman, 1996), as well as improve upon ABA techniques as a treatment for an individual
desirable behaviors (e.g., language, imitation, diagnosed with ASD. One component of this study
and social behavior) and ameliorate less desir- was training for the mother and father on provid-
able behaviors (e.g., tantrums, self-injury, and ing intervention for the target goal areas while at
stereotypy; Charlop & Trasowech, 1991; Moes & home. Although the specific details of the parent
Frea, 2002). Third, when parents are trained to training were not described, the inclusion of par-
provide intervention, treatment effects can gener- ents within the study lends credence to the impor-
alize and maintain over time (e.g., Koegel, tance of parent training throughout the course of
Schreibman, Britten, Burke, O’Neill, 1982). intervention for individuals diagnosed with ASD.
Parent training may result in better generalization In 1973, Lovaas et al. were the first to evaluate
and maintenance as individuals diagnosed with a comprehensive behavioral intervention program
ASD can receive more hours of intervention, for individuals diagnosed with ASD. The study
through parent-mediated intervention, and in set- consisted of 20 participants between 3 and
tings which may lead to longer-lasting changes. 10 years old. All participants received interven-
Fourth, research has demonstrated that parent tion for 12–14 months in an inpatient setting. The
training can reduce the stress and depression intervention consisted of the implementation of
often reported by parents of children with a dis- behavior analytic principles (e.g., reinforcement,
ability (e.g., Estes et al., 2009). Finally, research shaping, and punishment) to improve desired
has shown that parents who are trained to provide behaviors (e.g., appropriate speech, play, and
intervention often demonstrate an increased opti- social nonverbal behavior) and to eliminate unde-
mism about their child’s future, as well as more sired behaviors (i.e., self-stimulation and echola-
positive feelings about influencing their chil- lia). Some of the participants’ parents were trained
dren’s development (e.g., Koegel et al., 1982). (group 2), while others did not receive training
As this chapter will show, and other profes- (group 1). The researchers used s­tandardized
sionals and organizations have documented, par- measures (i.e., Stanford Binet IQ Test and
ent training meets the criteria to be considered an Vineland Social Maturity Scores) and various
evidence-based practice (National Autism Center, response measures to evaluate the effectiveness of
8  Parent Training 111

the intervention. Overall, the results indicated of methods for a variety of skills for their chil-
meaningful improvements for the participants. dren diagnosed with ASD.
With respect to the effects of parent training, the
authors stated “…follow-up measures recorded 1
to 4 years after treatment indicated that large dif- Parent Demographics
ferences between groups of children were related
to the post-treatment environment (those groups The plethora of research on parent training has
whose parents were trained to carry out behavior resulted in many parent participants with varying
therapy continued to improve; while children who demographics. Researcher typically provides
were institutionalized regressed)” (Lovaas, varying degrees of information on these demo-
Koegel, Simmons, & Long, 1973, p. 156). Thus, graphics when discussing their participants. Age is
the results suggested the importance of including one demographic frequently noted. Within the par-
parent training as part of a comprehensive treat- ent training literature, there is a wide age range of
ment model to ensure maintenance of skills the parent participants, with the youngest parent
acquired throughout the course of treatment. noted at 21 years old (Anan, Warner, McGillivary,
The Lovaas et al. (1973) study was a catalyst Chong, & Hines, 2008) and the oldest at 52 years
for other seminal research in the behavioral treat- old (Poslawsky et al., 2015). Gender is another
ment of ASD (e.g., Lovaas, 1987). Lovaas (1987) commonly noted demographic within the litera-
evaluated the effects of intensive, comprehensive ture. The majority of studies on parent training
behavioral treatment compared to a non-­intensive, have reported training only mothers (Koegel,
eclectic approach. Thirty-eight children were Glahn, & Nieminen, 1978; Park, Alver-Morgran,
quasi-randomly assigned into 2 groups, 19 in the Canella-Malone, 2011; Reagon & Higbee, 2009);
intensive group and 19 children in the control however, there have been a few studies in which
group. Within the study, Lovaas (1987) stated, both mothers and fathers are included (e.g., Estes
“The parents worked as part of the treatment team et al., 2014; Rocha, Schreibman, & Stahmer, 2007;
throughout the intervention; they were extensively Vismara et al., 2013). Some less commonly
trained in the treatment procedures so that treat- reported, and often not reported, demographics
ment could take place for almost all of the sub- within the parent training literature are socioeco-
jects’ waking hours, 365 days a year.” (p. 5). Those nomic status (SES), education, nationality, and
involved in the study have stated that parents culture.
became experts in ABA and ASD and in some
cases were the best behavior analyst(s) on the
child’s team (Leaf, McEachin, & Taubman, 2008). Training Methods
The results of the study not only showed the need
for intensive and comprehensive intervention but Several methods have been utilized for training
also illustrated the benefits of including parent parents. Some common methods include, but are
training in an intensive, comprehensive model. not limited to, (1) demonstration and role-play
These seminal studies and other early investi- (e.g., behavioral skills training or the teaching
gations on ABA-based treatment for individuals interaction procedure; Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013a;
diagnosed with ASD, as well as the work of other Rocha et al. 2007), (2) video modeling (e.g.,
professionals/researchers evaluating the effects Harris et al. 1981), (3) didactic instruction (e.g.,
of parent training for parents of children with or Farmer & Reupert, 2013), and (4) active coaching
without ASD (e.g., Baker, Heifetz, & Murphy, (Kasari et al., 2015). What follows is a general
1980; Forehand, Middlebrook, Rogers, & Steffe, overview of these methods with illustrated exam-
1983; Harris, Wolchik, & Weitz, 1981; Patterson ples of each. However, each of these techniques
& Fleischman, 1979), have served as a spring- has additional benefits and limitations, and clini-
board for a plethora of research studies evaluat- cians should examine the literature on each when
ing the effects of parent training, using a variety selecting a method for parent training.
112 J.B. Leaf et al.

Demonstration and Role-Play Within this study, the researchers demonstrated


the effectiveness of BST in a group and a one-to-­
One common method used to train parents occurs one instructional format to teach parents how to
when the trainer demonstrates the targeted behav- implement components of Project Impact. Project
ior and the parent participates in role-plays. Impact is a teaching procedure that uses a combi-
Demonstration and role-play commonly take two nation of naturalistic behavioral intervention
different forms within the literature: (1) behav- with a developmental approach to teach students
ioral skills training (BST) that involves the trainer various social behaviors and to improve language
describing and demonstrating the skill, providing development. In this study, parents attended six
opportunities for the learner to practice the skill, group training sessions and six individual train-
and the trainer providing feedback (e.g., ing sessions. The researchers evaluated improve-
Seiverling, Williams, Sturmey, & Hart, 2012) and ment via formal standardized assessments for the
(2) the teaching interaction procedure (TIP) in children (e.g., Social Responsiveness Scale) as
which the teacher describes the skill, discusses well as treatment fidelity evaluations. The results
rationales for why the skill is important, demon- showed that parents improved their delivery of
strates the skill, role-plays the skill, and provides the intervention components and that the children
feedback (e.g., Rocha et al., 2007). showed improvements on the targeted skills.
There are numerous benefits for the use of TIPs have also been demonstrated as a suc-
demonstration and role-playing during the course cessful method to train parents in the implementa-
of training. For one, demonstrations provide an tion of various behavior analytic techniques. For
opportunity for the trainer to model examples and example, Rocha et al. (2007) implemented a TIP
non-examples of the targeted skill. As such, mod- to teach three parents how to implement Pivotal
eling sets the occasion for observational learning Response Training (PRT) and Discrete Trial
of the targeted skill(s). Second, role-plays can lead Teaching (DTT) to increase joint attention for his
to increased opportunities to provide positive rein- or her child. Parents were taught various proce-
forcement for approximations in a non-­threatening, dures associated with DTT (e.g., providing an
structured environment, therefore potentially appropriate instruction, providing feedback, and
decreasing stress and increasing the parent’s confi- completing the trial) and PRT (e.g., using choice,
dence to display the skill in the criterion context. motivation, and following his/her child’s lead).
This method also allows the trainer to train loosely Training consisted of the researcher providing
and program common stimuli and for training to information about the procedures and rationales,
align closely with the natural contingencies, all of (e.g., why joint attention is important) followed
which are important in promoting generalization by a teacher modeling the behavior, the parent
across environments (Stokes & Baer, 1977). implementing the procedure, and receiving feed-
Finally, the inclusion of rationales (a component back on their implementation. Results of the study
of TIPs) may lead to better understanding of the showed that parents increased the amount of joint
importance of the skill and may result in longer attention bids provided, and children demon-
maintenance of the skill in the absence of the strated improvement in joint attention.
trainer and in the natural environment. That is,
rationales can put the skill into context for the
trainee (e.g., it is important to have the environ- Video Modeling
ment appropriately arranged before working on a
skill, such as requesting, to allow for many pre- Another common training method explored within
pared learning opportunities to be captured effi- the parent training literature is video modeling
ciently) which may lead to more generalized skills (e.g., Berquist & Charlop, 2014). Video modeling
that maintain for longer periods of time. has many benefits as a training tool. For one, simi-
Ingersoll and Wainer (2013b) provide an lar to role-playing, video modeling provides
example of the use of BST during parent training. examples and non-examples of the targeted skill.
8  Parent Training 113

Also, when video models contain multiple exem- with other procedures within the parent training
plars, they increase the likelihood of generaliza- literature (e.g., Farmer & Reupert, 2013).
tion of the skill (Stokes & Baer, 1977). Unlike Didactic instruction offers several benefits for
role-plays, video modeling provides parents with a parents and trainers. It can provide parents foun-
permanent product that they can reference in the dational information which may lead to a better
absence of the trainer. Video modeling is com- understanding of the importance of the various
monly included as a component of other training procedures that they are taught. When didactic
methods. instruction is provided in a group instructional
Harris et al. (1981) taught 11 parents how to format, it provides parents the opportunity to
implement a variety of behavior analytic tech- learn from each other, develop support networks,
niques (e.g., shaping, data collection, chaining, and solve problem with other parents. With
and prompting) focusing on the language devel- respect to the trainer, didactic instruction allows
opment of their children. The intervention was for training large numbers of parents, which may
conducted within a group instructional format result in more efficient training.
and consisted of BST with the addition of video Farmer and Reupert (2013) provide an exam-
models. Although the authors of the study stated ple of a study that used didactic instruction as part
that videos were included, no description was of a parent training intervention. The researchers
provided as to what was specifically shown on conducted a 6-week parent education program for
the videos. At the conclusion of the study, 86 parents living in rural Australia. The program
improvements were observed in the children’s was implemented in a group instructional format
language skills. with each group lasting 6 h. Each week’s session
More recently, Berquist and Charlop (2014) covered a new topic (e.g., what is autism, social
taught six parents how to evaluate an intervention understanding, and sensory processing). At the
that consisted of multiple components, including conclusion of the 6 weeks, parents self-reported
video modeling. Training consisted of a combi- (i.e., parents filled out a Likert scale across 15 dif-
nation of a manual and training sessions using ferent questions) an increase in knowledge of the
BST. A video was used in conjunction with the various topics.
manual and contained a variety of information
for evaluating interventions (e.g., operationally
defining targeted behavior, how data collection Active Coaching
can be determined to be effective, and identifying
the claim of the intervention). Parents were Another form of parent training is known as
taught how to evaluate a treatment across 14 dif- active coaching. Active coaching consists of the
ferent dimensions (e.g., graphed results, identify- trainer providing in vivo feedback, while the
ing question of interest, and identifying target trainee attempts to demonstrate the targeted skill.
behavior). The results of a multiple baseline Typically, active coaching is implemented simul-
design showed an increase in the parents display- taneously with other procedures, such as didactic
ing the dimensions of evaluative behaviors. instruction (e.g., Kasari et al., 2015) and/or mod-
eling (e.g., Radley et al., 2014).
Active coaching has many benefits as a
Didactic Instruction method to train parents. For instance, active
coaching sets the occasion for trainers to provide
Didactic instruction, as applied to parent training, immediate feedback. Immediate feedback may
provides parents with information on how to be more desired than delayed feedback when tar-
implement various procedures and increase their geting new skills (Cooper, Heron, & Heward,
general understanding of those procedures. 2007) to prevent incorrect implementation of the
Although didactic instruction can be imple- intervention for an extended period of time. Also,
mented in isolation, it is commonly implemented active coaching is conducive to training in the
114 J.B. Leaf et al.

environment in which the skill is to occur as Instructional Formats


opposed to an analogue setting. Targeting a skill
in the environment in which it is to be used The aforementioned training methods are com-
increases the likelihood of the behavior coming monly implemented in three different instruc-
under control of the naturally occuring stimulus tional formats. The first, and most common,
conditions (Stokes & Baer, 1977). instructional format within the literature is a one-­
In an example of active coaching, Kasari, to-­one instructional format. One-to-one instruc-
Gulsrud, Paparella, Hellemann, and Berry (2015) tional formats provide the opportunity for the
compared the JASPER parent-mediated model to trainer to work directly with the parents on an
a psychoeducational intervention (PEI) for 86 par- individual basis. Researchers have demonstrated
ents. The parents were randomly assigned to the the effectiveness of a one-to-one format for train-
PEI or JASPER condition. The JASPER model ing parents using a variety of training methods,
consisted of 10 h of active coaching targeting joint including video modeling (e.g., Berquist &
engagement through a combination of develop- Charlop, 2014) and demonstration and role-play
mental and behavioral procedures. Parents were (e.g., Rocha et al., 2007).
taught to recognize their child’s developmental A second instructional format in which vari-
level of play, how to jointly engage in an activity, ous training techniques can be implemented is
and how to keep their child engaged. The PEI group instruction. Group instruction consists of
model consisted of 10 h of didactic instruction two or more parents participating in the interven-
during which parents were taught about autism, tion simultaneously. Group instruction sets the
improving social behavior, and managing parental occasion for observational learning which may
stress. The primary measure was joint engagement result in more efficient training targeted (e.g.,
between the parent and child. Additional measures Leaf et al., 2013) as parents can acquire skills not
included child play skills, standardized assess- directly. Group instruction has been used within
ments of the child’s skill level (e.g., Reynell recep- the literature with video modeling (e.g., Harris
tive language test), and measures of parental stress et al., 1981), demonstration and role-play (e.g.,
(e.g., Parental Stress Index). The results of the Laugeson, Frankel, Mogil, & Dillon, 2009), and
study indicated that parents assigned to the didactic instruction (e.g., Farmer & Reupert,
JASPER model showed higher levels of joint 2013). The PEERS model of social skills groups
engagement, but there were mixed results on the (for a detailed description of the PEERS Model
other child-­specific measures. Although in regard see, Laugeson et al., 2009; Yoo et al., 2014) is a
to stress measures, the parents in the PEI condition prime example of parent training that occurs in a
showed lower levels post-intervention when com- group instructional format. For example,
pared to parents in the JASPER condition. Laugeson et al. (2009) utilized BST within a
Although there are many benefits to active group instructional format to teach 33 parents to
coaching, there are some disadvantages found improve their child’s friendships with peers.
within the literature. First of all, in many studies, After 12 sessions of intervention, parents more
the procedures associated with active coaching effectively facilitated relationships using the pro-
are not thoroughly described which may make it cedures taught.
difficult to replicate. Second, active coaching Group instruction and one-to-one instruction
may be labor intensive as it requires one-on-one can also occur in combination (e.g., Anan et al.,
intervention with the parent and child and, there- 2008). For example, Harris, Wolchik, and Milch
fore, less efficient than other methods of parent (1983) conducted and evaluated the effects of
training. Third, since it is usually combined with training 11 parents of children diagnosed with
other training procedures, it is often difficult to ASD. The authors targeted a variety of skills
determine if active coaching itself or another (e.g., data collection, shaping, promoting gener-
component of the training package is responsible alization) using BST. The researchers conducted
for the behavior change. training in a group instructional format and
8  Parent Training 115

c­onducted home visits to provide one-to-one they had a better understanding and appreciation
training. The researchers measured the parents’ of how to help their child.
speech-oriented language toward their respective Although there are advantages to telehealth,
child and found an improvement following there are some disadvantages as well. For one,
intervention. the trainer can only observe what is occurring on
An increasingly common instructional format the screen, which makes it difficult to assess what
for parent training is telehealth (e.g., Suess et al., other events may be influencing the parent’s
2014; Vismara et al., 2013), which involves the behavior. Second, telehealth does not allow the
use of telecommunication technologies (e.g., trainer to model the correct behavior/procedure
video conferencing) to provide training to par- directly with the individual diagnosed with
ents remotely. This format is often used due to ASD. Finally, telehealth has to be implemented
large geographical distances between the family with extreme caution to protect the client’s rights
and the trainer (Vismara et al., 2013). Telehealth and to avoid HIPPA violations.
has advantages over more traditional instruc-
tional formats (i.e., in person). For instance, tele-
health can be used to provide training for parents Parent Targets
who otherwise would not be able to access train-
ing due to distance or limited services. Within the literature on parent training, parents
Additionally, depending on the nature of the have been trained to implement a variety of teach-
training, telehealth can be accessed at the par- ing procedures. Some of these procedures have
ents’ leisure, minimizing scheduling conflicts. included, but are not limited to, DTT (e.g., Neef,
Telehealth is also amenable to training occurring 1995), PRT (e.g., Buckley, Ente, & Ruef, 2014),
in multiple environments, which can be individu- ESDM (e.g., Vismara, Colombi, & Rogers, 2009),
alized and selected based on parent responding. the Picture Exchange Communication System
For example, a more structured environment can (PECS; e.g., Park, Alber-Morgan, & Cannella-
be selected when necessary and systematically Malone, 2011), and Functional Communication
transferred to the natural environment. Training (FCT; e.g., Suess et al., 2014).
In an example of the use of parent training via
telehealth, Vismara et al. (2013) trained eight Discrete Trial Teaching  DTT is a commonly
parents in the principles of the Early Start Denver implemented procedure during the course of
Model (ESDM; for detailed description of treatment for many individuals diagnosed with
ESDM, see Estes et al., 2014; Vismara et al., ASD. DTT consists of three primary compo-
2009; Vismara, McCormick, Young, Nadhan, & nents: the teacher delivering an instruction, a
Monlux, 2013). The intervention occurred across response made from the learner, and a teacher-­
12 sessions, each lasting 1.5 h, within a one-to-­ delivered consequence (Lovaas, 1987). Lovaas
one instructional format. The sessions consisted et al. (1973) and Lovaas (1987) included parent
of the parent discussing the child behaviors that training on the implementation of DTT. Since
had occurred in the last week, followed by a these publications, there have been numerous
10 min observation of the child and parent inter- studies which have also involved the training of
acting, and then discussing the skill topics from parents on the implementation of DTT (e.g.,
previous sessions, new skill topics, and how to Crockett, Fleming, Doepke, & Stevens, 2007;
implement these in generalized environments. Koegel et al., 1978; Lafaskis & Sturmey, 2007;
The main dependent variables for the parents Rocha et al., 2007; Schreibman, Kaneko, &
were parent-child interaction, parent satisfaction, Koegel, 1991).
and fidelity of treatment. After treatment had For example, Neef (1995) investigated the use
concluded, the parents implemented the proce- of a pyramidal training approach (i.e., trainees
dures with higher levels of treatment fidelity and becoming trainers) compared to professional-led
higher levels of engagement and reported that training with 26 parents (20 mothers, 6 fathers).
116 J.B. Leaf et al.

Training involved how to select and arrange Early Start Denver Model  ESDM is a compre-
stimuli, provide instructions and prompts, deliver hensive treatment approach for children under
contingent consequences, record data, and struc- 4 years of age (Estes et al., 2014; Vismara et al.,
ture the teaching session (all of which are compo- 2009, 2013). ESDM incorporates a developmen-
nents of DTT). The pyramidal approach involved tal and naturalistic behavioral approach and
training five parents, referred to as Tier 1 parents, includes parent involvement as a core concept
until mastery. Those parents then conducted the within the treatment process.
training for additional parents, referred to as Tier In an example of training parents in the
2 parents, and were matched based on demo- ESDM, Vismara et al. (2009) evaluated the
graphics and child skill level. The Tier 2 parents effects of parent training with eight parents who
then provided the training for the next group of received 12 weeks of training with each training
parents, and this pattern was continued until all session lasting 1 h. Vismara and colleagues uti-
of the parents were trained, thus the term “pyra- lized BST and provided parents with a manual on
midal training.” All parents in the professional-­ ESDM principles to teach parents to implement
led training group were trained exclusively by 14 different components of ESDM. Additionally,
professionals rather than previously trained par- the researchers evaluated child progress across
ents. The percentage of steps demonstrated cor- numerous behaviors (e.g., verbal utterances, imi-
rectly across both groups improved from baseline tative behaviors, and attentiveness). The training
to intervention; however, parents who received resulted in improved implementation of ESDM
the pyramidal training performed better on gen- components by the parent participants, which
eralization probes. also corresponded with improvement across the
child measures.
Pivotal Response Training  PRT is a naturalis-
tic treatment intervention that focuses on teach- Picture Exchange Communication System  It
ing pivotal behaviors for children diagnosed has been reported that approximately 25% of
with autism spectrum disorder. These behaviors children diagnosed with ASD will not develop
are considered to be pivotal as they lead to functional vocal language (Tager-Flusberg, Paul,
widespread behavioral gains. PRT focuses on & Lord, 2005). To help children communicate,
increasing motivation, responsivity to multiple the use of augmentative and alternative commu-
cues, self-management, and social initiations. nication systems, such as the PECS (Bondy &
Several studies have explored training parents Frost, 1994), is sometimes required. PECS is a
in the PRT model. For example, Buckley, Ente, systematic teaching approach that uses pictures
and Ruef (2014) provided training to a parent of to help children communicate. Researchers have
a child with an ASD at the family’s home, which demonstrated the effectiveness of PECS to
consisted of providing instructional materials improve communication skills (e.g., Park, Alber-­
on PRT, video models, reviewing videos of the Morgran, & Cannella-Malone, 2011) and increase
parent implementing the intervention, and role-­ spontaneous speech (e.g., Anderson, Moore, &
playing. Targeted skills included, but were not Bournce, 2007) with individuals diagnosed with
limited to, letting the child select the activities/ ASD. However, there have been relatively few
materials, interspersing mastered and acquisi- studies that have evaluated parents’ roles in
tion tasks, and providing choices (Buckley PECS implementation (Ben Chaabane Alber-
et al., 2014). Data was collected on the child’s Morgan, & DeBar, 2009; Park et al., 2011).
rate of compliance and the parent’s target skills, Park et al. (2011) provided an example of one of
and both showed an increase in the rate of cor- the few studies that included parents within the
rect responding following training. Measures of PECS implementation. Park and colleagues trained
improved quality of life (i.e., interviews follow- three mothers of 2-year-old children with an ASD
ing the intervention) also indicated that the par- to implement Phase 1, Phase 2, Phase 3A, and
ent enjoyed the training and felt the quality of Phase 3B of PECS (for detailed description of the
life improved for herself and her child. Phases of PECS see; Bondy & Frost, 1994).
8  Parent Training 117

Training was conducted utilizing BST. The results explored training parents on a variety of proce-
showed an increase in the percentage of indepen- dures (e.g., Cordisco, Strain, & Depew, 1988;
dent picture exchanges and a high level of treat- Harris et al., 1983; Heitzman-­Powell, Buzhardt,
ment integrity across each of the three mothers. Rusinko, Miller, 2014; Koegel et al., 1978;
Sallows & Graupner, 2005).
Functional Communication Training  When Lerman, Swiezy, Perkins-Parks, and Roane
attempting to ameliorate aberrant behavior, it is (2000) provide an example of training three par-
important to find a socially appropriate, functional ents on a variety of behavior change techniques
alternative behavior. One procedure which has based upon the principles of ABA. The behavior
demonstrated effectiveness in teaching such behav- change techniques included the use of differen-
iors is FCT (Carr & Durand, 1985). FCT has been tial reinforcement, instructional and communi-
used to teach responses that produce the same con- cation prompts, as well as how to respond to
sequence that the less desirable behavior would inappropriate behavior, increase compliance,
have produced (e.g., requesting a break to escape a and provide instructions. Training consisted of
task as opposed to engaging in physical aggres- written instructions outlining various concepts
sion). FCT is a commonly implemented technique and techniques, as well as in situ feedback. The
to address aberrant behavior (Tiger, Hanley, & results of a multiple baseline design showed that
Bruzek, 2008), and researchers have demonstrated the parents implemented the techniques with
that parents can be trained in its implementation greater accuracy following intervention and
(Wacker et al., 2005, 2013). child measures indicated the techniques were
Suess et al. (2014) provided an example of effective.
training parents to implement FCT via telehealth.
The training involved didactic instruction and
coaching for three parents to conduct FCT with Child Targets
their respective child following a functional
behavior assessment (FBA). The FBA was con- Many of the studies evaluating parent training
ducted to determine the likely function of the involve measures of child behavior as the primary
aberrant behavior so an appropriate replacement dependent variable. These measures provide an
behavior could be selected. The researchers mea- opportunity to determine if the technique(s) on
sured the percentage of steps completed correctly which the parents are trained were effective for
by the parents from a dyad-specific task analysis. their children. Many of the child skills targeted
Suess and colleagues’ results indicated an increase within the parent training literature fall within the
in the percentage of correct steps completed by core deficit areas of the ASD diagnosis, but there
the parents and a corresponding decrease in the are additional skills outside of the core deficits
children’s aberrant behavior. that are frequently targeted as well.

Multiple Component  Quality behavioral inter- Language  One of the diagnostic criteria for
vention requires a therapist to not only implement individuals diagnosed with ASD is an impair-
one procedure but a variety of procedures and to ment in language, which can range from mild
implement these procedures accurately (Leaf (e.g., difficulties with complex social language)
et al., 2016). Thus, a therapist should be fluent in to severe (e.g., having no appropriate vocal lan-
the implementation of procedures such as DTT, guage; American Psychiatric Association, 2013).
shaping, behavior reduction programs, social Behavioral interventions frequently address lan-
skills interventions, etc. (Leaf et al., 2016). Given guage skills for individuals diagnosed with
the amount of time parents spend with their chil- ASD. Therefore, it is not surprising that many
dren, some of which may involve providing inter- parent training programs have focused on train-
vention, it is equally important for parents to be ing parents to implement techniques to improve
fluent in a number of behavior change techniques. language. As such, child measures within the par-
As such, there have been several studies that have ent training research have shown that, following
118 J.B. Leaf et al.

training, parents were effective in increasing sound Reduction of Aberrant Behavior  Individuals
production (e.g., Harris et al., 1983), word produc- diagnosed with ASD can display a variety of aber-
tion (e.g., Harris et al., 1983), requesting (e.g., rant behaviors (e.g., stereotypic behavior, self-
Suess et al., 2014), spontaneous language (e.g., injury, aggression, sleeping challenges, etc.), all of
Charlop & Trasowech 1991; Ingersoll & Wainer, which can interfere with learning and decrease
2013a), social exchanges (e.g., Park et al., 2011), their overall quality of life (Bearss et al., 2015). As
and social communication (e.g., Ingersoll & such, there are many techniques that can decrease
Wainer, 2013b; Reagon & Higbee, 2009; Vismara the frequency, intensity, and duration of aberrant
et al., 2009). For instance, Charlop and Trasowech behavior. Decreasing the likelihood of aberrant
(1991) evaluated parent training focused on lan- behavior can also decrease stress and anxiety for
guage development for three parents of children parents and the rest of the family (Durand,
diagnosed with ASD using BST. Parents were Hieneman, Clarke, Wang, & Rinaldi, 2013).
taught to implement a progressive time delay Therefore, research on parent training has explored
prompt (i.e., gradually increasing the amount of training parents in techniques to ameliorate these
time before a prompt is provided) to help increase challenges. Within the parent training literature,
spontaneous speech from their respective child. child measures have helped show that parents who
Using a multiple baseline design, the results successfully implemented techniques on which
showed that there was an increase in the children’s they were trained resulted in a decrease in aggres-
spontaneous speech and generalization to other sion displayed by their child (e.g., Lerman et al.,
people and locations following parent training. 2000; Powers, Singer, Stevens, 1992), as well as
decreases in whining (Powers et al., 1992), non-
Social Skills  Another core deficit for individuals compliance (Lerman et al., 2000; Powers et al.,
diagnosed with ASD is a qualitative impairment in 1992), stereotypy (e.g., Bearss et al., 2015), irrita-
social behavior (American Psychiatric Association, bility (e.g., Bearss et al., 2015), self-injury (e.g.,
2013). As such, comprehensive, quality interven- Learman et al., 2000), sleeping issues (e.g., Malow
tion should address deficits in social behavior et al., 2014), and mealtime challenges (e.g.,
(Leaf et al., 2016). Much of the research involving Najdowski et al., 2010; Seiverling et al., 2012;
parents has focused on training techniques to Sharp, Bureel, & Jaquess, 2014).
increase specific social behaviors and/or to facili- In an example of parent training to decrease
tate pro-social relationships (Crockett et al., 2007; aberrant behavior, Bearss and colleagues (2015)
Kashinath, Woods, & Goldstein, 2006; Laugeson conducted a comparison investigation consisting
et al., 2009; Radley, Jenson, Clark, & O’Neill, of randomly placing 91 parents in a parent train-
2014; Yoo et al., 2014). One example of parent ing program and 89 parents in a parent education
training targeting social behavior was a study con- program across six different centers in the United
ducted by Kashinath et al. (2006) in which the States. The parent training program consisted of
researchers used BST to teach five parents how to BST, while the parent education program con-
implement a variety of behavioral procedures sisted of providing parents with didactic informa-
(e.g., cuing, time delay, and modeling). One of the tion. Using the Aberrant Behavior Checklist as
targeted skills was improving the child’s indoor their main measure, both treatments led to a
play, and the results showed that parent training decrease in aberrant behavior, but the results
led to improvements with this skill. Laugeson showed that parent training was superior to par-
et al. (2009) provide another example in which ent education for reducing aberrant behavior
parents were trained how to help facilitate and fos- according to the parents across both groups.
ter relationships (e.g., friendships) within the
PEERS model. After training occurred, partici- Other Skills  Parent training research has also
pants who were included in the PEERS model examined child behaviors that do not fall within
demonstrated an improvement in their social the core deficit categories of ASD. Additional
behavior and interactions with peers. parent training interventions have resulted in
8  Parent Training 119

i­mprovements of child skills within the areas of Future Directions


self-help skills (e.g., Cordisico et al., 1988), joint
attention (e.g., Kasari et al., 2015, Rocha et al., The research on parent training is robust.
2007), receptive instructions (e.g., Lafasakis & Researchers have shown that parent training can
Sturmey, 2007), discrimination (e.g., Koegel et al., be effective in changing the behavior of parents of
1978), and cognitive development (e.g., Anan individuals diagnosed with an ASD using multiple
et al., 2008). Researchers have also used parent methods (e.g., behavioral skills training, coaching,
training to help parents improve their stress levels video modeling, etc.). Researchers have also dem-
(e.g., Al-Khalaf, Dempsey, & Dally, 2014; Ali onstrated that parents who receive training can
Samadi & Mahmoodizadeh, 2014), increase their implement a variety of procedures (e.g., shaping,
self-efficacy (e.g., Poslawsky et al., 2015), increase discrete trial teaching, ESDM, etc.) that result in
general knowledge of autism (e.g., Farmer & meaningful changes for them and their children.
Reupert, 2013), and increase their ability to record Despite the extensive parent training literature
behavior (e.g., Herbert & Baer, 1972). base, there are several areas in which future
research and clinical practice could focus.

Types of Measurement
Parent Demographics
Researchers have used a variety of measures to
evaluate the effects of parent training. Numerous One potential area future researchers should
studies have used direct measures (i.e., objective address involves expanding the descriptions of
data) of the behavior of the parents (e.g., imple- parent participants. Researchers should make a
menting FCT, implementing shaping, implement- concerted effort to provide a complete descrip-
ing DTT) who participated (e.g., Berquist & tion of the demographics of the parents who are
Charlop, 2014; Corsidico et al., 1988; Crockett, participating in the training. There are demo-
Fleming, Doepke, Stevens, 2007; Harris et al., graphics that could potentially affect the effec-
1981, 1983; Herbet & Baer, 1972; Lafasakis & tiveness of an intervention, including, but not
Sturmey, 2007). Other studies have used subjec- limited to, the parents’ age and gender, education
tive, rather than objective, measures to demonstrate level, socioeconomic status (SES), and cultural
improvements in parent behavior (e.g., Cordisco characteristics. It is common for researchers to
et al. 1988; Farmer & Reupert, 2013; Heitzman- provide information regarding age and gender;
Powell et al., 2014). There have also been several however, there are examples in which little to no
studies that have used formal and/or standardized demographic information is reported, and demo-
assessments to measure progress (Anan et al., graphics, such as culture and SES, are typically
2008; Bearss et al., 2015b; Estes et al., 2014). never reported. Without providing demographic
While parent behavior is generally the pri- information, it would be difficult, if not impossi-
mary focus of parent training, the desired out- ble, to identify any relationship between parent
come of training parents is to produce positive variables and response to training.
behavior change with their children. Therefore, Reporting demographic information to help
measures of the child’s behavior change are com- identify the conditions under which certain train-
monly taken and, in some cases, are the primary ing methods can lead to better skill acquisition is
dependent variables (e.g., Charlop & Trasowech, crucial. For example, researchers have shown that
1991; Cordisco et al., 1988; Harris et al., 1983; parent training may be less effective for parents of
Herbert & Baer, 1972; Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013). lower SES (e.g., Clark & Baker, 1983; Knapp &
There also have been several studies that have Deluty, 1989). Some associated challenges with
combined various measures (e.g., Cordisco et al., this demographic, such as working multiple jobs,
1988; Harris et al., 1983; Herbert & Baer, 1972; may result in less effective training for reasons
Ingersoll & Wainer, 2013a). such as time limitations or scheduling challenges.
120 J.B. Leaf et al.

As such, future researchers should strive to pro- Measurement


vide a complete description of the parents partici-
pating in training to allow researchers to analyze The parent training literature includes a variety
their results with respect to these demographics. of ways in which researchers measure the effects
As a result, researchers and clinicians could of parent training. These measures include
attempt to identify which demographics result in direct objective measurement of parent behav-
better skill acquisition with respect to certain train- ior (e.g., Neef, 1995), direct objective measure-
ing procedures. This would also allow for future ment of child behavior (e.g., Rocha et al.,
research to investigate the best training procedures 2007), subjective data (e.g., Farmer & Reupert,
to use for different demographics to allow for all 2013), standardized assessments (e.g., Ingersoll
parents to better access effective parent training. & Wainer, 2013b), and/or a combination (e.g.,
Gender is another important demographic that Rocha et al. 2007). One of the hallmarks of
may influence the effectiveness of parent training. behavior analysis (and science in general) is the
It has been reported that mothers and fathers of reliance on objective data (Cooper et al., 2007).
individuals diagnosed with ASD have different Therefore, ­subjective measurement can provide
roles within the family context (Pleck & valuable information regarding the parent train-
Masciadrelli, 2004), have varying levels of stress ing program (e.g., social validity) but should
(Flippin & Crais, 2011), and interact with their not be relied upon as the main measure of
children in different manners (Flippin & Crais, effectiveness.
2011). If gender is part of the conditions under
which a certain method of parent training is effec-
tive, reporting information on parents’ gender Social Validity
within the research is critical. Furthermore,
researchers should make an effort to evaluate par- An additional measurement that should be
ent training for fathers of individuals diagnosed found in clinical practice is social validity
with an ASD because, while there have been some (Wolf, 1978). Although social validity was not
studies which have included fathers, it is far more originally identified as one of the seven dimen-
common for mothers to participate thus leaving sions of ABA (Baer, Wolf, & Risley, 1968,
father participation vastly underrepresented 1987), Wolf (1978) stated that measures of
(Flippin & Crais, 2011). A father’s involvement, social validity is how ABA would find its
interaction styles, and stress may be different than “heart,” so that our consumers would find an
a mother’s and may influence the selection of the opportunity to provide us with feedback. Parents
training procedure, format, and targets. should be involved from the onset of training in
The culture of the parent who participates in the selection of goals and procedures to be imple-
training is another demographic that is not com- mented. Additionally, researchers should mea-
monly reported. Culture plays a large role in how a sure satisfaction with the results of the training
family may interact with each other and other with the parents and, when possible, the indi-
­families, handle having a child with a diagnosis, viduals diagnosed with ASD. Although social
prioritize training targets, and view their role in validity has been included in some parent train-
intervention. It is difficult to examine research find- ing research, there are many studies in which it
ings with respect to cultural aspects when informa- has not. Future researchers should make an
tion on culture is not reported. More importantly, if effort to include social validity in every future
culture is not reported, it is a possibility that cul- study that evaluates parent training. Clinicians
tural characteristics were not taken into account should also measure social validity as part of a
when designing the parent training features that are comprehensive evaluation of their training pro-
under examination. Ignoring cultural characteris- gram to ensure satisfaction by those involved in
tics, even if unintentional, could lead to failures to the training and to inform clinicians of any
replicate, ineffective training, cultural insensitivity, modifications to the training that may make it
and reduced consumer acceptability. more socially valid for future use.
8  Parent Training 121

Training the Trainers trainers to effectively work with parents of chil-


dren with ASD. Additionally, future researchers
Both in future research and in clinical practice, should evaluate ways to train soft skills to the
behavior analysts must discover the most effec- individuals who will be providing the parent
tive and efficient ways to train professionals who training. Finally, future researchers should evalu-
will be providing parent training. As mentioned ate if the parent trainers who were taught soft
earlier, the most appropriate method may differ skills provided training that resulted in quicker
from trainee to trainee so this is also an important rates of learning for the parent trainees, higher
area to consider to ensure that training is as effec- levels of parent satisfaction, and greater parental
tive and efficient as possible. utilization of skills taught.
An important area to consider when teaching Training future behavior analysts how to work
professionals to train parents is how to do so with with parents is critical to providing a higher qual-
clinical sensitivity, that is, to do so with an under- ity of intervention leading to better outcomes for
standing of the struggles that parents of individu- individuals diagnosed with autism. Therefore,
als diagnosed with ASD go through on a daily how to provide effective parent training should
basis. It is important to teach the trainers to train be included as a component of a behavior ana-
parents with compassion and empathy, as well as lysts training (e.g., undergraduate programs,
maintaining balance between the child’s individ- graduate programs, and service providers).
ual needs and the needs of the entire family unit. Pertinent parent training skills should also be
These skills are critical if professionals are going required as part of certification/licensure. Thus,
to work effectively with parents. If behavior ana- training the trainers is not only an important com-
lysts are to focus on training in the absence of ponent of future research but also an important
these skills, parents may be less likely to feel component of clinical practice.
comfortable participating in training. In other
words, ignoring the contingencies under which
parents are operating and paying sole attention to A Progressive Model
the contingencies affecting the child’s behavior
may lead to ineffective or short-term changes in The majority of research on parent training has
parent behavior. For example, identifying that a focused on a professional-led training program
child’s challenging behavior is maintained by for parents to implement a single procedure (e.g.,
social positive reinforcement (e.g., parent atten- Suess et al., 2014), a few procedures (e.g., Barton
tion) and training the parent to ignore the behav- & Lissman, 2015), or how to implement a com-
ior without understanding the contingencies prehensive intervention (e.g., Buckley et al.,
operating for the parents may result in teaching 2014). Although parent training has been used to
the parent a “skill” that he/she cannot use in the teach parents to implement a variety of proce-
natural environment. So when in a grocery store, dures, the majority of these studies presumably
if the child engages in challenging behavior, have taught parents to implement the procedures
ignoring the child’s behavior may not be the most in a way that requires strict adherence to specific
ideal approach for the parent if providing atten- protocols. Furthermore, the underlying concep-
tion to the child serves a negatively reinforcing tual basis for the technique is often not trained
effect for the parent. which may lead to training parents to implement
Although these “soft skills” may be hard to the techniques inflexibly. Training parents to fol-
conceptualize and may not be as simple to define low a protocol may be easier to train, measure,
as a more concrete procedure, such as prompting, and is often the current model of the field (Leaf
they are critical skills that need to be taught to et al. 2016); however, this type of training could
future parent trainers. A first step would be for be considered a prescriptive model (i.e., parents
future researchers to identify and operationally are taught to implement specific procedures
define all of the soft skills that are needed for under specific contextual variables) rather than a
122 J.B. Leaf et al.

flexible model in which the parents can make in-­ From the beginning of the applied research and
the-­moment changes based on the child’s behav- clinical implementation of ABA-based proce-
ior (i.e., a progressive model; Leaf et al., 2016). dures for individuals diagnosed with ASD, pro-
Training in a progressive model consists of fessionals have demonstrated the advantages and
training the parents on the principles underlying importance of parent training. Today, parent
the procedures and rationales for their use as well training has support as an evidence-based
as on the procedures themselves. This may allow ­procedure (Smith & Iadarola, 2015) which can be
for greater overarching impact, longer mainte- used to instruct parents how to implement a vari-
nance, and generalization of skills (e.g., Leaf et al., ety of procedures (e.g., BST, DTT) to teach a
2016). Additionally, this could be considered more wide assortment of skills (e.g., language, social,
of a psychoeducational model in which parents self-­help). Although there are several areas that
develop a broader understanding of behavioral should be evaluated by future researchers and
principles. Training in this model contrasts with explored by clinicians, there is a breadth of evi-
training that is solely focused on following a spe- dence supporting parent training as part of a com-
cific, strict protocol (e.g., a prescriptive model). prehensive treatment program. Providing parent
While several studies have evaluated compo- training can result in better outcomes for indi-
nents of a progressive model (Leaf et al., 2016), viduals diagnosed with ASD and an improved
none have specifically evaluated the model with quality of life for parents, children, and all mem-
respect to parent training. However, components of bers of the family unit.
the progressive model were utilized as part of
Lovaas et al. (1973) and Lovaas (1987). Within a
progressive model, as applied to parent training, References
the parent would be trained to use clinical judg-
ment, in-the-moment assessment and decision-­ Al-Khalaf, A., Dempsey, I., & Dally, K. (2014). The effect
of an education program for mothers of children with
making, and flexible teaching while implementing autism spectrum disorder in Jordan. International
a variety of behaviorally based techniques. When Journal for the Advancement of Counselling, 36(2),
parents are trained in this model, instead of adher- 175–187.
ing to a strict protocol, they would be trained to American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
understand the principles of ABA, as well as when Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.
and how to adjust teaching and make in-the- Anan, R. M., Warner, L. J., McGillivary, J. E., Chong,
moment assessments. Future researchers should I. M., & Hines, S. J. (2008). Group Intensive Family
investigate the use of this model for parent training Training (GIFT) for preschoolers with autism spec-
trum disorders. Behavioral Interventions, 23(3),
and focus on measures of parents’ clinical judg- 165–180.
ments, in-the-moment assessments and decision- Anderson, A., Moore, D., & Bourne, T. (2007). Functional
making, flexibility in teaching and prompt fading, communication and other concomitant behaviour
and implementation of multiple procedures simul- change following PECS training: A case study.
Behavior Change, 24, 1–8.
taneously. Additional measures could include com- Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
prehension of guiding principles as opposed to current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
learning specific, isolated procedures. Training in Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1(1), 91–97.
this way may result in long-term success for both Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1987). Some
still-current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
the parent and the individual diagnosed with ASD. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20(4), 313–327.
Baker, B. L., Heifetz, L. J., & Murphy, D. M. (1980).
Behavioral training for parents of mentally retarded
Conclusion children: One-year follow-up. American Journal of
Mental Deficiency, 85, 31–38.
Barton, E. E., & Lissman, D. C. (2015). Group parent
For over 50 years, the principles of ABA have training combined with follow-up coaching for par-
been utilized to implement interventions for indi- ents of children with developmental delays. Infants &
viduals diagnosed with ASD (Smith, 2012). Young Children, 28(3), 220–236.
8  Parent Training 123

Bearss, K., Burrell, T. L., Stewart, L., & Scahill, L. young children with autism. Journal of Autism and
(2015a). Parent training in autism spectrum disor- Developmental Disorders, 44, 353–365.
der: What’s in a name? Clinical Child and Family Farmer, J., & Reupert, A. (2013). Understanding autism
Psychology Review, 18(2), 170–182. and understanding my child with autism: An evaluation
Bearss, K., Johnson, C., Smith, T., Lecavalier, L., Swiezy, of a group parent education program in rural Australia.
N., Aman, M., et al. (2015b). Effect of parent train- The Australian Journal of Rural Health, 21(1), 20–27.
ing vs parent education on behavioral problems in Flippin, M., & Crais, E. R. (2011). The need for more
children with autism spectrum disorder: A random- effective father involvement in early autism inter-
ized clinical trial. JAMA: The Journal of the American vention: A systematic review and recommendations.
Medical Association, 313(15), 1524–1533. Journal of Early Intervention, 33(1), 24–50.
Ben Chaabane, D. B., Alber-Morgan, S. R., & DeBar, Forehand, R., Middlebrook, J., Rogers, T., & Steffe, M.
R. M. (2009). The effects of parent-implemented (1983). Dropping out of parent training. Behaviour
PECS training on improvisation of mands by children Research and Therapy, 21(6), 663–668.
with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Harris, S. L., Wolchik, S. A., & Weitz, S. (1981). The
42(3), 671–677. acquisition of language skills by autistic children:
Berquist, K. L., & Charlop, M. H. (2014). Teaching par- Can parents do the job? Journal of Autism and
ents of children with autism to evaluate interventions. Developmental Disorders, 11(4), 373–384.
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, Harris, S. L., Wolchik, S. A., & Milch, R. E. (1983). Changing
46, 371–389. the speech of autistic children and their parents. Child
Bondy, A. S., & Frost, L. A. (1994). The picture exchange and Family Behavior Therapy, 4(2–3), 151–173.
communication system. Focus on Autism and Other Heitzman-Powell, L. S., Buzhardt, J., Rusinko, L. C.,
Developmental Disabilities, 9(3), 1–19. & Miller, T. M. (2014). Formative evaluation of an
Buckley, T. W., Ente, A. P., & Ruef, M. B. (2014). ABA outreach training program for parents of chil-
Improving a family’s overall quality of life through dren with autism in remote areas. Focus on Autism and
parent training in pivotal response treatment. Journal OtherbDevelopmental Disabilities, 29(1), 23–38.
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 16(1), 60–63. Herbert, E. W., & Baer, D. M. (1972). Training parents
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behav- as behavior modifiers: Self-recording of contingent
ior problems through functional communication attention. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 5(2),
training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 139–149.
18(2), 111–126. Ingersoll, B., & Wainer, A. (2013a). Initial efficacy of
Charlop, M. H., & Trasowech, J. E. (1991). Increasing project ImPACT: A parent-mediated social commu-
autistic children’s daily spontaneous speech. Journal nication intervention for young children with ASD.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(4), 747–761. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Christensen, D. L., Baio, J., Braun, K. V., et al. (2012). 43(12), 2943–2952.
Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum Ingersoll, B. R., & Wainer, A. L. (2013b). Pilot study of a
disorder among children aged 8 years – autism and school-based parent training program for preschoolers
developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 with ASD. Autism, 17, 434–448.
sites. MMWR, 65(3), 1–23. Kasari, C., Gulsrud, A., Paparella, T., Hellemann, G.,
Clark, D. B., & Baker, B. L. (1983). Predicting outcome & Berry, K. (2015). Randomized comparative effi-
in parent training. Journal of Consulting and Clinical cacy study of parent-mediated interventions for tod-
Psychology, 51, 309–311. dlers with autism. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Psychology, 83(3), 554–563.
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Kashinath, S., Woods, J., & Goldstein, H. (2006). Enhancing
Pearson. generalized teaching strategy use in daily routines by
Cordisco, L. K., Strain, P. S., & Depew, N. (1988). parents of children with autism. Journal of Speech,
Assessment for generalization of parenting skills in Language, and Hearing Research, 49(3), 466–485.
home settings. Journal of the Association for Persons Knapp, P. A., & Deluty, R. H. (1989). Relative effec-
with Severe Handicaps, 13(3), 202–210. tiveness of two behavioral parent training programs.
Crockett, J. L., Fleming, R. K., Doepke, K. J., & Stevens, Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 314–322.
J. S. (2007). Parent training: Acquisition and general- Koegel, R. L., Glahn, T. J., & Nieminen, G. S. (1978).
ization of discrete trials teaching skills with parents Generalization of parent-training results. Journal of
of children with autism. Research in Developmental Applied Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 95–109.
Disabilities, 28(1), 23–36. Koegel, R. L., Schreibman, L., Britten, K. R., & O’Neill,
Durand, V. M., Hieneman, M., Clarke, S., Wang, M., & R. E. (1982). A comparison of parent training to direct
Rinaldi, M. L. (2013). Positive family intervention child treatment. In R. L. Koegel, A. Rincover, & A. L.
for severe challenging behavior I: A multisite ran- Egel (Eds.), Educating and understanding autistic
domized clinical trial. Journal of Positive Behavior children. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.
Interventions, 15(3), 133–143. Koegel, R. L., Bimbela, A., & Schreibman, L. (1996).
Estes, A., Vismara, L., Mercado, C., Fitzpatrick, A., Collateral effects of parent training on family interac-
Elder, L., Greenson, J., et al. (2014). The impact of tions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
parent-delivered intervention on parents of very 26(3), 347–359.
124 J.B. Leaf et al.

Lafasakis, M., & Sturmey, P. (2007). Training parent practice, guidelines, for autism spectrum disorders,
implementation of discrete-trial teaching: Effects on Natural Autism Center.
generalization of parent teaching and child correct Neef, N. A. (1995). Pyramidal parent training by
responding. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, peers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28(3),
40(4), 685–689. 333–337.
Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Mogil, C., & Dillon, A. R. Park, J. H., Alber-Morgan, S. R.. 2., & Cannella-Malone,
(2009). Parent-assisted social skills training to improve H. (2011). Effects of mother-implemented Picture
friendships in teens with autism spectrum disorders. Exchange Communication System (PECS) training on
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, independent communicative behaviors of young chil-
39(4), 596–606. dren with autism spectrum disorders. Topics in Early
Leaf, R. B., McEachin, J. J., & Taubman, M. (2008). Childhood Special Education, 31(1), 37–47.
Sense and nonsense in the behavioral treatment of Patterson, G. R., & Fleischman, M. J. (1979). Maintenance
autism: It has to be said. New York, NY: Different of treatment effects: Some considerations concerning
Roads to Learning. family systems and follow-up data. Behavior Therapy,
Leaf, J. B., Tsuji, K. H., Lentell, A. E., Dale, S. E., 10, 168–185.
Kassardjian, A., Tuabman, M., … Oppenheim-Leaf, Pleck, J. H., & Masciadrelli, B. P. (2004). Parental involve-
M. L. (2013). A comparison of discrete trial teach- ment by U.S. residential fathers: Levels, sources, and
ing implemented in a one-to-one instructional for- consequences. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the
mat and in a group instructional format. Behavioral father in child development (4th ed.pp. 222–271).
Interventions, 28, 82–106. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Leaf, J. B., Leaf, R., McEachin, J., Taubman, M., Ala’i-­ Poslawsky, I. E., Naber, F. B., Bakermans-Kranenburg,
Rosales, S., Ross, R. K., et al. (2016). Applied behav- M. J., van Daalen, E., van Engeland, H., & van
ior analysis is a science and, therefore, progressive. IJzendoorn, M. H. (2015). Video-feedback interven-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, tion to promote positive parenting adapted to autism
46(2), 720–731. (VIPP-AUTI): A randomized controlled trial. Autism,
Lerman, D. C., Swiezy, N., Perkins-Parks, S., & Roane, 19(5), 588–603.
H. S. (2000). Skill acquisition in parents of chil- Powers, L. E., Singer, G., & Stevens, T. (1992). Behavioral
dren with developmental disabilities: Interaction parent training in home and community general-
between skill type and instructional format. Research ization settings. Education and Training in Mental
in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Retardation, 27(1), 13–27.
Journal, 21(3), 183–196. Radley, K. C., Jenson, W. R., Clark, E., & O'Neill,
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal R. E. (2014). The feasibility and effects of a parent-­
educational and intellectual functioning in young facilitated social skills training program on social
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical engagement of children with autism spectrum disor-
Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. ders. Psychology in the Schools, 51(3), 241–255.
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. Reagon, K. A., & Higbee, T. S. (2009). Parent-­
(1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures implemented script fading to promote play-based
on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of verbal initiations in children with autism. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. Applied Behavior Analysis, 42(3), 659–664.
Malow, B. A., Adkins, K. W., Reynolds, A., Weiss, S. K., Rocha, M. L., Schreibman, L., & Stahmer, A. C. (2007).
Loh, A., Fawkes, D., et al. (2014). Parent-based sleep Effectiveness of training parents to teach joint attention
education for children with autism spectrum disor- in children with autism. Journal of Early Intervention,
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 154–172.
44(1), 216–228. Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive
Moes, D. R., & Frea, W. D. (2002). Contextualized behavioral treatment for children with autism: Four-­
behavioral support in early intervention for children year outcome and predictors. American Journal on
with autism and their families. Journal of Autism and Mental Retardation, 110, 417–438.
Developmental Disorders, 32(6), 519–533. Samadi, S. A., & Mahmoodizadeh, A. (2014). Omid early
Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Reagon, K., Penrod, intervention resource kit for children with autism
B., Higbee, T. S., & Tarbox, J. (2010). Utilizing a spectrum disorders and their families. Early Child
home-based parent training approach in the treatment Development and Care, 184(3), 354–369.
of food selectivity. Behavioral Interventions, 25(2), Schreibman, L., Kaneko, W. M., & Koegel, R. L. (1991).
89–107. Positive affect of parents of autistic children: A com-
National Standard Report. (2009). The national stan- parison across two teaching techniques. Behavior
dards project-addressing the need for evidence-based Therapy, 22(4), 479–490.
practice, guidelines, for autism spectrum disorders, Seiverling, L., Williams, K., Sturmey, P., & Hart, S.
Natural Autism Center. (2012). Effects of behavioral skills training on paren-
National Standard Report. (2015). The national standards tal treatment of children’s food selectivity. Journal of
project Phase 2-addressing the need for ­evidence-­based Applied Behavior Analysis, 45(1), 197–203.
8  Parent Training 125

Sharp, W. G., Burrell, T. L., & Jaquess, D. L. (2014). The Vismara, L. A., Colombi, C., & Rogers, S. J. (2009).
autism MEAL plan: A parent-training curriculum to Can one hour per week of therapy lead to lasting
manage eating aversions and low intake among chil- changes in young children with autism? Autism,
dren with autism. Autism, 18(6), 712–722. 13(1), 93–115.
Smith, T. (2012). Evolution of research on interven- Vismara, L. A., McCormick, C., Young, G. S., Nadhan, A.,
tions for individuals with autism spectrum disorder: & Monlux, K. (2013). Preliminary findings of a tele-
Implications for behavior analysts. The Behavior health approach to parent training in autism. Journal
Analyst Today, 35(1), 101–113. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12),
Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2015). Evidence base update for 2953–2969.
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical Child & Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Harding, J. W., Barretto, A.,
Adolescent Psychology, 44(6), 897–922. Rankin, B., & Ganzer, J. (2005). Treatment effec-
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized tiveness, stimulus, generalization, and acceptabil-
trial of intensive early intervention for children with ity to parents of functional communication training.
pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal Educational Psychology, 25, 233–256.
on Mental Retardation, 105(4), 269–285. Wacker, D. P., Lee, J. F., Padilla Dalmau, Y. C., Kopelman,
Stokes, T., & Baer, D. (1977). An implicit technology of T. G., Lindgren S. D., Kuhle, J., Pelzel, K. E.,
generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Dyson, S., Schieltz, K. M., & Waldron D. B. (2013).
10(2), 349–367. Conducting functional communication training via
Suess, A. N., Romani, P. W., Wacker, D. P., Dyson, S. M., telehealth to reduce the problem behavior of young
Kuhle, J. L., Lee, J. F., et al. (2014). Evaluating the children with Autism. Journal of Developmental and
treatment fidelity of parents who conduct in-home func- Physical Disabilities, 25(1), 35–48.
tional communication training with coaching via tele- Wolf, M., Risley, T., & Mees, H. (1963). Application
health. Journal of Behavioral Education, 23(1), 34–59. of operant conditioning procedures to the behaviour
Symon, J. B. (2005). Expanding interventions for children problems of an autistic child. Behaviour Research and
with autism: Parents as trainers. Journal of Positive Therapy, 1(2), 305–312.
Behavior Interventions, 7(3), 159–173. Wolf, M. M. (1978). Social validity: The case for sub-
Tager-Flusberg, H., Paul, R., & Lord, C. (2005). Language jective measurement or how applied behavior analy-
and communication in autism. In F. R. Volkmar, sis is finding its heart. Journal of Applied Behavior
R. Paul, A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of Analysis, 11(2), 203–214.
autism and pervasive developmental disorders (3rd Yoo, H. J., Bahn, G., Cho, I. H., Kim, E. K., Kim, J. H.,
ed.pp. 335–364). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley and Sons. Min, J. W., et al. (2014). A randomized controlled trial
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional of the Korean version of the PEERS® parent-assisted
communication training: A review and practical guide. social skills training program for teens with ASD.
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(1), 16–23. Autism Research, 7(1), 145–161.
Treatment of Core Symptoms
of Autism Spectrum Disorder 9
Matthew T. Brodhead, Mandy J. Rispoli,
Oliver Wendt, Jessica S. Akers,
Kristina R. Gerencser, and So Yeon Kim

Introduction Social Skills

ASD is comprised of core deficits in social and Deficits in social interaction and social communica-
communication skills and high levels of engage- tion are a core deficit of ASD (American Psychiatric
ment in restrictive, repetitive behaviors and inter- Association, 2013). To meet the DSM-V diagnostic
ests. To date, treatments based on the science of criteria for ASD, an individual must present with or
behavior analysis have been shown to be the most have a history of deficits in (a) social-emotional
effective treatment of core symptoms of ASD reciprocity, (b) deficits in nonverbal communicative
(Odom, Boyd, Hall, & Hume, 2009). Further, the behaviors used for social interaction, and (c) deficits
earlier these behavioral interventions are initi- in developing, maintaining, and understanding rela-
ated, the better the treatment outcomes (Smith, tionships (American Psychiatric Association,
Klorman, & Mruzek, 2015). This chapter pro- 2013). This section surveys representative treat-
vides an overview of treatment options for social ment options for a range of social interaction skills,
skills, communication skills, and restrictive, starting from basic (i.e., eye contact and joint atten-
repetitive behaviors and interests. tion), intermediate (i.e., play skills), and advanced
(i.e., perspective taking and lying). The following
section details strategies for teaching communica-
tive social skills in additional detail.

M.T. Brodhead (*)


Department of Educational Studies, Basic Social Skills
Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Michigan State University, East Lansing, One of the core deficits – that is identified as an
Michigan, USA
early indicator – for individuals with ASD is lack
e-mail: mtb@msu.edu
of eye contact. Many individuals with ASD do
M.J. Rispoli • O. Wendt • S.Y. Kim
not develop eye contact without specific training.
Department of Educational Studies, Purdue
University, West Lafayette, IN, USA Thus, eye contact is one of the first skills taught
to learners in an early intensive behavior inter-
J.S. Akers
University of Nebraska Medical Center, vention (EIBI) program. Eye contact with both
Omaha, NE, USA an instructor and instructional materials is a piv-
K.R. Gerencser otal behavior in order to learn new skills and
Utah State University, Logan, UT, USA interact socially.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 127


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_9
128 M.T. Brodhead et al.

Several techniques have been developed to made with eye contact during play. The therapist
teach eye contact. Early behavior analysts used a attempted to manipulate the motivating operation
verbal cue (e.g., “Look at me”) and a differential for specific items/activities (e.g., catching the
reinforcement and prompting procedures to bring ball and waiting for the student to request the ball
eye contact under instructional control of the ver- before returning it) in order to contrive opportu-
bal cue (Foxx, 1977; Greer & Ross, 2007; Lovaas, nities for the participant to make requests. The
1981). Although eye contact can be successfully intervention was effective in increasing requests
taught through this procedure, it is rather con- made with eye contact for the participants and
trived and may not come under control of the maintained at varying degrees following 3 months
appropriate antecedents and consequences, thus after the intervention. On the other hand, acquisi-
having little generality outside of the instruc- tion of mands made with eye contact increased at
tional activity. More recently, behavior analysts a quicker rate following the intervention with
have shifted to teaching eye contact under natural each therapist, thus demonstrating some evidence
contexts, such as embedded within discrete trial of generalization. The results of these studies
instruction (e.g., imitation, matching, etc.), mand demonstrate the possible utility of using natural
training, and play. social contexts combined with differential rein-
Mand training, which takes advantage of a forcement and prompting procedures to increase
learner’s motivation, is one contextually appro- eye contact.
priate context that can be used to increase eye More recently, O’Handley, Radley, and
contact and social initiations. Charlop-Christy, Whipple (2015) compared the effects of an inter-
Carpenter, Le, LeBlanc, and Kellet (2002) taught vention package, which consisted of social sto-
three children with ASD to mand using the ries and video modeling, on the eye contact of six
Picture Exchange Communication System adolescents with ASD. Findings of this study
(PECS; Frost & Bondy, 1994) and evaluated indicated that using only social stories led to
potential collateral effects related to social behav- moderated improvements, but after video model-
iors, such as eye contact and joint attention (JA). ing was combined to social stories, participants
Following mand training, an increase in eye con- demonstrated further improvements. Applying
tact and JA, from 25% in baseline to 54% in video modeling in isolation showed strong inter-
intervention, was observed across all three par- vention effects, and combining social stories to
ticipants. However, eye contact was not required video modeling contributed to minimal addi-
during the mand to receive access to the requested tional improvements.
item. More recently, Carbone, O’Brien, Sweeney-­ The acquisition of eye contact may also be a
Kerwin, and Albert (2013) assessed the effective- prerequisite skill for teaching more advanced
ness of differential reinforcement to increase social skills such as joint attention (Ninci et al.,
mands made with eye contact for a child with 2013; Taylor & Hoch, 2008). Joint attention
ASD. During baseline, all vocal mands were refers to a set of behaviors that involve the shared
reinforced regardless of eye contact. Throughout attention between a social partner and a stimulus,
baseline, the participant rarely made eye contact and attention could be shared by using various
with the researcher while requesting. During topographies. Some of the topographies may
treatment, the researcher withheld reinforcement include shift in eye gaze, gestures, vocal or verbal
(i.e., access to the item) until the participant made communication using one or more words, or any
eye contact. Following the differential reinforce- combination of those (Rudy, Betz, Malone,
ment procedure, percentage of mands with eye Henry, & Chong, 2014). Joint attention skills are
contact increased from a mean of 10% in baseline considered a pivotal skill in a child’s social and
to a mean of 77%. communication development (Adamson &
Ninci et al. (2013) also investigated the effects Bakeman, 1984). Thus, joint attention should be
of a differential reinforcement with the addition one of the earliest social skills taught in a child’s
of a prompting procedure to increase mands EIBI curriculum.
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 129

There are two forms of joint attention, (e.g., pointing and/or orienting, eye gaze shift,
responding to joint attention bids and initiating vocal and nonvocal comments/initiations) and
joint attention bids. Responding to other’s joint teach each skill individually. In summary, teach-
attention bids could include changes in gaze ing these pivotal skills, eye contact and joint
direction or verbal responses. Initiating joint attention, are of upmost importance to interven-
attention bids could include asking some else’s tionists working with individuals with ASD. The
attention by pointing an item or emitting verbal way in which interventionists teach eye contact
words (e.g., saying “Look!”). Both forms of joint and JA can impact the generality of these skills.
attention can be taught together as one complete Recently, Rudy et al. (2014) investigated the
joint attention skill or each form separately. effects of video modeling on initiate bids for joint
Several techniques have been used to teach joint attention in children with ASD. Video model
attention skills including differential reinforce- consisted of a 5-year-old girl and an adult dyad,
ment and prompting procedures (Taylor & Hoch, and they demonstrate three components of joint
2008), video modeling (Rudy et al., 2014), and attention bids: (a) pointing and/or orienting
social scripts with fading (Pollard, Betz, & toward the object, (b) emitting a vocal statement
Higbee, 2012). (e.g., “Look, tree”), and (c) shifting eye gaze
The earliest form of joint attention, which from the object to the therapist and back to the
emerges between 9 and 18 months for typically object. Results of this study implied that using
developing children, is nonverbal joint attention. video modeling alone was effective in teaching
Nonverbal join attention involves shifting eye three components of joint attention bids to two
contact between a stimulus and a familiar person participants, whereas the one student was
(Adamson & Bakeman, 1984). Krstovska-­ required both video modeling and in vivo prompts
Guerrero and Jones (2015) investigated the to learn joint attention bids.
effects of a differential reinforcement and
prompting procedure to teach young children
(20–29 months) with ASD early forms of Intermediate Social Skills
responding to joint attention bids and initiating
joint attention bids. Joint attention skills can be Play is important for several aspects of child
generalized to similar conditions with the child’s development, including gross motor skills, coor-
mother and in novel contexts (e.g., responding to dination, and language development (Garvey,
name). Appropriate initiations and responses to 1990). Typically, children first explore and con-
bids for joint attention continued 3 months fol- tact their environment through play. However,
lowing the intervention. children with ASD often engage in repetitive and
Taylor and Hoch (2008) used a least-to-most ritualistic behaviors (Harrop, McConachie,
prompting procedure and natural consequences Emsley, Leadbitter, & Green, 2014) that can
(i.e., social attention) to teach three children with impede naturalistic play. Moreover, children with
ASD how to engage in three components of joint ASD could show difficulties in play activities due
attention: (a) gaze shift from an object to an adult, to the lack of motivation or understanding the
(b) vocal response to joint attention bids, and (c) basic rule of play (e.g., taking turns). They may
vocal initiations of joint attention bids. Following not frequently initiate conversations or socially
training on responding to joint attention bids, respond to others during play. Based on this
increase in gaze shift and vocal responses were necessity, three research-based teaching strate-
observed, but vocal initiations did not increase gies were generally used to promote play skills in
until explicitly taught. Thus, teaching responding children with ASD: (a) video modeling, (b)
to joint attention bids does not guarantee general- visual activity schedules, and (c) social scripts
ization to initiating joint attention bids. Due to and script fading.
the complexity of joint attention, it may be In a recent study, MacManus, MacDonald,
important to break down the component skills and Ahearn (2015) taught three children with
130 M.T. Brodhead et al.

ASD to engage in a variety of play responses tial study, Wichnick, Vener, Keating, and Poulson
with three different toy sets. The primary purpose (2010b) taught three children with ASD to initi-
of this study was to evaluate the effectiveness of ate play and conversation. Participants were pro-
video modeling and matrix training as a treat- vided with ten plastic bags filled with two small
ment package for increasing the generalized play toys. During treatment, seven of the bags also
behaviors of children with ASD. Researchers contained auditory scripts to prompt play initia-
found that the percentage of scripted and recom- tions (e.g., “Let’s share toys”). Participants
bined actions and vocalizations increased for all opened one of the bags, removed both toys, and
three participants after video modeling was intro- handed the second toy to a play partner. If the bag
duced. Additionally, they found that generaliza- contained a script, he/she also engaged in the
tion across toy sets occurred to some degree for scripted initiation. Before treatment, participants
all participants. rarely made initiations during play; however,
Visual activity schedules are considered as an once treatment was introduced, the number of
evidence-based practice for individuals with initiations increased and remained elevated after
ASD (Kight, Sartini, & Spriggs, 2015) that scripts were faded.
include a series of photos, images, and pictures, In a follow-up study, Wichnick, Vener, Pyrtek,
which can describe a sequence of skills or behav- and Poulson (2010a) taught participants to
iors. Initially, adults physically guide schedule respond to the initiations made by peers. The par-
following and rapidly fade prompts until the ticipants and procedures were the same as those
child can independently complete the schedule employed in the initial study. Participants were
(MacDuff, Krantz, & McClannahan, 1993). taught to engage in an appropriate response when
Activity schedules have been used to promote not another peer initiated play (e.g., “This is fun”).
only solitary play (Morrison, Sainato, Before treatment, participants rarely responded
Benchaaban, & Endo, 2002) but also peer play. to peer initiations; however, after treatment, the
For example, Brodhead, Higbee, Pollard, Akers, number of responses greatly increased. These
and Gerencser (2014) taught children with ASD responses were not limited to those specifically
to play hide-and-seek with peers using joint taught but also included novel responses.
activity schedules. Participants successfully Other researchers have also investigated the
engaged in both hider and seeker roles and also usefulness of a script training procedure to pro-
generalized hiding and seeking locations. mote play-based conversations (Groskreutz,
Children with ASD often struggle with initiat- Peters, Groskreutz, & Higbee, 2015). Three chil-
ing and maintaining back-and-forth conversa- dren with ASD were directly taught three script
tions (American Psychiatric Association [APA], frames to facilitate commenting about various
2013). Social scripting is another technology that play activities. The script frames were “I found
specifically targets increasing spontaneous vocal- the ____,” “I’m playing with the ____,” and
izations (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993). “Look at this ____.” During treatment sessions,
Children with ASD are taught to engage in 15 scripts (five of each type of script frame) were
scripted phrases that fit a specific context. Once placed on various components of the toy set. If
the child can readily engage in the scripted 30 s elapsed and the participant did not make a
responses, the scripts are systematically faded comment, the researcher prompted a scripted
until the child’s behavior comes under the control response. After script training was introduced,
of stimuli in the natural environment. The broader the number of unique play comments increased
goal of this intervention is for the child to engage for participants.
in novel phrases in addition to those that were In summary, targeting conversation and play
directly taught. skills with children with ASD is extremely
Across two studies, researchers taught three important, as one of the defining features of ASD
children with ASD to initiate conversation with is the deficit in social communication (APA,
peers and to respond to peer initiations. In the ini- 2013). Video modeling, visual activity schedules,
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 131

and social scripting are relatively simple inter- A similar approach was used to teach children
ventions that can be employed to address these with autism to detect to and respond to deceptive
skill deficits. These interventions promote inde- statements. Ranick, Persicke, Tarbox, and
pendence and generalization to novel responses, Kornack (2013) argued that responding to decep-
which can lead to more naturalistic social tive statements is important because it may
interactions. reduce the likelihood that individuals with autism
fall victim to bullying. Using MET in a treatment
package that also included rules, modeling, role-­
Complex Social Skills play, and feedback, Ranick et al. taught three
children with autism to question deceptive state-
Despite the support for behavioral interventions ments that occurred during play situations.
in the treatment of social behaviors, there is much Following training, all three participants demon-
less support for the use of such interventions in strated mastery of responding to deceptive state-
teaching complex social behaviors (Ranick, ments and were able to generalize that skill to
Persicke, Tarbox, & Kornack, 2013). However, novel examples of deception and their peers.
support for the use of behavioral principles in Using behavioral skills training (BST), which
teaching complex social skills is emerging involves instruction through modeling, instruc-
through the application of relational frame theory tions, rehearsal, and feedback in order to improve
(RFT), a post-Skinnerian approach to language the skills of a learner in a given situation,
and cognition development. RFT proposes that Miltenberger (2012) and Bergstrom, Najdowski,
an individual’s ability to “derive stimulus rela- Alvarado, and Tarbox (2016) taught three chil-
tions is learned behavior” (Hayes et al., 2001, dren to tell socially appropriate lies. The social
p. 22). That is, RFT advocates that the ability to skill of teaching appropriate lies is important
relate things to one another is learned behavior. because they allow an individual to avoid giving
Though most research on RFT has been con- away a surprise, keep a secret during a game, and
ducted on typically developing populations provide praise or support for a physical appear-
(Dymond, May, Munnelly, & Hoon, 2010), there ance that may be less than flattering (Bergstrom
is emerging support for the use of RFT to teach et al.). This intervention serves as another exam-
complex social behaviors to individuals with ple of an effective strategy to teach complex
autism. For example, given that individuals with social skills to individuals with autism.
autism have deficits in the ability to understand
verbal irony, Pexman et al. (2011) and Persicke,
Tarbox, Ranick, and St. Clair (2013) evaluated Communication
the effects of a training package to teach children
with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm. Communication deficits are characterized by
The training package included multiple exemplar delayed or atypical development in the area of
training (MET). MET involves training a behav- communication. Sturmey and Sevin (1994)
ior in the presence of multiple stimulus condi- observed that poor communication skills are at
tions in order to promote the generalization of the core of most autism definitions. Indeed, cur-
behavior (Rosales, Rehfeldt, & Lovett, 2011). In rent diagnostic criteria for ASD emphasize a pro-
this case, MET involves exposing the individual found impairment in verbal and nonverbal
with autism to multiple opportunities to recog- communication used for social interaction
nize and detect sarcasm. Instructor feedback was (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The
provided on the correctness of each opportunity degree of this communication disorder can vary
to respond to sarcasm. Following the study, all widely in individuals with ASD. Some children
three participants demonstrated mastery and acquire speech and language slowly during the
maintenance of the skill of responding to sarcasm preschool years; estimates are that up to 50%
for up to 3 months after treatment. can use phrased speech by the time they enter
132 M.T. Brodhead et al.

primary school (see Howlin, Magiati, & from automatic positive reinforcement (Sigafoos
Charman, 2009). Another portion of about et al., 2009). On the other hand, any indirectly
30–50% experience a severe lack in the develop- operated behavior that necessitates another per-
ment of speech and language by the time they son to mediate reinforcement is considered “ver-
enter kindergarten (National Research Council, bal.” Therefore, verbal behavior can take many
2001; Tager-­ Flusberg & Kasari, 2013). These forms of communicative modalities such as
individuals are often described as “nonverbal” or vocalizations, writing, gestures, manual signs,
only “minimally verbal” (Tager-Flusberg & exchanging pictures, or using a speech-­generating
Kasari, 2013). Interventions to promote commu- device. In more depth, verbal behavior consists of
nication, and speech and language in particular, several classes of verbal operants (see Sigafoos
therefore span a wider range of approaches et al., 2009, for more information).
including verbal and nonverbal communication Based on the verbal behavior framework,
modalities. The following is an outline of the communicative intervention aims at establishing
most common contemporary communication a growing repertoire of proper communicative
interventions that are consistent with a behav- forms that will be used as mands, tacts, echoics,
ioral paradigm of communication training and intraverbals, and autoclitics. Each of these verbal
based on empirically validated principles of operants should be taught through direct instruc-
learning. tion (Sundberg & Michael, 2001). Language
interventions derived from Skinner’s analysis of
verbal behavior show strong effectiveness for
 ehavioral Intervention Principles
B teaching the verbal operants outlined above.
Across Communication Modalities Skinner’s model emphasizes the function rather
than the form of spoken language and under-
To have the greatest benefit on the developmental scores the importance of context (LaFrance &
trajectory, speech and language intervention Miguel, 2014). By stressing function and context,
should be started as early as the child is identified this model provides clinicians with a viable tool
as having a difficulty; in addition, speech and lan- to teach and shape speech and language, espe-
guage training should be integrated into any other cially in cases where communicative repertoires
type of intervention program that the individual are severely limited.
is receiving.
Contemporary ABA-based approaches to
speech and language intervention originated I nterventions for the Verbal
from Skinner’s (1957) analysis of verbal behav- Individual
ior (Sigafoos, O’Reilly, Schlosser, & Lancioni,
2007). According to Skinner, verbal behavior is Behaviorally oriented speech and language pro-
reinforced by other people. For example, one can grams for children that have communicative
think of the direct act of opening a window (oper- speech typically target four major aspects of
ant behavior) to breathe fresh air (reinforcement); speech-language development (Sigafoos et al.
the verbal behavior equivalent (saying, “open the 2009). First, intervention often aims at increasing
window”) is only meaningful in the presence of a vocalizations and establishing imitative speech.
communication partner willing and able to react Second, a general objective is to enlarge the
to and reinforce this communicative act. Verbal learner’s vocabulary by (a) establishing new
behavior includes any type of response form that words and phrases, (b) enhancing the complexity
will effectively modify a communication part- of grammatical structures, and (c) developing the
ner’s behavior. Natural speech is not always learner’s conversational skills. Third, interven-
equivalent with verbal behavior, because speech tion typically targets using the newly acquired
is not automatically tied to the behavior of a lis- speech forms in more functional and spontaneous
tener. Echolalia, as an example, often results ways; related content goals may include (a)
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 133

establishing the spontaneous and generalized strategies that can be easily infused into their
abilities to mand and tact, (b) transforming echo- daily family schedule. During the treatment ses-
lalic utterances into more meaningful functional sions, certain variables are manipulated in a natu-
speech, and (c) sustaining verbal behavior in gen- ral language teaching context; for example, the
eral. Fourth, intervention aims to facilitate the clinician may use stimulus items that are func-
various pragmatic aspects of verbal behavior, tional and vary these, employ natural reinforcers,
such as developing better articulation, expanding and reinforce any communicative attempt. The
length of utterance, and fine-tuning prosody. It is major use of PRT has been for the acquisition of
critical to note that these four general interven- early, very specific language skills; PRT has also
tion objectives are not mutually exclusive, and been applied for increasing the frequency and
they are also not bound to any particular instruc- spontaneity of utterances (Williams & Marra,
tional sequence. Examples for evidence-based 2011).
communication intervention programs that are
firmly grounded in behavioral principles include
the following examples. I nterventions for the Minimally
Verbal Individual: Augmentative
Applied Verbal Behavior  Applied verbal and Alternative Communication
behavior (AVB) programs (e.g., Greer & Ross
2008) incorporate behavioral procedures to teach Individuals with ASD who do not develop suffi-
verbal operants. AVB programs have proven to cient natural speech or writing to meet their daily
be effective in teaching children with ASD to communication needs are candidates for inter-
acquire spoken words, produce questions, gener- vention in the area of augmentative and alterna-
ate four-­term sentences (verbs+colors+shape/ tive communication (AAC). Such individuals
size+labels), and respond to “what,” “how,” and may show only pre-intentional communication,
“why” questions (Williams & Marra, 2011). such as reaching for a desired item, or communi-
cation may show intent through behaviors such
Pivotal Response Training  Another interven- as pointing (Yoder, McCathren, Warren, &
tion program that has been used successfully to Watson, 2001). When speech does develop, it is
teach language to children on the autism spec- often limited to unusual or echolalic verbaliza-
trum is pivotal response training (PRT) (Koegel, tions (Paul, 2005).
Koegel, Harrower, & Carter, 1999). A “pivotal AAC is defined as the supplementation or
behavior” is considered one from which other replacement of natural speech and/or writing
behaviors originate. Based on principles of ABA using aided and/or unaided strategies.
blended with developmental approaches, PRT Blissymbols, pictographs, Sigsymbols, tangible
targets pivotal behaviors related to motivation, symbols, and electronically produced speech are
responsivity to multiple cues, self-management, examples of aided AAC. Manual signs, gestures,
and self-­initiations. Creating a focus on these and body language are examples of unaided
pivotal skills leads to ancillary gains in untar- AAC. The use of aided symbols requires a trans-
geted areas and ideally to generalized long-last- mission device, whereas the use of unaided sym-
ing improvements in language, behavior, and bols requires only the body (Lloyd, Fuller, &
social outcomes. The comprehensive PRT pro- Arvidson, 1997). Major types of AAC interven-
gram emphasizes consistent and coordinated pro- tion for individuals on the autism spectrum
gramming across the child’s environments, include the following approaches.
including parents as much as possible (Park,
2013). Parents take on a major role in the treat- Manual Signs, Gestures, and Total
ment process and receive in-depth PRT training. Communication  Manual signing was one of the
This partnership model has parents outline clini- first AAC strategies used with minimally verbal
cally important treatment goals and intervention individuals with autism (Schlosser & Wendt,
134 M.T. Brodhead et al.

2008). It was first trialed in the 1970s and has training, clinicians should be cautious about
been applied successfully with this population motor skill requirements on the individual with
for over 30 years. The term manual signs can ASD. Possible motor apraxia may impede
indicate a natural sign language (e.g., American acquisition and production of a manual sign rep-
Sign Language aka ASL) or refer to the genera- ertoire (Hilton, Zhang, White, Klohr, &
tion of manual signs as a code for a spoken lan- Constantino, 2012; Isenhower et al., 2012).
guage (Blischak, Lloyd, & Fuller, 1997). Related research suggests that manual signing
Gestures are body movements or coordinated can be part of a multimodal AAC system for
sequences of motor responses to represent an individuals with ASD but that it should not be
object, idea, action, or relationship omitting the the only means of communication. A combina-
linguistic features of manual signs. Examples for tion with other forms of AAC seems more ben-
gestures include pointing or yes-no headshakes. eficial (Williams & Marra).
Using gestures is a nonlinguistic form of unaided
communication that develops early in life. Before Graphic Symbol Sets and Systems  Compared
the start of linguistic development, infants typi- to manual signing, graphic symbols are a some-
cally use gestures in symbol formation when what newer AAC mode for individuals with
communicating and interacting with communica- ASD. During the 1980s clinicians started to
tion partners (Loncke & Bos, 1997). embrace the potential benefits of graphic sym-
Consequently, gestural development is an impor- bols because of their non-transient nature (e.g.,
tant precursor to later development of language Mirenda & Schuler, 1988). Graphic symbols can
skills (Morford & Goldin-Meadow, 1992). be organized as sets or systems. Sets represent
Individuals with ASD, however, rarely develop collections of symbols that do not have defined
gestural use as an alternative communication rules for their creation and expansion, while sys-
strategy by themselves and need concerted inter- tems have an established rule repertoire (see
vention to acquire this skill (Loveland, Landry, Lloyd et al., 1997). Graphic symbols most often
Hughes, Hall, & McEvoy, 1988). used in ASD include PCS, line drawings, colored
By the mid-1980s, an AAC approach photographs, and Premack (all sets) and blissym-
emerged that combined manual signing with bols, orthography, and rebus (all systems)
speech. This procedure is labeled as “total” or (Schlosser & Wendt, 2008). Graphic symbol sets
“simultaneous” communication (Mirenda & and systems that are more iconic in nature (i.e.,
Erickson, 2000). Total communication empha- they demonstrate greater visual resemblance
sizes the use of the most appropriate communi- between symbol and referent) appear to be easier
cation strategy for the individual and is typically to learn (Kozleski, 1991).
an adaptation of ASL. Research reports indicate Research suggests that graphic symbols are
increases in initiating communicative acts after most effective for targeting mand skills (Schlosser
manual sign training in children with ASD & Wendt, 2008). Beyond manding, graphic sym-
(Goldstein, 2002). Other studies showed bols may be helpful as visual supports to facili-
improvements in early vocalizations or spoken tate transitioning activities (Dettmer, Simpson,
words, while some research suggests limited Myles, & Ganz, 2000). Yet, the research base on
productive use of manuals signs when taught to graphic symbols has not reached a critical mass
individuals with ASD. The majority of research to draw conclusions whether one graphic symbol
does not support language progress beyond a set/system may be preferable over others.
few words when implementing manual signs;
however, the total communication approach Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs)  SGDs
shows promising effects particularly for those are another viable option for minimally verbal
children with autism who are minimally verbal individuals with autism. SGDs include dedi-
­
and have poor verbal imitation (Williams & cated electronic communication devices, talk-
Marra, 2011). When implementing manual sign ing word processors, and handheld multipurpose
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 135

mobile devices (e.g., iPad®, iPod®, Android® Instructional Approaches


tablets) equipped with AAC applications (apps). for Augmentative and Alternative
All of these have built-in technology that allows Communication in Autism
a user to communicate via digitized and/or syn-
thetic speech. Digitized speech is generated by To maximize the effects of AAC intervention, it
recording a human voice and converting it into is critical to put an effective instructional
an electronic waveform. The quality of digitized approach around the provision of AAC technol-
speech depends on the sampling rate used dur- ogy. Merely equipping the learner with AAC
ing the conversion process. SGDs and apps that materials or devices will not automatically lead
apply higher sampling rates in general generate to improved communication. Incorporating
higher-­quality speech output compared to those behavioral learning principles into AAC inter-
that rely on lower sampling rates. Recording vention can be a powerful tool to create a proper
quality may also be impeded by noisy environ- instructional framework. Examples for evidence-­
ments, equipment quality, speaker age, and based approaches are the picture exchange com-
quality of the speaker’s natural voice (Drager & munication system and matrix training.
Finke, 2012).
Synthetic speech is generated by a text-to-­ Picture Exchange Communication System
speech algorithm built within the device that (PECS)  The PECS has attained widespread
allows to produce an unlimited amount of sponta- use and popularity in the autism field during
neous speech by converting alphabets, digits, the last 20 years (Bondy & Frost, 1994). PECS
words, and sentences into speech output. is a manualized treatment for beginning com-
Intelligibility of high-quality text-to-speech municators that uses behavioral strategies and
engines can approach that of natural speech. a series of training phases to teach the use of
Variables that influence synthetic speech quality graphic symbol cards for spontaneous commu-
include listening conditions, experience, and nication with others. PECS involves six phases.
adjustment to the nature of synthetic speech and In phase I: physical exchange, learners are
the particular listening tasks (Schlosser & Koul, taught to exchange a graphic symbol for a
2015). desired object. In phase II: expanding sponta-
Research into the effects of SGDs for mini- neity, learners are taught to exchange a symbol
mally verbal individuals with ASD has evolved with different communication partner across
much later than for other AAC options (e.g., increasing distances. In phase III: picture dis-
manual signing). Recently there has been crimination, the task for the learner is to dis-
increased research activity in this area, especially criminate among symbols for requesting.
on the use of mobile technologies with AAC-­ Consequently, in phase IV: sentence structure,
specific apps; this is not an unsurprising trend in the learner is instructed to attach an “I want”
light of the current impact of mobile technologies symbol to a blank sentence strip, followed by
on the AAC field (e.g., McNaughton & Light, the symbol for a desired item, and to exchange
2013). The majority of studies document benefits the sentence strip with a communication part-
from SGDs when these are used as part of treat- ner. In phase V: responding to “What do you
ment packages to target requesting skills or chal- want?,” the learner is required to respond
lenging behaviors (Schlosser & Koul, 2015). immediately to a question prompt. Finally,
Some research has started to document effects on phase VI: responsive and spontaneous com-
natural speech production and social-­menting uses the acquired skills to develop
communicative behaviors in individuals with responses to further questions (i.e., “What do
ASD (Kasari et al., 2014; Boesch, Wendt, you see?”) and spontaneous commenting
Subramanian, & Hsu, 2013), but further investi- (Bondy & Frost, 2001). Reviews of the PECS
gations are warranted to draw more definite con- intervention literature indicate that the
clusions for clinical practice. approach is successful in teaching initial com-
136 M.T. Brodhead et al.

munication skills and in some cases facilitated  estricted, Repetitive Behaviors


R
spoken language acquisition (Ganz, Davis, and Interests
Lund, Goodwyn, & Simpson, 2012; Williams
& Marra, 2011). For example, in a sample of The final core symptom of ASD is restricted or
66 participants who received PECS interven- repetitive behaviors, interests, or activities
tion for over a year, 39 (59%) developed natu- (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). To
ral speech as a primary communication mode meet the DSM-V diagnostic criteria for ASD, an
(Brunner & Seung, 2009). individual must present with or have a history of
at least two categories of restricted, repetitive
Matrix Training  Matrix training is a proper behaviors and interests (RRBI). RRBI categories
choice of instruction when learners possess an include (a) stereotyped or repetitive motor move-
initial core lexicon of 40–50 symbols and begin ments, use of objects, or vocalizations (e.g., hand
to create symbol combinations. Matrix training flapping, body rocking, lining up objects, echola-
can be implemented by using symbols in an lia or repetitive speech); (b) insistence on same-
AAC context (e.g., manual signs or graphic ness, strict adherence to routines, or ritualistic
symbols) or by using spoken words. Matrix verbal or nonverbal behavior (e.g., major distress
strategies use linguistic elements (e.g., nouns, related to changes in routines, transitions); (c)
verbs, etc.) presented in systematic combination highly circumscribed or perseverative interests
matrices, which are arranged to induce general- (e.g., intense focus or attachment to unusual
ized rule-like behavior. The learner is taught to objects, topics, or interests); or (d) hyper- or
combine a limited set of symbols in one seman- hyposensitivity to sensory input (e.g., indiffer-
tic category with another set in a related seman- ence to pain, excessive sniffing of objects, licking
tic category to facilitate the acquisition of objects, covering ears) (American Psychiatric
generalized combining of lexical items (Nelson, Association, 2013). RRBI in individuals with
1993). For example, a 2×2 matrix can be ASD is heterogeneous and can take the form of a
designed with two colors on one axis and two variety of motor, vocal, and ritualistic behaviors,
objects on the other axis, allowing four different which vary greatly in terms of form, frequency,
color-object combinations. If two of the four and intensity. To assist in examining the etiology,
combinations are taught, the learner may be able trajectory, and treatment of RRBI, researchers
to generalize the skill to the untaught combina- have factored RRBI into two subgroups: lower-­
tions. For example, if a child is taught to label order and higher-order RRBI (Turner, 1999).
“yellow apple” and “red pear,” the combina- Lower-order RRBI are characterized by repet-
tions “yellow pear” and “red apple” may emerge itive motor movements, vocalizations, or object
without direct instruction, a process that is manipulation, while higher-order RRBI relates to
known as “recombinative generalization” insistence on sameness, circumscribed persever-
(Goldstein, 1983). Clinical research indicates ative interests, and rigid adherence to rules or
that matrix training is effective in teaching rituals (Boyd, McDonough, , & Bodfish, 2012;
action-­object, graphic symbol combinations on Patterson, Smith, & Jelen, 2010).
a communication board for individuals with
developmental disabilities including ASD
(Nigam, Schlosser, & Lloyd, 2006). Successful RRBI Treatment Overview
intervention results have also been reported for
teaching spelling, play-based behaviors, and RRBI have been reported to be the most difficult
enhanced expressive and receptive communica- aspect of ASD for parents to manage (Bishop,
tion skills, although mostly for participants with Richler, Cain, & Lord, 2007) and present unique
developmental delay and/or intellectual disabil- challenges for treatment. RRBI can negatively
ity (Chae & Wendt, 2012). impact socialization (Watt et al., 2008), reduce
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 137

access to and benefit from instruction, lead to initiation instruction, and self-monitoring. As
stigmatization (Cunningham & Shriebman, social interaction skills improved, motor stereo-
2008), and contribute to placement in restrictive typy decreased.
settings (Boyd et al., 2012; Green et al., 2007;
Honey, Rodgers, & McConachie, 2012). Not Noncontingent Reinforcement (NCR)  NCR
only do RRBI interfere with learning and social involves systematically providing access to the
development, but interruption or blocking of maintaining consequence of challenging behav-
RRBI has been shown to evoke additional chal- ior on a fixed time-based schedule. For lower-­
lenging behaviors including tantrums, aggres- order RRBI maintained by automatic
sion, and self-injury (e.g., Hagopian & Adelinis, reinforcement, NCR often involves access to
2001). The best treatment outcomes for RRBI stimulation matched to the properties of the
result from behavioral treatments based on the RRBI (Piazza et al., 2000). For example, Rapp
principles of operant conditioning (e.g., Boyd (2007) provided two boys with ASD who
et al., 2012; Patterson, Smith, & Jelen, 2010). engaged in repetitive vocalizations noncontin-
Behavioral treatments can be classified as ante- gent access to music and found repetitive lan-
cedent interventions, consequence interventions, guage decreased. Ahearn, Clark, DeBar, and
or combinations of antecedent and consequence Florentino (2005) found that access to highly
interventions. preferred toys that did not match the properties of
the children’s RRBI also decreased RRBI. This
suggests that if the properties of RRBI are not
Antecedent Treatments able to be identified or matched to the sources of
stimulation (toys, music, etc.), then noncontin-
Antecedent treatments prevent RRBI through gent access to highly preferred toys may be effec-
altering the environment, providing competing tive in reducing lower-order RRBI. Other
sources of stimulation and reinforcement, and research has shown that NCR is more effective in
reducing of motivation to engage in reducing lower-order RRBI when paired with
RRBI. Antecedent interventions often consist of consequence-based interventions (e.g., blocking
focused intervention practices (Odom et al., RRBI) (Patterson et al., 2010).
2009) which can be implemented in isolation or
as part of a larger, comprehensive treatment Environmental Enrichment (EE)  EE is simi-
package. Focused antecedent interventions for lar to NCR in that access to stimulation is pro-
lower-order RRBI include teaching new skills or vided irrespective of the individual’s engagement
behaviors, noncontingent reinforcement (NCR), in RRBI (e.g., Vollmer, Marcus & LeBlanc,
environmental enrichment, antecedent exercise, 1994). EE involves increasing the quantity or
presession access to RRBI, and discrimination quality of reinforces within a setting. EE is effec-
training. tive at reducing RRBI when the reinforcing prop-
erties of the environment successfully compete
Teaching New Skills  It has been posited that with and become more valued than the automatic
one reason individuals engage in RRBI is because reinforcement obtained through RRBI (Rapp &
they lack adaptive behaviors to access reinforce- Vollmer, 2005). For example, a teacher could
ment (Boyd et al., 2012). By teaching appropriate conduct a preference assessment to identify a
leisure, play, and social interaction skills, child’s preferred toys and activities (see Karsten,
researchers have shown that RRBI can decrease Carr, & Lapper, 2011, for guidelines for selecting
(Lanovaz, Robertson, Soerono, & Watkins, an appropriate preference assessment). He or she
2013). Loftin, Odom, and Lantz (2008) taught could then identify times during the day when the
social interaction skills to three children with child is likely to engage in lower-order RRBI and
ASD. Intervention involved peer training, social present the preferred toys and activities to the
138 M.T. Brodhead et al.

child during those times. Vollmer et al. (1994) card, he could engage freely in RRBI. However
found that when children with ASD were given in the presence of a red card, RRBI was physi-
access to their preferred items, lower-order RRBI cally interrupted (e.g., manual guidance of his
decreased. However, for some individuals, EE hands back to his lap, removal of preferred stim-
may actually lead to an increase in vocal stereo- uli in the presence of repetitive vocalizations).
typy. Van Camp et al. (2000) observed rates of When the child did not engage in RRBI in the
hand flapping in a young boy and found that hand presence of the red card, the researchers pre-
flapping was higher in the presence of preferred sented the green card, signaling that the RRBI
toys than when the child was alone without envi- would not be interrupted. Thus, this intervention
ronmental stimulation. Given this variability in involved a stimulus control procedure and rein-
outcomes, it is important to evaluate the effects of forcement of the absence of RRBI with subse-
environmental enrichment on RRBI for the indi- quent access to RRBI. This procedure was then
vidual prior to adopting the intervention whole generalized to the child’s school and community
scale. environments.

Presession Access  Presession access to RRBI Embedding Interests  Higher-order RRBI


may also function to reduce the value of auto- involve perseverative or circumscribed interests
matic reinforcement (e.g., Rapp & Vollmer, 2005; and insistence on sameness. As such, antecedent
Rispoli et al., 2011). Lang et al. (2010) evaluated interventions are designed to expand interests
presession access as a treatment for repetitive and enhance flexibility. One approach with
object manipulation in four young children with emerging research support is to capitalize on per-
ASD. When the children were given unrestricted severative interests through a strength-based
access to engage in object manipulation immedi- approach. Perseverative interests have been
ately prior to a play intervention, they engaged in embedded into instruction via instructional mate-
lower levels of object manipulation and increased rials (Adams, 1998), social interaction topics
levels of functional play during the subsequent (Baker, Koegel, & Koegel, 1998), and play
play intervention session. Practitioners interested (Boyd, Conroy, Mancil, Nakao, & Alter, 2007).
in preventing subsequent engagement in lower- For example, Baker et al. compared the effects of
order RRBI may schedule periods of unrestricted perseverative interest embedded in social games
access to engage in RRBI prior to target tasks or on the social interactions with three children with
activities. ASD. The children’s restricted interests included
facts about the United States, Disney characters,
Stimulus Control  Related to scheduling oppor- and children’s movies. When the socially appro-
tunities for individuals with ASD to engage in priate games were centered on the child’s perse-
RRBI, research has shown that the use of visual verative interest (such as playing tag on a giant
and verbal cues can assist in signaling to the indi- map of the outline of the United States), social
vidual when it is appropriate to engage in RRBI interaction increased for all three participants and
and when it is inappropriate (Conroy, Asmus & generalized to social interactions with topics out-
Sellers, 2005). Such an intervention involves side of perseverative interests.
bringing RRBI under the control of a specific
stimulus and then only presenting that stimulus Functional Communication Training
when it is acceptable to engage in RRBI. For (FCT)  FCT has shown promise in the treatment
example, O’Connor, Prieto, Hoffmann, of challenging behavior associated with ritual
DeQuinzio, and Taylor (2011) evaluated a stimu- interruption (e.g., Kuhn, Hardesty, & Sweeney,
lus control procedure on repetitive motor and 2009). Rispoli et al. (2014) evaluated FCT to
vocal behaviors in an 11-year-old boy with teach three young boys with ASD and higher-
ASD. Through discrimination training, they order RRBI to appropriately request access to
taught the child that in the presence of a green their preferred ritual. When the children requested
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 139

appropriately, they were permitted to engage in Dittcher, 2014). Response interruption and redi-
the ritual. When they engaged in challenging rection involves manually preventing repetitive
behavior, the ritual was terminated. To reduce the motor movements or verbally interrupting repeti-
time spent engaged in the ritual, the participants tive vocalizations (Ahearn et al., 2007). Response
were taught to only request access to the ritual blocking is related to response interruption, but
once a visual timer had elapsed. Using this sig- rather than blocking the RRBI itself, access to
naled delay to reinforcement, the children were reinforcement obtained by the RRBI is prevented
able to tolerate interruption of rituals for up to (Rapp & Vollmer, 2005). This is referred to as
1 min. For one child, results generalized occurred sensory extinction. Rincover, Newsom, and Carr
across rituals. (1979) hypothesized that a child who spun plates
on hard surfaces was doing so to hear the sound
the plate made when it clattered against the table.
Consequence-Based Treatments The researchers covered the table with a soft
material to block the sound of the plate spinning.
Differential Reinforcement (DR)  DR proce- With this sensory extinction procedure, plate
dures involve providing poorer quality or less spinning decreased, and the authors were able to
quantity of reinforcement for RRBI and greater introduce new appropriate play skills which
quality and quantity of reinforcement for either allowed the child to access similar auditory
appropriate behaviors or the absence of RRBI. A reinforcement.
target criterion for RRBI or a replacement Although punishment procedures for RRBI
response is established and systematically rein- are common in practice, they have not been
forced. Taylor, Hoch, and Weissman (2005) shown to be effective in maintaining reductions
implemented a differential reinforcement proce- over RRBI over time (Cunningham &
dure with a 6-year-­ old girl with ASD who Schreibman, 2008) and may serve to increase
engaged in vocal stereotypy. The teacher told the RRBI when punishment procedures are not in
child that if she played quietly until the timer place (Rapp & Vollmer, 2005). There are also
rang, she could have access to musical toys (toys ethical concerns that arise when implementing
matched to the reinforcing properties of vocal punishment procedures, and such procedures can
stereotypy). A card with the word “Quiet” and a lead to aggressive behaviors or instructional envi-
digital timer set and placed in view of the child. ronments taking on aversive characteristics (Rapp
Results showed that when the differential rein- & Vollmer). It is recommended that punishment
forcement procedure was in place, vocal stereo- procedure be incorporated into treatment pack-
typy decreased. Differential reinforcement can ages that include antecedent and reinforcement
also be used to increase variability of behaviors procedures.
(Miller & Neuringer, 2000). For example, a child
who engages with toys in the same manner each
day would be reinforced for engaging with toys Summary
in different ways (Boyd, McDonough, Rupp,
Khan, & Bodfish, 2011). The above sections outline strategies for the
treatment of core symptoms of ASD. Though the
Punishment Procedures  Punishment proce- type of treatment differs depending on the needs
dures for RRBI include response cost, response of each learner, one commonality between treat-
interruption and redirection, and sensory extinc- ment options is that effective treatments are based
tion. Response cost involves removing access to on the principles of behavior analysis. To date, no
a desired object or activity contingent upon other forms of treatment have reported the posi-
RRBI. For example, a child who is earning tokens tive results obtained by behavioral interventions
to exchange for a preferred activity may lose a for individuals with ASD. Because there are a
token each time she engages in RRBI (Lapime & number of alternative treatments for ASD (see
140 M.T. Brodhead et al.

Foxx & Mulick, 2016), it is important for con- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42,
2006–2012.
sumers to appraise alternative treatments, as
Blischak, D. M., Lloyd, L. L., & Fuller, D. R. (1997).
some treatments may be harmful or Unaided AAC symbols. In L. L. Lloyd, D. R. Fuller,
­counterproductive to treatment goals (Brodhead, & H. H. Arvidson (Eds.), Augmentative and alterna-
2015). Finally, when implementing behavior- tive communication: A handbook of principles and
practices (pp. 38–42). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn
analytic treatments for individuals with ASD, it
and Bacon.
is of great importance that treatment is designed, Bodfish, J. W. (2003). Interview for repetitive behav-
implemented, and supervised by qualified profes- iors. Unpublished rating scale. University of North
sionals. In some cases, the input of multiple pro- Carolina, Chapel Hill, NC.
Boesch, M. C., Wendt, O., Subramanian, A., & Hsu, N.
fessionals may be necessary, as treatment of
(2013). Comparative efficacy of the Picture Exchange
social skills, communication, and repetitive Communication System (PECS) versus a speech-­
behaviors are separate areas of expertise. Failure generating device: Effects on social-communicative
to provide proper oversight of the implementa- skills and speech development. Augmentative and
Alternative Communication, 29, 197–209. doi:10.310
tion of behavioral procedures may result in less
9/07434618.2013.818059.
than adequate treatment of core symptoms of Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (1994). The picture exchange com-
ASD. munication system. Focus on Autistic Behavior, 9,
1–19. doi:10.1177/108835769400900301.
Bondy, A., & Frost, L. (2001). The picture exchange com-
munication system. Behavior Modification, 25, 725–
References 744. doi:10.1177/0145445501255004.
Boyd, B. A., Conroy, M. A., Mancil, G. R., Nakao, T., &
Adams, L. W. (1998). Incorporating narrow interests into Alter, P. J. (2007). Effects of circumscribed interests
school tasks of children with autism. (Doctoral dis- on the social behaviors of children with autism spec-
sertation). Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertation and trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Theses database (Order No. 9943180). Disorders, 37, 1550–1561. doi:10.1007/
Adamson, L. B., & Bakeman, R. (1984). Mothers’ com- s10803-006-0286-8.
municative acts: Changes during infancy. Infant Boyd, B. A., McDonough, S. G., & Bodfish, J. W.
Behavior & Development, 7, 467–478. doi:10.1016/ (2012). Evidence-based behavioral interventions
S0163-6383(84)80006-5. for repetitive behaviors in autism. Journal of Autism
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., DeBar, R., & Florentino, and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1236–1248.
C. (2005). On the role of preference in response com- doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1284-z.
petition. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, Boyd, B. A., McDonough, S. G., Rupp, B., Khan, F., &
247–250. doi:10.1901/jaba.2005.36-04. Bodfish, J. (2011). Effects of a family-implemented
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. F., & Chung, treatment on the repetitive behaviors of children
B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal stereotypy with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Disorders, 41, 1330–1341. doi:10.1007/
Analysis, 40, 263–275. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.30-06. s10803-010-1156-y.
Aman, M. G., Singh, N. N., Stewart, A. W., & Field, Brodhead, M. T. (2015). Maintaining professional rela-
C. J. (1985). Psychometric characteristics of the aber- tionships in an interdisciplinary setting: Strategies
rant behavior checklist. American Journal of Mental for navigating nonbehavioral treatment recommenda-
Deficiency, 89, 492–502. tions for individuals with autism. Behavior Analysis in
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic Practice, 8, 70–78. doi:10.1007/s40617-015-0042-7.
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Brodhead, M. T., Higbee, T. S., Pollard, J. S., Akers, J. S.,
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. & Gerencser, K. R. (2014). The use of linked activity
Baker, M. J., Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1998). schedules to teach children with autism to play hide-­
Increasing the social behavior of young children with and-­seek. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47,
autism using their obsessive behaviors. Journal of the 645–650. doi:10.1002/jaba.145.
Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 23, Brunner, D. L., & Seung, H. (2009). Evaluation of
300–308. doi:10.2511/rpsd.23.4.300. the efficacy of communication-based treatments
Bergstrom, R., Najdowski, A. C., Alvarado, M., & for autism spectrum disorders: A literature review.
Tarbox, J. (2016). Teaching children with autism Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31, 15–41.
to tell socially appropriate lies. Journal of Applied doi:10.1177/1525740108324097.
Behavior Analysis. doi:10.1002/jaba.295. Carbone, V. J., O’Brien, L., Sweeney-Kerwin, E. J., &
Bishop, S. L., Richler, J., Cain, A., & Lord, C. (2007). Albert, K. M. (2013). Teaching eye contact to chil-
Brief report: Repetitive behaviors in young children dren with autism: A conceptual analysis of and single
with autism spectrum disorder and developmen- case study. Education and Treatment of Children, 36,
tally similar peer: A follow up to Watt e al. (2008). 139–159. doi:10.1353/etc.2013.0013.
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 141

Chae, S., & Wendt, O. (2012). The effects of matrix strat- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32,
egy intervention on improving word combination 373–395.
skills in preschool children with intellectual disabili- Green, V. A., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., Pituch, K. A.,
ties. Evidence-based Practice Briefs, 6, 57–65. Didden, R., Lancioni, G. E., & Singh, N. N. (2007).
Charlop-Christy, M. H., Carpenter, M., Le, L., LeBlanc, Behavioral flexibility in individuals with autism:
L. A., & Kellet, K. (2002). Using the picture exchange Theory, assessment, and intervention. In L. B.
communication system (PECS) with children with Zhao (Ed.), Autism research advances (pp. 63–77).
autism: Assessment of PECS acquisition, speech, New York, NY: Nova Science.
social-communicative behavior, and problem behav- Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2007). Verbal behavior
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 2123– analysis: Including and expending new verbal capa-
2231. doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-2. bilities in children with language delays. Boston, MA:
Conroy, M. A., Asmus, J. M., & Sellers, J. A. (2005). Pearson.
The use of an antecedent-based intervention to Greer, D. R., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior
decrease stereotypic behavior in a general education analysis: Inducing and expanding new verbal capa-
classroom: A case study. Focus on Autism and Other bilities in children with language delays. Boston, MA:
Developmental Disabilities, 20, 223–230. doi:10.117 Pearson Education.
7/10883576050200040401. Groskreutz, M. P., Peters, A., Groskreutz, N. C., & Higbee,
Cunningham, A. B., & Shreibman, L. (2008). Stereotypy T. S. (2015). Increasing play-based commenting in
in autism: The importance of function. Research in children with autism spectrum disorder using a novel
Autism Spectrum Disorder, 2, 469–479. doi:10.1016/j. script-frame procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior
rasd.2007.09.006. Analysis, 48, 442–447. doi:10.1002/jaba.194.
Dettmer, S., Simpson, R. L., Smith Myles, B., & Ganz, Hagopian, L. P., & Adelinis, J. D. (2001). Response
J. (2000). The use of visual supports to facili- blocking with and without redirection for the treat-
tate transitions of students with autism. Focus on ment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 15, 34, 527–530. doi:10.1901/jaba.2001.34-527.
163–169. Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003).
Drager, K. D., & Finke, E. H. (2012). Intelligibility of Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review.
children’s speech in digitized speech. Augmentative Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185.
and Alternative Communication, 28, 181–189. doi:10. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147.
3109/07434618.2012.704524. Harrop, C., McConachie, H., Emsley, R., Leadbitter, K.,
Dymond, S., May, R. J., Munnelly, A., & Hoon, A. E. Green, J., & The PACT Consortium (2014). Restricted
(2010). Evaluating the evidence base for relational and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disor-
frame theory: A citation analysis. The Behavior ders and typical development: Cross-sectional and
Analyst, 33, 97–117. longitudinal comparisons. Journal of Autism and
Foxx, R. M., & Azrin, N. H. (1973). The elimination Developmental Disorders, 44, 1207–1219.
of autistic self-stimulatory behavior by overcorrec- Hayes, S. C., Fox, E., Gifford, E. V., Wilson, K. G.,
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 1–14. Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived rela-
doi:10.1901/jaba.1973.6-1. tional responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes,
Foxx, R. M., & Mulick, J. A. (Eds.). (2016). Controversial D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational
therapies for autism and intellectual disabilities: frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human
Fad, fashion, and science in professional practice. language and cognition (pp. 21–49). New York, NY:
New York, NY: Routledge. Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
Frost, L. A., & Bondy, A. S. (1994). The picture exchange Hilton, C. L., Zhang, Y., White, M. R., Klohr, C. L.,
communication system training manual. Cherry Hill, & Constantino, J. (2012). Motor impairment
NJ: Pyramid Educational Consultants. in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for
Ganz, J. B., Davis, J. L., Lund, E. M., Goodwyn, F. D., autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 16, 430–441.
& Simpson, R. L. (2012). Meta-analysis of PECS doi:10.1177/1362361311423018.
with individuals with ASD: Investigation of tar- Honey, E., Rodgers, J., & McConachie, H. (2012).
geted versus non-targeted outcomes, participant Measurement of restricted and repetitive behaviour
characteristics, and implementation phase. Research in children with autism spectrum disorder: Selecting
in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 406–418. a questionnaire or interview. Research in Autism
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.09.023. Spectrum Disorders, 6, 757–776. doi:10.1016/j.
Garvey, C. (1990). Play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard rasd.2011.10.011.
University Press. Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic
Goldstein, H. (1983). Recombinative generalization: review of early intensive behavioral interventions for
Relationships between environmental conditions and children with autism. American Journal of Intellectual
the linguistic repertoires of language learners. Analysis Developmental disabilities, 114, 23–41. doi:10.1352/
and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 3, 2009.114:23;nd41.
279–293. doi:10.1016/0270-4684(83)90002-2. Isenhower, R. W., Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Helt,
Goldstein, H. (2002). Communication intervention for M., Schmidt, R. C., & Fein, D. (2012). Rhythmic
children with autism: A review of treatment efficacy. bimanual coordination is impaired in young chil-
142 M.T. Brodhead et al.

dren with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Laprime, A. P., & Dittrich, G. A. (2014). An evaluation of
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 25–31. doi:10.1016/j. a treatment package consisting of discrimination train-
rasd.2011.08.005. ing and differential reinforcement with response cost
Karsten, A. M., Carr, J. E., & Lepper, T. L. (2011). and a social story on vocal stereotypy for a preschooler
Description of a practitioner model for identifying with autism in a preschool classroom. Education and
preferred stimuli with individuals with autism spec- Treatment of Children, 37, 407–430. doi:10.1353/
trum disorders. Behavior Modification, 35, 347–369. etc.2014.0028.
doi: 10.1177/0145445511405184. Lloyd, L. L., Fuller, D. R., & Arvidson, H. H. (Eds.).
Kasari, C., Kaiser, A., Goods, K., Nietfeld, J., Mathy, P., (1997). Augmentative and alternative communication:
Landa, R., … Almirall, D. (2014). Communication A handbook of principles and practices. Needham
interventions for minimally verbal children with Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
autism: A sequential multiple assignment randomized Loftin, R. L., Odom, S. L., & Lantz, J. F. (2008). Social
trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and interaction and repetitive motor behaviors. Journal of
Adolescent Psychiatry, 53, 635–646. doi:10.1016/j. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1124–1135.
jaac.2014.01.019. Loncke, F., & Bos, H. (1997). Unaided AAC symbols. In
Knight, V., Sartini, E., & Spriggs, A. D. (2015). Evaluating L. L. Lloyd, D. R. Fuller, & H. H. Arvidson (Eds.),
visual activity schedules as evidence-based prac- Augmentative and alternative communication: A
tice for individuals with autism spectrum disorders. handbook of principles and practices (pp. 80–106).
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
157–178. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2201-z. Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & children. The “me” book. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Carter, C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention Loveland, K. A., Landry, S. H., Hughes, S. O., Hall,
I: Overview of approach. Research and Practice S. K., & McEvoy, R. E. (1988). Speech acts and
for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 24, 174–185. the pragmatic deficits of autism. Journal of Speech
doi:10.2511/rpsd.24.3.174. and Hearing Research, 31, 593–604. doi:10.1044/
Kozleski, E. (1991). Visual symbol acquisition by stu- jshr.3104.593.
dents with autism. Exceptionality, 2, 173–194. MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E.
doi:10.1080/09362839109524782. (1993). Teaching children with autism to use pho-
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching tographic activity schedules: Maintenance and gen-
children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a eralization of complex response chains. Journal of
script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 89–97. doi:10.1901/
Analysis, 26, 121–132. doi:10.1901/jaba.1993.26-12. jaba.1993.26-89.
Krstovska-Guerrero, I., & Jones, E. A. (2015). Social-­ MacManus, C., MacDonald, R., & Ahearn, W. H. (2015).
communication intervention for toddlers with autism Teaching and generalizing pretend play in children
spectrum disorder: Eye gaze in the context of request- with autism using video modeling and matrix training.
ing and joint attention. Journal of Developmental Behavioral Interventions, 30, 191–218. doi:10.1002/
and Physical Disabilities, 27, 1–18. doi:10.1007/ bin.1406.
s10882-015-9466-9. McNaughton, D., & Light, J. (2013). The iPad and mobile
Kuhn, D. E., Hardesty, S. L., & Sweeney, N. M. (2009). technology revolution: Benefits and challenges for
Assessment and treatment of excessive straightening individuals who require augmentative and alterna-
and destructive behavior in an adolescent diagnosed tive communication. Augmentative and Alternative
with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, Communication, 29, 107–116. doi:10.3109/0743461
355–360. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-355. 8.2013.784930.
LaFrance, D. L., & Miguel, C. F. (2014). Teaching verbal Miller, N., & Neuringer, A. (2000). Reinforcing vari-
behavior to children with autism spectrum disorders. ability in adolescents with autism. Journal of Applied
In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & J. L. Matson Behavior Analysis, 33, 151–165. doi:10.1901/
(Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for autism jaba.2000.33-151.
spectrum disorders (pp. 403–436). New York, NY: Miltenberger, R. (2012). Behavior modification (5th ed.).
Springer. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Machalicek, W., Mirenda, P., & Erickson, K. A. (2000). Augmentative
Rispoli, M., Lancioni, G., Aguilar, J., & Fragale, communication and literacy. In A. M. Wetherby &
C. (2010). The effects of an abolishing operation B. M. Prizant (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorders: A
intervention component on play skills, challenging transactional developmental perspective (pp. 333–
behavior and stereotypy. Behavior Modification, 34, 367). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co..
267–289. Mirenda, P., & Schuler, A. (1988). Augmenting com-
Lanovaz, M. J., Robertson, K. M., Soerono, K., & Watkins, munication for persons with autism: Issues and
N. (2013). Effects of reducing stereotypy on other strategies. Topics in Language Disorders, 9, 24–43.
behaviors: A systematic review. Research in Autism doi:10.1097/00011363-198812000-00004.
Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1234–1243. doi:10.1016/j. Morford, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1992). Comprehension
rasd.2013.07.009. and production of gesture in ­combination with speech
9  Treatment of Core Symptoms of Autism Spectrum Disorder 143

in one-word speakers. Journal of Child Language, 19, Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1097–1112.
559–580. doi:10.1017/s0305000900011569. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1131-7.
Morrison, R. S., Sainato, D. M., Benchaaban, D., & Endo, Piazza, C. C., Adelinis, J. D., Hanley, G. P., Goh, H. L.,
S. (2002). Increasing play skills of children with & Delia, M. D. (2000). An evaluation of the effects
autism using activity schedules and correspondence of matched stimuli on behaviors maintained by auto-
training. Journal of Early Intervention, 25, 58–72. matic reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior
doi:10.1177/105381510202500106. Analysis, 33, 13–27. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2000.33-13.
National Research Council. (2001). Educating children Pollard, J. S., Betz, A. M., & Higbee, T. S. (2012).
with autism. Washington, DC: NRC. Script fading to promote unscripted bids for joint
Nelson, N. W. (1993). Childhood language disorders in attention in children with autism. Journal of Applied
context: Infancy through adolescence. New York, NY: Behavior Analysis, 45, 387–393. doi: 10.190/
MacMillan. jaba.2012.45-387.
Nigam, R., Schlosser, R. W., & Lloyd, L. L. Ranick, J., Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., & Kornack, J. A.
(2006). Concomitant use of the matrix strat- (2013). Teaching children with autism to detect and
egy and the ­ mand-­ model procedure in teach- respond to deceptive statements. Research in Autism
ing graphic symbol combinations. Augmentative Spectrum Disorders, 7, 503–508. doi:10.1016/j.
and Alternative Communication, 22, 160–177. rasd.2012.12.001.
doi:10.1080/07434610600650052. Rapp, J. T. (2007). Further evaluation of methods to iden-
Ninci, J., Lang, R., Davenport, K., Lee, A., Garner, J., tify matched stimulation. Journal of Applied Behavior
Moore, M., … Lancioni, G. (2013). An analysis of the Analysis, 40, 73–88. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.142-05.
generalization and maintenance of eye contact taught Rapp, J. T., & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A
during play. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 16, review of behavioral assessment and treatment.
301–307. doi:10.3109/17518423.2012.730557. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 527–547.
O'Connor, A. S., Prieto, J., Hoffmann, B., DeQuinzio, J. A., doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.11.005.
& Taylor, B. A. (2011). A stimulus control procedure Rehfeldt, R. A., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (Eds.). (2009).
to decrease motor and vocal stereotypy. Behavioral Derived relational responding applications for learn-
Interventions, 26(3), 231–242. doi:10.1002/bin.335. ers with autism and other developmental disabilities:
Odom, S. L., Boyd, B. A., Hall, L. J., & Hume, K. (2009). A progressive guide to change. Oakland, CA: New
Evaluation of comprehensive treatment model for Harbinger Publications.
individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Journal Rincover, A., Newsom, C. D., & Carr, E. G. (1979). Using
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 425–436. sensory extinction procedures in the treatment of com-
doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0825-1. pulsive like behavior of developmentally disabled chil-
O’Handley, R. D., Radley, K. C., & Whipple, H. M. dren. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
(2015). The relative effects of social stories and video 47, 695–701. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.47.4.695.
modeling toward increasing eye contact of adolescents Rispoli, M., Camargo, S., Machalicek, W., Lang, R., &
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Sigafoos, J. (2014). Functional communication train-
Spectrum Disorders, 11, 101–111. doi:10.1016/j. ing in the treatment of problem behavior maintained
rasd.2014.12.009. by access to rituals. Journal of Applied Behavior
Park, M. N. (2013). Pivotal response training. In F. R. Analysis, 47, 580–593. doi:10.1002/jaba.130.
Volkmar (Ed.), Encyclopedia of autism spectrum dis- Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M., Lang, R., Machalicek, W.,
orders (pp. 2266–2267). New York, NY: Springer. Davis, T., Lancioni, G., & Sigafoos, J. (2011). Effects
Patterson, S. Y., Smith, V., & Jelen, M. (2010). Behavioral of motivating operations on problem and academic
intervention practices for stereotypic and repeti- behavior in classrooms. Journal of Applied Behavior
tive behavior in individuals with autism spectrum Analysis, 44, 187–192. doi: 10.190/jaba.2011.44-187.
disorder: A systematic review. Developmental Rosales, R., Rehfeldt, R. A., & Lovett, S. (2011). Effects
Medicine and Child Neurology, 52, 318–327. of multiple exemplar training on the emergence of
doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03597.x. derived relations in preschool children learning a sec-
Paul, R. (2005). Assessing communication in autism spec- ond language. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27,
trum disorders. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, A. Klin, & 61–74.
D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and pervasive Rudy, N. A., Betz, A. M., Malone, E., Henry, J. E.,
developmental disorders (pp. 799–816). Hoboken, NJ: & Chong, I. M. (2014). Effects of video model-
Wiley. ing on teaching bids for joint attention to children
Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St Clair, M. (2013). with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 29, 269–285.
Teaching children with autism to detect and respond doi:10.1002/bin.1398.
to sarcasm. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, Schlosser, R. W., & Koul, R. K. (2015). Speech out-
913–920. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.08.005. put technologies in interventions for individuals
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, with autism spectrum disorders: A scoping review.
E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 31,
Processing of ironic language in children with high-­ 285–309. doi:10.3109/07434618.2015.1063689.
functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of
144 M.T. Brodhead et al.

Schlosser, R. W., & Wendt, O. (2008). Augmentative and Turner, M. (1999). Annotation: Repetitive behav-
alternative communication intervention for children ior in autism: A review of psychological
with autism: A systematic review. In J. K. Luiselli, research. Journal of Child Psychology and
D. C. Russo, & W. P. Christian (Eds.), Effective prac- Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 40, 839–849.
tices for children with autism: Educational and behav- doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00502.
ior support interventions that work (pp. 325–389). Van Camp, C. M., Lerman, D. C., Kelley, M. E., Roane,
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. H. S., Contrucci, S. A., & Vorndran, C. M. (2000).
Sigafoos, J., Drasgow, E., Duker, P. C., O’Reilly, M. F., Further analysis of idiosyncratic antecedent influ-
Lancioni, G. E., & Reichle, J. (2009). General inter- ences during the assessment and treatment of problem
vention approaches for teaching speech and its pre- behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33,
requisites. In A. Fitzer & P. Sturmey (Eds.), Language 207–221. doi:10.1901/jaba.2000.33-207.
and autism (pp. 110–121). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, G. A., & LeBlanc, L. (1994).
Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., Schlosser, R. W., & Treatment of self-injury and hand mouthing fol-
Lancioni, G. E. (2007). Communication intervention. lowing inconclusive functional analyses. Journal of
In P. Sturmey & A. Fitzer (Eds.), Autism spectrum Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 331–344. doi:10.1901/
disorders: Applied behavior analysis, evidence, and jaba.1994.27-331.
practice (pp. 151–185). Austin, TX: PRO-ED. Watt, N., Wetherby, A., Barber, A., & Morgan, L. (2008).
Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Englewood Cliffs, Repetitive and stereotyped behaviors in children with
NJ: Prentice Hall. autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life.
Smith, T., Klorman, R., & Mruzek, D. (2015). Predicting Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38,
outcome of community-based early intensive behav- 1518–1533.
ioral intervention for children with autism. Journal Wichnick, A. M., Vener, S. M., Pyrtek, M., & Poulson,
of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1271–1282. C. L. (2010a). The effect of a script-fading pro-
doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0002-2. cedure on responses to peer initiations among
Sturmey, P., & Sevin, J. A. (1994). Defining and assess- young children with autism. Research in Autism
ing autism. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Autism in children Spectrum Disorders, 4, 290–299. doi:10.1016/j.
and adults: Etiology, assessment, and intervention rasd.2009.09.016.
(pp. 13–36). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole. Wichnick, A. M., Vener, S. M., Keating, C., & Poulson,
Sundberg, M. L., & Michael, J. (2001). The benefits of C. L. (2010b). The effect of a script-fading proce-
Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior for children dure on unscripted social initiations and novel utter-
with autism. Behavior Modification, 25, 698–724. ances among young children with autism. Research in
doi:10.1177/0145445501255003. Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 51–64. doi:10.1016/j.
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally rasd.2009.07.006.
verbal school-aged children with autism spectrum Williams, D. L., & Marra, L. J. (2011). Language
disorder: The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism interventions. In M. J. Lubetsky, B. L. Handen, &
Research, 6, 468–478. doi:10.1002/aur.1329. J. J. McGonigle (Eds.), Autism spectrum disorder
Taylor, B. A., & Hoch, H. (2008). Teaching children with (pp. 173–191). New York, NY: Oxford University
autism to respond to and initiate bids for joint atten- Press.
tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 377– Yoder, P., McCathren, R. B., Warren, S. F., & Watson, A. L.
391. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-377. (2001). Important distinctions in measuring maternal
Taylor, B. A., Hoch, H., & Weissman, M. (2005). The responses to communication in prelinguistic children
analysis and treatment of vocal stereotypy in a child with disabilities. Communication Disorders Quarterly,
with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 239–253. 22, 135–147. doi:10.1177/152574010102200303.
doi:10.1002/bin.200.
Comorbid Challenging Behaviors
10
Marc J. Lanovaz, John T. Rapp, Alexie Gendron,
Isabelle Préfontaine, and Stéphanie Turgeon

 haracteristics of Challenging
C
Introduction Behaviors

Researchers have shown that individuals with Although the research literature contains multi-
autism spectrum disorders (ASD) engage in higher ple definitions of challenging behaviors,
levels of challenging behaviors than individuals researchers generally agree that a behavior is
with other developmental disabilities (Arron, challenging when it poses a threat to the devel-
Oliver, Moss, Berg, & Burbidge, 2011; Dominick, opment, health, or security of the individual with
Davis, Lainhart, Tager-Flusberg, & Folstein, 2007; ASD or others (e.g., caregivers, educators, sib-
McClintock, Hall, & Oliver, 2003; Rojahn, lings) and when functional abilities are compro-
Wilkins, Matson, & Boisjoli, 2010). Individuals mised (Dunlap et al., 2006; Minshawi, Hurwitz,
with ASD also present the highest prevalences for Morris, & McDougle, 2014; Rojahn, Matson,
specific forms of challenging behaviors including Lott, Esbensen, & Smalls, 2001). As part of the
self-injury, aggression, and stereotypy (Chebli, current chapter, we will use this broad definition
Martin, & Lanovaz, 2016; Emerson et al., 2001; when referring to challenging behaviors. Usually
Farmer & Aman, 2011; Matson & Shoemaker, described by their observable properties, chal-
2009; Rojahn et al., 2009). These results indicate lenging behaviors vary in terms of nature, fre-
that challenging behaviors are clearly a significant quency, duration, and intensity (McGill, Hughes,
issue in children, adolescents, and adults with ASD Teer, & Rye, 2001). Frequently reported topog-
that practitioners must take into consideration raphies of challenging behaviors are self-injuri-
when developing treatment plans. ous behaviors (SIB), aggression and destruction,
and stereotypy (Baghdadli, Pascal, Grisi, &
The literature review and analyses presented in the chap- Aussilloux, 2003; Chebli et al. 2016; Emerson
ter were funded in part by a grant and a salary award from et al., 2001; Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007;
the Fonds de recherche du Québec – Santé (#30827,
McTiernan, Leader, Healy, & Mannion, 2011;
#32612) to the first author.
Rojahn et al., 2001). The occurrence of each
M.J. Lanovaz • A. Gendron • I. Préfontaine topography is not mutually exclusive: the behav-
S. Turgeon
Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC, Canada ioral profiles of individuals with ASD often
include occurrences of multiple different forms
J.T. Rapp (*)
Auburn University, Auburn, AL, USA of challenging behaviors (Mazurek, Kanne, &
e-mail: jtr0014@auburn.edu Wodka, 2013; McClintock et al., 2003).

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 145


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_10
146 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

Researchers typically define SIB as self-­ ing, object spinning, and twirling (Chebli et al.,
directed behaviors that may inflict physical harm 2016; Crosland, Zarcone, Schroeder, Zarcone, &
to one’s own body (Carr, 1977; Iwata, Dorsey, Fowler, 2005). Examples of vocal stereotypy
Slifer, & Bauman, 1982/1994). Commonly involve any repetitive sounds and non-contextual
observed forms of SIB include head banging, phrases that happen without apparent intention to
hair pulling, biting, eye poking, scratching, self-­ interact such as unrecognizable words or vocal-
punching, self-slapping, and self-pinching izations, non-contextual laughing, giggling, and
(Baghdadli, et al., 2003; Carr, 1977; Iwata et al., repetition of words or phrases (Lanovaz &
1994; Matson & LoVullo, 2008). Cases involving Sladeczek, 2012; MacDonald et al., 2007).
intake of inedible items (pica) or fluids and the Challenging behaviors can take other forms
use of objects or furniture to harm one’s self have that have not been discussed previously such as
also been reported in the research literature noncompliance (Plumet & Veneziano, 2014;
(Kahng, Hausman, & Jann, 2011; Luiselli, Wilder, Harris, Reagan, & Rasey, 2007).
Cochran, & Huber, 2005; Mitter, Romani, Greer, Noncompliance can be generalized to all people
& Fisher, 2015). and environments, or it can be specific to one per-
A second common category of challenging son, a type of demand, or a location. Other exam-
behaviors is aggression and destruction, which ples of challenging behaviors reported in the
are often associated with high risks of injuries to research literature include elopement or running
one’s self and others (Matson, Boisjoli, Rojahn, away, stripping, inappropriate touching, and food
& Hess, 2009). Aggression is a challenging stealing (Luiselli et al., 1999; Newman,
behavior that is directed toward somebody else Summerhill, Mosley, & Tooth, 2003; Olive,
that causes, or has the potential to cause, physical Lang, & Davis, 2008; O’Reilly, Edrisinha,
or psychological harm. Some prevailing topogra- Sigafoos, Lancioni, & Andrews, 2006; Schmidt,
phies of aggression are shouting, cursing, insult- Drasgow, Halle, Martin, & Bliss, 2014; Vaughn,
ing, threatening, hitting, pinching, biting, kicking, Wilson, & Dunlap, 2002).
and hair pulling (Roane & Kadey, 2011). On the
other hand, destruction is the act of damaging
property by throwing, breaking, knocking over, Prevalence of Challenging Behaviors
or tearing objects or furniture apart (Mitter et al.,
2015; Roane & Kadey, 2011). Destruction is sim- Identifying the exact prevalence of challenging
ilar in form to aggression, but it is directed toward behaviors in individuals with ASD is problematic
objects rather than other individuals. due to the diverse methodologies used across
A third common category of challenging studies. These differences in methodologies
behaviors is stereotypy, which is generally include the use of small or heterogeneous sam-
defined as repetitive and invariant behaviors, ples in respect to diagnosis (i.e., ASD, autism,
activities, or interests that have no apparent social PDD-NOS), sex and age, and variations in opera-
function (MacDonald et al., 2007; Rapp & tional definitions of challenging behaviors. Other
Vollmer, 2005). At a young age, stereotypy is issues are the adoption of a single data collection
common among typically developing children method or of a single informant as well as the use
(Thelen, 1979). Its frequency tends to stay stable of non-psychometrically validated data collec-
or to decrease between the ages of 2 and 4 in chil- tion instruments. That said, we will offer a gen-
dren without disabilities, while it generally eral overview of prevalence using specific studies
increases in children with ASD (MacDonald to provide an estimate for each topography.
et al., 2007). Manifestations of stereotypy can Multiple studies have evaluated the overall
include motor or vocal behaviors that vary across prevalence of challenging behaviors in i­ ndividuals
individuals, time, and settings. Examples of with ASD. Parents and caregivers have reported
repetitive motor movements include hand flap- prevalences of challenging behaviors ranging
ping, body rocking, pacing, head rolling or weav- from 36% to 94% in multiple samples of indi-
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 147

viduals with ASD (Baghdadli, et al., 2003; When considering destruction alone, Matson and
Bodfish, Symons, Parker, & Lewis, 2000; Rivet (2008) indicated that at least 29% of their
Murphy, Healy, & Leader, 2009). In a more sample of adults with ASD engaged in this type of
recent study, McTiernan et al. (2011) reported behavior.
that 94% of their sample presented one or more Finally, stereotypy appears to have the highest
topographies of challenging behaviors using staff prevalence, which is expected given that it is a
members as informants. Some authors have stud- defining feature of ASD. In a recent systematic
ied the risk factors associated with high preva- review, Chebli et al. (2016) reported that 88% of
lences of challenging behaviors within clinical individuals with ASD engaged in at least one
populations. High levels of impulsivity, low lev- form of stereotypy. Chebli et al. found that sen-
els of communication skills, and high severity of sory stereotypy (e.g., gazing at lights, rubbing, or
ASD characteristics were found to predict higher sniffing objects) was the highest recorded type of
prevalences and severity of challenging behav- stereotypy, followed by object stereotypy (e.g.,
iors (Arron et al., 2011; Matson & Shoemaker, spinning toys), locomotion (e.g., pacing), hand/
2009; Mazurek et al., 2013; Rojahn et al., 2009). finger movement (e.g., hand flapping), and vocal
Regarding more specific forms of challenging stereotypy (e.g., echolalia).
behaviors, studies of SIB have reported preva-
lences from 20% to 69% in individuals with ASD
(Baghdadli et al., 2003; Bodfish et al., 2000; Impact of Challenging Behaviors
Matson & Rivet, 2008; Mazurek et al., 2013;
McTiernan et al., 2011; Richards, Oliver, Nelson, Engagement in challenging behaviors may have
& Moss, 2012; Rojahn et al., 2009). In a recent serious consequences on individuals with ASD
study by Rattaz, Michelon, and Baghdadli and those around them (e.g., caregivers, instruc-
(2015), parents of 152 adolescents with ASD tors). This section highlights some of the poten-
completed the Aberrant Behavior Checklist. The tial impacts of untreated challenging behaviors.
analysis of the results indicated that 36% of the Regardless of form, one of the main collateral
sample engaged in at least one form of SIB. The effects of engaging in challenging behaviors is
severity of autistic symptomatology was found to increased levels of parental stress (Lecavalier,
be the most important risk factor for displaying Leone, & Wiltz, 2006). A study conducted by
SIB. As with other studies, low levels of commu- Tomanik, Harris, and Hawkins (2004) found that
nication skills and impulsivity were also identi- communication difficulties, limited interaction
fied as predictors for engagement in the with others, and restricted abilities to care for
behavior. one’s self were also highly correlated with mater-
Prevalence estimates for aggression and nal stress. Increased levels of stress may result in
destruction also vary considerably across studies. higher psychological distress, more mental health
Tyrer et al. (2006) found that 29% of the adults issues, and marital conflicts.
with autism presented aggressive behaviors, The topography of challenging behaviors with
while Mazurek et al. (2013) reported prevalence the most obvious impact on the individual is
of 54% in their sample of 1584 of children aged SIB. Engaging in SIB may produce bruises, swell-
2–17 years old. The latter study also noted age as ing, lacerations, fractures, induced blindness,
being significantly associated with aggression. In physical malformations, and infections (Carr,
fact, the researchers found that the highest preva- 1977; Luiselli et al., 2005; Minshawi, Hurwitz,
lence was in children aged from 5 to 7 years old. Morris, & McDougle, 2014; Underwood,
McTiernan et al. (2011) reported a prevalence Figueroa, Thyer, & Nzeocha, 1989). In extreme
similar to the one found by Mazurek et al. (2013). cases, self-injury can lead to medical interven-
According to their results, 56% of their sample of tions, hospitalization, and even death (Baghdadli
174 participants with ASD aged from 3 to 14 years et al., 2003; Mandell, 2008; Minshawi, Hurwitz,
exhibited aggressive or destructive behaviors. Morris, & McDougle, 2014). In addition to
148 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

physical harm, SIB reduces an individual’s well- Assessment


being as it negatively affects social skills, leads to
social stigmatization, increases isolation, limits When aiming to reduce engagement in challeng-
educational and vocational opportunities, and ing behaviors in individuals with ASD, the first
restricts one’s access to community-­based activi- step is to identify the stimuli that evoke and
ties (Luiselli et al., 2005; Minshawi, Hurwitz, maintain the behavior in the individual’s environ-
Morris, & McDougle, 2014). ment. That is, the practitioner should identify
Given their consequences for others, aggres- antecedent events that may trigger or evoke
sion and destructive behaviors may also interfere engagement in the challenging behaviors as well
with opportunities to be included in learning envi- as the reinforcers that maintain their occurrence.
ronments and community activities. Moreover, The following sections examine common ante-
individuals who exhibit aggression and their care- cedent and consequent events associated with
givers are at risk of suffering from physical and challenging behaviors and methods to identify
emotional distress (Matson et al., 2009; Roane & them.
Kadey, 2011). For individuals with ASD, untreated
aggressive behaviors may result in their removal
from school settings, residential settings, and work Antecedent Events
environments (Marcus, Vollmer, Swanson, Roane,
& Ringdahl, 2001). Mandell (2008) also found Antecedents are generally defined as events or
that aggressiveness toward others poses a consid- stimuli that immediately precede the occurrence
erable risk of hospitalization in psychiatric facili- of a behavior (Smith & Iwata, 1997). Various
ties for children diagnosed with ASD. Additionally, environmental and intrinsic stimuli such as
aggression and destruction can induce social objects, settings, time, type of activities, persons,
impairments, high financial costs, and exposure to and sensations can function as setting events for
harmful substances (Roane & Kadey, 2011). challenging behaviors (McGill, Teer, Rye, &
Stereotypy is a time-consuming and invasive Hughes, 2003; Simó-Pinatella et al., 2013). Their
behavior that typically interferes with engagement identification is an important step in the reduc-
in functional activities. As a result, engaging in ste- tion of challenging behaviors as it emphasizes the
reotypy may compromise interactions with peers, circumstances in which the behaviors occur. With
adaptive functioning, and learning (Cunningham & this in mind, two types of antecedent events
Schreibman, 2008; Lanovaz, Robertson, Soerono, should be acknowledged when analyzing chal-
& Watkins, 2013). The individual’s abilities to exe- lenging behaviors: discriminative stimuli and
cute daily living tasks, to communicate appropri- motivating operations (MO).
ately, and to engage in functional activities may Discriminative stimuli are precise events or
also be affected (Matson, Kiely, & Bamburg, stimulus changes that signal the availability or
1997). Furthermore, individuals who engage in ste- non-availability of reinforcement (Langthorne &
reotypy may suffer from prejudices, restricted McGill, 2009; Simó-Pinatella et al., 2013). This
learning opportunities, and limited social integra- differential availability of the reinforcer results
tion (Jones, Wint, & Ellis, 1990). Cunningham and from the relationship between a stimulus condi-
Schreibman (2008) also noted that social stigmati- tion, a given behavior, and the subsequent out-
zation is associated with a feeling of discomfort in come (Michael, 1993). To be considered
parents of children who engage in stereotypy in discriminative, the presence of the stimulus condi-
public environments. Consistent with studies on tion must have previously preceded a specific
other forms of challenging behaviors displayed by behavior that resulted in reinforcement. Second, in
individuals with ASD, Harrop, McBee, and Boyd the absence of the stimulus condition, the same
(2016) found that preschoolers’ engagement in behavior must not have produced reinforcement
restricted and repetitive behaviors was correlated (Michael, 2000). As a result, the frequency of the
with increased caregiver stress. behavior is modified according to the availability
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 149

of reinforcement. The behavior is more frequent in Functions of Challenging Behaviors


the presence of the discriminative stimuli because
of the concomitant possibility of reinforcement, The development of challenging behaviors can
while the frequency of the behavior is decreased in be fully appreciated through the observation of
the absence of the discriminative stimuli since no their function, which is described as the rein-
reinforcement is expected (Langthorne & McGill, forcement contingency maintaining the behav-
2009; Michael, 1982). Several variables can serve iors (Hanley, Iwata, & McCord, 2003).
as discriminative stimuli for challenging behaviors Challenging behaviors are generally followed by
such as the characteristics of the environmental environmental and internal consequences. These
context, the presence or absence of a preferred consequences maintain, reinforce, or discourage
item, and the presence of a specific individual the reoccurrence of challenging behaviors. If an
(e.g., Conners et al., 2000; Kang et al., 2010; individual’s response to the antecedent is fol-
O’Reilly et al., 2000). lowed by desirable consequences, the probability
A second type of antecedent events is the of this behavior reoccurring increases. Therefore,
MO. The presence of MO sets the capacity of an the function of the behavior is to access the tar-
event to serve as reinforcer or punisher by trig- geted consequence. A behavior can either occur
gering two interrelated phenomena termed value-­ in order to gain access to something desirable or
altering and behavior-altering effects to terminate an unwanted situation or stimulus
(Langthorne, McGill, & Oliver, 2014; Laraway, event (Horner & Carr, 1997; Iwata et al.,
Snycerski, Michael, & Poling, 2003; Laraway, 1982/1994). These two effects, embodied by
Snycerksi, Olson, Becker, & Poling, 2014). The social positive, social negative, and automatic
value-altering effect alters the effectiveness of reinforcement, serve as a description for the
reinforcers or punishers (Langthorne et al., 2014). maintenance of challenging behaviors.
The value of reinforcement or punishment is Positive social reinforcement is a type of rein-
either increased (i.e., established) or decreased forcement that is mediated by another person and
(i.e., abolished) in the presence of the MO. In is associated with the addition of a stimulus
contrast, the behavior-altering effect involves the event. In practical settings, challenging behaviors
impact of the MO on the actual behavior. The lat- maintained by social positive reinforcement are
ter is either encouraged (evoked) or discouraged generally categorized within one of two func-
(abated; Laraway et al., 2003). Taken together, tions: attention and tangible. Attention-­
value-altering and behavior-altering effects have maintained challenging behaviors are reinforced
considerable impact on the frequency of chal- by the social response of others to the behavior.
lenging behaviors. Some potential MO for chal- This response may be either motor (e.g., facial
lenging behaviors include sleep deprivation expressions, physical contact) or verbal (e.g.,
(Horner, Day, & Day, 1997; O’Reilly, 1995; comforting words, maintenance of conversation).
Reed, Dolezal, Cooper-Brown, & Wacker, 2005), Forms of attention that may seem less desirable
menstrual discomfort (Carr & Smith, 1995; Carr, (e.g., reprimands) may also maintain engagement
Smith, Giacin, Whelan, & Pancari, 2003; in challenging behaviors (Olive et al., 2008). A
Douglas, 2004; Hamilton, Marshal, & Murray, common indicator of the attention function is that
2011), as well as certain drugs and illnesses an individual will seek eye contact while engag-
(Kennedy & Meyer, 1996; Luiselli, et al., 2005; ing in challenging behaviors. The individual may
Mello, Mendelson, & Kuehnle, 1982; Nickels also react when attention of others is diverted or
et al., 2009; O’Reilly, 1997; Rapp, Swanson, & provided to someone else. As an example,
Dornbush, 2007; Valdovinos & Kennedy, 2004). Schmidt et al. (2014) showed that the aggression,
For practitioners, identifying both discriminative inappropriate touching, and cursing of an adoles-
stimuli and MO is important as they will have an cent with ASD occurred most often when an
impact on the selection of an intervention and its adult entered the room and began a conversation
effect on challenging behaviors. with the therapist.
150 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

Access to tangible items is also a type of social Fodstad et al., 2014; Rapp & Vollmer, 2005). A
reinforcement that maintains challenging behav- behavior is considered as nonsocially reinforced
iors (Vollmer, Marcus, Ringdahl, & Roane, when it persists in the absence of social rein-
1995). When a challenging behavior has a tangi- forcement (Querim et al., 2013). For example,
ble function, engaging in the behavior results in Dominguez, Wilder, Cheung, and Rey (2014)
the delivery of a tangible item, an edible, or an found that engagement in rumination was inde-
activity (e.g., watching a movie). In these cases, pendent of social consequences in a child with
challenging behaviors may occur when access to ASD. Researchers have also shown that various
a preferred item or activity is restricted, refused, forms of stereotypy are generally, albeit not
or withdrawn. For example, researchers have always, maintained by nonsocial reinforcement
shown that children with ASD who exhibit ritual- (Beavers, Iwata, & Lerman, 2013; Matson,
istic behavior might engage in challenging Bamburg, Cherry, & Paclawskyj, 1999; Rapp &
behaviors if their routine is interrupted or blocked Vollmer, 2005; Wilke et al., 2012).
(e.g., Rispoli, Camargo, Machalicek, Lang, &
Sigafoos, 2014).
Negative social reinforcement involves the Functional Assessment
removal of a stimulus by another individual. It is
generally associated with the escape (termination Assessment of challenging behaviors should be
or attenuation of a putatively unpleasant stimulus viewed as a generative, multiple-step process.
event) or avoidance function (prevention of a The first step often involves the use of indirect or
putatively unpleasant stimulus event). anecdotal assessment to gather general informa-
Challenging behaviors maintained by negative tion about the conditions during which the chal-
social reinforcement are followed by escape or lenging behavior occurs. The second step is for
avoidance of an aversive event (Carr, 1977) such trained practitioners to conduct direct observa-
as the termination of an instruction, task, demand, tions of the challenging behavior during “high-­
or routine or the withdrawal of an individual or probability” conditions (presumably identified
stimulus (e.g., loud sounds, bright lights). via indirect or informant assessment) in order to
Schindler and Horner (2005) provide an example (a) determine the baseline rate or level of the
of challenging behaviors maintained by escape. challenging behavior and (b) identify antecedents
The researchers found that the high pitch and fre- (i.e., potential MO or discriminative stimuli) and
quent screaming of a young girl with ASD was consequent (i.e., potential reinforcers) events for
maintained by escaping components of an activ- the challenging behavior. Results of recent sur-
ity, which were subjectively rated as difficult. vey studies suggest that many practitioners often
Finally, automatic reinforcement, also referred rely, perhaps to a fault, on the findings from basic
to as nonsocial reinforcement, involves contin- descriptive assessments to develop behavioral
gencies that are independent from the social envi- interventions for challenging behaviors (Oliver,
ronment (Vollmer, 1994). Researchers Pratt, & Normand, 2015; Roscoe, Phillips, Kelly,
hypothesize that challenging behaviors main- Farber, & Dube, 2015). The third step of the
tained by nonsocial reinforcement generate their assessment process should involve a functional
own sensory consequences, such as visual stimu- analysis (FA) of one or more probable operant
lation, vestibular stimulation, tactile input, and functions of the challenging behavior. At a mini-
auditory stimulation (Lovaas, Newsom, & mum, the FA should involve direct, systematic
Hickman, 1987; Rapp, 2008). As for behavior manipulation of one or more antecedent events,
maintained by social consequences, challenging consequent events, or both (Hanley et al., 2003).
behaviors serving a nonsocial function can be
described as positively or negatively reinforced, Indirect or Anecdotal Assessment  Informant-­
but the technology to differentiate between the based assessments typically involve structured
two is not well developed (Minshawi, Hurwitz, questionnaires that are delivered by a practitioner
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 151

to a caregiver of the individual referred for the cases when relations between antecedent events,
treatment of challenging behaviors. Two struc- consequent events, and challenging behaviors are
tured questionnaires with varying degrees of evident, results from this level of assessment
empirical support are the Questions About should be used primarily to further develop spe-
Behavior Function (QABF), which is a 25-item cific conditions to be tested in a FA.
questionnaire (Matson & Vollmer, 1995), and the Structured descriptive assessments are con-
Functional Analysis Screening Tool (FAST), ducted in a manner that is similar to descriptive
which is a 16-item questionnaire (Iwata, DeLeon, assessments with conditional and unconditional
& Roscoe, 2013). In general, the QABF has been probabilities with the exception that practitioner
evaluated in studies with a wider range of partici- directly manipulates the antecedent events (con-
pants and challenging behaviors (e.g., Applegate, sequences are left to vary). Because the ante-
Matson, & Cherry, 1999; Lanovaz, Argumedes, cedent conditions are controlled by the
Roy, Duquette, & Watkins, 2013; Paclawskyj, practitioner, the observations can be organized
Matson, Rush, Smalls, & Vollmer, 2000, 2001; into sessions with equivalent durations (e.g.,
Smith, Smith, Dracobly, & Peterson-Pace, 2012; 10 min) containing specific antecedent changes
Watkins & Rapp, 2013) than the (e.g., demands provided or attention withheld).
FAST. Nevertheless, the results of either assess- The results from each session can then be plot-
ment should be used primarily to develop one or ted into multielement design graphs and visu-
more hypotheses about the operant function of ally inspected for elevated data paths (e.g.,
challenging behaviors. Practitioners can also Anderson & Long, 2002; English & Anderson,
acquire qualitative information about events sur- 2006). Even though structured descriptive
rounding challenging behavior via parent-­ assessments do not offer a clear time saving
conducted ABC narratives; however, the compared to a typical FA, this approach may be
reliability and validity of those observations have better suited to evaluating the stimulus events
been mixed (e.g., Lanovaz et al., 2013; Lerman, that evoke challenging behaviors in classroom
Hovanetz, Strobel, & Tetreault, 2009). settings or other contexts that are difficult to
simulate with a standard or modified FA.
Direct Assessment  On the continuum of direct
assessment tools, practitioners can utilize a low-­ Functional Analysis  Functional analytic proce-
effort descriptive assessment or a high-effort dures (Iwata et al., 1982/1994) have been used to
structured descriptive assessment. At the most assess the operant function of a wide range of
basic level, a practitioner conducting a descrip- challenging behaviors by individuals with ASD
tive assessment may simply collect data on com- and other neurodevelopmental disorders (Beavers
mon consequent events for engaging in et al., 2013; Hanley et al., 2003). As previously
challenging behaviors. At a more complex level, noted, standard FA procedures involve conditions
a descriptive assessment may include data collec- that test for (a) social positive reinforcement in the
tion on various antecedent events (e.g., demands, form of contingent attention, contingent access to
tangibles restricted) and consequent events (e.g., activities or items, or both, (b) social negative
escape provided, tangible provided). This inten- reinforcement in the form of escape or termination
sive data collection allows the practitioner to cal- of subjectively unpleasant environmental events
culate conditional and unconditional (sometimes such as academic or vocational demands, and (c)
referred to as background probabilities) probabil- nonsocial reinforcement whereby challenging
ities of challenging behaviors in relation to vari- behaviors persist without changes to the individu-
ous antecedent and consequent events; however, al’s external environment. The standard FA with
the intensive analysis does not necessarily multiple test conditions provided during 10-min
increase the probability of identifying the correct sessions are generally recognized as the gold stan-
function of challenging behaviors (e.g., Pence, dard for assessing the operant function of chal-
Roscoe, Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009). Except in lenging behaviors, but many practitioners lack the
152 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

resources needed to conduct the standard condi- intervention. Descriptions of such analyses are
tions (e.g., Oliver et al., 2015; Roscoe et al., 2015). beyond the scope of this section, but we refer
As alternatives, practitioners may opt to use a brief readers to Lanovaz, Rapp, and Fletcher (2010)
FA or alternative methodologies. and Rapp and Lanovaz (2016) for one compre-
In general, practitioners should use the results hensive option.
of the indirect assessment and direct observations
to develop a hypothesis that can be directly evalu- Single-Function Test  For this FA format, the
ated with a brief FA methodology. Some of these practitioner consolidates information obtained
brief FA variations have been used widely in the from the indirect and descriptive assessments
treatment literature, whereas others have only to develop a specific hypothesis about the oper-
preliminary support. It is important to recognize ant function of the challenging behavior. Based
that specific types of challenging behaviors lend on the hypothesis, the practitioner develops a
themselves to one or more of these FA approaches. specific test condition to assess the effects of
Iwata and Dozier (2008) outlined the relative one specific antecedent or consequent event.
merits of brief FA variations and illustrated hypo- The control condition is then developed to con-
thetical results for each variation. As outlined by trol the event that is manipulated in the test
Iwata and Dozier, each approach can be fit to one condition. The practitioner then conducts three
or more single-case experimental designs (with or more sessions for each condition in an alter-
minor exceptions), and each has relative advan- nating format.
tages (e.g., time saving, good contextual fit) and
disadvantages (e.g., limited scope of function). Latency Analysis of Standard Conditions  Using
this format, practitioners arrange to conduct stan-
Consecutive No-Interaction Sessions  This FA dard FA conditions of 5 min or 10 min in dura-
variation should be used when the practitioner tion; however, the dependent variable is the
suspects that the challenging behavior in ques- latency to engagement in the challenging behav-
tion is nonsocially reinforced (Iwata & Dozier, ior, and the respective session is terminated fol-
2008; Querim et al., 2013). This practice was ini- lowing an occurrence of the said behavior.
tially part of the third phase of a progressive Because the dependent variable differs from a
model proposed by Vollmer et al. (1995) to verify standard FA, the visual analysis differs slightly as
the persistence of behavior in the absence of well. The practitioner identifies the stimulus
social consequences. When applied, a practitio- event maintaining the challenging behavior based
ner may verify that an individual’s challenging on the data path with the shortest latency to
behavior is maintained by a nonsocial conse- engagement. When aptly implemented, the con-
quence by showing that the behavior persists trol condition yields an elevated data path (indi-
across three or more consecutive no-interaction cating the absence of the target behavior for the
10-min sessions. Challenging behaviors that duration of the session), and the test condition
decrease markedly across sessions are presumed that contains the functional reinforcer for the
to be socially reinforced and should be subjected challenging behavior produces a lower-level data
to further assessment with other FA variations. path. The primary advantages of using this
The primary advantage of this approach is the method are the potential time savings and its suit-
substantial time savings for practitioners. By ability for specific forms of challenging behav-
contrast, the primary limitation is that the out- iors such as elopement or pica (Neidert, Iwata,
come does not directly inform practitioners of Dempsey, & Thomason-Sassi, 2013; Thomason-­
indicated interventions. That is, behaviors that Sassi, Iwata, Neidert, & Roscoe, 2011). A poten-
persist across such conditions are likely to be tial disadvantage of this FA variation is that it
maintained by automatic positive reinforcement may produce false negatives (i.e., failure to detect
(Rapp & Vollmer, 2005), but more refined analy- a true function for the challenging behavior) due
ses are required to develop a functionally matched to the heavy reliance on antecedent control.
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 153

Trial-Based Conditions  This FA format shares necessary to consider an intervention as empirically


features of the latency analysis (i.e., a trial ends supported (e.g., Briss et al., 2000; Chambless et al.,
with engagement in the challenging behavior) 1998; Kratochwill et al., 2010). The current chapter
and single-function pairwise analyses (i.e., spe- will focus on criteria for single-­case experiments
cific control trials are designed for each individ- because most published studies on reducing chal-
ual test trial condition). The dependent measure lenging behaviors in ASD have made use of single-
is the percentage of trials with challenging behav- case experimental designs (Brosnan & Healy, 2011;
iors across control and test trials for each poten- Carr, Severtson, & Lepper, 2009; DiGennaro Reed,
tial function. The primary advantage of this FA Hirst, & Hyman, 2012). Chambless et al. (1998)
variation is that it is well-suited to classroom and propose a minimum of nine well-designed single-
other instructional formats (Bloom, Iwata, Fritz, case experiments in their definition of empirically
Roscoe, & Carreau, 2011; Rispoli et al., 2014; supported, whereas Kratochwill et al. (2010) rec-
Sigafoos & Saggers, 1995). Nonetheless, due to ommend a minimum of 20 single-case studies fit-
the number of trials that must be conducted for ting specific criteria. To address this discrepancy,
each test-specific control condition, this FA vari- Lanovaz and Rapp (2016) recently proposed report-
ation is unlikely to save time for practitioners. ing the success rate of a treatment to determine
whether it is empirically supported. Specifically, a
Analysis of Precursor Behaviors  This FA varia- treatment is considered as empirically supported
tion, which is not intended to be a briefer iteration, when the success rate can be estimated within a
can be particularly useful for practitioners when range of 40% or less and the treatment produces an
(1) provided consequences for high-­intensity chal- acceptable success rate, which we set at 50% or
lenging behaviors (e.g., some forms of SIB) are more for the current chapter. The number of suc-
undesirable and (2) the target behavior is consis- cessful experiments necessary is thus dependent on
tently preceded by less-intensive behavior the success rate; treatments with higher success
(Dracobly & Smith, 2012; Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, rates necessitate fewer replications than treatments
& Bloom, 2013; Herscovitch, Roscoe, Libby, with success rates closer to 50% when identifying
Bourret, & Ahearn, 2009; Najdowski, Wallace, those with empirical support.
Ellsworth, MacAleese, & Cleveland, 2008; Smith To identify empirically supported treatments
& Churchill, 2002). Because this analysis requires for the chapter, we first conducted a literature
a detailed descriptive assessment with conditional search of PsycInfo® using the following search
and unconditional probabilities to identify a terms (keywords: autis* OR asd OR pdd or
behavior that reliably precedes more intensive Asperg* OR “pervasive development*”) AND
challenging behavior, it may actually require more (keywords: agress* OR “problem behav*” OR
time than a standard FA that is based only on the “challenging behav*” OR “self-injur*” OR
primary topography of challenging behaviors. “repetitive behave* OR opposition OR noncom-
This approach does allow practitioners to evaluate pliance OR stereotyp*) AND (any field: treat-
the function of potentially harmful challenging ment OR intervention). We also hand searched
behaviors without having to directly reinforce the references of a series of systematic reviews
instances of such behavior. on challenging behaviors in individuals with
developmental disabilities (Brosnan & Healy,
2011; Carr et al., 2009; Chowdhury & Benson,
Empirically Supported Treatments 2011; Kurtz, Boelter, Jarmolowicz, Chin, &
Hagopian, 2011; Lanovaz et al., 2013; Petscher,
When developing treatment plans to reduce chal- Rey, & Bailey, 2009). Then, the last three authors
lenging behaviors in individuals with ASD, practi- read the titles and abstracts (and article if neces-
tioners must identify empirically supported sary) to identify those that tested the effects of a
treatments. Multiple criteria have been developed to treatment for reducing challenging behaviors in
define the quality and quantity of research support individuals with ASD.
154 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

For each study, we collected data for each  ocially Reinforced Challenging
S
participant individually (i.e., design, function, Behaviors
treatment, and effect). For our analyses, we
excluded datasets that used quasi-experimental Functional Communication Training (FCT) 
designs (e.g., AB, ABC) or that did not specify According to our search and our analysis, FCT is
the function of challenging behaviors. the treatment with the most empirical support for
Furthermore, we only included datasets that reducing socially reinforced challenging behav-
tested the effects of interventions individually. iors. The treatment consists of teaching the indi-
Multicomponent treatments were not included vidual who engages in challenging behaviors an
in our analyses with the following exceptions. alternative communicative response that serves
First, the interventions could include an extinc- the same function (Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek,
tion component. Second, we included self-man- 2008). This response can involve exchanging a
agement treatments that involved a differential picture, signing, activating a microswitch, using
reinforcement component as the former were a speech enhancement device, or vocally request-
rarely implemented without the latter. Similarly, ing (Heath, Ganz, Parker, Burke, & Ninci, 2015).
response interruption and redirection (RIRD) Regardless of the form of the communicative
was included in punishment-­based procedures response, researchers generally agree that FCT is
even though it included a reinforcement compo- most effective when combined with extinction,
nent. Finally, we did not exclude studies that which involves the withholding of reinforcement
involved minor additions (e.g., fading, prompt- when challenging behaviors occur (Hagopian,
ing, schedule thinning). Fisher, Sullivan, Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998;
We considered a treatment effective (i.e., a Shirley, Iwata, Kahng, Mazaleski, & Lerman,
success) when (a) engagement in the challenging 1997; Wacker et al., 1990). When conducting our
behavior decreased and (b) the researchers dem- literature search, we found a total of 29 studies,
onstrated experimental control over the chal- including 54 participants with ASD, for a success
lenging behavior (based on our visual analysis or rate of 98% CI [90%, 99%] when using FCT to
the visual analysis of the authors when the reduce socially reinforced challenging behaviors.
graphs were unavailable). If an individual was In an example of FCT, Hanley, Jin, Vanselow,
subjected to minor variations of an intervention and Hanratty (2014) reduced challenging behav-
(e.g., with different reinforcers, with varied iors in four children with ASD by teaching a
schedules), we only included the participant communicative response while implementing an
once in the analysis of the target intervention, extinction component. Interestingly, the research-
and we counted the experiment as a success if ers also conducted a denial and delay tolerance
reductions and experimental control were dem- procedure to facilitate the implementation of the
onstrated with at least one treatment parameter. treatment in practical settings. Schmidt et al.
As discussed previously, one of the main factors (2014) taught three boys with ASD to sign for the
that guide practitioners in selecting a treatment reinforcer maintaining challenging behaviors
is the function of the challenging behavior. Thus, (i.e., edible or attention), which produced reduc-
we separately identified treatments that met the tions in aggression and increases in appropriate
single-case design criteria for empirically sup- demands in all participants. Two of the partici-
ported treatments for socially reinforced chal- pants also showed subsequent generalization and
lenging behaviors and nonsocially reinforced maintenance of the learned responses.
challenging behaviors. In the following sections, The main advantage of using FCT is that the
we describe the treatments that met the criteria practitioner teaches the individual a novel com-
to be considered empirically supported based on municative response, which can be pivotal in the
the number of studies that we found for each reduction of other challenging behaviors with the
broad function category. same function and in the development of prosocial
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 155

behaviors. The implementation of FCT also has reducing challenging behaviors in two of these
its challenges. Notably, the individual may engage four participants, underlining the importance of
in the communicative responses (a) when the par- identifying potent reinforcers prior to treatment.
ent or staff is unavailable to provide the reinforcer Similarly, Piazza et al. (1996) reduced multiply
or (b) at high frequencies which make the com- controlled destructive behaviors in an 11-year-­
municative behavior as disruptive to the routine old boy with ASD by implementing DRA for
as the initial challenging behavior. To address compliance with instructions. The intervention
these concerns, researchers have recommended reduced challenging behaviors to near-zero levels
using a multiple schedule wherein a FCT condi- while maintaining increasingly higher expecta-
tion is alternated with an extinction condition tions for task completion.
(Hanley, Iwata, & Thomson, 2001; Jarmolowicz, In the same vein as FCT, the main advantage
DeLeon, & Kuhn, 2009; Kuhn, Chirighin, & of DRA is that the intervention simultaneously
Zelenka, 2010). Initially, the FCT condition is strengthens an appropriate behavior. The indi-
longer than the extinction condition, but the dura- viduals may thus benefit from learning new
tion of each is modified until the FCT is imple- responses (e.g., play, compliance, on task) that
mented for durations that are realistic within the could improve their adaptive functioning. On the
applied setting. Another limitation is that teaching other hand, one concern with DRA is that the
the initial communicative response may be time alternative response may not necessarily be
consuming, especially for individuals with severe incompatible with engagement in challenging
to profound intellectual disability. As such, the behaviors. Therefore, there is the risk that the
treatment may fail to produce short-­term changes. individual may access reinforcement following
both the alternative behavior and the challenging
Differential Reinforcement of Alternative behavior if an extinction component is not imple-
Behavior (DRA)  Another reinforcement-based mented concurrently. Practitioners may also face
procedure with empirical support for reducing challenges when attempting to identify an alter-
engagement in socially reinforced challenging native behavior, especially if the challenging
behaviors is DRA. During DRA, the individual behavior occurs in multiple settings.
receives a reinforcer contingent on engaging in an
alternative appropriate behavior (Petscher et al., Noncontingent Reinforcement (NCR) 
2009). This alternative behavior may take on many Noncontingent reinforcement consists of provid-
forms such as playing, following instructions, or ing access to a preferred stimulus on a regular or
being on task (McClean & Grey, 2012; Piazza, continuous basis, independently of the occurrence
Moes, & Fisher, 1996; Ringdahl et al., 2002). As of challenging behaviors (Carr et al., 2009).
with FCT, research suggests that DRA is typically Generally, the preferred stimulus is matched to
more effective when combined with extinction the function of the challenging behavior and is
(Richman, Wacker, Asmus, & Casey, 1998). The provided on a schedule equal or more frequent
main difference with FCT is that the appropriate than that received for engaging in challenging
behavior is not necessarily a communicative behaviors (Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, Smith, &
response. Based on 14 studies with 20 participants Mazaleski, 1993). However, stimuli unrelated to
with ASD, the success rate of DRA for reducing function have also been shown to be effective at
challenging behaviors maintained by social rein- reducing socially reinforced challenging behav-
forcement was 100% CI [84%, 100%]. iors (Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski, 1997; Fisher,
In a recent example of DRA, Slocum and DeLeon, Rodriguez-Catter, & Keeney, 2004;
Vollmer (2015) found that providing access to Lalli, Casey, & Kates, 1997). In a recent meta-
preferred edible items contingent on compliance analysis, Richman, Barnard-Brak, Grubb, Bosch,
reduced aggression behaviors in four children and Abby (2015) showed that unrelated stimuli
with ASD. The results also indicated that using are less effective than functional stimuli and that
30-s breaks as reinforcers was only effective in thinning the schedule reduces the effectiveness of
156 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

NCR. Furthermore, NCR can be effective even  onsocially Reinforced Challenging


N
when reinforcement remains available for engage- Behaviors
ment in challenging behaviors (Hagopian,
Crockett, Stone, Deleon, & Bowman, 2000). Our Punishment Contingencies  Punishment
literature search indicates that NCR was effective involves the addition of an aversive stimulus or the
at reducing socially reinforced challenging behav- removal of a preferred stimulus (or reinforcer)
ior in 100% CI [77%, 100%] of 13 individuals contingent on the occurrence of challenging
with ASD from ten studies with whom the proce- behaviors. The use of punishment has been the
dures were implemented. topic of the most studies for reducing engagement
Hagopian, Fisher, and Legacy (1994) provide in nonsocially reinforced challenging behaviors
an interesting example of NCR to reduce (e.g., Ahearn, Clark, MacDonald, & Chung, 2007;
attention-­maintained challenging behaviors. Anderson & Le, 2011; Cook, Rapp, Gomes,
Specifically, they provided access to noncontin- Frazer, & Lindblad, 2014; Doughty, Anderson,
gent social interactions to 5-year-old quadruplets Doughty, Williams, & Saunders, 2007; Peters &
with ASD and showed that the intervention was Thompson, 2013). The punishment contingencies
effective at the reducing destructive behaviors in that have been implemented for nonsocially rein-
all four participants. The researchers also showed forced behaviors include reprimands, overcorrec-
that denser schedules produced larger reductions tion, response blocking, and RIRD. The success
than leaner schedules. In a study on multiply con- rate for punishment-based procedures for treating
trolled challenging behaviors (i.e., tangible and nonsocially reinforced behaviors currently stands
escape function), Ingvarsson, Kahng, and at 87% [77%, 93%] for 63 individuals with ASD
Hausman (2008) found that providing access to who participated in 27 different studies.
edible items on a fixed-time schedule reduced Ahearn et al. (2007) examined the effects of
engagement in aggression, disruption, and RIRD on engagement in vocal stereotypy in four
SIB. Notably, the study also showed that the children with ASD. The intervention consisted of
implementation of NCR was associated with an presenting three consecutive demands contingent
increase in compliance in the participant. on engagement in challenging behaviors. In their
From a practical standpoint, NCR has the initial study, RIRD reduced vocal stereotypy to
advantage of being easy to implement; the parent near-zero levels in all four participants and
or trainer only has to provide the stimuli on a time- increased appropriate vocalizations in three of
based or continuous schedule. This ease of use them. In a study of positive practice overcorrec-
makes it possible to implement the procedures tion, Peters and Thomson (2013) examined its
with multiple individuals who engage in challeng- effects on the stereotypy of three individuals with
ing behaviors in group settings (Hagopian et al., ASD. During overcorrection, the trainer prompted
1994). Another benefit of NCR is that it generally the individual to stop and practice appropriate
produces immediate reductions in engagement in engagement for 30 s contingent on the occur-
challenging behaviors. The treatment may also rence of stereotypy. Their results indicated that
produce some negative side effects. The imple- the procedures reduced motor stereotypy for the
mentation of NCR may occasionally result in a three participants while increasing engagement
temporary increase in the frequency or intensity of for two of three participants.
the challenging behaviors, and the delivery of Punishment contingencies are often used in
stimuli on a time-based schedule may adventi- applied settings as the intervention produces rapid
tiously reinforce challenging behaviors (Vollmer, reductions in challenging behaviors. However, cli-
Ringdahl, Roane, & Marcus, 1997). To address nicians should be wary of the challenges associ-
this issue, one simple solution is to implement a ated with the implementation of punishment-based
hold, wherein the stimulus is never delivered interventions as well as of its multiple side effects
within a certain period of time (e.g., 5 s) following (see Lerman & Vorndran, 2002 for detailed discus-
engagement in challenging behaviors. sion). First, punishment contingencies must be
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 157

applied on a continuous schedule in order to be using the same example would be a toy that pro-
effective in reducing engagement in challenging duces visual and tactile stimulation. The success
behaviors. Second, all topographies must be tar- rate of NCR for nonsocially reinforced behaviors
geted by the punishment contingency; if not, for 66 individuals from 25 different studies is
engagement in other forms of challenging behav- 74% [63%, 83%].
iors may continue or increase (Lanovaz et al., Britton et al. (2002) examined the effects of
2013; Rapp, Vollmer, St. Peter, Dozier, & Cotnoir, introducing prompting within a NCR treatment
2004). Third, the implementation of punishment- for a 26-year-old woman with ASD and intellec-
based interventions may produce an escalation of tual disability. The results indicated that the
the target behavior or the emergence of aggressive prompting produced higher rates of engagement
behaviors, which can be counterproductive. Given with a preferred stimulus during treatment while
the side effects of punishment and its aversive being associated with lower levels of nonsocially
nature, professionals have an ethical obligation to reinforced face touching. In a comprehensive
limit its use and prioritize the least restrictive inter- study of NCR, Rapp et al. (2013) compared the
vention procedures (Vollmer et al., 2011). effects of matched and unmatched stimuli on the
Punishment-­based procedures should always be vocal stereotypy of 21 children with ASD. In
combined with other interventions and be used their sample, providing matched stimuli noncon-
only when alternatives are unavailable or ineffec- tingently reduced vocal stereotypy in 8 of 11 par-
tive. An additional limitation specific to RIRD ticipants whereas unmatched stimuli produced
should also be noted. In two recent studies, reductions in only 1 of 10 participants. Moreover,
researchers have shown that the success of punish- NCR produced increases in collateral forms of
ment-based RIRD may be an artifact of the mea- motor stereotypy in 8 of 14 participants.
surement procedures (Carroll & Kodak, 2014; The implementation of NCR with nonsocially
Wunderlich & Vollmer, 2015). That is, uninter- reinforced challenging behaviors has similar
rupted measurement of stereotypy suggests that advantages to those maintained by social rein-
RIRD does not necessarily reduce overall levels of forcement: the treatment produces rapid
stereotypy. Thus, practitioners should carefully ­reductions in the target behavior and is easy to
monitor its effects or consider other types of pun- implement. The intervention also has some dif-
ishment contingencies to reduce engagement in ferent disadvantages when it comes to challeng-
nonsocially reinforced challenging behaviors. ing behaviors maintained by nonsocial
reinforcement. The preferred stimulus is gener-
NCR  Based on our literature search, NCR is one ally provided on a continuous basis, which may
of the treatments with the most empirical support interfere or be incompatible with engagement in
for the treatment of nonsocially reinforced chal- other important behaviors (e.g., completing
lenging behaviors in individuals with ASD (e.g., tasks). As indicated previously, even when NCR
Britton, Carr, Landaburu, & Romick, 2002; reduces one form of nonsocially reinforced chal-
Luiselli, Ricciardi, Zubow, & Laster, 2004; Rapp lenging behaviors, it may be replaced by other
et al., 2013; Reid, Parsons, & Lattimore, 2010; untargeted forms (Rapp et al., 2013). To address
Saylor, Sidener, Reeve, Fetherston, & Progar, this limitation while also increasing interactions
2012). For nonsocially reinforced behaviors, a with the preferred stimulus, some researchers
preferred item is generally provided on a continu- recommend combining the intervention with a
ous basis. This preferred stimulus may either be prompting procedure for appropriate behaviors
matched or unmatched to the stimulation gener- (Britton et al., 2002; Lanovaz et al., 2014).
ated by the nonsocially reinforced behavior
(Rapp, 2007). An example of matched stimulus Self-management  Individuals with ASD may
for vocal stereotypy is music as both the chal- also manage their own intervention to reduce
lenging behavior and music produce auditory engagement in challenging behaviors. Self-­
stimulation. In contrast, an unmatched stimulus management procedures generally consist of a
158 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

combination of awareness training, self-­recording plexity of teaching the recording procedures, self-
of the challenging behaviors, and delivery of management may not be an option for most
reinforcement for meeting preset goals
­ individuals with an associated moderate, severe,
(e.g., Crutchfield, Mason, Chambers, Wills, & or profound intellectual disability.
Mason, 2015; Fritz, Iwata, Rolider, Camp, &
Neidert, 2012; Shabani, Wilder, & Flood, 2001; Differential Reinforcement of Other Behavior
Stahmer & Schreibman, 1992; Tiger, Fisher, & (DRO)  Researchers have repeatedly shown that
Bouxsein, 2009). For the differential reinforcement DRO may be an effective treatment for reducing
component, a preferred stimulus is typically pro- engagement in nonsocially reinforced challeng-
vided for not engaging in challenging behaviors for ing behaviors (e.g., Lanovaz & Argumedes,
specific periods of time (as in a DRO schedule) or 2010; Rozenblat, Brown, Brown, Reeve, &
for accurate recording (as in a DRA schedule). The Reeve, 2009; Taylor, Hoch, & Weisman, 2005;
reinforcer may be self-managed (i.e., self-delivered Vollmer et al., 1995). In general, DRO consists of
by the individual with ASD) or delivered by some- providing a reinforcer contingent on the absence
one else. Based on data from 19 individuals with of challenging behaviors. The schedule can be
ASD from 11 different studies, self-management either momentary or based on an interval. During
was effective with 95% [75%, 99%] of individuals momentary DRO, the reinforcer is provided if the
with ASD with whom it was attempted. challenging behavior is not occurring at a specific
Fritz et al. (2012) compared the effects of dif- point in time, whereas, during interval-based
ferential reinforcement for self-recording and for DRO, the behavior must not occur during an
not engaging in nonsocially reinforced stereo- entire interval of a specified duration in order to
typy in two adults and one boy with ASD. Their provide the reinforcer. If the behavior occurs dur-
results indicated that the reinforcement of self-­ ing the latter, the time interval is reset by the
recording was effective for only one participant trainer. Minimally, the interval of the
whereas reinforcement needed to be provided for ­reinforcement schedule must be equal or shorter
not engaging in challenging behaviors for the than the average time between two occurrences
remaining two participants to produce reductions of the challenging behavior. The success rate of
to near-zero levels. In a recent study, Crutchfield DRO in the research literature currently stands at
et al. (2015) used the I-Connect, a self-­monitoring 81% [57%, 93%] for 16 participants with ASD,
app, to reduce stereotypy in two adolescents with but the ten studies used varied interval durations.
ASD in a school setting. The use of the app pro- Taylor et al. (2005) provided access to a pre-
duced reductions in stereotypy despite the ferred musical toy in the absence of vocal stereo-
absence of planned reinforcement. It should be typy to a 6-year-old girl with ASD within a
noted that albeit less frequent, the challenging classroom setting. The intervention reduced the
behaviors still occurred on a regular basis. challenging behaviors to near-zero levels even
One of the strengths of self-management is that when the interval schedule was gradually
the practitioner is encouraging the individual to increased to 5 min. In another study, Rozenblat
manage his or her own behavior, which promotes et al. (2009) compared two DRO schedules on
independence and self-determination for individu- the nonsocially reinforced repetitive vocaliza-
als with ASD. The intervention does not always tions of three children with ASD. Their results
require a trainer, which may facilitate and increase indicated that the denser schedule reduced chal-
the frequency of its implementation. In contrast, lenging behavior to near-zero levels in all three
one of the limitations of the treatment is that some participants and that it was systematically more
studies suggest that the delivery of reinforcers by effective than the leaner schedule.
an external individual may be necessary (Fritz Although DRO is a relevant option when other
et al., 2012). A second limitation is that most stud- treatments (e.g., NCR) have failed to produce
ies have been conducted with individuals with a reductions in the target behavior, practitioners
mild or no intellectual disability. Given the com- should remain aware of two challenges when
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 159

implementing the intervention in applied settings Engaging in physical exercises produces mul-
with individuals with ASD. First, the DRO sched- tiple benefits beyond the reduction of challenging
ule may need to be very dense (e.g., 5 s or less) for behaviors, which makes it an interesting option
the treatment to initially reduce challenging behav- to consider (Bremer, Crozier, & Lloyd, 2016;
iors with high frequencies (Rozenblat et al. 2009); Sorensen & Zarrett, 2014). Notably, the interven-
the treatment may thus be too time consuming, tion may improve both the physical and mental
complex, or impractical to implement. Second, the health of individuals who engage in moderate-to-­
implementation of DRO for challenging behaviors vigorous exercises. From a practical standpoint,
that do not have a high frequency may be a chal- one of the main challenges is finding extra time
lenge with individuals who also have an intellec- to implement the intervention prior to other activ-
tual disability. As an example, if the reinforcer is ities. The intervention generally reduces nonso-
only provided once every 5 min and the trainer cially reinforced challenging behaviors to
cannot explain the contingency through the use of near-zero levels during the exercises, but practi-
rules, the DRO schedule may have no impact on tioners should note that the subsequent reduc-
the behavior targeted for reduction. tions are not generally as large (e.g., Celiberti
et al., 1997; Cuvo et al., 2001; Morrison et al.,
Physical Exercise  An antecedent-based proce- 2011). Combining physical exercise with other
dure with support to reduce engagement in nonso- empirically supported interventions may address
cially reinforced challenging behaviors is physical this concern.
exercise. Researchers have examined the effects of
multiple forms of physical exercise including
walking, jogging, swinging, cycling, and jumping Practical Considerations
on a trampoline (Celiberti, Bobo, Kelly, Harris, &
Handleman, 1997; Cuvo, May, & Post, 2001; To improve the effectiveness and maintenance of
Morrison, Roscoe, & Atwell, 2011; Neely, Rispoli, behavior changes, practitioners should consider
Gerow, & Ninci, 2015). Vigorous exercises (e.g., multiple factors when planning interventions to
jogging) may produce larger reductions than less reduce engagement in challenging behaviors.
rigorous exercises (e.g., walking; Celiberti et al., Specifically, preference assessment, stimulus
1997). Our review identified five studies with 14 control, thinning the reinforcement schedule, and
participants with ASD using exercise for reducing combining interventions are factors that practi-
nonsocially reinforced challenging behaviors; the tioners should keep in mind when implementing
success rate was 93% [69%, 99%]. most behavioral interventions. Thus, the current
For example, Morrison et al. (2011) examined section outlines important points to consider
the effects of engaging in preferred exercises for when practitioners design their treatment plans.
10 min in four individuals with ASD. The results Prior to implementing any intervention that
of their study indicated that the intervention involves a preferred stimulus or reinforcer (e.g.,
reduced both immediate and subsequent levels of NCR, DRA, DRO), practitioners should first
challenging behaviors in three of the four partici- conduct a preference assessment. The purpose of
pants. In a more recent study, Neely et al. (2015) a preference assessment is to identify the pre-
compared the effects of jumping on a trampoline ferred stimuli that will be used as part of treat-
for brief periods of time or until indicators of ment. Researchers have shown that
behavioral satiation were observed. The longer experimentally identified preferred stimuli pro-
periods of exercises (i.e., until satiation) not only duced better outcomes than less preferred stimuli
reduced engagement in stereotypy but also (Kang et al., 2013). Providing a full description
increased academic engagement for the two par- of the multiple preference assessment procedures
ticipants. The results of both these studies sug- is beyond the scope of this chapter. Nonetheless,
gest that physical exercise can maintain some of practitioners should note that the most popular
its suppressive effect following its termination. methods, according to a survey conducted by
160 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

Graff and Karsten (2012), are the paired-choice progress. Practitioners should consider these
method (Fisher et al., 1992), the multiple stimu- options when attempting to facilitate the imple-
lus with replacement method (DeLeon & Iwata, mentation of their interventions in applied
1996), and the free-operant method (Roane, settings.
Vollmer, Ringdahl, & Marcus, 1998). These As part of the chapter, we reviewed each
methods have also been adapted to assess prefer- empirically supported intervention individually
ence for stimuli other than edible and tangible for clarity. That said, treatments consisting of
items such as music (Horrocks & Higbee, 2008), multiple interventions are among the most
video recordings (Chebli & Lanovaz, 2016), and reported in the research literature (DiGennaro
social interactions (Nuernberger, Smith, Czapar, Reed et al., 2012). We encourage practitioners to
& Klatt, 2012; Smaby, MacDonald, Ahearn, & consider implementing multicomponent treat-
Dube, 2007). ments when planning and designing interven-
In certain settings, it may not be possible for tions to reduce engagement in challenging
practitioners to implement interventions that behaviors in individuals with ASD. Practitioners
require dense schedules of reinforcement or pun- should remember that adding components may
ishment across the entire day. A solution to this also make the treatment more complex and time
issue is implementing the intervention for only consuming and should thus carefully weigh the
short periods of time during the day. In these benefits and drawbacks.
cases, the intervention should include a stimulus
that signals that the intervention contingencies
are currently in place (i.e., a discriminative stim- Conclusions
ulus). For individuals with an associated intel-
lectual disability, this signal is typically a visual Multiple treatments can be considered as empiri-
cue (e.g., a colored poster or card, a bracelet) cally supported for the reduction of challenging
that the intervention is or is not being imple- behaviors in individuals with ASD. We provided
mented. Practitioners should consider including a description and a value of success rate for each
such cues within any intervention that they rec- intervention in the current chapter. Interestingly,
ommend as these may make the intervention we identified more empirically supported inter-
more effective at maintaining lower levels of ventions for nonsocially reinforced challenging
challenging behaviors (Doughty et al., 2007; behaviors than for socially reinforced behaviors.
Hanley et al., 2001). These stimuli can be gradu- One potential explanation for this discrepancy is
ally faded, which can facilitate the maintenance that the presence of repetitive behaviors, which
and generalization of behavior changes (Cooper, are generally nonsocially reinforced, is a diag-
Heron, & Heward, 2007). nostic criterion for ASD. Therefore, it should not
To make an intervention easier to maintain in be surprising that researchers have conducted
the long term, practitioners generally aim to more studies on this topic within the ASD popu-
reduce the amount of reinforcement provided lation. Another noteworthy observation is that
once an intervention has been shown to be effec- success rates for interventions for socially rein-
tive. To this end, researchers have showed that forced challenging behaviors were on average
practitioners may gradually delay reinforcement higher than those for nonsocially reinforced
or thin the reinforcement schedule to make the behaviors. The lack of direct control over the
intervention more manageable (Hanley et al., maintaining consequence may explain part of the
2014; Taylor et al., 2005). Delaying reinforce- lower success rates for challenging behaviors
ment involves waiting for increasingly longer maintained by nonsocial reinforcement. Our lit-
periods of time prior to providing the reinforcer erature search also underlines the importance of
contingent on an appropriate behavior, whereas conducting additional research on standardized
schedule thinning consists of providing the rein- treatments for reducing challenging behaviors
forcer on a leaner schedule as the clients make using controlled trials.
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 161

Our success rates should be considered as esti- Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 622–
627. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00507.x.
mates rather than absolute values. Our search was
Beavers, G. A., Iwata, B. A., & Lerman, D. C. (2013).
limited to the PsycInfo database and to a handful Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
of systematic reviews; a search of other databases of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
or using the specific names of intervention may Analysis, 46, 1–21. doi:10.1002/jaba.30.
Bloom, S. E., Iwata, B. A., Fritz, J. N., Roscoe, E. M., &
have yielded more studies. It should also be noted
Carreau, A. B. (2011). Classroom application of a trial-
that we did not assess the quality of the single-­ based functional analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior
case designs as proposed by Kratochwill et al. Analysis, 44, 19–31. doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-19.
(2013). Instead, we considered all studies that Bodfish, J. W., Symons, F. J., Parker, D. E., & Lewis,
M. H. (2000). Varieties of repetitive behavior in
used a single-case experimental design. Finally,
autism: Comparisons to mental retardation. Journal
our definition of success was based on the dem- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 237–243.
onstration of experimental control; however, doi:10.1023/a:1005596502855.
some treatments may have produced relatively Bremer, E., Crozier, M., & Lloyd, M. (2016). A system-
atic review of the behavioural outcomes following
small changes. Nevertheless, we believe that our
exercise interventions for children and youth with
results should serve as general guidelines to sup- autism spectrum disorder. Autism. Advanced online
port practitioners in the selection of treatments to publication. doi:10.1177/1362361315616002.
reduce engagement in challenging behaviors in Briss, P. A., Zaza, S., Pappaioanou, M., Fielding, J.,
Wright-De Agüero, L., Truman, B. I., ... & Harris,
individuals with ASD. As importantly, we
J. R. (2000). Developing an evidence-based guide to
emphasize that a systematic, rigorous, and func- community preventive services – methods. American
tional approach to treatment is key to success in Journal of Preventive Medicine, 18, 35–43. doi:
the reduction of challenging behaviors in this 10.1016/S0749-3797(99)00119-1.
Britton, L. N., Carr, J. E., Landaburu, H. J., & Romick,
population.
K. S. (2002). The efficacy of noncontingent rein-
forcement as treatment for automatically reinforced
stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, 17, 93–103.
References doi:10.1002/bin.110.
Brosnan, J., & Healy, O. (2011). A review of behav-
ioral interventions for the treatment of aggres-
Ahearn, W. H., Clark, K. M., MacDonald, R. P., & Chung,
sion in individuals with developmental disabilities.
B. I. (2007). Assessing and treating vocal stereotypy
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 437–446.
in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.12.023.
Analysis, 40, 263–275. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.30-06.
Carr, E. G. (1977). The motivation of self-inju-
Anderson, J., & Le, D. D. (2011). Abatement of intractable
rious behavior: A review of some hypoth-
vocal stereotypy using an overcorrection procedure.
eses. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 800–816.
Behavioral Interventions, 26, 134–146. doi:10.1002/
doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.4.800.
bin.326.
Carr, J. E., Severtson, J. M., & Lepper, T. L. (2009).
Anderson, C. M., & Long, E. S. (2002). Use of a struc-
Noncontingent reinforcement is an empirically sup-
tured descriptive assessment methodology to iden-
ported treatment for problem behavior exhibited
tify variables affecting problem behavior. Journal of
by individuals with developmental disabilities.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 137–154. doi:10.1901/
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 44–57.
jaba.2002.35-137.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2008.03.002.
Applegate, H. J. E., Matson, J. L., & Cherry, K. E. (1999).
Carr, E. G., & Smith, C. E. (1995). Biological set-
An evaluation of functional variables affecting severe
ting events for self-injury. Mental Retardation and
problem behaviors in adults with mental retardation
Developmental Disabilities, 1, 94–98. doi:10.1002/
by using the questions about behavioral function scale
mrdd.1410010204.
(QABF). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20,
Carr, E. G., Smith, C. E., Giacin, T. A., Whelan,
229–237. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(99)00005-0.
B. M., & Pancari, J. (2003). Menstrual discom-
Arron, K., Oliver, C., Moss, J., Berg, K., & Burbidge, C.
fort as a biological setting event for severe problem
(2011). The prevalence and phenomenology of self-­
behavior: Assessment and intervention. American
injurious and aggressive behaviour in genetic syn-
Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 117–133.
dromes. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research,
doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2003)108<0117:MDAABS>
55, 109–120. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01337.x.
2.0.CO;2.
Baghdadli, A., Pascal, C., Grisi, S., & Aussilloux, C.
Carroll, R. A., & Kodak, T. (2014). An evaluation of inter-
(2003). Risk factors for self-injurious behaviours
rupted and uninterrupted measurement of vocal ste-
among 222 young children with autistic disorders.
reotypy on perceived treatment outcomes. Journal of
162 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

Applied Behavior Analysis, 47, 264–276. doi:10.1002/ adults with profound mental retardation. Research in
jaba.118. Developmental Disabilities, 22, 183–204.
Celiberti, D. A., Bobo, H. E., Kelly, K. S., Harris, DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluation of a
S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (1997). The differential multiple-stimulus presentation format for assessing
and temporal effects of antecedent exercise on the reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior
self-stimulatory behavior of a child with autism. Analysis, 29, 519–533. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-519.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 139–150. DiGennaro Reed, F. D., Hirst, J. M., & Hyman, S. R.
doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(96)00032-7. (2012). Assessment and treatment of stereotypic
Chambless, D. L., Baker, M. J., Baucom, D. H., Beutler, behavior in children with autism and other develop-
L. E., Calhoun, K. S., Crits-Christoph, P., ... & Woody, mental disabilities: A thirty year review. Research in
S. R. (1998). Update on empirically validated thera- Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 422–430. doi:10.1016/j.
pies II. The Clinical Psychologist, 51, 3–16. rasd.2011.07.003.
Chebli, S. S., & Lanovaz, M. J. (2016). Using computer Dominguez, A., Wilder, D. A., Cheung, K., & Rey, C.
tablets to assess preference for videos in children (2014). The use of verbal reprimand to decrease rumi-
with autism. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 9, 50–53. nation in a child with autism. Behavioral Interventions,
doi:10.1007/s40617-016-0109-0. 29, 339–345. doi:10.1002/bin.1390.
Chebli, S. S., Martin, V., & Lanovaz, M. J. (2016). Dominick, K. C., Davis, N. O., Lainhart, J., Tager-­
Prevalence of stereotypy in individuals with devel- Flusberg, H., & Folstein, S. (2007). Atypical behav-
opmental disabilities: A systematic review. Review iors in children with autism and children with a history
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 3, of language impairment. Research in Developmental
107–118. doi:10.1007/s40489-016-0069-x . Disabilities, 28, 145–162. doi:10.1016/j.
Chowdhury, M., & Benson, B. A. (2011). Use of differ- ridd.2006.02.003.
ential reinforcement to reduce behavior problems in Doughty, S. S., Anderson, C. M., Doughty, A. H., Williams,
adults with intellectual disabilities: A methodological D. C., & Saunders, K. J. (2007). Discriminative con-
review. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32, trol of punished stereotyped behavior in humans.
383–394. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.015. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87,
Conners, J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S. W., Hanley, 325–336. doi:10.1901/jeab.2007.39-05.
G. P., Worsdell, A. S., & Thompson, R. H. (2000). Douglas, O. L. (2004). Menstrually related self-injurious
Differential responding in the presence and absence of behavior in adolescents with autism. Journal of the
discriminative stimuli during multielement functional American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 43, 1193. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000135624.89971.d1.
299–308. doi:10.1901/jaba.2000.33-299. Dracobly, J. D., & Smith, R. G. (2012). Progressing
Cook, J. L., Rapp, J. T., Gomes, L. A., Frazer, T. J., & from identification and functional analysis of precur-
Lindblad, T. L. (2014). Effects of verbal reprimands sor behavior to treatment of self-injurious behavior.
on targeted and untargeted stereotypy. Behavioral Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 361–374.
Interventions, 29, 106–124. doi:10.1002/bin.1378. doi:10.1901/jaba.2012.45-361.
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Dunlap, G., Strain, P. S., Fox, L., Carta, J. J., Conroy,
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle M., Smith, B. J., ... & Sailor, W. (2006). Prevention
River, NJ: Pearson. and intervention with young children’s challenging
Crosland, K., Zarcone, J. R., Schroeder, S., Zarcone, T., behavior: Perspectives regarding current knowledge.
& Fowler, S. (2005). Use of antecedent analysis and a Behavioral Disorders, 32, 29–45.
force sensitive platform to compare stereotyped move- Emerson, E., Kiernan, C., Alborz, A., Reeves, D., Mason,
ments and motor tics. American Journal on Mental H., Swarbrick, R.,. .. Hatton, C. (2001). The preva-
Retardation, 110, 181–192. doi:10.1352/08958017(2 lence of challenging behaviors: A total population
005)110<181:UOAAAA>2.0.CO;2. study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22,
Crutchfield, S. A., Mason, R. A., Chambers, A., Wills, H. P., 77–93. doi: 10.1016/S0891-4222(00)00061-5
& Mason, B. A. (2015). Use of a self-­monitoring appli- English, C. L., & Anderson, C. M. (2006). Evaluating
cation to reduce stereotypic behavior in adolescents the treatment utility of the analog functional analysis
with autism: A preliminary investigation of I-connect. and the structured descriptive assessment. Journal of
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, Positive Behavior Interventions, 8, 212–229. doi:10.1
1146–1155. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2272-x. 177/10983007060080040401.
Cunningham, A. B., & Schreibman, L. (2008). Stereotypy Farmer, C. A., & Aman, M. G. (2011). Aggressive behav-
in autism: The importance of function. Research in ior in a sample of children with autism spectrum dis-
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 469–479. doi:10.1016/j. orders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
rasd.2007.09.006. 317–323. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.04.014.
Cuvo, A. J., May, M. E., & Post, T. M. (2001). Effects Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1997).
of living room, Snoezelen room, and outdoor activi- Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as
ties on stereotypic behavior and engagement by treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 163

Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249. doi:10.1901/ Hanley, G. P., Jin, C. S., Vanselow, N. R., & Hanratty,
jaba.1997.30-239. L. A. (2014). Producing meaningful improvements
Fisher, W. W., DeLeon, I. G., Rodriguez-Catter, V., & in problem behavior of children with autism via syn-
Keeney, K. M. (2004). Enhancing the effects of thesized analyses and treatments. Journal of Applied
extinction on attention-maintained behavior through Behavior Analysis, 47, 16–36. doi:10.1002/jaba.106.
noncontingent delivery of attention or stimuli identi- Harrop, C., McBee, M., & Boyd, B. A. (2016). How
fied via a competing stimulus assessment. Journal of are child restricted and repetitive behaviors associ-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 171–184. doi:10.1901/ ated with caregiver stress over time? A parallel pro-
jaba.2004.37-171. cess multilevel growth model. Journal of Autism and
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, Developmental Disabilities. Advance online publica-
L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A comparison tion. doi:10.1007/s10803-016-2707-7.
of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for per- Heath, A. K., Ganz, J. B., Parker, R., Burke, M., & Ninci,
sons with severe and profound disabilities. Journal of J. (2015). A meta-analytic review of functional com-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498. doi:10.1901/ munication training across mode of communication,
jaba.1992.25-491. age, and disability. Review Journal of Autism and
Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Hammond, J. L., & Bloom, Developmental Disorders, 2, 155–166. doi:10.1007/
S. E. (2013). Experimental analysis of precursors to s40489-014-0044-3.
severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Herscovitch, B., Roscoe, E. M., Libby, M. E., Bourret,
Analysis, 46, 101–129. doi:10.1002/jaba.27. J. C., & Ahearn, W. H. (2009). A procedure for iden-
Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Rolider, N. U., Camp, E. M., tifying precursors to problem behavior. Journal of
& Neidert, P. L. (2012). Analysis of self-recording in Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 697–702. doi:10.1901/
self-management interventions for stereotypy. Journal jaba.2009.42-697.
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 55–68. doi:10.1901/ Horner, R. H., & Carr, E. G. (1997). Behavioral sup-
jaba.2012.45-55. port for students with severe disabilities: Functional
Graff, R. B., & Karsten, A. M. (2012). Assessing prefer- assessment and comprehensive intervention.
ences of individuals with developmental disabilities: The Journal of Special Education, 31, 84–104.
A survey of current practices. Behavior Analysis in doi:10.1177/002246699703100108.
Practice, 5, 37–48. Horner, R., Day, M. H., & Day, J. R. (1997). Using
Hagopian, L. P., Crockett, J. L., Stone, M., DeLeon, I. G., neutralizing routines to reduce problem behaviors.
& Bowman, L. G. (2000). Effects of noncontingent Journal of Applied Behavior Anlaysis, 30, 601–614.
reinforcement on problem behavior and stimulus doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-601.
engagement: The role of satiation, extinction, and Horrocks, E., & Higbee, T. S. (2008). An evaluation of
alternative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior a stimulus preference assessment of auditory stim-
Analysis, 33, 433–449. doi:10.1901/jaba.2000.33-433. uli for adolescents with developmental disabilities.
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., & Legacy, S. M. (1994). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 29, 11–20.
Schedule effects of noncontingent reinforcement on doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2006.09.003.
attention-maintained destructive behavior in identical Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S., & Hausman, N. L. (2008).
quadruplets. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Some effects of noncontingent positive reinforce-
27, 317–325. doi:10.1901/jaba.1994.27-317. ment on multiply controlled problem behavior and
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Sullivan, M. T., Acquisto, compliance in a demand context. Journal of Applied
J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness of func- Behavior Analysis, 41, 435–440. doi:10.1901/
tional communication training with and without jaba.2008.41-435.
extinction and punishment: A summary of 21 inpatient Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Roscoe, E. M. (2013).
cases. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 211– Reliability and validity of the functional analysis
235. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-211. screening tool. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Hamilton, A., Marshal, M. P., & Murray, P. J. (2011). 46, 271–284. doi:10.1002/jaba.31.
Autism spectrum disorders and menstruation. Journal Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
of Adolescent Health, 49, 442–445. doi:10.1016/j. & Richman, G. S. (1994a). Toward a functional
jadohealth.2011.01.015. analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Analysis, 27, 197–209. doi:10.1901/jaba.1994.27-
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. ­197. (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention in
Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 36, 147–185. Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982).
doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-147. Iwata, B. A., & Dozier, C. L. (2008). Clinical applica-
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001). tion of functional analysis methodology. Behavior
Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment Analysis in Practice, 1, 3–9.
with functional communication training. Journal of Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. doi:10.1901/ Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., Rodgers, T. A., …
jaba.2001.34-17. Willis, K. D. (1994b). The functions of self-injurious
164 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

behavior: An experimental epidemiological analysis. Behavior Analysis, 30, 127–137. doi:10.1901/


Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240. jaba.1997.30-127.
doi: 10.1901/jaba.1994.27-215. Langthorne, P., & McGill, P. (2009). A tutorial on the con-
Jarmolowicz, D. P., DeLeon, I. G., & Kuhn, S. A. C. cept of the motivating operation and its importance to
(2009). Functional communication during sig- application. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 2, 22–311.
naled reinforcement and/or extinction. Behavioral Langthorne, P., McGill, P., & Oliver, C. (2014). The moti-
Interventions, 24, 265–273. doi:10.1002/bin.288. vating operation and negatively reinforced problem
Jones, R. S. P., Wint, D., & Ellis, N. C. (1990). The behavior : A systematic review. Behavior Modification,
social effects of stereotyped behavior. Journal 38, 107–159. doi:10.1177/0145445513509649.
of Intellectual Disability Research, 34, 261–268. Lanovaz, M. J., & Argumedes, M. (2010). Immediate
doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.1990.tb01537.x. and subsequent effects of differential reinforcement
Kahng, S., Hausman, N. L., & Jann, K. E. (2011). Self-­ of other behavior and noncontingent matched stimu-
injurious behavior. In J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Teaching lation on stereotypy. Behavioral Interventions, 25,
and behavior support for children and adults with 229–238. doi:10.1002/bin.308.
autism spectrum disorders: A practitioner’s guide Lanovaz, M. J., Argumedes, M., Roy, D., Duquette, J. R.,
(pp. 136–142). New York, NY: Oxford University & Watkins, N. (2013). Using ABC narrative recording
Press. to identify the function of problem behavior: A pilot
Kang, S., Lang, R. B., O’Reilly, M. F., Davis, T. N., study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34,
Machalicek, W., Rispoli, M. J., & Chan, J. M. (2010). 2734–2742. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.05.038.
Problem behavior during preference assessments: An Lanovaz, M. J., & Rapp, J. T. (2016). Using single-case
empirical analysis and practical recommendations. experiments to support evidence-based decisions:
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 137–141. How much is enough? Behavior Modification, 40,
doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-137. 377–395. doi:10.1177/0145445515613584 .
Kang, S., O’Reilly, M., Lancioni, G., Falcomata, T. S., Lanovaz, M. J., Rapp, J. T., & Fletcher, S. M. (2010).
Sigafoos, J., & Xu, Z. (2013). Comparison of the Expanding functional analysis of automatically rein-
predictive validity and consistency among preference forced behavior using a three-component multiple
assessment procedures: A review of the literature. schedule. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11,
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 1125– 17–27.
1133. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.021. Lanovaz, M. J., Rapp, J. T., Maciw, I., Prégent-Pelletier,
Kennedy, C. H., & Meyer, K. A. (1996). Sleep deprivation, E., Dorion, C., Ferguson, S., & Saade, S. (2014).
allergy symptoms, and negatively reinforced problem Effects of multiple interventions for reducing vocal
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, stereotypy: Developing a sequential intervention
133–135. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-133. model. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8,
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R. H., Levin, 529–545. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.01.009.
J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, Lanovaz, M. J., Robertson, K. M., Soerono, K., & Watkins,
W. R. (2010). Single-case designs technical documen- N. (2013). Effects of reducing stereotypy on other
tation. Retrieved from http://ies.ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/ behaviors: A systematic review. Research in Autism
wwc_scd.pdf Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1234–1243. doi:10.1016/j.
Kratochwill, T. R., Hitchcock, J., Horner, R. H., Levin, rasd.2013.07.009.
J. R., Odom, S. L., Rindskopf, D. M., & Shadish, Lanovaz, M. J., & Sladeczek, I. E. (2012). Vocal stereotypy
W. R. (2013). Single-case intervention research design in children with autism spectrum disorders: A review
standards. Remedial and Special Education, 34, of behavioral interventions. Behavior Modification,
26–38. doi:10.1177/0741932512452794. 36, 146–164. doi:10.1177/0145445511427192.
Kuhn, D. E., Chirighin, A. E., & Zelenka, K. (2010). Laraway, S., Snycerksi, S., Olson, R., Becker, B., &
Discriminated functional communication: A proce- Poling, A. (2014). The motivating operations con-
dural extension of functional communication training. cept: Current status and critical response. The
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 249–264. Psychological Record, 64, 601–623. doi:10.1007/
doi:10.1901/jaba.2010.43-249. s40732-014-0080-5.
Kurtz, P. F., Boelter, E. W., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Chin, Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003).
M. D., & Hagopian, L. P. (2011). An analysis of Motivating operations and terms to describe them:
functional communication training as an empirically Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior
supported treatment for problem behavior displayed Analysis, 36, 407–414. doi:10.1901/jaba.2003.36-407.
by individuals with intellectual disabilities. Research Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact
in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2935–2942. of behaviour problems on caregiver stress in young
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2010.11.015. people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal
Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 172–183.
reinforcement as treatment for severe problem behav- doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00732.x.
ior: Some procedural variations. Journal of Applied
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 165

Lerman, D. C., Hovanetz, A., Strobel, M., & Tetreault, the DASH-II and Vineland adaptive behavior scales.
A. (2009). Accuracy of teacher-collected descriptive Research in Developmental Disabilities, 18, 471–476.
analysis data: A comparison of narrative and structured doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(97)00023-1.
recording formats. Journal of Behavioral Education, Matson, J. L., & LoVullo, S. V. (2008). A review of behav-
18, 157–172. doi:10.1007/s10864-009-9084-7. ioral treatment for self-injurious behaviors of persons
Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the sta- with autism spectrum disorders. Behavior Modification,
tus of knowledge for using punishment: Implications 32, 61–76. doi:10.1177/0145445507304581.
for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. (2007). Assessing
Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464. doi:10.1901/ challenging behaviors in children with autism spec-
jaba.2002.35-431. trum disorders: A review. Research and Developmental
Lovaas, I., Newson, C., & Hickman, C. (1987). Self-­ Disabilities, 28, 567–579. doi:10.1016/j.
stimulatory behavior and perceptual reinforcement. ridd.2006.08.001.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 45–68. Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008). Characteristics of
doi:10.1901/jaba.1987.20-45. challenging behaviours in adults with autistic disor-
Luiselli, J. K., Cochran, M. L., & Huber, S. A. (2005). der, PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal of
Effects of otitis media on a child with autism receiv- Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 33, 323–329.
ing behavioral intervention for self-injury. Child & doi:10.1080/13668250802492600.
Family Behavior Therapy, 27, 51–56. doi:10.1300/ Matson, J. L., & Shoemaker, M. (2009). Intellectual dis-
J019v27n02_05. ability and its relationship to autism spectrum dis-
Luiselli, J. K., Ricciardi, J. N., Zubow, M., & Laster, K. orders. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
(2004). Practice of an alternative behavior as interven- 1107–1114. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2009.06.003.
tion for object stereotypy: Comparison of contingent Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. R. (1995). The questions
and noncontingent implementation across evoking about behavioral function (QABF) user’s guide. Baton
stimuli. The Behavior Analyst Today, 5, 304–312. Rouge, LA: Scientific Publishers.
doi:10.1037/h0100037. Mazurek, M. O., Kanne, S. M., & Wodka, E. L. (2013).
Luiselli, J. K., Wolongevicz, J., Egan, P., Amirault, Physical aggression in children and adolescents
D., Sciaraffa, N., & Treml, T. (1999). The Family with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
support program: Description of a preventive, Spectrum Disorders, 7, 455–465. doi:10.1016/j.
community-­ based behavioral intervention for chil- rasd.2012.11.004.
dren with pervasive developmental disorders. Child McClean, B., & Grey, I. (2012). An evaluation of an inter-
& Family Behavior Therapy, 21, 1–18. doi:10.1300/ vention sequence outline in positive behaviour support
J019v21n01_01. for people with autism and severe escape-motivated
MacDonald, R., Green, G., Mansfield, R., Geckeler, A., challenging behaviour. Journal of Intellectual and
Gardenier, N., Anderson, J., … Sanchez, J. (2007). Developmental Disability, 37, 209–220. doi:10.3109
Stereotypy in young children with autism and typi- /13668250.2012.704982.
cally developing children. Research in Developmental McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark-
Disabilities, 28, 266–277. doi:10.1016/j. ers associated with challenging behaviours in people
ridd.2006.01.004. with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study.
Mandell, D. S. (2008). Psychiatric hospitalization among Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 405–
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of 416. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00517.x.
Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 38, 1059– McGill, P., Hughes, D., Teer, K., & Rye, L. (2001).
1065. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0481-2. Variability in staff reports of the frequency of challeng-
Marcus, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Swanson, V., Roane, H. R., ing behavior. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
& Ringdahl, J. E. (2001). An experimental analysis 22, 221–231. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(01)00069-5.
of aggression. Behavior Modification, 25, 189–213. McGill, P., Teer, K., Rye, L., & Hughes, D. (2003). Staff
doi:10.1177/0145445501252002. reports of setting events associated with challeng-
Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Cherry, K. E., & Paclawskyj, ing behavior. Behavior Modification, 27, 265–282.
T. R. (1999). A validity study on the questions about doi:10.1177/0145445503251604.
behavioral function (QABF) scale: Predicting treat- McTiernan, A., Leader, G., Healy, O., & Mannion, A.
ment success for self-injury, aggression, and stereo- (2011). Analysis of risk factors and early predictors of
typies. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 20, challenging behavior for children with autism spec-
163–175. doi:10.1016/S0891-4222(98)00039-0. trum disorder. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J., Rojahn, J., & Hess, J. (2009). 5, 1215–1222. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.009.
A factor analysis of challenging behaviors assessed Mello, N. K., Mendelson, J. H., & Kuehnle, J. C. (1982).
with the baby and infant screen for children with Buprenorphine effects on human heroin self-­administration:
autism traits (BISCUIT-part 3). Research in Autism An operant analysis. Journal of Pharmacology and
Spectrum Disorders, 3, 714–722. doi:10.1016/j. Experimental Therapeutics, 223, 30–39.
rasd.2009.01.008. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discrimina-
Matson, J. L., Kiely, S., & Bamburg, J. (1997). The effect tive and motivational functions of stimuli. Journal of
of stereotypies on adaptive skills as assessed with
166 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-149. 29, 75–81. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e31815f24f7.
Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Nuernberger, J. E., Smith, C. A., Czapar, K. N., & Klatt,
Analyst, 16, 191–206. K. P. (2012). Assessing preference for social interac-
Michael, J. (2000). Implications and refinements of the tion in children diagnosed with autism. Behavioral
establishing operation concept. Journal of Applied Interventions, 27, 33–44. doi:10.1002/bin.1336.
Behavior Analysis, 33, 401–410. doi:10.1901/ O’Reilly, M. F. (1995). Functional analysis and treatment
jaba.2000.33-401. of escape-maintained aggression correlated with sleep
Minshawi, N. F., Hurwitz, S., Fodstad, J. C., Biebl, S., deprivation. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28,
Morris, D. H., & McDougle, C. J. (2014). The associa- 225–226. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-225.
tion between self-injurious behaviors and autism spec- O’Reilly, M. F. (1997). Functional analysis of episodic
trum disorders. Psychology Research and Behavior self-injury correlated with recurrent otitis media.
Management, 7, 125–136. doi:10.2147/PRBM. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 165–167.
S44635. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-165.
Minshawi, N. F., Hurwitz, S., Morris, D., & McDougle, O’Reilly, M. F., Edrisinha, C., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni, G.,
C. J. (2014). Multidisciplinary assessment and treat- & Andrews, A. (2006). Isolating the evocative and
ment of self-injurious behavior in autism spectrum abative effects of an establishing operation on chal-
disorder and intellectual disability: Integration of lenging behavior. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 195–
psychological and biological theory and approach. 204. doi:10.1002/bin.215.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 45, O’Reilly, M. K., Richman, D. M., Lancioni, G., Hilery,
1541–1568. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2307-3. J., Lindauer, S., Crosland, K., & Lacey, C. (2000).
Mitter, D. R., Romani, P. W., Greer, B. D., & Fisher, Using brief functional assessments to identify specific
W. W. (2015). Assessment and treatment of pica contexts for problem behavior maintained by posi-
and destruction of holiday decorations. Journal of tive and negative reinforcement. European Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 912–917. doi:10.1002/ Behavior Analysis, 1, 135–142. doi:10.1080/1502114
jaba.255. 9.2000.11434164.
Morrison, H., Roscoe, E. M., & Atwell, A. (2011). An eval- Olive, M. L., Lang, R. B., & Davis, T. N. (2008). An
uation of antecedent exercise on behavior maintained analysis of the effects of functional communication
by automatic reinforcement using a three-­component and a voice output communication aid for a child
multiple schedule. Journal of Applied Behavior with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism
Analysis, 44, 523–541. doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-523. Spectrum Disorders, 2, 223–236. doi:10.1016/j.
Murphy, O., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2009). Risk fac- rasd.2007.06.002.
tors for challenging behaviors among 157 chil- Oliver, A. C., Pratt, L. A., & Normand, M. P. (2015). A
dren with autism spectrum disorder in Ireland. survey of functional behavior assessment methods used
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 474–482. by behavior analysts in practice. Journal of Applied
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2008.09.008. Behavior Analysis, 48, 817–829. doi:10.1002/jaba.256.
Najdowski, A. C., Wallace, M. D., Ellsworth, C. L., Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls, Y.,
MacAleese, A. N., & Cleveland, J. M. (2008). & Vollmer, T. R. (2000). Questions about behavioral
Functional analyses and treatment of precursor behav- function (QABF): A behavioral checklist for func-
ior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 97–105. tional assessment of aberrant behavior. Research in
doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-97. Developmental Disabilities, 21, 223–229. doi:10.1016/
Neely, L., Rispoli, M., Gerow, S., & Ninci, J. (2015). Effects S0891-4222(00)00036-6.
of antecedent exercise on academic engagement and Paclawskyj, T. R., Matson, J. L., Rush, K. S., Smalls,
stereotypy during instruction. Behavior Modification, Y., & Vollmer, T. R. (2001). Assessment of the con-
39, 98–116. doi:10.1177/0145445514552891. vergent validity of the questions about behavioral
Neidert, P. L., Iwata, B. A., Dempsey, C. M., & Thomason-­ function (QABF) scale with analogue functional anal-
Sassi, J. L. (2013). Latency of response during the ysis and the motivation assessment scale. Journal of
functional analysis of elopement. Journal of Applied Intellectual Intellectual Disability Research, 45, 484–
Behavior Analysis, 46, 312–316. doi:10.1002/jaba.11. 494. doi:10.1046/j.1365–2788.2001.00364.x.
Newman, D. W., Summerhill, L., Mosley, E., & Tooth, C. Pence, S. T., Roscoe, E. M., Bourret, J. C., & Ahearn,
(2003). Working with an adult male with Down’s syn- W. H. (2009). The relative contributions of three
drome, autism and challenging behaviour: Evaluation descriptive methods: Implications for behavioral
of a programme of staff support and organizational assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42,
change. British Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31, 425–446. doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-425.
85–90. doi:10.1046/j.1468-3156.2003.00227.x. Peters, L. C., & Thompson, R. H. (2013). Some indirect
Nickels, K. C., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, effects of positive practice overcorrection. Journal of
A. L., Voigt, R. G., & Barbaresi, W. J. (2009). Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 613–625. doi:10.1002/
Stimulant medication treatment of target behaviors jaba.63.
in children with autism: A population-based study. Petscher, E. S., Rey, C., & Bailey, J. S. (2009). A review
of empirical support for differential reinforcement
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 167

of alternative behavior. Research in Developmental Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 243–245. doi:10.1901/
Disabilities, 30, 409–425. doi:10.1016/j. jaba.2005.42-04.
ridd.2008.08.008. Reid, D. H., Parsons, M. B., & Lattimore, L. P. (2010).
Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996). Designing and evaluating assessment-based interven-
Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior and tions to reduce stereotypy among adults with autism in
demand fading in the treatment of escape-maintained a community job. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3, 27.
destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Richards, C., Oliver, C., Nelson, L., & Moss, J. (2012).
Analysis, 29, 569–572. doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-569. Self-injurious behaviour in individuals with autism
Plumet, M.-H., & Veneziano, E. (2014). Typical and spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Journal
atypical pragmatic functioning of ASD children of Intellectual Disability Research, 56, 476–489.
and their partners: A study of oppositional episodes doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01537.x.
in everyday interactions. Journal of Autism and Richman, D. M., Barnard-Brak, L., Grubb, L., Bosch, A.,
Developmental Disorders, 45, 53–67. doi:10.1007/ & Abby, L. (2015). Meta-analysis of noncontingent
s10803-014-2164-0. reinforcement effects on problem behavior. Journal of
Querim, A. C., Iwata, B. A., Roscoe, E. M., Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 131–152. doi:10.1002/
Schlichenmeyer, K. J., Ortega, J. V., & Hurl, K. E. jaba.189.
(2013). Functional analysis screening for problem Richman, D. M., Wacker, D. P., Asmus, J. M., & Casey,
behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement. S. D. (1998). Functional analysis and extinction of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 46, 47–60. different behavior problems exhibited by the same
doi:10.1002/jaba.26. individual. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,
Rapp, J. T. (2007). Further evaluation of methods to iden- 475–478. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-475.
tify matched stimulation. Journal of Applied Behavior Ringdahl, J. E., Kitsukawa, K., Andelman, M. S., Call,
Analysis, 40, 73–88. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.142-05. N., Winborn, L., Barretto, A., & Reed, G. K. (2002).
Rapp, J. T. (2008). Conjugate reinforcement: A brief Differential reinforcement with and without instruc-
review and suggestions for applications to the assess- tional fading. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
ment of automatically reinforced behavior. Behavioral 35, 291–294. doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-291.
Interventions, 23, 113–136. doi:10.1002/bin.259. Rispoli, M., Camargo, S., Machalicek, W., Lang, R., &
Rapp, J. T., & Lanovaz, M. J. (2016). Stereotypy. In N. N. Sigafoos, J. (2014). Functional communication train-
Singh (Ed.), Clinical handbook of evidence-based ing in the treatment of problem behavior maintained
practices for individuals with intellectual and devel- by access to rituals. Journal of Applied Behavior
opmental disabilities. New York, NY: Springer. Analysis, 47, 580–593. doi:10.1002/jaba.130.
Rapp, J. T., Swanson, G., & Dornbusch, K. (2007). Roane, H. S., & Kadey, H. J. (2011). Aggression and
Temporary increases in problem behavior and destruction. In J. K. Luiselli (Ed.), Teaching and
sleep disruption following decreases in medi- behavior support for children and adults with autism
cation: A descriptive analysis of conditional spectrum disorders: A practitioner’s guide (pp. 143–
rates. Behavior Modification, 31, 825–846. 150). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
doi:10.1177/0145445507301653. Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus,
Rapp, J. T., Swanson, G., Sheridan, S., Enloe, K., Maltese, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief stimulus preference
D., Sennott, L., … Lanovaz, M. J. (2013). Immediate assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31,
and subsequent effects of matched and unmatched 605–620. doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-605.
stimuli on targeted vocal stereotypy and untargeted Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Lott, D., Esbensen, A. J., &
motor stereotypy. Behavior Modification, 37, 543– Smalls, Y. (2001). The behavior problems inventory:
567. doi: 10.1177/0145445512461650 An instrument for the assessment of self-injury, ste-
Rapp, J. T., & Vollmer, T. R. (2005). Stereotypy I: A reotyped behavior, and aggression/destruction in indi-
review of behavioral assessment and treatment. viduals with developmental disabilities. Journal of
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 26, 527–547. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 577–588.
doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.11.005. doi:10.1023/A:1013299028321.
Rapp, J. T., Vollmer, T. R., St. Peter, C., Dozier, C. L., Rojahn, J., Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J. C., Knight,
& Cotnoir, N. M. (2004). Analysis of response alloca- C., Sevin, J. A., & Sharp, B. (2009). Cutoffs, norms,
tion in individuals with multiple forms of stereotyped and patterns of problem behaviors in children with an
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, ASD on the baby and infant screen for children with
481–501. doi:10.1901/jaba.2004.37-481. aUtIsm traits (BISCUIT-part 3). Research in Autism
Rattaz, C., Michelon, C., & Baghdadli, A. (2015). Spectrum Disorders, 3, 989–998. doi:10.1016/j.
Symptom severity as a risk factor for self-injurious rasd.2009.06.002.
behaviours in adolescents with autism spectrum dis- Rojahn, J., Wilkins, J., Matson, J. L., & Boisjoli, J. (2010). A
orders. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 59, comparison of adults with intellectual disabilities with
730–741. doi:10.1111/jir.12177. and without ASD on parallel measures of challenging
Reed, G. K., Deloezal, D. N., Cooper-Brown, L. J., & behaviour: The behavior problems inventory-01 (BPI-­
Wacker, D. P. (2005). The effects of sleep disrup- 01) and autism Spectrum disorders-behavior problems
tion on the treatment of a feeling disorder. Journal of for intellectually disabled adults (ASD-BPA). Journal
168 M.J. Lanovaz et al.

of Applied Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 27, Smith, R. G., & Churchill, R. M. (2002). Identification
179–185. doi:10.1111/j.1468-3148.2009.00519.x. of environmental determinants of behavior disorders
Roscoe, E. M., Phillips, K. M., Kelly, M. A., Farber, R., through functional analysis of precursor behaviors.
& Dube, W. V. (2015). A statewide survey assessing Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 125–135.
practitioners’ use and perceived utility of functional doi:10.1901/jaba.2002.35-125.
assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, Smith, R. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1997). Antecedent influences
830–844. doi:10.1002/jaba.259. on behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior
Rozenblat, E., Brown, J. L., Brown, A. K., Reeve, S. A., & Analysis, 30, 343–375. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-343.
Reeve, K. F. (2009). Effects of adjusting DRO sched- Smith, C. M., Smith, R. G., Dracobly, J. D., & Peterson-­
ules on the reduction of stereotypic vocalizations in Pace, A. (2012). Multiple-respondent anecdotal
children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 24, assessments: An analysis of interrater agreement and
1–15. doi:10.1002/bin.270. correspondence with analogue assessment outcomes.
Saylor, S., Sidener, T. M., Reeve, S. A., Fetherston, A., Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 779–795.
& Progar, P. R. (2012). Effects of three types of non- doi:10.1901/jaba.2012.45-779.
contingent auditory stimulation on vocal stereotypy Sorensen, C., & Zarrett, N. (2014). Benefits of physical
in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior activity for adolescents with autism spectrum disor-
Analysis, 45, 185–190. doi:10.1901/jaba.2012.45-185. ders: A comprehensive review. Review Journal of
Schindler, H. R., & Horner, R. H. (2005). Generalized Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1, 344–353.
reduction of problem behavior of young children doi:10.1007/s40489-014-0027-4.
with autism: Building trans-situational interventions. Stahmer, A. C., & Schreibman, L. (1992). Teaching chil-
American Journal of Mental Retardation, 110, 36–47. dren with autism appropriate play in unsupervised
doi:10.1352/0895-8017(2005)110<36:GROPBO>2.0 environments using a self-management treatment
.CO;2. package. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25,
Schmidt, J. D., Drasgow, E., Halle, J. W., Martin, C. A., 447–459. doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-447.
& Bliss, S. A. (2014). Discrete-trial functional analy- Taylor, B. A., Hoch, H., & Weissman, M. (2005). The
sis and functional communication training with three analysis and treatment of vocal stereotypy in a child
individuals with autism and severe problem behavior. with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20, 239–253.
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 16, 44–55. doi:10.1002/bin.200.
doi:10.1177/1098300712470519. Thelen, E. (1979). Rhythmical stereotypies in nor-
Shabani, D. B., Wilder, D. A., & Flood, W. A. (2001). mal human infants. Animal Behavior, 27, 699–715.
Reducing stereotypic behavior through discrimination doi:10.1016/0003-3472(79)90006-X.
training, differential reinforcement of other behavior, Thomason-Sassi, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Neidert, P. L., &
and self-monitoring. Behavioral Interventions, 16, Roscoe, E. M. (2011). Response latency as an index of
279–286. doi:10.1002/bin.96. response strength during functional analysis of prob-
Shirley, M. J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S., Mazaleski, J. L., lem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
& Lerman, D. C. (1997). Does functional communica- 44, 51–67. doi:10.1901/jaba.2011.44-51.
tion training compete with ongoing contingencies of Tiger, J. H., Fisher, W. W., & Bouxsein, K. J. (2009).
reinforcement? An analysis during response acquisi- Therapist- and self-monitored DRO contingencies
tion and maintenance. Journal of Applied Behavior as a treatment for the self-injurious skin picking of
Analysis, 30, 93–104. doi:10.1901/jaba.1997.30-93. a young man with Asperger syndrome. Journal of
Sigafoos, J., & Saggers, E. (1995). A discrete-trial approach Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 315–319. doi:10.1901/
to the functional analysis of aggressive behaviour jaba.2009.42-315.
in two boys with autism. Journal of Intellectual and Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional
Developmental Disabilities, 20, 287–297. communication training: A review and practical guide.
Simó-Pinatella, D., Font-Roura, J., Planella-Morató, J., Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23.
McGill, P., Alomar-Kurz, E., & Giné, C. (2013). Types Tomanik, S., Harris, G. E., & Hawkins, J. (2004). The
of motivating operations in interventions with problem relationship between behaviours exhibited by children
behavior: A systematic review. Behavior Modification, with autism and maternal stress. Journal of Intellectual
37, 3–38. doi:10.1177/0145445512448096. and Developmental Disability, 29, 16–26. doi:10.1080
Slocum, S. K., & Vollmer, T. R. (2015). A comparison /13668250410001662892.
of positive and negative reinforcement for compli- Tyrer, F., McGrother, C. W., Thorp, C. F., Donaldson,
ance to treat problem behavior maintained by escape. M., Bhaumik, S., Watson, J. M., & Hollin, C. (2006).
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 563–574. Physical aggression towards others in adults with
doi:10.1002/jaba.216. learning disabilities: Prevalence and associated fac-
Smaby, K., MacDonald, R. P., Ahearn, W. H., & Dube, tors. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50,
W. V. (2007). Assessment protocol for identifying pre- 295–304. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00774.x.
ferred social consequences. Behavioral Interventions, Underwood, L. A., Figueroa, R. G., Thyer, B. A., & Nzeocha,
22, 311–318. doi:10.1002/bin.242. A. (1989). Interruption and DRI in the treatment of
self-injurious behavior among mentally retarded and
10  Comorbid Challenging Behaviors 169

autistic self-restrainers. Behavior Modification, 13, Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., Roane, H. S., &
471–481. doi:10.1177/01454455890134006. Marcus, B. A. (1997). Negative side effects of
Valdovinos, M. G., & Kennedy, C. H. (2004). A behavior-­ noncontingent reinforcement. Journal of Applied
analytic conceptualization of the side effects of psycho- Behavior Analysis, 30, 161–164. doi:10.1901/
tropic medication. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 231–238. jaba.1997.30-161.
Vaughn, B. J., Wilson, D., & Dunlap, G. (2002). Wacker, D. P., Steege, M. W., Northup, J., Sasso, G., Berg,
Family-centered interventions to resolve prob- W., Reimers, T., ... & Donn, L. (1990). A component
lem behaviors in a fast-food restaurant. Journal analysis of functional communication training across
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 38–45. three topographies of severe behavior problems.
doi:10.1177/109830070200400107. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 417–429.
Vollmer, T. R. (1994). The concept of automatic doi: 10.1901/jaba.1990.23-417
reinforcement: Implications for behavioral Watkins, N., & Rapp, J. T. (2013). The convergent valid-
research in developmental disabilities. Research ity of the questions about behavioral function scale
in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 187–207. and functional analysis for problem behavior dis-
doi:10.1016/0891-4222(94)90011-6. played by individuals with autism spectrum disorder.
Vollmer, T. R., Hagopian, L. P., Bailey, J. S., Dorsey, Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 11–16.
M. F., Hanley, G. P., Lennox, D., ... & Spreat, S. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.003.
(2011). The association for behavior analysis inter- Wilder, D. A., Harris, C., Reagan, R., & Rasey, A. (2007).
national position statement on restraint and seclusion. Functional analysis and treatment of noncompliance
The Behavior Analyst, 34, 103–110. by preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior
Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G., & Analysis, 40, 173–177. doi:10.1901/jaba.2007.44-06.
Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in the treat- Wilke, A. E., Tarbox, J., Dixon, D. R., Kenzer, A. L.,
ment of attention-maintained self-injurious behavior: Bishop, M. R., & Kakavand, H. (2012). Indirect
Noncontingent reinforcement and differential reinforce- functional assessment of stereotypy in children
ment of other behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Analysis, 26, 9–21. doi:10.1901/jaba.1993.26-9. Spectrum Disorders, 6, 824–828. doi:10.1016/j.
Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane, rasd.2011.11.003.
H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to Wunderlich, K. L., & Vollmer, T. R. (2015). Data analysis
extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of response interruption and redirection as a treatment
of aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior for vocal stereotypy. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 28, 561–576. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-561. Analysis, 48, 749–764. doi:10.1002/jaba.227.
Treatment of Socially Reinforced
Problem Behavior 11
Brian D. Greer and Wayne W. Fisher

When the reinforcer for problem behavior is fied via a functional analysis, behavior analysts
mediated by the behavior of another individual, can use that information to develop effective
problem behavior is said to be socially rein- treatment strategies. In this chapter, we will dis-
forced. For example, a child may learn that dis- cuss the operant mechanisms involved in the
rupting in a classroom produces valuable social maintenance and treatment of socially reinforced
interactions (e.g., attention in the form of laugh- problem behavior, as well as treatment options
ing) from his peers, or the child may come to and strategies that can be used to improve the
learn that aggression results in the teacher sus- efficacy and practicality of treatment.
pending him from school, which then allows him
to escape difficult class assignments. Both atten-
tion and escape from nonpreferred demands are  nalyzing the Operant Mechanisms
A
common social reinforcers for problem behavior Involved in the Maintenance
(Beavers, Iwata, & Lerman, 2013; Hanley, Iwata, of Problem Behavior
McCord, 2003; Iwata et al., 1994). However, any
behavior of another individual can come to rein- When treatment strategies are based on the results
force and maintain problem behavior, and these of a functional analysis, the behavior analyst is bet-
relations are often demonstrated experimentally ter equipped to treat problem behavior because the
through a functional analysis (Iwata, Dorsey, behavior analyst has identified important variables
Slifer, Bauman, & Richman, 1982/1994). Once that control the occurrence of the problem behav-
the function of problem behavior has been identi- ior. Functional-analysis methodology emphasizes
three operant mechanisms that frequently deter-
mine whether problem behavior will or will not
Grants 5R01HD079113 and 1R01HD083214 from the occur. Each condition of the functional analysis
National Institute of Child Health and Human
specifically alters these operant mechanisms in
Development provided partial support for this work.
Address correspondence to Brian D. Greer, Center for unique ways to produce or mitigate the occurrence
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 985450 Nebraska Medical of problem behavior (Iwata, Pace, Cowdery, &
Center, Omaha, Nebraska 68198 (email: brian.greer@ Miltenberger, 1994; Iwata et al., 1994). Careful
unmc.edu).
attention to and manipulation of these same oper-
B.D. Greer (*) • W.W. Fisher ant mechanisms following the completion of the
Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders, University of
functional analysis enables the behavior analyst to
Nebraska Medical Center’s Munroe-Meyer Institute,
Omaha, NE 68198, USA develop maximally effective treatments for prob-
e-mail: brian.greer@unmc.edu lem behavior.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 171


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_11
172 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

Discriminative Stimuli Motivating Operations

The first operant mechanism programmed within The second operant mechanism programmed
each condition of a functional analysis is the dis- within a functional analysis is the motivating
criminative stimulus, which signals the availabil- operation, which alters the value of a particular
ity of the putative reinforcer evaluated in that consequence as a reinforcer and changes the
condition. For example, the therapist signals the momentary probability of responses that have
availability of her attention by reading a maga- historically resulted in that consequence
zine only in the attention condition. This proce- (Laraway, Snycerski, Michael, & Poling, 2003).
dural detail is designed to facilitate The reinforcer-test conditions of the functional
attention-maintained problem behavior within analysis specifically program an establishing
session. Research on discriminative stimuli by operation (i.e., an increase in motivation for the
Conners et al. (2000) showed that programming putative reinforcer), whereas the control condi-
additional, condition-specific discriminative tion programs an abolishing operation (i.e., a
stimuli (i.e., a specific therapist in a specific room decrease in motivation for the putative reinforc-
color) can facilitate discriminated responding ers arranged to follow problem behavior in the
between conditions of the functional analysis, test conditions). For example, the therapist issues
leading to clearer functional-analysis outcomes. demands in the escape (test) condition, which
Extending these findings to treatment suggests increases the child’s motivation to escape those
that progressing from assessment to treatment demands by engaging in problem behavior. In
should also be signaled using discriminative contrast, the therapist issues no demands in the
stimuli, such as by introducing treatment in a toy-play (control) condition, which then
unique stimulus context (e.g., with a novel thera- decreases the child’s motivation for escape.
pist and in a therapy room separate from the one Treatments that abolish the child’s motivation for
used for the functional analysis). These discrimi- the identified reinforcer (e.g., by arranging a
native stimuli should later be faded to encourage dense schedule of noncontingent or differential
generalization of the treatment effects. reinforcement) have been researched extensively
Research by Mace et al. (2010) found evidence (Carr et al., 2000; Tiger, Hanley, & Bruzek, 2008)
to support the use of unique discriminative stimuli and have been shown to be highly effective at
when initiating treatment. In Experiment 3 of that decreasing rates of problem behavior (Carr,
study, Mace et al. conducted baseline sessions in Severtson, Lepper, 2009; Kurtz, Boelter,
three contexts with two males who engaged in Jarmolowicz, Chin, & Hagopian, 2011; Richman,
socially reinforced problem behavior. Each con- Barnard-Brak, Grubb, Bosch, & Abby, 2015).
text differed based on its location, the reinforce- Recent research by DeRosa, Fisher, and Steege
ment schedules in effect, and the color of the (2015) and by Fisher, Greer, Mitteer, Fuhrman,
clothing that the therapist wore. Once rates of Romani, and Zangrillo (in press) suggest that it is
problem behavior stabilized within each baseline imperative to maintain minimal exposure to the
condition, extinction began, while the therapist establishing operation for problem behavior dur-
wore either the same colored clothing as the one ing the early stages of treatment.
used in baseline or a novel color. Both participants The study by DeRosa et al. (2015) compared
displayed considerably lower rates of problem the efficacy of two forms of a d­ifferential-
behavior when the therapist wore the novel cloth- reinforcement-of-alternative-behavior (DRA)
ing color, even though the same contingencies procedure called functional communication train-
remained in place across both conditions. These ing (FCT) as treatment for the social functions of
results suggest that problem behavior treated in a two boys’ problem behavior. In one version of
unique stimulus context may lessen the persis- FCT, the therapist physically guided each boy to
tence of problem behavior and produce more rapid touch a picture card to access the reinforcer main-
reductions in rates of problem behavior. taining problem behavior. The therapist then
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 173

inserted a delay to the prompt to encourage inde- Consequences


pendent communication responses. However, all
communication responses (prompted and inde- The third operant mechanism programmed within
pendent) resulted in immediate access to the rein- each functional-analysis condition is the conse-
forcer for problem behavior. In the other version quence that follows the target response. A brief
of FCT, the therapist used the same general teach- break occurs only following problem behavior dur-
ing procedures to teach each boy to use a vocal ing the escape condition, whereas the therapist
communication response; however, although the delivers attention only after problem behavior in
therapist could model the vocal response, she the attention condition. These putative reinforcers
could not ensure that the child would imitate the are typically delivered immediately after the target
communication response. This procedural differ- response and, according to a dense, often continu-
ence between the two forms of FCT led to impor- ous schedule (i.e., fixed ratio [FR] 1), during the
tant disparities in the amount of time in which the functional analysis. The immediacy and consis-
participants were exposed to the establishing tency with which the therapist implements the pro-
operation for problem behavior. That is, the card grammed consequences for problem behavior
condition produced much shorter durations of often promote stability in responding within the
exposure to the establishing operation relative to test conditions of the functional analysis. In addi-
the vocal condition because the therapist could tion, in cases in which the initial functional analysis
quickly and reliably prompt, and then immedi- produces inconclusive findings, researchers have
ately reinforce, the communication response in shown that idiosyncratic variables may influence
the card condition but not in the vocal condition. whether or to what extent a given consequence
Results for both participants showed less bursting functions as reinforcement for problem behavior
and more rapid and larger reductions in problem (e.g., certain forms of attention may reinforce prob-
behavior when the therapist controlled the estab- lem behavior more so than others; Fisher, Ninness,
lishing operation for problem behavior (i.e., when Piazza, & Owen-DeSchryver, 1996; Kodak,
the picture card was used). Northup, & Kelley, 2007; Piazza, Bowman,
A recent study by Fisher et al. (in press) pro- Contrucci, Delia, Adelinis, & Goh, 1999).
vided a more direct comparison of the effects of As mentioned above, the same consequences
differential exposure to the establishing operation delivered following problem behavior in the
for problem behavior when treating socially rein- reinforcer-­test conditions of the functional analy-
forced problem behavior. Researchers in that study sis are delivered noncontingently in the control
compared limited and extended exposures to the condition. The use of a changeover delay
establishing operation for two boys’ self-­ injury (Herrnstein, 1961) helps to ensure that problem
and/or aggression by withholding access to the behavior is not adventitiously reinforced by
communication materials for a fixed period of delaying the scheduled delivery of the time-based
time or by waiting a set period of time to physi- reinforcer until problem behavior has ceased for
cally guide the communication response. Unlike a requisite period of time (e.g., 3 or 5 s).
the participants in the DeRosa et al. (2015) study,
both participants in the Fisher et al. study learned
only to touch or exchange a picture card (and not General Approaches
to emit a vocal communication response), which to the Function-Based Treatment
allowed for a more direct comparison of differen- of Problem Behavior
tial exposures to the establishing operation. Results
of that study closely replicated those of the study Iwata, Vollmer, Zarcone, and Rodgers (1993)
by DeRosa et al. and provided further evidence of described three general function-based
the importance of maintaining precise control over approaches to treating problem behavior that
the establishing operation for problem behavior involve manipulating its maintaining conse-
during the initial stages of treatment. quence. Those approaches included:
174 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

(a) Providing the functional reinforcer accord- whether an extinction burst will occur when
ing to a noncontingent reinforcement (NCR) these reinforcement-based treatments are
schedule initiated.
(b) Preventing the response from producing the The final approach (DRA) arranges reinforcer
functional reinforcer (i.e., extinction) deliveries contingent upon an alternative
(c) Reassigning the reinforcement contingency response and can be implemented either with or
to an alternative, more appropriate response without extinction and punishment (Fisher et al.,
(i.e., DRA) while also suspending the rein- 1993; Hagopian, Fisher, Thibault-Sullivan,
forcement contingency for problem Acquisto, & LeBlanc, 1998). As noted above,
behavior FCT is a common type of DRA procedure that
involves using the functional reinforcer for prob-
The first approach (NCR) typically produces a lem behavior to establish an appropriate form of
large and rapid reduction in problem behavior communication that can be recognized and then
because the functional reinforcer is delivered on reinforced by other individuals. We now turn to
a dense (often continuous) schedule and thereby more detailed discussion of these approaches to
functions as an abolishing operation for problem treatment, as well as strategies that have been
behavior (Hagopian, Fisher, & Legacy, 1994; shown to improve treatment efficacy and
Pace, Iwata, Cowdery, Andree, & McIntyre, practicality.
1993; Vollmer, Iwata, Zarcone, Smith, &
Mazaleski, 1993). That is, delivering the func-
tional reinforcer according to a dense, noncontin-  eveloping Specific Function-Based
D
gent schedule lessens the value of that stimulus Treatments for Problem Behavior
as reinforcement and reduces the probability of
behavior (e.g., problem behavior) that has pro- The most commonly prescribed function-based
duced that stimulus in the past. interventions for problem behavior involve one
The second approach discussed by Iwata or more of the following elements: (a) removal
et al. (1993) involves extinction or termination of the establishing operation for problem behav-
of the response-reinforcer relation, thereby ior by programming a dense NCR schedule, (b)
decreasing the probability of problem behavior. discontinuation of the reinforcement contin-
Although extinction is an empirically supported gency for problem behavior (i.e., extinction), (c)
function-­based treatment for problem behavior provision of the functional reinforcer for an
when implemented in isolation (Iwata et al., appropriate alternative response (i.e., DRA/
1994), extinction is often combined with other FCT), (d) provision of a competing reinforcer,
reinforcement-­ based procedures (e.g., DRA, and (e) removal of the functional reinforcer con-
NCR) to reduce rates of problem behavior tingent on problem behavior (i.e., punishment).
while continuing to allow the individual access In practice, many of these elements are com-
to the functional reinforcer. Additionally, bined, often producing large reductions in rates
Lerman, Iwata, and Wallace (1999) found fewer of problem behavior.
instances of response bursting (i.e., increased
rates of problem behavior during treatment)
and less extinction-­ induced aggression when Noncontingent Reinforcement
extinction was combined with other treatment
procedures, including DRA or NCR, than when Scholars have noted that the term “noncontingent
extinction was used alone. The research reinforcement” is a misnomer because reinforce-
described earlier by DeRosa et al. (2015) and ment strengthens target responding, whereas
Fisher et al. (in press) shows that the duration NCR schedules often weaken the target response
of exposure to the establishing operation during (Poling & Normand, 1999). Similar issues arise
DRA and NCR is a primary determinant as to with terms such as “time-based” and “response-­
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 175

independent” reinforcement schedules, which do the mean interresponse time. To our knowledge,
not acknowledge the historical contingency no study has yet to compare the efficacy of these
between problem behavior and the reinforcing two approaches when selecting the initial NCR
stimulus or that the stimulus was identified via a schedule. However, in a recent study by Fisher
functional analysis (Fisher, Greer, & Bouxsein, et al. (in press), researchers made post-hoc com-
under review). We use the term “NCR” to parisons of these two approaches to that of a third
describe the time-based delivery of the functional approach that consisted of collecting additional
reinforcer for problem behavior while also data during a progressive-interval schedule in
acknowledging these terminological difficulties. which problem behavior resulted in reinforce-
Vollmer et al. (1993) first applied NCR to the ment according to an escalating (interval) sched-
treatment of attention-maintained problem ule of reinforcement. This progressive-interval
behavior. In baseline of that study, self-injurious schedule terminated following the first instance
behavior resulted in 10-s access to vocal atten- of problem behavior. Based on these results,
tion (i.e., statements of disproval and concern), Fisher et al. showed that the interval at which
as well as light touches to the client’s arm or problem behavior occurred in the progressive-­
shoulder. NCR began following baseline and interval schedule reliably occasioned problem
consisted of the therapist providing continuous behavior with one participant when the reinforcer
attention in the form of general conversation and was later withheld repeatedly for the same
praise. The investigators later thinned the NCR amount of time, whereas little to no problem
schedule of attention by delivering attention behavior occurred with a relatively dense sched-
according to a fixed-time (FT) schedule every ule of reinforcement. Post-hoc comparisons
5 min. Applications of NCR to treat problem between the three approaches to selecting an ini-
behavior should also begin with similarly dense tial reinforcement schedule indicated that mean
NCR schedules before attempting reinforcer-­ latencies to problem behavior and mean interre-
schedule thinning (Hagopian et al., 1994) and sponse times would have suggested an even
should, when possible, involve the delivery of a leaner schedule of reinforcement than did the
large magnitude of the NCR stimulus (Roscoe, progressive-interval assessment. Regardless of
Iwata, & Rand, 2003). the approach, it is paramount to ensure that the
initial NCR schedule is sufficiently dense so as to
Setting the Initial NCR Schedule  Researchers not occasion problem behavior.
have established two general methods for setting
the initial NCR schedule used to treat problem Differential Reinforcement  Differential rein-
behavior, and both approaches involve within-­ forcement involves the delivery of reinforcers
session data analysis of previously collected data after some prespecified criterion has been met,
that is then used to tailor the initial NCR schedule and there exist multiple forms of differential rein-
for each individual. Lalli, Casey, and Kates forcement that are applicable to treating socially
(1997) calculated the mean latency to the first reinforced problem behavior (e.g., DRA, differ-
instance of problem behavior that occurred in the ential reinforcement of low rates of behavior
corresponding test condition of the functional [DRL], differential reinforcement of other behav-
analysis and then used this duration as the initial ior [DRO]). Of these procedures, DRA offers the
NCR schedule (e.g., FT 90 s for the participant behavior analyst possibilities that the other
named Donny). Kahng, Iwata, DeLeon, and differential-­
reinforcement procedures do not.
Wallace (2000) used a different type and source First, DRA has the capacity to teach individuals
of data to determine their initial NCR schedules. with socially reinforced problem behavior a new
Researchers in that study calculated mean inter- form of communication (i.e., FCT), which may
response times for problem behavior that be a goal within the individual’s education plan,
occurred in the final three baseline sessions and irrespective of treating problem behavior.
then set the initial NCR schedule to be equal to Second, once the individual is reliably emitting
176 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

the alternative response, DRA often results in the the alternative form of communication. Because
delivery of a large portion of the available rein- this alternative form of communication is estab-
forcer deliveries. Third, some researchers have lished by delivering the functional reinforcer
argued that DRA procedures (e.g., FCT) allow contingent on its occurrence, the response is
the individual to “control” his or her access to the often referred to as a functional communication
reinforcer (Carr & Durand, 1985), which allows response (FCR).
the individual to enjoy some degree of autonomy.
In addition, these authors suggested that “control Selecting and Teaching the FCR  Tiger et al.
over reinforcement” contributes to the effective- (2008) reviewed the extant literature on FCT and
ness of FCT. However, two investigations found offered practical advice regarding how to select
that NCR, which does not allow the client to con- and teach the FCR during FCT. These authors
trol the schedule of reinforcement, and FCT, recommended selecting an initial FCR that is of
which does, produced equivalent reductions in low effort for the individual (Horner & Day,
problem behavior (Hanley, Piazza, Fisher, 1991) and easy for other individuals to identify
Contrucci, & Maglieri, 1997; Kahng, Iwata, before considering a higher-effort response
DeLeon, & Worsdell, 1997). Nevertheless, (Hernandez, Hanley, Ingvarsson, & Tiger, 2007).
Hanley et al. found that participants preferred Responses that already exist in the individual’s
FCT over NCR when given a choice between the repertoire can be a good starting point in helping
interventions. Finally, Durand and Carr (1991) to identify a low-effort FCR. For example, if the
found that FCT promotes generalization and individual mands for preferred items using the
maintenance of treatment effects because the vocal response, “Toys” outside of FCT sessions,
communication response may prompt both this same vocal response could be targeted as the
trained and untrained caregivers to deliver DRA FCR during FCT, provided the individual’s prob-
appropriately. These are a few of the reasons why lem behavior is maintained by access to preferred
interventions that include a DRA component tangibles. However, the behavior analyst should
have been studied and used extensively. carefully attend to whether he or she can quickly
and reliably occasion the response before select-
Functional Communication Training  FCT has ing it as the FCR for use in FCT. The initial stages
all of the benefits discussed above in regard to of FCT involve presenting, and then immediately
DRA, but it also contains other elements that removing, the establishing operation for the indi-
make it particularly appealing for use in treating vidual’s problem behavior, and the results of the
socially reinforced problem behavior. As men- DeRosa et al. (2015) study described above high-
tioned previously, FCT reassigns the functional light the importance of maintaining precise con-
reinforcer for problem behavior (as identified via trol over the establishing operation for problem
a functional analysis) to an alternative and more behavior by presenting a prompt that ensures that
appropriate form of communication. For exam- the response occurs reliably (i.e., a controlling
ple, if the functional-analysis results suggest that prompt). For this reason, we tend to target FCRs
escape from nonpreferred tasks reinforces prob- that involve gross- or fine-motor movements
lem behavior, the behavior analyst would then (e.g., exchanging or touching a card that depicts
teach that individual to use an alternative form of a picture of the individual consuming the func-
communication (e.g., saying “Break, please”) to tional reinforcer), rather than a vocal FCR, which
request breaks. Using the functional reinforcer cannot be guided.
for problem behavior (rather than an arbitrary Once the FCR deemed appropriate for use in
reinforcer) likely ensures a sustained establishing FCT is identified, the behavior analyst must then
operation is present, which may prove beneficial ensure its reliable occurrence in the presence of
when teaching the alternative communication the establishing operation for the individual’s
response. Another benefit of FCT is that empha- problem behavior. To accomplish this, the behav-
sis is placed on identifying, and then teaching, ior analyst should program multiple opportuni-
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 177

ties to teach the FCR under the stimulus lishing communication an important goal for the
conditions that evoke problem behavior by pre- individual? (c) Is establishing compliance an
senting the establishing operation, immediately important goal for the individual? and (d) What
prompting the FCR, and then immediately there- if the previous recommendations contradict one
after providing the functional reinforcer follow- another? To address the first question (How dan-
ing each instance of the FCR (i.e., FR-1 schedule; gerous is the problem behavior?), behavior ana-
Tiger et al., 2008). Although there are a variety of lysts should consider reviewing the Self-Injury
prompting strategies that can be used to teach the Trauma (SIT) Scale developed by Iwata, Pace,
FCR (e.g., most-to-least, least-to-most, prompt Kissel, Nau, and Farber (1990) or the UNMC-­
delay), behavior analysts in our clinic tend to use MMI’s Center for Autism Spectrum Disorders’
a form of errorless learning to teach the FCR that Destructive Behavior Severity Scale (DBSS;
involves a progressive prompt-delay procedure Fisher, Rodriguez, Luczynski, & Kelley, 2013)
(Charlop, Schreibman, & Thibodeau, 1985). to help quantify the frequency, severity, and risk
Following two consecutive ten-trial sessions with associated with self-injurious or other destruc-
near-zero rates of problem behavior in which the tive behavior. Although the SIT Scale was
behavior analyst presents the establishing opera- designed to assess self-injurious behavior, it may
tion, immediately delivers the controlling prompt also prove helpful in quantifying the extent to
for the FCR (e.g., by guiding a picture-card which aggression is harmful to other individuals,
exchange), and then immediately thereafter as aggression can produce similar types of sur-
delivers the functional reinforcer, the behav- face tissue damage as self-injury. SIT Scale or
ior analyst then systematically increases the time DBBS results indicating a high frequency, sever-
between presenting the establishing operation ity, or risk associated with self-injury or other
and then delivering the controlling prompt for the destructive behavior suggest the behavior ana-
FCR. We often increase this delay for every two lyst should:
consecutive sessions with low rates of problem
behavior using the following delay progression: (a) Ensure the safety of all parties who interact
2 s, 5 s, 10 s, and 20 s. FCRs that occur prior to with the referred individual, especially dur-
the controlling prompt are scored as independent ing the assessment and treatment process
FCRs and immediately terminate the establishing (e.g., by participating in trainings on assaul-
operation for problem behavior by producing the tive behavior-management procedures,
functional reinforcer. These pretraining proce- wearing protective equipment, padding hard
dures that involve presenting the establishing surfaces, removing potentially dangerous or
operation and then systematically delaying the destructible materials)
presentation of the controlling prompt for the (b) Modify functional-analysis conditions to

FCR encourage rapid acquisition of independent ensure safety while maintaining accurate and
FCRs by transferring stimulus control from the valid functional-analysis results (see Iwata &
controlling prompt to the presentation of the Dozier, 2008 for helpful recommendations)
establishing operation. Our pretraining proce- (c) Develop a function-based treatment that rap-
dures typically terminate following two consecu- idly reduces the problem behavior
tive sessions with low rates of problem behavior
and independent FCRs occurring on 90% or more In such cases, it may be better to deliver the
of opportunities. functional reinforcer for problem behavior
according to a dense NCR schedule, rather than
Choosing Between DRA and NCR  The deci- contingent upon some alternative response (i.e.,
sion whether to use DRA or NCR to treat socially DRA, FCT) or following the omission of the tar-
reinforced problem behavior can be facilitated get response (i.e., DRO). However, another rea-
by addressing four general questions: (a) How sonable alternative would be to implement the
dangerous is the problem behavior? (b) Is estab- procedures designed to limit exposure to the
178 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

establishing operation for problem behavior lish or promote compliance unless the behav-
developed by Fisher and colleagues (DeRosa ior analyst explicitly arranges reinforcement for
et al., 2015; Fisher et al., in press). compliance (cf. Lomas-Mevers, Fisher, Kelley,
Vollmer et al. (1993) suggested three reasons & Fredricks, 2014).
why NCR may prove superior to DRO when DRA as applied to escape-maintained prob-
treating problem behavior. First, in two of their lem behavior often targets compliance directly
three participants, NCR better attenuated by providing the functional reinforcer (i.e.,
extinction-­ induced problem behavior than did escape) only following instances of compliance.
DRO. Second, NCR resulted in a richer schedule FCT as applied to this situation often consists of
of reinforcer deliveries when compared to DRO providing escape only after the individual has (a)
at similar interval durations. Third, NCR does not complied with the current demand and then (b)
require the caregiver’s constant vigilance, as emitted the FCR (e.g., saying, “Break, please”).
does DRO. For these reasons, function-based Thus, FCT used to treat social-negatively rein-
treatments that consist of arranging a dense NCR forced problem behavior is often described as a
schedule are often a good starting point when chained schedule of reinforcement because the
treating severe or dangerous problem behavior. individual is required to first comply with the
A second question that should be considered demand and then to emit the FCR before the rein-
when deciding between DRA and NCR is forcer is delivered. When establishing compli-
whether establishing communication is an impor- ance is important, behavior analysts should
tant goal for the individual. DRA procedures consider providing reinforcement directly for this
(e.g., FCT) have the capacity to teach new forms response by using DRA or FCT.
of communication that then replace problem A fourth question that may arise when decid-
behavior, whereas NCR may discourage the ing between DRA and NCR is what to do if the
acquisition of appropriate mands for the func- previous recommendations contradict one
tional reinforcer (Goh, Iwata, & DeLeon, 2000). another. For example, extremely severe problem
If establishing communication is an important behavior (e.g., eye gouging) warrants the use of
goal for the individual, FCT may be an appropri- NCR procedures, whereas communication may
ate initial treatment strategy. However, behavior also be an important goal for the same individual,
analysts may consider using DRA, even if estab- which would necessitate the use of DRA proce-
lishing communication is not a priority for the dures. Luckily, researchers have explored the
individual. combination of DRA with NCR as treatment for
A third question that should be considered is problem behavior (Fritz, Iwata, Hammond, &
whether establishing compliance is an important Bloom, 2013; Goh et al., 2000; Marcus &
goal for the individual. Problem behavior main- Vollmer, 1996). Results from those studies have
tained by social-negative reinforcement in the generally shown reductions in rates of problem
form of escape from nonpreferred demands often behavior with this unique combination of treat-
necessitates teaching the individual to comply ment components (Carr et al., 2000), but these
with those same nonpreferred demands that studies have also shown that alternative respond-
occasion problem behavior. In other words, ing may not occur reliably until the NCR sched-
some caregiver-delivered demands (e.g., “Do not ule is sufficiently thin (Goh et al., 2000).
touch the hot stove”) must result in compliance, As discussed previously, a dense schedule of
irrespective of problem behavior. NCR as NCR reduces motivation for the functional rein-
applied to escape-maintained problem behavior forcer, which causes a reduction in rates of prob-
is sometimes referred to as noncontingent escape lem behavior but also decreases motivation to
(NCE) and has been shown to be an effective emit the alternative response. NCR-schedule
intervention for treating escape-maintained thinning gradually reintroduces the establishing
problem behavior (Vollmer, Marcus, & Ringdahl, operation for the functional reinforcer, which
1995). However, NCE alone is unlikely to estab- then increases the likelihood of the alternative
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 179

response. However, NCR-schedule thinning may when the device is inoperative or otherwise una-
have the unintended effect of also increasing the vailable. Providing alternative reinforcers when
likelihood of problem behavior. Behavior ana- the functional reinforcer is unavailable has shown
lysts may find it helpful to occasionally prompt to be an effective strategy for maintaining low
the alternative response while completing NCR-­ rates of problem behavior (Austin & Tiger, 2015;
schedule thinning, at least until independent Hagopian, Contrucci Kuhn, Long, & Rush, 2005;
alternative responses are well established (Goh Hanley, Piazza, & Fisher, 1997; Rooker, Jessel,
et al., 2000). Behavior analysts may consider rep- Kurtz, & Hagopian, 2013).
licating these procedures with individuals who Alternatively, some individuals benefit from
would benefit from the immediate reduction in the combined delivery of the functional rein-
problem behavior while also teaching an alterna- forcer along with an alternative reinforcer
tive form of communication. (Zangrillo, Fisher, Greer, Owen, & DeSouza,
2016). Zangrillo et al. recently showed lower
rates of two boys’ negatively reinforced problem
I mproving the Efficacy of DRA behavior, and higher levels of compliance, when
and NCR Interventions escape coincided with access to preferred toys
than when compliance produced escape alone.
There are a number of strategies for improving Researchers have shown similar results when
the efficacy of DRA and NCR interventions. treatment involved the delivery of multiple func-
Three of the most common strategies include: (a) tional reinforcers than when treatment targeted
providing alternative reinforcers along with the only one of the functional reinforcers (Piazza
functional reinforcer or during times in which the et al., 1997; Piazza, Moes, & Fisher, 1996).
functional reinforcer is unavailable, (b) terminat- Given the advantages of incorporating alter-
ing the response-reinforcer relation that main- native reinforcers when treating socially rein-
tains problem behavior (i.e., extinction), and (c) forced problem behavior, behavior analysts
arranging a mild punisher to follow problem should consider identifying alternative reinforc-
behavior. In the following sections, we describe ers early on in the assessment and treatment proc-
these strategies in greater detail. ess and should do so using a systematic and
empirical approach. Fortunately, the competing-­
Alternative Reinforcers  One question that stimulus assessment (Piazza et al. 1998; Shore,
often arises when treating socially reinforced Iwata, DeLeon, Kahng, & Smith, 1997), which is
problem behavior is whether there will be times often used to identify stimuli that compete with
in which it will be impossible or impractical to automatically reinforced problem behavior (i.e.,
deliver the functional reinforcer. For example, if by producing reinforcers that are substitutable for
access to preferred video games on a child’s elec- the reinforcers automatically produced by prob-
tronic tablet device maintains problem behavior, lem behavior), can be easily adapted to identify
the device may periodically become inoperative alternative reinforcers for socially reinforced
(e.g., when the Internet connection is lost or problem behavior. When used with automatically
when the batteries drain and child is away from reinforced problem behavior, the competing-­
an electrical outlet), a situation which may then stimulus assessment involves the brief delivery
occasion problem behavior. Anticipating situa- of preferred stimuli singly while data collectors
tions like this require the behavior analyst to measure rates of problem behavior and levels of
identify alternative reinforcers that are substitut- item interaction. Stimuli that result in low rates
able for the functional reinforcer and that com- of problem behavior and high levels of item
pete with the occurrence problem behavior. In the interaction suggest suitable competing stimuli for
example above, attention from a caregiver may automatically reinforced problem behavior.
substitute for the video games on the tablet device Fisher, O’Conner, Kurtz, DeLeon, and Gotjen
and thus may compete with problem behavior (2000) extended the competing-stimulus assess-
180 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

ment by adapting it for use with individuals with analysts should continue to deliver reinforcement
socially reinforced problem behavior. In the (e.g., according to a DRA or NCR schedule)
adapted version of the competing-stimulus throughout treatment to reduce the likelihood of
assessment, attention-maintained problem behav- response bursting, extinction-induced aggres-
ior continued to produce attention (similar to the sion, and, more generally, resistance to extinc-
reinforcers that would be produced automatically tion. For example, research has shown that
in the traditional competing-stimulus assess- extinction implemented with NCR results in
ment), while therapists delivered stimuli (e.g., greater and more immediate reductions in prob-
toys, music, edibles) singly and measured prob- lem behavior than extinction alone (Fisher,
lem behavior and item interaction. Stimuli that DeLeon, Rodriguez-Catter, & Keeney, 2004).
compete with socially reinforced problem behav- Idiosyncratic characteristics of the individual
ior during this adapted competing-stimulus (e.g., those large in stature or exceedingly strong),
assessment have been shown to reduce rates of the topography of problem behavior (e.g., partic-
problem behavior when those stimuli are deliv- ularly dangerous behaviors that must be termi-
ered continuously with (Hanley, Piazza, & Fisher, nated for safety), or limitations in the individual’s
1997) and without extinction (Fisher et al., 2000). home or school environment (e.g., sick or elderly
caregivers) may make the use of extinction
Extinction  Perhaps the most common strategy impractical in some cases and may preclude its
for improving the efficacy of DRA and NCR use as a viable intervention component. For these
interventions is to terminate the response-­ cases, the behavior analyst should consider con-
reinforcer relation by withholding the functional ceptualizing problem behavior and adaptive
reinforcer following problem behavior, which behavior as concurrent operants that are main-
then results in a weakening of the operant tained by the same functional reinforcer (Fisher
response, a procedure and process termed extinc- & Mazur, 1997). That is, parameters shown to
tion (Iwata et al., 1994). In many cases, extinc- affect response allocation among response alter-
tion may be a necessary component of treatment natives within a concurrent-operants arrange-
(Fisher et al., 1993; Hagopian et al., 1998; ment (e.g., response effort, reinforcement rate,
Mazaleski, Iwata, Vollmer, Zarcone, & Smith, immediacy, magnitude, and quality) may suggest
1993; Zarcone, Iwata, Hughes, & Vollmer, 1993). practical modifications to the treatment plan that
Hagopian et al. (1998) conducted a medical rec- encourage adaptive behavior and discourage
ord (chart) review of 21 inpatients whose prob- problem behavior even when problem behavior
lem behavior was treated using FCT procedures continues to produce the functional reinforcer.
that were implemented with or without the use of Response and reinforcement parameters have
extinction and punishment. FCT with extinction proven important predictors of the overall effi-
was effective for the majority of individuals, pro- cacy of treatment when interventions have not
ducing at least an 80% reduction in baseline rates included an extinction component (Horner &
of problem behavior in 60% of applications (15 Day, 1991; Peck et al., 1996; Piazza et al., 1997).
of 25), whereas only 9% of applications (1 of 11) When treating negatively reinforced problem
met this reduction criterion without the use of behavior, multiple research studies have demon-
extinction. strated another intervention possibility that also
Although extinction can constitute an effec- does not involve severing the response-reinforcer
tive intervention for automatically and socially relation maintaining problem behavior. Lalli
reinforced problem behavior when implemented et al. (1999) was the first to clearly show that
in isolation (Iwata et al., 1994; Iwata, Pace, providing positive reinforcers (i.e., edible items)
Kalsher, Cowdery, & Cataldo, 1990), the above- following compliance can be an effective (albeit
mentioned results of Lerman et al. (1999), as well nonfunction-based) treatment for negatively
as those of an earlier review on extinction by reinforced problem behavior, even though prob-
Lerman and Iwata (1996b), suggest that behavior lem behavior continued to produce escape. These
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 181

findings have been replicated by subsequent total applications of FCT reported in that paper
studies, often with better treatment outcomes (63%) required some form of punishment to treat
than when compliance produced the functional socially reinforced problem behavior. These 17
reinforcer (escape; see Payne & Dozier, 2013 for applications of punishment produced a 90% or
a recent review). For example, Piazza et al. greater reduction in baseline rates of problem
(1997) found that extinction was unnecessary for behavior in all 17 applications (100%), with the
two of three participants when compliance pro- schedule of reinforcement being successfully
duced a tangible item. Lalli et al. postulated that thinned in 13 of those 17 applications (76%). In
these findings may be due to a stronger prefer- contrast, FCT with extinction produced at least a
ence for positive reinforcers than negative rein- 90% reduction in baseline rates of problem behav-
forcers; however, this remains speculative ior in only 11 of 25 applications (44%). Fisher
(Payne & Dozier, 2013). Other studies have et al. (1993) compared FCT alone to FCT with
demonstrated that noncontingent delivery of and without extinction and punishment and found
positive reinforcement (e.g., food) can produce similarly robust and consistent reductions in rates
clinically significant reductions in problem of problem behavior when using FCT with pun-
behavior (Ingvarsson, Kahng, & Hausman, ishment. These promising results often occasion
2008; Lomas, Fisher, & Kelley, 2010; Mevers questions regarding how to identify an effective
et al., 2014), which suggests that positive rein- punishing stimulus for use in treatment.
forcement in a demand context can also produce One approach to selecting punishment proce-
an abolishing effect on negatively reinforced dures is to base the punisher on the results of the
problem behavior. Regardless of the specific functional analysis. For example, if the func-
mechanism involved, it is clear that providing tional analysis indicates that problem behavior is
positive reinforcers (alone or in combination reinforced by contingent attention, then a logical
with escape) for compliance or on a time-based punishment procedure would be to deliver a brief
schedule may be a practical solution when time-out from attention contingent on problem
escape cannot be withheld following negatively behavior (Greer, Neidert, Dozier, Payne,
reinforced problem behavior. Zonneveld, & Harper, 2013; Hagopian et al.,
1998). Similarly, a brief time-out from tangible
Punishment  Individuals have a right to the items is often an effective punishment procedure
most effective treatment procedures available for problem behavior reinforced by access to the
(Van Houten et al., 1988), but occasionally the same tangible items (Greer et al., 2013). Finally,
abovementioned treatment modifications fail to guiding the individual to complete a series of
suppress socially reinforced problem behavior to three to five additional demands contingent on
clinically significant levels. In these situations, problem behavior (sometimes called contingent
arranging a mild punisher to follow problem demands) can be an effective punisher for
behavior may be necessary (Fisher et al., 1993; ­problem behavior reinforced by escape (Fisher
Hagopian et al., 1998). However, in many cases et al., 1993).
the punishing stimulus need not be intense or Another method to empirically identify pun-
contacted often by the individual to remain effec- ishers is based on the stimulus-avoidance assess-
tive. Lerman and Iwata (1996a) showed that pro- ment and the brief punisher assessment described
cedures even as mild as response blocking (i.e., by Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, Hagopian, and
physically preventing the completion of the Langdon (1994) and Fisher, Piazza, Bowman,
response) may decrease problem behavior Kurtz, Sherer, and Lachman (1994). The
through the process of punishment (cf. Smith, stimulus-­avoidance assessment involves the non-
Russo, & Le, 1999). contingent application of stimuli suspected of
When punishment is warranted, its effects are having aversive properties (e.g., guiding the indi-
often robust, consistent, and quickly observed. In vidual’s hands down, providing demands, issuing
the study by Hagopian et al. (1998), 17 of the 27 a time-out from reinforcement) one at a time
182 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

while measuring behaviors suggestive of non- ageable rates and the individual has displayed
preference for the procedure (i.e., negative vocal- high rates of the FCR during FCT, behavior ana-
izations, avoidance movements) and preference lysts typically proceed to reinforcement schedule
for the procedure (i.e., positive vocalizations) thinning. For example, Hanley, Iwata, and
and while also measuring treatment integrity Thompson (2001) gradually thinned the schedule
(i.e., successful escapes from the procedure). An of reinforcement for the FCR over the course of
avoidance index is then calculated by summing seven steps, reaching a terminal schedule that
the rates of negative vocalizations and avoidance consisted of 1 min of reinforcement alternated
movements and subtracting the rate of positive with 4 min of extinction. The final schedule
vocalizations. Procedures that correlate with high decreased reinforcer deliveries by about 80%,
avoidance indices are more likely to function as making the treatment much more practical for
punishment in a brief punisher assessment (Fisher implementation in the natural environment
et al. 1994; Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, Kurtz, et al., because it regularly allowed caregivers periods
1994). of 4–8 min during which they could attend to
During the brief punisher assessment, Fisher other matters (e.g., talk on the phone, change a
and colleagues compared a subset of punishment diaper).
procedures, using a multielement design, to deter- Chained and multiple schedules can be used to
mine the extent to which each procedure reduced increase the practicality of FCT procedures and
problem behavior when delivered contingent on teach individuals to tolerate periods in which the
that response. This process of empirically deriv- functional reinforcer is unavailable without
ing punishers should always be considered when increasing problem behavior by bringing the
including punishment to treat socially reinforced FCR under stimulus control. Once acquired, the
problem behavior. Additionally, behavior analysts stimulus control afforded by these compound
should continue to deliver reinforcement (e.g., reinforcement schedules can then be used to help
according to a DRA or NCR schedule) through- facilitate the transfer of treatment effects to novel
out treatments that include a punishment compo- therapists and settings, as well as to primary
nent (Lerman & Vorndran, 2002). caregivers.

Steps Involved in Chained-Schedule Thinning 


Improving the Practicality of FCT Chained schedules are often used to thin rein-
forcement schedules during FCT when treating
In addition to ensuring that treatments developed negatively reinforced problem behavior (Fisher
for socially reinforced problem behavior are et al., 1993; Lalli, Casey, & Kates, 1995). Periods
effective across contexts, behavior analysts must in which the FCR will and will not p­ roduce the
also consider the practicality of the intervention functional reinforcer in a chained schedule are
when implemented by caregivers in the home, signaled by discriminative stimuli. The stimulus
school, and community settings. These terminal used to signal the period in which reinforcement
treatments for socially reinforced problem behav- is available for the FCR is typically called the SD
ior often involve aspects of FCT (Tiger et al., (pronounced “S-dee”), and the stimulus used to
2008). As such, we now highlight general strate- signal the period in which reinforcement for the
gies for improving the practicality of this com- FCR is unavailable is typically called the SΔ (pro-
mon intervention while also discussing recent nounced “S-delta”). These stimuli also are some-
research that has evaluated ways of further times called S+ and S-, respectively.
enhancing the practicality of FCT-based With chained schedules, the change from
interventions. extinction (SΔ) to the reinforcement (SD) compo-
nent is response dependent, and the change from
Reinforcement Schedule Thinning with FCT  reinforcement (SD) to extinction (SΔ) is typically
Once problem behavior has decreased to man- time based. That is, after the FCR has been well
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 183

established in the child’s repertoire, schedule cedure called response restriction (Fyffe, Kahng,
thinning begins by presenting the SD and deliver- Fittro, & Russell, 2004; Roane, Fisher, Sgro,
ing the functional reinforcer immediately follow- Falcomata, & Pabico, 2004). When response
ing the first FCR. After the child consumes the restriction is used, the individual retains access to
reinforcer (usually escape from work activities), the FCR so long as the reinforcer can be deliv-
the SD is replaced by the SΔ, and the FCR remains ered (e.g., when the child has completed the nec-
on extinction until the individual has completed essary work assignment), and it is removed when
one instructional demand. Compliance results in reinforcement is unavailable (e.g., while the child
replacement of the SΔ with the SD, and the first is expected to complete the assignment). When
FCR emitted in the presence of the SD produces the individual’s access to the FCR can be manip-
the functional reinforcer. After one or a few ses- ulated, this procedure can reduce excessive FCRs
sions with low levels of problem behavior, the that may occur during schedule thinning (Fisher
work requirement for switching from the SΔ to et al., 2014). In the Fisher et al. study, we reduced
the SD component increases. Typically, the thera- FCRs during the SΔ component using response
pist requires the individual to complete one addi- restriction for four children while maintaining
tional instructional demand (i.e., two demands) high FCR rates in the presence of the SD and low
before the SΔ is replaced by the SD. Over time, the overall rates of problem behavior. Response
work requirement (or the number of instructional restriction can be used in both chained- and
demands the individual must complete in the SΔ multiple-­schedule thinning.
component) increases gradually until the work
and break intervals are similar to what is expected Steps Involved in Multiple-Schedule Thinning 
of the child in his or her natural environment Hanley et al. (2001) evaluated reinforcement
(e.g., completing two math worksheets followed schedule thinning using a multiple schedule.
by a 5-min SD or break interval). In our program, Multiple schedules are similar to chained sched-
we typically increase the duration of the SD com- ules in that discriminative stimuli are used to sig-
ponent as the individual is exposed to longer and nal the availability and unavailability of
longer periods of the SΔ, which keeps constant reinforcement. In multiple schedules, the alterna-
the work-to-reinforcement ratio (or unit price in tion between these components is time-­ based
behavioral-economics terms; Roane, Falcomata, rather than response-based. With the Hanley
& Fisher, 2007). et al. procedure, the initial reinforcement compo-
One important question that often arises dur- nent lasted 45 s, and the extinction component
ing reinforcement schedule thinning is, “Will it lasted 15 s. Thus, initially the individual had to
be disruptive to the social environment if the tolerate only brief periods in which ­reinforcement
individual emits the FCR when reinforcement is for the FCR was unavailable. Hanley et al. alter-
unavailable?” Individuals who undergo rein- nated the reinforcement and extinction compo-
forcement schedule thinning may continue to nents in a quasi-random fashion, and they
request reinforcement at times when it cannot be correlated each component with a unique dis-
delivered (i.e., during the SΔ component; Fisher, criminative stimulus, using different colored
Greer, Querim, & DeRosa, 2014). For example, a cards to signal the reinforcement and extinction
child using an FCR card to request breaks from components of the multiple schedule. In our pro-
instructional activities may repeatedly attempt to gram, we typically begin with a two-sided card
exchange the card rather than comply with with unique colors on each side (e.g., yellow on
instructions. If continued requests for reinforce- one side, blue on the other). The card is attached
ment are disruptive to caregivers or others (e.g., to a swivel on a lanyard that the therapist wears
peers in a classroom), one common modification around his or her neck, which makes it easy to
to FCT involves preventing access to the FCR by quickly switch the card from one side to the other.
removing the response materials during times Once reinforcement schedule thinning is com-
when reinforcement is unavailable through a pro- plete, we typically switch to a brightly colored
184 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

rubber wristband, which is worn as the SD, and with higher levels or more severe types of prob-
we use a different colored wristband or the lem behavior.
absence of the wristband (i.e., therapist places the
wristband in a pocket) as the SΔ. We often switch Rapid Schedule Thinning with Multiple
to a wristband because parents tend to find it Schedules  Our research group recently
more socially acceptable for use in public (i.e., it showed that for at least some individuals it
is less likely to call attention to the child and might not be necessary to gradually thin the
family). relative durations of the reinforcement and
extinction components of a multiple schedule
Overall Effectiveness of Chained and Multiple (Betz, Fisher, Roane, Mintz, & Owen, 2013). In
Schedules  Greer, Fisher, Saini, Owen, and Experiment 3 of the Betz et al. study, we rap-
Jones (2016) recently summarized the results of idly transitioned from a relatively rich to a rela-
25 consecutive applications of chained or multi- tively lean multiple schedule (i.e., from a
ple schedules during treatment of problem behav- multiple 60–60 to a multiple 60–240 with two
ior with FCT. When these signaled, compound cases) without proceeding though any of the
schedules were used to thin the schedule of rein- intermediate steps used by Hanley et al. (2001).
forcement for the FCR (without alternative rein- With two additional cases, we removed the dis-
forcement or punishment), they resulted in a criminative stimuli from the multiple schedule
mean reduction in problem behavior of 96% rel- (producing a mixed-schedule baseline) and rap-
ative to baseline rates. This percentage exceeded idly transitioned from a rich mixed schedule
reductions in problem behavior reported in previ- (i.e., mixed 60–60) to a lean multiple schedule
ous large studies that used delayed reinforcement of reinforcement (i.e., multiple 60–240). In all
schedules in at least some schedule-thinning four of these cases, we obtained comparable
applications (Hagopian et al. 1998; Rooker et al. results to those produced by the gradual-fading
2013). Moreover, the chained and multiple procedure evaluated by Hanley et al. That is, we
schedules used in the Greer et al. study produced reduced reinforcer deliveries by about 80% in a
at least a 90% reduction in problem behavior single step while maintaining the strength of
without or before alternative reinforcement or the FCR in the presence of the SD. In addition,
punishment in 73% of applications and at least an the rapid switch to the lean schedule was not
80% reduction in 91% of applications. In addi- associated with an increase in problem behav-
tion, we added a punishment component in only ior. It should be noted, however, that the partici-
1 of the 25 applications (4%), a smaller amount pants in the Betz et al. study displayed
than the percentages reported by both Rooker instruction-following behavior, and Betz et al.
et al. (16%) and Hagopian et al. (68%). Finally, included contingency-specifying rules during
in 88% of applications of these compound sched- the multiple schedule (i.e., telling the partici-
ules, we were able to successfully thin the rein- pants what would happen if they displayed the
forcement schedule for the FCR to a point where FCR in the presence of the SD and SΔ). Thus, it
participants tolerated periods of at least 4–8 min remains uncertain whether rapid shifts from
without accessing the functional reinforcer (i.e., rich to lean multiple schedules would produce
two back-to-back 4-min SΔ periods produced an equivalent results with participants who do not
8-min block in which reinforcement was unavail- have relatively well-developed instruction-fol-
able). It should be noted, however, that the par- lowing repertoires or without the inclusion of
ticipants in the Greer et al. study consisted of contingency-specifying rules.
intensive outpatients, whereas Rooker et al.
included a mix of inpatients and intensive outpa- Facilitating the Transfer of Treatment Effects
tients, and Hagopian et al. included only inpa- with Multiple Schedules  One commonly rec-
tients. Thus, more research is needed to replicate ommended method of promoting generalization
these findings with individuals who may present of treatment effects involves programming com-
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 185

mon stimuli in both the initial treatment context child’s mother, treatment effects immediately
and the generalization context (Stokes & Baer, and fully transferred to her in both a low-atten-
1977), and the stimuli used in a multiple schedule tion and high-demand context.
may be uniquely suited for this method of pro-
moting generalization. Therefore, we also Mitigating Resurgence of Problem Behavior
recently conducted a study to evaluate the extent Using Multiple Schedules  A number of con-
to which multiple schedules could be used to ceptual, review, and research papers published
facilitate the transfer of FCT treatment effects recently have identified a major limitation of
from one setting to another or from one therapist DRA interventions like FCT, a limitation that is
to another (Fisher, Greer, Fuhrman, & Querim, predicted by quantitative models of behavioral
2015). Fisher et al. conducted baselines using momentum theory (BMT; Nevin & Shahan,
mixed schedules of reinforcement for the FCR 2011). This limitation involves an increase in
with novel therapists or in novel settings, and problem behavior when the FCR contacts a dis-
levels of the FCR were low and undifferentiated rupter, such as the FCR failing to produce rein-
in both the reinforcement and extinction compo- forcement for a period of time, which is
nents. We then introduced multiple schedules sometimes called an extinction challenge. For
that were identical to the mixed schedules except example, Volkert, Lerman, Call, and Trosclair-­
that discriminative stimuli were used to signal Lasserre (2009) found that problem behavior
periods in which the FCR would and would not increased substantially for five of six participants
produce reinforcement. We introduced the multi- during periods in which the FCR failed to pro-
ple schedules across settings or therapists in duce reinforcement (i.e., during an extinction
accordance with a multiple-baseline design. After challenge) or when the density of reinforcement
exposure to the multiple schedule in one setting for the FCR decreased precipitously (i.e., from an
(or with one therapist), treatment effects rapidly FR 1 to an FR 12). An increase in problem behav-
transferred to the subsequent settings (or thera- ior when a disruptor is introduced following
pists) for all participants. treatment with alternative reinforcement and
The transfer of an intervention’s treatment extinction (e.g., FCT) is referred to as resurgence
effects from the initial therapist to the primary (Greer, Fisher, Romani, & Saini, 2016; Pritchard,
caregivers presents a unique challenge. That is, Hoerger, & Mace, 2014).
parents and other caregivers typically have a The phenomenon of resurgence is robust and
long history of delivering reinforcement for has been observed in basic, translational, and
problem behavior and little or no history of rein- clinical research, and a number of researchers
forcing the child’s newly learned FCR. Thus, have hypothesized that resurgence represents a
primary caregivers may often function as dis- major contributor to treatment relapse for a
criminative stimuli that exert counter-therapeutic variety of behavior disorders (Leitenberg,
stimulus control (occasioning problem behavior Rawson, & Bath, 1970; Lieving, Hagopian,
rather than the FCR). Therefore, we recently ini- Long, & O’Connor, 2004; Lieving & Lattal,
tiated a research project to determine whether 2003; Mace et al., 2010; Pritchard et al., 2014;
multiple schedules could be used to facilitate Volkert et al., 2009; Winterbauer & Bouton,
transfer of FCT treatment effects to primary 2010). In basic research, resurgence is a tempo-
caregivers. Thus far, we have evaluated this pos- rary phenomenon. That is, the target response
sibility with one primary caregiver using a increases shortly after the disruptor is intro-
multiple-baseline-across-­b ehavioral-function duced, but responding typically decreases
design (Greer, Fisher, Lichtblau, Mitteer, & thereafter, often returning to the previous low
Briggs, under review). These preliminary results or near-zero levels. However, with clinical pop-
replicated the findings described above for ulations in natural settings (e.g., home, school),
Fisher et al. (2015). That is, when the multiple- when resurgence of problem behavior occurs,
schedule FCT treatment was introduced with the there is a strong risk that caregivers will view
186 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

the treatment as ineffective, resort to old habits, of problem behavior relative to traditional
and deliver the consequence that previously FCT. However, additional research is needed on
reinforced problem behavior (e.g., providing the long-term efficacy of programing discrimi-
escape following problem behavior rather than native stimuli during FCT in order to mitigate
the FCR). Under such a scenario, problem relapse.
behavior is likely to increase and maintain at
unacceptable levels (St. Peter Pipkin, Vollmer,
& Sloman, 2010). Concluding Comments
The alternative reinforcement delivered for
the FCR during FCT increases treatment effi- Functional-analysis research has shown that most
cacy relative to implementation of extinction forms of problem behavior are reinforced by
alone (Lerman et al., 1999), as long as the treat- social consequences. Three operant variables that
ment is implemented with fidelity. However, are critical to the maintenance of problem behav-
when treatment is disrupted (e.g., the parent ior are the discriminative stimuli that occasion
fails to deliver reinforcement for the FCR while such behavior, the establishing operations that
tending to an infant sibling), the alternative motivate and evoke the behavior, and the conse-
reinforcers delivered during FCT actually quences that reinforce the behavior. Understanding
increase the probability that problem behavior how these variables influence problem behavior is
will resurge (cf. Mace et al., 2010). BMT pre- critical to the development of effective interven-
dicts that any additional reinforcers delivered in tions. Behavior analysts should consider a num-
the stimulus context in which problem behavior ber of issues when selecting the most appropriate
historically produced reinforcement (e.g., via function-based intervention for a given patient. It
FCT or NCR) contributes to the momentum of is also important to adjust the treatment over time
problem behavior and increases the likelihood to ensure that it can be implemented in a practical
of resurgence when treatment disruption occurs. manner by caregivers in the natural environment.
Mace et al. provided data showing that training One such approach that has considerable empiri-
the FCR in a different stimulus context can help cal support is implementing FCT and then bring-
to circumvent this problem. Fuhrman, Fisher, ing the FCR under the stimulus control of a
and Greer (2016) similarly showed that bring- multiple schedule while placing problem behav-
ing the FCR and problem behavior under the ior on extinction. This approach can facilitate the
stimulus control of a multiple schedule also has rapid transfer of treatment effects across settings
the potential to mitigate or prevent resurgence and therapists and may mitigate treatment relapse
of problem behavior when treatment with FCT in the form of resurgence. However, the long-term
is disrupted. efficacy of this approach, and all function-based
Fuhrman et al. (2016) trained two participants interventions, for that matter, should be the focus
to emit the FCR in the presence of an SD and not of future research.
in the presence of an SΔ and then thinned the
reinforcement schedule by lengthening the dura-
tion of the SΔ relative to that of the SD. They then References
introduced an extinction challenge with the SΔ in
place to determine whether this preparation of Austin, J. E., & Tiger, J. H. (2015). Providing alternative
reinforcers to facilitate tolerance to delayed reinforce-
FCT would better mitigate resurgence of prob- ment following functional communication training.
lem behavior when compared to that of a tradi- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 663–668.
tional form of FCT that did not include Beavers, G. A., Iwata, B. A., & Lerman, D. C. (2013).
discriminative stimuli either during training or Thirty years of research on the functional analysis
of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
during the extinction challenge. Results for both Analysis, 46, 1–21.
participants suggested FCT implemented with Betz, A. M., Fisher, W. W., Roane, H. S., Mintz, J. C.,
the multiple schedule mitigated the resurgence & Owen, T. M. (2013). A component analysis of
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 187

reinforcer-­schedule thinning during functional com- gent delivery of high- and low-preference stimuli on
munication training. Journal of Applied Behavior attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of
Analysis, 46, 219–241. Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 79–83.
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior Fisher, W. W., DeLeon, I. G., Rodriguez-Catter, V., &
problems through functional communication training. Keeney, K. M. (2004). Enhancing the effects of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. extinction on attention-maintained behavior through
Carr, J. E., Coriaty, S., Wilder, D. A., Gaunt, B. T., Dozier, noncontingent delivery of attention or stimuli identi-
C. L., Britton, L. N., … Reed, C. L. (2000). A review fied via a competing stimulus assessment. Journal of
of “noncontingent” reinforcement as treatment for the Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 171–184.
aberrant behavior of individuals with developmental Fisher, W. W., Rodriguez, N. M., Luczynski, K. C.,
disabilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, & Kelley, M. E. (2013). Protective equipment. In
21, 377–391. D. Reed, F. DiGennaro Reed, & J. Luiselli (Eds.),
Carr, J. E., Severtson, J. M., & Lepper, T. L. (2009). Handbook of crisis intervention for individuals with
Noncontingent reinforcement is an empirically sup- developmental disabilities. New York, NY: Springer.
ported treatment for problem behavior exhibited by Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Querim, A. C., & DeRosa,
individuals with developmental disabilities. Research N. (2014). Decreasing excessive functional commu-
in Developmental Disabilities, 30, 44–57. nication responses while treating destructive behavior
Charlop, M. H., Schreibman, L., & Thibodeau, M. G. using response restriction. Research in Developmental
(1985). Increasing spontaneous verbal responding in Disabilities, 35, 2614–2623.
autistic children using a time delay procedure. Journal Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Fuhrman, A. M., & Querim,
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 155–166. A. C. (2015). Using multiple schedules during func-
Conners, J., Iwata, B. A., Kahng, S. W., Hanley, tional communication training to promote rapid trans-
G. P., Worsdell, A. S., & Thompson, R. H. (2000). fer of treatment effects. Journal of Applied Behavior
Differential responding in the presence and absence of Analysis, 48, 713–733.
discriminative stimuli during multielement functional Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., & Bouxsein, K. (under
analyses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, review). Developing function-based reinforcement
299–308. procedures for problem behavior. In W. W. Fisher, C.
DeRosa, N. M., Fisher, W. W., & Steege, M. W. (2015). C. Piazza, & H. S. Roane (Eds.), Handbook of applied
An evaluation of time in establishing operation on the behavior analysis. (2nd ed., pp. X–Y). New York:
effectiveness of functional communication training. Guilford Publishing.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 115–130. Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Mitteer, D. R., Fuhrman,
Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional com- A. M., Romani, P. W., & Zangrillo, A. N. (in press).
munication training to reduce challenging behavior: Further evaluation of differential exposure to estab-
Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal lishing operations during functional communication
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 251–264. training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis.
Fisher, W. W., & Mazur, J. E. (1997). Basic and applied Fritz, J. N., Iwata, B. A., Hammond, J. L., & Bloom,
research on choice responding. Journal of Applied S. E. (2013). Experimental analysis of precursors to
Behavior Analysis, 30, 387–410. severe problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R., Analysis, 46, 101–129.
Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com- Fuhrman, A. M., Fisher, W. W., & Greer, B. D. (2016).
munication training with and without extinction and A preliminary investigation on improving functional
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, communication training by mitigating resurgence of
26, 23–36. destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Fisher, W. W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Kurtz, P. F., Analysis, 49, 884–899.
Sherer, M. R., & Lachman, S. R. (1994a). A prelimi- Fyffe, C. E., Kahng, S., Fittro, E., & Russell, D. (2004).
nary evaluation of empirically derived consequences Functional analysis and treatment of inappropri-
for the treatment of pica. Journal of Applied Behavior ate sexual behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 27, 447–457. Analysis, 37, 401–404.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, Goh, H., Iwata, B. A., & DeLeon, I. G. (2000).
L. P., & Langdon, N. A. (1994b). Empirically derived Competition between noncontingent and contingent
consequences: A data-based method for prescrib- reinforcement schedules during response acquisition.
ing treatments for destructive behavior. Research in Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 195–205.
Developmental Disabilities, 15, 133–149. Greer, B. D., Neidert, P. L., Dozier, C. L., Payne, S. W.,
Fisher, W. W., Ninness, H. A., Piazza, C. C., & Owen-­ Zonneveld, K. L. M., & Harper, A. M. (2013).
DeSchryver, J. S. (1996). On the reinforcing effects Functional analysis and treatment of problem behav-
of the content of verbal behavior. Journal of Applied ior in early education classrooms. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 29, 235–238. Behavior Analysis, 46, 289–295.
Fisher, W. W., O’Conner, J. T., Kurtz, P. F., DeLeon, I. G., Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Saini, V., Owen, T. M., &
& Gotjen, D. L. (2000). The effects of noncontin- Jones, J. K. (2016a). Functional communication train-
188 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

ing during reinforcement schedule thinning: An analy- Iwata, B. A., & Dozier, C. L. (2008). Clinical applica-
sis of 25 applications. Journal of Applied Behavior tion of functional analysis methodology. Behavior
Analysis, 49, 105–121. Analysis in Practice, 1, 3–9.
Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Romani, P. W., & Saini, V. Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kalsher, M. J., Cowdery, G. E.,
(2016b). Behavioral momentum theory: A tutorial on & Cataldo, M. F. (1990a). Experimental analysis and
response persistence. The Behavior Analyst, 39, 269–291. extinction of self-injurious escape behavior. Journal of
Greer, B. D., Fisher, W. W., Lichtblau, K. R., Mitteer, D. Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 11–27.
R., & Briggs, A. M. (under review). Using multiple Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Kissel, R. C., Nau, P. A., &
and chained schedules during functional communica- Farber, J. M. (1990b). The self-injury trauma (SIT)
tion training to promote rapid transfer of treatment scale: A method for quantifying surface tissue damage
effects to a caregiver. caused by self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., & Legacy, S. M. (1994). Behavior Analysis, 23, 99–110.
Schedule effects of noncontingent reinforcement on Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Rodgers,
attention-maintained destructive behavior in identical T. A. (1993). Treatment classification and selection
quadruplets. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, based on behavioral function. In R. Van Houten &
27, 317–325. S. Axelrod (Eds.), Behavior analysis and treatment
Hagopian, L. P., Fisher, W. W., Thibault-Sullivan, M., (pp. 102–125). New York, NY: Plenum.
Acquisto, J., & LeBlanc, L. A. (1998). Effectiveness Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Dorsey, M. F., Zarcone, J. R.,
of functional communication training with and with- Vollmer, T. R., Smith, R. G., … Willis, K. D. (1994a).
out extinction and punishment: A summary of twenty-­ The functions of self-injurious behavior: An experi-
one inpatient cases. Journal of Applied Behavior mental–epidemiological analysis. Journal of Applied
Analysis, 31, 211–235. Behavior Analysis, 27, 215–240.
Hagopian, L. P., Contrucci Kuhn, S. A., Long, E. S., & Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E.,
Rush, K. S. (2005). Schedule thinning following & Richman, G. S. (1994b). Toward a functional
communication training: Using competing stimuli to analysis of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior
enhance tolerance to decrements in reinforcer density. Analysis, 27, 197–209. (Reprinted from Analysis
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 177–193. and Intervention in Developmental Disabilities, 2,
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Contrucci, 3–20,1982).
S. A., & Maglieri, K. A. (1997a). Evaluation of cli- Iwata, B. A., Pace, G. M., Cowdery, G. E., & Miltenberger,
ent preference for function-based treatment packages. R. G. (1994c). What makes extinction work: An
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 459–473. analysis of procedural form and function. Journal of
Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997b). Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 131–144.
Noncontingent presentation of attention and alterna- Kahng, B. A., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Worsdell,
tive stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained A. S. (1997). Evaluation of the “control over rein-
destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior forcement” component in functional communication
Analysis, 30, 229–237. training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30,
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & Thompson, R. H. (2001). 267–277.
Reinforcement schedule thinning following treatment Kahng, S. W., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., & Wallace,
with functional communication training. Journal of M. D. (2000). A comparison of procedures for pro-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 17–38. gramming noncontingent reinforcement schedules.
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 223–231.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. Kodak, T., Northup, J., & Kelley, M. E. (2007). An evalu-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. ation of the types of attention that maintain problem
Hernandez, E., Hanley, G. P., Ingvarsson, E. I., & Tiger, behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40,
J. H. (2007). A preliminary evaluation of the emer- 167–171.
gence of novel mand forms. Journal of Applied Kurtz, P. F., Boelter, E. W., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Chin,
Behavior Analysis, 40, 137–156. M. D., & Hagopian, L. P. (2011). An analysis of
Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of functional communication training as an empirically
response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. ­supported treatment for problem behavior displayed
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, by individuals with intellectual disabilities. Research
267–272. in Developmental Disabilities, 32, 2935–2942.
Horner, R. H., & Day, H. (1991). The effects of response Lalli, J. S., Casey, S., & Kates, K. (1995). Reducing
efficiency on functionally equivalent competing escape behavior and increasing task completion
behaviors. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, with functional communication training, extinction,
719–732. and response chaining. Journal of Applied Behavior
Ingvarsson, E. T., Kahng, S., & Hausman, N. L. (2008). Analysis, 28, 261–268.
Some effects of noncontingent positive reinforce- Lalli, J. S., Casey, S. D., & Kates, K. (1997). Noncontingent
ment on multiply controlled problem behavior and reinforcement as treatment for severe problem behav-
compliance in a demand context. Journal of Applied ior: Some procedural variations. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 41, 435–440. Behavior Analysis, 30, 127–137.
11  Socially Reinforced Problem Behavior 189

Lalli, J. S., Vollmer, T. R., Progar, P. R., Wright, tingent reinforcement delivery on problem behavior
C., Borrero, J., Daniel, D., … May, W. (1999). maintained by escape. Journal of Applied Behavior
Competition between positive and negative reinforce- Analysis, 47, 277–292.
ment in the treatment of escape behavior. Journal of Nevin, J. A., & Shahan, T. A. (2011). Behavioral momen-
Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 285–296. tum theory: Equations and applications. Journal of
Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. Applied Behavior Analysis, 44, 877–895.
(2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe Pace, G. M., Iwata, B. A., Cowdery, G. E., Andree, P. J.,
them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied & McIntyre, T. (1993). Stimulus (instructional) fading
Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414. during extinction of self-injurious behavior. Journal of
Leitenberg, H., Rawson, R. A., & Bath, K. (1970). Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 205–212.
Reinforcement of competing behavior during extinc- Payne, S. W., & Dozier, C. L. (2013). Positive reinforce-
tion. Science, 169, 301–303. ment as treatment for problem behavior maintained by
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996a). A methodology negative reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior
for distinguishing between extinction and punish- Analysis, 46, 699–703.
ment effects of response blocking. Journal of Applied Peck, S. M., Wacker, D. P., Berg, W. K., Cooper, L. J.,
Behavior Analysis, 29, 231–233. Brown, K. A., Richman, D., … Millard, T. (1996).
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1996b). Developing a tech- Choice-making treatment of young children’s severe
nology for the use of operant extinction in clinical set- behavior problems. Journal of Applied Behavior
tings: An examination of basic and applied research. Analysis, 29, 263–290.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 345–382. Piazza, C. C., Moes, D. R., & Fisher, W. W. (1996).
Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the sta- Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior and
tus of knowledge for using punishment: Implications demand fading in the treatment of escape-maintained
for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464. Analysis, 29, 569–572.
Lerman, D. C., Iwata, B. A., & Wallace, M. D. (1999). Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., Remick, M. L.,
Side effects of extinction: Prevalence of bursting Contrucci, S. A., & Aitken, T. L. (1997). The use of
and aggression during the treatment of self-injurious positive and negative reinforcement in the treatment
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, of escape-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of
1–8. Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 279–298.
Lieving, G. A., & Lattal, K. A. (2003). Recency, repeat- Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., Hanley, G. P., LeBlanc, L. A.,
ability, and reinforcer retrenchment: An experimental Worsdell, A. S., Lindauer, S. E., & Keeney, K. M.
analysis of resurgence. Journal of the Experimental (1998). Treatment of pica through multiple analyses of
Analysis of Behavior, 80, 217–233. its reinforcing functions. Journal of Applied Behavior
Lieving, G. A., Hagopian, L. P., Long, E. S., & O'Connor, Analysis, 31, 165–189.
J. (2004). Response-class hierarchies and resurgence Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Contrucci, S. A., Delia,
of severe problem behavior. The Psychological M. D., Adelinis, J. D., & Goh, H. (1999). An evalu-
Record, 54, 621–634. ation of the properties of attention as reinforcement
Lomas, J. E., Fisher, W. W., & Kelley, M. E. (2010). The for destructive and appropriate behavior. Journal of
effects of variable-time delivery of food items and Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 437–449.
praise on problem behavior reinforced by escape. Poling, A., & Normand, M. (1999). Noncontingent rein-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 425–435. forcement: An inappropriate description of time-based
Mace, F. C., McComas, J. J., Mauro, B. C., Progar, P. R., schedules that reduce behavior. Journal of Applied
Taylor, B., Ervin, R., & Zangrillo, A. N. (2010). Behavior Analysis, 32, 237–238.
Differential reinforcement of alternative behavior Pritchard, D., Hoerger, M., & Mace, F. C. (2014).
increases resistance to extinction: Clinical demonstra- Treatment relapse and behavioral momentum
tion, animal modeling, and clinical test of one solution. theory. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 47,
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 93, 814–833.
349–367. Richman, D. M., Barnard-Brak, L., Grubb, L., Bosch, A.,
Marcus, B. A., & Vollmer, T. R. (1996). Combining & Abby, L. (2015). Meta-analysis of noncontingent
noncontingent reinforcement and differential rein- reinforcement effects on problem behavior. Journal of
forcement schedules as treatment for aberrant Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, 131–152.
behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Roane, H. S., Fisher, W. W., Sgro, G. M., Falcomata,
29, 43–51. T. S., & Pabico, R. R. (2004). An alternative method
Mazaleski, J. L., Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, of thinning reinforcer delivery during differential rein-
J. R., & Smith, R. G. (1993). Analysis of the reinforce- forcement. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,
ment and extinction components in DRO contingen- 213–218.
cies with self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Roane, H. S., Falcomata, T. S., & Fisher, W. W. (2007).
Analysis, 26, 143–156. Applying the behavioral economics principle of unit
Mevers, J. E. L., Fisher, W. W., Kelley, M. E., & Fredricks, price to DRO schedule thinning. Journal of Applied
L. (2014). The effects of variable-time versus con- Behavior Analysis, 40, 529–534.
190 B.D. Greer and W.W. Fisher

Rooker, G. W., Jessel, J., Kurtz, P., & Hagopian, L. P. The right to effective behavioral treatment. Journal of
(2013). Functional communication training with and Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 381–384.
without alternative reinforcement and punishment: Volkert, V. M., Lerman, D. C., Call, N. A., & Trosclair-­
An analysis of 58 applications. Journal of Applied Lasserre, N. (2009). An evaluation of resurgence dur-
Behavior Analysis, 46, 708–722. ing treatment with functional communication training.
Roscoe, E. M., Iwata, B. A., & Rand, M. S. (2003). Effects Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 145–160.
of reinforcer consumption and magnitude on response Vollmer, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Zarcone, J. R., Smith, R. G.,
rates during noncontingent reinforcement. Journal of & Mazaleski, J. L. (1993). The role of attention in
Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 525–539. the treatment of attention-maintained self-injurious
Shore, B. A., Iwata, B. A., DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S., & behavior: Noncontingent reinforcement and differ-
Smith, R. G. (1997). An analysis of reinforcer sub- ential reinforcement of other behavior. Journal of
stitutability using object manipulation and SIB as Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 9–21.
competing responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., & Ringdahl, J. E. (1995).
Analysis, 30, 21–41. Noncontingent escape as treatment for self-injurious
Smith, R. G., Russo, L., & Le, D. D. (1999). Distinguishing behavior maintained by negative reinforcement.
between extinction and punishment effects of response Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 15–26.
blocking: A replication. Journal of Applied Behavior Winterbauer, N. E., & Bouton, M. E. (2010). Mechanisms
Analysis, 32, 367–370. of resurgence of an extinguished instrumental behav-
St. Peter Pipkin, C., Vollmer, T. R., & Sloman, K. N. ior. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
(2010). Effects of treatment integrity failures during Behavior Processes, 36, 343.
differential reinforcement of alternative behavior: Zangrillo, A. N., Fisher, W. W., Greer, B. D., Owen, T. M.,
A translational model. Journal of Applied Behavior & DeSouza, A. A. (2016). Treatment of escape-main-
Analysis, 43, 47–70. tained challenging behavior using chained schedules:
Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technol- an evaluation of the effects of thinning positive plus
ogy of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior negative reinforcement during functional communica-
Analysis, 10, 349–367. tion training. International Journal of Developmental
Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Bruzek, J. (2008). Functional Disabilities, 62, 147–156.
communication training: A review and practical guide. Zarcone, J. R., Iwata, B. A., Hughes, C. E., & Vollmer,
Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1, 16–23. T. R. (1993). Momentum versus extinction effects
Van Houten, R., Axelrod, S., Bailey, J. S., Favell, J. E., in the treatment of self-injurious escape behavior.
Foxx, R. M., Iwata, B. A., & Lovaas, O. I. (1988). Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 26, 135–136.
Philosophy and Common
Components of Early Intensive 12
Behavioral Interventions

Rebecca MacDonald, Diana Parry-Cruwys,
and Pamela Peterson

in very young children who later receive a diag-


Introduction nosis of autism. The early work of Berry
Brazelton indicates that infants are born with a
Over the past three decades, an expansive body phylogenetic tendency to orient to and follow
of empirical research has amassed documenting social stimuli such as a face and a voice
the impact that Early Intensive Behavioral (Brazelton, Koslowski, & Main, 1974). At around
Intervention (EIBI) can have on the lives of chil- 5 months old, infants are able to detect changes
dren with autism and their families. Using the in eye gaze during social interactions. They smile
principles of applied behavior analysis, EIBI pro- more when adults look at them and less when
grams can produce large gains in language, cog- adults’ eyes are averted (Symons, Hains, & Muir,
nitive, and social behavior resulting in 1998); furthermore, they respond differentially to
remediation of the core deficits in children with adult affective behaviors such as smiling or
autism. Lovaas’s (1987) seminal study first docu- frowning (Rochat & Striano, 1999; Trevarthen,
mented that young children who received 40 h of 1979). As early as 6 months of age, infants follow
intensive behavioral treatment for 2 years made their mothers’ gaze shifts to objects in the envi-
substantially greater gains than children who ronment (Morales, Mundy, & Rojas, 1998). At
received less intensive services. More recently 9–12 months of age, infants start to engage in
Howard, Stanislaw, Green, Cohen, and Sparkman joint attention and social referencing, which
(2014) again substantiated the powerful impact involves turning to look at the adult’s face when
of behaviorally based interventions compared to presented with something novel in their environ-
more eclectic, community-based treatment mod- ment (Feinman, 1982; Moore & Corkum, 1994).
els. The goal of this chapter will be to review the By 1 year of age, typically developing children
evidence supporting EIBI, to identify its critical are fully engaged in their social surroundings.
elements, and to describe the implementation of They orient to their name and engage in eye con-
these elements in the EIBI literature. tact, referential pointing, and social referencing
There is an established body of literature (Osterling & Dawson, 1994) – these social
focusing on early identification markers present behaviors occur with adults and peers alike. The
relevant experimental question which remains is
that concerning the point at which this pattern of
R. MacDonald (*) • D. Parry-Cruwys • P. Peterson
responding starts to deviate in children who are
The New England Center for Children,
Southborough, MA, USA later diagnosed with autism. One way to evaluate
e-mail: bmacdonald@necc.org this change in trajectory is to conduct prospective

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 191


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_12
192 R. MacDonald et al.

studies of patterns of behavior from birth through who were later diagnosed with ASD showed
the third year of life. Researchers who are part of lower scores on language, visual discrimination,
the Baby Siblings Research Consortium have and fine motor tasks. More recently Landa et al.
examined the developmental course of an autism (2013) evaluated the social language and social
diagnosis (Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006; Landa, development of children who were diagnosed
Gross, Stuart, & Faherty, 2013; Ozonoff et al., early (by 18 months) versus children who
2010, 2015; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). Siblings received a later diagnosis (by 36 months). Again,
are used in these prospective studies because evi- at 6 months the children were undifferentiated,
dence suggests that the reoccurrence risk for but the early-diagnosed children began to show
autism in siblings is 18.7% at 3 years old (Ozonoff clinically deviant language development at
et al., 2011). 14 months, while the later-diagnosed children
showed only subclinical speech and motor delays
at 14–18 months of age. By 36 months both
Early ASD Markers groups showed impairments in language and
social behavior. Of concern is the fact that while
Early screening and detection tools have allowed 41% of the early-diagnosed children entered
children as young as 12–18 months to receive an early intervention prior to 18 months, only 12%
ASD diagnosis and thus begin treatment. The of the later-diagnosed children received early
Autism Observation Scale for Infants (AOSI; intervention. Considering that both the early- and
Bryson, Zwaigenbaum, McDermott, Rombough, later-diagnosed groups would have seemingly
& Brian, 2008) is a semi-structured play-based benefitted from early intervention, additional
evaluation in which 18 risk facts are assessed research has continued to look for reliable early
using a 0–3 rating scale; Zwaigenbaum et al. markers of ASD which would allow for early,
(2005) found that as early as 12 months of age the confident diagnosis and subsequent intervention.
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) group was dis- While most studies using IQ and autism-­
tinguishable from those children who would not specific diagnostic tools show undifferentiated
receive a diagnosis by 3 years of age. These atyp- development before 12 months, Jones and Klin
icalities fell into three major categories: social/ (2013) have documented earlier differences in
language, visual attention, and early tempera- patterns of eye fixation. Using eye tracking mea-
ment. Specifically, children showed reduced eye surements, they found that 4- to 6-month-old
contact, failure to orient to name, lack of imita- infants showed differences in percentages of eye
tion and smiling, and delayed receptive and fixation while viewing a video of a person talk-
expressive language. In addition, they showed ing. The children later diagnosed with ASD were
prolonged latency to disengage their visual atten- more likely to visually fixate on the mouth and
tion. Their temperament was more passive, and hands of the speaker, while children who were
they showed an extreme distress reaction to envi- not diagnosed were more likely to focus on the
ronmental changes as well as visual fixation on speaker’s eyes. These are the first data suggesting
objects. Landa and Garrett-Mayer (2006) evalu- that this restricted pattern of visual fixation is
ated 60 siblings at ages 6, 14, and 24 months, present at such an early age, although these
using the Mullen Scales of Early Learning, results stand in contrast to previous research in
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule which decreased eye contact at 6 months was not
(ADOS), and clinical judgment to evaluate tra- predictive of an ASD diagnosis for at-risk sib-
jectory deviations. They categorized children as lings (Young, Merin, Rogers, & Ozonoff, 2009).
either autism spectrum disorder, language Though these differences appear to be subclini-
delayed, or unimpaired at age 3. They found that cal, meaning they are not observable in daily
at 6 months there were no differences between interactions with these children, they may sug-
groups, but by 14 months, there were clear devia- gest very early emergence of some of the markers
tions in developmental trajectories. The children of autism.
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 193

The preponderance of evidence, however, educational placement of children with autism


suggests that motor, cognition, and prelinguistic (CWA) laid the groundwork for the large-scale
language development are intact at 6 months old use of applied behavior analytic techniques in
and that autism symptomatology emerges during autism education. In this study, 19 CWA were
the latter half of the first year of life. This could assigned to the EIBI group receiving 40 h per
suggest an environmental influence in the devel- week of 1:1 behavioral treatment, and 19 CWA
opment of autism. Infants at risk for autism spend were assigned to a minimal treatment group
less time interacting socially with adults and receiving 10 h per week of 1:1 behavioral treat-
other children in their environment. If a child ment. Children included in the study were less
fails to initiate eye contact with others, is less than 40 months of age. Assignment to groups
responsive to the social initiations of others, and was based on availability of therapists. A second
engages in less language and play, they are less control group of 21 CWA receiving minimal
likely to engage in sustained social interactions. eclectic intervention in a different facility was
If a child does not reciprocate a parent’s social also included. After 2 years of treatment, the
initiations, there is an increased risk that these experimental group demonstrated statistically
social initiations on the part of the adult will be significant increases in IQ over the control
extinguished, resulting in a reduced number of groups, and differences in placement between the
daily social interactions provided by the adult groups were also statistically significant. In the
(Adamson, McArthur, Markov, Dunbar, & experimental group, nine children were consid-
Bakeman, 2001). A central goal of EIBI is active ered recovered, meaning they entered a regular
learning in the context of others. The content of education class by first grade and had an IQ
programming focuses on both the cognitive/lan- within normal range. Eight children were consid-
guage and social areas of development by requir- ered aphasic, meaning they were placed in a first
ing language and play-based interaction on the grade class but continued to receive additional
part of the child, first with adults and then with support for a language delay and had an IQ dem-
peers. Evidence suggests that environmental onstrating mild impairment. Two children were
enrichment can play a role in the developing considered autistic, meaning they were placed in
brain (Dawson, 2008). While there is some evi- a substantially separate class and demonstrated
dence for this in animal models (Loupe, an IQ in the severely impaired range. Using the
Schroeder, & Tessel, 1995), any evidence using same categories for the control groups, one child
humans is in its infancy. That being said, evaluat- was considered recovered, 18 were considered
ing the impact of EIBI on the remediation of aphasic, and 21 remained in the autistic group
atypical patterns of behavior in children who are following 2 years of treatment. Significant differ-
diagnosed with autism is an exciting venture for ences in functioning level and IQ remained pres-
behavior analysts. If the environment can play a ent for these groups in a follow-up study
role in the development of autism symptomatol- conducted 6 years later (McEachin, Smith, &
ogy, then harnessing the power of environmental Lovaas, 1993). The original nine best outcome
arrangements using the principles of applied participants in the EIBI group continued to dem-
behavior analysis has the potential for reversing onstrate IQs within the normal range and had
this deviant developmental pattern. reduced levels of maladaptive behavior, aside
from one participant who returned to the special
education setting. Lovaas’s study continues to
Seminal Research serve as a landmark example of the potential of
EIBI treatment to produce and maintain levels of
A review of early EIBI research highlights sev- typical functioning in CWA.
eral integral components of successful EIBI Although Lovaas’s (1987) study demonstrated
treatment. Lovaas’s seminal 1987 study examin- significant gains for its participants receiving
ing the effects of EIBI on the IQ and subsequent EIBI, participants were not randomly assigned to
194 R. MacDonald et al.

groups. Smith, Groen, and Wynn (2000) com- differences were not seen between the two treat-
pleted a randomized controlled trial (RCT) ment groups, but both demonstrated a positive
involving 28 CWA (mean age of 36 months) who outcome in favor of EIBI. Sallows and Graupner
were randomly assigned in matched pairs to extended Lovaas’s finding, demonstrating that,
either an experimental group or a parent-training independent of setting and level of therapist
group. The experimental group (comprised of 15 supervision, a high percentage of CWAs receiv-
children) received 30 h per week of center-based ing EIBI can achieve a normal IQ.
EIBI, and the parent-training group (comprised Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, Green, and
of 14 children) received 5 h per week of parent Stanis-law (2005) compared EIBI treatment
training in EIBI, along with their regular public group of 29 children (receiving 1:1 treatment,
education program, which provided 10–15 h of 25–40 h per week) with two treatment groups,
eclectic treatment per week. At follow-up, chil- one receiving intensive eclectic treatment (receiv-
dren who participated in the experimental group ing 1:1 or 1:2 instruction for 30 h per week) and
showed significant gains in IQ, receptive and one receiving non-intensive early intervention
expressive language, and visual spatial skills over provided by the public school (receiving small
the control group. Additionally, four children group instruction for 15 h per week). Pre- and
from the experimental group were placed in regu- posttreatment assessments included measures for
lar education with no support at follow-up, com- IQ, language, nonverbal intelligence, and adap-
pared to none from the control group. These tive behavior. No differences were seen at intake
results provide a final requisite piece, randomiza- between groups, but after 14 months of treat-
tion of groups, to show that EIBI is a superior ment, the EIBI group showed statistically signifi-
model to eclectic treatment when attempting to cant gains over both control groups in all areas.
improve functioning in young CWA. The EIBI group scored, on average, in the normal
Few studies rival Lovaas’s in the percentage range on cognitive, nonverbal, and communica-
of treatment group reaching a level of typical tion skills. In addition, the EIBI group showed a
functioning, an exception being a study by higher learning rate for receptive and expressive
Sallows and Graupner (2005). These researchers language than did the control groups following
randomly assigned 23 CWAs (aged 24–42 months, treatment. The authors suggest that the quality of
IQ 35 or higher) to a parent-managed or clinic-­ treatment as measured by learning opportunities
managed treatment group. Both groups received was more instrumental in producing large gains
EIBI treatment, with the parent-managed group for children with autism than merely length of
receiving 6–7 fewer hours per week of services time per week in treatment. In a recent follow-up
than the clinic-managed group, which averaged report of these children, the original findings still
39 h per week of treatment during the first year. remain (Howard et al., 2014): children from the
The parent-managed group received approxi- EIBI treatment group, now in elementary school,
mately 6 h per month of in-home supervision by continue to show greater gains than the children
a trained EIBI therapist, compared to the clinic-­ in other treatment groups.
managed group, which received 6–10 h per week Recently, MacDonald, Parry-Cruwys, Dupere,
of supervision. In a pre-/posttreatment compari- and Ahearn (2014) evaluated the effects of EIBI on
son, significant changes in IQ were seen for sev- 83 toddlers with autism who entered treatment
eral children in both groups. After 1 year of before the age of 3. All children participated in
treatment, five children in the clinic-managed 20–30 h per week of EIBI. Data from these chil-
group and three children in the parent-managed dren was compared to 58 same-aged peers using a
group achieved IQ scores within the normal func- direct measurement tool called the Early Skills
tioning range (at least 85), and after 3–4 years of Assessment Tool (ESAT; MacDonald et al., 2006),
treatment, three additional children in the parent-­ which assessed cognitive/language, joint attention,
managed group also reached this level. In total, play, and stereotypy. Children were categorized
11 of 23 (48%) of the participants in the study into three groups by age at entry into treatment.
achieved a normal IQ posttreatment. Significant These groups included children 18–23 months old
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 195

(1-year-olds), 24–29 months old (early 2-year- ture since that time, seven of the studies had a
olds), 30–36 months old (late 2-year-olds), and lower age limit of 24 months at the beginning of
36–48 months old (3-year-­olds). While they found treatment, and six of the studies began working
significant gains in all groups, the greatest gains with children who were 1 year old. It is difficult
were seen in the youngest group. Over 90% of the to determine the actual number of children of
1-year-olds were close to their typical age-matched each age group in these studies, as age is often
peers after 13 months of treatment, while the per- not a factor in evaluating the efficacy of the pro-
centage of children achieving similar gains was cedure. However, there are two studies that
reduced as age of entry increased. Seven of the clearly show the effects of beginning treatment
eight children who entered treatment at 1 year of before the child’s second birthday. Green et al.
age and were available for follow-up were per- (2002) demonstrated in a single-case analysis
forming at grade level (grades 3–7), had lost their that EIBI resulted in rapid learning and eventual
diagnosis, and were full members of their school loss of diagnosis for a child who began treatment
and community. These findings suggest that begin- at 14 months old. MacDonald et al. (2014) com-
ning treatment before the second birthday results pared data from children who began treatment
in the best outcome. prior to their second birthday versus children
who began after their second birthday and found
that 90% of the children who began treatment at
 ommon Elements of Early
C 1 year old were performing within two standard
Intensive Behavioral Treatment deviations of their typical same age peers on
direct measures of cognition, joint attention, and
The question remains: What exactly is EIBI and play a year later; children who began treatment at
how does it differ from other early intervention 30–48 months later made less dramatic gains.
treatment models? Early Intensive Behavioral These data suggest that treatment should begin as
Intervention (EIBI) is based in the principles of early as possible, underscoring the need for early
applied behavior analysis (ABA). While all EIBI screening and diagnosis.
programs are not the same, there are a number of Age at intake seems to be inversely correlated
features that are commonly reported in research with better outcomes (Makrygianni & Reed,
programs. Several authors have identified key 2010; Perry et al., 2011). All but 1 of the 12 stud-
elements that are common in most EIBI pro- ies summarized in Table 12.1 worked with a pop-
grams (Green, 2011; Green, Brennan, & Fein, ulation whose mean age was between 30 and
2002; Lovaas, 2003). An analysis of the literature 36 months; however only three studies actually
reveals striking similarities in delivery of services started with groups of children who were all
across studies. Table 12.1 summarizes the 12 under 3 years of age (Green et al., 2002;
most cited articles in which EIBI procedures MacDonald et al., 2014; Zachor, Ben-Itzchak,
were implemented with young children with Rabinovich, & Lahat, 2007). EIBI requires care-
ASD. We will reference these studies as we ful systematic building of skills, and the earlier
review the common elements of EIBI. the treatment can begin, the better the chances of
changing the trajectory of the ASD condition. In
this chapter we will focus on those programs
Begin Treatment Early beginning treatment before 3 years of age.

As you can see from the data, the earlier treat-


ment begins, the better the outcomes. In the Behavioral Conceptual Framework
1980s, Lovaas established the Young Autism
Project at UCLA. The focus of the project was to Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention is a treat-
begin intensive behavioral treatment before ment model that is based on the principles of
40 months of age. His 1987 study was the culmi- applied behavior analysis. Beginning with the
nation of this early work. In reviewing the litera- Lovaas UCLA treatment model, a behavioral
Table 12.1  Summary of treatment components
196

Behavioral Instruction/ Intensity and


Study Ages start framework Family participation setting Inclusion Curriculum duration Quality assurance
Lovaas 34–40 months Behavioral Parents trained in 1:1 home based Supported Lovaas “The ME 40 h for ABA-trained student
(1987)c techniquesa ABA, extend treatment preschool setting Book” (1981) 2–6 years therapists
Mean 34 months Punishmentb to all waking hours
Smith et al. <46 months Behavioral Parents trained in 1:1 home based Supported school Lovaas “The ME 30 h per Supervisors
(1997) Mean 36 months techniquesa ABA, worked with setting when Book” (1981) week, 2 years participated in UCLA
therapist 5–10 h/week; prerequisites internship, ABA
team meetings acquired trained student
therapists
Sheinkopf and 23–47 months Behavioral Parents received 1:1 in school All placed in Lovaas “The ME 27 h per Paraprofessionals and
Siegel (1998) Mean 33.8 techniquesa manual (Lovaas, 1981) setting special education Book” (1981) week, parents served as
of ABA treatment settings, moved 16 months therapists, supervised
to supported by behaviorally
classroom trained clinicians
Smith et al. 18–42 months Behavioral 5 h/week of direct 1:1 home based Supported in Lovaas “The ME 25 h per week
(2000) Mean 36 months techniquesa teaching alongside general education Book” (1981) first year,
therapist for 1st setting when 2–3 years
3 months prerequisites
acquired
Green et al. 14 months Behavioral Mother provided 3–8 h 1:1 home based Supported Matching, 25–33 h per Behavioral
(2002) techniquesa per week, collected preschool setting, imitation, week, 3+ psychologist: staff
Incidental data, attended ABA prerequisite language, play, years training and supervise
teachingb workshops required social, program 2–4 h every
generalization other week
Eikeseth et al. 4–7 years old Behavioral Worked with therapist 1:1 center Supported Lovaas “The ME 28 h first year, Behavioral
(2002, 2007) techniquesa for 4 h a week for first based mainstream Book” (1981) 31 months psychologist provide
3 months preschool setting 10 h per week of staff
outside of 1:1 supervision
sessions; playdates Weekly 2-h team
meeting
Sallows and 24–42 months Behavioral Parents trained in 1:1 home based Supported Lovaas “The ME 39 h per Supervised by senior
Graupner techniquesa ABA, participation in preschool setting Book” (1981) week, behavior therapist with
(2005) Mean Video weekly team meetings 1–2 half days a 48 months 2000 h of training.
33.23 months modeling week ABA-trained student
therapists
R. MacDonald et al.
Behavioral Instruction/ Intensity and
Study Ages start framework Family participation setting Inclusion Curriculum duration Quality assurance
Howard et al. Mean 30 months Behavioral Parents trained in 1:1 home, Supported school ABA treatment 25–40 h per Behavioral
(2005, 2014) techniquesa ABA, assist with data school, and and playdates manuals: week, psychologist provides
Naturalistic collection, community Maurice et al. 13–14 months training and staff
teachingb participation in team (1996) and supervision
General case meeting 2 times a Maurice et al.
instruction month (2001)
Cohen et al. 18–42 months Behavioral Parents attended 1:1 home based Supported UCLA 35–40 h per BCBA supervised
(2006) techniquesa 12–18 h workshop, preschool setting curriculum week, 36+ cases and trained
Mean 30 months Incidental participated in weekly outside of 1:1 months tutors. Tutors passed
teachingb training session, no sessions; playdates observation
requirement for direct assessment and oral
intervention test of UCLS manual
Zachor et al. 22–34 months Behavioral No information 1:1 center Participation in Maurice et al. 35 h a week Therapists supervised
(2007) Mean techniquesa based supported inclusion (1996) by a trained behavior
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions

27.7 months preschool setting when analyst


setting prerequisites
acquired; playdates
Remington 26–42 months Behavioral Parents delivered 1:1 1:1 homebased Supported Green et al. 26 h per Supervised by senior
et al. (2007) techniquesa treatment; reviewed and preschool preschool setting; (2002) week, behavior therapist
Natural and practiced playdates 24 months trained in ABA. They
environment modifications with trained therapists
Teachingb clinicians
MacDonald 18–36 months Behavioral Parent trained in ABA 1:1 homebased Supported Maurice et al. 20–30 h per BCBA provided
et al. (2014) techniquesa and offered weekly and preschool preschool or (1996) ACE © week, avg. training and staff
Mean: Naturalistic coaching by therapist daycare setting curriculum 13 months supervision
20.23 months teachingb
26.68 months
30.94 months
42.00 months
a
Behavioral techniques: discrete trial instruction, discrimination learning, shaping behavior, prompting and fading, task analysis, reinforcement, and generalization of data
b
Incidental/naturalistic teaching: providing instruction in the context of the natural setting and reinforcement matches behavior not arbitrary
c
Punishment: contingent use of loud “no” paired with a slap on the thigh
197
198 R. MacDonald et al.

treatment relies on the use of operant teaching opportunities that arise naturally within the
techniques. These include shaping successive child’s environment. Due to looser stimulus con-
approximations of the target behavior using posi- trol and the use of functional reinforcers, natural-
tive reinforcement, systematic use of prompting istic methods of instruction may promote
and fading procedures, and the design of struc- increased generalization and maintenance of
tured discrete trial instruction to teach language skills compared to a strictly discrete trial approach
and cognitive skills. The focus is on the establish- (McGee, Krantz, & McClannahan, 1985). While
ment of stimulus control over imitation and other only five of the articles reviewed specified natu-
discrimination performances. Direct and condi- ralistic teaching as part of their protocol (Cohen,
tioned reinforcers are determined through a vari- Amerine-Dickens, & Smith, 2006; Green et al.,
ety of preference assessments. Self-help skills are 2002; Howard et al., 2005; MacDonald et al.,
taught using task analysis and chaining proce- 2014; Remington et al., 2007), a closer examina-
dures. Special attention is given to programming tion of the Young Autism Project protocol reveals
for the generalization of these skills across stim- the systematic transfer of control from a very
uli, people, and contexts as children with autism restricted discrete trial setting to performance in
often fail to demonstrate generalization of skills a community setting. The behavioral principles
on their own. Behavioral protocols are also inte- remain the same, but the literature shows how the
gral to the reduction of behavioral excesses, like language that we use describing some of the
tantrums, aggression, and stereotypy. More same concepts has evolved over the years. The
recently, these protocols have involved a func- goal all along has been for EIBI to result in func-
tional analysis of the target behavior. Finally, tional life skills in the natural community for
data analysis is integral to an EIBI program as every child.
acquisition data are used to evaluate progress and
make decisions regarding treatment.
A review of the 12 studies that used EIBI Family Participation
reveals a remarkable consistency in the imple-
mentation of behavioral programming. All stud- Active parent participation in treatment was a
ies reported using discrete trial instruction to hallmark of Lovaas’s (1987) study. Parents were
establish discrimination learning, from beginning required to learn the behavioral teaching tech-
imitation training to more advanced language niques and expected to use these during interac-
concepts. They all report using both direct and tions with their child during all waking hours of
conditioned reinforcers in establishing skills and the day. Gains made during treatment sessions
shaping behavior. Prompting and fading are inte- are likely to be displayed only with the therapist
gral to instruction, and data are used to make pro- under very specific conditions unless they are
gram decisions. A few exceptions exist, however, practiced with family members in their home. No
beginning with Lovaas’s use of contingent pun- other studies have made these stringent require-
ishment in the form of a loud “no” or a slap on ments of parents; however, all have required
the thigh. Only one other study reported use of some level of parent participation. The degree of
any aversive stimuli (Smith et al., 2000), and parent participation varies widely across studies
these procedures were discontinued early in with respect to several aspects of intervention,
treatment. including attendance at team meetings or
Another departure from the initial Lovaas ­workshops, direct implementation of EIBI pro-
model is the introduction of more naturalistic gramming, and the collection of data. Across sev-
teaching. Incidental teaching, more recently eral studies, the minimum requirement is the
referred to as naturalistic teaching, is a less struc- parents’ active participation in team meetings on
tured alternative to traditional discrete trial train- a weekly or monthly basis with other members of
ing (DTT). These methods allow the the treatment team, including therapists and
behavior-change agent to take advantage of supervising clinicians (Eikeseth et al. 2007;
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 199

Howard et al., 2005; MacDonald et al., 2014: the child moves into more naturalistic environ-
Sallows & Graupner, 2005). Parents are often ments, such as supported inclusion settings
encouraged to acquire the relevant skills in order (Green et al., 2002; Howard et al., 2005). While
to effectively implement behavioral techniques parent participation differed in quality and quan-
and are considered an integral part of the child’s tity across the majority of reviewed studies, all
treatment team. To further the generalization and studies acknowledged the parent’s role as a part
maintenance of skills taught by trained clinicians, of the treatment team and noted the function of
as well as the provision of intervention outside of parental implementation of behavioral techniques
therapist intervention hours, several studies have with respect to the generalization of acquired
included parent-training components. Green skills across environments.
et al. (2002) and Cohen et al. (2006) required par-
ents of participants to attend quarterly and weekly
workshops, respectively, providing information Instructional Format
pertaining to the use of techniques of applied
behavior analysis and intensive intervention. In Across all studies reviewed, instruction is pro-
addition to parent participation in workshops, vided initially in a one-on-one format for at least
many parents participated in didactic instruction the first 12 months. Nine of the 12 studies pro-
with a behavior therapist who then provided vided this instruction in a home-based setting ini-
feedback for the parent. Smith and colleagues tially, while the remaining studies provided
conducted two studies in which parents were instruction in a center-based or school setting.
asked to set aside at least 5 h a week during which The protocol described by Cohen et al. (2006)
time they worked alongside the behavior thera- best illustrates the teaching format of most EIBI
pist in order to attain proficiency in the use of programs through the course of treatment. They
behavioral techniques and were required to pro- identify three primary stages: in home 1:1 instruc-
vide a number of one-to-one, direct instruction tion, peer play groups, and school inclusion.
hours (Smith, Eikeseth, Klevstrand, & Lovaas, In the beginning, much of the instructional
1997; Smith et al., 2000). programming occurs in discrete trials. Therapists
Requirements for provision of direct instruc- work individually with the child in a distraction-­
tion by parents varied widely, with some studies free environment to establish stimulus control
involving parental delivery of instruction for a over responding. For some children, they may
minimum number of hours, while others outline start with a ratio of 6–8 trials to a 1- to 2-min
no requirement for instruction (Cohen et al., break, with a longer play break at the end of each
2006; Eikeseth et al., 2002; Green, 2011; hour. These play breaks should include opportu-
Remington et al., 2007; Smith et al., 1997; Smith nities for children to practice the skills they are
et al., 2000). Sheinkopf and Siegel (1998) con- learning in discrete trial sessions, for example,
ducted a retrospective analysis in which it was requesting a toy out of reach or imitating actions
noted that it was the parents who almost exclu- during a song. Cohen and colleagues define skill
sively delivered behavioral treatment (based on acquisition as 90% accuracy with target stimuli
Lovaas’s 1981 manual). Similarly, Smith et al. and concept mastery as 90% accuracy with novel
(2000) included a parent-training comparison items. This is an important distinction when so
group in which parents delivered the majority of much early 1:1 instruction is provided in a
behavioral treatment (compared to a treatment ­discrete trial format where all variables can be
group who received more intensive intervention controlled.
provided by both professionals and parents). While discrete trial methods of instruction
Besides direct instruction, parents may also take have been empirically proven to be effective in
part in the collection of data both in home and teaching skills to young children with autism, the
across settings and may observe or provide highly structured approach does introduce con-
instruction alongside the behavioral therapist as cern with respect to the generalization and main-
200 R. MacDonald et al.

tenance of said skills. The use of mass trials while younger children were serviced in their
within a structured session with a limited number home. Regardless of the setting in which treat-
of therapists and the delivery of somewhat arbi- ment was started, the nature of instruction in
trary reinforcers introduce contingencies that center-based programs was similar to the home-­
may not exist in the child’s day-to-day environ- based teaching described in other studies. In
ment. As a result, skills acquired in the more summary, most of the EIBI programs reviewed
structured environment may fail to generalize to began treatment using 1:1 instruction in the
different settings and individuals, and may fail to child’s home.
maintain over time (Lovaas, Koegel, Simmons, &
Long, 1973; McGee et al., 1985). Given these
concerns, researchers implementing EIBI proto- Integration and Generalization
cols are careful to move quickly from discrete
trial to more natural and complex learning envi- As the child shows progress, skills are general-
ronments to practice newly acquired skills and ized across settings. A generalization criterion of
generalize them across people and stimuli. the performance of skills across teachers and set-
One-to-one instruction allows for individual- tings is included in acquisition, and incidental
ized interventions to maximize success during teaching is used to address skill acquisition
learning. Once children acquire spoken phrases, across the day, in both the home and community.
verbal requests, appropriate play, and self-help As previously mentioned, parent involvement in
skills like dressing, Smith et al. (2000) suggest treatment and the use of behavioral techniques by
they are ready for more naturalistic instruction in parents are considered essential to the generaliza-
group settings such as preschools. Skills acquired tion of skills. The majority of studies focusing on
through 1:1 instruction need to be practiced and the delivery of intensive behavioral treatment to
reinforced across a variety of novel settings, young children with autism focus on skill acqui-
stimuli, and people. This notion of the need for sition within the home setting during the first
prerequisite skills to be mastered prior to a reduc- year of treatment. During this time, treatment
tion in the intensity of instruction is common in may be delivered in the home or community set-
EIBI programs (Johnson, Meyer, & Taylor, tings, and siblings or similarly aged peers may
1996). join the child in “playdates” during which the
Green et al. (2002), in their program for a therapist or parent facilitates social skills, such as
14-month-old, reported beginning with 1:1 waiting, turn-taking, peer imitation, initiations of
instruction for 25–33 h and increasing the num- play, verbal and nonverbal interactions, and
ber of hours during their second year of interven- responses to such peer-initiated interactions
tion to 30 h of 1:1 and 6–8 h of play and (Cohen et al., 2006; Green et al., 2002; Howard
school-based time. An analysis of the skills et al., 2005; Sallows & Graupner, 2005). Peers or
acquired during the first year suggests progres- siblings were also taught to provide prompts and
sion was made in accordance with Cohen and reinforcement for the aforementioned skills in
Smith’s recommendations. order to further facilitate acquisition. Across sev-
Three studies began treatment in center-based eral studies, these skills were seen as the basis for
programs. Eikeseth et al. (2002) began treatment successful systematic integration into more natu-
with school-aged children (ages 4–7 years) using ralistic settings, such as mainstream or inclusion
1:1 instruction in a center-based setting in preschool settings; in fact, Johnson et al. (1996)
Norway, while Zachor et al. (2007) began treat- proposed specific behavioral criteria for moving
ment in a center-based preschool setting with from 1:1 individualized instruction to an inte-
children under 3 years of age in Israel. MacDonald grated setting. Proficiency in the areas of lan-
et al. (2014) began services for children 3 years guage (e.g., following directions, answering
and older in a center-based program primarily questions, and communicating needs), social
due to age-related funding source requirements, skills (e.g., turn-taking, waiting quietly, imitation
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 201

of peers, and initiations of play with peer), aca- Level of integration is often used a measure of
demic skills (e.g., observational learning, raising treatment outcome (i.e., placement within a gen-
hand), and behavior skills (e.g., responding to eral education setting, special education setting,
delayed contingencies) are suggested as prereq- with or without professional support).
uisites for placement in an inclusion setting.
Three of the studies reviewed withheld students
from such settings until the relevant skills had Comprehensive Curriculum
been acquired (Smith et al., 1997, 2000; Zachor
et al., 2007). An EIBI curriculum blends a combination of
Following acquisition of necessary prerequi- behavior analysis and typical child development
site skills, or upon clinician suggestion, children to teach across a variety of skill areas (i.e., eye
were introduced into a mainstream or inclusion contact, imitation, communication, self-care,
setting with a therapist acting as a “shadow” or etc.). In general, skills are broken down into
aide, who prompted appropriate social interac- teachable units and arranged hierarchically from
tions, as well as the following of group instruc- simple to more complex performances. Children
tions or participation in group activities. Across are engaged in active learning with an emphasis
studies, as the child progressed within the educa- on positive reinforcement. A hallmark of EIBI is
tional setting, the therapist faded her own partici- the use of direct observation and measurement to
pation within the classroom, and the number of both identify target behaviors to teach and regu-
one-to-one intervention hours provided outside larly evaluate progress in learning. Skills are tar-
of the classroom decreased, unless inspection of geted across skill domains, including functional
data indicated that the child would benefit from language and other communication skills (e.g.,
continued home-based intervention (Green et al., receptive and expressive language, following
2002; Lovaas, 1987; Smith et al., 2000). In addi- instructions), discrimination skills (e.g., session
tion to prior preparation for an integrated setting, behavior, attending, matching, higher-order read-
Green et al. (2002) and Sallows and Graupner ing, and math skills), social skills (e.g., eye con-
(2005) observed participating children in the tact in response to name, greetings, waiting,
integrated setting and incorporated modifications imitation, joint attention, play skills, peer interac-
to treatment that targeted areas of deficiency tion), self-help skills (e.g., hand washing, dress-
related to effective functioning in the inclusive ing, safety skills), and occupational therapy (e.g.,
setting. gross and fine motor skills, utensil and cup use).
Several studies have assessed the effective- These skills are common teaching targets in an
ness of community-based programs (Cohen EIBI program, mimicking typical child develop-
et al., 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2002, 2007). Children ment, and recommended by several sources (Leaf
participating in these studies received one-to-one & McEachin, 1999; Maurice, Green, & Luce,
instruction by a trained behavior therapist; when 1996). These skills also largely mirror those on
the child was not receiving one-to-one instruc- Dickson, MacDonald, Mansfield, Guilhardi, and
tion, they were mainstreamed in a classroom with Ahearn’s (2014) New England Center for
typically developing peers with the therapist as a Children Core Skills Assessment, a sequence of
shadow. Sheinkopf and Siegel (1998) initially basic skills needed for independence. The skills
placed all participating children within a special on the Core Skills Assessment were socially
education classroom at the outset of treatment ­validated as relevant skills for CWA to learn by a
and subsequently moved participants to inclusion group of parents of children with autism and
or mainstream settings based on assessment of educators.
relevant skills. Across all studies, integration into A review of the studies using EIBI with young
an inclusion or mainstream setting with same-age children revealed that in seven of the studies ther-
typical peers was included as a part of the inten- apists used the seminal book Teaching Develop-­
sive treatment sequence (Remington et al., 2007). mentally Disabled Children: The Me Book
202 R. MacDonald et al.

written by Ivar Lovaas in 1981. This book was program. Using the principles of behavior analy-
written to document the behavioral techniques sis reviewed earlier, skills are targeted and taught
and curriculum being used in the UCLA Young in a progression that allows for the development
Autism Project. The content of this manual of more complex behavioral repertoires.
includes how to teach readiness skills, imitation Combined, these manuals provide a comprehen-
and early language, self-help skills, and advanced sive scope and sequence of skills to teach, guid-
language skills. The targeted first skills empha- ance on how to teach these skills, as well as
size the importance of good session behavior and strategies for generalization and maintenance. In
compliance. Reduction of stereotypy and other addition, Maurice et al. (2001) described other
disruptive behavior is seen as imperative to pro- common challenges professionals face when
viding effective teaching. The manual prescribes working with individuals with ASD, including
using overcorrection and punishment to expedite feeding difficulties and interactions with peers.
this process. When Lovaas published this paper Joint attention and play are two other core
in 1987, the use of punishment was very contro- deficits that are critical curriculum areas for
versial. The majority of studies reviewed since autism treatment (Adamson, Bakeman, Deckner,
that time have not used punishment in their treat- & Romski, 2009; Carpenter, Pennington, &
ment. Another procedure described throughout Rogers, 2002; Kasari, 2002). Joint attention is the
the manual is the use of a contingent loud “no” initiation of a gaze shift or gesture on the part of
when the child made an error. Again, this proce- the child to share an experience or object with an
dure is not widely used in EIBI programming, adult (Mundy, Sigman, & Kasari, 1994). Joint
neither experimental nor clinical. attention can also refer to the responding of a
Once the child has mastered the readiness child to a bid (either a gaze shift or a gesture) for
skills of session behavior and compliance, joint attention by the adult. Behavioral interven-
instruction can begin. Since the 1981 publication, tions have been effective in establishing joint
there have been numerous iterations of this origi- attention (Klein et al. 2009; MacDonald 2011;
nal manual (Leaf & McEachin, 1999; Lovaas, Taylor & Hoch, 2008; Whalen & Schreibman,
2003). All provide comprehensive and clear 2003); however, the curriculum sequence for
descriptions of teaching procedures for establish- teaching this skill has not been widely published.
ing early imitative and social behavior in young In the MacDonald et al. (2014) study, joint atten-
children with autism. Green et al. (2002) also tion was taught using a curriculum sequence
provide a nice description of the curriculum developed by the first author, described in two
sequence they used with a 14-month-old child. published sources. The first is a book chapter
The first year of instruction involved the develop- chronicling a child’s progression from eye con-
ment of imitative and communication repertoires, tact to social referencing during conversations
gradually increasing in complexity over the year. with others (MacDonald, 2011), and the second
Establishing these foundational skills allowed for is a brief report outlining the specific skills within
the rapid acquisition of more complex skills dur- the curriculum sequence (MacDonald, 2013a, b).
ing the second and third years of treatment. The curriculum is an integral part of the New
The other most commonly used EIBI curricu- England Center for Children’s online curriculum
lum resources include Behavioral Intervention called the Autism Curriculum Encyclopedia
for Young Children with Autism: A Manual for (ACE©).
Parents and Professionals by Maurice et al. Appropriate play is another important variable
(1996) and Making a Difference: Behavioral in the social development of CWA (Wolery &
Intervention for Autism by Maurice, Green, and Garfinkle, 2002). CWA often do not develop play
Foxx (2001). The first manual offers a compre- skills beyond the repetitive manipulation of
hensive curriculum by Taylor and McDonough objects. This deficit in functional toy manipula-
(1996) in which they outline beginning, interme- tion also prohibits them from engaging in more
diate, and advanced skills to target in an EIBI complex pretend play, alone or with other chil-
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 203

dren (Lifter, 2000; Rutherford, Young, Hepburn, Zachor et al., 2007) per week of intervention,
& Rogers, 2007). The curriculum sequence used lasting from less than 14 months (Howard et al.,
by MacDonald et al. (2014) involves four levels 2005; MacDonald et al., 2014; Zachor et al.,
of play, beginning with toy construction and pro- 2007) up to 2 or more years (e.g., Cohen et al.,
gressing to reciprocal pretend play with a peer 2006; Smith et al., 2000). Despite these differ-
(MacDonald, 2013a, b). Toy construction ences in EIBI implementation, all studies found
includes simple structured play activities, such as positive effects on multiple measures of change
completing a puzzle or assembling a toy like Mr. (including IQ, standardized measures of func-
Potato Head, which can be taught using physical tioning, and observational changes in behavior)
prompting in a task analysis format or discrete following EIBI treatment, compared to either
trial training (Leaf & McEachin, 1999). The cur- pre-intervention measures and/or a treatment-as-­
riculum sequence requires increasingly more usual control group (Eldevik et al., 2009).
complex solitary play behavior, such as pretend Occasional differences in intensity and duration
play, like having a tea party or making pizza, to existed even within studies; Howard et al. (2005)
cooperative pretend play such as cooking on a provided fewer hours per week to children under
grill with a friend. As with joint attention, a vari- 3 years of age. Other researchers recommended
ety of behavioral teaching have been effective to starting at the highest intensity of intervention
teach play, including modeling, both in vivo and tapering number of session hours per week as
(Gena, Couloura, & Kymissis, 2005; Goldstein & children became more ready for inclusion oppor-
Cisar, 1992) and video modeling (MacDonald, tunities (e.g., Smith et al., 2000).
Clark, Garrigan, & Vangala, 2005), pivotal The procedural question of just how much
response training (Thorp, Stahmer, & EIBI is “enough” remains to be answered. How
Schreibman, 1995), and reciprocal imitation an individual child will respond to treatment, and
(Ingersoll & Schreibman, 2006). Changes in play whether that child may show greater improve-
behavior have rarely been analyzed in relation to ment with a more intensive dose or duration of
EIBI outcome, although the social validity of intervention, is a difficult prediction to make at
increasing play behavior as a measurable out- the start of treatment. Eldevik, Eikeseth, Jahr, and
come of treatment cannot be overstated (Wolery Smith (2006) retroactively compared yoked pairs
& Garfinkle, 2002). of children receiving low-intensity EIBI (12 h per
week) or similar levels of eclectic treatment.
While the EIBI group made greater gains than the
Intensity and Duration eclectic treatment group, the gains were not at the
same level seen in studies with a more robust
Although intensive is considered a critical com- behavioral intervention, a finding the authors
ponent of EIBI, researchers have used varying potentially attributed to both the lower intensity
definitions of intensive when implementing EIBI, of the intervention as well as the lower pre-­
both in hours of direct intervention provided per intervention IQ scores of the participants (Eldevik
week as well as the length of the intervention et al., 2006). Howard et al. (2005) took a slightly
over time. Only one study has replicated the different tack and compared high-intensity EIBI
Lovaas’s (1987) intensity of treatment delivery, (approximately 30–35 h per week) with high-­
which was 40 h per week over a minimum of intensity eclectic treatment (approximately
2 years and up to 6 years; Sallows and Graupner 20–25 h per week), with EIBI emerging as the
(2005) provided a mean of 39 h per week of ser- clear winner. This indicates that it is not the
vice delivery for 4 years to individuals in their intensity or merely number of hours the interven-
study with positive results. Other EIBI interven- tion is in place that makes EIBI effective but
tions have provided between 25 (e.g., Green rather the content of the intervention. When con-
et al., 2002; Remington et al., 2007; Sheinkopf & sidering the EIBI literature as a whole, hard-and-­
Siegel, 1998) and 35 h (e.g., Cohen et al., 2006; fast recommendations on intensity and duration
204 R. MacDonald et al.

of services remain elusive. However, by taking training specific to the job at hand seemed to be
Eldevik et al. (2006) into account, we can safely top priority over previous experience or prior
recommend that EIBI occurs at an intensity level of education. Supervisors were generally
greater than 12 h per week. Based on the existing masters-level behavior analysts with experience
literature, 25 h per week of EIBI is the common in EIBI. Many methods of quality control by the
minimum amount prescribed. Similarly, little to supervisors were listed, including working with
no research has been done on implementation of students themselves, observing paraprofessionals
EIBI for short duration (less than 12 months). working with students and providing feedback,
Positive effects were seen when CWA received meeting with the paraprofessionals as a group
EIBI for a mean of 13 months (e.g., MacDonald weekly, and meeting with paraprofessionals indi-
et al., 2014), and Lovaas (1987) (among others) vidually weekly to discuss student progress.
noted that the greatest gains are generally seen in Large-scale clinical oversight was generally pro-
the first 1–2 years of treatment. vided by a clinical psychologist or PhD-level
behavior analyst and often consisted of monthly
consultation to individual cases and group meet-
Quality of Service Delivery ings. Although correct implementation of EIBI
and careful oversight of student progress by qual-
Proper implementation of EIBI and effective ified professionals are of utmost importance to
monitoring of progress and procedural integrity the researchers, as evinced by the attention they
are critical to an EIBI program’s success. Each of give to describing these details, additional infor-
the EIBI studies reviewed in Table 12.1 discussed mation regarding how paraprofessionals were
protocols in place for training and supervising trained and caseload size for supervisors is war-
staff and reviewing student’s programs over time. ranted to paint a complete picture of how quality
While varying levels of detail were offered and assurance was managed in these EIBI programs.
terminology differed in the descriptions, a com-
mon structure for training and supervision of the
EIBI program was this: paraprofessionals worked Conclusion
directly with students; those paraprofessionals
were trained and supervised by masters-level These critical components of EIBI outline the
behavior analysts (BCBA); and the program as a parameters under which positive, significant, and
whole was overseen or consulted to by a clinical enduring change have been produced for young
psychologist or PhD-level behavior analyst children with autism. Behavioral research has
(BCBA-D). pointed to tried-and-true techniques including
Many paraprofessionals were in school to be prompting, reinforcement, errorless learning,
special educators and had bachelor’s degrees systematic breakdown of skills, repeated prac-
(e.g., MacDonald et al., 2014), although some tice, moving through an established developmen-
had high school diplomas and worked in public tal sequence across domains, and programming
schools (e.g., Smith et al., 1997), and others were for generalization in establishing fundamental
college students working toward a bachelor’s communication, discrimination, and social skills
degree (Lovaas, 1987). All had additional train- for children with ASD. Much has been learned
ing specific to EIBI and had been trained by over the past three decades about how best to
masters-­level therapists (e.g., Remington et al., teach CWA. Future areas of research do, how-
2007; Smith et al., 2000). Some were trained ever, remain.
using Lovaas’s (1981) manual (Sheinkopf &
Siegel, 1998), and some were trained by visiting Measures of Change  While positive outcome
the UCLA program site and completing an following EIBI has been demonstrated across
internship there (Smith et al., 1997). Generally, numerous studies, the method of measuring
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 205

change has, at times, differed. Much of the litera- year or more, and starting at as young an age as
ture on EIBI outcome has used standardized test- possible (ideally, before 2 years of age)
ing to determine if significant changes have (MacDonald et al., 2014). However, questions
occurred during treatment, including IQ and continue to be raised regarding precisely what the
measures of overall functioning (e.g., the ideal parameters of EIBI implementation should
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale). Eldevik be: Is 25 h per week enough? If we can do 40 h
et al. (2009) used effect size to equalize changes per week, should we? For the very young, what
across tests when analyzing EIBI comparison should be the balance between EIBI, gross motor
outcome data, generally finding effect sizes exploration, and receiving the recommended
between 0.5 and 1 in favor of EIBI treatment for sleep allowances, including naps? Will imple-
varying testing types (i.e., IQ and adaptive behav- menting EIBI for only a few months reap the
ior scores). Although global scores of function- same rewards, or produce as lasting a change, as
ing have a clear-cut place in assessing change for EIBI implemented for 1–2 years? Research into
CWA, additional measures may be helpful creat- these questions could enrich our understanding
ing a richer picture of the type of change experi- of how EIBI can be implemented most effec-
enced by CWA receiving EIBI and whether those tively and efficiently.
changes improve their social functioning.
Because CWA often continue to function behind Training  Proper implementation of EIBI
their typically developing peers even when requires nuance and skill. Therapists working
improvements are seen, these tests may not cap- with young children are often simultaneously
ture smaller-scale changes in behavior or changes working to shape session behavior and attending,
in behavior that are specific to an autism profile identify and continually reassess reinforcers,
(such as social impairment) over a more general shape compliance to manual guidance and
developmentally delayed profile (such as cogni- prompting, establish eye contact and social inter-
tive impairment). Drawing from the findings of action as reinforcing, and modify teaching to an
the Baby Sibling Research Consortium, mea- appropriate developmental level. Providing inci-
sures of those early markers might be more sensi- dental teaching opportunities and setting up the
tive assessment of treatment effects (Ozonoff environment for naturalistic teaching are addi-
et al., 2015). Additional types of behavioral tional necessary skills. While the EIBI manuals
change, such as changes in interpersonal social used in the field address these topics, additional
skills, play behavior, or a reduction in socially information on how therapists were trained, as
stigmatizing behavior, may be harder to catego- well as more information on how the supervisory
rize with a test predominantly measuring changes systems operate to provide observation, feed-
in IQ. For this, pairing a repeated measures anal- back, and consult, would be a welcome addition
ysis of operationally defined and observationally to the literature in order to better standardize
measured autism-specific deficits, such as eye these practices. Additionally, parent training and
contact, joint attention, imitation, and play, may involvement varied across studies. Examining the
be a welcome component in determining what effectiveness of parent training in EIBI and the
type of behavior corresponds to a change in IQ or social validity of this training would be an excel-
adaptive functioning. Additional research in lent area of continued research.
these areas is needed. In EIBI, we have an effective tool in mitigat-
ing some of the detrimental effects of an ASD
Ideal Parameters of Treatment  Regarding diagnosis for very young children. The ­techniques
EIBI intensity, duration, and ideal starting age for used in EIBI and the effects produced are well
treatment, the literature supports a “more is bet- documented through comprehensive training
ter” approach, converging on recommendations manuals (e.g., Maurice et al., 1996) and seminal
for more EIBI (greater than 25 h per week), for a work (e.g., Lovaas, 1987), respectively. Two vari-
206 R. MacDonald et al.

ables may impede implementation: funding and sive behavioral treatment between ages 4 and 7 a com-
parison controlled study. Behavior Modification, 31(3),
staffing. Increased use of distance technology 264–278.
and training of Registered Behavior Technicians Eldevik, S., Eikeseth, S., Jahr, E., & Smith, T. (2006).
(RBT) by the Behavior Analysis Certification Effects of low-intensity behavioral treatment for chil-
Board (BACB) are two strategies that are having dren with autism and mental retardation. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 211–224.
a positive impact on service availability. The task Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
is now to diagnose early and place children into Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analysis of
EIBI treatment programs that can make a lifelong early intensive behavioral intervention for children
difference in their developmental trajectory. with autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent
Psychology, 38(3), 439–450.
Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-­
Palmer Quarterly (1982–), 28, 445–470.
References Gena, A., Couloura, S., & Kymissis, E. (2005). Modifying
the affective behavior of preschoolers with autism
Adamson, L. B., McArthur, D., Markov, Y., Dunbar, using in-vivo or video modeling and reinforcement
B., & Bakeman, R. (2001). Autism and joint atten- contingencies. Journal of Autism and Developmental
tion: Young children’s responses to maternal bids. Disorders, 35(5), 545–556.
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 22(4), Goldstein, H., & Cisar, C. L. (1992). Promoting interac-
439–453. tion during sociodramatic play: Teaching scripts to
Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., Deckner, D. F., & Romski, typical preschoolers and classmates with disabilities.
M. (2009). Joint engagement and the emergence of Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25(2), 265–280.
language in children with autism and Down syndrome. Green, G. (2011). Early intensive behavior analytic
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, intervention for autism spectrum disorders. In E. A.
39(1), 84–96. Mayville & J. A. Mulick (Eds.), Behavioral founda-
Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The tions of effective autism treatment. New York, NY:
origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant inter- Sloane Publishing.
action. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The Green, G., Brennan, L. C., & Fein, D. (2002). Intensive
effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York, NY: behavioral treatment for a toddler at high risk for
Wiley. autism. Behavior Modification, 26(1), 69–102.
Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., McDermott, C., Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G.,
Rombough, V., & Brian, J. (2008). The autism obser- & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive
vation scale for infants: Scale development and reli- behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young
ability data. Journal of Autism and Developmental children with autism. Research in Developmental
Disorders, 38(4), 731–738. Disabilities, 26(4), 359–383.
Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. E., & Rogers, S. J. (2002). Howard, J. S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman, C. R.,
Interrelations among social-cognitive skills in & Cohen, H. G. (2014). Comparison of behavior
young children with autism. Journal of Autism and analytic and eclectic early interventions for young
Developmental Disorders, 32(2), 91–106. children with autism after three years. Research in
Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T. (2006). Developmental Disabilities, 35(12), 3326–3344.
Early intensive behavioral treatment: Replication of Ingersoll, B., & Schreibman, L. (2006). Teaching recip-
the UCLA model in a community setting. Journal rocal imitation skills to young children with autism
of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, using a naturalistic behavioral approach: Effects on
S145–S155. language, pretend play, and joint attention. Journal of
Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(4), 487–505.
plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum Johnson, S. C., Meyer, L., & Taylor, B. A. (1996).
disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20(3), Supported inclusion. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & S. C.
775–803. Luce (Eds.), Behavioral intervention for young chil-
Dickson, C. A., MacDonald, R. P., Mansfield, R., dren with autism: A manual for parents and profes-
Guilhardi, P., Johnson, C., & Ahearn, W. H. (2014). sionals. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Social validation of the New England Center for Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present
children-­core skills assessment. Journal of Autism and but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed
Developmental Disorders, 44(1), 65–74. with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431.
Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2002). Kasari, C. (2002). Assessing change in early intervention
Intensive behavioral treatmentat school for 4- to programs for children with autism. Journal of Autism
7-year-old children with autism. Behavior Modification, and Developmental Disorders, 32(5), 447–461.
26(1), 49–68. Klein, J. L., MacDonald, R. P., Vaillancourt, G., Ahearn,
Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2007). W. H., & Dube, W. V. (2009). Teaching discrimina-
Outcome for children with autism who began inten-
12  Early Intensive Behavioral Interventions 207

tion of adult gaze direction to children with autism. MacDonald, R., Parry-Cruwys, D., Dupere, S., & Ahearn,
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(1), 42–49. W. (2014). Assessing progress and outcome of early
Landa, R., & Garrett-Mayer, E. (2006). Development in intensive behavioral intervention for toddlers with
infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(12), 3632–3644.
47(6), 629–638. Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). A meta-analytic
Landa, R. J., Gross, A. L., Stuart, E. A., & Faherty, A. review of the effectiveness of behavioural early inter-
(2013). Developmental trajectories in children with vention programs for children with autistic spectrum
and without autism spectrum disorders: The first 3 disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders,
years. Child Development, 84(2), 429–442. 4(4), 577–593.
Leaf, R., & McEachin, J. (1999). A work in progress: Maurice, C., Green, G., & Luce, S. C. (1996). In
Behavior management strategies and a curriculum for C. Maurice, G. Green, & S. C. Luce (Eds.), Behavioral
intensive behavioral treatment of autism. New York, intervention for young children with autism: A manual
NY: DRL. for parents and professionals. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Lifter, K. (2000). Linking assessment to intervention for chil- Maurice, C., Green, G., & Foxx, R. M. (Eds.). (2001).
dren with developmental disabilities or at-­risk for devel- Making a difference: Behavioral intervention for
opmental delay: The developmental play assessment autism. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
(DPA) instrument. In K. Gitlin-­Weiner, A. Sandgrund, McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-­
C. Schaefer, K. Gitlin-Weiner, A. Sandgrund, & term outcome for children with autism who received
C. Schaefer (Eds.), Play diagnosis and assessment (2nd early intensive behavioral treatment. American
ed.pp. 228–261). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Journal on Mental Retardation, 97(4), 359–372.
Loupe, P. S., Schroeder, S. R., & Tessel, R. (1995). FR McGee, G. G., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E.
discrimination training effects in SHR and micro- (1985). The facilitative effects of incidental teaching
encephalic rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and on preposition use by autistic children. Journal of
Behavior, 51, 869–876. Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 17–31.
Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled Moore, C., & Corkum, V. (1994). Social understanding at
children: The ME book. Baltimore, MD: University the end of the first year of life. Developmental Review,
Park Press. 14(4), 349–372.
Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal Morales, M., Mundy, P., & Rojas, J. (1998). Following
educational and intellectual functioning in young the direction of gaze and language development in
autistic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 6-month-olds. Infant Behavior & Development, 21(2),
Psychology, 55(1), 3–9. 373–377.
Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop- Mundy, P., Sigman, S., & Kasari, C. (1994). Joint atten-
mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Austin, tion, developmental level, and symptom presentation
TX: PRO-ED. in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6,
Lovaas, O. I., Koegel, R., Simmons, J. Q., & Long, J. S. 389–401.
(1973). Some generalization and follow-up measures Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of
on autistic children in behavior therapy. Journal of children with autism: A study of first birthday home
Applied Behavior Analysis, 6(1), 131–165. videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental
MacDonald, R. P. F. (2011). Hey, look, it’s a train! In Disorders, 24(3), 247–257.
E. Cipani (Ed.), Children and autism: Stories of tri- Ozonoff, S., Iosif, A. M., Baguio, F., Cook, I. C., Hill,
umph and hope. New York, NY: Springer Publishing M. M., Hutman, T., & Steinfeld, M. B. (2010). A pro-
Company. spective study of the emergence of early behavioral
MacDonald, R. P. F. (2013a). Establishing repertoires of signs of autism. Journal of the American Academy of
pretend play in children with autism using video mod- Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(3), 256–266.
eling. In M. Weiss & A. Bondy (Eds.), Social behavior Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Carter, A., Messinger, D.,
in children with autism spectrum disorder. Bethesda, Yirmiya, N., Zwaigenbaum, L., & Hutman, T. (2011).
MD: Woodbine Press. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: A baby
MacDonald, R. P. F. (2013b). Teaching joint attention. siblings research consortium study. Pediatrics, 128(3),
Autism News of Orange County and the Rest of the e488–e495.
World, 7, 10–13. Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Landa, R. J., Brian, J.,
MacDonald, R., Clark, M., Garrigan, E., & Vangala, M. Bryson, S., Charman, T., & Zwaigenbaum, L. (2015).
(2005). Using video modeling to teach pretend play to Diagnostic stability in young children at risk for
children with autism. Behavioral Interventions, 20(4), autism spectrum disorder: A baby siblings research
225–238. consortium study. Journal of Child Psychology and
MacDonald, R., Anderson, J., Dube, W. V., Geckeler, Psychiatry, 56(9), 988–998.
A., Green, G., Holcomb, W., & Sanchez, J. (2006). Perry, A., Cummings, A., Geier, J. D., Freeman, N. L.,
Behavioral assessment of joint attention: A meth- Hughes, S., Managhan, T., & Williams, J. (2011).
odological report. Research in Developmental Predictors of outcome for children receiving intensive
Disabilities, 27(2), 138–150. behavioral intervention in a large, community-based
208 R. MacDonald et al.

program. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, tion. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41(3),
5(1), 592–603. 377–391.
Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., Degli Taylor, B. A., & McDonough, K. A. (1996). Selecting
Espinosa, F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., & Ward, N. (2007). teaching programs. In C. Maurice, G. Green, & S. C.
Early intensive behavioral intervention: Outcomes for Luce (Eds.), Behavioral intervention for young chil-
children with autism and their parents after two years. dren with autism: A manual for parents and profes-
American Journal on Mental Retardation, 112(6), sionals. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
418–438. Thorp, D. M., Stahmer, A. C., & Schreibman, L. (1995).
Rochat, P., & Striano, T. (1999). Emerging self-­exploration Effects of sociodramatic play training on children
by 2-month-old infants. Developmental Science, 2(2), with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
206–218. Disorders, 25(3), 265–282.
Rutherford, M. D., Young, G. S., Hepburn, S., & Rogers, Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation
S. J. (2007). A longitudinal study of pretend play in early infancy. A description of primary intersub-
in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental jectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The
Disorders, 37(6), 1024–1039. beginnings of human communication. London, UK:
Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive Cambridge Univ. Press.
behavioral treatment for children with autism: Four-­ Whalen, C., & Schreibman, L. (2003). Joint attention
year outcome and predictors. American Journal on training for CWA using behavior modification proce-
Mental Retardation, 110(6), 417–438. dures. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
Sheinkopf, S. J., & Siegel, B. (1998). Home-based behav- 44(3), 456–468.
ioral treatment of young children with autism. Journal Wolery, M., & Garfinkle, A. N. (2002). Measures in
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(1), 15–23. intervention research with young children who
Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. have autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
(1997). Intensive behavioral treatment for preschool- Disorders, 32(5), 463–478.
ers with severe retardation and pervasive developmen- Young, G. S., Merin, N., Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff,
tal disorder. American Journal on Mental Retardation, S. (2009). Gaze behavior and affect at 6 months:
102(3), 238–249. Predicting clinical outcomes and language develop-
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized ment in typically developing infants and infants at risk
trial of intensive early intervention for children with for autism. Developmental Science, 12(5), 798–814.
pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal Zachor, D. A., Ben-Itzchak, E., Rabinovich, A. L., &
on Mental Retardation, 105(4), 269–285. Lahat, E. (2007). Change in autism core symptoms
Symons, L. A., Hains, S. M., & Muir, D. W. (1998). Look with intervention. Research in Autism Spectrum
at me: Five-month-old infants’ sensitivity to very Disorders, 1(4), 304–317.
small deviations in eye-gaze during social interactions. Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian,
Infant Behavior & Development, 21(3), 531–536. J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of
Taylor, B. A., & Hoch, H. (2008). Teaching children with autism in the first year of life. International Journal of
autism to respond to and initiate bids for joint atten- Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2), 143–152.
Additional Treatment Parameters
and Issues Requiring Study: Early 13
Intensive Behavioral Intervention
(EIBI)

Svein Eikeseth

ciples, and finally, the relationship between


Introduction behavioral changes and collateral changes in
neurobiological functioning.
Research has shown that early and intensive behav- A comprehensive and complete review of
ioral intervention (EIBI) may produce clinically these topics, however, is beyond the scope of this
meaningful gains in language, social, and cognitive chapter. Instead, the review is somewhat selec-
functioning and reduce aberrant behaviors associ- tive, and parameters not mentioned here may also
ated with ASD (Eldevik et al., 2009; Reichow, be of importance (c.f., Matson & Smith, 2008).
Barton, Assouline, & Hume, 2012). Yet, there is a Nevertheless, I hope that this chapter will give
considerable variation in outcome made by indi- some ideas for further research on topics related
vidual children (Dawson, Bernier, & Ring, 2012; to EIBI. The first part of this chapter will address
Eldevik et al., 2010; Howlin, Magiati, & Charman, research on predictors of individual differences
2009; Warren et al., 2011). This has prompted an in EIBI outcome, starting with pretreatment
interest in examining additional parameters that variables.
may define and improve the treatment.
The purpose of this chapter is to identify and
discuss such variables. More specifically, vari-  redictors of Individual Differences
P
ables addressed are predictors of individual dif- in EIBI Outcome
ferences in EIBI outcome, ways to optimize the
delivery of the intervention, ways to improving The variation in outcome made by individual
effectiveness of the treatment, whether improve- children has prompted an interest in discovering
ments are durable over time, how EIBI compares predictors of treatment outcome, to help identi-
with alternative behavioral approaches, whether fying which children will benefit most from the
the model has been successfully adapted to dif- intervention. Knowledge of such variables may
ferent settings and populations, whether autism also be used to select the most effective treat-
can be understood based on reinforcement prin- ment for individual children and to direct
research on targeted interventions for subgroups
of children who benefit less from existing treat-
S. Eikeseth (*) ments. I will start by examining the extent to
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Oslo and
which certain child characteristics that can be
Akershus University College of Applied Sciences,
P. O. Box 4, St. Olavs Plass, N-0130 Oslo, Norway assessed before treatment starts may be useful in
e-mail: svein.eikeseth@hioa.no predicting outcome.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 209


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_13
210 S. Eikeseth

Pretreatment Variables as a predictor of outcome. If so, using certain test


items, index scores or cognitive domains as puta-
Intellectual Functioning  Parents and profes- tive predictors may prove to be a better and more
sionals often ask whether individual characteris- reliable predictor. This could be examined in the
tics of a particular child can predict how well the future research.
child may benefit from EIBI. For example, do
children with more advanced cognitive skills Chronological Age at Intake  In EIBI, a goal is
show more progress in treatment as compared to to start treatment as early as possible in the child’s
those who start treatment with lower cognitive life. Optimally, intervention should start in
scores? Indeed, intellectual functioning at intake infancy and ideally before the full-blown ASD
is the variable that has been most investigated as symptoms have emerged. This is because the
a potential predictor for outcome, and several potential for neural plasticity such as neural
studies have shown that high IQ at intake is asso- regeneration, repair, and/or reorganization is pre-
ciated with high IQ at the end of the treatment sumed to be greater than the younger the children
(Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr & Eldevik, 2002; Harris & are. Also, younger children have had less time to
Handleman, 2000; Hayward, Eikeseth, Gale, & acquire behavioral delays and hence may have
Morgan, 2009). That is, children who start with a not fallen so far behind typical development as
higher IQ tend to end with a higher IQ after the compared to children that are older. Also, younger
intervention is finished. Progress, however, can children may have had less time to develop and
also be measured as change in scores between sustain behavioral excesses such as aggressive,
intake and follow-up. For example, a child may stereotyped, and ritualistic behaviors. For these
obtain an IQ score of 100 at follow-up, that is, a and other reasons, children’s chronological age at
score within the normal range. However, this par- the start of treatment has been investigated as a
ticular child may have had a score of 90 at intake potential predictor of treatment outcome. Harris
and hence has made an IQ gain of 10 points. and Handleman (2000) found that young age at
Another child may have a follow-up score of 70 the start of treatment predicted better outcome,
and an intake score of 40. Even though this child when outcome was measured by school place-
does not score within the normal range, the child ment. Using a number of mastered behavioral
has had an improvement of 30 IQ points, that is, objectives as an outcome measure, Granpeesheh,
three times as much as how the child scored Dixon, Tarbox, Kaplan, and Wilke (2009) found
within the normal range. that children between 2 and 5 years of age had
When change in IQ has been used as an out- better skill acquisition as compared to older chil-
come variable, intake IQ is no longer a clear pre- dren. Also, age at intake has been found to be
dictor of outcome: Some studies have found that associated with better cognitive outcomes of a
intake IQ predicts IQ change between intake and community-based intensive behavioral interven-
follow-up (Ben-Itzchak, Watson, & Zachor, tion for kindergarten- and preschool-aged chil-
2014; Ben-Itzchak & Zachor, 2007; Smith, dren, but less so in older (school aged) children
Klorman, & Mruzek, 2015), but other studies (Blacklock, Perry, & Geier, 2014; Perry et al.,
have failed to find a relation between intake IQ 2011). Recently, Smith, Klorman, and Mruzek
and IQ change (Cohen, Amerine-Dickens, & (2015) found that age at intake predicted out-
Smith 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2002; Eldevik, come on several measures including cognitive
Hastings, Jahr, & Hughes, 2012; Eldevik et al., functioning, adaptive behavior, and autism
2009; Hayward et al., 2009; Sallows & Graupner, severity.
2005; Smith, Groen, & Wynn, 2000). As with IQ, however, the predictive value of
Clearly, more research is needed to settle this age at intake is lower when outcome is measured
issue. Perhaps a full-scale IQ, which has been as changes in scores between intake and follow-
used as putative predictor in the studies reviewed, ­up. Indeed, when outcome is measured as change
is too gross of a measure and, hence, not optimal in cognitive or adaptive functioning, several stud-
13  Additional Treatment Variables 211

ies have failed to find a relation between intake tangible items, as well as their preference for
age and outcome (Eikeseth, Klintwall, Jahr, & engaging in stereotyped behavior. Results showed
Karlsson, 2012; Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, that children who preferred a higher number of
2002; Hayward et al., 2009; Magiati, Charman, different edible and tangible items had better
& Howlin, 2007). This includes data from three treatment outcome as compared to the children
meta-studies (Eldevik et al., 2010; Makrygianni who showed a preference for fewer items and that
& Reed, 2010; Reichow, 2012). children who showed a higher number stereo-
Hence, age at intake is an uncertain predic- typed behaviors had poorer treatment outcome as
tor of outcome, and more research is needed to compared to those who showed fewer stereo-
determine the extent to which age at intake is typed behaviors.
an important factor associated with improved Several other studies have shown that object
outcome. Perhaps age of intake plays a greater interest and width of interest are important pre-
role for interventions for infants and younger dictors of response to another type of behavioral
children, as compared to children approaching treatment called Pivotal Response Treatment
school age. This could be a topic for future (Schreibman, Stahmer, Barlett, & Dufek, 2009;
research. Sherer & Schreibman, 2005; Vivanti,
Dissanayake, Zierhut, & Rogers, 2012; Yoder &
Stone, 2006).
 ocial Engagement, Toy Interest,
S Hence, although several pretreatment vari-
Preference Assessment, ables have been found to predict outcome, the
and Stereotyped Behavior strongest and most reliable pretreatment predic-
tor to date seems to be children’s level of interest
Since abnormalities in social interest and/or in objects and activities. Interestingly, there is
social skills are a core indicator of ASD, research- less evidence for the predictive value of social
ers have examined whether level of social interest and social skills.
engagement at intake may predict outcome. In a
recent study, social approach, joint attention, and
imitation were assessed through parent reports  ariables During Treatment: Early
V
and behavioral observations. Results showed that Response to Treatment
social engagement at intake predicted higher
cognitive scores and higher scores in adaptive Another angle in the search for reliable predic-
functioning at outcome. Social engagement did tors is to examine the extent to which factors
not predict a reduction in autism symptoms at identified early in treatment can be associated
follow-up (Smith et al., 2015). with outcome. Indeed, research suggest that ini-
Children’s interest in toys and routines before tial response to treatment is such a factor (Lovaas
treatment starts has been assessed in a couple of & Smith, 1988). Weiss (1999) found that children
studies. Klintwall, Macari, Eikeseth, and who had the quickest acquisition of vocal imita-
Chawarska (2015) assessed children’s interest in tion skills and the quickest acquisition of lan-
toys, activities, and social routines during the guage comprehension skills early in treatment
administration of the Autism Diagnostic showed the best outcome after 2 years. Similar
Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000) results were reported by Sallows and Graupner
and found that this was a strong predictor of (2005) who used an “early learning measure” to
change in cognitive skills and adaptive behaviors. predict which children would achieve test scores
In that study, social interest as measured by the in the normal range by the end of treatment.
ADOS did not predict outcome, in contrast to Children who acquired verbal imitation within
what was found in the Smith et al. (2015) study. the first 16 months of treatment, or before the age
In a similar study, Klintwall and Eikeseth (2012) of 42 months, all ended treatment with test scores
assessed children’s preferences for edible and in the normal range.
212 S. Eikeseth

 ecommendation for Future
R Intensity of Treatment
Research
Since EIBI is designed to address all of the
Some general recommendations for future individual child’s deficit and excess behaviors
research may be considered. Firstly, whenever rather than correcting the underlying cause(s) of
possible, the selection of putative predictors the condition (which if corrected could result in a
should be theory driven. For example, the study subsequent normalization of the children’s
of social engagement as a putative predictor behavior), it is reasonable to assume that the
(e.g., Smith et al., 2015) is clearly theory treatment must be intensive and long term to have
driven, as it is consistent with, for example, the optimal effects.
social motivation hypothesis of ASD (Dawson Lovaas (1987) provided 40 h of one-to-one
2008). Secondly, predictors should be narrow intervention per week and reported that 47% of
and specific rather than broad. For example, the preschool-aged children achieved normal
since full-­scale IQ has been associated with cognitive and educational functioning at the time
outcome, future research should aim to identify when they finished first grade in regular schools.
which items, index scores, or cognitive domains Sallows and Graupner (2005) provided the same
are responsible and which are not. Finally, number of treatment hours, assessing children
putative predictors should be based on how the after 4 years of treatment. They found that chil-
treatment works. For example, the edible and dren made a gain of 25 IQ points, 7 points in
tangible items assessed in the Klintwall and adaptive functioning, and that 48% achieved nor-
Eikeseth (2012) study were stimuli that could mal cognitive functioning and succeeded in regu-
be used as contrived reinforcers during treat- lar classrooms at the chronological age of 7 years.
ment. That is, the higher the number of items Other studies have typically provided less inten-
that the children showed interest in, the larger sive treatment, and results have also been some-
the repertoire of putative reinforcers available what mixed. Unfortunately, in these studies,
for the therapists. Also, the stereotyped behav- treatment hours are typically confounded by
iors assessed in that study were repetitive other variables that can affect outcome such as
behaviors that could interfere with intervention treatment fidelity, intervention setting, parental
and hence hamper progress. involvement, and length of treatment.
Some studies have explored whether intensity
of treatment may predict outcome. For example,
Optimal Delivery Granpeesheh et al. (2009) found a correlation
of the Intervention between number of intervention hours and mas-
tered behavioral objectives during the first
Although EIBI is an evidence-based interven- 4 months of EIBI. Reed, Osborne, and Corness
tion (Eikeseth, 2009; Reicow & Wolery, 2009; (2007) compared high-intensity (30 h per week)
Rogers & Vismara, 2008; Smith & Iadarola, with low-intensity (12 h per week) EIBI and
2015), there are still a number of issues that found that the high-intensity group made signifi-
merit further research. For example, it is not yet cantly larger gains. In another meta-study by
clear what constitutes the optimal treatment Eldevik et al. (2010), intensity of treatment was
intensity and intervention length. Also, since found to be the only consistent predictor of treat-
there is a great variability in individual chil- ment outcome, as measured by improvement in
dren’s response to the treatment, we need to both IQ and adaptive functioning, a finding repli-
learn more about how to individualize the treat- cated in a another meta-analysis by Makrygianni
ment, as well as how to make the treatment and Reed (2010).
more effective for those who respond less favor- Although there is some evidence for the notion
able to existing intervention. This is the topic of that treatment intensity is an important treatment
the following section. variable, existing research has a number of
13  Additional Treatment Variables 213

limitations, and to date, there are no experimental first year of intervention, then lost 2 IQ points
studies published, which have been designed during the second year of intervention, and
explicitly to study low- versus high-intensive gained 3 IQ points during the third year of inter-
treatment. Hence, more research is needed to vention. Eikeseth et al. (2002, 2007) found that
address the effects of treatment intensity on out- children increased their IQ score with 17 points
come. This is important because the intervention during the first year of treatment and then made
is labor intensive, long lasting, and costly, and an additional gain of 8 points during the subse-
hence, children may receive low-intensive treat- quent 2 years of intervention. Although the
ment due to cost issues and staff availability. If development was somewhat different across
such low-intensive treatment is ineffective, the these two studies, the total IQ gain between
savings obtained from reducing the number of intake and follow-up after approximately 3 year
one-to-one intervention may indeed prove to be of intervention was almost identical, 28 points in
more costly for the society because the lifetime the Howard et al. study and 25 IQ points in the
cost of caring for individuals with ASD is high. Eikeseth et al. study. This is similar to other stud-
An improvement in adaptive and independent ies reporting outcome after 3 or more years
skills may reduce these costs significantly. (Sallows and Graupner (2005) and Cohen et al.
Moreover, the economical savings of having an both reported an IQ gain of 25). This data clearly
individual move from being a receiver of social shows that although children make most progress
benefits to become a taxpayer (which may hap- during the first year of treatment, they continue to
pen for some of the children receiving EIBI) are excel in deployment as long as treatment is car-
enormous. Another possibility is that the inter- ried out, but it is not yet known what will happen
vention in some cases may be too intensive and if treatment is continued for more than 3–4 years.
hence produce client “burnout” (Matson & Hence, researchers should follow children for
Smith, 2008). longer period and examine the extent to which
children continue to improve with treatment and
the extent to which improvement is sustained
Length of Intervention after treatment has ended.
Assessing adaptive skills, the data is similar to
The length at which the intervention is provided that of IQ, but gains in adaptive skills have been
is another variable that may affect outcome and more modest as compared to IQ (Cohen et al.,
hence be related to the question of optimal deliv- 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2007; Howard et al., 2014).
ery of the intervention. Logically, the longer the Howard et al. (2014) found that the children
time the child is exposed to a good learning envi- made a gain of 9 points during the first year of
ronment, the more skills should the child learn. intervention, then lost 1 point during the second
Several studies have assessed children’s cog- year of intervention, and lost another 4 points
nitive skills at intake and then again every year during the third year of intervention. Eikeseth
for several years of intervention. Interestingly, et al. (2002, 2007) found that children increased
these studies suggest that most treatment gains their adaptive score with 11 points during the first
are made during the first year of treatment (Cohen year of treatment and then made an additional
et al., 2006; Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, & Eldevik, gain of 1 point during the subsequent 2 years of
2007; Howard, Stanislaw, Green, Sparkman, & intervention. The total gain adaptive behavior
Cohen, 2014; Kovshoff, Hastings, & Remington, between intake and follow-up after ­approximately
2012; Sallows & Graupner, 2005). However, 3 years of intervention was 4 points in the Howard
these studies also show that children continue to et al. study, 12 points in the Eikeseth et al. study,
make gains after the first year of intervention and 9 points in the Cohen et al. study.
albeit not as much as during the first year. For It is unclear why children make lager gains in
example, Howard et al. (2014) found that the IQ and adaptive behavior during the first year of
children made a gain of 27 IQ points during the treatment, but several factors may be relevant.
214 S. Eikeseth

Firstly, perhaps children are learning some test-­ appropriateness of the child’s curriculum and
taking skills early in treatment, such as sitting which skills to be assessed (Gould, Dixon,
quietly during the assessment, attending to the Najdowski, Smith, & Tarbox, 2011).
test materials, listening and responding to the
assessor, etc., and that this increase in compli-
ance behavior improves overall performance. Supervision: Intensity and Quality
Secondly, perhaps improvement in test scores is
made because the children have acquired some The extent to which programs are supervised by
key skills other than compliance early in treat- a competent clinician is another important vari-
ment and that these key skills have stronger able that may be related to treatment quality and
effects on test scores as compared to the subse- optimal outcome. A competent supervisor is
quent skills they acquired. Such skills may be required to have knowledge of advanced learning
improved language pre-academic skills such as principles, which may be assessed through the
completing puzzles, matching, and other Behavior Analyst Certification Board
problem-­solving behaviors. One way of address- Examination. In addition, extensive clinical
ing this issue is to assess children’s cognitive and experience is required including experience of
adaptive skills several times (e.g., every 3 months) beginning, intermediate, and advanced programs
during the first say 18 months of intervention. It designed to increase language, play, and social,
is also unclear why children are making more emotional, academic, and daily living skills.
progress in IQ as compared to adaptive behavior, Moreover, experience with different types of
and this question could also be addressed in learners (e.g., auditory and visual learners, chil-
future studies. One possibility is that the curricu- dren exhibiting over-selective responding or
lum has a stronger focus on skills assessed by extreme problem behaviors) is fundamental, so is
cognitive test as compared to the skills involved supervised experience in designing and imple-
in the instrument assessing adaptive behaviors. menting individualized programs and knowledge
of functional assessment and reinforcement pro-
cedures to reduce inappropriate behavior. A pro-
Treatment Quality cedure for assessing supervisors’ competency
was validated by Davis, Smith, and Donahoe
Research has indicated that not all EIBI programs (2002; see also Eikeseth, 2010).
are equally effective (Bibby Eikeseth, Martin, In addition to quality of supervision is the
Mudford, & Reeves, 2001; Eldevik, Eikeseth, intensity in which qualified supervision is pro-
Jahr, & Smith, 2006; Magiati et al., 2007). One vided. Intensity of supervision may depend on
reason for this may be that less effective pro- circumstances such as costs, availability of spe-
grams do not meet the standards in terms of inter- cialists in early and intensive behavioral inter-
vention quality. Intervention quality involves the vention, or other logistical issues. Two studies
extent to which the behavioral principles and have reported supervision, on average, every
methods are applied appropriately. Moreover, 3 months (Bibby, Eikeseth, Martin Mudford, &
intervention quality involves the appropriateness Reeves, 2002; Magiati et al., 2007). Other studies
of the child’s curriculum, such as the extent to have reported more frequent supervision such as
which the targets are appropriate to the child’s up to 10 h per week (Eikeseth et al., 2002).
skill level and learning style. Assessment of To date, only two studies have addressed asso-
intervention quality may be conducted qualita- ciations between supervision intensity and out-
tively, for example, by being site visited by come. Recently, Dixon et al. (2016) found an
experts in the field that are independent of the association between supervision hours and
specific service provider. Such assessment can acquisition of learning objectives. However, the
also be done quantitatively. However, additional participants who received greater supervision
research is needed on how to best assess the hours typically also received a higher number of
13  Additional Treatment Variables 215

treatment hours, and when treatment hours and & Reed, 2010), but to date, no studies have exam-
supervision hours were analyzed together, the ined experimentally the role of parental involve-
number of treatment hours contributed more ment in EIBI. Hence, further research is needed to
strongly to the acquisition of learning objectives assess the effects of parental involvement on child
as compared to the number of supervision hours. outcome and to develop effective parent training
The study also identified a strong relation strategies (Strauss et al., 2012).
between supervisor competency and acquisition Several studies have shown that parents of
of targets and a relation between the supervisors’ children with ASD report higher levels of stress
experience and acquisition of targets. A stronger as compared to parents of typically developing
association between intensity of supervision and children or parents of children with other dis-
outcome was reported by Eikeseth, Hayward, abilities (Bouma & Schweitzer, 1990; Hastings
Gale, Gitlesen, and Eldevik (2009). In that study, & Johnson, 2001; Silva & Schalock, 2011;
intensity of supervision ranged from 3 to 8 h per Zablotsky, Bradshaw, & Stuart, 2013). In addi-
month per child, and significant correlation was tion to increased levels of stress, mothers of
found between supervision intensity and children with ASD are at greater risk for persis-
improvement in IQ. tent mental health problems, including depres-
It is possible that the effect of supervision may sion (Glidden & Schoolcraft, 2003; Montes &
not be linear. That is, a relative low intensity in Halterman, 2007). Higher levels of stress in
supervision may produce little or no benefit; a parents of children with ASD may be due to the
certain level of intensity may yield optimal effect, prevalence of problem behaviors associated
while increasing supervision beyond this point with the disorder (Hastings & Beck, 2004;
may add little benefit above the optimal level. Hastings & Johnson; McStay, Dissanayake,
Also, what constitutes the optimal level of super- Scheeren, Koot, & Begeer, 2013) or to chil-
vision may vary from child to child and depend dren’s lower intellectual and adaptive function-
on child characteristics, parental involvement, ing (Rivard, Terroux, Parent-Boursier, &
and the competency of the therapists. Mercier, 2014). A recent study assessed changes
in stress levels in parents after receiving EIBI
(Eikeseth, Klintwall, Hayward & Gale, 2015).
 arental Involvement and Parental
P Results showed that both mothers and fathers
Stress reported elevated parental stress before the
EIBI program started, which is consistent with
In EIBI programs, parents are trained on inter- previous investigations (e.g., Bouma &
vention methods to become co-teachers for their Schweitzer; Lecavalier, Leone, & Wiltz, 2006;
child. They also learn how to manage the child’s McStay et al., 2013; Silva & Schalock, 2012;
challenging behaviors and how to help the child Zablotsky et al., 2013). Results also showed
use the skills he or she has learned in everyday that levels of stress for mothers decreased sig-
life. Parents are also taught how they can main- nificantly after 1 year of EIBI, but decreases in
tain a good family environment and how to care fathers’ scores were not significant. Intake
for siblings. If parents are able to become skilled parental stress was neither associated with
“therapists,” they may extend the intervention intake scores (IQ or intake adaptive function-
into the child’s everyday life, and this may ing) nor was it associated with outcome. This
increase the intensity of the intervention and pro- finding is consistent with that reported by Shine
vide numerous opportunities for working on gen- and Perry (2010). With only a couple of studies
eralization and maintenance of newly acquired published so far, research on changes in paren-
adaptive skills. teral stress as a function of EIBI and research
One meta-analysis has found an association on the importance of parental involvement in
between parent training and outcome (Makrygianni EIBI outcome are still in its infancy.
216 S. Eikeseth

Improving Effectiveness to respond correctly when a particular object is


of Treatment Procedures requested (i.e., object one). Next, the child is
taught to respond correctly when another object
Another important line of research is to increase is requested (object two). After mastery of these
effectiveness of the treatment procedures cur- two first steps, the teacher starts to request
rently used in EIBI. What follows is a review of object one and object two in a semi-random
some topics that are central for improving the and, finally, in a random order. The structure of
effectiveness of EIBI, starting with discrimina- those trials is typically based on an error analy-
tion training. sis of the child’s performance.
Some investigators have suggested that all
of the steps before presenting the stimuli in a
Improving Effectiveness random order should be omitted. This is
of Discrimination Training because the initial steps may likely establish
error patterns that hamper subsequent learning
Discrimination training concerns how teaching of the discrimination (Green 2001). This
trials are structured to optimize learning. Some hypothesis was supported by Grow, Carr,
of the seminal work on discrimination training Kodak, Jostad, and Kisamore (2011) and
with individuals with ASD in applied settings Holmes, Eikeseth, and Schultze (2015). Both
was conducted in the 1960s and 1970s by studies showed more rapid acquisition of
Lovaas, Schreibman, and colleagues, when receptive labeling skills when stimuli were
they developed and evaluated procedures to presented randomly from the beginning of the
teach communication and imitation to individu- training. In these studies, however, all partici-
als with ASD (Lovaas, 1977; Lovaas, Berberich, pants had already acquired a number of recep-
Perloff, & Schaeffer, 1966; Lovaas & Newsom, tive labels before entering the study. Future
1976; Lovaas, Schreibman Koegel, & Rehm, research could use participants who have not
1971; Schreibman, 1975). Today, procedures to yet learned any receptive labels and examine
establish discrimination and stimulus control whether these children also respond best to the
are central to EIBI, and analysis shows that random rotation procedure.
relatively simple skills (e.g., receptive identifi- Which type of discrimination training proce-
cation of objects) may involve complex dis- dure that is more effective may also depend on
criminations (Eikeseth, Smith, Klintwall, the complexity of the skill taught. For example, it
2014). Even so, after these studies were pub- is possible that one procedure is more effective in
lished in the 1960s and 1970s, not much applied teaching a “simple skill” to a particular partici-
research has been conducted to evaluate and pant, whereas the other procedure is more effec-
develop more effective discrimination training tive in teaching a more complex discrimination in
procedures, for example, to teach language the same participant. Future research could
comprehension. Likely, the most common dis- explore these possibilities.
crimination training procedure used today is Other procedures to facilitate discrimina-
identical or similar to the one developed in the tion have been evaluated such as revised
1970s (Lovaas) and outlined more recently by blocked-trial procedure (Smeets & Striefel,
Lovaas (2003). This procedure is designed to 1994), combined blocking procedure (e.g.,
break down the discriminations into multiple, Perez-Gonzalez & Williams, 2002; Williams,
less complex steps and to introduce steps to Perez-Gonzalez, & Queiroz, 2005), and sound
gradually increase the complexity of the task as discrimination (Eikesth & Hayward, 2009).
the simpler discriminations are acquired. When Clearly, d­iscrimination and stimulus control
teaching receptive labeling of objects, for are essential in EIBI, and more research is
example, training consists of teaching the child needed in this area.
13  Additional Treatment Variables 217

 iscrete Trial Teaching


D uli properties as preferred stimuli, by establishing
Versus Naturalistic Teaching the social stimuli as conditioned reinforces. This
can be done by paring the social stimuli with
Another important topic for research is whether other stimuli that already function as strong rein-
teaching skills in child-led naturalistic settings is forcers, with the result that the previously neutral
more effective than teaching the same skills dur- social stimulus becomes reinforcing. Research
ing discrete trial teaching. Potential advantages suggest that an effective procedure to do so is to
of discrete trial teaching are that stimulus pro- present the neutral social stimulus to the child,
gramming can be planned and structured, specific require the child to make a specific response in
prompt-fading procedures can be used, and the presence of the neutral social stimulus, and
potent reinforcer can be programmed. The poten- finally reinforce that response with potent rein-
tial advantage of naturalistic child-led teaching is forcer (Dozier, Iwata, Thomason-Sassi, Worsdell,
that the child’s attention and motivation may be & Wilson, 2012; Holth, Vandbakk, Finstad,
stronger as compared to discrete trial teaching Grønnerud, & Akselsen Sørensen, 2009; Isaksen
because the child is initiating the interaction with & Holth, 2009; Lauten & Birnbrauer,1974;
the teacher and not the other way around. Also, in Lovaas et al., 1966; Taylor-Santa, Sidener, Carr,
naturalistic child-led teaching, the reinforcer is & Reeve, 2014). In this way, a number of social
the natural consequence that the behavior pro- stimuli such as verbal statements and facial
duces, such as getting to play with the horse after expressions (e.g., smile and nodding) may be
requesting it. This type of teaching may also conditioned to become reinforcers and preferred
more likely be followed by generalization. stimuli (c.f., Isaksen & Holth). Indeed, Maffei,
In one study, Delprato (2001) reported larger Singer-Dudek, and Keohane (2014) showed that
gains for children who received naturalistic after establishing the behavior of observing faces
teaching as compared to discrete trial teaching. and listening to voices as conditioned reinforcers,
However, perhaps discrete trial teaching is more the participants with ASD demonstrated
effective for teaching basic skills such as motor increased attention to the presence of a person,
and vocal imitation, and naturalistic child-led listened and responded to instructions more read-
interventions are more efficient for teaching gen- ily, and showed increased learning. Greer,
eralized language use and to expand a basic Pistoljevic, Cahill, and Du (2011) found that after
behavioral repertoire (Schreibman, Dufek, & adult voices had been established as conditioned
Cunningham, 2011; Smith, 2001). In any case, as reinforcers, orientation toward adult voices
when conducting such studies, it is pertinent that and listening to adult voices increased in free-­
the interventions compared are at a gold stan- choice settings. Also, after establishing the adult
dard, and it is unclear whether this was the case voices as conditioned reinforcers, the participants
in the Delprato (2001) study. Clearly, additional required fewer teaching trials to master tasks
research to study child-led naturalistic teaching involving verbal instructions.
and discrete trial teaching is necessary. In contrast to social stimuli, certain nonsocial
(sensory) stimuli may function as potent rein-
forcers for the behaviors of children with
 stablishing Social Stimuli
E ASD. Possibly, these reinforcers may compete
as Reinforcers with social reinforcers in such a way that a child
engages in stereotyped behaviors (such as hand
Because social stimuli typically do not function flapping to produce sensory reinforcers) rather
as a reinforcer for the behavior of children with than in social behaviors (such as joint attention
ASD (Ferster, 1961), it is important to develop behavior to produce social reinforcement).
effective interventions to increase the children’s The research on social reinforcement is impor-
preference for social stimuli. Classical and oper- tant and promising, but more research is needed
ant conditioning may be used to give social stim- to refine procedures to establish social stimuli as
218 S. Eikeseth

reinforcers, as well as to investigate ways to Sarokoff, Taylor, & Poulson, 2001; Thiemann &
maintain the reinforcing properties of the social Goldstein, 2001).
stimuli over time once they are established. Also, Only a few studies, however, have directly
research could focus on discovering procedures compared the efficacy of different prompting tac-
for reducing the reinforcing effects of nonsocial tics (Finkel & Williams, 2001; Ingvarsson &
(sensory) stimuli. Hollobaugh, 2011; Vedora, Meunier, & Mackay,
2009). Ingvarsson and Hollobaugh (2010) com-
pared the efficacy of echoic prompts and picture
Language Interventions prompts and found that although both types of
prompts were effective in teaching intraverbal
Language interventions are a central part of responses to children with ASD, picture prompts
EIBI. Children with ASD often require explicit resulted in fewer trials to mastery. Similar results
teaching to learn language and communication. have been reported in two studies comparing the
One type of language intervention which recently efficacy of textual prompts and echoic prompts
has received attention from researchers is teach- (Finkel & Williams, 2001; Vedora et al., 2009).
ing intraverbal behaviors, such as answering Interestingly, the participants’ idiosyncratic
question “how are you?” with “fine” and saying learning history may affect which type of prompt
“car” in response to hearing “you drive a…?” is more effective. Ingvarsson and Le (2011)
Together with other verbal operants (echoic, showed that echoic prompts were more efficient
mand, tact), intraverbal behavior forms an impor- at teaching intraverbals to children with ASD
tant part of a person’s ability to engage in appro- than picture prompts and suggested that this may
priate social interaction, enabling them to answer have been because the participants in their study
questions and have conversations. While typical had a history of receiving echoic prompts in their
children acquire intraverbal behavior without daily teaching programs. This hypothesis was
explicit training, children with ASD may learn supported in a study by Coon and Miguel (2012)
intraverbal behavior only after specific program- who showed that the prompt procedure most
ming (Sundberg & Sundberg, 2011). recently used to teach intraverbal behavior was
To examine ways in which children with ASD more effective in teaching new intraverbal behav-
can be more successfully taught intraverbal ior, when compared to prompt procedures not
behavior, one line of research has focused on pro- used recently.
cedures that enable transfer of stimulus control Another recent line of research has shown that
from a stimulus that is already known to exert children with ASD may “work out” the answer
control over the verbal response (the prompt) to for new, untaught questions after learning to
the target antecedent stimulus for the intraverbal answer some other questions (Pérez-González
response. The intervention involves fading the et al., 2007; Grannan & Rehfeldt, 2012; May,
prompt so that the target intraverbal response Hawkins & Dymond, 2013). For example, May
comes under the control of the target verbal ante- et al. (2013) found that adolescents with ASD
cedent stimulus complex. Prompts involve vocal were able to answer untaught question such as
(echoic) prompts (Ingvarsson & Hollobaugh, “what food does Simon eat?” and “which mon-
2010; Ingvarsson, Tiger, Hanley, & Stephenson, ster eats chips?” after learning to answer other
2007; Pérez-González, García-Asenjo, Williams, related questions such as to say “Simon” when
& Carnerero, 2007; Petursdottir, Carr, Lechago, asked “what is the name of this monster?” (while
& Almason, 2008; Secan, Egel, & Tilley, 1989; shown a picture of the monster Simon) and to
Watkins, Pack-Teixeira, & Howard, 1989), pic- learn to say “chips” when asked “what food does
ture prompts (Braam & Poling, 1983; Goldsmith, this monster eat?” (also while shown a picture of
LeBlanc, & Sautter, 2007; Luciano, 1986; the monster Simon).
Miguel, Petursdottir, & Carr, 2005), or textual Another possibility is that teaching certain
prompts (Krantz & McClannahan, 1993, 1998; receptive language skills may facilitate the
13  Additional Treatment Variables 219

acquisition of intraverbal behavior. Recently, ing a period of EIBI treatment of 1–4 years (c.f.,
Smith et al. (2016) showed that emergent intra- Smith et al., 2000; Eikeseth et al., 2007; Howard,
verbal forms occurred for children with ASD Sparkman, Cohen, Green & Stanislaw, 2005;
after they had learned certain receptive skills. ForCohen et al., 2006; Remington et al., 2007). Only
example, the children were able to say “tomato” a couple of studies have assessed children some-
when asked, “what do you eat that is red?” after time after an EIBI program has been terminated.
learning to point to the object tomato when hear- In one such study, Kovshoff, Hastings, and
ing, “what do you eat that is red?” Other studies, Remington (2011) found that although a EIBI
however, have failed to show acquisition of intra- group scored significantly higher on IQ, lan-
verbal behavior as a result of receptive training guage, and daily living skills compared to a com-
(Miguel et al., 2005; Petursdottir et al., 2008; parison group after 2 years of EIBI, these
Petursdottir, Olafsdottir, & Aradottir, 2008; differences were no longer present 2 years after
Petursdottir & Haflidadottir, 2009). Hence, more the program had ended, suggesting that gains
research of this type is warranted (c.f., Eikeseth may not be sustained over time when services are
& Smith, 2013). terminated. Some of the participants in the EIBI
group had received a university-managed EIBI
program, whereas the other participants in the
Durability of Improvements EIBI group had received a parent-managed EIBI
Over Time provision. A post hoc analysis showed that the
children who had received the university-­
Typically, EIBI programs begin in preschool managed provision typically lost their gains after
years; are initially home based or kindergarten the treatment had ended, while the gains were
based, with one-to-one teaching; and later include maintained for the children who had received the
a gradual integration into preschool and school parent-managed provision. Perhaps the parental
settings. As the children grow older, provision is involvement was greater in the parent-managed
adapted based on age and ability. Adaptations EIBI group as compared to the university-­
may include greater emphasis on different parts commissioned group, and children in the former
of the curriculum, such as academic and social group maintained their gains because the behav-
skills for those children progressing in school ioral intervention continued for those programs
and functional independence skills in the home that were parent managed (Kovshoff et al., 2011).
and the community for those children who learn This could be a topic for further research.
at a slower rate. This treatment can continue as McEachin, Smith, and Lovaas (1993) reported
the child becomes older, to teach further age-­ follow-up results on a group of 19 children with a
appropriate skills, and many EIBI programs con- mean age of 11.5 years who were originally the
tinue in this way. This is because the principles experimental group in a study conducted by
and techniques used in EIBI are effective for Lovaas (1987). Lovaas reported that the children
learners of different ages (Heinicke & Carr, 2014; who received EIBI made more gains than chil-
LeBlanc, 2010) and are not diagnosis specific dren in the control group, and McEachin, Smith,
(Eldevik, Jahr, Eikeseth, Hastings & Hughes, and Lovaas reported that these children had
2010; Smith, Eikeseth, Klevstrand & Lovaas, maintained their gains. This is the oldest group of
1997). EIBI programs can continue at the same children studied to date, and results suggest, in
intensity or may be reduced or phased out if the contrast to the Kovshoff et al. (2011) study, that
child is able to learn in a typical environment. gains made during EIBI can be maintained.
Full-time behavioral intervention can be the main Hence, there is an urgent need for additional fol-
educational provision through school-age years low-­up research in adolescence and adulthood to
and continue as a lifelong provision. examine the extent to which treatment gains are
Most outcome studies conducted to date have maintained over a long period of time, whether
measured progress in children with ASD follow- certain types of maintenance treatment are
220 S. Eikeseth

required, and which children will succeed which integrate behavioral strategies with
without such maintenance treatment and who developmental social-pragmatic models. The
will not. ESDM has been evaluated in a well-designed
randomized controlled trial (RCT, Dawson et al.,
2010). Results showed that ESDM resulted in
 IBI’s Effects on Behaviors Other
E significant improvement in cognitive and adap-
Than Cognitive and Adaptive Skills tive behavior, but only small, nonsignificant
effects on ASD symptoms. Increases in cognitive
Although outcome measures typically have and adaptive function for the ESDM children
involved IQ and adaptive functioning, other out- after 2 years of intervention were 17.6 and –0.85,
come measures have also been employed when respectively.
evaluating EIBI, such as a measure of autism LEAP is a behavioral program that integrates
symptoms, behavioral problems, and social children with ASD with typically developing
skills. peers in early typical education settings. LEAP
A few studies have shown that EIBI may result has been evaluated in a well-designed
in the reduction of autism symptoms, as assessed RCT. Results showed that after 2 years of inter-
by the Autism Diagnostic Instrument-Revised vention, LEAP had moderate, beneficial effects
(ADI-R; Lord et al., 1994; Sallows & Graupner, on ASD symptoms and large, positive effects on
2005), the Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS, cognitive, language, and social interaction as
Schopler, Reichler, & Renner, 1986; Eikeseth et al., compared to the comparison group (Strain &
2012), and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Bovey, 2011).
Schedule (ADOS, Lord, Rutter, DiLavore, & Risi, The Pivotal Response Training (PRT; Koegel
1999; Ben-Itzchak et al., 2007). One study found et al., 1989) is a naturalistic intervention that
no significant difference in the reduction in autism aims to establish key or “pivotal” responses that,
symptoms as result of EIBI (Remington et al., when acquired, have the potential to improve per-
2007), as measured by the Autism Screening formance across many other skill areas. PRT has
Questionnaire (Berument, Rutter, Lord, Pickles, & been successful for teaching a variety of skills
Bailey, 1999). and particularly for reducing social deficits
Also, improvement in social behavior, joint (Koegel, Koegel, Shoshan, & McNerney, 1999;
attention, play, and imitation, as well as a reduc- Kuhn, Bodkin, Devlin, & Doggett, 2008), but
tion in maladaptive behaviors, aggressive behav- controlled group studies and systematic reviews
iors, and stereotyped behaviors, as a result of are available only for spoken communication.
EIBI has been reported (Ben-Itzchak & Zachor, The Princeton Child Development Institute
2007; Cohen et al., 2006; Eikeseth et al., 2007; model (Fenske, Zalenski, Kranz, & McClannahan,
Remington et al., 2007), but most studies to date 1985) is another behavioral approach with docu-
have not included such measures. Hence, future mented support from numerous single-subject
outcome research should be designed to include studies of children with ASD, but controlled
such measures. group studies are not yet available.
Hence, EIBI is one of the several comprehen-
sive behavioral interventions that can benefit
 omparing EIBI with Alternative
C children with ASD. Clearly, however, EIBI is the
Behavioral and Reinforcement-­ service model that is best researched and best
Based Approaches documented (Smith & Iadarola, 2016). Because
EIBI has already been established as effective,
A number of other comprehensive behavioral EIBI may be used as a benchmark or as a com-
programs exist (Smith & Iadarola, (2016), most parison condition when evaluating other treat-
notably is the Early Start Denver Model (ESDM), ment models.
13  Additional Treatment Variables 221

 dapting the Model to Different


A whether treatment goals may differ across the
Settings and Populations different treatment settings and cultures.
A potential advantage with school-based
Treatment Settings interventions is that a large number of typically
developing peers and a large number of teachers
Originally, EIBI was designed as a home-based are available for practicing generalization and
program, where children received intensive one-­ maintenance. Moreover, school-based programs
to-­
one treatment at home before they entered may provide more training on how to function in
school. An important reason for starting the inter- social settings and provide a larger focus on eat-
vention at home was to establish certain prerequi- ing and mealtime behaviors, as well as focusing
site skills that the children needed to know in on certain daily living skills such as dressing and
order to succeed in school. The school then following routines. A potential disadvantage with
became the arena for mainstreaming the child the school-based intervention may be that it
with regular peers by gradually fading the treat- reduces parental involvement (see below), which
ment step by step to determine the minimal level is considered a key factor in EIBI. Again, these
of support needed to function in and learn from speculations need to be validated empirically and
the school environment. For some children, the hence could be topics for future research.
behavioral intervention gradually faded out
because the child was able to learn from regular
education. EIBI for Other Populations
In Scandinavia as well as in other countries,
children enter kindergarten at an earlier age as EIBI is designed specifically to target the behav-
compared to children in the USA. This is also the ioral excesses and deficits exhibited by individual
case for children with special needs such as children, and the intervention is tailored to
ASD. Hence, typically, when children with ASD replace such deficit and excess behaviors with
are referred for treatment, they already have a functional and adaptive skills. Hence, EIBI is not
place in a kindergarten or preschool for typically designed to address “autism” per se or the “cause
developing children, and in addition, a special of autism” (Lovaas, 2003). For those and other
education resource has been allocated specifi- reasons, it can be hypothesized that EIBI may be
cally for the child. For those and perhaps other effective for other types of neurodevelopmental
reasons, the arena for EIBI has in some countries conditions as well.
become the kindergarten or preschool rather than To date, only two outcome studies have exam-
the home setting. EIBI has been evaluated in ined the effects of EIBI for non-autistic popula-
these settings, and outcome from school-based tions, both addressing intellectual disabilities.
programs seems comparable to effects obtained Smith, Eikeseth, Klevstrand, and Lovaas (1997)
from home programs in the USA (c.f., the com- evaluated the effects of EIBI provided to 11 chil-
parison above between the Howard et al. (2014) dren with severe intellectual disabilities. A com-
and the Cohen et al. (2006) studies which were parison group of ten children received minimal
home-based to the school-based study by treatment. Groups were similar on all measures at
Eikeseth et al. (2002, 2007)). However, there has intake, with a mean age at intake of 3.08 years
been no studies designed to compare differences and mean intake IQ of 28. The mean IQ gain for
between home-based programs and school-based the EIBI group after a minimum of 24 months of
programs, and until this research has been done, intervention was 8 points, while the comparison
no firm conclusions can be drawn regarding group, on average, lost 3 IQ points over the same
whether they are equally effective or not. There period. Moreover, children in the EIBI group
might be other differences between school-based acquired more expressive language than children
programs and home-based programs, which also in the comparison group. Hence, EIBI may result
merit further research. For example, it is unclear in meaningful gains for preschool-aged children
222 S. Eikeseth

with severe intellectual disabilities, although the on the eye regions of the adult and more on other
children in the study remained significantly regions such as the mouth, body, and objects in
developmentally delayed. the environment, when compared to the typically
Eldevik et al. (2010) evaluated the effects of developing infants. In another landmark, eye-­
EIBI for children with mild to moderate intellec- tracking study by Pierce, Conant, Hazin, Stoner,
tual disabilities. Eleven children received 10 h and Desmond (2011) found that 40% of the par-
per week of one-to-one EIBI. The mean intake ticipants with ASD preferred looking at geomet-
age for this group was 54 months, and the mean ric patterns rather than the social stimuli,
intake IQ was 56. Fourteen children with a mean compared to only 2% of typically developing
intake age of 46 months and a mean intake IQ of peers. If a toddler spent more than 69% of his or
50 received treatment as usual and constituted the her time fixating on geometric patterns, then the
comparison group. After 1 year of intervention, positive predictive value for accurately classify-
changes in intelligence and adaptive behavior ing that toddler as having an ASD was 100%.
scores were statistically significant in favor of the These studies do not only show that partici-
EIBI group (effect sizes of 1.13 for IQ change pants with ASD show less preference for social
and 0.95 for change in adaptive behavior com- stimuli, as previously suggested by Ferster (1961)
posite). Children in the EIBI group gained an and others; they also demonstrate that partici-
average of 16 IQ points and 3 points in adaptive pants with ASD show an increased preference for
behavior. Sixty-four percent of the children in the nonsocial stimuli. This occurs most likely
EIBI group met the criteria for reliable change in because the different types of stimuli have differ-
IQ, as compared to 14% of the comparison group. ent reinforcement valence for the behavior of
The results from these two studies give some infants with a subsequent ASD diagnosis, as
support for the notion that EIBI may be effective compared to typically developing infants and
for populations other than children with infants with developmental delays. Note that oth-
ASD. However, additional research is needed to ers (most notably Lovaas, Newson, & Hickman,
further examine this possibility, preferably using 1987) have used the term sensory reinforcers to
a RCT design. denote the type of stimuli here referred to as non-
social reinforcers.
These findings have occasioned a nonsocial
Mediators of EIBI reinforcement hypothesis of ASD (Eikeseth,
2016), which asserts that infants develop ASD
 an Reinforcement Be Used
C largely because they – for some yet unknown
to Understand and Treat ASD biological reasons – have an affinity for nonso-
cial (sensory) reinforcers, in contrast to typically
Since reinforcement is central to EIBI, the ques- developing children who have an affinity for
tion arises as to whether reinforcement may not social reinforcers. Nonsocial (sensory) reinforc-
only be a key to treating ASD but also could be a ers may select autistic behavior by reinforcing
key to the understanding of ASD. Over the past looking at geometrical patterns in the environ-
decade, there has been a great deal of research on ment, moving objects, and listening to nonverbal
infants at risk of developing ASD. The aim of this sounds and sounds that are synchronized with
research has been to identify biological and/or movements, rather than attending to the eyes and
behavioral markers that reliably predict later voice of caregivers, as seen in typical develop-
onset of ASD. Using eye-tracking technology, ment. As the infants acquire more advanced
Jones and Klin (2013) assessed eye fixation in motor skills, nonsocial (sensory) reinforcers will
infants later diagnosed with ASD and compared not only select looking; they will also select a
the results to that of typically developing infants. more advanced repertoire of repetitive and ste-
Between 2 and 6 months of age, infants who later reotyped behaviors such as hand flapping, object
received a diagnosis of ASD begun fixating less twirling, lining of objects, ear cupping, rocking,
13  Additional Treatment Variables 223

spinning of body, pacing, toe walking, repeatedly that in turn produce a wider range of social
switching lights on and off, etc., all of which are reinforcers. The change in child behavior leading
defining characteristics of ASD. Hence, the to a change in parent behavior in turn leading to
increased operant control of fingers, hands, change in child behavior and so on is often
arms, and feet makes additional contingencies referred to as transactional model of development
possible, and these contingencies may lead to (Novak & Pelaez, 2004). The behavior of the
stereotyped and repetitive behaviors (e.g., toe infant changes, and so does the behavior of the
walking and spinning of body are possible after parent, and this transaction conditions previously
learning to walk). neutral social stimuli such as body language and
Hence, if nonsocial (sensory) stimuli are more various facial expressions as reinforcers
reinforcing than social stimuli, the environment (Eikeseth, 2016).
will select stereotyped and repetitive behaviors According to the nonsocial reinforcement
over social behavior. Verbal operants such as hypothesis of ASD, the key to treat or even pre-
tacts and intraverbals are shaped and maintained vent ASD is to decrease the infants’ affinity for
by social reinforcement and are often missing or nonsocial (sensory) reinforcers and to increase
delayed in children with ASD. Echoic behavior, their affinity for social stimuli. Logically, both
which is more often seen in children with ASD, biological and behavioral interventions could
does not require the same type of social rein- achieve this. Behaviorally, intervention should
forcement since copying a stimulus may be rein- not only focus on establishing social stimuli as
forcing in itself. In children with a slightly reinforcers but also aim to reducing the reinforc-
different nervous system (such as children with ing valence of nonsocial stimuli.
ASD), the affinity for nonsocial stimuli at the To support the nonsocial reinforcement
expense of the affinity for social stimuli may hypothesis of ASD, it needs to be demonstrated
result in a subsequent negative spiral in acquiring that children with ASD (or better yet infants who
social interests, language skills, and social behav- later develop ASD) not only prefer looking at
iors. Slight differences early in life in affinity for nonsocial stimuli but that they also are willing to
nonsocial stimuli can also have negative effects work harder to obtain nonsocial (sensory) rein-
on the establishment of previously neutral stimuli forcers as compared to social reinforcers.
as conditioned reinforcers, which in turn will
hamper the development of language, social
skills, and social interests.  IBI, Behavior Change,
E
For typically developing infants, in contrast, and Collateral Changes
the highly potent social reinforcers will select in Neurobiology
fixation to the eye regions of adults and attentive
listening to human voices. The social reinforcers This vast improvement in functioning shown by
will subsequently shape additional social skills some children with ASD as a result of EIBI cre-
such as social smiling, imitation, and babbling, ates a valuable opportunity to study behavior-­
which in turn are behavioral cusps that bring the brain interactions. Corresponding changes in
infant into additional social contingencies that brain activation or brain functioning with the
increase the complexity of the social interaction achievement of improved or normal behavioral
between the infant and the caregivers. This in functioning as a result of EIBI, however, have not
turn provides the infant with additional social yet been demonstrated. A chief reason for this is
consequences. This type of early social dyad that current scanning technologies such as func-
between the parent and the infant is highly rein- tional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can-
forcing also for the caregivers. These social con- not be applied to most children with ASD as the
tingencies will select increasingly advanced children move their heads excessively confound-
types of social skills, such as joint attention, and ing the results and/or are afraid to go into the
more advanced social communication behaviors MRI scanner.
224 S. Eikeseth

EEG is another type of technology that can be patterns of brain activity. Another line of research
used to study changes of brain activity, and this using NIRS technology can be to examine brain
procedure is more available and less invasive, as activation when exposing children with ASD and
compared to fMRI. Yet, there has been no studies typically developing children to social and non-
examining the extent to which changes in cogni- social stimuli. As of now, this is a vide open field
tive and adaptive functioning after EIBI are asso- of research, and almost any form of data integrat-
ciated with changed patterns of brain activity. ing EIBI and changes or normalization in brain
However, Dawson et al. (2012) examined changes activation is of great importance and of vast
in EEG activity for children with ASD who par- interest.
ticipated in an outcome study evaluating the
effects of the ESDM. Event-related potentials
were measured during the presentation of two Concluding Remarks
conditions: faces and objects. Results showed
that the ESDM group and typical children showed Although EIBI is widely used and well docu-
a shorter latency and increased cortical activation mented, there are still a well of questions to be
when viewing faces, whereas the community asked. Also, much of the research discussed in
intervention group showed the opposite pattern. the current paper is data in need of replication.
Greater cortical activation while viewing faces This is because much of the research is based on
was associated with improved social behavior, noncontrolled studies, quasi-random studies,
suggesting that behavioral intervention may and/or ad hoc analysis of data obtained from
affect brain activation. studies designed to assess something else, such
Recently, an alternate imaging tool has been as when searching for predictors ad hoc in a study
available, and this scanning technology provides designed to evaluate outcome. This may be an
much of the same information as fMRI but with- important reason for why many conflicting
out the constraints of head motion and fear. This results are reported. Thus, future research should
scanning technology is called functional near-­ not only attempt to answer questions such as the
infrared spectroscopy (NIRS), it is relatively ones outlined in this chapter but also strive to use
inexpensive, and it has been extensively used in a more stringent experimental methodology than
infants to examine brain activation. generally has been the case until today.
NIRS includes wearing a net or a headband of
optical sensors on the head. The equipment is
portable and does not have any safety risks. To References
assess blood flow in the brain, NIRS uses a low-­
power light source with detectors that see how Ben-Itzchak, E., Watson, L., & Zachor, D. (2014).
Cognitive ability is associated with different outcome
the light changes as it passes through the brain trajectories in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
tissue. Brain blood flow can indicate which parts Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2221–2229.
of the brain are active during different tasks. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2091-0.
So far, there has been only a few studies pub- Ben-Itzchak, E., & Zachor, D. A. (2007). The effects of
intellectual functioning and autism severity on out-
lished using NIRS in children with ASD (c.f., come of early behavioral intervention for children
Mori et al., 2015; Keehn, Wagner, Tager-Flusberg with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
& Nelson, 2013; Kita et al., 2011). Research 28, 287–303.
using NIRS technology could examine the extent Berument, S. K., Rutter, M., Lord, C., Pickles, A., &
Bailey, A. (1999). Autism screening questionnaire:
to which children receiving normal functioning Diagnostic validity. The British Journal of Psychiatry,
as a result of EIBI show corresponding normal- 175, 444–451. doi:10.1192/bjp.175.5.444.
ized patterns of brain activity. More fine-grained Bibby, P., Eikeseth, S., Martin, N. T., Mudford, O., &
analysis could examine the extent to which acqui- Reeves, D. (2001). Progress and outcomes for chil-
dren with autism receiving parent-managed intensive
sition of specific receptive, expressive, and social intervention. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
skills may result in corresponding normalized 22, 425–447.
13  Additional Treatment Variables 225

Blacklock, K., Perry, A., & Geier, J. D. (2014). Examining autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
the effectiveness of intensive behavioural intervention 158–178.
in children with autism aged 6 and older. Journal on Eikeseth, S. (2010). Examination of qualifications
Developmental Disabilities, 20, 37–49. required of an EIBI professional. European Journal of
Bouma, R., & Schweitzer, R. (1990). The impact of Behavior Analysis, 11, 239–246.
chronic childhood illness on family stress: A com- Eikeseth, S. (2016). Psychopathology as a result of selec-
parison between autism and cystic fibrosis. Journal of tion by consequences exemplified by autism spectrum
Clinical Psychology, 46, 722–730. disorders (ASD). Norsk Tidsskrift for Atferdsanalyse
Braam, S. J., & Poling, S. (1983). Development of (Norwegian Journal of Behavior Analysis), 43, 35–38.
intraverbal behavior in mentally retarded individu- Eikeseth, S., & Hayward, D. (2009). Discrimination
als through transfer of stimulus control procedures: of object names and object sounds in children with
Classification of verbal responses. Applied Research autism: A procedure for teaching verbal compre-
in Mental Retardation, 4, 279–302. hension. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42,
Cohen, H., Amerine-Dickens, M., & Smith, T. (2006). 807–812.
Early intensive behavioral treatment: Replication of the Eikeseth, S., Hayward, D., Gale, C., Gitlesen, J. P., &
UCLA model in a community setting. Developmental Eldevik, S. (2009). Intensity of supervision and out-
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 145–155. come for preschool aged children receiving early and
Coon, J. T., & Miguel, C. F. (2012). The role of increased intensive behavioral interventions: A preliminary
exposure to transfer-of-stimulus control procedures study. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3,
on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. Journal of 67–73.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 657–666. Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Hayward, D., & Gale, C.
Davis, B. J., Smith, T., & Donahoe, P. (2002). Evaluating (2015). Stress in parents of children with autism
supervisors in the UCLA treatment model for children participating in early and intensive behavioral inter-
with autism: Validation of an assessment procedure. vention. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 16,
Behavior Therapy, 33, 601–614. 112–120. doi:10.1080/15021149.2015.1066566.
Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain Eikeseth, S., Klintwall, L., Jahr, E., & Karlsson, P. (2012).
plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum disor- Outcome for children with autism receiving early and
der. Development and Psychopathology, 20, 775–803. intensive behavioral intervention in mainstream pre-
Dawson, G., Bernier, R., & Ring, R. H. (2012). Social school and kindergarten settings. Research in Autism
attention: A possible early indicator of efficacy in Spectrum Disorders, 6, 829–835.
autism clinical trials. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Eikeseth, S., & Smith, D. P. (2013). An analysis of verbal
Disorders, 4, 1–12. doi:10.1186/1866-1955-4-11. stimulus control in intraverbal behavior: Implications
Dawson, G., Jones, E. J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K., for practice and applied research. The Analysis of
Lowy, R., Faja, S., … Webb, S. J. (2012). Early Verbal Behavior, 29, 125–135.
behavioral intervention is associated with normalized Eikeseth, S., Smith, D. P., & Klintwall, L. (2014).
brain activity in young children with autism. Journal Discrete trial teaching and discrimination training.
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent In P. Sturmey, J. Tarbox, D. Dixon, & J. L. Matson
Psychiatry, 51, 1150–1159. http://doi.org/10.1016/j. (Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for autism
jaac.2012.08.018. spectrum disorders: Research, practice, and policy
Dawson, G., Rogers, S., Munson, J., Smith, M., Winter, J., (pp. 293–324). New York, NY: Springer.
Greenson, J., … Varley, J. (2010). Randomized, con- Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2002).
trolled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4- to
The early start Denver model. Pediatrics, 125, 17–23. 7-year-old children with autism. A 1-year comparison
Delprato, D. J. (2001). Comparisons of discrete-trial controlled study. Behavior Modification, 26, 49–68.
and normalized behavioral language intervention for Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2007).
young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Outcome for children with autism who began ­intensive
Developmental Disorders, 31, 315–325. behavioral treatment between ages 4 and 7: A com-
Dixon, D. R., Linstead, E., Granpeesheh, D., Novack, parison controlled study. Behavior Modification, 31,
M. N., French, R., Stevens, E., … Powell, A. (2016). 264–278.
Intensity, supervisor qualifications, and caseload on Eldevik, J., Eikeseth, H., & Hughes. (2010). Early
outcomes in the treatment of autism spectrum dis- behavioral intervention for children with intellec-
order. Behavior Analysis in Practice. doi:10.1007/ tual disability. Behavior Modification, 34, 16–34.
s40617-016-0132-1. doi:10.1177/0145445509351961.
Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., Thomason-Sassi, J., Worsdell, Eldevik, S., Eikeseth, S., Jahr, E., & Smith, T. (2006).
A. S., & Wilson, D. M. (2012). A comparison of two Effects of low-intensity behavioral treatment for chil-
pairing procedures to establish praise as a reinforcer. dren with autism and mental retardation. Journal of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analyses, 45, 721–735. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 211–224.
doi:10.1901/jaba.2012.45-721. Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E.,
Eikeseth, S. (2009). Outcome of comprehensive psycho-­ Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2009). Meta-analysis of
educational interventions for young children with early intensive behavioral intervention for children
226 S. Eikeseth

with autism. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Harris, S. L., & Handleman, J. S. (2000). Age and IQ at
Psychology, 38, 439–450. intake as predictors of placement for young children
Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Hughes, J. C., Jahr, E., with autism: A four- to six-year follow-up. Journal of
Eikeseth, S., & Cross, S. (2010). Using partici- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 137–142.
pant data to extend the evidence base for intensive Hastings, R. P., & Beck, A. (2004). Practitioner review:
behavioral intervention for children with autism. Stress intervention for parents of children with intel-
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental lectual disabilities. Journal of Child Psychology and
Disabilities, 115, 381–405. Psychiatry, 45, 1338–1349.
Eldevik, S., Hastings, R. P., Jahr, E., & Hughes, J. C. Hastings, R. P., & Johnson, E. (2001). Stress in UK
(2012). Outcomes of behavioral intervention for chil- families conducting intensive home-based behavioral
dren with autism in mainstream pre-school settings. intervention for their young child with autism. Journal
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 327–336.
210–220. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1234-9. Hayward, D. W., Eikeseth, S., Gale, C., & Morgan, S.
Fenske, E. C., Zalenski, S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, (2009). Assessing progress during treatment for young
L. E. (1985). Age at intervention and treatment children with autism receiving intensive behavioural
outcome for autistic children in a comprehensive interventions. Autism, 13, 613–633.
intervention program. Analysis and Intervention in Heinicke, M. R., & Carr, J. E. (2014). A meta-analysis
Developmental Disabilities, 5, 49–58. of single-case design intervention research in acquired
Ferster, C. B. (1961). Positive reinforcement and behav- brain injury rehabilitation. Behavioral Interventions,
ioral deficits in autistic children. Child Development, 29, 77–105.
32, 437–456. Holmes, E. J., Eikeseth, S., & Schulze, K. A. (2015).
Finkel, A. S., & Williams, R. L. (2001). A comparison of Teaching individuals with autism receptive labeling
textual and echoic prompts on the acquisition of intra- skills involving conditional discriminations: A com-
verbal behavior in a six-year old boy with autism. The parison of mass trial and intermixing before random
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 18, 61–70. rotation, random rotation only, and combined block-
Glidden, L. M., & Schoolcraft, S. A. (2003). Depression: ing. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11, 1–12.
Its trajectory and correlates in mothers rearing chil- doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2014.11.013.
dren with intellectual disability. Journal of Intellectual Holth, P., Vandbakk, M., Finstad, J., Grønnerud, E. M., &
Disability Research, 47, 250–263. Sørensen, J. M. A. (2009). An operant analysis of joint
Goldsmith, T. R., LeBlanc, L. A., & Sautter, R. A. (2007). attention and the establishment of conditioned social
Teaching intraverbal behavior to children with autism. reinforcers. European Journal of Behavior Analysis,
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 1–13. 10, 143–158.
Gould, E., Dixon, D. R., Najdowski, A. C., Smith, M. N., Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G.,
& Tarbox, J. (2011). A review of assessments for & Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive
determining the content of early intensive behavioral behavior analytic and eclectic treatments for young
intervention programs for autism spectrum disorders. children with autism. Research in Developmental
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 990–1002. Disabilities, 26, 359–383.
Grannan, L., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2012). Emergent intraver- Howard, J. S., Stanislaw, H., Green, G., Sparkman, C. R.,
bal responses via tact and match-to sample instruction. & Cohen, H. G. (2014). Comparison of behavior
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 45, 601–605. analytic and eclectic early interventions for young
Granpeesheh, D., Dixon, D. R., Tarbox, J., Kaplan, A. M., children with autism after three years. Research
& Wilke, A. E. (2009). The effects of age and treat- in Developmental Disabilities, 35, 3326–3344.
ment intensity on behavioral intervention outcomes doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2014.08.021.
for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research Howlin, P., Magiati, I., & Charman, T. (2009). Systematic
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 1014–1022. review of early intensive behavioral interventions for
doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.01.012. children with autism. American Journal on Intellectual
Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for learn- and Developmental Disabilities, 114, 23–41.
ers with autism: Advances in stimulus control tech- Ingvarsson, E. I., & Hollobaugh, T. (2011). A compari-
nology. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental son of prompting tactics to establish intraverbals in
Disorders, 16, 72–85. children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior
Greer, R. D., Pistoljevic, N., Cahill, C., & Du, L. (2011). Analysis, 44, 659–664.
Effects of conditioning voices as reinforcers for lis- Ingvarsson, E. I., & Le, D. D. (2011). Further evalua-
tener responses on rate of learning, awareness, and tion of prompting tactics for establishing intraverbal
preferences for listening to stories in preschoolers with responding in children with autism. The Analysis of
autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 103–124. Verbal Behavior, 27, 75–93.
Grow, L. L., Carr, J. E., Kodak, T. M., Jostad, C. M., & Ingvarsson, E. T., & Hollobaugh, T. (2010). Acquisition
Kisamore, A. N. (2011). A comparison of methods for of intraverbal behavior: Teaching children with
teaching receptive labelling to children with autism autism to mand for answers to questions. Journal of
spectrum disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Applied Behavior Analysis, 43, 1–17. doi:10.1901/
Analysis, 44, 475–498. jaba.2010.43-1.
13  Additional Treatment Variables 227

Ingvarsson, E. T., Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Stephenson, Introduction to the special issue. Education and
K. M. (2007). An evaluation of intraverbal training Treatment of Children, 33, 333–337.
procedures to generate socially appropriate responses Lecavalier, L., Leone, S., & Wiltz, J. (2006). The impact
to novel questions. Journal of Applied Behavior of behaviour problems on caregiver stress in young
Analysis, 40, 411–429. people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
Isaksen, J., & Holth, P. (2009). An operant approach to Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 172–183.
teaching joint attention skills to children with autism. Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook Jr, E. H., Leventhal,
Behavioral Interventions, 24, 215–236. B. L., DiLavore, P. C., … Rutter, M. (2000). The
Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A
but in decline in 2-6-month-old infants later diagnosed standard measure of social and communication defi-
with autism. Nature, 504, 427–431. cits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal
Kita, Y., Gunji, A., Inoue, Y., Goto, T., Sakihara, K., Kaga, of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223.
M., … Hosokawa, T. (2011). Self-face recognition Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diag-
in children with autism spectrum disorders: A near-­ nostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diag-
infrared spectroscopy study. Brain & Development, nostic interview for caregivers of individuals with
33, 494–503. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2010.11.007. possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal
Klintwall, L., & Eikeseth, S. (2012). Number and con- of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.
trollability of reinforcers as predictors of individual Lovaas, O. I. (1977). The autistic child: Language devel-
outcome for children with autism receiving early and opment through behavior modification. New York,
intensive behavioral intervention. Research in Autism NY: Irvington Publishers.
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 493–499. Lovaas, O. I. (1987). Behavioral treatment and normal
Klintwall, L., Macari, S., Eikeseth, S., & Chawarska, intellectual and educational functioning in autis-
K. (2015). Interest level in 2-year-olds with autism tic children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
spectrum disorder predicts rate of verbal,nonverbal, Psychology, 55, 3–9.
and adaptive skill acquisition. Autism, 19, 925–933. Lovaas, O. I. (2003). Teaching individuals with develop-
doi:10.1177/1362361314555376. mental delays: Basic intervention techniques. Austin,
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Shoshan, Y., & McNerney, E. TX: Pro-Ed.
(1999). Pivotal response intervention II: Preliminary Lovaas, O. I., Berberich, J. P., Perloff, B. F., & Schaeffer,
long-term outcome data. Research and Practice B. (1966). Acquisition of imitative speech by schizo-
for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 24, 186–198. phrenic children. Science, 151, 705–707.
doi:10.2511/rpsd.24.3.186. Lovaas, O. I., Freitag, G., Kinder, M. I., Rubenstein, B. D.,
Koegel, R. L., Schreffirnan, L., Good, A., Cerniglia, L., Schaeffer, B., & Simmons, J. Q. (1966). Establishment
Murphy, C., & Koegel, L. K. (1989). How to teach of social reinforcers in two schizophrenic children
pivotal behaviors to children with autism: A train- on the basis of food. Journal of Experimental Child
ing manual. Retrieved from http://www.users.qwest. Psychology, 4, 109–125.
net/~tbharris/prt.htm. Lovaas, O. I., & Newsom, C. D. (1976). Behavior
Kovshoff, H., Hastings, R. P., Remington, B. (2011) modification with psychotic children. In
Two-year outcomes for children with autism after the H. Leitenberg (Ed.), Handbook of behavior modi-
cessation of early intensive behavioral intervention. fication and therapy (pp. 303–360). New York, NY:
Behavior Modification, 35, 427–450. Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1993). Teaching Lovaas, O. I., Schreibman, L., Koegel, R., & Rehm,
children with autism to initiate to peers: Effects of a R. (1971). Selective responding by autistic chil-
script-fading procedure. Journal of Applied Behavior dren to multiple sensory input. Journal of Abnormal
Analysis, 26, 121–132. Psychology, 77, 211–222.
Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E. (1998). Social inter- Lovaas, O. I., & Smith, T. (1988). Intensive behavioral
action skills for children with autism: A script-fading treatment for young autistic children. Advances in
procedure for beginning readers. Journal of Applied clinical child psychology. In B. B. Lahey & A. E.
Behavior Analysis, 31, 191–202. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in clinical child psychology
Kuhn, L. R., Bodkin, A. E., Devlin, S. D., & Doggett, (vol. II, pp. 285–323). New York, NY: Plenum.
R. A. (2008). Using pivotal response training with Luciano, M. C. (1986). Acquisition, maintenance and gen-
peers in special education to facilitate play in two eralization of productive intraverbal behavior through
children with autism. Education and Training in transfer of stimulus control procedures. Applied
Developmental Disabilities, 43, 37–45. Research in Mental Retardation, 7, 1–20.
Lauten, M. H., & Birnbrauer, J. S. (1974). The efficacy of Maffei-Lewis, J., Singer-Dudek, J., & Keohane, D. D.
«right» as a function of its relationship with reinforce- (2014). The effects of the establishment of adult faces
ment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, and/or voices as conditioned reinforcers for children
159–166. with ASD and related disorders. Acta de Investigacion
LeBlanc, L. A. (2010). Using video-based interven- Psicologica (Psychological Research Records), 4,
tions with individuals with autism spectrum disorders: 1621–1641.
228 S. Eikeseth

Magiati, I., Charman, T., & Howlin, P. (2007). A two- Perry, A., Cummings, A., Dunn Geier, J., Freeman, N. L.,
year prospective follow-up study of community- Hughes, S., Managhan, T., … Williams, J. (2011).
based early intensive behavioural intervention and Predictors of outcome for children receiving intensive
specialist nursery provision for children with autism behavioral intervention in a large, community-based
spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and program. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
Psychiatry, 48, 803–812. 592–603.
Makrygianni, M. K., & Reed, P. (2010). A meta-analytic Petursdottir, A. I., Carr, J. E., Lechago, S. A., & Almason,
review of the effectiveness of behavioural early inter- S. M. (2008). An evaluation of intraverbal training
vention programs for children with autistic spectrum and listener training for teaching categorization skills.
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 53–68.
577–593. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2010.01.014. Petursdottir, A. I., & Haflidadottir, R. D. (2009). A com-
Matson, J. L., & Smith, K. R. (2008). Current sta- parison of four strategies for teaching a small foreign-­
tus of intensive behavioral interventions for young language vocabulary. Journal of Applied Behavior
children with autism and PDD-NOS. Research in Analysis, 42, 685–690.
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 60–74. doi:10.1016/j. Petursdottir, A. I., Olafsdottir, A. R., & Aradottir, B.
rasd.2007.03.003. (2008). The effects of tact and listener training on
May, R. J., Hawkins, E., & Dymond, S. (2013). Brief the emergence of bidirectional intraverbal relations.
report: Effects of tact training on emergent intra- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41, 411–415.
verbal vocal responses in adolescents with autism. Pierce, K., Conant, D., Hazin, R., Stoner, R., & Desmond,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, J. (2011). Preference for geometric patterns early in
43, 996–1004. life as a risk factor for autism. Archives of General
McEachin, J. J., Smith, T., & Lovaas, O. I. (1993). Long-­ Psychiatry, 68, 101–109.
term outcome for children with autism who received Reed, P., Osborne, L. A., & Corness, M. (2007). Brief
early intensive behavioral treatment. American report: Relative effectiveness of different home-based
Journal of Mental Retardation, 97, 359–372. behavioral approaches to early teaching intervention.
McStay, R. L., Dissanayake, C., Scheeren, A., Koot, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,
H. M., & Begeer, S. (2013). Parenting stress and 1815–1821.
autism: The role of age, autism severity, qual- Reichow, B. (2012). Overview of meta-analyses on early
ity of life and problem behavior of children and intensive behavioral intervention for young children
adolescents with autism. Autism, 18, 502–510. with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
doi:10.1177/1362361313485163. Developmental Disorders, 42, 512–520.
Miguel, C. F., Petursdottir, A. I., & Carr, J. E. (2005). The Reichow, B., Barton, E., Assouline, B., & Hume, K.
effects of multiple-tact and listener-discrimination (2012). Early intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI)
training on the acquisition of intraverbal behavior. The for young children with autism spectrum disorders
Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 27–41. (ASD). Cochrane Database Systematic Review, 10,
Montes, G., & Halterman, J. S. (2007). Psychological CD009260.
functioning and coping among mothers of children Reichow, B., & Wolery, M. (2009). Comprehensive
with autism: A population-based study. Pediatrics, synthesis of early intensive behavioral interventions
119, 1040–1046. for young children with autism based on the UCLA
Mori, K., Toda, Y., Ito, H., Mori, T., Mori, K., Goji, young autism project model. Journal of Autism and
A., … Kagami, S. (2015). Neuroimaging in autism Developmental Disorders, 39, 23–41.
spectrum disorders :1H-MRS and NIRS study. The Remington, B., Hastings, R. P., Kovshoff, H., Espinosa,
Journal of Medical Investigation, 62, 29–36. http:// F., Jahr, E., Brown, T., et al. (2007). Early intensive
doi.org/10.2152/jmi.62.29. behavioral intervention: Outcomes for children with
Novak, G., & Pelaez, M. (2004). Child and adolescent autism and their parents after two years. American
development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Journal on Mental Retardation, 112, 418–438.
Pérez-González, L. A., García-Asenjo, L., Williams, G., Rivard, M., Terroux, A., Parent-Boursier, C., & Mercier,
& Carnerero, J. J. (2007). Emergence of intraverbal C. (2014). Determinants of stress in parents of chil-
antonyms in children with pervasive developmental dren with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 1609–1620.
697–701. http://doi.org/10.1901/jaba.2007.697-701. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-2028-z.
Perez-Gonzalez, L. A., Garcia-Asenjo, L., Williams, G., Rogers, S. J., & Vismara, L. A. (2008). Evidence-based
& Carnerero, J. J. (2007). Emergence of intraverbal comprehensive treatments for early autism. Journal of
antonyms in children with pervasive developmental Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 37, 8–38.
disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 40, Sallows, G. O., & Graupner, T. D. (2005). Intensive
697–701. behavioral treatment for children with autism: Four-­
Perez-Gonzalez, L. A., & Williams, G. (2002). Multi year outcome and predictors. American Journal on
component procedure to teach conditional discrimi- Mental Retardation, 110, 417–438.
nations to children with autism. American Journal on Sarokoff, R. A., Taylor, B. A., & Paulson, C. L. (2001).
Mental Retardation, 4, 293–301. Teaching children with autism to engage in conver-
13  Additional Treatment Variables 229

sational exchanges: Script fading with embedded tex- pervasive developmental disorder. American Journal
tual stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, on Mental Retardation, 105, 269–285.
81–84. Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2015). Evidence base update for
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1986). The autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical Child
Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS): For diagnos- Adolescence Psychology, 44, 897–922. doi:10.1080/1
tic screening and classification of autism. New York, 5374416.2015.1077448.
NY: Irvington. Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2016). Evidence base update for
Schreibman, L. (1975). Effects of within-stimulus and autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Child
extra-stimulus prompting on discrimination learn- & Adolecent Psychology, 44, 897–922.
ing in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Smith, T., Klorman, R., & Mruzek, D. W. J. (2015).
Analysis, 8, 91–112. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1901/ Predicting outcome of community-based early
jaba.1975.8-91. intensive behavioral intervention for children with
Schreibman, L., Dufek, S., & Cunningham, A. B. autism. Abnormal Child Psychology, 43, 1271–1282.
(2011). Identifying moderators of treatment out- doi:10.1007/s10802-015-0002-2.
come for children with autism. In J. L. Matson Strain, P. S., & Bovey, E. H. (2011). Randomized con-
& P. Sturmey (Eds.), International handbook trol trial of the LEAP model of early intervention for
of autism and pervasive developmental disor- young children with autism spectrum disorders. Topics
ders (pp. 295–305). New York, NJ: Springer. in Early Childhood Special Education, 31, 133–154.
doi:10.1007/978-1-4419-8065-6_18. doi:10.1177/0271121411408740.
Schreibman, L., Stahmer, A. C., Barlett, V. C., & Dufek, Strauss, K., Vicari, S., Valeri, G., D'Elia, L., Arima, S.,
S. (2009). Brief report: Toward refinement of a pre- & Fava, L. (2012). Parent inclusion in early intensive
dictive behavioral profile for treatment outcome in behavioral intervention: The influence of parental
children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum stress, parent treatment fidelity and parent-mediated
Disorders, 3, 163–172. generalization of behavior targets on child outcomes.
Secan, K. E., Egel, A. L., & Tilley, C. S. (1989). Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33, 688–703.
Acquisition, generalization, and maintenance of doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.11.008.
question-­answering skills in autistic children. Journal Sundberg, M. L., & Sundberg, C. A. (2011). Intraverbal
of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 181–196. behavior and verbal conditional discriminations
Sherer, M. R., & Schreibman, L. (2005). Individual in typically developing children and children with
behavioral profiles and predictors of treatment autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 27, 23–43.
effectiveness for children with autism. Journal of Taylor-Santa, C., Sidener, T. M., Carr, J. E., & Reeve,
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 525–538. K. F. (2014). A discrimination training procedure to
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.3.525. establish conditioned reinforcers for children with
Shine, R., & Perry, A. (2010). Brief report: The relation- autism. Behavioral Interventions, 29, 157–176.
ship between parental stress and intervention outcome doi:10.1002/bin.1384.
of children with autism. Journal on Developmental Thiemann, K. S., & Goldstein, H. (2001). Social stories,
Disabilities, 16, 64–66. written text cues, and video feedback: Effects on social
Silva, L. M. T., & Schalock, M. (2012). Autism parenting communication of children with autism. Journal of
stress index: Initial psychometric evidence. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 34, 425–446.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42, 566–574. Vedora, J., Meunier, L., & Mackay, H. (2009). Teaching
doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1274-1. intraverbal behavior to children with autism: A com-
Smeets, P. M., & Striefel, S. (1994). A revised blocked-­ parison of textual and echoic prompts. The Analysis of
trial procedure for establishing arbitrary match- Verbal Behavior, 25, 79–86.
ing in children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Vivanti, G., Dissanayake, C., Zierhut, C., & Rogers, S. J.
Psychology, 47, 241–261. (2012). Brief report: Predictors of outcomes in the
Smith, D. P., Eikeseth, S., Fletcher, S. E., Montebelli, L., early start Denver model delivered in a group setting.
Smith, H. R., & Taylor, J. C. (2016). Emergent intra- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43,
verbal forms may occur as a result of listener training 1717–1724. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1705-7.
for children with autism. Analysis of Verbal Behavior, Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-
32, 27–37. doi:10.1007/s40616-016-0057-3. Feig, J. H., Glasser, A., & Veenstra-VanderWeele,
Smith, T. (2001). Discrete trial training in the treatment J. (2011). A systematic review of early inten-
of autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental sive intervention for autism spectrum disor-
Disabilities, 16, 86–92. ders. Pediatrics, 127, e1303–e1311. http://doi.
Smith, T., Eikeseth, S., Klevstrand, M., & Lovaas, O. I. org/10.1542/peds.2011-0426.
(1997). Intensive behavioral treatment for preschool- Watkins, C. L., Pack-Teixeira, L., & Howard, J. S. (1989).
ers with severe mental retardation and pervasive Teaching intraverbal behavior to severely retarded
developmental disorder. American Journal on Mental children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 7, 69–81.
Retardation, 102, 238–249. Weiss, M. J. (1999). Differential rates of skill acquisition
Smith, T., Groen, A. D., & Wynn, J. W. (2000). Randomized and outcomes of early intensive behavioral interven-
trial of intensive early intervention for children with tion for autism. Behavioral Interventions, 14, 3–22.
230 S. Eikeseth

Williams, G., Perez-Gonzalez, L. A., & Queiroz, A. B. M. Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 426–435.
(2005). Using a combined blocking procedure to teach doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.426.
colour discrimination to a participant with autism. Zablotsky, B., Bradshaw, C. P., & Stuart, E. A. (2013). The
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 555–558. association between mental health, stress, and cop-
Yoder, P., & Stone, W. L. (2006). Randomized com- ing supports in mothers of children with autism spec-
parison of two communication interventions for pre- trum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental
schoolers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Disorders, 43, 1380–1393.
Social Skills Training for Children
and Adolescents with Autism 14
Spectrum Disorder

Keith C. Radley, Roderick D. O’Handley,
and Christian V. Sabey

Children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD),


Introduction however, do not naturally acquire these skills.
Although the underlying cause of social deficits
Social skills may be defined as a set of verbal and in ASD is not yet well understood (Weiss, LaRue,
nonverbal behaviors that allow an interaction & Newcomer, 2009), researchers have identified
between two or more people to be mutually bene- common features of social deficits in individuals
ficial and reinforcing (Morgan & Jenson, 1988). with ASD.
Indeed, social skills may best be conceptualized
functionally as behaviors that maximize the poten-
tial for reinforcement during social interactions  ocial Deficits in Autism Spectrum
S
while simultaneously minimizing the possibility Disorder
of unfavorable responses (e.g., Elliott, Racine, &
Busse, 1995). Effective use of social skills not Social skills deficits represent a defining charac-
only allows an individual to contact favorable con- teristic of individuals with ASD, regardless of lan-
sequences but also allows an individual to be per- guage or cognitive abilities (Carter, Davis, Klin, &
ceived as similar to their peer group—more so Volkmar, 2005). The social impairments of indi-
than simply possessing appropriately developed viduals with ASD are diverse but can be placed
academic skills (Greenspan, 1980). within two broad social skills deficit categories,
Much like academic skills, social skills repre- which include verbal and nonverbal deficits.
sent learned behaviors (Elliott et al., 1995). For Verbal social skills deficits include, for example,
most individuals, acquisition of social skills does difficulty sharing conversations (Elder, Caterino,
not require explicit instruction. Instead, social Chao, Shacknai, & DeSimone, 2006), maintaining
skills are naturally acquired and shaped through conversations (Gutstein & Whitney, 2002), and
observation and interaction with others (e.g., poorly regulated speech prosody (Starr, Szatmari,
Whalon, Conroy, Martinez, & Werch, 2015). Bryson, & Zwaigenbaum, 2003). Among nonver-
bal social skills deficits, individuals with ASD
have difficulty recognizing facial expressions
K.C. Radley (*) • R.D. O’Handley
(Celani, Battacchi, & Arcidiacono, 1999), use
University of Southern Mississippi,
Hattiesburg, MS, USA poorly regulated eye contact (Pelios & Lund,
e-mail: keith.radley@usm.edu 2001; Senju & Johnson, 2009), and have difficulty
C.V. Sabey establishing joint attention (Willemsen-Swinkles,
Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, USA Buitelaar, Weijen, & van Engeland, 1998).

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 231


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_14
232 K.C. Radley et al.

Deficits of discrete verbal and/or nonverbal ability to use specific social skills, they may sim-
communicative behaviors make it difficult for ply have an inability to recognize environmental
individuals with ASD to engage in fluid recipro- cues that typically “trigger” when a particular
cal social interactions with others. Individuals social skill should be used (Bauminger, 2003;
with ASD are less likely to initiate interactions, Mesibov, 1984).
typically respond to fewer interactions from oth- Self-control skills deficits occur when the indi-
ers, and typically interact with others for less vidual has difficulty learning a target skill due to
time than their counterpart typically developing emotional arousal that blocks the acquisition of
peers (McConnell, 2002). As a result, individuals the target skill. Emotional arousal may be dem-
with ASD are at increased risk for social isola- onstrated by engagement of disruptive behaviors
tion, peer rejection (Chamberlain, 2001), and that are often incompatible with the target skill.
comorbid diagnoses of depression and anxiety Finally, self-control performance deficits are
(Bellini, Peters, Benner, & Hopf, 2007; Tantam, demonstrated when the individual has learned the
2003). The development of comorbid disorders target skill but is unable to reliably demonstrate
may be represented by coexisting disruptive the skill because of emotional arousal and the
behaviors such as aggression toward others, tan- individual’s inability to mitigate or override this
trums, or self-injurious behavior, each of which arousal. Gresham (1981) contributed an addi-
interfere with the natural development or strate- tional deficit category when he described fluency
gic teaching of competent social skills. In addi- deficits. Fluency deficits are demonstrated when
tion, it is unlikely that these deficits will remit an individual has the ability to demonstrate the
naturally. If left untreated, social skills deficits target skill and is motivated to do so but does so
are likely to persist into adolescence and may in an awkward fashion. In many cases, it may be
become more obvious as the individual is left to the practitioner to determine and define
required to enter increasingly complex social set- what constitutes fluent use of a social skill.
tings (Tantam, 2003). With the pervasiveness, persistence, and spe-
cific types of deficits in mind, researchers have
evaluated many approaches rooted in applied
Categories of Social Skills Deficits behavior analysis for promoting social skills
development. The mechanisms of action facili-
There are several reasons for which an individual tating the effects of social skill interventions
may not exhibit a particular social skill in a spe- typically include a combination of observation,
cific social setting. Gresham and Elliott (1987) modeling, and repeated trials for skill rehearsal,
outlined four categories of social skills deficits accompanied with positive and/or corrective
that impact an individual’s ability to learn and feedback (Elliott, Roach, & Beddow, 2008), but
demonstrate specific skills in generalized con- the extent to which each one of these interven-
texts. Skills deficits represent the absence of a tion components are emphasized during treat-
particular skill within the individual’s behavioral ment is based on the type of social skills deficit
repertoire. Performance deficits are evident when the individual presents with. There are many dif-
the individual does have the target skill within ferent available social skill interventions, which
their repertoire but does not reliably demonstrate is the most frequently utilized intervention
the skill when expected to in a social setting. approach for addressing verbal and nonverbal
Insufficient motivation to demonstrate a target social deficits associated with ASD (Goin-
skill is a common contributor to performance Kochel, Myers, & Mackintosh, 2007). Given the
deficits. However, deficiencies in social skills variety of social skill interventions available for
development or demonstration may not always implementation, it is imperative that specific
be a result of insufficient motivation or lack of intervention approaches are linked with accurate
social interest. Even when individuals have the and reliable assessment data.
14  Social Skills Training for Children 233

Assessment of Social Skills cit. One additional goal suggested is to (e) iden-
tify environmental conditions caregivers would
A number of assessment procedures may be used like for specific social skills to occur. It is useful
during the assessment of social skills deficits and to consider these goals throughout the assess-
any interfering behaviors. As with the assessment ment process.
of any behavioral deficit or excess, assessment It is not uncommon for interviewees to
methods range along a continuum of indirect to describe social skills deficits and interfering
direct processes that may be used conjointly or in behaviors in vague constructs. Interviewees may
isolation. Incorporating multiple assessment state, for instance, that “Tom does not have many
approaches may increase the likelihood of obtain- friends and he is disruptive.” Follow-up questions
ing data that is comprehensive, accurate, and rep- should yield precise information regarding the
resentative of the social skill problem and any dimensions of target behaviors, including their
interfering behaviors. The utility of the assess- frequency, duration, and/or intensity. Example
ment process is demonstrated to the extent that questions include: “What do conversations with
the selected assessment tools assist in measuring Tom and his peers look like; what specific behav-
important behavioral parameters that are subse- iors is Tom demonstrating that makes you call
quently linked to a corresponding evidence-based him ‘disruptive;’ how often do these behaviors
treatment. In addition, these measures may also occur; how long do these behaviors last; does
be useful as baseline data and methods for prog- Tom have few friends because he can’t (skill defi-
ress monitoring throughout treatment. Each of cit) have positive peer interactions or because he
these approaches has advantages and disadvan- won’t (performance deficit) have positive peer
tages inherent to their methodology and with interactions?”
respect to their appropriateness and feasibility for Once specific behavioral parameters are
a particular setting. Practitioners are encouraged defined, questions are designed to evoke answers
to consider and become familiar with multiple related to relevant antecedent and consequent
assessment techniques, their validity and reliabil- variables associated with the social skills deficit
ity, as well as the available empirical support. and any interfering behaviors. In general, ques-
tions should be asked that will evoke answers
Indirect Assessment  Assessment of social related to when and where specific behaviors
skills and interfering behaviors may begin with occur, rather than why the interviewee thinks a
indirect assessment. Indirect measures are indi- behavior does or does not occur. Asking “why”
cated by the degree of behavioral inferences that implies that the interviewee knows why the
are made with the data collected. The specific behavior does or does not occur and provides a
behaviors of interest are not directly observed; mentalistic explanation for the behavior, which
rather, data regarding the behaviors of interest are may offer little value when developing a treat-
collected via parent, teacher, or self-report behav- ment protocol and is inconsistent with dimen-
ioral interviews and behavior rating scales. sions of applied behavior analysis (Baer, Wolf,
& Risley, 1968). Collecting contextual informa-
Behavioral Interviews  Conducting behavioral tion is useful for determining whether salient
interviews is often an important initial step dur- cues or consistent reinforcement for appropriate
ing the assessment of social skills. Gresham social behavior exists within the individual’s
(1998) outlined four primary goals of the inter- environment. It is also useful for identifying
view process: (a) identify and define social skills stimuli in the environment that are possibly
deficits, (b) identify and define any interfering evoking and maintaining competing behaviors.
behaviors, (c) identify and define environmental Again, specific questions are asked to solicit this
conditions in which interfering behaviors may information, which can later be corroborated
occur, and (d) determine whether the individual with data collected from subsequent assessment
is exhibiting a skill, performance, or fluency defi- procedures.
234 K.C. Radley et al.

Questions regarding specific behavioral to determine whether these rating scales are sen-
parameters and where and when these behaviors sitive enough to detect improvement of social
occur may be asked of the target individual. Even skills following treatment.
if the individual does not have good insight The Social Skills Rating System (SSRS;
regarding environmental variables associated Gresham & Elliott, 1990) was initially developed
with their specific social skills deficits, a direct to measure the presence of social behavior diffi-
interview may provide useful information if the culties within typically developing individuals
behavior of interest is the individual’s conversa- with disruptive behavior problems and has been
tional abilities. Interviewing the target individual the most commonly cited measure for assessing
may also help determine whether they are capa- social skills and evaluating treatment outcomes
ble to later complete pertinent rating scales or for individuals with ASD (White, Keonig, &
whether they are able to later collect self-­ Scahill, 2007). Unfortunately, many studies that
monitoring behavioral data regarding specific included the SSRS failed to document improve-
behaviors of interest. ment of social skills with intervention, either as a
Regardless of whether the target individual result of ineffective treatment or because the
can be interviewed, there is typically a need to SSRS could not detect small changes as a result
collect information from the individual’s caregiv- of intervention (White et al., 2007). The revised
ers. Caregivers may include teachers and parents version of the SSRS, the Social Skills
or any individual with close familiarity with the Improvement System (SSIS, Gresham & Elliott,
individual and their environment. In addition to 2008), now includes a 15-item Autism Spectrum
collecting contextual information, interviewing Scale used to assess for behavior problems and
significant others has the benefit of assisting the social skills associated with ASD, but further evi-
practitioner determine whether caregivers have dence examining the predictive and discriminant
some control over the individual’s environment validity of the Autism Scale is needed (Gresham,
to help facilitate the emergence of target skills in Elliott, Vance, & Cook, 2011). Still, the broader
their natural setting. In addition, the practitioner benefit of the SSIS is that teacher, parental, and
may begin to gauge the caregiver’s motivation for self-report versions of the SSIS are available for
improving the behavior of interest and begin to dissemination to provide an initial estimate of the
hypothesize the degree to which caregivers may frequency and importance of social skills across a
be willing to implement any and all behavioral variety of situations and across informants.
recommendations as an extension of social skills Two additional rating scales that may be used
training. Determining who has relative control to identify specific social skills deficits include
over the individual’s environment and determin- the Autism Social Skills Profile (ASSP; Bellini &
ing to what extent they are willing to implement Hopf, 2007) and the Emotion Regulation and
suggested interventions is useful information Social Skills Questionnaire (ERSSQ; Beaumont
when programming for skill generalization. & Sofronoff, 2008). Both the ASSP and the
ERSSQ were designed to measure the presence
Rating Scales  Rating scales can be used in com- of unique social skills deficits by developing
bination with behavioral interviews. Behavioral norm-referenced scores from a population of
rating scales represent an efficient way for mea- individuals with ASD, specifically. Most other
suring parameters of social skills deficits, their behavioral rating scales, including the SSIS, were
relative importance, and the existence of any designed and norm-referenced for use with a
competing behaviors. Rating scales may be broader population of individuals, perhaps limit-
administered and scored prior to conducting ing their ability to detect small changes during
behavioral interviews to help guide the types of and after treatment. That norms were created for
questions asked during interview. A number of the ASSP and ERSSQ with individuals with ASD
rating scales have been used to link with social limits their use for diagnostic classification, but
skills training, but additional research is needed the specificity in which questions are asked
14  Social Skills Training for Children 235

makes them particularly useful for linking assess- frequency count include the number of times an
ment with intervention. Few studies have cap- individual initiates social interactions with peers
tured positive treatment outcomes in response to or the number of times an individual says “thank
social skills training using the ASSP (e.g., you” or “good morning.” Frequency counts can
O’Handley, Ford, Radley, Hilbig, & Wimberly, also be combined with temporal parameters to
2016) and the ERSSQ, indicating a need for fur- derive a rate of behavior, such as the number of
ther research in this regard. times an individual engages in aggressive behav-
ior (e.g., hits/kicks other) during predetermined
Direct Assessment  Assessment of social skills sets of time. Other social skills may be measured
and interfering behaviors should include direct based on their temporal extent, or the duration in
assessment. Direct assessment includes a number which the behavior occurs. It may be that the fre-
of naturalistic observation procedures that argu- quency of social initiations is less relevant than
ably provide the most valid assessment of the the amount of time an individual engages during
behaviors of interest because relatively little social interaction. It is possible to measure the
inference is made regarding their occurrence or total duration of all social interactions or, incor-
absence, especially in comparison to indirect porating the dimension of frequency, measure the
methods of assessment. Goals during direct duration of interactions per occurrence.
assessment are similar to those during behavioral Finally, temporal locus refers to measurement
interview: (a) and (b) confirm and define social of when a particular behavior occurs with regard
skills and interfering behaviors, (c) and (d) iden- to other relevant events in the environment.
tify contextual variables associated with social Measurement of response latency and interre-
skills and interfering behaviors, and (e) differen- sponse time are two ways to measure temporal
tiate between the type of deficit exhibited. One locus. Response latency describes the time
additional goal is to (f) determine which dimen- between the presentation of a stimulus and the
sion of behavior represents the most socially beginning of a subsequent response. One exam-
valid dimension of behavior to measure. ple is measuring how long it takes an individual
Rating scale data provide a general descrip- to respond after being asked a question.
tion of the behaviors of concern, and information Interresponse time is the moment in time a behav-
from the behavioral interview help to formulate ior occurs with regard to the occurrence of the
sound operational definitions of specific social previous same response. This may include mea-
skills deficits and interfering behaviors. surement of the time in between instances of
Collecting valid naturalistic observation data aggressive behaviors.
begins with the development of accurate opera- A number of direct observational procedures
tional definitions. Sound operational definitions are available for providing quantitative data
are objective, inclusive, exclusive, and clearly regarding each dimension of behavior and associ-
describe the dimension of behavior being mea- ated contextual variables. Readers are referred to
sured. Social skills and competing behaviors can Cooper et al. (2007) for a thorough description of
each be measured along dimensions of frequency, each of these observation methods.
temporal extent, or temporal locus (Cooper,
Heron, & Heward, 2007). Which dimension of Direct Observational Recording Procedures 
behavior is measured will be based on the type of Two methods of direct observation that include
social skills deficit and interfering behavior of systematic approaches for the collection of quan-
interest. titative behavioral data include event recording
Frequency refers to the number of times a and time sampling. Event recording is used when
behavior may occur across time. Frequency observers are able to record the occurrence of a
counts may be used to monitor behaviors that specific behavior. Event recordings are useful
have easily defined beginnings and endings. when behaviors occur at relatively low rates and
Social skills that can be measured using a simple when measuring the frequency dimension of
236 K.C. Radley et al.

behavior. These data can be converted into momentary time samples of increasing intervals
rates or percentages. Calculating frequency- or to total duration of social engagement.
duration-­based percentages or rates of behavior Momentary time sampling yielded data that most
has the benefit of transforming direct observation closely approximated total duration of social
data into easily understood proportional parame- engagement, whereas partial interval time sam-
ters that can be used to compare data across ses- pling consistently overestimated duration of
sions even if the amount of time differs across social engagement. Treatment interpretations
observation sessions. from an expert panel were significantly impacted
Although continuous recording methods can by the manner in which data were collected.
be used to record any dimension of target behav- Current research suggests that practitioners use
iors, their use in research and applied settings is momentary time sampling when measuring
associated with logistical and measurement social engagement or duration-based behaviors.
challenges such as observer drift or observer
fatigue (Gardenier, MacDonald, & Green, 2004). Selecting Ecologically Valid Social
Time-­sampling observation procedures may be Behaviors  It is common for caregivers to report
used when it is difficult to collect continuous multiple social skills deficits and/or multiple
observation data. Disparate time-sampling pro- competing behaviors. In order to determine
cedures include whole interval, partial interval, which of these is the most ecologically valid
and momentary time sampling. Each procedure behavior to measure, it is useful to consider
requires that observations are divided into inter- whether they each belong to similar response
vals, with intervals marked to note the occur- classes and are either similar in form (topo-
rence or absence of target behaviors. Intervals graphical response class) or are governed by the
are marked using whole interval time sampling same environmental variables (functional
when a specific behavior is demonstrated or response class). Rather than intervening with
absent for the duration of the interval. Intervals multiple social skills deficits, deficits similar in
are marked using partial interval time sampling form (e.g., avoiding peers, poor eye contact)
when a target behavior occurs at any point dur- may be combined when developing the opera-
ing an interval. Finally, intervals are marked tional definition of a broader skills deficit (e.g.,
using momentary time sampling when a target social engagement).
behavior occurs only during the moment in Selecting a pivotal behavior, or a behavior
which an interval begins. that once learned, yields concurrent improve-
Time-sampling data are presented as the per- ments in other previously unlearned or untrained
centage of interval occurrence, dividing the num- behaviors that may represent behaviors warrant-
ber of times an interval was marked by the total ing priority during intervention. Poor eye contact,
number of intervals. Importantly, the accuracy of for instance, represents a foundational behavior
the behavior sample yielded will vary as a func- of individuals with ASD, which may inhibit other
tion of the time-sampling method used (Powell, social skills from developing (Donnelly, Luyben,
Martindale, Kulp, Martindale, & Bauman, 1977; & Zan, 2009). Finally, in addition to consider-
Radley, O’Handley, & Labrot, 2015). Whole ation of response classes and pivotal behaviors,
interval time sampling typically underestimates which social skill to first intervene will also
the presence of a behavior, partial interval time depend on the extent to which caregivers report it
sampling tends to overestimate the presence of a as being problematic. Given the nature of social
behavior, and momentary time sampling typi- skills deficits, that being that they occur infre-
cally overestimates and underestimates the pres- quently, it is not unusual for caregivers to first
ence of a behavior, though to a much lesser report interfering behaviors.
degree. For instance, Radley et al. (2015) The occurrence of these behaviors may pre-
recorded the duration of social engagement of vent the development of social skills (self-control
children with ASD, comparing partial and skills deficit) or the ability to demonstrate the
14  Social Skills Training for Children 237

social skill even when learned (self-control per- Interventions


formance deficit). For these reasons, it is some-
times warranted to first intervene with interfering As deficits in social skills represent a core deficit
behaviors. Doing so may require sound func- of ASD, substantial research has been devoted to
tional behavioral assessment, which is done to a improving social functioning of individuals with
degree using the behavioral interview and ASD. Despite this, research has often found
employment of rating scales and direct observa- social skills training to produce mixed interven-
tion. To assist with assessing contextual vari- tion effects. For example, Bellini, Peters, Benner,
ables, conditional probability analysis may be and Hopf (2007) found school-based intervention
used in conjunction with any of the aforemen- to result in only minimal improvements in social
tioned time-sampling procedures. Conditional skills—particularly in non-training settings.
probability analysis allows observers to gauge Bellini and colleagues noted that social skills
the likelihood that a behavior will occur in close training efficacy is further reduced when training
temporal proximity to an environmental variable is completed in pullout groups, a frequent train-
or to other behaviors of interest. A proportion of ing setting (e.g., Gresham, Sugai, & Horner,
a behavior that were preceded by a particular 2001). Despite the mixed state of much of the
antecedent and followed by a particular conse- social skills training literature, several interven-
quence is calculated. One limitation worth not- tion strategies have emerged as empirically sup-
ing, however, is that behaviors maintained on thin ported strategies for promoting the acquisition,
intermittent schedules of reinforcement may not generalization, and maintenance of social skills.
be captured.
When the collective assessment data do not Behavioral Skills Training  Behavioral skills
clearly identify a functional response class, a training (BST) is a multicomponent, active learn-
functional analysis may be needed. Functional ing approach to teaching new skills. It is different
analysis is an assessment procedure in which from passive information-giving approaches in
antecedent and consequent variables in the indi- that the training requires the learner to emit the
vidual’s natural environment are arranged so behaviors required to perform the new skill rather
that their relative effects on the interfering than just listening to or reading about the behav-
behavior can be measured systematically iors required to perform the new skill (Himle,
(Cooper et al., 2007). Functional analyses are Miltenberger, Gatheridge, & Flessner, 2004).
used to test whether the individual is engaging Behavioral skills training includes four core
in the behavior to obtain a stimulus (e.g., atten- components: instructions, modeling, rehearsal,
tion and/or tangibles) and/or to avoid a stimulus and feedback (Miltenberger, 2015). Although
(e.g., avoid interactions, avoid task demands). these components have been used in skills train-
The results of the functional analysis can be ing interventions dating back to the 1970s,
used to create an intervention designed to Breidenbach (1984) first described the combina-
weaken the relationship between the behavior tion of the four primary components as behav-
and the identified function, while simultane- ioral skills training.
ously strengthening the relationship between The instructions component consists of
the identified function and a more adaptive describing the discrete behaviors that are required
behavior (i.e., social skills). to perform the skill and the conditions under
Once behaviors have been operationalized, which the behavior is meant to occur. The instruc-
contextual variables have been identified, types tions need to be age and ability appropriate as
of skills deficits have been differentiated, and well as appropriate for the culture and context of
behaviors of interest are determined to be eco- the learner. For example, when teaching a pre-
logically valid; assessment data may be linked school child to introduce himself to other chil-
with intervention. dren, teaching the child to shake hands with his
238 K.C. Radley et al.

peers may be inappropriate in that preschool prompt for future attempts at the skill. As with
­students don’t shake hands when they introduce all feedback, it should be immediate, focused on
themselves. The instructions may be presented the behavior, and address one aspect needing
vocally or in writing. It may be helpful to have correction at a time. In some cases researchers
the learner repeat the instructions to ensure that have added in situ training, which consists of
she heard them correctly (Miltenberger, 2015). exposing the leaner to a realistic situation that
Modeling consists of demonstrating the requires the newly trained skill and providing
behaviors described in the instructions for the feedback based on their performance in that situ-
learner. Miltenberger (2015) described some rec- ation (Miltenberger et al., 2004).
ommendations for maximizing the effects of the Researchers have demonstrated the effective-
model including (a) selecting models that are ness of BST with developing children and adults
similar to the learner or otherwise credible based for training a wide variety of skills. For example,
on status, (b) modeling the skill in the context(s) Miltenberger et al. (2004) used BST to train chil-
where the behavior will be required, (c) modeling dren to safely respond to finding a gun, and
how the behavior results in access to a reinforcer, Johnson et al. (2005) used BST to train children
and (d) modeling the skill as many times as in abduction-prevention skills. Additionally,
required for the learner to acquire the new skill. researchers have used BST to train staff to imple-
The model can take a variety of forms including ment effective instructional techniques (e.g.,
live in-person modeling (Miles & Wilder, 2009), discrete-­trail teaching and Picture Exchange
video modeling (Poche, Yoder, & Miltenberger, System) for individuals with disabilities (Rosales,
1988), story format (Wurtele, 1990), or comput- Stone, & Rehfeldt, 2009; Sarokoff & Sturmey,
erized models (Vanselow & Hanley, 2014). 2004, 2008). Although BST has been demon-
Whatever form the modeling takes, the learner strated to be an effective instructional approach
needs to have the skill demonstrated for her as a for teaching new skills to typically developing
second level of exposure to the skill. individuals, the research on its effectiveness
The rehearsal portion of BST consists of hav- when applied to individuals with ASD has been
ing the learner produce the skill either vocally slower to develop.
(i.e., vocally recounting the steps) or physically In a recent review of BST and the teaching
acting out the behaviors that constitute the skill. interaction procedure, Leaf et al. (2015) identi-
Rehearsal serves at least two important functions. fied six studies in which researchers used BST to
First, it gives the interventionist an opportunity to teach new skills to individuals with ASD. The
assess the learner’s knowledge and understand- studies ranged from 1988 to 2013 with the major-
ing of the skill. Second, it gives the learner oppor- ity of studies occurring after 2010. In three of the
tunities to practice the skill in a low-risk situation. six studies, researchers taught social skills to
There are a few factors that may increase the individuals with ASD (Kornacki, Ringdahl,
effectiveness of the rehearsal process including Sjostrom, & Nuernberger, 2013; Nuernberger,
(a) rehearsing the skill in the context where the Ringdahl, Vargo, Crumpecker, & Gunnarsson,
skill will be needed, (b) ensuring that the rehears- 2013; Taras, Matson, & Leary, 1988), and in all
als are successful, (c) reinforcing successful three studies, BST was identified as effective or
rehearsals, and (d) rehearsing the behavior until somewhat effective. An additional search revealed
the learner demonstrates fluency with the skill that there were three other studies not included in
(Miltenberger, 2015). Leaf’s review in which researchers used BST to
The final component in BST is feedback, teach social skills to individuals with ASD
which consists of informing the learner of what (McFee, 2010; Peters & Thompson, 2015;
he did correctly and what he did incorrectly. As Stewart, Carr, & LeBlanc, 2007). Although one
Miltenberger (2015) indicates, feedback serves study was a dissertation (McFee, 2010) and one
as a reinforcing consequence for appropriate was a data-based case study (Stewart et al., 2007),
demonstrations of the skill and an antecedent in all three studies, BST produced improved
14  Social Skills Training for Children 239

social skills among participants with ASD. Some self-modeling. Video modeling of others describes
of the specific skill that were taught in these stud- the process of allowing an individual to watch
ies include responding to listener signals, chang- known or unknown peers, or adults demonstrate
ing topics, initiating a social interaction, the target behavior without error (e.g., McCoy &
responding to social interaction, and responding Hermansen, 2007). Videos are filmed from a
to body language. third-person point of view. Although social learn-
The existing research supporting the use of ing theory describes models as more effective
BST to teach new skills to a broad range of indi- when they are similar to an observer (e.g.,
viduals, including those with disabilities, is Bandura, 1977), researchers have often used indi-
strong. However, the research supporting the use viduals of different ages and genders with positive
of BST to teach individuals with ASD new social results (e.g., Mason et al., 2013). Video models of
skills is emerging but very promising. This is an others are typically easier to create than other
area of social skills training that is ripe for further forms of video models as they require less editing
exploration both in terms of the various contexts and less prompting (Ganz, Earles-Vollrath, &
in which BST may be used to teach new social Cook, 2011). An example of video modeling of
skills to individuals with ASD (e.g., home, others is found in O’Handley, Radley, and
school, residential treatment, etc.) and also in Whipple (2015), who utilized a video model of
terms of the types of individuals with ASD that two adults engaged in conversation to promote
may respond well to BST as a social skills train- eye contact of high school-age individuals with
ing procedure (e.g., high-functioning autism, ASD. Following implementation of the video
Asperger’s syndrome). Additional research will modeling of other interventions, rapid improve-
be very helpful in terms of establishing the effec- ments in the duration of eye contact during con-
tiveness of BST as a social skills training inter- versations in both training and generalization
vention for individuals with ASD. settings were observed.
Point-of-view video modeling differs from
Video Modeling  Video modeling describes an video modeling of adults or peers in that the vid-
intervention procedure in which an individual eos are filmed from a first-person perspective. As
views a video depicting successful demonstra- such, the individual demonstrating the target skill
tion of a target behavior. The video modeling is not directly observed. Instead, the video depicts
literature often describes social learning theory, performance of the target social skill as if the
in which an individual learns through observa- individual were engaged in the skill (McCoy &
tion instead of direct contact with contingencies, Hermansen, 2007). Some have suggested that
as the theoretical basis for the intervention point-of-view modeling may be more effective
(Bandura, 1969). Video modeling has been uti- than other types of video modeling as videos
lized to address a wide range of behaviors in from a first-person perspective may be better able
individuals with ASD, from academic skills to highlight relevant stimuli (Tetreault & Lerman,
(e.g., Morlock, Reynolds, Fisher, & Comer, 2010). Despite the potential benefits of point-of-­
2015) to vocational skills (e.g., Alexander, Ayres, view video modeling, the modality is less rela-
Smith, Shepley, & Mataras, 2013). For individu- tively less researched than other forms of video
als with ASD, video modeling may be a particu- modeling (Lee, 2015). In the few examples of
larly appropriate intervention modality, as point-of-view modeling to teach social skills,
visually based intervention strategies counteract results have demonstrated improvements in skills
stimulus overselectivity by directing attention to such as functional play (Scheflen, Freeman, &
the most relevant stimuli within an environment Paparella, 2012) and eye contact (Tetreault &
(Shipley-Benamou, Lutzker, & Taubman, 2002). Lerman, 2010).
Video modeling interventions may be divided The final subtype of video modeling, video
into three primary subtypes: video modeling of self-modeling, describes the process of watch-
others, point-of-view video modeling, and video ing oneself engaging in the target social skill.
240 K.C. Radley et al.

Video self-modeling may further be divided into communication skills, and self-help skills.
two subtypes: positive self-review and feedfor- Additionally, the meta-analysis found video
ward (Hitchcock, Dowrick, & Prater, 2003). modeling of others and video self-modeling to be
Positive self-review describes video recording similarly effective. The positive effects of video
intact, uncoached examples of target behaviors. modeling have also been documented in addi-
In order to utilize positive self-review, an indi- tional meta-analyses and reviews, with authors
vidual must have the target skill in their reper- concluding that video modeling meets criteria for
toire. Feedforward is a type of video modeling an evidence-based practice (Reichow & Volkmar,
in which video segments of coached behaviors 2010; Wang, Cui, & Parrila, 2011; Wang &
are edited together to produce an example of the Spillane, 2009).
desired social skill. The final video appears to Despite the fact that substantial research sup-
show the individual performing the target skill ports the use of video modeling, it should be
flawlessly. For example, Bellini, Akullian, and noted that researchers have frequently evaluated
Hopf (2007) utilized a feedforward procedure to video modeling as part of intervention packages
increase social engagement of young children including other behavior modification strategies
with ASD by prompting participants to interact (e.g., reinforcement, self-monitoring; Reichow &
and then editing recordings to remove facilita- Volkmar, 2010). Although not limited to inter-
tor-delivered prompts. Although not exclusive ventions addressing social skills in individuals
to research in social skills training for individu- with ASD, Mason and colleagues (2013) found
als with ASD, meta-analyses have found both video modeling with reinforcement to be more
subtypes of video self-modeling to be similarly effective than video modeling alone. The addi-
effective (Mason et al., 2013). tion of other supplemental intervention strate-
Although several types of video modeling gies, however, was not found to be beneficial.
may be implemented, video modeling interven- Although these findings indicate that practitio-
tions generally have several features in common. ners should implement video modeling in con-
Edited videos should be approximately 3–5 min junction with reinforcement, the literature is still
long (Buggey, 2005), and several videos should lacking regarding the necessity of other elements
be created to depict skill use in a variety of con- that may be included in video models—such as
texts—contexts that are similar to the actual set- instruction (Kroeger, Schultz, & Newsom, 2007)
ting in which the individual will be expected to and voice-overs (O’Handley et al., 2015).
use the skill. Although the literature differs on the In addition to considering elements that should
frequency with which video models should be be included in video modeling interventions,
shown, it has been recommended that the video practitioners must also consider participant vari-
should be viewed in a consistent setting at a time ables that may impact the success of the interven-
immediately before the skill is expected to be uti- tion. The ability to attend to a video has been
lized (Sigafoos, O’Reilly, & de la Cruz, 2007). suggested as a necessary but insufficient skill
Additionally, providing opportunities to practice (Macdonald, Dickson, Martineau, & Ahearn,
the skill following viewing may be beneficial 2016; McCoy & Hermansen, 2007), with shorter
(Ganz et al., 2011). videos potentially resulting in positive effects on
In general, video modeling has been found to attending (Tereshko, MacDonald, & Ahearn,
be effective in addressing social deficits of indi- 2010). Other skills that may be necessary include
viduals with ASD. For example, a review of 22 delayed matching and imitation skills (Macdonald
studies found video modeling to be effective for et al., 2016).
teaching a variety of play-related behaviors
(Fragale, 2014). In a meta-analysis of 23 video Social Narratives  Although known by various
modeling interventions, Bellini and Akullian terms within the literature, such as Social Stories
(2007) found video modeling to be an effective (e.g., Pane, Sidener, Vladescu, & Nirgudkar,
intervention strategy for addressing social skills, 2015) or social scripts (e.g., Boutot, 2009), the
14  Social Skills Training for Children 241

terms describe an intervention procedure in results in a permanent product that may be utilized
which use of a target behavior is explained within repeatedly and, once developed, may be imple-
the context of a story. Social narratives are typi- mented with minimal adult supervision—with
cally written from the perspective of the individ- both factors functioning to increase the social
ual and take place within a setting that the validity of the procedure. Taken together, these
individual is likely to encounter and describe elements have resulted in social narratives being
antecedents, the target behavior, and conse- implemented with frequency in applied settings
quences of the performance of the behavior. (Weiss et al., 2009).
Power Cards are a related intervention strategy Despite the popularity of social narrative
that differs in that stories are written to describe a interventions, research is somewhat mixed
special interest character in a situation that regarding the efficacy of the intervention. Several
requires use of the target skill (e.g., Daubert, studies have found social narratives to be benefi-
Hornstein, & Tincani, 2015). As individuals with cial in promoting acquisition of discrete social
ASD may benefit from visual supports, social nar- skills in children with ASD (e.g., Scattone, 2008)
ratives are often supplemented with pictures and and decreasing disruptive social behaviors (e.g.,
may be followed by comprehension questions Ozdemir, 2008). However, other research has
(e.g., Scattone, Tingstrom, & Wilczynski, 2006). found social narratives to be ineffective or less
Construction of social narratives typically effective than other intervention strategies (e.g.,
involves several steps. Following identification O’Handley et al., 2015; Reichow & Sabornie,
and operationalization of the target behavior, a 2009). Meta-analyses have also produced mixed
functional analysis of the behavior may be con- results regarding the utility of social narratives.
ducted to better allow the interventionist to indi- For example, Wang and Spillane’s (2009) meta-­
vidualize the social story based on the function of analysis of 36 studies found social narratives to
the target behavior (Pane et al., 2015). Next, the meet criteria for an evidence-based practice. A
social narrative is constructed using descriptive, follow-up meta-analysis indicated questionable
directive, perspective, and affirmative sentences effects on the acquisition of social skills but
(Gray, 2000). Descriptive sentences are used to greater efficacy for decreasing disruptive social
define the context of and discriminative stimuli behaviors (Kokina & Kern, 2010). Findings of
for skill use. Directive sentences direct the indi- Kokina and Kern suggesting the ineffectiveness
vidual to perform the target social skill. of social narratives in promoting acquisition of
Perspective sentences describe the thoughts and skills and the relative efficacy in decreasing
feelings of other individuals in the environment. inappropriate behaviors were further replicated
Lastly, affirmative sentences are used to (Qi, Barton, Collier, Lin, & Montoya, 2015).
strengthen the meaning of the story; refer to com- Interestingly, Qi and colleagues found social sto-
monly held beliefs, rules, or norms; or reassure ries to meet What Works Clearinghouse criteria
the individual. Gray recommends approximately for an evidence-based practice when evaluating
two to five descriptive, perspective, or affirmative nonoverlap of data, despite the fact that visual
sentences for each directive sentence. analysis indicated that the procedure should not
Several benefits are associated with the be considered evidence-based.
implementation of a social narrative interven- Although the literature is mixed regarding the
tion. First, construction and facilitation of social utility of social narratives in addressing social
narratives do not require substantial training— skills in individuals with ASD, several findings
allowing the procedure to be implemented by a are relatively clear. In general, social narratives
variety of personnel (Crozier & Siieo, 2005). are best used to decrease inappropriate behaviors
Social narratives are also extremely flexible, (Qi et al., 2015). The addition of comprehension
capable of being written to address a range of checks and visual supports (e.g., illustrations)
behaviors in diverse settings. Like video modeling, appears to increase the efficacy of social narra-
the development of a social narrative intervention tive interventions (Kokina & Kern, 2010). Social
242 K.C. Radley et al.

narratives also appear to be better suited for behavior management (Cooper et al., 2007). This
addressing discrete behaviors rather than com- also serves to increase the independence of the
plex and abstract behavioral sequences. Lastly, individual with ASD, increasing overall quality
social narratives may be ineffective when imple- of life. Self-management interventions also ben-
mented in isolation and should therefore be uti- efit from portability across settings. Social skills
lized in the context of comprehensive social skills training decreases when intervention is imple-
intervention (Clark, Radley, & Phosaly, 2014). mented in pullout settings (Bellini, Peters,
Benner, & Hopf, 2007). As self-monitoring is
Self-Management  Self-management interven- self-mediated, the individual with ASD can eas-
tions increase independence of an individual due ily implement the intervention in a variety of set-
to the fact that the individual serves as his or her tings without modification (e.g., recess,
own intervention agent. These interventions often classroom, vocational setting), resulting in
involve combination of one or more of the fol- greater generalizability of effects (Cooper et al.,
lowing five components: self-observation, self-­ 2007). For school-age individuals with ASD, the
recording, self-evaluation, self-instruction, and portability of self-management interventions also
self-reinforcement (Lee, Simpson, & Shogren, serves to promote inclusion in less restrictive
2007). Self-observation involves training the settings.
individual to discriminate between the occur- When applied to address social skills in indi-
rence and nonoccurrence of a target behavior. viduals with ASD, self-management procedures
This often includes teaching the individual to have been found to result in increasing target
self-question (e.g., “Am I making eye contact?”). behaviors while decreasing inappropriate behav-
Self-observation is often preceded by a temporal iors. For example, a meta-analysis of self-­
(e.g., after lunch), tactile (e.g., vibration from management research with individuals with ASD
MotivAider), or auditory cue (e.g., teacher found the procedure to be effective in increasing
instruction; Clark et al., 2014) to signal that the the frequency of behaviors such as eye contact,
individual should determine whether they are social initiations, appropriate play, and reciprocal
engaged in the target behavior. The individual communication (Lee et al., 2007). Stereotypy,
then may self-record the occurrence or nonoccur- which negatively affects an individual’s ability to
rence of the target behavior using a data collec- interact with others, has been found to decrease
tion form or some other data collection device following introduction of self-management
(Lee et al., 2007). Self-evaluation refers to the (Koegel & Koegel, 1990). Additionally, self-­
process of determining progress toward a goal. management interventions have been found to be
For example, an individual may have a goal of effective in children as young as preschool (Lee
providing ten compliments to peers while at work et al., 2007) to adulthood (Southall & Gast, 2011).
or initiating three social interactions during a In general, the literature suggests that self-­
recess period. Self-instruction involves the use of management is an effective procedure for pro-
self-statements utilized to guide a target behav- moting social skills in individuals with ASD.
ior. Lastly, self-reinforcement is the contingent In utilizing self-management interventions in
delivery of a reward. This component is intended applied settings, practitioners should be aware
to increase the future frequency with which an that self-management procedures are best applied
individual will engage in a target behavior. to behaviors that are already in an individual’s
Utilization of self-management interventions repertoire (Southall & Gast, 2011). In the case
has several advantages over interventions medi- that an individual does not already possess some
ated by another individual (e.g., teacher, parent). form of the terminal behavior, explicit training in
As intervention implementation is primarily the behavior should be provided prior to use of
mediated by the individual with ASD, little over- self-management. Practitioners should also
sight from another individual is required—allow- consider the level of functioning of individuals
ing for greater time to engage in tasks other than with whom they are considering implementing
14  Social Skills Training for Children 243

self-­
monitoring. If the self-management proce- individuals with ASD. Peer-mediated interven-
dure to be implement involves accurate calcula- tions have several advantages to adult-mediated
tion or monitoring of the passage of time, it may interventions. First, if peers can serve as inter-
not be suitable for all individuals, and adapta- ventionists, then the number of available inter-
tions to the procedure should be considered (e.g., ventionists is greatly multiplied. In a context
Hume, Loftin, & Lantz, 2009). Lastly, although such as a school, where adult time is often
the literature varies regarding the exact self-­ stretched thin, peer-mediated interventions may
management components that should be included offer an efficient approach to providing interven-
in an intervention, reviews have identified com- tion in a way that does not strain already thin
ponents that seem integral. Self-observation, resources (Chan et al., 2009). Second, socially
self-recording, and self-reinforcement were competent peers may function as better models
included in all studies evaluated by Southall and of appropriate social behavior than adults because
Gast (2011) and as such should be considered as the behavior of the peer is not contrived or
core components when utilized by practitioners. mechanical, as that of an adult trying to model
the behavior of a child would be. Third, a peer
Peer-Mediated Intervention  Humans have can model the behavior in the context where the
relied on social communities for survival and behavior needs to happen and elicit natural
success dating back to before recorded history. responses from other children, where an adult
Given the lengthy common learning history that model may elicit unnatural response from peers.
humans share as social beings, it is clear that In other word, the accuracy of the model is
human behavior often comes under the control of improved when a peer does the modeling. Fourth,
the social behavior of peers. In modern history Strain, Odom, and McConnell (1984) point out
researchers have discovered that peer behavior that in order to have adequate social develop-
can be systematically leveraged to influence the ment, individuals, particularly those with certain
behavior of individuals who demonstrate social types of disabilities, require exposure to socially
deficits (Odom & Strain, 1984). For individuals capable peers who serve as models. Although
with ASD, social deficits make up one of the exposure to socially capable peers may be a nec-
defining characteristics of their disability essary component of adequate social develop-
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013). ment, for many individuals with ASD, exposure
Consequently, peer-mediated interventions are a alone is not sufficient to produce age- or grade-­
natural fit for improving the social skills of indi- appropriate social behavior (Laushey & Heflin,
viduals with ASD. In recent years there has been 2000). Consequently, something more systematic
a flurry of interest in peer-mediated interventions than just full inclusion needs to occur, and peer-­
for individuals with ASD. In 2015 alone at least mediated interventions are one effective way to
five reviews or meta-analyses have been pub- do something systematic. Finally, peer-mediated
lished addressing the use of peer-mediated inter- interventions address the reality that social inter-
ventions to support children and adolescents with actions are reciprocal. As such, it makes sense to
ASD (Boudreau, Corkum, Meko, & Smith, 2015; intervene on both the socially delayed child as
Cole, 2015; Watkins, Kuhn, Ledbetter-Cho, well as the socially competent child. Teaching
Gevarter, & O’Reilly, 2015; Watkins et al., 2014; socially competent peers to interact with children
Whalon et al., 2015). This level of interest sug- with ASD, even as part of in intervention, may
gests that researchers see peer-mediated inter- break down barriers that would otherwise inhibit
ventions as a promising approach for improving social interactions. Given these advantages, it
the social outcomes of individuals with ASD. makes sense that peer-mediated intervention has
Peer-mediated social skills intervention has received so much attention among researchers
become an important behavioral technology that and interventionists.
leverages the influence and ability of socially Peer-mediated interventions can take a vari-
capable peers to improve the social behavior of ety of forms. Odom and Strain (1984) reviewed
244 K.C. Radley et al.

the literature on peer-mediated approaches for reviewed only studies including students with
promoting social interaction and identified three ASD in inclusive school settings. Their review
broad classes of peer-mediated intervention. The indicated 12 of the 14 studies identified had either
first they call proximity, defined as having adequate or strong evidence supporting the use of
socially delayed children play with socially peer-mediated interventions. Additionally, they
competent peers who have had no special train- found that the most commonly used class of peer-­
ing. Interventionists may encourage the target mediated intervention (Odom & Strain, 1984)
child to play with their typically developing was peer initiation with more than 70% of studies
peers and even teach the target child how to play using this strategy. In a review of evidence-based
with the peers, but the peers receive no instruc- interventions for improving the social communi-
tion on facilitating social interactions with the cation of children with ASD, Watkins et al.
target child. The second class of intervention (2015) indicated that peer-mediated intervention
that Odom and Strain identify is prompt and meets criteria to be considered an evidence-based
reinforce. In this type of intervention, socially practice. Their review indicated that there are 15
competent children are trained to prompt the tar- single-case studies of sufficient quality to support
get children to engage in a social interaction and the use of peer-mediated strategies for improving
then to reinforce the target children by engaging the social behavior of students from preschool
in a preferred activity and providing praise or through high school. Camargo et al. (2014) cor-
other affirming statements. The third type of roborated this finding in a review of behaviorally
intervention that they identified was peer initia- based interventions for social skills instruction.
tion in which peers are trained to initiate social In their review, Camargo and colleagues found
interactions with target children. When the that among the 30 studies identified for inclusion,
socially competent peer initiates the social inter- the most common approach to treatment was
actions, the target child has opportunities to some form of peer-mediated intervention. They
respond appropriately and engage more socially also found that behaviorally based interventions,
appropriate behavior. In their review, Odom and including peer-mediated interventions, meet cri-
Strain concluded that prompt and reinforce and teria to be considered an evidence-based practice.
peer initiation were more effective approaches to The wealth of evidence supporting the use of
peer-­mediated intervention than proximity. More peer-mediated interventions across settings, ages,
recent reviews have addressed these types of researchers, and time makes it a clear case for
interventions as they apply specifically to chil- such interventions as a strong choice for improv-
dren with ASD. ing the social outcomes of children and adoles-
Whalon et al. (2015) conducted a review of cents with ASD.
single-case studies addressing school-based
social skills interventions for students with BLISS  For many children with ASD, the school
ASD. They identified 37 studies including 105 is a natural place to receive treatment for a social
children that met criteria for inclusion. Of these deficit. It is an obvious fit for social skills training
studies, six addressed peer-mediated interven- because so much of the time at school is spent in
tions, all of which produced improved social social interactions with peers and adults. Socially
skills among participants. In three of the studies competent peers are present bidding for interac-
the peers were trained to socially engage with the tion during class, recess, lunch, and even on the
participants, while in the remaining three the par- way to and from school. All of this exposure to
ticipants and socially competent peers were peers requires frequent social interactions and
trained together. The average effect size for these provides several naturally occurring opportuni-
interventions was NAP of .95 and Tau-U of .87, ties to practice social behaviors. However, it is
both indicating large effects on improving the the necessity of these very interactions that often
social behavior of participants. Watkins et al. makes school such a challenge for students with
(2014) conducted a similar review, but they ASD. Additionally, adults are present in most of
14  Social Skills Training for Children 245

these same contexts providing supervision, delivery, and (d) reinforcement and progress
instruction, and correction. The adults too can monitoring (Sabey, 2015). Before beginning the
present social challenges for students with ASD assessment, a teacher or principal needs to iden-
when the adults request and expect appropriate tify an interventionist. The only requirements for
social behavior in the form of good manners, aca- the interventionist are that the individual has the
demic responses, and following directions. flexibility to be available during the time the skill
Although it may seem that schools are set up to needs to be taught and can follow a simple lesson
magnify social deficits, they also provide the very plan. In one study, Ross and Sabey (2015) used
useful combination of socially competent peers student teachers as interventionists. In another
and adults trained to teach new skills. This com- study Sabey (2015) used a computer specialist, a
bination makes the school an ideal place to teach librarian, a speech pathologist, and a special edu-
new social skills. Yet, in spite of this ideal combi- cation teacher as interventionists. In each case
nation, some students with ASD do not receive the interventionist had time available (5–15 min)
the social support that they need in order to fully when the skill would be expected to happen.
benefit from their schooling in the same manner Once an interventionist is identified, then the
as their typically developing peers (Gutierrez, assessment process follows.
Hale, Gossens-Archuleta, & Sobrino-Sanchez, The assessment can be conducted using estab-
2007; Hunt & Goetz, 1997; Kohler, Strain, & lished rating scales such as the Social Skills
Shearer, 1996). Improvement System checklist (Gresham &
The reasons that student with ASD do not Elliott, 2008) and the School Social Behavior
receive adequate social skills support are varied Scales (Merrell, 1993) or by conducting direct
and context specific, but some common reasons observations of the target student’s behavior in
include the fact that many students with ASD are comparison to same-grade, same-sex peers. In
served in the general education setting either case, it is important to identify the specific
(Chakrabarti & Fombonne, 2001), and many deficits that the target student has in order to
teachers and staff may feel unprepared to deliver ensure that all social skills instruction is tightly
effective social skills instruction. Additionally, focused on the needs of the student. Focusing
providing social skills instruction to a small num- only on those needs identified by the assessment
ber of students can take time away from the larger can improve the efficiency of the social skills
group of students who require instruction. training by ensuring that staff does not waste time
Finally, taking time for social skills instruction teaching skills that the student does not need.
can take time away from the academic instruction Once the specific deficits have been identified,
of the student with ASD making academic suc- the interventionist can begin to develop lesson
cess even more challenging. plans. The lesson plans can be taken from an
To address these challenges, Ross and Sabey existing curriculum or developed from scratch.
(2015) have developed a flexible framework to Wherever the lesson plans come from, it is impor-
facilitate the delivery of effective social skills tant that they follow an effective teaching cycle
instruction within the school setting in a way that such as that used in behavioral skills training
does not detract from the learning of the student (Miltenberger, 2015). The lessons ought to be
with ASD or other students and that does not designed to offer the student several opportuni-
require an inordinate amount of staff time. The ties to practice the skill in the context where the
framework is called BLISS (brief, localized, skill will be needed. Within the BLISS frame-
intensive, social skills). This framework is flexi- work, lessons are designed to last for between 5
ble in that it can be implemented with a wide and 15 min and to be taught in the setting where
variety of curricula, in a variety of contexts, by a the skill is needed (e.g., playground, classroom,
variety of staff, to address a variety of social hallway, or cafeteria). Once the lesson plans are
skills. The framework consists of four steps, (a) developed, then the interventionist can begin to
assessment, (b) lesson development, (c) lesson deliver the instruction.
246 K.C. Radley et al.

It is in the lesson delivery phase that the reinforcement and socially competent peers
BLISS framework addresses many of the existing increases the likelihood that the student will
challenges with delivering social skills instruc- engage in the skill in the future and that the
tion in schools. The lessons are scheduled to socially competent peer will be involved.
begin when the student in transitioning into the In two studies, researchers used the BLISS
context where the skill will be required. For framework to improve the social skills of stu-
example, if the social skill will be needed during dents with ASD and typically developing student
lunch recess (e.g., joining a game), then the inter- at risk of engaging in serious problem behavior
ventionist meets the student on the way out to (Ross & Sabey, 2015; Sabey, 2015). In both stud-
recess either in a hall or office where there are ies participants made meaningful improvements
limited distractions. In this setting the interven- in their social skills and interventionists endorsed
tionist provides instruction by describing the skill the social validity of the intervention. Although
and modeling what the skill looks like. The inter- more research is needed to further establish the
ventionist then has the student demonstrate the effectiveness of the BLISS framework, it is a
skill and provides feedback until the student is promising approach for integrating effective
proficient. Then the student and interventionist social skills instruction in the school setting
transition to the area where the skill will be used where students with ASD need the additional
(e.g., playground), and the student practices the support.
skill again in increasingly natural scenarios (e.g.,
with socially competent student playing a game). Manualized Interventions  Although the proce-
Once the student demonstrates proficiency with dures previously discussed have received support
the skill in the target context, the interventionist as strategies for addressing social skills in indi-
gives the student an opportunity for independent viduals with ASD, these procedures are often not
practice by giving the student an assignment to implemented with frequency by practitioners
practice the newly acquired skill (e.g., join three (e.g., Hess, Morrier, Heflin, & Ivey, 2008;
different games today at recess). Because the les- Morrier, Hess, & Heflin, 2011). Factors such as
sons are short, they can be delivered daily or at limited resources and training in evidence-based
least several times a week, which may improve practices likely contribute to the underuse of
the impact of the intervention (Gresham et al., these procedures (Bellini & McConnell, 2010).
2001). In the case where a student does not reach Dingfelder and Mandell (2011) suggest that
proficiency within the 5–15-min lesson, the inter- many evidence-based practices have not been
ventionist gives the student an assignment and incorporated into training curricula or manuals
then picks up the lesson the next day where they that are easily utilized by practitioners, impeding
left off the day before. It is important that lessons the transition of research to practice. To over-
not take more than 15 min so that interventionists come this limitation, several intervention manu-
can deliver the lesson and be back to their other als have been developed that incorporate
duties without serious disruption. empirically validated social skills training strate-
After the lesson is delivered the intervention- gies for individuals with ASD.
ist can monitor progress by using either self-­
monitoring or some type of observation. To Program for the Education and Enrichment of
monitor progress, the student may be required to Relational Skills (PEERS)  The PEERS program
check off each time she successfully completes (Laugeson & Frankel, 2010) is a 16-lesson cur-
the skill, or a playground supervisor may con- riculum designed to address social skills of ado-
duct brief observations to ensure that the student lescents and young adults with ASD. Skills
is meeting the goal. When students meet their within the program build upon each other and are
goals, the interventionist gives them and a intended to be delivered in a set order. Lessons
socially competent peer of their choosing access from the PEERS program are intended to be
to preferred reinforcers. Pairing success with delivered daily, with each lesson being approxi-
14  Social Skills Training for Children 247

mately 30–60 min in duration. Behavioral skills the target skill is introduced by the animated
training makes up a core training strategy within superheroes. The animated characters provide
PEERS. viewers with three to five discrete steps for dem-
Each skill taught within the PEERS curricu- onstration of the target social skill. Individuals
lum is presented across five lessons. During the then view several video models of unknown, simi-
first lesson, skills from the previous unit are larly aged peers engaging in the target skill. The
reviewed. Day 2 is comprised of didactic instruc- group facilitator then provides an in vivo example
tion in the target skill and modeling by group and non-example of the skill and engages the
facilitators. Next, participants engage in behav- group members in behavioral skills training.
ioral rehearsal and receive feedback on target During behavioral skills training, participants are
skill usage. Days 4 and 5 consist of activities that provided with self-monitoring cards for recording
are designed to be enjoyable to the participants the frequency of accurate skills demonstration.
while requiring demonstration of the recently Participants then view an animated social narra-
taught skill. tive which describes use of the target skill and
The PEERS curriculum has been evaluated in play a game that requires the participants to utilize
multiple studies, with results indicating benefi- the skill. Finally, the group facilitator instructs
cial effects of the program on participants. In one participants to continue utilizing self-monitoring
of the earliest evaluations of the program, cards in generalized settings and provides a
researchers found participation in the program to reward for participating in the group.
result in improvements in parent rating of social Several studies have investigated the effect of
skills (Laugeson, Frankel, Mogil, & Dillon, the curriculum on social skills of children in school
2009). Concurrent increases in the frequency settings. For example Block, Radley, Jenson,
with which participants hosted get-togethers was Clark, and O’Neill (2015) utilized lessons from
also observed. Participation in the program has the program to address social skills in four ele-
also been found to result in improvements in mentary-age children with ASD. Implementation
areas related to social skills, such as mannerisms, of the program resulted substantial increases in
cooperation, social awareness, and social motiva- the number of social responses of participants
tion (Laugeson, Frankel, Gantman, Dillon, & during unstructured recess periods, and both par-
Mogil, 2012). Moreover, improvements in social ents and teachers reported improved social func-
behaviors due to participation in the PEERS pro- tioning following termination of the intervention.
gram have been found to be maintained following School-based implementation with elementary-
termination of the intervention (e.g., Laugeson, age children with ASD has also been found to
Gantman, Kapp, Orenski, & Ellingsen, 2015; result in improvements in parent report of social
Laugeson et al., 2012). functioning and sociometric ratings completed by
peers, further suggesting generalized effects of the
Superheroes Social Skills  Superheroes Social intervention (Radley, Ford, Battaglia, & McHugh,
Skills (Jenson et al., 2011) is an 18-lesson social 2014; Radley, McHugh, Taber, Battaglia, & Ford,
skills curriculum intended for elementary-age 2015). Improved accuracy in target social skills
individuals with ASD. The program utilizes ani- was also found following intervention with
mated superhero characters to present instruction preschool-­ age children with ASD (Radley,
in target skills. Additionally, the program incor- Hanglein, & Arak, 2016). Similarly, several stud-
porates behavioral skills training, video model- ies facilitated in clinic settings have found
ing, social narratives, inclusion of typically ­intervention to result in improved skill accuracy in
developing peers, and self-management strate- training and generalization settings (Radley et al.,
gies in order to promote acquisition and general- 2014, 2015).
ization of target skills.
Skills taught within the Superheroes Social Secret Agent Society  Previously known as the
Skills program all follow a similar format. First, Junior Detective Training Program, the Secret
248 K.C. Radley et al.

Agent Society program (Beaumont & Sofronoff, viable approach to social skills training.
2008) is a multimedia social skills training pro- Moreover, in two separate reviews, Gresham
gram for high-functioning children with ASD. et al. (2001) and Bellini, Peters, Benner, and
The program incorporates both group and com- Hopf (2007) indicated that social skills training
puter-based instruction and activities to promote often does not produce the generalization effects
acquisition of target social skills. Similar to the that would be so beneficial. These findings make
previously described manualized programs, the it clear that current practice in social skills train-
Secret Agent Society program incorporates mod- ing has not yet found a replicable way to help
eling and role-play. Additionally, the program children with ASD generalize newly trained
also places emphasis on mindfulness-based strat- skills to appropriate novel situations.
egies and relaxation training. In response to this dilemma, McIntosh and
Although fewer studies have evaluated the MacKay (2008) provide a series of recommenda-
Secret Agent Society program, extant research tions for increasing the likelihood that newly
indicates positive effects of participation in the trained skills will generalize. These recommen-
program. In an initial randomized trial of the dations are organized into steps to take prior to
intervention, significant improvements in both intervention, during intervention, and after inter-
parent and teacher ratings of social skills were vention. Prior to intervention, it is important to
noted (Beaumont & Sofronoff, 2008). In addi- identify all the target settings, instructors, and
tion, intervention effects were maintained at a people that are related to the new skill. Identifying
5-month follow-up. Further research has found all of these aspects of the target settings will
intervention to result in improved adaptive func- inform where, when, and with whom the instruc-
tioning and decreases in internalizing symptoms tion takes place in order to maximize the number
(Thomson, Burnham Riosa, & Weiss, 2015). The of relevant contextual stimuli that are present
Secret Agent Society has also been evaluated as a during training. This assessment also helps to
parent-delivered social skills program (Sofronoff, plan for the numbers of different exemplar set-
Silva, & Beaumont, 2015). Similar to clinician-­ tings that will need to be trained. During inter-
facilitated intervention, the parent-delivered vention, it is important to provide explicit
intervention resulted in improvements in social instruction, teach across a variety of relevant set-
behavior and child anxiety as reported by par- tings (e.g., locations, people, situations, etc.), and
ents. Results were also maintained over time. practice the skill enough for the child to develop
School-based implementation of the curriculum fluency. After intervention, the authors recom-
has been found to result in improved social func- mend providing ongoing coaching, incentivizing
tioning, with generalized improvements observed successful demonstrations of the skill, and reduc-
in home settings (Beaumont, Rotolone, & ing the effectiveness of competing behaviors.
Sofronoff, 2015). Finally, as with all good intervention efforts,
McIntosh and MacKay recommend measuring
the impact of social skills instruction, specifi-
Limitations and Future Directions cally in terms of generalization. To monitor
progress, they recommend four strategies: (a)
Programming and Assessing for Generali-­ directly observing the student in novel contexts,
zation  The ultimate goal of social skills training (b) using rating scales to get reports from a vari-
is generalization or the ability to teach a skill in ety of responders, (c) having the student self-
one setting and have it occur in other appropriate monitor his progress, and (d) asking peers to
settings in which the skill has not been explicitly report on the occurrence of the behavior in novel
trained. The need to explicitly teach a social skill settings. If social skills interventions to ulti-
in every possible situation where the skill would mately be worthwhile, interventionists must
be needed is far too cumbersome a task to be a deliberately and carefully plan for generalization
14  Social Skills Training for Children 249

and then assess the extent of their success. that the restricted range of social behaviors
Additionally, researchers need to spend more demonstrated by individuals with ASD directly
time and attention on promoting, measuring, and limits access to contingencies that maintain
reporting the generalization of intervention behavior in generalized settings, many programs
effects across settings. have focused on accuracy in skills demonstration
without explicitly focusing on accurate variabil-
Pullout Training  An issue that is related to gen- ity (e.g., Barry et al., 2003; Plavnick, Kaid, &
eralization is the use of a pullout social skills les- MacFarland, 2015). A focus on promotion of
sons in which the target child is removed from both variability and accuracy in skills demonstra-
the typical instructional setting to receive social tion is a relatively recent phenomenon, with a lit-
skills training. Gresham et al. (2001) point out erature review identifying only 14 studies
that this is a common practice when delivering targeting behavioral variability in individuals
social skills training. It is often the school psy- with ASD (Wolfe et al., 2014). In general, meth-
chologist, social worker, or speech pathologist ods found to be effective for promoting social
that pulls students out of class or some other skill variability include explicit instruction in tar-
inclusive setting in order to meet with other stu- get behavior topographies, introduction of con-
dents with social deficits to participate in small tingencies that support varied responding, and
group social skills instruction. This approach prompting variation in social skills demonstra-
compromises the effectiveness of the instruction tion. As social skills programming will only be
because it removes all of the relevant contextual judged to be effective when contrived contingen-
stimuli that may cue the student as to when to cies within the context of social skills training are
engage in the target behavior. It is most often the faded out and target social skills are maintained
case that the child with ASD does not need to by naturally occurring contingencies, it is essen-
work on social skills with the related service staff tial that both researchers and practitioners con-
or with other students with ASD. It is with typi- sider training for accurate variability as a
cally developing peers and general education component of social skills interventions.
staff that children with ASD need to learn to
interact with, and yet none of these people are
present during training. Effective social skills Summary
interventions must occur in the context where the
skill needs to happen. To that end, researchers Social skills deficits represent a core feature of
and publishers of social skills training programs ASD, which, if left untreated, result in negative
need to find ways to embed training into the natu- outcomes throughout the lifespan. Although
rally occurring context. Additionally, it will be social skills interventions are frequently imple-
important to consider how typically available mented, results of social skills training have often
individuals (e.g., parents, general education been found to be limited. Because of this, thor-
teachers, peers) can be recruited to deliver effec- ough assessment, utilization of empirically sup-
tive social skills training in an inclusive way. ported interventions, and ongoing progress
monitoring represent necessary components of
Variability of Behaviors  Restricted and repeti- social skills training. Despite the fact that several
tive behaviors represent a core feature of ASD interventions have been found to be effective,
(American Psychiatric Association, 2013). This future research is needed to identify essential
contributes to social behaviors that are often components of interventions, strategies capable
restricted in range (e.g., Leekam, Prior, & of being implemented with ease in generalized
Uljarevic, 2011; Wolfe, Slocum, & Kunnavatana, settings, interventions that produce generalized
2014), which in turn results in difficulty adapting behavior change, and techniques for enhancing
and generalizing social skills. Despite the fact accurate variability of social behaviors.
250 K.C. Radley et al.

References skills program in increasing social responses and


initiations of children with autism spectrum disor-
der. International Journal of School & Educational
Alexander, J. L., Ayres, K. M., Smith, K. A., Shepley,
Psychology, 3, 16–24.
S. B., & Mataras, T. K. (2013). Using video modeling
Boudreau, A. M., Corkum, P., Meko, K., & Smith, I. M.
on an iPad to teach generalized matching on a sort-
(2015). Peer-mediated pivotal response treatment
ing mail task to adolescents with autism. Research in
for young children with autism spectrum disorders:
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1346–1357.
A systematic review. Canadian Journal of School
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
Psychology, 30, 218–235.
and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-­5
Boutot, E. A. (2009). Using “I Will” cards and social
(5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric
coaches to improve social behaviors of students with
Association.
asperger syndrome. Intervention in School and Clinic,
Baer, D. M., Wolf, M. M., & Risley, T. R. (1968). Some
44, 276–281.
current dimensions of applied behavior analysis.
Breidenbach, D. C. (1984). Behavioral skills training
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 91–97.
for students: A preventive program. Social Work in
Bandura, A. (1969). Social learning of moral judgments.
Education, 6, 231–240.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 11,
Buggey, T. (2005). Video self-modeling applications with
275–279.
students with autism spectrum disorder in a small
Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood
private school setting. Focus on Autism and Other
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Developmental Disabilities, 20, 52–63.
Barry, T. D., Klinger, L. G., Lee, J. M., Palardy, N.,
Camargo, S., Rispoli, M., Ganz, J., Hong, E., Davis,
Gilmore, T., & Bodin, S. D. (2003). Examining the
H., & Mason, R. (2014). A review of the quality of
effectiveness of an outpatient clinic-based social
behaviorally-based intervention research to improve
skills group for high-functioning children with autism.
social interaction skills of children with ASD in inclu-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33,
sive settings. Journal of Autism and Developmental
685–701.
Disorders, 44, 2096–2116.
Bauminger, N. (2003). Peer interaction and loneliness
Carter, A., Davis, N., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005).
in high functioning children with autism. Journal of
Social development in autism. In F. Volkmar, A. Klin,
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 489–507.
R. Paul, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism and
Beaumont, R., & Sofronoff, K. (2008). A multi-­
pervasive developmental disorders (3rd ed.pp. 312–
component social skills intervention for children with
334). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Asperger syndrome: The junior detective training pro-
Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999).
gram. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial
and Allied Disciplines, 49, 743–753.
expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism
Beaumont, R., Rotolone, C., & Sofronoff, K. (2015). The
and Developmental Disorders, 29, 57–66.
secret agent society social skills program for children
Chakrabarti, S., & Fombonne, E. (2001). Pervasive devel-
with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A
opmental disorders in preschool children. JAMA, 285,
comparison of two school variants. Psychology in the
3093–3099.
Schools, 52, 390–402.
Chamberlain, B. O. (2001). Isolation or involvement?
Bellini, S., & Akullian, J. (2007). A meta-analysis of
The social networks of children with autism included
video modeling and video self-modeling interventions
in regular classes. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation).
for children and adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
University of California, Los Angeles.
order. Exceptional Children, 73, 264–287.
Chan, J. M., Lang, R., Rispoli, M., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos,
Bellini, S., & Hopf, A. (2007). The development of the
J., & Cole, H. (2009). Use of peer-mediated interven-
autism social skills profile. Focus on Autism and Other
tions in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders:
Developmental Disabilities, 22, 80–87.
A systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum
Bellini, S., & McConnell, L. L. (2010). Strength-based
Disorders, 3, 876–889.
educational programming for students with autism
Clark, E., Radley, K. C., & Phosaly, L. (2014). Best prac-
spectrum disorders: A case for video self-modeling.
tices in assessment and intervention of children with
Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for
high-functioning autism spectrum disorders. In P. L.
Children and Youth, 54, 220–227.
Harrison & A. Thomas (Eds.), Best practices in school
Bellini, S., Akullian, J., & Hopf, A. (2007). Increasing
psychology: Data-based and collaborative decision
social engagement in young children with autism
making (pp. 417–431). Bethesda, MD: NASP.
spectrum disorders using video selfmodeling. School
Cole, C. L. (2015). Peer-mediated intervention for
Psychology Review, 36, 80–90.
social communication difficulties in adolescents with
Bellini, S., Peters, J. K., Benner, L., & Hopf, A. (2007).
autism: Literature review and research recommenda-
A meta-analysis of school-based social skills inter-
tions. World Academy of Science, Engineering and
ventions for children with autism spectrum disorders.
Technology, International Journal of Medical, Health,
Remedial and Special Education, 28, 153–162.
Biomedical, Bioengineering and Pharmaceutical
Block, H. M., Radley, K. C., Jenson, W. R., Clark, E., &
Engineering, 9, 342–345.
O’Neill, R. E. (2015). Effects of a multimedia social
14  Social Skills Training for Children 251

Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Handbook of child behavior therapy (pp. 475–497).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Saddle River, NJ: New York, NY: Plenum Press.
Pearson. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1987). The relation-
Crozier, S., & Siieo, N. M. (2005). Encouraging posi- ship between adaptive behavior and social skills:
tive behavior with social stories: An intervention for Issues in definition and assessment. Journal of Special
children with autism spectrum disorders. Teaching Education, 21, 167–181.
Exceptional Children, 37, 26–31. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills
Daubert, A., Hornstein, S., & Tincani, M. (2015). Effects rating system (SSRS). Circle Pines, MN: American
of a modified power card strategy on turn tak- Guidance Service.
ing and social commenting of children with autism Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (2008). Social skills
spectrum disorder playing board games. Journal of improvement system: Rating scales. Bloomington,
Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 27, 93–110. MN: Pearson Assessments.
Dingfelder, H. E., & Mandell, D. S. (2011). Bridging the Gresham, F. M., Sugai, G., & Horner, R. H. (2001).
research-to-practice gap in autism intervention: An Interpreting outcomes of social skills training for stu-
application of diffusion of innovation theory. Journal dents with high-incidence disabilities. Exceptional
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 597–609. Children, 67, 331–344.
Donnelly, J. I., Luyben, P. D., & Zan, C. S. (2009). Gresham, F. M., Elliott, S. N., Vance, M. J., & Cook, C. R.
Increasing eye contact toward learning materials (2011). Comparability of the social skills rating sys-
in a toddler with autism. Journal of Prevention & tem to the social skills improvement system: Content
Intervention in the Community, 37, 170–176. and psychometric comparisons across elementary and
Elder, L. M., Caterino, L. C., Chao, J., Shacknai, D., & secondary age levels. School Psychology Quarterly,
DeSimone, G. (2006). The efficacy of social skills 26, 27–44.
treatment for children with Asperger syndrome. Gutierrez, A., Jr., Hale, M. N., Gossens-Archuleta, K.,
Education and Treatment of Children, 29, 635–663. & Sobrino-Sanchez, V. (2007). Evaluating the social
Elliott, S. N., Racine, C. N., & Busse, R. T. (1995). behavior of preschool children with autism in an
Best practices in preschool social skills training. inclusive playground setting. International Journal of
In A. Thomas & J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in Special Education, 22, 26–30.
school psychology-III (pp. 1009–1020). Washington, Gutstein, S. E., & Whitney, T. (2002). Asperger syndrome
DC: National Association of School Psychologists. and the development of social competence. Focus on
Elliott, S. N., Roach, A., & Beddow, P. (2008). Best prac- Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17,
tices in preschool social skills training. In A. Thomas 161–171.
& J. Grimes (Eds.), Best practices in school psychol- Hess, K. L., Morrier, M. J., Heflin, L. J., & Ivey, M. L.
ogy (pp. 1531–1546). Bethesda, MD: NASP. (2008). Autism treatment survey: Services received
Fragale, C. L. (2014). Video modeling interventions by children with autism spectrum disorders in
to improve play skills of children with autism spec- public school classrooms. Journal of Autism and
trum disorders: A systematic literature review. Review Developmental Disorders, 38, 961–971.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1, Himle, M. B., Miltenberger, R. G., Gatheridge, B. J., &
165–178. Flessner, C. A. (2004). An evaluation of two proce-
Ganz, J., Earles-Vollrath, T., & Cook, K. (2011). Video dures for training skills to prevent gun play in chil-
modeling a visually based intervention for children dren. Pediatrics, 113, 70–77.
with autism spectrum disorder. Teaching Exceptional Hitchcock, C. H., Dowrick, P. W., & Prater, M. A. (2003).
Children, 43, 8–19. Video self-modeling intervention in school-based set-
Gardenier, N., MacDonald, R., & Green, G. (2004). tings. Remedial and Special Education, 24, 36–45.
Comparison of direct observational methods for mea- Hume, K., Loftin, R., & Lantz, J. (2009). Increasing inde-
suring stereotypic behavior in children with autism pendence in autism spectrum disorders: A review of
spectrum disorders. Research in Developmental three focused interventions. Journal of Autism and
Disabilities, 25, 99–118. Developmental Disorders, 39, 1329–1338.
Goin-Kochel, R., Myers, B., & Mackintosh, V. (2007). Hunt, P., & Goetz, L. (1997). Research on inclusive
Parental reports on the use of treatments and therapies educational programs, practices, and outcomes for
for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research students with severe disabilities. Journal of Special
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1, 195–209. Education, 31, 3.
Gray, C. (2000). The new social story book (Illustrated Jenson, W.R., Bowen, J., Clark, E., Block, H.M., Gabrielsen,
ed.). Arlington, TX: Future Horizons. T., Hood, J.A., … Springer, B. (2011). Superheroes
Greenspan, S. (1980). Is social competence synonymous social skills. Eugene, OR: Pacific Northwest.
with school performance? American Psychologist, 35, Johnson, B. M., Miltenberger, R. G., Egemo-Helm, K.,
938–939. Jostad, C. M., Flessner, C., & Gatheridge, B. (2005).
Gresham, F. M. (1981). Assessment of children’s social Evaluation of behavioral skills training for teaching
skills. Journal of School Psychology, 19, 120–133. abduction-prevention skills to young children. Journal
Gresham, F. M. (1998). Social skills training with chil- of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 67–78.
dren: Social learning and applied behavior analytic Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (1990). Extended reduc-
approaches. In T. S. Watson & F. M. Fresham (Eds.), tions in stereotypic behavior of students with autism
252 K.C. Radley et al.

through a self-management treatment package. students with autism: A meta-analysis. Focus on


Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 119–127. Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 22,
Kohler, F. W., Strain, P. S., & Shearer, D. D. (1996). 2–13.
Examining levels of social inclusion within an inte- Leekam, S. R., Prior, M. R., & Uljarevic, M. (2011).
grated preschool for children with autism. In L. K. Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum
Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive disorders: A review of research in the last decade.
behavioral support: Including people with difficult Psychological Bulletin, 137, 562–593.
behavior in the community (pp. 305–332). Baltimore, Macdonald, R. P. F., Dickson, C. A., Martineau, M., &
MD: Paul H Brookes Publishing. Ahearn, W. H. (2016). Prerequisite skills that sup-
Kokina, A., & Kern, L. (2010). Social story interven- port learning through video modeling. Education and
tions for students with autism spectrum disorders: A Treatment of Children, 38, 33–47.
meta-analysis. Journal of Autism and Developmental Mason, R. A., Ganz, J. B., Parker, R. I., Boles, M. B.,
Disorders, 40, 812–826. Davis, H. S., & Rispoli, M. J. (2013). Video-based
Kornacki, L. T., Ringdahl, J. E., Sjostrom, A., & modeling: Differential effects due to treatment pro-
Nuernberger, J. E. (2013). A component analysis of tocol. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7,
a behavioral skills training package used to teach 120–131.
conversation skills to young adults with autism spec- McConnell, S. (2002). Interventions to facilitate social
trum and other developmental disorders. Research in interactions for young children with autism: Review
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 1370–1376. of available research and recommendations for edu-
Kroeger, K. A., Schultz, J. R., & Newsom, C. (2007). A cational intervention and future research. Journal of
comparison of two group-delivered social skills pro- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 351–372.
grams for young children with autism. Journal of McCoy, K., & Hermansen, E. (2007). Video modeling
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 808–817. for individuals with autism: A review of model types
Laugeson, E. A., & Frankel, F. (2010). Social skills for and effects. Education and Treatment of Children, 30,
teenagers with developmental and autism spectrum 183–213.
disorders: The PEERS treatment manual. New York, McFee, K. M. (2010). Teaching social skills to children
NY: Routledge. with Asperger’s and high functioning autism: An
Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Mogil, C., & Dillon, A. R. evaluation of video self-modeling and behavior skills
(2009). Parent-assisted social skills training to improve training. Retrieved from http://scholarcommons.usf.
friendships in teens with autism spectrum disorders. edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=4816&context=etd
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, McIntosh, K., & MacKay, L. D. (2008). Enhancing gener-
596–606. alization of social skills: Making social skills curricula
Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Gantman, A., Dillon, A. R., effective after the lesson. Beyond Behavior, 18, 18–25.
& Mogil, C. (2012). Evidence-based social skills Merrell, K. W. (1993). School social behavior scales: Test
training for adolescents with autism spectrum disor- manual. Bradon, VT: Clinical Psychology Publishing
ders: The UCLA PEERS program. Journal of Autism Company.
and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1025–1036. Mesibov, G. B. (1984). Social skills training with verbal
Laugeson, E. A., Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K., autistic adolescents and adults: A program model.
& Ellingsen, R. (2015). A randomized controlled trial Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14,
to improve social skills in young adults with autism 395–404.
spectrum disorder: The UCLA PEERS® program. Miles, N. I., & Wilder, D. A. (2009). The effects of behav-
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, ioral skills training on caregiver implementation of
3978–3989. guided compliance. Journal of Applied Behavior
Laushey, K. M., & Heflin, L. J. (2000). Enhancing social Analysis, 42, 405–410.
skills of kindergarten children with autism through the Miltenberger, R. (2015). Behavior modification:
training of multiple peers as tutors. Journal of Autism Principles and procedures (6th ed.). Boston: Cengage
and Developmental Disorders, 30, 183–193. Learning.
Leaf, J. B., Townley-Cochran, D., Taubman, M., Cihon, Miltenberger, R. G., Flessner, C., Gatheridge, B., Johnson,
J. H., Oppenheim-Leaf, M. L., Kassardjian, A., … B., Satterlund, M., & Egemo, K. (2004). Evaluation
Pentz, T. G. (2015). The teaching interaction proce- of behavioral skills training to prevent gun play in
dure and behavioral skills training for individuals children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37,
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder: a review 513–516.
and commentary. Review Journal of Autism and Morgan, D. P., & Jenson, W. R. (1988). Teaching behav-
Developmental Disorders, 2, 402–413. iorally disordered students. Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Lee, J. N. (2015). The effectiveness of point-of-view Morlock, L., Reynolds, J. L., Fisher, S., & Comer, R. J.
video modeling as a social skills intervention for chil- (2015). Video modeling and word identification in
dren with autism spectrum disorders. Review Journal adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Child
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2, 414–428. Language Teaching and Therapy, 31, 101–111.
Lee, S. H., Simpson, R. L., & Shogren, K. A. (2007). Morrier, M. J., Hess, K. L., & Heflin, L. J. (2011). Teacher
Effects and implications of delf-management for training for implementation of teaching strategies for
14  Social Skills Training for Children 253

students with autism spectrum disorders. Teacher setting. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
Education and Special Education: The Journal of Disabilities, 29, 216–229.
the Teacher Education Division of the Council for Radley, K. C., Ford, W. B., McHugh, M. B.,
Exceptional Children, 34, 119–132. Dadakhodjaeva, K., O’Handley, R. D., Battaglia,
Nuernberger, J. E., Ringdahl, J. E., Vargo, K. K., A. A., & Lum, J. D. K. (2015c). Brief report: Use of
Crumpecker, A. C., & Gunnarsson, K. F. (2013). Using superheroes social skills to promote accurate social
a behavioral skills training package to teach conversa- skill use in children with autism spectrum disorder.
tion skills to young adults with autism spectrum dis- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45,
orders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 3048–3054.
411–417. Radley, K. C., Hanglein, J., & Arak, M. (2016). School-­
O’Handley, R. D., Radley, K. C., & Whipple, H. M. based social skills training for preschool-age children
(2015). The relative effects of social stories and video with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. Epub ahead of
modeling toward increasing eye contact of adolescents press.
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Radley, K. C., McHugh, M. B., Taber, T., Battaglia,
Spectrum Disorders, 11, 101–111. A. A., & Ford, W. B. (2015a). School-based social
O’Handley, R. D., Ford, W. B., Radley, K. C., Hilbig, K., skills training for children with autism spectrum dis-
& Wimberly, J. (2016). Social skills training for ado- order. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
lescents with intellectual disabilities: A school-based Disabilities. Epub ahead of press.
evaluation. Behavior Modification. Epub ahead of Radley, K. C., O’Handley, R. D., & Labrot, Z. (2015b).
press. A comparison of momentary time sampling and par-
Odom, S. L., & Strain, P. S. (1984). Peer-mediated tial-interval recording for assessment of effects of
approaches to promoting children’s social interaction: social skills training. Psychology in the Schools, 52,
A review. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54, 363–378.
544–557. Radley, K. C., O’Handley, R. D., Ness, E. J., Ford, W. B.,
Ozdemir, S. (2008). The effectiveness of social stories Battaglia, A. A., McHugh, M. B., & McLemore, C. E.
on decreasing disruptive behaviors of children with (2014b). Promoting social skill use and generalization
autism: Three case studies. Journal of Autism and in children with autism spectrum disorder. Research in
Developmental Disorders, 38, 1689–1696. Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8, 669–680.
Pane, H. M., Sidener, T. M., Vladescu, J. C., & Nirgudkar, Reichow, B., & Sabornie, E. J. (2009). Brief report:
A. (2015). Evaluating function-based social stories Increasing verbal greeting initiations for a stu-
with children with autism. Behavior Modification, 39, dent with autism via a social story intervention.
912–931. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39,
Pelios, L. V., & Lund, S. K. (2001). A selective over- 1740–1743.
view of issues on classification, causation, and early Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills
intensive behavioral intervention for autism. Behavior interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation
Modification, 25, 678–697. for evidence-based practices within a best evi-
Peters, L. C., & Thompson, R. H. (2015). Teaching chil- dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and
dren with autism to respond to conversation partners’ Developmental Disorders, 40, 149–166.
interest. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 48, Rosales, R., Stone, K., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2009). The
544–562. effects of behavioral skills training on implementa-
Plavnick, J. B., Kaid, T., & MacFarland, M. C. (2015). tion of the picture exchange communication system.
Effects of a school-based social skills training pro- Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 541–549.
gram for adolescents with autism spectrum disorder Ross, S. W., & Sabey, C. V. (2015). Check-in check-out +
and intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and social skills: Enhancing the effects of check-in check-­
Developmental Disorders, 45, 2674–2690. out for students with social skill deficits. Remedial and
Poche, C., Yoder, P., & Miltenberger, R. (1988). Teaching Special Education, 36, 246–257.
self-protection to children using television techniques. Scattone, D. (2008). Enhancing the conversation skills
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 21, 253–261. of a boy with Asperger’s disorder through social
Powell, J., Martindale, A., Kulp, S., Martindale, A., & stories and video modeling. Journal of Autism and
Bauman, R. (1977). Taking a closer look: Time sam- Developmental Disorders, 38, 395–400.
pling and measurement error. Journal of Applied Scattone, D., Tingstrom, D. H., & Wilczynski, S. M.
Behavior Analysis, 10(325), 332. (2006). Increasing appropriate social interactions of
Qi, C. H., Barton, E. E., Collier, M., Lin, Y. L., & Montoya, children with autism spectrum disorders using social
C. (2015). A systematic review of effects of social sto- stories. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
ries interventions for individuals with autism spectrum Disabilities, 21, 211–222.
disorder. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Sabey, C. (2015). The effects of Brief, Localized,
Disabilities. Epub ahead of press. Intensive, Social Skills (BLISS) training on social
Radley, K. C., Ford, W. B., Battaglia, A. A., & McHugh, outcomes for students with autism spectrum disorder
M. B. (2014a). The effects of a social skills train- in inclusive school settings: Form and function. All
ing package on social engagement of children with Graduate Theses and Dissertations. Retrieved from
autism spectrum disorders in a generalized recess http://digitalcommons.usu.edu/etd/4562
254 K.C. Radley et al.

Sarokoff, R. A., & Sturmey, P. (2004). The effects of video modeling. Education and Treatment of Children,
behavioral skills training on staff implementation of 33, 395–419.
discrete-trial teaching. Journal of Applied Behavior Thomson, K., Burnham Riosa, P., & Weiss, J. A. (2015).
Analysis, 37, 535–538. Brief report of preliminary outcomes of an emotion
Sarokoff, R. A., & Sturmey, P. (2008). The effects of regulation intervention for children with autism spec-
instructions, rehearsal, modeling, and feedback on trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
acquisition and generalization of staff use of discrete Disorders, 45, 3487–3495.
trial teaching and student correct responses. Research Vanselow, N. R., & Hanley, G. P. (2014). An evaluation of
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2, 125–136. computerized behavioral skills training to teach safety
Scheflen, S., Freeman, S., & Paparella, T. (2012). Using skills to young children. Journal of Applied Behavior
video modeling to teach young children with autism Analysis, 47, 51–69.
developmentally appropriate play and connected Wang, P., & Spillane, A. (2009). Evidence-based social
speech. Education and Training in Autism and skills interventions for children with autism: A meta-­
Developmental Disabilities, 47, 302–318. analysis. Education and Training in Developmental
Senju, A., & Johnson, M. (2009). Atypical eye contact Disabilities, 44, 318–342.
in autism: Models, mechanisms, and development. Wang, S. Y., Cui, Y., & Parrila, R. (2011). Examining the
Neuroscience & Behavioral Reviews, 33, 1204–1214. effectiveness of peer-mediated and video-modeling
Shipley-Benamou, R., Lutzker, J. R., & Taubman, M. social skills interventions for children with autism
(2002). Teaching daily living skills to children with spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis in single-case
autism through instructional video modeling. Journal research using HLM. Research in Autism Spectrum
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 166–177. Disorders, 5, 562–569.
Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., & de la Cruz, B. (2007). How Watkins, L., Kuhn, M., Ledbetter-Cho, K., Gevarter, C., &
to use video modeling and video prompting. Austin, O’Reilly, M. (2015). Evidence-based social communi-
TX: PRO-ED. cation interventions for children with autism spectrum
Sofronoff, K., Silva, J., & Beaumont, R. (2015). The disorder. The Indian Journal of Pediatrics, 84, 1–8.
secret agent society social-emotional skills program Watkins, L., O’Reilly, M., Kuhn, M., Gevarter, C.,
for children with a high-functioning autism spectrum Lancioni, G. E., Sigafoos, J., & Lang, R. (2014). A
disorder: A parent-directed trial. Focus on autism and review of peer-mediated social interaction interven-
other developmental disabilities. Psychology in the tions for students with autism in inclusive settings.
Schools. Epub ahead of press. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45,
Southall, C. M., & Gast, D. L. (2011). Self-management 1070–1083.
procedures: A comparison across the autism spectrum. Weiss, M. J., LaRue, R. H., & Newcomer, A. (2009).
Education and Training in Autism and Developmental Social skills and autism: Understanding and address-
Disabilities, 46, 155–171. ing the deficits. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Applied behavior
Starr, E., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S., & Zwaigenbaum, L. analysis for children with autism spectrum disorders
(2003). Stability and change among high-­functioning (pp. 129–144). New York, NY: Springer.
children with pervasive developmental disorders: Whalon, K. J., Conroy, M. A., Martinez, J. R., & Werch,
A 2-year outcome study. Journal of Autism and B. L. (2015). School-based peer-related social compe-
Developmental Disorders, 33, 15–22. tence interventions for children with autism spectrum
Stewart, K. K., Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2007). disorder: A meta-analysis and descriptive review of
Evaluation of family-implemented behavioral skills single case research design studies. Journal of Autism
training for teaching social skills to a child with and Developmental Disorders, 45, 1513–1531.
Asperger’s disorder. Clinical Case Studies, 6, 252–262. White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social
Strain, P. S., Odom, S. L., & McConnell, S. (1984). skills development in children with autism spectrum
Promoting social reciprocity of exceptional children: disorders: A review of the intervention research.
Identification, target behavior selection, and interven- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37,
tion. Remedial and Special Education, 5, 21–28. 1858–1868.
Tantam, D. (2003). The challenge of adolescents and Willemsen-Swinkles, S. H. N., Buitelaar, J. K., Weijen,
adults with Asperger syndrome. Child and Adolescent F. G., & van Engeland, H. (1998). Timing of social
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12, 143–163. gaze behavior in children with pervasive developmen-
Taras, M. E., Matson, J. L., & Leary, C. (1988). Training tal disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental
social interpersonal skills in two autistic children. Disorders, 28, 199–210.
Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Wolfe, K., Slocum, T. A., & Kunnavatana, S. S. (2014).
Psychiatry, 19, 275–280. Promoting behavioral variability in individuals with
Tereshko, L., MacDonald, R., & Ahearn, W. H. (2010). autism spectrum disorders: A literature review. Focus
Strategies for teaching children with autism to imitate on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 29,
response chains using video modeling. Research in 180–190.
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 4, 479–489. Wurtele, S. K. (1990). Teaching personal safety skills
Tetreault, A. S., & Lerman, D. C. (2010). Teaching social to four-year-old children: A behavioral approach.
skills to children with autism using point-of-view Behavior Therapy, 21, 25–32.
Curriculums
15
Hsu-Min Chiang

rials are now available for use with individual


Introduction with ASD. However, it is often not easy to find the
curriculum that is ideally suited to a particular
Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) intervention purpose or participants (Hickson,
are characterized by deficits in social communi- Blackman, & Reis, 1995).
cation and interaction as well as displaying A single curriculum may only address a set of
restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviors and skills in a particular area for a particular group of
interests (American Psychiatric Association students. Given that each intervention program
[APA], 2013). The Individuals with Disabilities may target different skills of individuals with
Education Act (IDEA) mandates that early inter- ASD and have clients with different abilities, it is
ventions be provided to infants and toddlers with necessary to combine or adapt materials from
ASD and special education and related services different sources or to design the curriculum that
be provided to students with ASD aged 3–21 years best meets the intervention goals and the needs of
(Public Law 108–446, 2004). Various interven- the clients. This chapter represents an effort to
tions for individuals with ASD are available to present the information regarding how to design
address their needs; however, most modern ASD a curriculum for teaching individuals with
interventions are educational in nature (Mesibov ASD. There are five steps that may be used as
& Shea, 2011). general guidelines for developing a curriculum.
Each interveniton has its own curriculum and In the next part of this chapter, detailed informa-
curriculum can be defined as “a plan for achieving tion regarding each step and examples are pre-
goals” (Ornstein & Hinkins, 2013, p. 8). Planned sented. An example of a curriculum targeting
activities directed by the interventionists to their multiple intervention goals is presented in the
clients are the central pieces of an intervention. last part of this chapter.
An intervention curriculum guides intervention-
ists what to teach, how to teach, and when to
move on to a new target behavior or the next level How to Design a Curriculum
of instruction. A wide variety of curriculum mate-
There are five steps that may be used for design-
ing a curriculum. The first step is to understand
the needs of individuals with ASD. The second
H.-M. Chiang, PhD (*)
University of Macau, Taipa, Macau step is to set up intervention goals based on cli-
e-mail: hchiang@umac.mo ents’ needs. The third step is to select the assess-

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 255


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_15
256 H.-M. Chiang

ments that will be used to determine if the Individuals with ASD who have ID are less likely
intervention has met its goals. The fourth step is to participate in postsecondary education and
to design lesson/instructional plans for each employment than those who do not have ID
intervention session. The last step is to select (Chiang, Cheung, Hickson, Xiang, & Tsai, 2012;
instructional strategies that will be used during Chiang, Cheung, Li, & Tsai, 2013). However,
intervention. having a higher IQ does not ensure a better adult
outcome; the majority of individuals with high
functioning ASD or Asperger’s disorder do not
Step 1. Understand Clients’ Needs have a competitive job or live independently
(Engstrom, Ekstrom, & Emilsson, 2003). In order
Individuals with different autism diagnoses may to have an independent adult life, one must be
demonstrate different social interaction and com- able to transfer cognitive functioning into the
munication abilities, and severity levels may vary performance of daily life activities (Sparrow,
across individuals. A wide range of language Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005). However, individuals
abilities can be found in individuals with ASD with ASD tend to have poorer adaptive behavior
(Kjelgaard & Tager-Flusberg, 2001; Lindgren, than would be expected by their cognitive abili-
Folstein, Tomblin, & Tager-Flusberg, 2009; ties (Bolte & Poustka, 2002; Duncan & Bishop,
Maljaars, Noens, Scholte, & van Berckelaer-­ 2015; Freeman et al., 1991; Klin et al., 2007; Liss
Onnes, 2012). Some individuals with ASD et al., 2001; Saulnier & Klin, 2007; Tomanik,
develop verbal skills but others do not. For the Pearson, Loveland, Lane, & Shaw, 2007). Thus,
people with ASD who have acquired verbal having the interventions targeting daily living
skills, their expressive skills may not be suffi- skills is also important for individuals with ASD.
cient to deal with different social demands. For Given the needs of individuals with ASD may
the people with ASD who do not develop verbal vary across individuals and functional areas, it is
skills or have limited verbal skills, they may rely important to understand individual client’s needs
on using gestures to communicate with other before starting an intervention. Observing cli-
people (Chiang & Lin, 2008) and, thus, need to ents’ behaviors, conducing necessary assess-
be taught of using symbolic communication ments, and/or interviewing parents and clients
methods. Many children with ASD have greater can reveal the baseline level of their abilities and
impairment in language comprehension than lan- needs.
guage expression (Lloyd, Paintin, & Botting,
2006; Luyster, Kadlec, Carter, & Tager-Flusberg,
2008; Weismer, Lord, & Esler, 2010). Language Step 2. Set Up Goals
comprehension seems to be delayed in most indi-
viduals with ASD (Kjellmer et al., 2012; After knowing clients’ needs, the next step is to
Manolitsi & Botting, 2011; Thurm, Lord, Lee, & determine the desired outcomes that an intervention
Newschaffer, 2007). Thus, it is important to have aims to achieve. An intervention can aim to address
the interventions aiming to improve language a single goal or multiple goals depending on the
and social communication skills of individuals length of the intervention and available resources.
with ASD. An intervention goal can be conceptualized as a spe-
Some individuals with ASD also have intel- cific skill or cluster of skills that a learner will acquire
lectual disability (ID) (APA, 2013). About 64% during intervention (Sailor & Guess, 1983). For
of children with ASD have comorbid ID or bor- each intervention goal, there should be short-term
derline intellectual functioning (Wingate et al., objectives that are measurable intermedia steps
2014). Cognitive functioning has been found to between a client’s current level of performance and
be associated with adult outcomes in individuals an intervention goal (Fiscus & Mandell, 1983). Both
with ASD (Eaves & Ho, 2008; Farley et al., 2009; goals and objectives should be stated in a clear, pre-
Howlin, Goode, Hutton, & Rutter, 2004).
15 Curriculums 257

cise, and m­ easurable way. The Case Study of Mark Academics Martin will solve When presenting
illustrates intervention goals and objectives. addition problems with addition
with sums up to problems with
50 sums up to 2050,
Martin will solve
Case Study: Martin the questions
correctly 4 out of
Martin is a 10-year-old boy with ASD who also has 5 trials without
moderate ID. He loves drawing and is very good at prompts
drawing. He can follow directions and complete Martin will write When presenting
assigned tasks with minimal prompting. However, he a paragraph with pictures,
has significant delays in language skills, social including at least Martin will write
interaction skills, academic skills, and independent three sentences three sentences
living skills about an without errors on
Intervention Short-term interested topic 4 out of 5 trials
goals objectives without prompts
Language Martin will When Independent Martin will place Given his
answer Wh presenting with living cell phone calls mom’s cell
questions Who questions, phone number,
Martin will Martin will
verbally answer successfully
the questions place the phone
correctly 4 out call on 4 out of
of 5 trials 5 trials without
without prompts prompts
Martin will When presenting Martin will wear When
request desired with a desired a coat when it is presenting with
items verbally item, Marin will cold outside weather
say, Can I have information,
xx (the name of Martin will
the item)? 4 out wear a coat
of 5 trials when going
without prompts outside on 4 out
Social Martin will When seeing of 5 trials
interaction initiate social peers are without prompts
interactions with playing toy Depending on the nature of the intervention, vari-
peers cars, Martin
will say, Can I
ous types of goals may be addressed through an
play with you? intervention. The intervention goals can be gen-
4 out of 5 trials erally grouped into two basic categories. They
without prompts are remedy weakness and cultivate strength.
Martin will When peers
respond to social ask him to play
interactions with them, Remedy Weakness  The majority of the interven-
initiated by peers Martin will join tion programs for individuals with ASD are
them 4 out of 5 designed to address their deficits. The main focus
trials without of this remedy weakness concept is that the deficits
prompts
of students with ASD will be decreased after inter-
ventions. An example of this concept is the
Individualized Education Program (IEP) for
­students with disabilities. The IDEA mandates that
each student with a disability should have a written
statement (i.e., IEP) including his/her present lev-
els of academic achievement and functional perfor-
mance (Public Law 108–446, 2004). Goals in
relation to academic and functional skills should be
written in the education plan as well as how the
258 H.-M. Chiang

student’s progress toward meeting the goals will be referenced tests allow interventionists to measure
measured (Public Law 108–446, 2004). Although a learner’s performance against predetermined
this model can show how effectively an interven- criteria. These tests often are the commercially
tion helps individuals with ASD decrease their published tests (e.g. intelligence tests, academic
deficits, due to the constant focus on deficits, it may achievement tests, language tests, adaptive
potentially harm their self-­esteem and bring other behavior tests) which can be purchased through
negative implications to them and their families. publishers.

Cultivate Strength  Individuals with ASD are like Informal Procedures  These assessment proce-
people without ASD which have their own dures include observation, work sample analysis,
strengths. However, their strengths are often not criterion-referenced tests, task analysis, check-
discussed in literature or addressed in interven- lists, rating scales, interviews, and questionnaires
tions. There has been a movement for service pro- (Hickson et al., 1995). These assessments are
viders to conduct strength-based assessment on often made by interventionists to determine if
people with ASD to identify their personal, famil- learners have acquired target skills that they have
ial, and broader contextual strengths (Cosden, been specifically taught.
Koegel, Koegel, Greenwell, & Klein, 2006). In The effectiveness of an intervention can be
contrast to the deficits-focused model, this approach determined through carefully following its evalu-
emphasizes the positive aspects of an individual. It ation plan. The evaluation plan should consist of
encourages interventionists to establish goals that assessment material(s) and an evaluation design.
go beyond repairing someone’s deficits to those of Depending on the number of participants in an
cultivating the individual’s strengths and promote a intervention, the evaluation design can be a
better quality of life in the family (Huebner & single-­subject design or a pretest-posttest group
Gilman, 2004). Some individuals with ASD show experimental design.
strengths and talents in drawing, playing music
instruments, solving math problems, and writing Single-Subject Design  This design allows deter-
computer code. However, limited interventions mining the intervention effects on a single partici-
have been designed to cultivate their interests and pant or a small group of participants. There are
strengths. Thus, more strength-based interventions three commonly used single-subject designs
should be encouraged. including the reversal or withdraw design (ABA)
design, multiple baseline design, and changing cri-
terion design (Rusch, Rose, & Greenwood, 1988).
Step 3. Determine Assessments
The Reversal or Withdraw Design (ABA)
In order to determine whether the intervention Design  This design consists of three phases:
has achieved its goals, ways of assessment and baseline 1 (A), intervention condition (B), and
assessment criteria should be specified. A com- baseline 2 (A). During baseline 1, the target
prehensive evaluation plan should be determined behavior(s) is/are observed without any
before starting an intervention. Various evalua- ­presentation of intervention. During intervention,
tion tools can be used, and they can be grouped the participant(s)’ target behavior(s) is/are
into two categories: formal standardized tests and observed while receiving intervention. After some
informal procedures. stable and substantial changes of the target
behavior(s) are observed, the treatment condition
Formal Standardized Tests  These tests can be will be withdrawn. The baseline condition will
either norm referenced or criterion referenced then be reintroduced. For example, if one wishes
(Hickson et al., 1995). Norm-referenced tests to determine whether using participants’ interested
allow interventionists to compare a learner’s per- items would increase their attending behaviors,
formance to that of same-aged peers. Criterion- he/she can use an ABA design. If the numbers of
15 Curriculums 259

participants’ attending behaviors during the inter- mances of the participants can be determined by
vention condition are higher than those during the using statistical analyses. If participants’ posttest
baseline 1 and baseline 2 conditions, it can be con- scores are significantly higher than their pretest
cluded that the intervention was effective. scores, it may be concluded that the intervention
has positive influences on the participants.
The Multiple Baseline Design  This design can be
used across subjects, behaviors, or settings (Hickson Pretest-Posttest Control-Group Design 
et al., 1995). Baseline data are collected on the same Participants are randomly assigned to either of the
behavior across two or more participants, two or intervention group or the non-­intervention (con-
more behaviors on the same participant, or on the trol) group. Both groups will then receive pretests.
same behavior across two or more settings for the After pretests, only the intervention group will
same participant (Christensen, Burke Johnson, & receive the intervention. After the intervention has
Turner, 2011). After baseline data are collected, the completed, both groups will then receive post-
intervention will be introduced. If the number of tests. Statistical analyses are used to determine if
target behavior(s) recorded during the intervention the intervention effects exceed what would be
condition is noticeably higher than that during each expected by chance (Hickson et al., 1995).
baseline condition, it can be concluded that the
intervention was effective.
 tep 4. Design Lesson/Instructional
S
The Changing Criterion Design  This design Plans
begins on collecting baseline data. During the
first intervention phase, a criterion level is deter- Each intervention session should have its lesson/
mined. If a participant has successfully reached instructional plan to address its goals and objec-
the criterion level across several trials, a higher tives. Lesson/instructional plans guide interven-
criterion level will be introduced during the next tionists what to teach and what materials to use
intervention phase. If a participant’s target behav- during each intervention session. They also help
ior improved as the criterion level was increased, interventionists stay organized and ensure inter-
this overall pattern of results is the evidence of vention time is used appropriately and appropri-
the positive results of the intervention on the par- ate instruction is delivered to help learners
ticipant (Christensen et al., 2011). develop target skills (Selmi, Gallagher, & Mora-­
Flores, 2015). They do not always need to be
Pretest-Posttest Group Experimental Design  written in great details, but they need to be com-
There are two types of this design. One allows a pleted before a planned intervention session. A
comparison of the mean performance of the same lesson/instructional plan should at least include
participants before the intervention and after the
intervention (i.e., one-group pretest-­ posttest Table 15.1  Sample lesson plan
design). Another one allows the comparison of two Session #
groups (intervention vs. non-intervention) before Topic:
and after the intervention (i.e., pretest-posttest con- Activity 1: Time: Materials:
trol-group design). Teaching procedures:
(a)
One-Group Pretest-Posttest Design  Before start- (b)
ing an intervention, all participants receive pre- (c)
tests. After pretests, all participants begin to Activity 2: Time: Materials:
receive the intervention. Upon completion of the Teaching procedures:
intervention, all participants receive posttests (a)
which are the same as the pretests. The differ- (b)
ences between the pretest and posttest perfor- (c)
260 H.-M. Chiang

the sequence of learning activities, instructional Reinforcement  A learner’s desired item or activ-
time for each learning activity, and the materials ity is given to him/her once he/she has produced
to be used during a lesson. A sample lesson plan the target behavior. For example, a learner says
format can be found in Table 15.1. water to request drinking water. An adult heard
the learner saying water. He/she then allows the
learner to drink water.
Step 5. Select Instructional Strategies
Motivation Improvement  Motivation is one of the
After goals are determined, the teaching strategies pivotal areas that can have positive influence on the
that will be used during intervention should then learning of children with ASD (Koegel, Koegel,
be decided. Teaching strategies are the instruc- Harrower, & Carter, 1999). It can be defined as
tional tools that interventionists will use to assist “observable characteristics of a child’s respond-
learners to acquire target behaviors. They include ing” (Koegel et al., 1999, p. 178). Improvement in
instructional methods, behavior management motivation can be described as responsiveness to
techniques, and arrangements of the classroom social and environmental stimuli is increased
(Hickson et al., 1995). Guided by the philosophi- (Koegel, Carter, & Koegel, 1998). If a learner has
cal orientations of the intervention program, cli- high level of motivation in learning, it will be eas-
ents’ characteristics, and parents’ expectations, ier for interventionists to provide instruction, and it
appropriate teaching strategies should be selected. will also be easier for the learner to learn new skills.
Thus, it is important to apply strategies to improve
Instructional Methods  There are various learners’ learning motivation. Several strategies
evidence-­based instructional methods for teach- can be used to improve learner’s learning motiva-
ing individuals with ASD, and they can be tion. They include child choice, interspersal of
grouped into four categories, including skill acquisition and maintenance tasks, natural rein-
acquisition, motivation improvement, skill main- forcement, and reinforcing attempts.
tenance, and skill generalization.
Child Choice  Allow learners to choose preferred
Skill Acquisition  There are several approaches items or activities and the items or activities chosen
typically used to assist individuals with ASD to by them are then used in or as the learning activities
acquire new skills, including modeling, prompts, (Koegel et al., 1999). This simple s­trategy can
and reinforcement. effectively improve learners’ learning motivation.
For example, an adult asks a child if he/she would
Modeling  This is a teaching strategy that an adult like to use a pen or marker to do writing. If the
or a peer demonstrates a target behavior to a learner child wants to use a marker, he/she is allowed to
(Wong et al., 2013). For example, the target behav- use it for writing (Koegel, Singh, & Koegel, 2010).
ior is saying water. An adult demonstrates saying
water to a learner. Another form of modeling is Interspersal of Acquisition and Maintenance
video modeling which offers a visual model of the Tasks  Students often are not interested in com-
target behavior via videos (Wong et al., 2013). pleting the tasks that they feel challenging or dif-
ficult. Learning new tasks often make students feel
Prompts  Prompts, such as visual, verbal, ges- challenging and want to escape. However, in order
tural, or physical assistance, are given to a learner to achieve intervention goals, students are required
to assist him/her to learn a target skill (Wong to complete challenging tasks. Blending difficult
et al., 2013). For example, a learner is learning to tasks with easy tasks can increase students’ learn-
say car. An adult presents a toy car and provides ing motivation. Numerous previously learned
a verbal instruction (say car) to verbally prompt tasks are interspersed with the target skill that the
the learner to acquire the skill of saying car. learner has not already learned (Koegel & Koegel,
15 Curriculums 261

2012). For example, a learner can add two num- Skill Generalization  Given the restricted and
bers less than 10 to yield a sum smaller than 10 but repetitive patterns of behaviors and interests in
is learning the tasks adding two numbers less than individuals with ASD, skill generalization is
10 to yield a sum greater than 10. The easy ques- often found to be difficult for this population.
tions can be interspersed with the difficult ones to They may not apply the learn skills to non-­
increase students’ learning motivation. training setting or non-training interaction part-
ners. Several strategies could be used to improve
Natural Reinforcement  Another strategy to skill generalization (Stokes & Osnes, 1986).
improve motivation is to use natural reinforcers.
A natural reinforcer is the item/activity that is 1. The target skills should be useful to the learn-
directly and functionally related to the target task er’s daily life.
(Koegel & Koegel, 2012). Once a learner dis- 2. The target skills should result in receiving
plays a target behavior, he/she should receive the natural reinforcers.
item or activity that is directly related to the target 3. Use a wide variety of stimulus exemplars.
behavior. For example, a cup of water is given to 4. Use a wide variety of response exemplars.
a learner once he/she said water. 5. Provide training in various training conditions
and settings.
Reinforcing Attempts  Before a learner can suc-
cessfully display a target behavior, he/she may Behavior Management Techniques  A high
need to practice the behavior many times. proportion of children with ASD who are nonver-
Depending on the learner’s learning speed, his/her bal or have limited spoken language use chal-
learning process may be long. If interventionists lenging behaviors (e.g., tantrum, aggression) as a
only provide reinforcement to learners when they form of expressive communication (Chiang,
successfully produce target behaviors, they may 2008). Children’s problem behaviors may bring
lose motivation to learn new skills. Thus, reinforc- challenges for interventionists to deliver
ing learning attempts while a learner is trying to interventions to them and decrease children’s
­
acquire a new skill is important. Reinforcing learning time. Thus, it is critical to apply appro-
attempts means that a learner’s goal-­ oriented priate strategies to manage learners’ behaviors.
attempts are reinforced (Koegel & Koegel, 2012).
For example, a learner is learning to name balloon Functional Analysis of Problem Behaviors  The
and want to play with a balloon, but he/she has first step to design a program to manage learners’
difficulty sounding the whole word. He/she looked problem behaviors is to conduct a thorough func-
at the adult and said ba. The adult reinforced his/ tional analysis of the problem behaviors (Hickson
her attempt by giving him/her a balloon. et al., 1995). This analysis is to identify the ante-
cedent conditions that trigger the problem behav-
Skill Maintenance  Maintenance refers to learners ior, define the challenge (i.e., a description of the
who continue to display learned behaviors after an problem behavior), and identify the consequent
intervention has completed. Several strategies may conditions that maintain the problem behavior
be used to improve skill maintenance (Westling & (Horner, Albin, Todd, Newton, & Sprague, 2011).
Fox, 2009). (A) Continue practice. After learners Once this information is obtained, intervention
have acquired a target skill, he/she should be taught alternatives may be introduced to learners. For
to continue to practice the skill. (B) Multiple oppor- example, if a learner is likely to bite his teacher to
tunities to practice. Learners should be given escape from the task demands. Changing the nature
opportunities to practice one skill at different times, of the task demands to be more interesting to the
instead of all at once. (C) Intermittent reinforce- learners may decrease his biting behavior. Also,
ment. Once a learner has learned a target skill, rein- teaching the learner to say No or point to a picture
forcement should not be given to the learner with No sign when he wants to reject an uninter-
immediately but every couple trials or minutes. ested task may replace the biting behavior.
262 H.-M. Chiang

Extinction with Positive Reinforcement of Other actively involved with friends and participate in
Behaviors  Extinction means that interventionists organized activities (Wagner, Cadwallader, Garza,
intentionally withhold reinforcement following a & Cameto, 2004). Thus, a thematically structured
learner’s behavior or ignore a learner’s behavior teaching Saturday enrichment program is devel-
(Snell & Brown, 2011). Extinction is most effective oped for children with ASD. This program aims to
when positive reinforcing other behaviors is also improve their communication, social interaction,
used (Kerr & Nelson, 1989). For example, if a learner and academic skills. It uses strategies from Applied
uses screaming to get adults’ attention, in order to Behavior Analysis (ABA), the Pivotal Response
decrease the challenging behavior, adults will not Treatment (PRT) (Koegel et al., 1999), and the
look at the learner when he screams but will look at Treatment and Education of Autistic and Related
him to praise him when he does something good. Communication Handicapped Children (TEACCH)
program (Mesibov & Shea, 2010).
Arrangements of the Classroom  Learners The curriculum used in this program is the
with ASD have the tendency to become attached thematically structured teaching (TST) curricu-
to routines and settings (Mesibov, Shea, & lum. The TST curriculum has two main compo-
Schopler, 2004). Thus, structured physical nents: (a) thematic learning content and (b)
environment and clear sequence of learning structured teaching schedule. The thematic learn-
events are important to assist them in learning ing content means all the learning content is asso-
(Mesibov et al., 2004). The learning environ- ciated with the learning theme. For example, if
ment can be structured by using visual cues or the theme of a session is transportation, all learn-
furniture arrangement to let learners know ing content for the intervention session will be
which activities occur in specific areas associated with transportation (e.g., car, train,
(Mesibov & Shea, 2010). Visual cues (e.g., pic- airplane, bus ticket, gas price, etc.). The
tures, written words) can be used to make structured teaching schedule means that the
­
schedules meaningful and understandable to schedule of teaching activities is consistent
learners (Mesibov & Shea, 2010). throughout the intervention. For example, the
intervention session one teaching schedule is
identical to that of session two and the rest of the
 n Example of a Curriculum
A intervention sessions.
Targeting Multiple Skills This curriculum covers ten intervention ses-
sions. In each session, there is a theme. The
A large proportion of children with typical develop- themes in this curriculum are not overlapped;
ment (TD) participate in organized activities thus, there are a total of ten themes. Each session
(Hjorthol & Fyhri, 2009). Organized activities (e.g., is composed of four 30-mins structured sequen-
extracurricular activities, after-school programs, tial teaching segments: (a) dance party and greet-
community programs) refer to adult-­ sponsored ing; (b) an interactive story and story stations; (c)
activities that have regularly scheduled meetings language, mathematics, and science; and (d) arts
(Bohnert, Fredricks, & Randall, 2010). Participating projects and goodbye.
in organized activities have been suggested to be During the first segment, the theme of the
positively related to physical, psychosocial, cogni- week is introduced to children, and children
tive, and educational functioning development danced to theme-related music. Each song has its
(Bohnert & Garber, 2007; Gardner, Roth, & Brooks- choreography which is developed based on its
Gunn, 2008; Guevre-­ mont, Findlay, & Kohen, lyric. For example, for the transportation theme,
2014; Mahoney & Vest, 2012; Randall & Bohnert, songs related to cars, airplanes, trains, and boats
2009). However, compared to students with learn- are selected, and dance moves resembling these
ing disability and students with speech/language vehicles are developed. Interventionists show
impairment, students with ASD are less likely to be dance moves to children and prompt them to
15 Curriculums 263

dance to the music and interact with their peers. short essays related to the weekly story. During
After dance time is finished, interventionists lead the second 10 min, children are instructed to
children to sing a hello song to each child and learn mathematics by his/her assigned teacher.
teacher present. Mathematics concepts (e.g., numeracy, calcula-
During the second segment, one intervention- tion, measurement, distance, etc.) related to the
ist interactively reads a story to the group; he/she weekly story and theme are chosen. For exam-
has interaction with children, while he/she is ple, for the transportation theme, children work
reading the story. Child engagement is encour- on calculation questions and word problem-
aged by acting out the story and answering the solving questions related to trains. Instruction
questions related to the story. The weekly story is and learning materials are designed based on
specially chosen to match the theme of the week each child’s level. During the last 10 min, an
and appropriate to the reading level of the chil- adult leads science projects demonstration.
dren in the program. For example, for the trans- The science projects are chosen to be associated
portation theme, a story book talking about with the weekly theme. For example, for the
different trains and helping others is chosen. transportation theme, children are instructed to
One-on-one instruction is provided to the chil- use liquid detergent to power a boat. Children
dren who need more support to learn the story. are encouraged to have social interaction with
After the story is finished, children participate in other children while performing the science
station activities (e.g., playing games, watching project.
videos, doing role-play, reading books, playing During the fourth segment, an interventionist
play-doh, playing LEGO) related to the story and demonstrates the arts projects related to the
the theme of the week. For example, for the weekly theme. For example, for the transporta-
transportation week, children are instructed to tion theme, the arts projects include using cereal
use play-doh and LEGO to create vehicles, make boxes to make trains, using cardboards to make
paper airplanes, read books about vehicles, and cars, and using water bottles to make boats.
watch videos about vehicles. Children are encouraged to complete at least one
During the third segment, the first 10 min is art project. The last 10 min of this segment is
language instruction. Words related to the used to say goodbye. The activities of the day are
weekly story and theme are chosen. For exam- reviewed. Children are encouraged to show each
ple, for the transportation theme, the words other their art project(s) and are asked if they
boat, car, train, airplane, ticket, etc. are included. have made any new friends that day. A best helper
Each child is taught to learn vocabulary words for the day is identified during this time. A sam-
for the week by his/her assigned interventionist ple lesson plan of the TST curriculum can be
using both picture and letter flash cards. found below (Table 15.2).
Interventionists follow a specific sequence to
teach vocabulary: (a) show the child the picture
or word; (b) named the vocabulary word; (c) Conclusion
said, “This is (vocabulary word)”; and (d) ask
“What is this?”. At that point, the child is The guidelines for developing a curriculum men-
expected to say the vocabulary word. If the tioned above can be used for the practitioners
child does not, the word will be repeated and the who work with individuals with ASD and the
child will be prompted to say it. For the children parents who are interested in teaching their chil-
who complete the weekly vocabulary words dren at home. A common expression used by the
within 10 min, they are also instructed to answer professionals who work with individuals with
reading comprehension questions and write ASD is “If you see a person with ASD, you see a
Table 15.2  Sample TST lesson plan
264

Theme: Transportation
Section (A): Dance party/greeting
Time: 10:30–11:00
Detailed procedures
1. Tell children today’s theme
2. Play dance songs. Show children the designed dance moves. Have children join teachers to do the dance moves. Prompt children to have social interactions with other
children
Sample songs:
(a) We go traveling by http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=cSw50Jw0H34
(b) Over the mountains http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=U8v16WEVszM
(c) Transport http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=HJ1mO0MbqCg
(d) Wheels on the bus http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ppmwWwcaO8U, http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=Fh_aD5EswXk&list=UU4Hdb26_xnPQsntwLazMqYw
(e) Red light green light http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=KbRW73P24V8
(f) The airplane song http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=7Jim4SR3Nfg
(g) (I’d take a) car, train, airplane http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=VwQIN7QWyHM
3. After dance is completed (10 min before moving to next teaching segment), have children sit in a circle. Sing a hello song and greet with each other
Section (B): Interactive story and story stations
Time: 11:00–11:30
Detailed procedures
1. Read a transportation story (e.g., The Little Engine That Could by Watty Piper)
2. Act out the story while reading the text. Introduce target words and mathematics concepts. Ask children questions related to the story. Have children demonstrate the
emotions described in the story
3. After reading the story, have children participate in the story stations. Prompt children to have social interactions with others
(a) Station 1: children in the station one will use Legos to build trains and cars
(b) Station 2: children in the station two will make paper plans (e.g., http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=I0a0p8ygfQM)
(c) Station 3: children in the station three will play subway map jigsaw puzzles
(d) Station 4: children in the station four will play transportation games (sample games: http://www.primarygames.com/socstudies/transportation/games.htm; http://
www.coolgames.com/air-transport.html; http://pbskids.org/barney/children/games/transportation_game.html)
Section (C): Language, mathematics, and science
Time: 11:30–12:00
Detailed procedures
1. Provide children one-on-one instruction
2. Teach children the target words of the week. Have children complete the language worksheets specially designed for them
H.-M. Chiang
3. Teach children the target mathematics concepts of the week. Have children complete the mathematics worksheets specifically designed for them
4. Have children sit together to learn science projects
5. Demonstrate science projects to children and have children participate in at least one science project
Sample science projects
(a) Paper towel bridge http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=FAdmTzD46Kg
15 Curriculums

(b) Pepper and water http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=eR-ZV-_fQok


(c) Magnetic motor https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=hYOuWYMPj-s
(d) Air powered balloon car https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QzY9RH_JnL0
Section (D): Arts project and goodbye
Time: 12:00–12:30
1. Show children all the arts projects. Demonstrate how to complete each one
2. Have children choose the project that he/she wants to begin to work
Sample arts projects
(a) Cereal box rubber powered car http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ZfiV1oTF-ks
(b) Jet toy car http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=8NT0rIv7sIU
(c) Fast-food chain components rubber band-powered car http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=jmvqN3M1OoI
(d) Rubber band-powered helicopter http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=cIbGWDM3F6o
(e) Cup helicopter http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=mzUkzGy5P7c
(f) A simple rubber band-powered car http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=v3pbVAYkGf0
(g) Origami sailboat http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=ld_k-mVwHW0
(h) Water bottle boat http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_g48fJg_rCI;)
3. Have children use the arts projects that they just made as toys to invite others to play together
4. If there are some children who also want to do coloring, provide them with vehicle coloring sheets
5. Ask children to sit in a closing circle
6. Encourage them to show the group the arts projects they made and ask them what they have learned today
7. Announce the best helper of the week and have children say goodbye to each other and teachers
265
266 H.-M. Chiang

person with ASD.” This expression indicates that Cosden, M., Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Greenwell, A., &
Klein, E. (2006). Strength-based assessment for children
the abilities of an individual with ASD may be
with autism spectrum disorders. Research and Practice
different from another person with ASD. Thus, for Persons with Severe Disabilities, 31, 134–143.
being able to create the curriculum that best suits Duncan, A. W., & Bishop, S. L. (2015). Understanding
the needs of the intervention clients is important. the gap between cognitive abilities and daily liv-
ing skills in adolescents with autism spectrum dis-
This chapter describes the steps to develop a cur-
orders with average intelligence. Autism, 19, 64–72.
riculum and the strategies commonly used in doi:10.1177/1362361313510068
interventions for individuals with ASD. An Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (2008). Young adult outcome
example of a curriculum targeting multiple skills of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 38, 739–747. doi:10.1007/
is presented. Practitioners may find the informa-
s10803-007-0441-x
tion in this chapter is useful for teaching individ- Engstrom, I., Ekstrom, L., & Emilsson, B. (2003).
uals with ASD. Psychosocial functioning in a group of Swedish adults
with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism.
Autism, 7, 99–110. doi:10.1177/1362361303007001008
Farley, M.A., McMahon, W.M., Fombonne, E., Jenson,
References W.R., Miller, J., Gardner, M., ... Coon, H. (2009).
Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Research, 2, 109–118. doi:10.1002/aur.69
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Fiscus, E. D., & Mandell, C. J. (1983). Developing indi-
Bohnert, A., Fredricks, J., & Randall, E. (2010). vidualized education programs. St Paul, MN: West
Capturing unique dimensions of youth organized Publishing Company.
activity involvement: Theoretical and methodological Freeman, B. J., Rahbar, B., Ritvo, E. R., Bice, T. L.,
considerations. Review of Educational Research, 80, Yokota, A., & Ritvo, R. (1991). The stability of cogni-
576–610. doi:10.3102/0034654310364533 tive and behavioral parameters in autism: A 12-year
Bohnert, A. M., & Garber, J. (2007). Prospective relations prospective-study. Journal of the American Academy
between organized activity participation and psycho- of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 30, 479–482.
pathology during adolescence. Journal of Abnormal doi:10.1097/00004583-199105000-00020
Child Psychology, 35, 1021–1033. doi:10.1007/ Gardner, M., Roth, J., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2008).
s10802-007-9152-1 Adolescents’ participation in organized activities
Bolte, S., & Poustka, F. (2002). The relation between and developmental success 2 and 8 years after high
general cognitive level and adaptive behavior school: Do sponsorship, duration, and intensity
domains in individuals with autism with and with- matter? Developmental Psychology, 44, 814–830.
out co-morbid mental retardation. Child Psychiatry doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.3.814
and Human Development, 33, 165–172. doi:10.102 Guevremont, A., Findlay, L., & Kohen, D. (2014).
3/a:1020734325815 Organized extracurricular activities: Are in-school and
Chiang, H.-M. (2008). Expressive communication of chil- out-of-school activities associated with different out-
dren with autism: The use of challenging behaviour. comes for Canadian youth? Journal of School Health,
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 52, 966– 84, 317–325. doi:10.1111/josh.12154
972. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2008.01042.x Hickson, L., Blackman, L. S., & Reis, E. M. (1995).
Chiang, H.-M., & Lin, Y.-H. (2008). Expressive commu- Mental retardation: Foundations of educational pro-
nication of children with autism. Journal of Autism and gramming. Needham Heights, MA: Ally and Bacon.
Developmental Disorders, 38, 538–545. doi:10.1007/ Hjorthol, R., & Fyhri, A. (2009). Do organized lei-
s10803-007-0423-z sure activities for children encourage car-use?
Chiang, H. M., Cheung, Y. K., Hickson, L., Xiang, Transportation Research Part a-Policy and Practice,
R., & Tsai, L. Y. (2012). Predictive factors of par- 43, 209–218. doi:10.1016/j.tra.2008.11.005
ticipation in postsecondary education for high Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Todd, A. W., Newton, J. S.,
school leavers with autism. Journal of Autism and & Sprague, J. R. (2011). Designing and implement-
Developmental Disorders, 42, 685–696. doi:10.1007/ ing individualized positive behavior support. In M. E.
s10803-011-1297-7 Snell & F. Brown (Eds.), Instruction of students with
Chiang, H. M., Cheung, Y. K., Li, H. C., & Tsai, L. Y. severe disabilities (7th ed.pp. 257–303). Upper Saddle
(2013). Factors associated with participation in River, NJ: Pearson.
employment for high school leavers with autism. Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004).
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal
1832–1842. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1734-2 of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 212–229.
Christensen, L. B., Burke Johnson, R., & Turner, L. A. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x
(2011). Research methods, design, and analysis. Huebner, F. S., & Gilman, R. (2004). Perceived quality
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. of life: A neglected component of assessments and
15 Curriculums 267

intervention plans for students in school settings. Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1426–1438.
California School Psychologist, 9, 127–134. doi:10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1
Kjelgaard, M. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2001). An Kerr, M. M., & Nelson, C. M. (1989). Strategies for man-
investigation of language impairment in autism: aging behavior problems in the classroom (2nd ed.).
Implications for genetic subgroups. Language & Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308. Mahoney, J. L., & Vest, A. E. (2012). The over-­scheduling
Kjellmer, L., Hedvall, A., Holm, A., Fernell, E., Gillberg, hypothesis revisited: Intensity of organized activ-
C., & Norrelgen, F. (2012). Language comprehen- ity participation during adolescence and young adult
sion in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders outcomes. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 22,
without intellectual disability: Use of the Reynell 409–418. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2012.00808.x
developmental language scales. Research in Autism Maljaars, J., Noens, I., Scholte, E., & van Berckelaer-­
Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1119–1125. doi:10.1016/j. Onnes, I. (2012). Language in low-­ functioning
rasd.2012.03.003 children with autistic disorder: Differences
Klin, A., Saulnier, C. A., Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., between receptive and expressive skills and con-
Volkmar, F. R., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and com- current predictors of language. Journal of Autism
munication abilities and disabilities in higher func- and Developmental Disorders, 42, 2181–2191.
tioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders: doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1476-1
The Vineland and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Manolitsi, M., & Botting, N. (2011). Language abilities in
Developmental Disorders, 37, 748–759. doi:10.1007/ children with autism and language impairment: Using
s10803-006-0229-4 narrative as a additional source of clinical information.
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., Harrower, J. K., & Carter, Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 27, 39–55.
C. M. (1999). Pivotal response intervention I: doi:10.1177/0265659010369991
Overview of approach. Journal of the Association Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (2010). The TEACCH pro-
for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 24, 174–185. gram in the era of evidence-based practice. Journal of
doi:10.2511/rpsd.24.3.174 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 570–579.
Koegel, L. K., Singh, A. K., & Koegel, R. L. (2010). doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0901-6
Improving motivation for academics in children Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (2011). Evidence-based prac-
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental tices and autism. Autism, 15, 114–133.
Disorders, 40, 1057–1066. doi:10.1007/ Mesibov, G. B., Shea, V., & Schopler, E. (2004). The
s10803-010-0962-6 TEACCH approach to autism spectrum disorders.
Koegel, R. L., Carter, C. M., & Koegel, L. K. (1998). New York, NY: Springer.
Setting event to improve parent-teacher coordina- Ornstein, A. C., & Hinkins, F. P. (2013). Curriculum:
tion and motivation for children with autism. In J. K. Foundations, principles, and issues (6th ed.). Saddle
Luiselli & M. J. Cameron (Eds.), Antecedent con- River, NJ: Pearson.
trol: Innovative approaches to behavioral support Public Law 108–446. (2004). Individuals with disabili-
(pp. 167–186). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes. ties education act amendments of 2004,PL 108–446,
Koegel, R. L., & Koegel, L. K. (2012). The PRT pocket et seq.
guide: Pivotal response treatment for autism spec- Randall, E. T., & Bohnert, A. M. (2009). Organized
trum disorders. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes activity involvement, depressive symptoms, and
Publishing Co. social adjustment in adolescents: Ethnicity and socio-
Lindgren, K. A., Folstein, S. E., Tomblin, J. B., & Tager-­ economic status as moderators. Journal of Youth
Flusberg, H. (2009). Language and reading abilities of and Adolescence, 38, 1187–1198. doi:10.1007/
children with autism spectrum disorders and specific s10964-009-9417-9
language impairment and their first-degree relatives. Rusch, F. R., Rose, T., & Greenwood, C. R. (1988).
Autism Research, 2, 22–38. doi:10.1002/aur.63 Instruction to behavior analysis in special education.
Liss, M., Harel, B., Fein, D., Allen, D., Dunn, M., Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Feinstein, C., ... Rapin, I. (2001). Predictors and Sailor, W., & Guess, D. (1983). Severely handicapped
correlates of adaptive functioning in children with students: An instructional design. Boston, MA:
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Houghton Miffin.
Developmental Disorders, 31, 219–230. doi:10.102 Saulnier, C. A., & Klin, A. (2007). Brief report:
3/a:1010707417274. Social and communication abilities and disabili-
Lloyd, H., Paintin, K., & Botting, N. (2006). Performance ties in higher functioning individuals with autism
of children with different types of communication and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and
impairment on the clinical evaluation of language Developmental Disorders, 37, 788–793. doi:10.1007/
fundamentals (CELF). Child Language Teaching and s10803-006-0288-6
Therapy, 22, 47–67. Selmi, A. M., Gallagher, R. J., & Mora-Flores, E. R.
Luyster, R. J., Kadlec, M. B., Carter, A., & Tager-Flusberg, (2015). Early childhood curriculum for all learners:
H. (2008). Language assessment and development in Integrating play and literacy activities. Thousand
toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
268 H.-M. Chiang

Snell, M. E., & Brown, F. (2011). Selecting teaching Wagner, M., Cadwallader, T.W., Garza, N., & Cameto,
strategies and arranging educational environments. In R. (2004, 12/13/2007). Social activities of youth with
M. E. Snell & F. Brown (Eds.), Instruction of students disabilities Retrieved 03/18, 2015, from http://www.
with severe disabilities (7th ed., pp. 122–185). Upper ncset.org/publications/viewdesc.asp?id=1470
Saddle River, NJ: Pearson. Weismer, S. E., Lord, C., & Esler, A. (2010). Early lan-
Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005). guage patterns of toddlers on the autism spectrum
Vineland aaptive behavior scales: Survey forms man- compared to toddlers with developmental delay.
ual (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40,
Stokes, T. F., & Osnes, P. G. (1986). Programming the 1259–1273. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-0983-1
generalization of children’s social behavior. In P. S. Westling, D. L., & Fox, L. (2009). Teaching students with
Strain, M. Guralnick, & H. Walker (Eds.), Children’s severe disabilities. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson
social behavior: Development, assessment and modi- Education.
fication. Orlando, FL: Academic. Wingate, M., Kirby, R.S., Pettygrove, S., Cunniff, C.,
Thurm, A., Lord, C., Lee, L.-C., & Newschaffer, C. Schulz, E., Ghosh, T., ... Autism Dev Disabilities, M.
(2007). Predictors of language acquisition in preschool (2014). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of children aged 8 years: Autism and developmental dis-
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1721–1734. abilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States,
doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0300-1 2010. MMWR Surveillance Summaries, 63(2).
Tomanik, S. S., Pearson, D. A., Loveland, K. A., Lane, Wong, C., Odom, S. L., Hume, K., Cox, A. W., Fettig,
D. M., & Shaw, J. B. (2007). Improving the reli- A., Kucharczyk, S., & Schultz, T. R. (2013). Evidence-­
ability of autism diagnoses: Examining the util- based practices for children, youth, and young adults
ity of adaptive behavior. Journal of Autism and with autism spectrum disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: The
Developmental Disorders, 37, 921–928. doi:10.1007/ University of North Carolina, Frank Porter Graham
s10803-006-0227-6 Child Development Institute.
Augmentative and Alternative
Communication and Autism 16
Daphne Hartzheim

AAC options. The focus of this chapter is on


Introduction ­individuals with ASD, whose motoric abilities
are typically sufficient to directly operate differ-
This chapter provides an overview of alternative ent types of devices but whose verbal abilities are
methods of communication that can guide clini- insufficient for conventional communication.
cians in their work with individuals with an While the emphasis in characterizing individuals
autism spectrum disorder (ASD). The informa- with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) no longer
tion included in this chapter is meant to stimulate lies in a language disorder but focused on the social
and encourage the reader to consider communi- communication and reciprocity, it is estimated that
cation from a broad perspective outside of con- 30% of all individuals with ASD are either prever-
ventional verbal speech. Effective communication bal, remain nonverbal or minimally verbal, or need
occurs in a variety of ways. This chapter intro- augmented input to enhance comprehension of lan-
duces the reader into a unique and alternative guage (Wodka, Mathy & Kalb, 2013). Augmentative
way of communication and explains what AAC and alternative communication (AAC) aims to give
is. Different types of AAC systems will be these individuals a voice and enhance their lan-
defined and explained. Further, the chapter will guage comprehension. This chapter provides an
guide a clinician in their decision-making pro- overview of the most frequently used AAC devices,
cess concerning which AAC system to choose systems, and evidence-based interventions for those
and how to functionally implement the system diagnosed with an ASD.
into the natural environment. While this chapter
familiarizes the reader with introductory knowl-
edge, AAC encompasses a wide range of tech- What Is AAC?
nologies and is appropriate for many different
populations with diverse disorders (e.g., amyo- According to the International Society of
trophic lateral sclerosis, dysarthria, Parkinson’s Augmentative and Alternative Communication
disease, cerebral palsy, Down syndrome). (ISAAC), AAC is defined as a set of tools and
However, this chapter does not cover all possible strategies that an individual uses to solve every-
day communicative challenges. The mode in
which communication occurs is secondary, as
D. Hartzheim (*)
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA long as the intent and meaning are understood by
e-mail: dhartz4@lsu.edu the ­ communication partners. The mode of

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 269


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_16
270 D. Hartzheim

c­ommunication can be speech, text, gestures, appears to lie with aided AAC systems (van der
body language, touch, sign language, symbols, Meer, Sigafoos, O’Reilly & Lancioni, 2011).
pictures, speech-generating devices, etc. Low-technology (low-tech) AAC systems
Everyone uses multiple forms of communication, include anything that does not require battery
depending on the context and our communication operation, as simple as paper and pencil and
partner (What is AAC? 2016). alphabet boards. Sophisticated and well-­
While the definition of ISAAC focuses pri- manualized systems such as the Picture Exchange
marily on the different modes of communication, Communication System® (PECS®) and visual
the American Speech-Language-Hearing schedules are considered low-tech AAC.
Association (ASHA) released a position state- AAC systems with medium technology (mid-­
ment on AAC including the “why” of choosing tech) are those static devices which have the abil-
an alternative way to communicate. They state ity to store prerecorded messages, ranging from a
that AAC is an area of research, clinical, and edu- single message to multiple levels of messages to
cational practice. It involves attempts to study multiple messages on multiple levels. A care-
and when necessary compensate for temporary or giver, therapist, or anyone else interacting with
permanent impairments, activity limitations, and the individual can record those messages.
participation restrictions of individuals with Examples of mid-tech AAC systems are single-­
severe disorders of speech-language production button devices such as a BIGmack™, LITTLE
and/or comprehension, including spoken and Step-by-Step™, and GoTalk™.
written modes of communication. Incredible advances have been made with
“It is the position of the American Speech-­ high-technology (high-tech) AAC systems,
Language-­ Hearing Association that communica- which are dynamic display speech-generating
tion is the essence of human life and that all people devices (SGD). Upon direct selection or switch
have the right to communicate to the fullest extent activation of a cell or button, the device either
possible. No individuals should be denied this
right, irrespective of the type and/or severity of speaks the word or navigates to another level for
communication, linguistic, social, cognitive, more vocabulary words. Each device is pro-
motor, sensory, perceptual, and/or other disabili- grammed with a certain number of cells on each
ties they may present.” (Augmentative and page. Depending on the user, this can range from
Alternative Communication, 2016)
a grid of 2 × 2, totaling four cells or buttons on
Based on these two definitions, AAC is not one page but can extend to a 12 × 12 grid, totaling
only a research, clinical, and educational practice 144 cells. Usually, the SGDs come prepro-
but a means to provide an individual with the grammed with a language system for beginner,
basic human need for communication and con- intermediate, or advanced vocabulary options.
nection, regardless of the mode. With the contin- With developing language skills, the SGD can be
uation of this chapter, it is important to keep this adapted to the individual’s language needs. The
basic human need in mind, regardless of AAC user selects either one symbol or a combination
being a temporary or a permanent solution. of multiple symbols from the possible cells and a
When speaking about AAC, we differentiate digitized voice outputs the message. SGDs can
between unaided and aided AAC. Unaided AAC be designated communication devices that have
encompasses those modes that do not require the primarily been developed for communication
use of external materials, such as sign language, purposes. They usually allow for a number of dif-
touch, body language, facial expressions, ges- ferent access methods, such as activating a button
tures, etc. Aided AAC systems on the other hand with a switch or with eye gaze rather than a fin-
utilize external materials ranging from low-tech ger. Tablets and iPads® with designated commu-
systems to mid-tech systems to high-tech devices. nication applications are now frequently used by
In this chapter the focus will be on aided AAC individuals with ASD, if motoric abilities are suf-
systems. While manual signs have shown some ficient that no alternative access method is neces-
success with children with ASD, their preference sary. Tablets and iPads® are making SGDs more
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 271

commonplace, and social acceptance for alterna- (a) Communication autonomy: individuals
tive communication methods is increasing. These should be able to express themselves in
technologies should be a serious consideration regard to their own intentions (Von Tetzchner
when working with nonverbal individuals with & Grove, 2003).
ASD. Additionally, such devices have the poten- (b) Communication accessibility: familiar and
tial of greatly enhancing comprehension for ver- unfamiliar people in the environment under-
bal and nonverbal people with ASD. The stand and support the use of AAC form (Von
remainder of the chapter is used to discuss ASD-­ Tetzchner & Grove, 2003).
specific AAC considerations. Keep in mind how- (c) Communication competence: the individual
ever that this is a rapidly changing area and is able to demonstrate sufficient linguistic,
therefore new technology is always being operational, social, and strategic knowledge,
developed. judgment, and skill (Light, Beukelman &
Reichle, 2003).

 AC and Autism Spectrum
A Using AAC with the ASD population is a
Disorders well-established possibility, but the options are
wide-ranging to achieve the functional communi-
Given the core deficit of communication and the cation goals. First, an AAC assessment has to be
high percentage of individuals with ASD remain- conducted to determine the most appropriate,
ing nonverbal or minimally verbal throughout individualized system for the user. Second, the
their life, use of AAC is inevitable with this pop- system is implemented to achieve functional out-
ulation. Despite the use of AAC with individuals comes. A discussion of the assessment process
with expressive communication deficits, children and different types of interventions follow to
who are considered verbal or vocal can benefit accomplish the three long-term goals.
from AAC as well. The predominantly visual
systems are useful to enhance comprehension
and to also support expression when necessary. AAC Assessment
Communicating basic wants and needs can aid in
decreasing challenging behavior of people with Up to date, no autism-specific AAC assessment
ASD. Further, being able to communicate has the has been developed. An AAC assessment is dif-
potential to increase quality of life for the indi- ferent from other types of standardized assess-
vidual using the AAC system. Moreover, people ments. AAC assessments are focused on
in the AAC user’s environment can benefit from behavioral observations without standardiza-
successful communication. They may no longer tion. During the assessment, the clinician trials
have to guess about the wants and needs of the a variety of AAC systems, ranging from low to
person using the device. The use of AAC for lan- high tech, and records successful communica-
guage facilitation and communication is an tion trials. Complexity is increased with the
accepted practice for individuals with ASD. completion of a successful communication
However, the individual need must be deter- trial. For a high-­tech AAC system, different
mined. The design and implementation of AAC communication systems and applications are
systems must support functional independent trialed including designated AAC systems
communication (Porter & Cafiero, 2009). Such (Tobii/Dynavox®, PRC®, iPad® and tablet
independent functional communication should applications, and others).
occur in a variety of settings (e.g., school, home, A team comprised of those interacting and
grocery store, restaurant, etc.) with a number of working with the individual is involved in the
different people (e.g., caregivers, siblings, family assessment process. The team can be made up
members, teachers, peers, etc.). Long-term func- of the client and their family, therapists (speech-­
tional outcomes should focus on: language pathologist, occupational therapist,
272 D. Hartzheim

Table 16.1  Roles of assessment team members


Team member Potential roles during assessment
Client Determines preference of AAC system
Programs the device if possible
Determines preferences of activities
Family Chooses appropriate device
Advocates for the client
Involves in activities of daily living
Develops vocabulary
Speech-language pathologist Assesses linguistic abilities (receptive and expressive language)
Assesses communicative functions
Assesses ideal symbol representation
Assesses communicative opportunities
Assesses communicative barriers
Determines future direction of communication needs
Occupational therapist Determines sensory needs
Assesses fine motor skills for accessibility of AAC system
Assesses ideal positioning
Determines future direction of motor skills
Physical therapist Assesses motor skills for accessibility of AAC system
Assesses ideal positioning
Determines future direction of motor skills
Behavior therapist Assesses function of challenging behavior
Determines prompting hierarchies
Determines preference of items with reinforcing properties
Develops generalization strategies
Psychologist Determines impact of AAC system on daily living of client and family
Assesses comorbidities
Assesses cognitive skills
Physician Assesses and prognoses of disorders
Diagnoses motor and sensory skills
Prescribes AAC system
Social worker Coordinates services
Counsels with client and family
Teacher Determines vocabulary needs
Establishes interaction profiles
Determines academic barriers
Determines future direction of needs

physical therapist, and behavioral therapist), Together, they are involved in the process of
psychologist, physician, social worker, teacher, gathering information and analyzing all relevant
etc. The team approach is crucial to ensure suc- knowledge to be able to make informed decisions
cess in the use of the AAC system across all about the adequacy of current communication, com-
environments and to generalize and maintain munication needs, AAC equipment, instructions,
skills acquired during interventions. Potential and outcomes (Mirenda, 2001). According to Light,
roles of each team member are displayed in Roberts, DiMarco, and Greiner (1998), five primary
Table 16.1. components are included in an AAC assessment:
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 273

1. Identification of communication needs: and to enhance their linguistic abilities. The


This portion of the assessment refers to focus is on functionality and independence.
detecting all situations in which communica- 5 . Intervention planning for the facilitators,
tion could occur, but where it does not due to including instruction in interaction strategies
a number of reasons, such as lack of motiva- to facilitate communication and instruction in
tion, lack of communication skills, or lack of the operation and ongoing development of the
opportunity. These needs can be all encom- AAC system:
passing for every situation and every potential This refers to teaching communication
communication partner, or they can be limited partners how to generalize communication to
to certain conditions (e.g., school, recess, ALL situations of daily living. If the family or
home, work). caregiver of the AAC user is involved in the
2. Assessment of skills (i.e., receptive language, implementation of the system, he or she will
expressive communication, natural speech, show more success across different situations.
symbol representation skills, literacy skills,
cognitive organization, positioning and access Ultimately, these five components function as
skills, sensory perceptual skills as they relate guiding principles for the overall AAC
to AAC): ­assessment. Overall, an AAC assessment should
During the skill assessment, standardized center around an individual’s ability to partici-
procedures can be employed such as a lan- pate in age-appropriate, everyday life activities.
guage assessment. Often, observational mea- This type of assessment and intervention model
sures and caregiver reports are useful to is based on the Participation Model described by
determine the current level of functioning. It Mirenda (2001). The Participation Model pro-
is during this ongoing skill assessment that vides a systematic way to conduct an AAC
different types of AAC systems can be trialed. assessment. It takes into consideration aspects of
The team is consulted for different skills. age-appropriate participation of the individual as
3. Identification of partner interaction strategies well as skills and knowledge of support person-
that facilitate communication and of environ- nel such as family, teachers, and therapist.
mental barriers that impede communication: Assessment based on the Participation Model
Each individual has a unique interaction occurs in four phases (referral, initial assessment
style. Some foster communication more than and intervention planning, assessment for tomor-
others and some limit communication. row, and follow-up assessment) (Beukelman &
Observation of current interactions with dif- Mirenda, 2013).
ferent communication partners can provide
valuable insight into how AAC can be imple-
mented. It is even possible that due to a his-  hase I: Referral for Augmentative
P
tory of unsuccessful communication attempts, and Alternative Communication
an individual may no longer have an opportu- Assessment
nity to interact with peers because he or she is
no longer around peers. Depending on the availability of qualified AAC
4. Intervention planning, including the develop- specialists, this phase can be brief or lengthy.
ment of appropriate AAC systems and instruc- Typically, therapists or teachers initiate the refer-
tion in skills to enhance communicative ral. Lack of effective communication resulting in
competence: social isolation, potential challenging behavior,
During this portion the assessor, together and stagnation of academic progress can trigger
with the team, establishes goals for the AAC referral to an AAC specialist. Usually, if the
user to independently operate the system, to teacher or therapist themselves are not proficient
learn functional communication strategies, in providing an AAC assessment, they will assist
274 D. Hartzheim

the family in finding appropriate resources. verbally prompted by an adult. She is p­ articipating
They may provide valuable information to the with verbal assistance in this particular interac-
AAC specialist regarding application for funding tion. While this girl needs support to interact with
of an AAC system (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). her peers, she at least has the opportunity to do
so. At times a number of barriers to communica-
tion can hinder successful participation in the
Phase II: Initial Assessment environment. The Participation Model differenti-
ates between opportunity barriers and access bar-
This phase is designed to gather information on riers (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013).
current skill levels and to develop interventions Opportunity barriers are those barriers that
based on the present level of functioning. In a hinder the AAC user from typical interactions
team effort the child with ASD receives a holistic with peers and others. Those can be due to certain
assessment. Cognitive, linguistic, physical, lan- policies, lack of practice, knowledge, skill, or
guage, and sensory capabilities are evaluated by negative attitude toward alternative means of
qualified members of the AAC team. Additionally, communication.
the AAC specialist assesses current communica- A policy barrier can be a formal legislative
tion and interaction patterns of the potential user regulation that, for example, separates a student
and barriers that interfere with age-appropriate with disabilities from their peers by placing them
interactions. Creating a participation profile can in isolated classrooms. Lack of resources for stu-
assist in analyzing interaction patterns and com- dent support may cause a school district to create
pare them to their peers. policies causing a student to be bused to a school
Creating a participation profile begins with outside their typical boundaries, therefore isolat-
identifying all possible situations and people that ing the student from children in their own neigh-
the individual regularly encounters, which can be borhood. These policies lead to social isolation of
at home, school, and any other setting, with par- the student and therefore to a lack of opportunity
ents, caregivers, siblings, extended family, to interact with peers.
friends, therapists, unfamiliar people, etc. It also A practice barrier refers to such barriers that
includes what types of activities the individual occur if we “get stuck in old ways.” At times, it
would most likely be involved in. When analyz- may occur that long-standing habits on how things
ing the interactions of the potential AAC user, it have been done in the past are difficult to disrupt,
is crucial to also observe typically developing regardless of family, school, or workplace.
peers. It gives insight into what their peers are Practice barriers can certainly be a result of
interested in, how they interact with each other, knowledge barriers, which are a lack of informa-
and what might be inappropriate behavior. Such tion on the side of the clinician about the different
observations should be with a representative peer types of systems that are available and how each
example (i.e., same age, gender, social environ- system works. It is not possible to teach some-
ment) for accurate interaction characteristics. thing without first mastering at least the basics.
Following the peer analysis of interaction and Knowledge barriers, including AAC interven-
communication patterns, the individual with tion options, technologies, and intervention strat-
ASD should be observed to identify whether he egies, may be the most challenging barrier to
or she is participating spontaneously and without overcome. Contrarily, it may become less chal-
assistance, with assistance from others, or not at lenging with AAC technology moving into more
all. For example, a 5-year-old girl with ASD mainstream technology (e.g., tablets, laptops).
might be in the same room as her peers when The designated communication applications may
playing with a pretend kitchen set. She may be still leave therapists intimidated and unsure of
seated at the table while her two friends are pre- how to apply them in interventions. Such barriers
paring the meal. She, however, plays no active would be considered skill barriers, in which a
part in the interaction with the two friends unless therapist may have learned about different types
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 275

of systems and have learned appropriate software determine expressive and receptive language
navigation but still lacks the skill and practice to skills but also explore precursor skills to lan-
implement these systems functionally. guage development, such as use of gestures,
Another opportunity barrier is the attitude and facial expression and body language, joint atten-
beliefs held by not only the potential AAC user tion, imitation skills, initiation of communicative
but also family members, therapists, and teachers. acts, and different communicative intents.
Subtle beliefs that an AAC system is inappropri- Additionally, the Verbal Behavior Milestones
ate need to be addressed with high sensitivity to Assessment and Placement Program (VB-MAPP,
overcome such barriers. They can, however, be Sundberg, 2008) can add valuable information
extremely disruptive to the successful implemen- regarding barriers to communication based on its
tation of an AAC system. It is therefore important barrier assessment. Compared to traditional lan-
to consult with all members of the assessment and guage assessments, this tool was developed from
intervention team in the decision-­making process a theoretical background of applied behavior
about which AAC system to choose. It can foster analysis (ABA) and verbal behavior. In this
conversations about attitudes and may provide a regard, it looks at communication patterns but
forum to prevent future conflict and frustration in from a behavioral viewpoint as opposed to a
regard to the implementation of the AAC system developmental stand. Language and communica-
(Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). tion from the perspective of a behavior analyst
Different from opportunity barriers, access are no different than any other behavior and are
barriers pertain to the individual’s capacity, atti- shaped through antecedent conditions and conse-
tude, and resources to contact an AAC system quences. No evaluation should rely on results of
and communicate with such a system (Beukelman a single assessment tool but look at skills holisti-
& Mirenda, 2013). While mobility can be a chal- cally. It should also be noted that while children
lenge for an individual with ASD, it is not typi- with verbal communication may not require an
cally a main concern for AAC selection, since entire AAC system, they may have difficulty
most display abilities of navigating a tablet or comprehending acoustic-only signals and there-
grabbing objects independently. A discussion on fore benefit from some of the visual enhancement
AAC system preference later in this chapter pro- strategies discussed later in this chapter.
vides insight to different reasons for considering
multiple AAC options in the assessment phase.
During this second phase of the assessment Phase III: Assessment for Tomorrow
and in accordance with the Participation Model,
individuals with ASD should be evaluated in This portion of the assessment is dedicated to
regard to their current level of communication establish a robust AAC system that can grow
and their potential for verbal speech. A trained with the individual and their skill level, as well as
speech-language pathologist should perform this be appropriate for ALL environments and inter-
portion of the assessment. They can choose from actions (Beukelman & Mirenda, 2013). For
a variety of different direct and indirect speech example, if a preschool child with ASD is using a
and language assessment tools, ranging from SGD, it should be programmed with a software
observation, norm-referenced, criterion-­system that can increase in vocabulary size and
referenced, and standardized assessments. be used in an academic setting, home, and social
Besides tests used for evaluating ASD itself, typi- setting. When choosing such a system, it should
cal language assessments for a child that does not be noted that with changes in the vocabulary size,
have verbal language are the Communication and sometimes the page overlay changes with differ-
Symbolic Behavior Scale (CSBS, Wetherby & ent cell sizes and grid setups. While this may not
Prizant, 2003), Rossetti Infant-Toddler Language cause any difficulty for some children, such
Scale (Rossetti, 1990), and the Communication changes can be problematic if the child struggles
Matrix (Rowland, 1996). These tools not only with changes in routines and has been proficient
276 D. Hartzheim

with the system prior to the overlay change. It discuss intervention based on those two systems.
could cause a regression of previously mastered An emphasis is placed on aided AAC for indi-
communication. Therefore, choosing a system viduals with ASD.
that accounts for growth from the outset is As a general rule, intervention should be
important. driven by an individual’s daily communication
needs at home, school, and the social community.
First, the type of input the individual requires for
Phase IV: Follow-Up Assessment enhanced linguistic comprehension needs to be
determined. Then, establishing with whom,
During a follow-up assessment of the AAC sys- where, when, why, about what, and how the indi-
tem that was chosen during phase II, the AAC vidual needs to communicate follows. It is impor-
specialist now evaluates if changes need to be tant to build the intervention based on the answers
made due to changes in communication and to these questions (Light et al., 1998).
interaction patterns (Beukelman & Mirenda, Individual intervention planning has to be
2013). If a child with ASD has become indepen- based on finding a successful way to provide
dent in using their system in one subject at school, every individual with ASD with a viable, robust,
it is time to generalize to all school subjects. The flexible, and generative communication system.
AAC system however may need to be modified The system should support language in the long-­
for those different classes. In another situation, term if necessary. The focus needs to be on pre-
the child may use their system in one social situ- suming competence and paying little attention to
ation at home with siblings but not with children past failures to communicate. We capitalize on
at school. This could be because the child has not specific strengths (e.g., visual memory and visual
acquired this skill to interact with peers at school processing), and we should focus on compensa-
yet, but it could also be due to lack of access to tory supports to address specific limitations (e.g.,
appropriate vocabulary on the AAC device. difficulties with comprehension of social markers
Follow-up is dependent on the growth curve of and linguistic information). Establishing a suc-
the AAC user, as well as others interacting with cessful AAC option for someone with ASD is a
the child. If the caregivers are proficient in adapt- continued innovative process, in which we never
ing AAC systems, follow-up appointments may stop to search for the solution to barriers. This
need to be less frequent. However, caregivers requires the continued team approach throughout
cannot replace professional clinical decision-­ intervention and beyond. Our overall goal is to
making and appropriate developmental facilitate an individual’s ability either to commu-
judgment. nicate more effectively with others or understand
communication from others (Mirenda, 2001,
2008).
AAC Intervention

After the thorough AAC assessment has been Aided AAC Systems
completed, goals need to be formulated for oper-
ational abilities of the AAC system, for an Aided AAC systems for individuals with autism
increase in linguistic skills, to enhance social range from low-tech to high-tech techniques; the
engagement and to learn strategies to repair com- most common low tech being PECS® or other
munication breakdowns (Light et al., 1998). picture-based interventions. SGDs are also used
After developing goals in cooperation with the frequently with individuals with autism.
family and the individual, different intervention Interventions have been shown to have a moder-
approaches have been shown to be effective for ate to high treatment effect on participants across
different types of AAC systems. Since we differ- all ages, diagnostic criteria, and intervention set-
entiate between aided and unaided AAC, we will tings. It is worthwhile to implement AAC across
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 277

the lifespan. The most common and most and home settings (Peterson et al., 1995). Positive
research-supported interventions are PECS® and results have been reported with getting dressed
treatments with SGDs (Ganz et al., 2012). (Pierce & Schreibman, 1994), independent writ-
Besides those most researched approaches, visual ing tasks (Hall, McClannahan, & Krantz, 1995),
schedules (i.e., daily schedules, activity sched- transitioning between two activities (Flannery &
ules), and modeling interventions will also be Horner, 1994; Krantz, MacDuff & McClannahan,
discussed. 1993; MacDuff, Krantz & McClannahan, 1993),
and independent play and joint play (Brodhead,
Higbee, Pollard, Akers, & Gerencser, 2014).
 isual and Picture-Based
V Visual-graphic daily schedules, using pictures
Interventions with text, text alone, or pictures alone, have
effected positive behaviors and self-management
Picture symbols can be used for a variety of inter- in Treatment and Education of Autistic and
vention techniques, such as functional communi- Related Communication in Handicapped
cation training (FCT), visual schedules, and Children (TEACCH) (Watson, Lord, Schaffer, &
choice-making. FCT refers to a set of procedures Schopler, 1989). Schedules have decreased
designed to specifically reduce challenging problem behaviors and increased independent
­
behavior by establishing the function of such self-­management (MacDuff et al., 1993).
behavior followed by teaching functionally A daily schedule in a classroom should be
equivalent communication skills (Peterson, placed in a designated area that is easily seen and
Bondy, Vincent & Finnegan, 1995; Vaughan & reached by the individual and the teacher. The
Horner, 1995). FCT requires a thorough assess- schedule can be an overview of the daily class-
ment to identify the function of the behavior of room routines or the progression of a single
concern, referred to as functional behavior activity. To ensure flexibility with different daily
assessment (FBA). Usually, such a FBA is con- routines, the pictures are commonly attached to
ducted by a trained Board Certified Behavior the schedule with Velcro. At the beginning of the
Analyst (BCBA). At the conclusion of the FBA day, the teacher explains the routine of the day.
and identification of the function of challenging He or she points to each picture on the schedule
behavior, systematic instruction related to teach- and explains in simple terms what each picture
ing functionally related alternative communica- represents. At the beginning of the activity or
tive behaviors commences. FCT interventions lesson, the teacher again points to the icon repre-
have the clear advantage in that they teach indi- senting the activity. Once an activity is complete,
viduals to communicate one or more functional the teacher or the student takes the picture of the
messages while at the same time providing posi- schedule and places it in a designated “finished”
tive alternatives to their problem behavior spot. This can be a “finished” column to the right
(Peterson, et al., 1995). A full discussion of FCT of the activity, or it can be a “finished” envelope
does not fall within the scope of this chapter. For at the bottom of the schedule. This procedure is
further reading on FCT, refer to Carr and Durand followed for each activity of the day. It is impor-
(1985), Fisher et al. (1993), Durand and Carr tant to be consistent in accompanying verbal out-
(1991), Durand (1999), and Durand (1990). put with the visual stimuli to ensure
Visual schedules come in many different comprehension and increase the student’s ability
styles and formats and can be used in a wide vari- to transition. The schedules can be individual-
ety of daily activities. The schedule displays a ized to the specific classroom situation in place-
sequence of activities either through pictures or ment, size, and visual enhancements. However,
written words. Schedules can promote indepen- caution needs to be taken not to make the sched-
dent on-task behavior and assist individuals in ules too elaborate and complicated and uninten-
understanding, following direction, and transi- tionally distracting from the actual purpose of
tioning predictable activity sequences in school the schedule. Pictures should be clearly visible
278 D. Hartzheim

Fig. 16.1  Visual schedule in the classroom

from the background and have simple photo line


drawings or easily readable words. Schedules
can be used for classroom routines as well as
specific activities, such as baking cookies. A
baking activity may be done in a group or indi-
vidually. The teacher again has a schedule pre-
pared for each step of the baking process (i.e.,
getting the ingredients, getting the tools, adding
of the ingredients step-by-step, preheating the
oven, mixing everything together, placing the
cookies on the cooking sheet and in the oven,
and cooling down). At the beginning of the activ-
ity, the teacher goes through the entire schedule
by pointing to each symbol and explaining each
step. At each level of the schedule, the teacher
points to the appropriate picture. An example of
a classroom visual schedule is displayed in
Fig. 16.1. Figure 16.2 shows an activity-specific
visual schedule of making chocolate chip cook-
ies. Schedules can be used for any routine-based
activity or in any environment. Figure 16.3 dis-
plays examples of visual schedules at home. Fig. 16.2  Visual schedule making cookies
They support children with ASD transitioning
between different activities and a variety of envi-
ronments. Enhancing receptive language with daily routines. An activity schedule is a 5 ×
visuals further aids language development. 8.5 in. binder with different pages in sheet pro-
Portable visual activity schedules are imple- tectors. Each page has one or multiple activities
mented to support independent play or other on it. For example, to promote independent play,
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 279

Fig. 16.3  Visual schedule at home

each page has one symbol displaying either a peer to play. Sophisticated joint activity sched-
closed or open-ended activity. The child is taught ules have even been used to teach children to play
through physical guidance to get the book, open hide-and-seek (Brodhead, Higbee, Pollard,
it, and point to the symbol on the first page. He or Akers, and Gerencser 2014). Portable activity
she then gets the toy and plays with it. If the sym- schedules promote independence and can be used
bol displays a close-ended activity (e.g., stacking for play as well as daily routines (e.g., bedtime,
toys or puzzle), the child returns the toy to its getting ready for school). See Fig. 16.4 for an
original location. If the symbol depicts an open-­ example of a portable visual activity schedule for
ended activity, a time frame has to be indicated independent play and bedtime routine.
with the symbol. This can be done in the form of Transitioning between activities or locations
numbers or colored dots. Again, the child is can be especially difficult for some individuals
taught to activate a timer with the appropriate with autism. A simple visual as displayed in
time frame through physical guidance. After set- Fig. 16.5 can aid transitioning between two dif-
ting the time, the child plays with the toy until the ferent activities. The teacher or therapist first
timer goes off, at which point the child returns points to what is required at this point and then a
the toy to its original location and returns to the reinforcer after successful transitioning. With all
activity schedule book. This is repeated for a of the visual enhancement strategies, simple lan-
number of toys until the last page. Here, a rein- guage is used to accompany the visual stimuli.
forcer is depicted and the child receives the rein- Visual symbols can further augment input
forcement with completion of the entire related to choice-making by individuals with
schedule. ASD. For this purpose, pictures of different
Initially, during training, the child can be choices are displayed. The child is asked to
physically guided through each activity, but choose one of the items. The visual support
prompting should be faded as quickly as possi- assists language comprehension and helps the
ble. Depending on the child, it may be necessary individual to successfully communicate prefer-
to start with one closed-ended activity. Later on ences. Opportunities for choice appear to be
the activity schedule can also support peer inter- effective in decreasing problem behavior (Frea,
action. One page can be dedicated to choosing a Arnold, & Vittimberga, 2001).
280 D. Hartzheim

Fig. 16.4  Activity schedule

ultimately lead to communication in daily life.


The learning activities should be incorporated
into daily functional activities of the individual.
Further, teaching should incorporate powerful
reinforcers, such as highly preferred snacks, toys,
and activities. Building upon this base, PECS
includes the “how” of teaching communication.
Error correction, a variety of evidence-based
teaching strategies, effective lesson plans, and
generalization methods are systematically inte-
grated into the six-phase manualized AAC
­system (Frost et al., 2002). While traditional ver-
bal imitation approaches often result in prompt-­
Fig. 16.5  Now-then visual transitioning dependent and non-spontaneous speech
(Schreibman, 2006), PECS relies on child-­
 icture Exchange Communication
P initiated communication and systematic fading of
System® (PECS)® physical prompts to minimize prompt depen-
dence (Ganz, Simpson, & Corbin-Newsome,
PECS is a low-tech AAC system that relies on 2008). Moreover, traditional language instruction
laminated picture symbols and a sturdy binder wherein students are taught to label or comment
containing pictures that are attached with hook on objects and to describe communicative func-
and loop fastener such as Velcro®. It is based on tions often holds limited motivation for children
principles of verbal behavior from applied behav- with ASD (Charlop-Christy & LeBlanc, 2001).
ior analytic (ABA) principles. The philosophy PECS provides concrete visual reminders of con-
behind PECS lies in the Pyramid Approach to crete objects available for requesting. Also,
Education®. At the base of teaching lie func- learners need to use only a small number of sim-
tional activities, powerful reinforcers, functional ple motor movements to request a variety of
communication, and contextually inappropriate items. Finally, the only prerequisite for PECS is
behaviors. In essence it comprises the science of to have a reinforcer, such as a favorite toy, activ-
learning and why learning occurs (Frost & ity, or food. PECS is certainly suited for individu-
Bondy, 2002). Given this basis, PECS should als with ASD (Ganz et al., 2008).
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 281

Picture symbols for the PECS system can be Table 16.2 Picture exchange communication system
phases
photographs of items with a solid background or
they can be colored line drawings from a picture Phases Description
communication system (PCS) database. The Phase I (how to Students learn to exchange
communicate) single pictures for items or
sizes of the pictures can vary depending on per-
activities they highly prefer
ceptual and motor abilities of the user. PECS is
Phase II (distance Still using single pictures,
taught over six main phases. The phases and a and persistence) students learn to generalize
short description of each phase is described in new skill by using it with
Table  16.2 (Frost et al., 2002) with pictures of different places, with different
people, and across distances.
PECS books in Fig. 16.6.
They are also taught to be more
PECS has shown to be effective across a vari- persistent communicators
ety of ages (i.e., preschool age, elementary school Phase III (picture Students learn to discriminate
age, and adults). PECS training has resulted in discrimination) between two or more pictures
decreased tantrums and other problem behaviors by selecting their favorite item.
The pictures are placed in a
(Charlop-Christy et al., 2002; Frea, Arnold, communication binder
Vittimberga, & Koegel, 2001); increased use of Phase IV (sentence Students learn to construct
requests incorporating function, color, and shape structure) simple sentences on a Sentence
(Marckel, Neef, & Ferreri, 2006); improved social Strip™ by combining an “I
interactions; and improved skill generalization want” symbol with a preferred
item that is being requested
(Ganz, Sigafoos, Simpson, & Cook, 2008).
Attributes and Students increase the length of
language expansion their sentence by adding
attributes, such as adjectives,
 ragmatic Organization Dynamic
P verbs, and prepositions
Display Phase V (answering Students learn to use PECS to
questions) answer the questions such as,
“What do you want?”
The Pragmatic Organization Dynamic Display
Phase VI Now students are taught to
(PODD) emerged in a clinical setting. It was (commenting) answer questions such as
designed to solve problems that frequently “What do you see?” “What do
occurred during intervention. Such problems you hear?” and “What is it?”
include vocabulary only being available during For this purpose, they learn to
construct sentences starting
certain activities and activity-specific vocabulary with “I see,” “I hear,” “I feel,”
without the option of communicating other “It is a,” etc.
things. If multiple displays were available to
ensure access to more vocabulary, it required a
high degree of cognitive and working memory to color-coded matching pages. It further provides
scan each display and discriminate between all operational commands such as “Turn the page” or
symbols. Moving between different pages and “Go to (category)” symbols to facilitate movement
inefficient vocabulary organization can be a chal- between different pages. Similar to other systems,
lenge (Porter & Cafiero, 2009). vocabulary is organized in association with the
PODD is designed to support spontaneous and main content of an activity and can appear multi-
functional communication in all daily environ- ple times on multiple pages. For individuals it may
ments. Depending on the current level of commu- be helpful to include words and phrases that sup-
nication and language abilities, pages have to be port behavioral and environmental regulation early
designed accordingly in regard to number of sym- on in the communication book. Another unique
bols per page. PODD features some unique strate- feature of the PODD system is pragmatic starters.
gies to enhance social communication. One such These are symbols that provide predictive links to
feature is the navigation between different pages pages that are commonly used for certain commu-
with “Go to page (number)” symbols that lead to nicative intents (e.g., I want something, I’m asking
282 D. Hartzheim

Fig. 16.6  PECS® books examples PECS® Pictures used with permission from Pyramid Educational Consultants Inc

a question, I want to go somewhere). Predictive which includes skills such as joint attention and
links can also be symbols depicting messages like engagement and object permanence.
I like this or something’s wrong. Depending on the While AAC has been shown to be effective in
selection, the AAC user makes the partner and enhancing expressive communication, it also
then navigates to the page corresponding with the supports receptive language through a variety of
predictive link. The page includes various things input strategies, such as aided modeling and
that he or she might want or need. When a conver- aided language stimulation. These techniques
sational breakdown occurs, PODD communica- evolved from the theoretical basis of typical lan-
tion books include symbols that aid in clarifying or guage development. By the time a typical child
repairing the breakdown (e.g., I don’t know, I starts using verbal language, it has had a vast
don’t understand, please explain this to me) (Porter amount of language modeling from people in his/
& Cafiero, 2009). her environment. Modeling language is crucial
When considering AAC options for an indi- for language development. Aided modeling pro-
vidual with communication difficulties, it is vides the individual with language input in the
often because there is a lack in receptive lan- mode that he/she would use for expression, in
guage skills. There is a differentiation between this case an AAC system.
expressive communication and receptive lan-
guage. Expressive language refers to the ability
to make one’s own wants and needs known to Augmented Communication Input
others through a variety of different modes (i.e.,
verbal output including words and sounds, writ- “A simple way to conceptualize the logic and
ten words, gestures, sign, body language, etc.). theory supporting the practice of AAC modeling
Sometimes challenging behavior can be the is to think of the analogy of a child learning a
result of an individual’s lack to communicate spoken or signed language. If you expected a
their wants and needs clearly to their conversa- child to learn to speak Spanish, the child should
tional partner due to a lack of expressive com- be immersed in environments that use the
munication skills. On the other hand, receptive Spanish language. The same logic is used for a
language refers to the ability to understand ver- child expected to learn sign language, in that he
bal (i.e., spoken words) and nonverbal (i.e., or she should be immersed in environments
facial expression, gestures, body language) where people are using sign. For children who
communication. Prerequisite skills to receptive are expected to communicate using AAC, the
language are attention, perception, and memory, logic continues that the child would be immersed
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 283

in and ­environment ‘speaking AAC’. Language Mayer-Johnson, 2004) in an interactive manner.


input is important to language acquisition.” Symbols are arranged on environmentally spe-
(Sennott, Light, & McNaughton, 2016) cific language boards. The speaking communi-
AAC modeling-based interventions for chil- cation partner touches symbols as he/she speaks
dren consistently produce large and clinically rel- to the nonspeaking communication partner.
evant effects on beginning language skills of ALS targets a variety of communication func-
individuals with complex communication needs tions such as initiation, questioning, respond-
using AAC across four primary domains (i.e., ing, and commenting between the client and the
pragmatic, semantic, syntactic, and morphologic). communication partner. (Goossens, Crain, &
Children increase communication turns, gain Elder, 1992).
vocabulary knowledge, communicate multi-­ In the natural aided language (NAL) inter-
symbol utterances, and demonstrate knowledge of vention, ALS was merged with naturalistic learn-
early morphological forms. These positive results ing. The visual systems are implemented in
are seen across a range of communication part- natural, real, and reinforcing environments. It is
ners and contexts such as play, shared reading, art comparable with incorporating a second (but
activities, and mealtimes (Sennot et al., 2016). visual) language into the everyday routines. This
Augmented language input uses visual-­ hybrid strategy incorporates the interactive, gen-
graphic symbols that can be pictorial, text-based, erative language basis of aided language stimula-
or a combination of pictures and text (Cafiero, tion with the naturalistic strategies of the natural
2001). Augmented input strategies require the language paradigm (Koegel, et al., 1987) and
communication partner to use speech as well as incidental teaching (Hart & Risley, 1995). NAL
AAC. The partner is signaling to the AAC user has been shown to increase the use of picture
the mode that he or she is expected to use. It pro- symbols for communication. With the use of
vides a model that is consistent with the output. It NAL, individuals have been able to learn a vari-
further signals acceptability of the AAC system ety of communicative intents. Simple requesting
as a mode for communication and that it can be expanded to initiation, responding, commenting,
used to send messages. Modeling AAC use can and questioning within teacher-directed activities
further provide opportunities for appropriate (Cafiero, 2001). While enhanced communication
play, for social interaction and for learning new is the primary goal, NAL has the potential to pos-
language concepts. AAC models may aid in lan- itively affect academics and challenging behavior
guage comprehension for individuals who strug- (Cafiero, 2001).
gle with speech-only input. It provides visual
enhancement of the spoken words. Visual-spatial
abilities of individuals with autism may be a rela- Speech-Generating Devices
tive strength, therefore supporting the use of
picture-­based communication systems (Drager, With advances in technology, voice output com-
2009). Aided language stimulation (ALS) and munication aids (VOCAs) become more accessi-
natural aided language (NAL) are two of the ble. VOCAs or SGD are high-tech AAC systems
most common aided modeling interventions. with dynamic displays. SGDs are portable, com-
Aided language stimulation (Goosens, Crain, puterized devices that produce synthetic or digi-
& Elder, 1992) is an augmentative communica- tized speech output when activated. A variety of
tion strategy that uses the contextually relevant, visual-graphic symbols are used to represent
reinforcing environments and activities of the messages, which are activated when an individ-
natural language paradigm (Koegel, O’Dell, & ual uses a finger, hand optical pointer, headstick,
Koegel, 1987; Koegel, Dyer, & Bell, 1987). It is switch, eye gaze, or some other means to select a
a receptive language training using visual sym- symbol from the SGD’s display. Which access
bols along with speech. ALS most commonly method is employed depends on the individual’s
uses Picture Communication Symbols (PCS, motor abilities. Most individuals with autism
284 D. Hartzheim

have motor abilities sufficient for direct selection Fringe vocabulary (i.e., content words with less
of symbols on a SGD using their fingers. frequent occurrence in language) is taught later
A high-tech AAC system can be a designated (e.g., “piano,” “cotton,” “stapler”). Auditory sig-
communication device (e.g., Tobii Dynavox®, nals occur with the activation of a button or a cell
PRC® devices) or a tablet with a communication on the device, which is typical for all SGDs.
application (e.g., Android or iPad®). While the Natural consequences are provided by the commu-
tablet option is less costly than a designated nication partner when he or she reacts appropri-
device, there are some downfalls. Speakers are ately to the auditory signal (“What is LAMP,”
often not sufficient for noisy environments, tab- 2016). While this type of system is available on a
lets are usually not as shock resistant to falling, designated device, the software can also be loaded
and the customer service, especially for program- onto a tablet or iPad. The application “Speak for
ming support, is often not included in the apps for Yourself” (Speak for Yourself AAC, 2016) is also
tablets and iPad®. Designated devices on the based on activating cells with a consistent motor
other hand provide support, troubleshooting help, pattern.
and implementation guidance from a trained rep- Compass® (Tobii Dynavox) is based on learn-
resentative. They are durable and usually have ing language in categories and situationally. This
better voice and speaker qualities. system comes pre-stored with a full-breadth of
Individuals with autism have a relative strength symbols, pictures, and phrases to support every-
regarding visual stimuli. In comparison to picture- day interactions. Built-in behavioral supports and
based low-tech AAC systems, high-­tech AAC sys- scripts assist in conversations. The Compass pag-
tems have a large database of pictures and words esets are designed to increase efficiency and
already preprogrammed on the device. The poten- speed of access for increased conversational flu-
tial for vocabulary growth, variability of output, idity (Communication without Compromise,
and increase in communicative intents is provided. 2016). This software is loaded on designated
Depending on the system, the displays are communication devices as well as available for
designed based on different theoretical bases of download on tablets and iPads®. Additional soft-
language learning. For example, Language ware with categorical vocabulary pagesets for
Acquisition through Motor Planning (LAMP®) tablets include Proloquo2Go® and TouchChat®.
Words for Life (PRC system) is based on motor Choosing which type of system to use depends
learning of language. It combines principles on the individual with autism as well as the abili-
related to teaching language such as readiness to ties and knowledge of the interventionist.
learn, joint engagement, consistent and unique Preference of the AAC user is an important com-
motor patterns, single words, auditory signals, and ponent when choosing a system.
natural consequences. Readiness to learn assures From research in the area of eye gaze, we can
that the AAC user is alert and attending to relevant learn a number of suggestions when designing
learning stimuli. Activities need to be designed AAC devices for individuals with autism. Social
around the individual’s skill level. Joint engage- visual information does not attract the attention
ment refers to the moment in which both commu- of those with autism or attract attention atypi-
nicators are participating in the same activity or cally. Important social information relayed by a
with the same object. The vocabulary on the device person or face may be lost (Gillespie-Smith &
is organized and programmed in a manner to allow Fletcher-Watson, 2014). The manner in which an
fluent communication upon mastery. The symbols AAC system is designed can enhance social com-
are arranged in a manner with a consistent motor prehension. First, in regard to delayed fixation on
pattern. With increase in vocabulary, each symbol social content, AAC displays should remain
remains in the same spot. Each cell communicates available long enough to process the relevant
single words so that novel sentences can be created information for comprehension. Symbols
independently by the user. Core ­vocabulary (i.e., depicting emotional expressions could be
­
those words that are most commonly used in a lan- designed with particular clarity enhancing focus
guage) is taught first (e.g., “want,” “go,” “you”). on social information (such as pointing arrows or
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 285

distinctive colors). Further, if possible, it may be tion. Availability of more affordable ­consumer-­level
useful to reduce social complexity in AAC dis- hardware and applications gives the consumer
plays by depicting fewer people per symbol. more flexibility when selecting an AAC system.
More social complexity leads to less focus on Such commonplace devices may result in a signifi-
that area in individuals with autism (Gillespie- cant paradigm shift in AAC, making it more
Smith et al., 2014). socially acceptable, affordable, portable, and read-
Eye tracking studies showed that children ily available. Designated communication apps for
with autism fixate on a target more quickly in a consumer devices (e.g., Proloquo2go®, GoTalk®,
visual search and use fewer eye movements Speak for Yourself®) may serve as full system,
(Kemner, van Ewijk, van Engeland, & Hooge, comparable to dedicated SGDs. Other apps how-
2008). This suggests that individuals with autism ever (e.g., Steps®, My Choice Board®,
might be particularly efficient at working with PicCalendar®) provide visual support for daily
AAC symbols in a traditional grid array sequences and activities. Caution needs to be taken
(Gillespie-Smith et al., 2014). with technological advances. They do not replace
the clinical decision-­ making process. Clinicians
and families need to be aware that with the intro-
Choosing an AAC System duction of any technology, a substantial amount of
training is required for mastery and for lasting posi-
Many factors play a role when choosing an tive results (Shane et al., 2012).
AAC system for an individual with limited or Visual modeling has already been established
no verbal communication skills. Some of those to be a crucial portion of AAC interventions. A
factors are social acceptability of the system, new modeling program, the Visual Immersion
therapist’s skill level, financial considerations, Program (VIP), is under development enhancing
universality of the system, portability, and visual modeling. It supports the theory that AAC
response effort. While all of these elements learners should be immersed in visually symbol-­
influence the decision-­making process, maybe rich environments across home, school, and com-
the most important aspect for consideration munity. Visuals should not only support
should be the preference of the user. More posi- expression but also comprehension. Further,
tive effects are recorded when a child has the communication with symbols should support a
ability to choose which AAC system he or she variety of communicative intents beyond protest-
wants. When given the opportunity, each child ing and requesting (e.g., questioning, comment-
demonstrated a clear preference for one AAC ing, directing). VIP is based on visual scenes to
system. Children demonstrated greater profi- represent complex ideas by not solely relying on
ciency and enhanced maintenance with the pre- auditory stimuli for language comprehension.
ferred AAC system. Assessing children’s Visual scenes hold great potential when used
preferences might be important with respect to consistently and frequently (Light & Drager,
the issue of inappropriate AAC abandonment, 2007). In VIP dynamic scenes are full-motion
which appears to be common. Logically, one video clips that depict entire scenes, events, or
might expect that preferred AAC options would concepts that unfold over time. VIP incorporates
be less likely to be abandoned (Van der Meer, animated symbols for over 110 verbs and prepo-
et al., 2013). sitions (ALP Animated Graphics) that were
developed by the Center for Communication
Enhancement of Children’s Hospital Boston.
Future Direction of AAC in Autism Individuals with ASD sometimes benefit from
animated representations of concepts (Shane,
Recently, dedicated hardware and software are et al., 2012). Introducing animated symbols into
starting to compete with less expensive, readily interventions to enhance comprehension of diffi-
available hardware (e.g., tablets and laptops) with cult, non-concrete concepts may hold potential
specialized applications to support communica- for future AAC systems for those with ASD.
286 D. Hartzheim

In conclusion, this chapter on AAC in the References


ASD population aimed to give an introduction
into what AAC is, how to best assess AAC needs, Augmentative and Alternative Communication –
American speech, language, and hearing association.
and evidence-based treatment approaches. From (2016). Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/public/
experience, AAC can be an intimidating treat- speech/disorders/AAC/
ment approach to explore for a number of differ- Beukelman, D., & Mirenda, P. (2013). Augmentative and
ent reasons. Some of those reasons may be the alternative communication: Supporting children and
adults with complex communication needs (4th ed.).
overwhelming amount of AAC options that can Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co..
be found on the market. I urge you to try one sys- Brodhead, M. T., Higbee, T. S., Pollard, J. S., Akers, J. S.,
tem at a time. Hopefully, this chapter provided & Gerencser, K. R. (2014). The use of linked activ-
the reader with encouragement and direction on ity schedules to teach children with autism to play
hide-and-seek. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
where to start and how to functionally incorpo- 47(3), 645–650.
rate AAC with individuals with ASD. AAC offers Cafiero, J. M. (2001). The effect of an augmentative
optimistic ideas for solving some of the most communication intervention on the communication,
challenging communication breakdowns. Those behavior, and academic program of an adolescent with
autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
ideas can be simple low-tech solutions, but they Disabilities, 16(3), 179–189.
can also be high-tech devices. A teacher or clini- Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior
cian needs to creatively think “outside the box” problems through functional communication train-
and approach communication from many differ- ing. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18(2),
111–126.
ent angles. While there are some well-researched Charlop-Christy, M. H., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2001).
AAC interventions available, technology is con- Naturalistic teaching strategies for acquisition, gener-
tinuously advancing, making it more accessible alization, and maintenance of speech in children with
for a greater number of individuals. Research autism. In P. Ghezzi, L. Williams, & J. Carr (Eds.),
Autism: Behavior analytic perspectives. Reno, NV:
needs to continuously be conducted to support Content Press.
effectiveness and efficacy of AAC interventions. Drager, K. D. (2009). Aided modeling interventions for
AAC may be a viable temporary or permanent children with autism spectrum disorders who require
communication option for some of the 30% of AAC. SIG 12 Perspectives on Augmentative and
Alternative Communication, 18(4), 114–120.
individuals with ASD who remain nonverbal Durand, V. M. (1990). Severe behavior problems: A func-
throughout their life. tional communication training approach. New York,
NY: Guilford Press.
Durand, V. M. (1999). Functional communication training
using assistive devices: Recruiting natural communi-
Helpful Resources ties of reinforcement. Journal of Applied Behavior
Analysis, 32(3), 247–267.
• Autism speaks list of apps for iPads and tab- Durand, V. M., & Carr, E. G. (1991). Functional com-
lets with supporting research https://www. munication training to reduce challenging behavior:
Maintenance and application in new settings. Journal
autismspeaks.org/autism-apps?page=8&tid_1 of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24(2), 251–264.
=39881&tid=All&tid_2=All&keys= Fisher, W., Piazza, C., Cataldo, M., Harrell, R.,
• University of Nebraska Augmentative and Jefferson, G., & Conner, R. (1993). Functional com-
Alternative Communication website http:// munication training with and without extinction and
punishment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
cehs.unl.edu/aac/ 26(1), 23–36.
• International Society for Augmentative and Flannery, K. B., & Horner, R. H. (1994). The relationship
Alternative Communication (ISAAC) https:// between predictability and problem behavior for stu-
www.isaac-online.org/english/home/ dents with severe disabilities. Journal of Behavioral
Education, 4(2), 157–176.
• Communication Matrix https://www.commu- Frea, W. D., Arnold, C. L., & Vittimberga, G. L. (2001).
nicationmatrix.org/ A demonstration of the effects of augmentative com-
• Examples of different visual schedules http:// munication on the extreme aggressive behavior of a
do2learn.com/picturecards/VisualSchedules/ child with autism within an integrated preschool set-
ting. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3(4),
index.htm 194–198.
16  Augmentative and Alternative Communication and ASD 287

Frea, W. D., Arnold, C. L., Vittimberga, G. L., & Koegel, Light, J., Beukelman, D., & Reichle, J. (2003).
R. L. (2001). A demonstration of the effects of aug- Communicative competence for individuals who use
mentative communication on the extreme aggressive AAC: From research to effective practice.
behavior of a child with autism within an integrated Light, J., & Drager, K. (2007). AAC technologies for
preschool setting. Journal of Positive Behavior young children with complex communication needs:
Interventions, 3, 194–198. State of the science and future research directions.
Frost, L., & Bondy, A. (2002). The picture exchange Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 23(3),
communication system training manual (2nd ed.). 204–216.
Cherry Hill, NJ: Pyramid Educational Products, Light, J. C., Roberts, B., Dimarco, R., & Greiner, N.
Incorporated. (1998). Augmentative and alternative communication
Ganz, J. B., Simpson, R. L., & Corbin-Newsome, to support receptive and expressive communication
J. (2008). The impact of the picture exchange commu- for people with autism. Journal of Communication
nication system on requesting and speech development Disorders, 31(2), 153–180.
in preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders and MacDuff, G. S., Krantz, P. J., & McClannahan, L. E.
similar characteristics. Research in Autism Spectrum (1993). Teaching children with autism to use pho-
Disorders, 2(1), 157–169. tographic activity schedules: Maintenance and gen-
Ganz, J. B., Sigafoos, J., Simpson, R. L., & Cook, K. E. eralization of complex response chains. Journal of
(2008). Generalization of a pictorial alternative com- Applied Behavior Analysis, 26(1), 89–97.
munication system across instructors and distance. Marckel, J. M., Neef, N. A., & Ferreri, S. J. (2006). A pre-
Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 24(2), liminary analysis of teaching improvisation with the
89–99. picture exchange communication system to children
Gillespie-Smith, K., & Fletcher-Watson, S. (2014). with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,
Designing AAC systems for children with autism: 39(1), 109–115.
Evidence from eye tracking research. Augmentative Mayer-Johnson, R. (2004). The picture communication
and Alternative Communication, 30(2), 160–171. symbols-PCS–software Boardmaker (p. 1). Porto
Goossens, C., Crain, S., & Elder, P. (1992). Engineering Alegre, Brazil: Clik Tecnologia Assistiva.
the classroom environment for interactive sym- Mirenda, P. (2001). Autism, augmentative communi-
bolic communication – An emphasis on the devel- cation, and assistive technology what do we really
opmental period, 18 months to five years. Southeast know? Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
Augmentative Communication Publications, 2430. Disabilities, 16(3), 141–151.
Hall, L. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Krantz, P. J. (1995). Mirenda, P. (2009). Promising innovations in AAC for
Promoting independence in integrated classrooms individuals with autism spectrum disorders. SIG
by teaching aides to use activity schedules and 12 Perspectives on Augmentative and Alternative
decreased prompts. Education and Training in Communication, 18(4), 112–113.
Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, Peterson, S., Bondy, A., Vincent, Y., & Finnegan, C.
208–217. (1995). Effects of altering communicative input for
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in students with autism and no speech: Two case studies.
the everyday experience of young American children. Augmentative and Alternative Communication, 11(2),
Paul H Brookes Publishing. 93–100.
ISAAC – What is AAC? (2016). Retrieved from https:// Pierce, K. L., & Schreibman, L. (1994). Teaching daily
www.isaac-online.org/english/what-is-aac/ living skills to children with autism in unsupervised
Kemner, C., Van Ewijk, L., Van Engeland, H., & Hooge, settings through pictorial self-management. Journal of
I. (2008). Brief report: Eye movements during visual Applied Behavior Analysis, 27(3), 471–481.
search tasks indicate enhanced stimulus discrim- Porter, G., & Cafiero, J. M. (2009). Pragmatic organiza-
inability in subjects with PDD. Journal of Autism and tion dynamic display (PODD) communication books:
Developmental Disorders, 38(3), 553–557. A promising practice for individuals with autism spec-
Koegel, R. L., Dyer, K., & Bell, L. K. (1987). The influ- trum disorders. SIG 12 Perspectives on Augmentative
ence of child-preferred activities on autistic children's and Alternative Communication, 18(4), 121–129.
social behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, PRC What is LAMP. (2016). Retrieved from https://
20(3), 243–252. aacapps.com/lamp/what-is-lamp.
Koegel, R. L., O’dell, M. C., & Koegel, L. K. (1987). Rossetti, L.M.. (1990). The Rossetti infant-toddler lan-
A natural language teaching paradigm for nonverbal guage scale: a measure of communication and interac-
autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental tion. LinguiSystems.
Disorders, 17(2), 187–200. Rowland, C. (1996). Communication matrix. Portland,
Krantz, P. J., MacDuff, M. T., & McClannahan, L. E. OR: Design to Learn.
(1993). Programming participation in family activities Schreibman, L. (2006). The science and fiction of autism.
for children with autism: Parents’ use of photographic Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
activity schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Sennott, S. C., Light, J. C., & McNaughton, D. (2016).
Analysis, 26(1), 137–138. AAC modeling intervention research review. Research
288 D. Hartzheim

and Practice for Persons with Severe Disabilities, van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., Sutherland, D., McLay, L.,
41(2), 101–115. Lang, R., Lancioni, G.E., ..., Marschik, P.B.. (2013).
Shane, H. C., Laubscher, E. H., Schlosser, R. W., Flynn, Preference-enhanced communication intervention and
S., Sorce, J. F., & Abramson, J. (2012). Applying tech- development of social communicative functions in a
nology to visually support language and communica- child with autism spectrum disorder. Clinical Case
tion in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Studies, 1534650113508221.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vaughn, B., & Horner, R. (1995). Effects of concrete versus
42(6), 1228–1235. verbal choice systems on problem behavior. Augmentative
Speak for Yourself AAC. (2016). Retrieved from http:// and Alternative Communication, 11(2), 89–92.
www.speakforyourself.org/ von Tetzchner, S., & Grove, N.. (2003). Augmentative and
Sundberg, M. L. (2008). Verbal behavior milestones alternative communication: Developmental issues.
assessment and placement program: The VB-MAPP. Whurr Publishers.
Concord, CA: AVBPress. Watson, L. R., Lord, C., & Schaffer, B.S.. (1989).
Tobii/dynavox Communication without Compromise. E.(1989). Teaching spontaneous communication to
(2016). Retrieved from http://www.tobiidynavox.com/ autistic and developmentally handicapped children.
compass-overview/ Wetherby, A. M., & Prizant, B. M. (2003). CSBS man-
van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, ual: Communication and symbolic behavior scales.
G. E. (2011). Assessing preferences for AAC options Brookes Pub.
in communication interventions for individuals with Wodka, E. L., Mathy, P., & Kalb, L. (2013). Predictors
developmental disabilities: A review of the litera- of phrase and fluent speech in children with autism
ture. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 32(5), and severe language delay. Pediatrics, 131(4),
1422–1431. e1128–e1134.
Vocational Training for Persons
with Autism Spectrum Disorder 17
Matthew J. Konst

factors include integration into a larger community,


 ocational Training for Persons
V independent living, employment, and education
with ASD (Hendricks & Wehman, 2009). These factors are
especially salient for persons with developmental
For individuals and their families, a diagnosis of disabilities (Defur & Patton, 1999; Schall &
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a major Wehman, 2008). As with typically developing
stressor. This is especially salient when children peers, the structure and supports in place throughout
are diagnosed as infants. Uncertainty surround- this transition vary from person to person. Unlike
ing symptom manifestation and outcome leads typically developing peers, however, this transition
parents to express concern about their child’s is further complicated by the influence of their pri-
future (Howlin, Goode, Hutton, & Rutter, 2004). mary disability, the presence of comorbid condi-
As the diagnostic nomenclature suggests, the tions, and the overall degree of impairment.
spectrum of symptoms observed in people with Reports from early analyses of adult outcome
ASD complicates the provision of definitive for individuals with ASD have historically been
prognostic information. Limited information gray. Following his initial description of ASD,
about prognosis for persons with ASD is avail- Kanner (1973) indicated that a bulk of participants
able. This is especially true once persons cross he followed across time were living at home, in
the threshold into adulthood. Factors contributing institutions, in psychiatric hospitals, or in sheltered
to this dearth of information include the limited living spaces. A recent survey of high school grad-
research available, heterogeneity of symptoms, uates with ASD suggested that only 4% of respon-
continuous changes in diagnostic criteria, differ- dents lived independently; over 75% indicated that
ence in outcome measures, and presence of they continue to reside with their parents (Wagner,
comorbid conditions (Howlin & Goode, 1998). Newman, Cameto, Garza, & Levine, 2005).
A multitude of factors confront each person as Elevated rates of unemployment preclude inde-
they transition into adulthood. On a broad level, pendent living and continue to be a pervasive issue
for persons with ASD around the world. Research
investigating employment for individuals with
M.J. Konst, PhD (*)
ASD has been conducted in countries around the
Kennedy Krieger Institute, Johns Hopkins University,
Baltimore, MD, USA world. Based upon a survey of adults with ASD in
Canada, Eaves and Ho (2008) reported that only
Department of Psychology, J. Iverson Riddle
Developmental Center, Morganton, NC 28655, USA 4% of respondents were competitively employed.
e-mail: mattkonst@gmail.com Roughly 45% of ­respondents indicated that they

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 289


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_17
290 M.J. Konst

had never held a job. In the United Kingdom, 18% the influence of core ASD symptomology and
of respondents endorsed participation in supported identified common challenges encountered across
or sheltered work environments (Howlin et al., vocational experiences. Respondents identified
2004). An additional 13% of respondents indi- factors such as interacting with peers and supervi-
cated that they were competitively employed. In sors, learning new tasks, intolerance, and job task
the United States, a survey of young adults with satisfaction as factors contributing to negative
ASD revealed an employment rate of 18% (Taylor work experiences. Positive work experiences
& Seltzer, 2011). A total of 6% of individuals indi- were associated with job assignments that were
cated that they were competitively employed, and complimentary to interests or skills and supervi-
12% reported participation in a supported work sors and coworkers who were tolerant and flexi-
environment. ble. Barriers to employment included difficulty
explaining termination from previous jobs or
periods of unemployment. Respondents also
Barriers to Employment expressed frustration with settling for jobs for
which they were overqualified. The survey by
Core Symptoms Mueller and colleagues (Müller et al., 2003)
identified four overarching barriers to employ-
In line with symptoms associated with ASD, ment. These were the (A) application process,
workplace interactions are a well-documented (B) social interactions, (C) communication with
hindrance to employment (Hagner & Cooney, coworkers and supervisors, (D) and adjusting to
2005; Hillier et al., 2007; Patterson & Rafferty, vocational demands.
2001). This theme has been consistent across Insufficient knowledge about the manifesta-
multiple studies focused on self-reported voca- tion of symptoms related to ASD may also be a
tional problems (see Camarena & Sarigiani, barrier to employment (Bolman, 2008; Müller
2009; Müller, Schuler, Burton, & Yates, 2003; et al., 2003). Other researchers have reported
Sperry & Mesibov, 2005). The degree of sever- that employers are less likely to hire persons
ity and type of social impairment are variable with ASD due to fears of maladaptive behav-
across persons with ASD. Social impairments iors (Nesbitt, 2000). The heterogeneity of
related to difficulty interpreting affect, inter- symptoms and the emphasis on individualiza-
preting facial expressions, modulating tone of tion of supports place a unique demand on
voice, and following social rules are common would-be employers. Aside from supports on
(Bolman, 2008; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002; the job, researchers also suggest that general
Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004; Van Bourgondien, psychoeducation may be beneficial for employ-
Reichle, & Palmer, 1997). Other impairments ers. Aside from task-specific training, addi-
pertaining to social interactions may also be tional interventions may be required to decrease
present (e.g., poor hygiene, grooming, inappro- stereotypic behaviors or other maladaptive
priate sexual behaviors; Koning & Magill- behaviors. Supports may also work to increase
Evans, 2001; Van Bourgondien et al., 1997). appropriate social and communication skills
Additional impairments associated with core (Burt, Fuller, & Lewis, 1991).
ASD symptoms include adjustments related to Core symptoms of ASD may also serve as
changes in personal and work-related routines relative strengths for specific jobs or tasks. Smith
and environments (Keel, Mesibov, & Woods, (1995) suggested that attributes commonly
1997). Vocational difficulties may also include observed in ASD populations may be beneficial
limited ability to work independently and resis- and can be used to inform placements for employ-
tance to change. ment such as preference for socially isolating or
Müller et al. (2003) evaluated the vocational repetitive work. Persons with ASD may also
experiences of persons with ASD via semi-­ excel at work requiring substantive focus and
structured interviews. This research also reflects attention to detail.
17  Vocational Training 291

Comorbid Conditions 1998; Smith, 1985; Smith & Coleman, 1986;


Taylor & Seltzer, 2011). The presence of one or
Additional barriers to employment include the more of these conditions necessitates the need
presence of comorbid conditions and maladap- for further structure and supports to identify and
tive behaviors. Following a review of the existent manipulate behavioral functions on the job
literature, Howlin (2000) reported that the esti- (Kemp & Carr, 1995). These behaviors are also
mated prevalence of comorbid conditions in ASD viewed negatively and stigmatized which may
populations has ranged from 9% to 89% of adults deter employment or encourage isolation.
with ASD. Multiple factors such as changes in Esbensen and colleagues (2010) noted that the
diagnostic criteria and participant selection likely presence of comorbid psychiatric conditions is
contribute to the significant variance in estimates. often a predictor of decreased independence and
Matson and Shoemaker (2009) outlined multiple social functioning. Hofvander and colleagues
factors that impact estimates of co-occurring ID (2000) reported that 50% of participants with ASD
in individuals with ASD. In general, researchers met criteria for an anxiety disorder. Not surpris-
have reported rates of ID in ASD populations ingly, the impact of symptoms associated with
between 70% (La Malfa, Lassi, Bertelli, Salvini, comorbid conditions is not on a single plane.
& Placidi, 2004) and 80% (Wing, 1996; Rutter, Adults with ASD have previously reported elevated
1983). Regardless of prevalence, researchers anxiety levels associated with the increased social
agree that the presence of comorbid ID within demands at work (Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004).
ASD populations is often associated with Additional researchers have demonstrated similar
decreased independence and functioning in areas states of heightened anxiety surrounding the initial
relevant to employment (Eaves & Ho, 2008; pursuit of jobs and the ensuing application and hir-
Farley et al., 2009; Howlin et al., 2004). These ing process (Camarena & Sarigiani, 2009).
key areas of impairment include increased need Aside from anxiety, persons with ASD also
for supports at home, in their social interactions, experience elevated rates of ADHD, depression,
and at work. General cognitive functioning pro- and bipolar disorder (Hofvander et al., 2009;
files for people with ASD exemplify further bar- Kim, Szatmari, Bryson, Streiner, & Wilson,
riers to employment. Multiple researchers have 2000). Comorbid conditions also include the
documented deficits in executive functioning presence of medical conditions such as CP, visual
(Landa & Goldberg, 2005; Lopez, Lincoln, impairments, or epilepsy (Bellstedt, Gillberg, &
Ozonoff, & Lai, 2005). These impairments Gillberg, 2005; Billstedt, 2000; Fombonne,
include impairments in working memory, atten- 2003). An analysis of 815 persons with ASD
tion, and motor planning (Hume & Odom, 2007; identified the absence of a comorbid condition as
Müller et al., 2003; Patterson & Rafferty, 2001). a significant predictor of participation in com-
Taylor and Seltzer (2010) noted that ASD petitive or supported employment (Schaller &
participants without comorbid ID were more Yang, 2005). Adults with ASD are denied voca-
likely to exhibit internalizing and externalizing tional training services twice as often as peers
maladaptive behaviors (e.g., SIB, property with other developmental disabilities due to
destruction, aggression) than peers with either symptom severity (Lawer, Brusilovskiy, Salzer,
condition alone. This association is likely due to & Mandell, 2009).
decreased external supports. This factor may be
especially salient for those in lower SES classes
that may experience additional barriers to ser- Rate of Employment
vice access (Liptak et al., 2008). The presence of
comorbid conditions such as SIB, property Despite intervention efforts, the core symptoms
destruction, stereotypic/ritualistic behaviors, of ASD are pervasive across the lifespan
aggression, tantrums, and pica also serves as (Gilchrist et al., 2001; Volkmar, Stier, & Cohen,
barriers to employment (Kobayashi & Murata, 1985). Fombonne (2003) estimated that there
292 M.J. Konst

were between 55,602 and 121,324 adolescents Despite all of the identified barriers and
(15–19 years of age) with ASD. Multiple c­ oncerns, multiple researchers have documented
researchers have suggested that over 90% of that individuals with ASD can flourish in sup-
adults with ASD are unemployed (Bellstedt ported and competitive employment settings
et al., 2005; Engstrom, Ekstrom, & Emilsson, (Gerhardt & Holmes, 2005; Hurlbutt & Chalmers,
2003). Other researchers estimate the rate of 2002, 2004; Smith, Belcher, & Juhrs, 1995).
unemployment within the ASD population Adults with ASD have been observed to flourish
ranges from 50% to 75% (Howlin et al., 2004; in a variety of industries (O’Brien & Daggett,
Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2002; Mawhood, Howlin, 2006); however, the importance of individualized
& Rutter, 2000). Although these ranges are dis- supports and services cannot be overlooked
crepant, elevated rates of unemployment are (Hendricks & Wehman, 2009).
consistent. Employment rates may be further
stratified by work environments. This discern-
ment may partially explain some of the observed  ransitioning from Primary
T
variance in estimates of employment rate. An Education to Vocational Training
analysis of the National Rehabilitation Services
Administration 911 database of job placement The Role of Primary Education
for 815 individuals with ASD following partici-
pation in vocational training services indicated Historically the options for postsecondary educa-
that persons with ASD were equally likely to be tion and employment have been limited for per-
placed in supported (44.8%) or competitive sons with ASD (O’brien & Daggett, 2006; Schall,
work environments (55.2%; Schaller & Yang, Cortijo-Doval, Targett, & Wehman, 2006).
2005). When compared to peers with other Despite a growing emphasis on postsecondary
developmental disabilities, adults with ASD education for persons with ASD, there is limited
exhibit some of the highest rates of unemploy- research of this factor demonstrating a positive
ment (Burke et al., 2010). This is likely due in impact on employment. Relative to people with
large part to the unique vocational needs of per- SLD, LD, and ID, people with ASD have poor
sons with ASD (Bellstedt et al., 2005; Müller employment and postsecondary education out-
et al., 2003). comes (Shattuck et al., 2012). A literature review
Although vocational training may appear like of participation in postsecondary education pro-
a relatively simple solution to the depressed rates grams suggested that participation – even among
of employment at face value, it is an inherently persons with “high-functioning ASD” – is vari-
difficult and complex system (Tuma, 1983). In a able with estimates ranging from 7% to 50%
competitive job market, job opportunities are (Howlin, 2000). Following a recent survey,
variable, and the duration of job availability is Cedurland, Hagberg, Billstedt, Gillberg, and
often uncertain. Further, those jobs requiring Gillberg (2008) indicated that only 2% of respon-
more initial training delay entrance into the job dents reported having a college degree and only
market and create further disconnect between the 11% of persons reported having attended any col-
accumulation of job skills and the ability to use lege. Postsecondary education attainment does
them (Witte & Kalleberg, 1995). Factors such as not absolve persons with ASD from employment
“fit” or the ability of an individual to perform a difficulties (Howlin, 2000). Contrary to intuition,
job-specific task or series of tasks and participate IQ levels have been observed to have only a
meaningfully in a work environment must also small impact on employment. Howlin et al.
be considered. Job fit becomes inherently diffi- (2004) reported that persons with an IQ less than
cult due to changes in economic demand and 70 were only slightly less likely to be employed
training requirements. Even if an individual is than persons with an IQ score greater than 70.
able to complete the necessary training and find a On a broad scale, legislative efforts have
job that utilizes their specific skills set, jobs may brought about an increased focused on transition
become obsolete due to decreased demand, planning for adolescents with 504 accommoda-
­technological advances, or market shifts. tions. The identification of functional goals and
17  Vocational Training 293

the specific skills sets necessary to achieve them and medical) following high school completion.
are important aspects of transition planning Nearly 40% of respondents indicated that they
(Iovannone, Dunlap, Huber, & Kincaid, 2003). were not receiving any services. Rate of service
Cameto and colleagues (2004) reported that provision was markedly decreased relative to
employment goals were included in most transi- rates observed during a previous screening with
tion plans they reviewed as part of a national the same participants (see Shattuck et al., 2011
database. The level of employment was typically for a full review).
broken down into three groups: competitive, sup- Given that the transition into adulthood
ported, and sheltered employment. Goals ori- involves a discontinuation of the supports
ented toward competitive employment were received in primary education, this lack of indi-
identified for nearly a quarter of participants. vidual preparedness and inclusion of external
Remaining recommendations for employment support services is surprising. Optimal transition
setting were evenly distributed between sup- planning should include active participation of
ported and sheltered work environments. the individual person to the maximum extent pos-
A recent investigation funded by the Office of sible (Wehman, 2006). When level of functioning
Special Education Programs of the USDOE is identified as a barrier, increased efforts should
revealed that 85% of adolescents with ASD had a be made to identify and incorporate the necessary
transition plan prior to exiting the primary educa- supports that will be present to assist individuals
tion system (Cameto, Levine, & Wagner, 2004). when primary education supports are discontin-
However, less than 75% of persons within this ued. This may include the identification and
group received direct instruction regarding their inclusion of adult support service agencies and
transition plans. Less than 33% of students with increased emphasis on the role of education for
ASD were active participants in transition meet- primary caregivers.
ings. Although parental participation during tran- Despite the increased focus on preparation for
sition planning was high, researchers reported that transitioning and employment, the current employ-
adult support agencies participated in less than one ment estimates are consistently low for individu-
third of transition meetings (Cameto et al., 2004). als with ASD. Even when employed, adults with
The transition period between primary educa- ASD typically earn less and work fewer hours
tion and the work environment is in need of fur- than peers (Cimera & Cowan, 2009). Researchers
ther research and development (Certo et al., suggest that adults with ASD are more likely to
2008). Although these plans are identified in the experience difficulties adjusting to new jobs, earn
primary education setting, there is currently little less than peers, and switch jobs more frequently
follow-through once the individual graduates. (Howlin, 2000; Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004;
A landmark study by Cameto and colleagues Jennes-Coussens, Magill-­Evans, & Koning, 2006;
(2004) indicated that nearly 90% of individuals Müller et al., 2003). These complications are more
with ASD had transition plans that identified the pervasive for ­persons with ASD when directly
need for post-school services. Over half of par- compared to peers with other developmental dis-
ticipants with ASD were identified as needing abilities (Cameto et al., 2004).
vocational training supports and services. One
quarter of participants were connected with a job
staffing agency as part of their transition plan. The Role of Vocational Training
Sheltered employment and supported employ-
ment agencies were contacted for another 66% of Tsang (1997) describe vocational training as a
participants (Cameto et al., 2004). Shattuck, program that places an emphasis on the provision
Wagner, Narendorf, Sterzing, and Hensley (2011) of job-oriented education with the express pur-
captured a glaring discrepancy in service provi- pose of increasing job-related skills and partici-
sion based upon survey results from recent high pation at work. This type of training typically
school graduates with ASD. Results suggested a includes job training on- or off-site or a combina-
significant decrease in the services received (i.e., tion of the two. Historical goals associated with
speech therapy, case management, mental health, vocational training were variable and included
294 M.J. Konst

development of specific occupational skills, with Asperger’s syndrome experience significant


enhancing perceived self-worth, vocational skills difficulty obtaining and retaining employment in
enhancement, and exploring potential areas of the absence of supports. Analyses of the cost
employment (Witte & Kalleberg, 1995). Benefits associated with service provision suggest that the
associated with participation in vocational train- provision of vocational training services to per-
ing include increased job opportunities, earnings, sons with ASD is more expensive than similar
satisfaction, and job retention (Tsang, 1997). programs for other disability groups (Cimera &
Economic benefits have also been associated Cowan, 2009; Lawer et al., 2009).
with vocational training for persons with devel- Early work in vocational training focused on
opmental disabilities. Mawhood and Howlin the management of stereotypic behaviors and
(1999) suggested that participation in vocational increasing social and communication skills (Burt
training programs may begin to repay (in the et al., 1991). Additional researchers focused on
form of taxes) monetary investments associated decreasing challenging behaviors such as aggres-
with the provision of such services within sion, SIB, and property destruction in addition to
2–4 years of employment. Subsequent research comorbid conditions such as pica to increase
has associated long-term employment with vocational success among individuals with ASD
increased earnings, contributions to tax systems, (Berkman & Meyer, 1988; Kemp & Carr, 1995;
and decreased reliance on federal benefits Smith, 1986, 1987). This research has been fol-
(Howlin, Alcock, & Burkin, 2005). lowed by an increased emphasis on identifying
Vocational training services are designed to appropriate and complimentary work environ-
assist people with disabilities in gaining and/or ments. Additional researchers have recom-
retaining employment (Hendricks, 2010). A num- mended that job placement should include
ber of factors make research surrounding voca- consideration of restricted interests and knowl-
tional training for individuals with ASD essential. edge of strengths and weaknesses (Hendricks,
First, minimal research has focused on this topic, 2010). Researchers have used assessment infor-
despite a relatively continuous increase in preva- mation to identify task preferences in an effort to
lence of ASD. Second, the symptomology and match these with vocational placements
increased prevalence of comorbid conditions (Lattimore, Parsons, & Reid, 2006; Nuehring &
necessitate vocational training supports that differ Sitlington, 2003). Assessments have also been
from provisions made for other persons with dis- used to identify supports and modifications that
abilities. Symptom manifestation is heterogeneous may be necessary based upon factors such as
and leads to increasingly individualized support communication and social skills (Hagner &
needs. Evaluations of preexisting vocational train- Cooney, 2005; Müller et al., 2003).
ing programs have indicated that programs are not As the interest surrounding vocational train-
always beneficial for individuals with ASD (Lawer ing has increased, so have attempts to identify
et al., 2009; Müller et al., 2003). There are pres- key components in an effort to create a more
ently only a limited amount of vocational programs structured or programmatic system. Hendricks
designed specifically for serving people with ASD. (2010) identified five key areas for consideration
Despite the observed overlap of ASD and ID, with regard to vocational supports. The authors
the absence of the latter can exclude persons with suggested that in addition to (A) job placement,
ASD from participation in state or federally consideration should also be given to (B) on-the-­
funded vocational training programs (Müller job provisions, (C) supervisor and peer interac-
et al., 2003; Taylor & Seltzer, 2011). People with tions, (D) work environment, and (E) anticipated
ASD are also excluded from vocational training future supports. Descriptions of programs such as
programs due to the severity of comorbid condi- the Treatment and Education of Autistic and
tions and problem behaviors (Lawer et al., 2009). related Communication-handicapped CHildren
The limited availability of vocational training (TEACCH; Keel et al., 1997) and Project
programs for adults with ASD negatively impacts SEARCH plus ASD Supports (Wehman et al.,
vocational outcome (Schaller & Yang, 2005). 2012) suggest that these are common factors in
Fast (2004) highlighted that a majority of persons current supported employment programs.
17  Vocational Training 295

Vocational Training components when working with persons with


ASD. One critical component of vocational train-
Training Methodology ing for persons with ASD is an emphasis on
increasing communication and social skills
The preparation of individuals for employment is (Morgan, 1996; Wehman et al., 2012). The need
the expressed goal of public education as stipu- for more intensive intervention translates to a
lated by the Individuals with Disabilities larger support staff. When training persons with-
Education Act (IDEA; 2004). The estimated out ASD, a 1:3 staff to intern ratio is commonly
unemployment rates for adults with ASD, how- reported. A 1:2 ratio is typically observed when
ever, indicate that our work is not complete. This providing the same benefits and instruction to
difference may be due in part to the ambiguity individuals with ASD. Wehman et al. (2012)
regarding service provision for adults. Moxon noted that they were able to fade to roughly a
and Gates (2001) drew attention to the stark con- 2.5:1 ratio by the end of vocational training in
trast between the services and supports available most cases. A combined service approach has
for school-aged individuals with ASD and adults been demonstrated as beneficial for supporting
with ASD. Prior to leaving public education, adults with ASD. Points of emphasis include
there are federal guidelines in place that provide assistive technology, counseling, job identifica-
directives, including systemic evaluation, for ser- tion assistance, on-the-job training, and assess-
vice provision in academic settings. The same ment and diagnosis (Lawer et al., 2009). The
funding and centralized system is not readily intensity and breadth of such supports also equate
accessible and available for adults. to resource investment. Adults with ASD are
Despite these ongoing efforts, at present, there noted to require the most expensive vocational
is limited research investigating guidelines for training services, but they benefit from services
providing vocational instruction to adults with as much as other atypically developing groups
ASD. This is especially true with regard to (Lawer et al., 2009).
research surrounding instruction of job tasks. The
bulk of available research within this domain is
based upon the principles of applied behavior Job-Based Supports
analysis and adapted from programs used to
instruct other persons with disabilities. Common On-the-job training has been emphasized as a
training components include modeling (live and key component of successful employment for
video), errorless learning, graduated guidance, people with ASD (Certo et al., 2003; Lawer et al.,
prompting systems, behavior chains, and the use 2009). Persons with ASD are three times more
of structured rewards (Burt et al., 1991). Behavior likely to be identified as requiring on-the-job
analytic strategies have also been adapted to supports than peers with any other developmen-
decrease maladaptive behaviors in the work envi- tal disability (Lawer et al., 2009). The benefits of
ronment (Burt et al., 1991; Foley & Staples, this approach are likely associated with multiple
2003). The use of strategies such as picture factors. From a generalization standpoint, on-the-­
schedules, coping strategies, functional analyses, job training allows the individual to learn a skills
reinforcement fading, and functional communi- set in the environment where they will be
cation has been previously used to decrease expected to perform newly acquired skills (Certo
aggression, SIB, property destruction, and other et al., 2003; Wehman, 2001). This direct experi-
inappropriate behaviors (Berkman & Meyer, ence may help to reduce the effort of vocational
1988; Kemp & Carr, 1995). training services that would otherwise be forced
A driving influence over the need for the to recreate a work environment and job demands.
development of specially designed vocational Early exposure to a new environment with struc-
training programs has been the unique need of tured supports may also help an individual with
this population. Factors that are not common ASD acclimate to the environment and demands.
components in vocational services for persons On-the-job training may also help establish a
without ASD have been identified as necessary routine and increase the individual’s comfort
296 M.J. Konst

level before they are expected to perform tasks has ­experimented with a combination of ­treatment
with decreased levels of support. approaches. Burke and colleagues (2010) use a
A job coach is often present during job training combination of behavioral skills training and
and serves as a form of direct support. The supports PDA-based cueing to train persons with ASD to
provided by the job coach are ideally intensive at complete a complex skills sequence to criterion
first and gradually faded (Hillier et al., 2007). levels. Through the use of a multiple baseline
Although job training is often individualized, design, the authors demonstrated faster rates of
Hillier et al. (2007) proposed a series of training job training using a combination of these two
goals to serve as a guide at the outset of employ- methods. The combination of both approaches
ment. Employment goals identified included abil- resulted in faster rates of skills acquisition rela-
ity to identify and complete job tasks satisfactorily, tive to either approach alone.
knowledge of workday scheduling (i.e., start time, Building on earlier prompting systems that
break time, and end time), awareness of emergency used PDAs, more recent research has begun to
procedures, and ability to navigate the work envi- integrate the use of computer systems. Augmen-
ronment. These are often components of support- tative communication devices have also been used
ive employment programs that promote job and to increase appropriate communication and inter-
social integration at work (Hillier et al., 2007; actions at work (Gray, 1998). Researchers have
Howlin et al., 2005; Keel et al., 1997). previously used a computer program to display
Despite the inherit benefits of on-the-job train- pictures and provide ­sequential verbal instructions
ing, these services are not always available. An to increase task completion (Riffel, Wehmeyer,
individual may not have a job offer in place; the Turnbull, & Lattimore, 2005). More recently,
task demands associated with the job may not be researchers have begun to explore the integration
immediately identified, or tasks may be variable of video modeling and virtual reality into voca-
and dependent on need. Previous researchers have tional training. Smith et al. (2014) used a virtual
attempted to circumvent these issues and further reality platform (virtual reality job interview train-
increase job assimilation by offering a combina- ing) in a randomized controlled trial to examine the
tion of simulated work tasks and on-the-­job train- efficacy of such technology. Participants demon-
ing (Lawer et al., 2009). Behavioral skills training strated significant improvements in job interview
has been used to teach pre- and post-placement skills and reported significant increases in confi-
vocational skills to persons with ASD (Hillier dence surrounding interviews (Smith et al., 2014).
et al., 2007). This training strategy emphasizes The use of interactive technology such as the Apple
instruction, modeling, rehearsal, and immediate iPod devices has been demonstrated to increase
feedback (Gunby, Carr, & LeBlanc, 2010; performance and task completion (Cihak, Kessler,
Miltenberger, 2008). These services are intensive & Alberto, 2008; Gentry, Lau, Molinelli, Fallen, &
and typically focus on individualized instruction Kriner, 2012; Kellems & Morningstar, 2012; Van
and service provision. Despite the use of this pro- Laarhoven, Johnson, Van Laarhoven-Myers,
cedure to train persons with ASD in various skills, Grider, & Grider, 2009). Despite these initial posi-
limited research on the use of behavioral skills tive results, to date, there have been limited sys-
training for vocation has been carried out. tematic evaluations of the use of technology for
vocational training in ASD populations.

 echnology Use in Vocational


T
Training Work Environment

As technology has increased, so has its integra- Individuals with ASD have previously been suc-
tion into vocational training approaches. Seeking cessfully employed in multiple settings (Hillier
to reduce the demand associated with behavior et al., 2007; Lawer et al., 2009). Placement is
skills training and expedite training, one group often dependent upon individual needs and the
17  Vocational Training 297

availability of specialized instruction focused on are more likely than peers to have sheltered
specific work-related requirements (Hagner & workshops identified in their transition plans
Cooney, 2005; Nuehring & Sitlington, 2003). A when exiting the primary education system.
recent shift has included an increased emphasis on People with ASD have higher rates of employ-
identifying appropriate and complimentary work ment in sheltered work environments relative to
environments based upon multiple factors (e.g., supported or competitive employment place-
skills level, work history, interest, support needs).ments. In one survey, over half of 66 individuals
Employment is most often broken down into with ASD indicated that they participate in a
three categories: competitive employment, sup- sheltered work environment (Taylor & Seltzer,
ported employment, and structured work environ- 2011). However, participation was strongly
ment. Placement into one of these categories is skewed based upon diagnosis as the researchers
not static, and in fact individuals may move across noted a significant discrepancy across partici-
groups throughout their career. Most often the pants. Specifically, an overwhelming majority
goal is for a person to ultimately be competitively (i.e., 97%) of participants had a comorbid diag-
employed without the use of supports. They may nosis of ID. Participation in sheltered employ-
not start out in such an environment and factors ment was less common for persons with ASD
such as changes in work environment, or task without comorbid ID. The implications of these
demand may necessitate the reintroduction of results could be quite drastic given that partici-
temporary or long-term work-based supports. pation in such services is a common goal identi-
fied in transition plans.
Early research surrounding sheltered work-
Sheltered Work Environments shops focused on underlying philosophies and
general models. More recently, Cimera (2011)
Whitehead (1979) described sheltered workshops investigated the utility of such programs from
as a facility with (A) a controlled environment an economical standpoint. Specifically, they
that (B) provides structured employment experi- sought to determine if participation significantly
ence and (C) focuses on rehabilitation and voca- altered employment outcomes. To evaluate this
tional training. Additional researchers stipulated idea, the authors analyzed 9,808 participants
that such a program is licensed by the Department that were separated into two groups matched by
of Labor and provides wages based upon a piece-­ gender, primary disability, and secondary dis-
rate system (Bond et al., 2001). An estimate by ability. Participants in the first group were
Braddock, Hemp, and Rizzolo (2008) indicated enrolled in a sheltered workshop prior to transi-
that there were approximately 542,127 adults tioning to supported employment. The second
with disabilities (i.e., mental, physical, and emo- group did not participate in a sheltered work-
tional) participating in sheltered workshops. shop program prior to supported employment.
These programs are meant to train persons with Minimal differences were observed in employ-
disabilities, providing them with perquisite skills ment rates; however, participants in the shel-
meant to increase the likelihood of vocational tered workshop worked significantly less and
success (Howlin et al., 2004). These programs subsequently earned significantly less. Cimera
also often include leisure and recreation activi- (2011) also reported that the expenses associ-
ties, group work placements, and special certifi- ated with participation in the sheltered work-
cations for participants. shop were approximately 75% higher than those
Sheltered workshops have traditionally associated with supported employment. The
served as a gateway to integration into sup- implications of these results are immense given
ported or competitive employment (Inge et al., that it is estimated that as much as 90% of per-
2009). This role is reflected in the transition sons with ASD and comorbid ID participate in
planning stage. Cameto, Marder, Wagner, and segregated service programs such as sheltered
Cardoso (2003) reported that people with ASD workshops (Butterworth et al., 2009).
298 M.J. Konst

Supported Work Environments The provision of ongoing external supports


may also provide additional supports and services
Drake and Becker (1996) defined supported that extend beyond the workplace. External sup-
employment as a job placement in the community port staff may advocate for the individual if fac-
with ongoing supports and coordination between tors related to the primary or a secondary diagnosis
worksite staff and vocational support staff. The begin to interfere with work performance (e.g.,
components constituting a supported employment surgery or hospitalization related to a secondary
environment are variable and dependent on need. medical condition). External supports may also
A supported work environment should include provide ongoing services such as counseling, cop-
specific provisions and individualized supports ing mechanisms, and social skills groups (Hillier
designed to enhance a person’s ability to perform et al., 2007; Howlin et al., 2005; Keel et al., 1997).
in the workplace (Smith & Philippen, 1999). Although supported employment programs are
Garcia-Villamisar and Hughes (2007) further increasingly more common, they are not always
identified that supported employment should be available (Wehman, West, & Kregel, 1999).
designed to enable participants to make meaning- Further, the provision of supported employment
ful contributions. A number of positive factors services is associated with a significantly greater
have been associated with inclusion in a supported cost relative to services associated with competi-
work environment, including increases in positive tive employment (Schaller & Yang, 2005).
social interactions, quality of life, salary, and rate
of employment (Chadsey & Beyer, 2001; Garcia-
Villamisar, Wehman, & Navarro, 2002; Hill, Competitive Work Environments
Wehman, Kregel, Banks, & Metzler, 1987;
Howlin et al., 2005). Additional researchers have The two-part definition of competitive employ-
also reported improved performance on measures ment proposed by Bond et al. (2001) included an
of cognitive abilities following employment emphasis on (A) placement in an integrated
(Garcia-Villamisar & Hughes, 2007). community-­based location and (B) earnings at or
Regardless of skills set and level of function- exceeding minimum wage. Taylor and Seltzer
ing, researchers assert that some degree of sup- (2011) defined competitive employment, as
ported employment is necessary – at least community-­based employment without supports.
initially – for people with ASD (Nuehring & Both descriptions emphasize community place-
Sitlington, 2003). Hendricks (2010) recom- ment as a key component. The latter definition,
mended that direct work supports be faded gradu- however, makes the distinction that no supports
ally across time. Additional researchers have or day services are present. Increased age, educa-
proposed that supports should not be completely tion, and the absence of secondary conditions
withdrawn, but monitored across time (Keel have been identified as predictors of successful
et al., 1997). This monitoring may lead to a placement in competitive employment (Schaller
decrease in external supports and may be achieved & Yang, 2005). Ozonoff, Rogers, and Hendron
by transitioning to internal supports (Hagner & (2003) suggested that increased age is likely
Cooney, 2005). Internal or natural supports often associated with increases in education as well as
incorporate an identified support system within life and work experience. Previously, placement
the company such as supervisors or coworkers into competitive, community-based jobs was
(Smith & Coleman, 1986). Internal supports pro- viewed as unlikely for people with ASD
vide more naturalistic and immediate feedback as (Mawhood & Howlin, 1999; Nesbitt, 2000;
changes occur in the work environment (Wehman, Smith, 1995). Subsequent research has dispelled
Inge, Revell, & Brooke, 2007). Although fading this notion. The rate of competitive employment
to naturalistic supports may be a goal, it is not is low relative to previously discussed vocational
always tenable and/or advantageous for the com- placements. Estimated rates have been as low as
pany or person with ASD. 6% (Taylor & Seltzer, 2011).
17  Vocational Training 299

The benefits associated with competitive before they become problematic. Persons partici-
employment are distinct. Competitive employ- pating in the TEACCH program are placed into
ment placement has been associated with higher one of three tiers based upon the level of services
earnings and hours worked (Gilmore, Schuster, required for successful placement: mobile crew,
Timmons, & Butterworth, 2000; Schaller & dispersed enclave, and individual placement
Yang, 2005). But placement directly into a com- (Keel et al., 1997).
petitive employment position can be difficult. The individual placement tier places the most
The person may be qualified for a minimum wage emphasis on independence in the work
position in a community-based environment. But ­environment (Keel et al., 1997). The person is
they may not be able to work in this environment initially paired with a job coach that provides
without some initial supports. The social demands supports during job identification and intensive
of the job, transportation needs, and organiza- on-the-job training. Within this tier, the intensity
tional skills may all be barriers to direct place- of supports is gradually faded, but individuals
ment. Oftentimes, maladaptive behaviors are typically receive ongoing supports. The dis-
severe enough to warrant behavior support plans persed enclave tier includes a single job coach
and specific interventions before gainful employ- that provides supports to multiple persons inde-
ment is achieved. With appropriate supports, pendently employed by the same employer
individuals may be able to work toward competi- (Keel et al., 1997). Persons within this tier
tive employment (Burt et al., 1991). Schaller and require daily supports, supervision, and training
Yang (2005) suggested that supported employ- to maintain employment. The mobile crew track
ment may initially be necessary as information includes the most intensive supports. Persons
regarding the individual will need to be gathered requiring this degree of support typically exhibit
continuously, across multiple sources (e.g., par- behaviors (e.g., aggression) that necessitate
ents, supervisors, and teachers) and environments direct supports and operate in a 2:1 or 3:1 client
(e.g., community, school, home, and work). The to staff ratio (Keel et al., 1997). This tier focuses
information gathered may then be used to iden- on the provision of community-based services
tify individual strengths and weaknesses and aid (e.g., housecleaning) under direct supervision of
in the development of individualized supports. a job coach. Though persons may initially par-
ticipate in the mobile crew track, placement is
not permanent. Keel et al. (1997) indicated that
Vocational Training Programs people may move to other tracks as their ability
and Associated Outcomes to operate independently increases.
The level of supports provided by the TEACCH-
Vocational Training Programs supported employment program extended beyond
the site of employment. Keel et al. (1997) empha-
Vocational programs such as TEACCH use a vari- sized that service provision must be proactive and
ety of methods to provide and maintain long-­term account for factors outside of work that may
supports. Of note, a supported employment pro- impact job performance. This is not a characteristic
gram is embedded within the broader TEACCH unique to persons with ASD; instead, most persons’
program. The TEACCH employment program is a work performance may be negatively affected by
global approach emphasizing the importance of extraneous variables (e.g., sleep disruption, trans-
collaboration between professionals and caregiv- portation issues, and illness). A major difference is
ers of adolescents with ASD (Schopler, 1994). an individual’s ability to cope with these factors. In
These methods include interacting with caregivers recognition of these factors, the supported employ-
and coworkers, site visits, and seeking updates ment program associated with TEACCH provided
from employers (Keel et al., 1997). Concerns iden- counseling and community skills training to better
tified in this manner may lead to the identification prepare individuals to cope (Keel et al., 1997).
of additional supports or additional training needs Initial research surrounding the TEACCH-
necessary to ameliorate employment ­difficulties supported employment program demonstrated a
300 M.J. Konst

96% placement rate for community employment attempting to match an individual’s interests with
and an 89% retention rate (Keel et al., 1997). No the positions available. Internships are only
control group was included in their analysis; how- selected when they provide experience relevant
ever, there has been limited follow-up research on to an individual’s career goals or provide the
the program. opportunity to obtain marketable skills that will
Keel et al. (1997) discussed three factors that lead to employment. The program also focuses
anecdotally influenced job retention. The authors on collaboration, across providers, employers,
noted that predictability – not repetition – was a the education system, and parents/caregivers
contributing factor to job retention. They empha- (Wehman et al., 2012). Primary case manage-
sized that the number of required job tasks was ment responsibilities are shared between the
not a significant factor as long as they were school and adult service agency. The inclusion of
scheduled or predictable. The focus on predict- both parties as equal partners encourages com-
ability is not limited to job tasks, but should also munication and aids in continuity of care as the
include the work environment. Factors such as persons with ASD reach adulthood.
noise or lighting in a work area should be assessed Results from an initial pilot study indicated that
prior to placement. The influence and impact of 78% of participants with severe disabilities were
environmental factors is variable across persons placed in competitive employment (Rutkowski
(Keel et al., 1997). Job retention was also notably et al., 2006). Recently a randomized clinical trial
influenced by the perceptions of coworkers and analyzing the extension of Project SEARCH for
employers. The authors reported that the provi- persons with ASD was carried out. Relative to 6%
sion of psychoeducation and the dissemination of of the control group, 87.5% of persons in the treat-
information related to ASD at job sites helped ment group were employed following program
foster a better work environment. participation (Wehman et al., 2014). Wehman
Project SEARCH is a community-based pro- et al. (2012) provided a review of the steps and
gram that emphasizes a close relationship with a supports necessary to extend the program to serv-
large business (e.g., hospital, bank, or govern- ing to adults with ASD. Given the unique needs of
ment office) that provides continuous job training persons with ASD, adaptations and additional sup-
and supports to persons with severe disabilities ports were identified as necessary components for
(Rutkowski, Daston, Van Kuiken, & Riehle, extending principles of the recent program. These
2006). Although initially developed for persons included the addition of a behavior analyst to tar-
with significant physical or cognitive disabilities get challenging behaviors. Behavior support plans
(e.g., cerebral palsy, visual impairments), the were introduced and included antecedent manipu-
program has been adapted to serve persons with lation and differential reinforcement strategies.
ASD (i.e., Project SEARCH plus ASD supports; The introduction of self-monitoring checklists
Wehman et al., 2012). The program requires col- was also introduced to target challenging behav-
laboration across schools, employers, and mem- iors and increase appropriate behaviors. A daily
bers of the vocational training system. schedule was created and reviewed with each stu-
Participation occurs during the final years of high dent to promote structure and to serve as a refer-
school and provides real-life work experience, ence. Included in this schedule were time devoted
placement assistance, and training for employ- to the instruction and generalization of social
ment and independent living skills. Participants skills in the work environment. Instruction
spend their entire school day at a specific job site included daily role-playing and practice to pro-
and complete an average of three, 10–12-week mote skills acquisition and generalization.
internships across the school year. Despite the separation of more than a decade,
Project SEARCH has an overarching focus on both of these programs employed similar
preparing the individual for competitive employ- approaches and made some of the same conclu-
ment. The program initially focuses on identify- sions. Similar to the conclusions of Keel et al.
ing and evaluating appropriate internships by (1997), Wehman et al. (2012) noted that the
17  Vocational Training 301

provision of supports must extend beyond the tors of executive functioning before and after
work environment to include social and commu- participation. Relative to peers in the sheltered
nication skills training. Both groups also noted workshop, people in the supported work group
the importance of visual supports and structure/ demonstrated significant improvements on 66%
routines at the site of employment. At a founda- of the executive functioning tasks (Garcia-
tional level, each program also demonstrated the Villamisar & Hughes, 2007).
positive effects of providing a continuum of care. The generalization of these findings is limited
Each program intervened while persons were still by the criteria used for participant selection and
completing their primary education. This factor randomization. In lieu of random assignment,
differs from other researchers who may other- participants were placed in the supported work
wise identify adults with ASD in various stages environments if they exhibited “acceptable”
of adulthood who are unemployed. The effects of vocational skills and had not previously worked
this approach are unknown at this time. But sev- in a supported work environment. Participants
eral factors of interest may be at play. The were excluded from the supported employment
approach taken by both programs decreases the program if they had a history of psychiatric disor-
likelihood of a person with ASD to “fall through ders, exhibited severe problem behaviors, or did
the cracks.” The establishment of a working rela- not have at least 2 years of supported employ-
tionship between school personnel, parents, and ment experience. The observed effects of partici-
adult service staff that is centered on gainful pation in supported work environments still
employment may have immense effects. In a sim- provide support for the inclusion of persons with
ilar manner, intervening with an emerging adult ASD in supported work programs.
and providing them with structured support and A series of studies carried out by Garcia-­
positive employment experiences may foster dif- Villamisar, Ross, and Wehman (2000) and
ferent attitudes toward employment while also García-Villamisar, Wehman, and Navarro (Garcia-
influencing employment outcomes. Although Villamisar et al., 2002) examined the immediate
each program targeted a different work environ- and longitudinal impact of participation in sup-
ment, they both further demonstrate the positive ported and sheltered employment for 52 persons
effects that systematic vocational services can with ASD. Variables of interest included overall
have on a person with ASD. quality of life and the manifestation of core ASD
symptomology. At a 30-month follow-up, partici-
pants in the supported work environment evi-
Vocational Training Outcomes denced improved quality of life scores. These
results were maintained at a 5-year follow-up
Indirect Outcomes (Garcia-Villamisar et al., 2002). No change was
Previous research has examined the cognitive observed for participants in the sheltered work-
benefits associated with participation in supported shop. No change in ASD symptom manifestation
employment relative to participation in sheltered was observed for persons in the supported work
work activities across a 3-year period (Garcia- environment, but persons in the sheltered work
Villamisar & Hughes, 2007). Persons in the shel- environment evidenced an increase in ASD
tered workshop were included as a wait list control symptoms (Garcia-Villamisar et al., 2000).
group that did not receive any additional supports
during the study. Participants in supported Direct Outcomes
employment were paired with a job coach and Mawhood and Howlin (1999) examined the
worked an average of 20 h per week with the aver- effects of participation in a supported employ-
age length of employment being 30 months. Job ment program across a 2-year period. Analyses
placements were primarily service-­based jobs in focused on the impact participation had upon job
the community (e.g., retail). Persons were admin- placement, duration of employment, and earn-
istered a battery of tests examining multiple fac- ings. The study included a wait list control group
302 M.J. Konst

for comparison. Vocational supports included aid Approximately 85% of participants indicated that
in identifying jobs, job training, psychoeduca- they were satisfied with their job placement.
tional trainings for employers and peers, and As part of their analyses, the authors also pro-
ongoing on-the-job training and support. vided estimates of overall cost of implementation
Mawhood and Howlin (1999) reported that including adjustments across time. The research
62% of persons in the supported employment pro- by Howlin et al. (2005) included some limitations
gram acquired employment relative to 25% of the in relation to generalization. Similar to the initial
control group. Persons in the supported work research, 92% of participants had an IQ greater
environment had higher rates of job retention and than 80. The authors also failed to include any
earned higher wages. No difference in hours form of a control group for direct comparison.
worked was observed across groups. The authors
noted that job placement was the most intensive
component, requiring extensive networking, Discussion
negotiation, and psychoeducation to identify
appropriate employment. Mawhood and Howlin Luecking and Gramlich (2003) made the asser-
(1999) included a broad discussion concerning tion that individuals with ASD should have the
the traditional lack of funding for job placement opportunity to participate in integrated employ-
and suggested that the high initial cost and demand ment. In addition to increased self-sufficiency
associated with such programs may decrease as and improvements related to quality of life,
networks are established. The project included a employment for persons with ASD also provides
thorough outline of project conception and devel- economic advantages. The addition of persons
opment lending to replication. Generalization of with ASD to the workforce increases their partici-
these findings is possible, but participants were pation in the economy and may help to decrease
excluded if they had an IQ below 70, had comor- their reliance on government funding (Järbrink,
bid conditions, or had any condition that would McCrone, Fombonne, Zandén, & Knapp, 2007).
“adversely affect” employment. The research outlined above provides a dem-
Howlin et al. (2005) followed up and extended onstration of the indirect and direct effects asso-
the earlier work by Mawhood and Howlin (1999) ciated with employment across multiple domains.
utilizing a longitudinal analysis of 147 partici- The research reviewed also provides the basis for
pants. Participants were followed for up to the development of vocational training programs
8 years to examine the longitudinal effects of designed specifically to assist people with
vocational training on employment. Howlin et al. ASD. The broad picture suggests that such pro-
(2005) replicated original findings, demonstrat- grams require a high initial investment coupled
ing that participants in supported work settings with the provision of ongoing supports. At this
evidenced elevated rates of employment. point, the evidence gathered suggests that partici-
Observed employment rates ranged from 54% to pation in supported employment is less expensive
70% dependent on the regions of employment and more advantageous than sheltered work-
analyzed (Howlin et al., 2005). Primary employ- shops. It is important to note that this may not be
ment industries were administrative, computing, true for all individuals with ASD. A majority of
or technical work. Job placement spanned a wide the research reviewed above included persons
array of placements including large private com- without ID. Future researchers will need to inves-
panies, government organizations, charitable tigate the same analyses in groups with comorbid
organizations, and small private companies ASD and ID, especially given the elevated rates
(Howlin et al., 2005). At follow-up, 68% of par- of comorbidity observed. Future researchers will
ticipants in the supported work group continued also be charged with continuing to develop and
to be employed 7 and 8 years later. Howlin et al. adapt these programs to fluctuations in the labor
(2005) also collected anecdotal information force and changes in job availability. Although
about job satisfaction and impression of the each study has notable limitations, they each
­overall process from participants and employers. have their merits and include ample information
17  Vocational Training 303

lending to replication. The importance of further often than atypically ­developing peers (Roux et al.,
advancements is underscored by the increasing 2013). This also means that persons with ASD are
prevalence of ASD diagnosis and the need to find more likely to remain financially dependent on
stable employment in adulthood. caregivers, even when competitively employed.
A cross-sectional study identified two signifi-
cant trends for vocational training for adults with
General Discussion ASD. First, adults with ASD are denied access to
such services twice as often when compared to dis-
Parents naturally have ideals and goals for their abled peers (Lawer et al., 2009). Second, on-­the-­
children and their futures. These ambitions do job supports are beneficial for job acquisition and
not simply disappear when their child is diag- retention (Lawer et al., 2009). This trend is proba-
nosed with ASD. Exemplifying this idea are the bly influenced by the structure of current voca-
questions parents commonly ask upon receiving tional programs. Initially, the adaptation of
a diagnosis (e.g., “Will they be able to have a vocational services from other populations simply
job?”; “Will they be able to attend school?”). As meant targeting the same concerns in a new popu-
scientists and practitioners, we are often averse to lation (e.g., stereotypical behaviors). Much of the
providing definitive answers to such questions. It early available research surrounding vocational
is not unwise to display caution and provide training for adults with ASD was adapted from use
broader responses to these questions given the with ID populations. These strategies were largely
heterogeneity of ASD and the relative dearth of behavioral in nature and based upon the principles
outcome information that would begin to inform of applied behavior analysis. These techniques
such questions. The topics of employment and were used to not only increase job performance
independence are often difficult to separate and and independence but also decrease maladaptive
provide concrete answers. behaviors. In addition to behavioral strategies, the
Research investigating the positive benefits of provision of on-the-job support emerged as a fun-
employment is clear. Employment has been dem- damental feature – at least initially – in most voca-
onstrated to have both direct and indirect benefits tional training research. Subsequent research has
across multiple domains (Garcia-Villamisar et al., become increasingly broad in scope with an
2002; Garcia-Villamisar & Hughes, 2007). The emerging emphasis on the development of global
employment of persons with ASD is also benefi- programs. In addition to preexisting components
cial to society (Howlin et al., 2005). Yet unem- of vocational training programs, adults with ASD
ployment rates for persons with ASD are have been shown to benefit from increased staff to
high – and stable – regardless of the country ana- employee ratios and the inclusion of training
lyzed (Eaves & Ho, 2008; Howlin, Goode, Hutton, emphasizing social communication (Morgan,
and Rutter, (2004); Taylor & Seltzer, 2011). 1996; Wehman et al., 2012). Functional skills set,
A number of barriers to participation in voca- median household income, and communication
tional training exist for adults with ASD. The ability have each been identified as predictors of
absence of a comorbid ID diagnosis has been noted positive employment outcomes (Cimera & Cowan,
to prohibit participation in state of federally funded 2009; Roux et al., 2013).
programs (Müller et al., 2003). Persons with ASD Researchers have continued to identify key
may also be excluded due to the severity of prob- components to guide program development.
lem behaviors (Lawer et al., 2009). Additional bar- These components most often include an empha-
riers include the previously limited research sis on supports for initial placement, on-the-job
demonstrating the efficacy of individual vocational training, psychoeducation, and the provision of
training programs and the intensive services typi- ongoing support (Hendricks, 2010). Oftentimes,
cally required (Müller et al., 2003; Lawer et al., a job coach or similar position is created to facili-
2009; Cimera & Cowan, 2009). Not surprisingly, tate the delivery of comprehensive services. The
persons with ASD are consistently observed to role of a job coach is multifaceted and dependent
work less, earn less, and be ­unemployed more on the needs of the individual. Common tasks
304 M.J. Konst

include on-the-job training, social integration, serve as an effective and necessary bridge that
provision of psychoeducation, and navigation of spans the gap, that is, the transition from primary
the job site. An emerging component of voca- education into adulthood and the workforce. This
tional training worthy of further investigation has approach works to initiate collaborative efforts
been the use of technology. Early research across care providers and to facilitate transitions.
focused on the use of PDAs to function as The bulk of available research on vocational
prompting systems designed to increase task per- training can best be separated into three work
formance. The combination of PDA use and environments. These environments are differenti-
behavioral skills training was shown to increase ated on multiple levels, but the intensity of sup-
training efficiency. Researchers have also moved port services provided is used as a distinguishing
to increasing the use of technology in vocational feature. A sheltered workshop is designed to pro-
training. Initial researchers have reported positive vide the most intensive degree of supports and
effects when using technology to prepare indi- structure. One of the primary goals of sheltered
viduals for interviews and general social interac- workshops is to provide vocational training and
tions in the workplace. Technology has also been exposure to the contingencies associated with
used as external prompting and to augment employment. A sheltered workshop placement
communication. has often been the placement recommended for
The development and expansion of programs emerging adults. Recently researchers have
such as TEACCH and Project SEARCH plus called into question the efficacy and economic
ASD Supports has been driven by the unique impact of this practice. Persons in a sheltered
vocational needs of adults with ASD (Keel et al., workshop earn less than peers in other settings
1997; Rutkowski et al., 2006; Wehman et al., (Cimera, 2011; Howlin et al., 2004). The cost
2012). Both programs emphasize a continuum of associated with managing a sheltered workshop
care that begins in late adolescence and continues is also greater than other employment settings.
through adulthood – specifically through the key To date, a large amount of research has
period when individuals age out of primary edu- focused on the provision of services and the
cation services. Supports include an emphasis on effects associated with supported work environ-
a close and collaborative working relationship ments. Areas that have been identified to be posi-
with employers and coworkers and focus on the tively impacted by participation in supported
provision of on-the-job supports. Both programs work environments include quality of life, ASD
also highlight the importance of supports outside symptom manifestation, cognition, and social
of work that may directly or indirectly impact skills. These benefits have been observed when
work performance (e.g., transportation or illness). persons were compared to wait list control groups
The intensity of supports is based upon individual and persons participating in sheltered workshops.
need. The TEACCH program further individual- Persons with ASD are noted to benefit from some
izes services by separating placement into a hier- degree of supports regardless of level of function-
archy based upon the anticipated support needs. ing (Nuehring & Sitlington, 2003). These sup-
One overarching commonality and point of focus ports may then be faded, directed at different
for both programs is the attention given to col- targets, or transferred to naturally occurring sup-
laboration across caregivers. These programs ports in the work environment (Hagner &
work to ensure that all parties ­necessary are pres- Cooney, 2005; Hendricks, 2010; Keel et al.,
ent and active members. Another commonality is 1997). Employment supports aside, additional
the antecedent-based approach to identifying and supports target factors outside of the work envi-
ameliorating disruptive behaviors, including the ronment that may negatively impact job perfor-
use of daily schedules to communicate with the mance (e.g., social skills; Hillier et al., 2007;
employee. The creators of TEACCH also empha- Howlin et al., 2005; Keel et al., 1997).
sized that predictability – not repetition – was key Descriptions of competitive employment
for positive job performance. Both programs focus on placement within the community and
17  Vocational Training 305

payment commensurate with minimum wage enrollment by people with ASD in the vocational
(Bond et al., 2001). Other researchers have indi- rehabilitation system increased by 121%. This
cated that competitive employment should also influx is especially problematic when traditional
be distinguished by the absence of supports approaches to vocational training are coupled
(Taylor & Seltzer, 2011). The idea of individuals with the increased intensity of services that have
with ASD being completely free of supports in been demonstrated to be necessary for people
the work environment is uncertain. Our definition with ASD (Hillier et al., 2007; Lawer et al., 2009;
of competitive employment would become Wehman et al., 2012).
clearer after defining what “supports” do and do The comparative research analyzing work
not consist of. Most employed persons rely on environment has grown as interest in vocational
some degree of natural or contrived support training has grown. At this point researchers have
structure, coworkers, supervisors, or human looked at various outcomes associated with voca-
resource staff. Given this concession, our defini- tional training: cognition and executive function-
tion of “support” should specify what is provided ing, wages earned versus hours worked, length of
by identifying them as (1) supports provided on employment, quality of life, ASD symptom
the job by an external entity (e.g., job coach), (2) severity, job placement, and job satisfaction. To
supports provided outside of the workplace by an date, relatively few studies have actually ana-
entity focusing on improving job performance or lyzed the same outcomes, reported the same
skills development, (3) technological supports information, used the same population, or used
provided on the job to increase performance, or compatible outcome measures, even when mea-
(4) supports provided on the job by an internal suring the comparable outcomes. The ramifica-
entity (e.g., supervisor). The presence of such tions of these discrepancies are large. The
supports would identify the placement as a sup- conclusions drawn from single studies are often
ported work environment; the fading and removal used to bolster a larger, overarching ideology and
of these supports would be considered placement conceptualization. Without commonalities across
in competitive employment if they were also multiple studies, however, the conclusions drawn
receiving minimum wage. are less cohesive and thus do less to advance
An analysis of employment databases sug- research surrounding vocational training.
gested that 55% of employed persons with ASD Research examining the specific components
were competitively employed (Schaller & Yang, contributing to the efficacy of vocational training
2005). This information must be interpreted with programs is limited. Research is emerging but is
care given the low overall rates of employment criticized due to poor experimental design (Taylor
for persons with ASD. Increases in hours worked et al., 2012). In their review of the existent litera-
and wages earned are positive factors associated ture base surrounding the efficacy of vocational
with competitive employment (Gilmore et al., training programs, Taylor et al. (2012) identified a
2000; Schaller & Yang, 2005). Yet researchers total of six publications that examined vocational
have demonstrated that the wages typically training and included more than 20 participants.
earned in competitive employment are still con- Determination of study design quality was based
sidered to be below the poverty level (Burgess & upon diagnostic approaches, statistical analysis,
Cimera, 2014). intervention description, outcome measurement,
and study design. The authors concluded that all
of the reviewed articles were of “poor quality.”
Future Directions Factors that were identified as contributing to
poor study design included nonrandom group
The increasing demand for vocational training is assignment, failure to describe interventions, fail-
underscored by the rise in persons with ASD ure to assess treatment fidelity, absence of a con-
entering adulthood. Cimera and Cowan (2009) trol group, and lack of systematic diagnostic
reported that across a 4-year span (2002–2006), process (Taylor et al., 2012). The results of this
306 M.J. Konst

review indicate that an emphasis on quality One clear need is a continued emphasis on the
research design is necessary for future research development and refinement of a core program or
endeavors. agency dedicated to facilitating vocational train-
Future researchers should strive to adhere to ing for adults with ASD. This would likely be at
the reporting of information directly relevant to a regional or state level to most effectively maxi-
vocational training as measures of outcome. A mize local resources, building partnerships with
review of previously reported research outcomes local businesses, and be in a position to navigate
would suggest that a few key factors are univer- the differences in service availability and provi-
sal to vocational training programs and can be sion. The programs reviewed above have each
reliably measured. These factors include (1) demonstrated one series of characteristics com-
duration of employment, (2) wages earned, (3) monly observed in traditional business models.
hours worked, (4) work environment, and (5) Most programs were initiated with a small con-
intensity of services provided. Service intensity certed effort, providing services in a localized
would best be captured as a percentage of hours geographic area to a few persons. These pro-
worked (e.g., 10% of total hours at work). grams were able to grow and expand to accom-
Factors that warrant further analysis include the modate rising demands as they became more
development of a systematic approach to guide efficient in service provision. This growth was
the titration of on-the-job supports. A likely typically associated with increased cost-­
approach may employ the use of a flowchart or effectiveness. The positive benefits were not
decision checklist that is used to guide the deci- always generalized across settings, even when a
sion to decrease supports based upon the indi- program was successful in a different geographic
vidual’s capability to perform job-related tasks location. Howlin et al. (2005) reported fluctua-
independently. tions in outcomes as they expanded to new geo-
Additional research regarding the increased graphic regions and encountered new challenges.
need for supports specific to social interactions This barrier is not unique to vocational training
is also necessary. Although multiple researchers and may be managed in the same manner that we
alluded to this component as a necessary feature promote generalization in other treatments and
of vocational training, very little information capacities, through adaptation and overt steps
about intensity, setting, and duration of services aimed at promoting generalization. The impor-
were provided. This is also associated with an tant factor is the continuation of efforts to iden-
absence in outcomes research demonstrating the tify difficulties as growth occurs, document the
efficacy of job-based socialization training. The adaptations made, and monitor their efficacy.
necessity of such work is underscored by the
investment associated with the provision of each
component of vocational training services. From References
an investment perspective, the ability to con-
cretely demonstrate effective training and place- Bellstedt, E., Gillberg, C., & Gillberg, C. (2005). Autism
ment is invaluable. The answer to questions after adolescence: Population-based 13- to 22-year
such as “How much support?” and “How many follow-­up study of 120 individuals with autism
diagnosed in childhood. Journal of Autism and
hours of social skills training?” is important to Developmental Disorders, 35, 351–360.
employers, employees, support staff, and care- Berkman, K. A., & Meyer, L. H. (1988). Alternative
givers. Across the continuum of work environ- strategies and multiple outcomes in the remedia-
ments, a growing emphasis on cost-effectiveness tion of severe self-injury: Going “all out” nonaver-
sively. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
has emerged. Researchers have repeatedly dem- Disabilities, 13(2), 76–86.
onstrated increased initial costs associated with Billstedt, C. G. E. (2000). Autism and Asperger syn-
the provision of more intensive services. This drome: Coexistence with other clinical disorders.
initial cost has also been reported to decrease Aacta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 102, 321–330.
Bolman, W. M. (2008). Brief report: 25-year follow-up
longitudinally as the program operates and of a high-functioning autistic child. Journal of Autism
increases efficiency. and Developmental Disorders, 38(1), 181–183.
17  Vocational Training 307

Bond, G. R., Resnick, S. G., Drake, R. E., Xie, H., through vocational tasks for students with ­moderate
McHugo, G. J., & Bebout, R. R. (2001). Does com- and severe intellectual disabilities. Education and
petitive employment improve nonvocational out- Training in Developmental Disabilities, 102–110.
comes for people with severe mental illness? Journal Cimera, R. E. (2011). Does being in sheltered workshops
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69(3), 489. improve the employment outcomes of supported
Braddock, D., Hemp, R., & Rizzolo, M. C. (2008). The employees with intellectual disabilities? Journal of
state of the states in developmental disabilities: Vocational Rehabilitation, 35(1), 21–27.
2008. Washington, DC: American Association on Cimera, R., & Cowan, R. (2009). The costs of services
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. and employment outcomes achieved by adults with
Burgess, S., & Cimera, R. E. (2014). Employment autism in the US. Autism: The International Journal
outcomes of transition-aged adults with autism of Research and Practice, 13, 285–302.
spectrum disorders: A state of the state’s report. Drake, R. E., & Becker, D. R. (1996). The individual
American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental placement and support model of supported employ-
Disabilities, 119(1), 64–83. ment. Psychiatric Services, 47(5), 473–475.
Burke, R. V., Andersen, M. N., Bowen, S. L., Howard, M. Eaves, L. C., & Ho, H. H. (2008). Young adult outcome
R., & Allen, K. D. (2010). Evaluation of two instruc- of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
tion methods to increase employment options for Developmental Disorders, 38(4), 739–747.
young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Research Engstrom, I., Ekstrom, L., & Emilsson, B. (2003).
in Developmental Disabilities, 31(6), 1223–1233. Psychological functioning in a group of Swedish
Burt, D. B., Fuller, S. P., & Lewis, K. R. (1991). Brief adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning
report: Competitive employment of adults with autism. autism. Autism, 7, 99–110.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Esbensen, A. J., Bishop, S., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg,
21(2), 237–242. J. S., & Taylor, J. L. (2010). Comparisons between
Butterworth, J., Smith, F. A., Hall, A. C., Migliore, A., individuals with autism spectrum disorders and
& Winsor, J. (2009). State data: The National Report individuals with Down syndrome in adulthood.
on employment services and outcomes. Boston, MA: American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental
Institute for Community Inclusion, University of Disabilities, 115(4), 277–290.
Massachusetts. Farley, M. A., McMahon, W. M., Fombonne, E., Jenson,
Camarena, P. M., & Sarigiani, P. A. (2009). Postsecondary W. R., Miller, J., Gardner, M., … Coon, H. (2009).
educational aspirations of high-functioning adolescents Twenty-year outcome for individuals with autism and
with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Focus average or near-average cognitive abilities. Autism
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. Research, 2(2), 109–118.
Cameto, R., Marder, C., Wagner, M., & Cardoso, Fast, Y. (2004). Employment for individuals with Asperger
D. (2003). Youth employment, NLTS2 data brief, syndrome or non-verbal learning disability: Stories
National Center on secondary education and transi- and strategies. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
tion. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota. Foley, B. E., & Staples, A. H. (2003). Developing aug-
Cameto, R., Levine, P., & Wagner, M. (2004). Transition mentative and alternative communication (AAC) and
Planning for Students with Disabilities: A Special Topic literacy interventions in a supported employment set-
Report of Findings from the National Longitudinal ting. Topics in Language Disorders, 23(4), 325–343.
Transition Study-2 (NLTS2). Washington, DC: National Fombonne, E. (2003). Epidemiological surveys of autism
Center for Special Education Research. and other pervasive developmental disorders: An
Cedurland, M., Hagberg, B., Billstedt, E., Gillberg, I. C., update. Journal of Autism and Other Developmental
& Gillberg, C. (2008). Asperger syndrome and autism: Disorders, 23, 365–382.
A comparative longitudinal follow-up study more than deFur, S. H., & Patton, J. R. (1999). Transition and school-
five years after original diagnosis. Journal of Autism based services: Interdisciplinary perspectives for
and Developmental Disorders, 38, 72–85. enhancing the transition process. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.
Certo, N. J., Luecking, R. G., Murphy, S., Brown, L., Garcia-Villamisar, D., & Hughes, C. (2007). Supported
Courey, S., & Belanger, D. (2008). Seamless transi- employment improves cognitive performance in
tion and long-term support for individuals with severe adults with autism. Journal Intellectual Disabilities
intellectual disabilities. Research and Practice for Research, 51, 142–150.
Persons with Severe Disabilities, 33, 85–95. Garcia-Villamisar, D., Ross, D., & Wehman, P. (2000).
Certo, N. J., Mautz, D., Pumpian, I., Sax, C., Smalley, Clinical differential analysis of persons with autism
K., Wade, H. A., ... Batterman, N. (2003). Review in a work setting: A follow-up study. Journal of
and discussion of a model for seamless transition to Vocational Rehabilitation, 14(3), 183–185.
adulthood. Education and Training in Developmental Garcia-Villamisar, D., Wehman, P., & Navarro, M. D.
Disabilities, 3–17. (2002). Changes in the quality of autistic people’s
Chadsey, J., & Beyer, S. (2001). Social relationships in the life that work in supported and sheltered employ-
workplace. Mental Retardation and Developmental ment. A 5-year follow-up study. Journal of Vocational
Disabilities Research Reviews, 7(2), 128–133. Rehabilitation, 17, 309–312.
Cihak, D. F., Kessler, K., & Alberto, P. A. (2008). Use of a Gentry, T., Lau, S., Molinelli, A., Fallen, A., & Kriner, R.
handheld prompting system to transition independently (2012). The apple iPod touch as a vocational support
308 M.J. Konst

aid for adults with autism: Three case studies. Journal vice for high-ability adults with autism or Asperger
of Vocational Rehabilitation, 37(2), 75–85. syndrome. Autism, 9(5), 533–549.
Gerhardt, P. F., & Holmes, D. L. (2005). Employment: Howlin, P., & Goode, S. (1998). Outcome in adult life
Options and issues for adolescents and adults with for individuals with autism. In F. Volkmar (Ed.),
autism spectrum disorders. In F. R. Volkmar, R. Paul, Autism and developmental disorders. New York, NY:
A. Klin, & D. Cohen (Eds.), Handbook of autism Cambridge University Press.
and pervasive developmental disorders, Vol. 2: Howlin, P., Goode, S., Hutton, J., & Rutter, M. (2004).
Assessment, interventions, and policy (3rd ed.pp. Adult outcome for children with autism. Journal of
1087–1101). New York: Wiley. Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(2), 212–229.
Gilchrist, A., Green, J., Cox, A., Burton, D., Rutter, M., Hume, K., & Odom, S. (2007). Effects of an indi-
& Le Couteur, A. (2001). Development and current vidual work system on the independent function-
functioning in adolescents with Asperger syndrome: A ing of students with autism. Journal of Autism and
comparative study. Journal of Child Psychology and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1166–1180.
Psychiatry, 42, 227–240. Hurlbutt, K., & Chalmers, L. (2002). Adults with autism speak
Gilmore, D. S., Schuster, J. L., Timmons, J. C., & out perceptions of their life experiences. Focus on Autism
Butterworth, J. (2000). An analysis of trends for peo- and Other Developmental Disabilities, 17(2), 103–111.
ple with MR, cerebral palsy, and epilepsy receiving Hurlbutt, K., & Chalmers, L. (2004). Employment and
services from state VR agencies ten years of progress. adults with Asperger syndrome. Focus on Autism and
Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 44(1), 30–38. Other Developmental Disabilities, 19, 215–222.
Gray, C. A. (1998). Social stories and comic strip con- Inge, K. J., Wehman, P., Revell, G., Erickson, D., Butterworth,
versations with students with Asperger syndrome J., & Gilmore, D. (2009). Survey results from a national
and high-functioning autism. In E. Schopler, G. B. survey of community rehabilitation providers hold-
Mesibox, & L. J. Kunce (Eds.), Asperger syndrome ing special wage certificates. Journal of Vocational
or high-functioning autism? (pp. 167–198). Springer Rehabilitation, 30, 67–85.
US). New York, NY: Plenum Press. Iovannone, R., Dunlap, G., Huber, H., & Kincaid, D.
Gunby, K. V., Carr, J. E., & LeBlanc, L. A. (2010). Teaching (2003). Effective educational practices for students
abduction-prevention skills to children with autism. with autism spectrum disorders. Focus on Autism and
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 43(1), 107–112. Other Developmental Disabilities, 18, 150–165.
Hagner, D., & Cooney, B. F. (2005). “I do that for every- Järbrink, K., McCrone, P., Fombonne, E., Zandén, H., &
body”: Supervising employees with autism. Focus on Knapp, M. (2007). Cost-impact of young adults with
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 20(2), high-functioning autistic spectrum disorder. Research
91–97. in Developmental Disabilities, 28(1), 94–104.
Hendricks, D. (2010). Employment and adults with Jennes-Coussens, M., Magill-Evans, J., & Koning, C.
autism spectrum disorders: Challenges and strate- (2006). The quality of life of young men with Asperger
gies for success. Journal of Vocational Rehabilitation, syndrome: A brief report. Autism, 10, 403–414.
32(2), 125–134. Kanner, L. (1973). Childhood psychosis: Initial studies
Hendricks, D. R., & Wehman, P. (2009). Transition from and new insights. New York, NY: Wiley.
school to adulthood for youth with autism spectrum Keel, J. H., Mesibov, G. B., & Woods, A. V. (1997).
disorders: Review and recommendations. Focus on TEACCH-supported employment program. Journal
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(1), 3–9.
Hill, M. L., Wehman, P. H., Kregel, J., Banks, P. D., & Kellems, R. O., & Morningstar, M. E. (2012). Using video
Metzler, H. M. (1987). Employment outcomes for modeling delivered through iPods to teach vocational
people with moderate and severe disabilities: An tasks to young adults with autism spectrum disorders.
eight-year longitudinal analysis of supported competi- Career Development and Transition for Exceptional
tive employment. Research and Practice for Persons Individuals, 35(3), 155–167.
with Severe Disabilities, 12(3), 182–189. Kemp, D. C., & Carr, E. G. (1995). Reduction of severe
Hillier, A., Campbell, H., Mastriani, K., Izzo, M. V., Kool-­ problem behavior in community employment using
Tucker, A. K., Cherry, L., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). an hypothesis-driven multicomponent intervention
Two-year evaluation of a vocational support program approach. Research and Practice for Persons with
for adults on the autism spectrum. Career Development Severe Disabilities, 20(4), 229–247.
for Exceptional Individuals, 30(1), 35–47. Kim, J. A., Szatmari, P., Bryson, S. E., Streiner, D. L.,
Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nydén, A., Wentz, & Wilson, F. J. (2000). The prevalence of anxiety
E., Ståhlberg, O., … Råstam, M. (2009). Psychiatric and and mood problems among children with autism and
psychosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence Asperger syndrome. Autism, 4(2), 117–132.
autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 9(1), 1. Kobayashi, R., & Murata, T. (1998). Behavioral charac-
Howlin, P. (2000). Outcome in adult life for more able teristics of 187 young adults with autism. Psychiatry
individuals with autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism, and Clinical Neurosciences, 52(4), 383–390.
4(1), 63–83. Koning, C., & Magill-Evans, J. (2001). Social and lan-
Howlin, P., Alcock, J., & Burkin, C. (2005). An 8 year guage skills in adolescent boys with Asperger syn-
follow-up of a specialist supported employment ser- drome. Autism, 5(1), 23–36.
17  Vocational Training 309

La Malfa, G., Lassi, S., Bertelli, M., Salvini, R., & Placidi, trum disabilities. Journal of Vocational Rehabilitation,
G. F. (2004). Autism and intellectual disability: A study of 18(3), 163–175.
prevalence on a sample of the Italian population. Journal Nesbitt, S. (2000). Why and why not? Factors influencing
of Intellectual Disability Research, 48(3), 262–267. employment for individuals with Asperger syndrome.
Landa, R. J., & Goldberg, M. C. (2005). Language, social, Autism, 4(4), 357–369.
and executive functions in high functioning autism: Nuehring, M. L., & Sitlington, P. L. (2003). Transition as
A continuum of performance. Journal of Autism and a vehicle: Moving from high school to an adult voca-
Developmental Disorders, 35(5), 557–573. tional service provider. Journal of Disability Policy
Lattimore, L. P., Parsons, M. B., & Reid, D. H. (2006). Studies, 14, 23–35.
Enhancing job site training on supported workers O’Brien, M., & Daggett, J. A. (2006). Beyond the autism
with autism: A reemphasis on simulation. Journal of diagnosis: A professional’s guide to helping families.
Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 91–102. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Lawer, L., Brusilovskiy, E., Salzer, M. S., & Mandell, Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S., & Hendron, R. (Eds.). (2003).
D. S. (2009). Use of vocational rehabilitative services Autism spectrum disorders: A research review for practi-
among adults with autism. Journal of Autism and tioners. London, UK: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Developmental Disorders, 39(3), 487–494. Patterson, A., & Rafferty, A. (2001). Making it to work:
Liptak, G. S., Benzoni, L. B., Mruzek, D. W., Nolan, K. W., Towards employment for the young adult with
Thingvoll, M. A., Wade, C. M., & Fryer, G. E. (2008). autism. International Journal of Language and
Disparities in diagnosis and access to health services Communication Disorders, 36(S1), 475–480.
for children with autism: data from the National Survey Riffel, L. A., Wehmeyer, M. L., Turnbull, A. P., & Lattimore,
of Children’s Health. Journal of Developmental and J. (2005). Promoting independent performance of
Behavioral Pediatrics, 29(3), 152–160. transition-related tasks using a palmtop PC-based self-
Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). directed visual and auditory prompting system. Journal
Examining the relationship between executive func- of Special Education Technology, 20(2), 5.
tions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autis- Roux, A. M., Shattuck, P. T., Cooper, B. P., Anderson, K. A.,
tic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Wagner, M., & Narendorf, S. C. (2013). Postsecondary
Disorders, 35(4), 445–460. employment experiences among young adults with an
Luecking, R., & Gramlich, M. (2003). Quality work-based autism spectrum disorder. Journal of the American
learning and postschool employment success. Issue Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(9),
Brief: Examining Current Challenges in Secondary 931–939.
Education and Transition, 2(2), 1–5. Rutkowski, S., Daston, M., Van Kuiken, D., & Riehle,
Matson, J. L., & Shoemaker, M. (2009). Intellectual disability E. (2006). Project SEARCH: A demand-side model
and its relationship to autism spectrum disorders. Research of high school transition. Journal of Vocational
in Developmental Disabilities, 30(6), 1107–1114. Rehabilitation, 25(2), 85–96.
Mawhood, L., & Howlin, P. (1999). The outcome of a Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogen-
supported employment scheme for high-functioning esis of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and
adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Autism, Psychiatry, 24(4), 513–531.
3(3), 229–254. Schall, C., Cortijo-Doval, E., Targett, P. S., & Wehman,
Mawhood, L., Howlin, P., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism P. (2006). Applications for youth with autism spec-
and developmental receptive language disorder—A trum disorders. In P. Wehman (Ed.), Life beyond the
comparative follow-up in early adult life. I: Cognitive classroom: Transition strategies for young people
and language outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology with disabilities (4th ed.pp. 535–575). Baltimore: Paul
and Psychiatry, 41, 547–559. H. Brookes Publishing Co..
Migliore, A., Timmons, J., Butterworth, J., & Lugas, J. Schall, C., & Wehman, P. (2008). Understanding the
(2012). Predictors of employment and postsecond- transition from school to adulthood for students with
ary education of youth with autism. Rehabilitation autism. In P. Wehman, M. D. Smith, & C. Schall
Counseling Bulletin, 55(3), 176–184. (Eds.), Autism and the transition to adulthood:
Miltenberger, R. G. (2008). Behavior modification: Success beyond the classroom (pp. 1–14). Baltimore:
Principles and procedures (4th ed.). Pacific Grove, Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
CA: Thomson/Wadsworth. Schaller, J., & Yang, N. K. (2005). Competitive employ-
Morgan, H. (1996). Underpinning philosophy in the pro- ment for people with autism correlates of successful
vision of services for adults with autism: A critique of closure in competitive and supported employment.
global values related to specific practice. Adults with Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin, 49(1), 4–16.
autism: A guide to theory and practice, 31–52. Schopler, E. (1994). A statewide program for the treat-
Moxon, L., & Gates, D. (2001). Children with autism: ment and education of autistic and related communi-
Supporting the transition to adulthood. Educational cation handicapped children (TEACCH). Psychoses
and Child Psychology, 18(2), 28–40. and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 3(7),
Müller, E., Schuler, A., Burton, B. A., & Yates, G. B. 91–103.
(2003). Meeting the vocational support needs of indi- Shattuck, P. T., Wagner, M., Narendorf, S., Sterzing, P.,
viduals with Asperger syndrome and other autism spec- & Hensley, M. (2011). Post–high school service use
among young adults with an autism spectrum disorder.
310 M.J. Konst

Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescent Medicine, 165(2), Van Bourgondien, M. E., Reichle, N. C., & Palmer,
141–146. A. (1997). Sexual behavior in adults with autism.
Shattuck, P. T., Narendorf, S. C., Cooper, B., Sterzing, P. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
R., Wagner, M., & Taylor, J. L. (2012). Postsecondary 27(2), 113–125.
education and employment among youth with Van Laarhoven, T., Johnson, J. W., Van Laarhoven-­
an autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics, 129(6), Myers, T., Grider, K. L., & Grider, K. M. (2009). The
1042–1049. effectiveness of using a video iPod as a prompting
Smith, M. D. (1985). Managing the aggressive and device in employment settings. Journal of Behavioral
self-injurious behavior of adults disabled by autism. Education, 18(2), 119–141.
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Volkmar, F. R., Stier, D. M., & Cohen, D. J. (1985). Age
Handicaps, 10(4), 228–232. of recognition of pervasive developmental disorder.
Smith, M. D. (1986). Managing the aggressive and self-­ American Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1450–1452.
injurious behaviors of adults disabled by autism in the Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Garza, N., &
community. Journal of the Association for Persons Levine, P. (2005). After high school: A first look
with Severe Handicaps, 10, 228–232. at the postschool experiences of youth with dis-
Smith, M. D. (1987). Treatment of pica in an adult dis- abilities. A report from the National Longitudinal
abled by autism by differential reinforcement of Transition Study–2 (NLTS-2). Menlo Park, CA: SRI
incompatible behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy International.
and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 285–288. Wehman, P. (2006). Individualized transition planning:
Smith, M. D. (1995). A guide to successful employ- Putting self-determination into action. In P. Wehman
ment for individuals with autism. Baltimore: Paul H. (Ed.), Life beyond the classroom: Transition strategies
Brookes Publishing Co. (stock# 1715, $31). for young people with disabilities (4th ed.pp. 71–96).
Smith, M. D., Belcher, R. G., & Juhrs, P. D. (1995). A Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co.
guide to successful employment for individuals with Wehman, P., Inge, K. J., Revell, W. G., & Brooke, V. A.
autism. Baltimore, MD: Brookes. (2007). Real work for real pay: Inclusive employment
Smith, M. D., & Coleman, D. (1986). Managing the for people with disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul
behavior of adults with autism in the job setting. H. Brookes Publishing Co..
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Wehman, P. (Ed.). (2001). Supported employment in busi-
16(2), 145–154. ness: Expanding the capacity of workers with disabili-
Smith, M. J., Ginger, E. J., Wright, K., Wright, M. A., ties. Training Resource Network Incorporated.
Taylor, J. L., Humm, L. B., … Fleming, M. F. (2014). Wehman, P. H., Schall, C. M., McDonough, J., Kregel,
Virtual reality job interview training in adults with J., Brooke, V., Molinelli, A., ... & Thiss, W. (2014).
autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Competitive employment for youth with autism
Developmental Disorders, 44(10), 2450–2463. spectrum disorders: Early results from a randomized
Smith, M. D., & Philippen, L. R. (1999). Community clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental
integration and supported employment. In D. B. Zager Disorders, 44(3), 487–500.
(Ed.), Autism: Identification, education, and treat- Wehman, P., Schall, C., McDonough, J., Molinelli, A.,
ment (2nd ed.pp. 301–321). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Riehle, E., Ham, W., & Thiss, W. R. (2012). Project
Erlbaum. SEARCH for youth with autism spectrum disor-
Sperry, L. A., & Mesibov, G. B. (2005). Perceptions of ders: Increasing competitive employment on transi-
social challenges of adults with autism spectrum dis- tion from high school. Journal of Positive Behavior
order. Autism, 9(4), 362–376. Interventions, 1–12.
Taylor, J. L., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N. A., Dove, D., Wehman, P., West, M., & Kregel, J. (1999). Supported
Veenstra-VanderWeele, J., & Warren, Z. (2012). A sys- employment program development and research
tematic review of vocational interventions for young needs: Looking ahead to the year 2000. Education and
adults with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental
130(3), 531–538. Disabilities, 3–19.
Taylor, J. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (2010). Changes in the Whitehead, C. W. (1979). Sheltered workshops in the
autism behavioral phenotype during the transition decade ahead: Better work and wages, or welfare.
to adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental Journal of Rehabilitation, 45(2), 77.
Disorders, 40(12), 1431–1446. Wing, L. (1996). Autistic spectrum disorders. British
Taylor, J. L., & Seltzer, M. M. (2011). Employment and Medical Journal, 312(7027), 327.
post-secondary educational activities for young adults Witte, J. C., & Kalleberg, A. L. (1995). Matching train-
with autism spectrum disorders during the transition ing and jobs: The fit between vocational education and
to adulthood. Journal of Autism and Developmental employment in the German labour market. European
Disorders, 41(5), 566–574. Sociological Review, 11(3), 293–317.
Tsang, M. C. (1997). The cost of vocational training.
International Journal of Manpower, 18(1/2), 63–89.
Tuma, N. B. (1983). Effects of labor market structure on
job-shift patterns. International Institute for Applied
Systems Analysis, 65, 83–89.
Supports for Postsecondary
Education 18
Lindsey W. Williams, Hillary H. Bush,
and Jennifer N. Shafer

not necessarily preclude pursuing higher educa-


Introduction tion. Many who wish to pursue higher education
are able to do so when provided supports to fit
In the past decade, college-based programs and their individual needs. Despite the plethora of
individualized supports for individuals with research on school-aged children with ASD and
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have afforded integration into general education classrooms,
many the opportunity to successfully attend and relatively little research has focused on factors
complete studies in higher education, and indi- needed for a successful pursuit of higher educa-
viduals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) are tion once these children transition into adulthood
opting to attend postsecondary education settings (Zager & Alpern, 2010). Fortunately, research
in increasing numbers. Individuals with ASD and program development in this area seem to be
who may not have been previously included in gaining traction. This chapter provides back-
postsecondary planning are now seeking the ground information about those with ASD enter-
opportunity to expand their independence and ing higher education, suggests strategies to
pursue a higher education (Gobbo & Shmulsky, incorporate into transition planning and assess-
2014). These rising rates may be attributed to an ment, provides overviews of the unique impact
increase in awareness of ASD, higher rates of of ASD-related characteristics in the postsecond-
diagnosis, and increased focus on planning for ary environment, and outlines legal concerns as
postsecondary outcomes during secondary edu- well as supports to maximize social and aca-
cation (Gobbo & Shmulsky, 2014). demic success.
While many individuals with a childhood
ASD diagnosis continue to need supports of
some type in adulthood, a need for supports does Background

Van Bergeijk, Klin, and Volkmar (2008) recently


predicted approximately 284,000–486,000 indi-
L.W. Williams, PhD, CRC (*) viduals under the age of 20 with ASD were plan-
J.N. Shafer, MS, CRC
University of North Carolina – Chapel Hill,
ning to begin postsecondary education in the
Chapel Hill, NC, USA United States. Statistics on the number of indi-
e-mail: lindseywilliswilliams@gmail.com viduals with ASD who actually attend higher
H.H. Bush, PhD education are scarce, but it is known that fewer
Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA, USA than 24% of adults with cognitive disabilities are

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 311


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_18
312 L.W. Williams et al.

employed (Butterworth, Migliore, Nord, & Gelb, with ASD reported they would “definitely” or
2012). Those who are employed are typically “probably” get some form of postsecondary edu-
underemployed and earn lower wages than non- cation; of these, 61.7% indicated high likelihood
disabled peers (Lysaght & Cobigo, 2014; Petner-­ of earning at least a 2-year degree, and 54.2%
Arrey, Howell-Moneta, & Lysaght, 2016). Even reported they would probably or definitely obtain
among typically developing individuals, on aver- a 4-year degree (Wagner, Newman, Cameto,
age unemployment is higher, and overall wages Levine, & Marder, 2007). However, a related
are lower in those without a college degree. It is study found only 22.9% of high school students
logical to assume that for individuals with ASD, with ASD had a goal in their transition plan
just as for their typically developing peers, post- directly related to postsecondary education
secondary education can provide greater employ- (Cameto, Levine, & Wagner, 2004). This is a dis-
ment opportunities depending on the individual’s couraging gap between the student’s expectations
motivation, skills, and interests. Furthermore, and formal transition plans. Caregivers can help
pursuing postsecondary education can be an minimize this gap by encouraging students to
important step in fostering independence and take an active role in their formal transition plan-
self-determination. ning over the years and fostering a sense of self-­
As Zager and Alpern (2010) point out, many efficacy in preparing for the future. Additionally,
individuals with ASD have intellectual disabili- this provides opportunity for the individual to
ties that preclude completion of college programs develop early self-advocacy skills that will be
without substantial modifications. These individ- necessary in the context of higher education.
uals often end up spending their first three or so
years of adulthood in the same high school set-
ting with younger students and/or students with  SD Characteristics in the Context
A
more severe disabilities. This is in conflict with of Higher Education
the Individuals with Disabilities Education
Improvement Act (2004), which states students In order to best engage in transition planning and
with disabilities should be included in educa- to identify supports that may be needed in the
tional settings with same-aged peers. Higher edu- postsecondary education environment, it is
cation programs facilitating continuing education important to have an understanding of how char-
in this more adult setting may be a solution to this acteristics of ASD can impact functioning in
problem, and whether the education culminates such a setting.
in a degree or not, postsecondary education or
training is a reasonable path to personal growth
and potential for individuals with disabilities Assets
(Mazzotti et al., 2009). It is also important to con-
sider that many individuals with ASD do not While individuals with high-functioning ASD
have intellectual disability but are unable to most will likely need a variety of supports in higher
effectively apply their cognitive potential in education, it is important to acknowledge the
higher education settings given all of the associ- strengths they bring as well. Unique experiences,
ated social and pragmatic demands these envi- interests, and perspectives can be a major asset to
ronments often entail. With the appropriate class discussion, and the diversity they bring to
supports, some adults with ASD can successfully campus can be of significant benefit to their fel-
pursue undergraduate and even graduate degrees low students, who can learn from their unique
(Tevrizian, 2015). outlooks and viewpoints. Individuals with ASD
The number of high school students with ASD can be very persistent and, particularly when
who plan to pursue higher education is notable. A studying in their areas of interest, able to focus on
national study of individuals receiving special one thing for lengthy periods of time. With clear
education services revealed 84.4% of individuals expectations, routine, and social and practical
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 313

support to meet their individual needs, individu- 2002) over obscure topics of interest, making
als with ASD can benefit greatly from habits of them stand out from peers. Difficulties with
persistence, focus, and attention to detail. speech and prosody may include inappropriate
Additionally, individuals with ASD tend to be volume and difficulty with using or understand-
very honest, straightforward, and loyal (Attwood, ing variations of verbal inflection. Socializing
2007). Indeed, they can greatly enrich their cam- with adolescents and adults generally requires a
pus community. thorough understanding of symbolic language,
including sarcasm and humor. In fact, adults with
ASD have reported that communication failures
Symptoms with coworkers often resulted from an inability to
understand double meanings, common idioms,
Symptoms of ASD including impairments in and nonverbal cues (Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004).
social skills, restricted and repetitive behaviors Difficulties with social understanding and
and interests, and difficulty with changes to rou- problems with engaging in reciprocal conversa-
tine can understandably interfere with success in tion can further impact communication with
not only academic but also in nonacademic but peers and negatively affect development of
essential areas of functioning. Even students with friendships. Language abilities are an integral
ASD who have adapted well to the greater inde- component in predicting friendship in adoles-
pendence of high school may have significant cents and adults (Howlin et al., 2000). In a study
difficulties transitioning between academic work of 235 adolescents and adults with ASD,
and other activities in the college environment, Orsmond, Krauss, and Seltzer (2004) found that
where day-to-day schedules are often are more only a small percentage had friendships meeting
variable and may be much more hectic than when the Autism Diagnostic Interview (Le Couteur,
they were in high school and where there may be Lord, & Rutter, 2003) friendship criteria (same-­
far fewer familiar faces. aged peers interacting in a reciprocal manner out-
side of organized settings).
Social Communication
In the often less-structured setting of higher edu-  heory of Mind
T
cation, difficulties with communication pragmat- As some abilities improve as an individual
ics can significantly impair success in and out of matures and progresses through different devel-
the classroom, as can the need for explicit instruc- opmental stages, the manifestation of core ASD
tions over sometimes vague or implied expecta- symptoms often changes as well (Shattuck et al.,
tions. For example, the vagueness of instruction 2007). Joint attention skills are necessary for con-
sometimes seen in college syllabi can present dif- versational skills in adolescence and adulthood;
ficulties. Farrell (2004) related an anecdote from furthermore, more advanced skills are required
a college student who stated they were at a loss as for initiating and maintaining conversation
to how to proceed with completing class assign- including understanding others’ interests, sensi-
ments due to the lack of deadlines on the syllabus tivity to social setting/context, and an under-
and vague instructions; the student reportedly standing of others’ previous knowledge (Zager &
never considered that they could ask the profes- Alpern, 2010). These skills require theory of
sor for assistance. mind.
Problems with social communication are a Theory of mind is the ability to recognize and
core feature of ASD, affecting abilities such as understand others’ thoughts, perceptions, and
joint attention and understanding abstract sym- beliefs as different from ones’ own. Individuals
bols or language (Zager & Alpern, 2010). Some with ASD have impaired theory of mind, impact-
studies of adolescents with ASD indicate extreme ing the ability to glean important information
talkativeness, sometimes with a “pedantic mono- from others, via their tone of voice, and nonver-
logue style” (Adams, Green, Gilchrist, & Cox, bal communication via posturing and facial
314 L.W. Williams et al.

expressions. Difficulty accurately anticipating or taining information in working memory (Attwood,


perceiving others’ reactions can lead to blunt 2007; Zelazo & Müller, 2010). Many individuals
honesty that can be inadvertently offensive, hurt- with ASD have difficulties with executive func-
ful, or embarrassing (Attwood, 2007). Problems tioning, affecting completion of multistep proj-
with theory of mind can also make it more diffi- ects as well as prompt return of assignments.
cult to accept different perspectives; conse- Difficulty abstracting the “big picture” from
quently, the individual may come across as details is another crucial component of success in
belligerent when faced with different points of most academic tasks, which can be challenging
view (Dillon, 2007). Problems with theory of for individuals with ASD. Executive functioning
mind can also affect some academic tasks, for problems also affect daily living skills, such as
example, analyzing characters in literature, dis- planning meals, remembering appointments, effi-
cussing feelings and emotions, or completing ciently managing completion of daily tasks
less-structured activities requiring imagination through to completion, and keeping up with
and creativity. needed items (e.g., keys). In sum, due to impair-
Adults with ASD may be unable to continu- ments in executive functioning skills, individuals
ously consider the listener’s point of view when with ASD pursuing higher education are likely to
engaged in conversation, leaving out important require not only academic support but social,
information required for the listener to fully communication, and independent living supports.
understand the background and context of the
topic at hand. Omissions can cause confusion and Comorbidities
communication breakdown (Colle et al., 2008). According to Siminoff et al. (2008), approxi-
When conversations become derailed, joint atten- mately 70% of adults with ASD have a comorbid
tion and theory of mind are necessary to recog- diagnosis, with the most common including
nize and address conversational breakdowns. An social anxiety, attention deficit/hyperactivity dis-
individual must also be able to discern when and order, obsessive-compulsive disorder, Tourette’s,
how to contribute to a conversation based on insomnia, and depression. It is critical to take any
social and cultural norms. Difficulty understand- comorbid conditions into account and ensure
ing nonverbal cues complicates such social inter- necessary supports are in place to address poten-
actions and can make it difficult to ascertain tially problematic symptoms that may arise.
when another person is interested, bored, Additionally, it may be necessarily to assist the
annoyed, or offended. Other studies show social individual in establishing routine care as neces-
difficulties in vocational and other adult settings sary upon arriving at a new environment if they
related to an inability to know when to ask ques- will be required to transition to new care provid-
tions, asking too many questions, or being too ers in a new location. Type and amount of support
blunt (Hurlbutt & Chalmers, 2004). In a study by needed for comorbidities varies widely and is
Müller and colleagues (2003), adults with ASD beyond the scope of this chapter, but in the past
were more often fired due to social and commu- several years, a significant amount of research
nication problems rather than poor job perfor- has been published on this topic. For an overview
mance. Accordingly, social and vocational of a variety of comorbidities in autism, the inter-
communication supports are considered critical ested reader may refer to Matson (2015) and
components of postsecondary programs for indi- Mazonne and Vitielo (2016).
viduals with ASD (Alpern & Zager, 2007).

Executive Function Other Considerations


Executive functioning skills are required for tasks
necessitating organization and planning, manag- The transition to postsecondary education can be a
ing time and prioritizing tasks, understanding major stressor for even the most well-adjusted indi-
abstract concepts and impulse control, and main- viduals. For those with ASD or other conditions,
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 315

the stressors of adapting to a new e­ nvironment with those who express this desire. A strong sense of
significantly greater independence and responsi- self-determination is also associated with greater
bilities may exacerbate symptoms and tax their pursuit of postsecondary education (Morningstar
capacity to cope with daily stressors. For those et al., 2010; Test, Mazzotti, Mustian, & Fowler,
with sensory sensitivities, transitioning to a com- 2009).
pletely novel environment can include unexpected The importance of a well-planned transition
challenges. from high school into higher education should
Sensory sensitivities to certain types of light- not be underestimated; success in postsecondary
ing, smells, noise, food textures, and the like can settings requires a great deal more than interest
be difficult to manage in new environments and intellect. Successful college students need to
where it may be impossible to anticipate where have the ability to develop and maintain social
and when these stimuli may be encountered. networks, attend to daily needs such as paying
These sensitivities can become especially prob- bills or tuition on time and attend to personal
lematic if the student, taken by surprise, lacks a needs, structure free time, and maintain flexibil-
plan to appropriately cope with or be excused ity to handle changes in class schedules and
from the situation when surprise encounters deadlines. Depending on the individual’s situa-
occur. Preemptively planning as much as possi- tion, they may need to be able to negotiate living
ble for when such occasions arise can be particu- with a roommate or housemates, learning new
larly helpful. For example, the individual can transportation routes, planning ahead to manage
make a habit of identifying the nearest restroom completion of large projects, and planning and
or convenient quiet, semiprivate area when preparing meals.
engaging in a new environment. They may con- In making plans for after high school gradua-
sider always carrying a snack in case they attend tion, Hurlbutt and Handler (2010) outlined the
functions where none of the foods are appetizing. need for practical experience (e.g., skills neces-
Caregivers familiar with the student’s individual sary for independent navigation and problem
sensitivities can assist the student in planning solving in daily living in the post-high school
ahead as much as possible to prepare for such setting) throughout the education of students
eventualities. with Asperger’s disorder. The authors suggested
a carefully considered, multiyear process to
assist students as they transition out of high
Planning Ahead school and into college or the workplace. The
plan should be guided by the teacher, parents,
Transition Plans and other involved caregivers, but it should also
be heavily driven by the individual’s goals and
Under the Individuals with Disabilities Education foster a sense of self-­determination. This pro-
Act, transition plans are required to specify the cess will necessitate activities that promote
transition services necessary to help students exploration of options and preferences, infor-
attain their goals (IDEA Partnership, 2004). mation gathering, and research about the desired
Studies of students with ASD have indicated par- life beyond high school. In Hurlbutt and
ticipation in their own transition planning during Handler’s example, this process resulted in cre-
the high school years is associated with greater ation of a career path binder functioning as a
likelihood of postsecondary education for indi- portfolio to house transition plans. The transi-
viduals with ASD (Chiang, Cheung, Hickson, tion from secondary school can be an uncertain
Xiang, & Tsai, 2012). Transition planning which experience for individuals with ASD, as man-
directly includes input from the student provides dated state and federal supports are no longer
an opportunity to represent their interests so that required upon high school graduation (Weigand,
they will be reflected in transition planning, thus 2011), further highlighting the need for a detailed,
improving secondary education outcomes for well-formulated transition plan.
316 L.W. Williams et al.

Whether and Where to Attend readiness must be taken into consideration in


making the choice whether to take any online
In individuals who have been diagnosed with classes and, if so, how many.
Asperger’s disorder (or have the same constella- An advantage of technical or vocational pro-
tion of symptoms under the DSM-5’s collapsed grams is that they often require fewer general
ASD diagnosis), obsessive, intense interests in education requirements, offering the opportunity
areas of academic study are common, and indi- to concentrate on areas of interest. This may be
viduals may be especially interested in technical particularly advantageous for individuals who
vocations (e.g., computer engineering, chemis- excel in some areas but have significant difficulty
try) that necessitate a degree (Baron-Cohen, in other areas. Benefits of beginning at a local
Wheelwright, Burtenshaw, & Hobson, 2007). community college include smaller campuses
This can make higher education an attractive plan that can be easier to navigate and, for many indi-
for after high school graduation (Baron-Cohen viduals, having the option to continue living at
et al., 2007). However, it may also be possible to home to ease the transition from high school.
find other suitable avenues for pursuing the indi- Community colleges are often able to provide
vidual’s goals. The question of whether to pursue more individual attention per student and often
postsecondary education is an important one; for have much smaller classes (Adreon & Durocher,
some individuals, vocational schools, apprentice- 2007). For individuals who ultimately wish to
ships, volunteer opportunities, or other avenues transfer to a 4-year institution, however, attend-
may be sufficient alternatives for meeting the ing community college will necessitate another
individual’s career goals. For others, taking time major transition. Larger institutions offer unique
away from academics to explore potential voca- advantages as well. Larger colleges and universi-
tional interests or taking a slower path to higher ties generally have greater diversity in their stu-
education by taking a class or two at a local com- dent body, offering a greater opportunity to meet
munity college may be of great benefit. Careful other individuals with similar interests. Larger
attention should be given to identifying the stu- institutions may also offer greater diversity of
dent’s intrinsic desire for this path independent of courses or majors as well as more well-resourced
parental expectations, as well as overall readi- career counseling and educational support depart-
ness, career goals, career requirements, and ments, although this is certainly not always the
appropriateness of fit of the desired career to the case and many excellent programs in smaller
individual’s strengths and abilities. schools exist. Additionally, some schools have
Many possibilities exist for higher education, programs that are specifically for individuals
including community colleges, vocational/tech- with developmental disabilities such as ASD.
nical schools, and traditional 4-year colleges and
universities. An additional factor to consider is
whether the institution offers online classes; Postsecondary Programs
many institutions offer courses that are partially Specifically for Students
or entirely online. Some online classes offer the with Intellectual Disabilities
option to complete the work at ones’ own pace and/or ASD
rather than within the strict timelines of one
semester. Other advantages can include avoiding In 2010, the US Department of Education initiated
problems with transportation and avoiding the a 5-year funding program to 27 higher education
anxiety of participating in a large classroom set- institutions in order to establish Transition
ting; on the other hand, learning to navigate Programs for Students with Intellectual Disabilities
transportation and participate in the social setting (TPSID). These federally funded programs pro-
of the classroom can be important learning vide not only educational opportunities for transi-
opportunities and key factors in increasing inde- tion-age youth with cognitive and intellectual
pendence. The individual’s personal goals and disabilities (ID) but also important opportunities
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 317

to engage in the social, occupational, and indepen- career development, peer mentorship, occupa-
dent living environments associated with tradi- tional therapy, physical therapy, and 24-h on-call
tional postsecondary education. With the support (Carlotti, 2014). While some of these
establishment of TPSID programs, postsecondary programs are offered free of charge to students
options became available to many students for with ASD, others charge fees that range from a
whom postsecondary education was not previ- few hundred dollars to upward of $8000 per aca-
ously an option or consideration. It is important to demic year. Like TPSID programs, peer-reviewed
note that TPSID programs are specifically for stu- research on these ASD-specific programs is cur-
dents with ID; however, considering that about rently limited, although program evaluation is
50% of individuals with ASD have a concurrent ongoing.
ID (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, Ultimately, deciding which college to attend is
2014), this is highly relevant for the ASD student a personal choice, and all students, regardless of
population. In fact, about one-­quarter (26%) of the disability status, often consider many factors dur-
883 students enrolled in TPSID programs during ing this process. However, students with ASD
the 2013–2014 academic year had ASD. While may have unique considerations, and may weigh
TPSID programs have a great deal of flexibility in different factors, or have different priorities,
the types of programming they offer, they gener- when choosing which postsecondary school to
ally include some or all of the following compo- attend. While TPSID programs and ASD pro-
nents, in addition to legally mandated academic grams recognize the needs of a growing student
accommodations: certificate and degree programs, population and offer innovative supports to stu-
“academically specialized courses” specifically dents with ASD, with and without concurrent ID,
for students with ID in addition to integrated col- they are not the right choice for all students with
lege classes, peer mentors and/or educational ASD. Students with ASD, like their neurotypical
coaches, regular check-ins regarding student prog- peers, have many options when it comes to their
ress across different domains, communication and postsecondary education.
coordination with families, employment intern-
ships, and specialized training for educators
(Grigal et al., 2015). Ongoing program evaluation Skill Building
of TPSID programs (883 students across 50 sites,
as of the 2013–2014 academic year) is being con- Caregivers and other support professionals can
ducted by the Think College National Coordinating help individuals with ASD develop and hone the
Center at the Institute for Community Inclusion at skills needed for transitioning to postsecondary
the University of Massachusetts Boston. education before the student leaves high school.
In addition to TPSID programs, there are a Freedman (2010) identified several categories of
number of non-TPSID programs in existence necessary skill sets to prepare an individual with
specifically for students with ASD. Many of ASD for success in higher education. For exam-
these programs are geared toward the needs of ple, Freedman identified many components of
students with ASD without concurrent ID. Like asking for help that caregivers should ideally fos-
TPSID programs, there is a great deal of diversity ter beginning during early childhood, including
in the programming and level of support offered understanding the role of various people in one’s
by programs for students with ASD. In addition life, understanding and developing a sense of
to legally mandated academic supports, these social reciprocity, knowing when help is needed,
programs may provide ASD support groups, spe- identifying whom to ask for help, and knowing
cialized classes and programming to increase life how and when to appropriately ask for help.
skills and adaptive behaviors, on-campus and Adreon and Durocher (2007) also suggested a
off-­campus social opportunities, special housing number of areas critical to preparing an ­individual
options, group or individual supports around def- with ASD to transition to higher education. These
icits in executive functioning and social skills, issues include:
318 L.W. Williams et al.

• Identifying the size and type of institution to Educational Plan (IEP) development. Caregivers
attend should be aware that a school diagnosis, while
• Deciding where to live very helpful and necessary for public school ser-
• Assessing and teaching necessary indepen- vices, is different than a formal medical diagnosis.
dent living skills Students who have received a diagnosis through
• Discussing when and how to disclose diagnosis the school but have not received a medical diagno-
• Learning how to identify needed academic sis of ASD should consider pursuing additional
supports and accommodations assessment and diagnosis before attending post-
• Identifying social supports secondary school, as a medical diagnosis is gener-
• Identifying strategies to assist in adjusting to ally needed to receive accommodation services.
the college environment (Adreon & Durocher, Because postsecondary schools are not required to
2007, p. 275) screen for the presence of any disabilities or condi-
tions that could interfere with learning, it is
Along the same lines, Autism Speaks (2016) unlikely that students without a previously estab-
released an employment guide for individuals with lished, formal diagnosis of ASD will be diagnosed
ASD that can also be utilized for those seeking post- and offered supports by their postsecondary
secondary education. The guide promotes the schools (Pinder-Amaker, 2014). Additionally,
development of “soft skills” that are critical in the after high school, there are far fewer laws that pro-
postsecondary environment. For example, caregiv- tect educational rights. Accordingly, students will
ers are encouraged to work on active listening and likely need to advocate for themselves to receive
effective, appropriate communication. In the post- specific accommodations (Freedman, 2010).
secondary environment, these skills become espe- Postsecondary schools generally require docu-
cially important because they often influence social mentation of a disability (e.g., neuropsychological
outcomes, such as how a professor perceives an assessment report, a letter from a licensed mental
email or how a student with ASD needs to ask for health professional) before agreeing to provide
help if they do not understand a topic in class. adjustments and accommodations. It is important
Suggestions for skill building include having the to note that postsecondary schools vary a great
person with ASD participate in role-playing or deal in their policies and procedures around pro-
modeling with the caregiver prior to encountering viding disability services. Also, it is important to
these situations. Learning how to work through keep in mind the heterogeneity and diversity
conflict or a situation in which the person with ASD among young people with ASD. A letter stating
might feel peer pressure is vital. Another suggestion that a student has an ASD diagnosis may not lead
for working through these common situations is uti- to the provision of appropriate accommodations
lizing Davidson and Henderson’s (2010) suggested and supports in a postsecondary setting. Instead, a
worksheet. Worksheets or other visual aids can be more detailed explanation of a student’s strengths
especially helpful as many individuals with ASD and weaknesses – as well as the presence of any
are visual learners. The individual could then work other conditions, psychological or otherwise,
through a step-by-step guideline for navigating var- which may negatively impact academic adjust-
ious situations and the appropriate response with ment (e.g., Cai & Richdale, 2016; Gelbar, Smith,
the best chance for leading to the desired outcome. & Reichow, 2014) – may be required in order to
deliver the accommodations that will best aid the
student.
Psychoeducational and  Vocational Psychoeducational and/or neuropsychological
Evaluation evaluation can also help the individual make
informed decisions in planning for the postsec-
Many children with ASD are assessed via the pub- ondary years. The type of testing would depend
lic school system, which provides a school-­based on the complexity of the individual’s needs;
diagnosis of ASD to inform Individualized ­psychoeducational testing tends to focus primar-
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 319

ily on areas affecting academic performance and assessing their own interests and skills may be a
is not quite as comprehensive as neuropsycho- key in selecting the appropriate college or trade
logical evaluations, though they often do include school. For example, a larger university may
some measures of emotional and adaptive func- offer the opportunity for a person with very nar-
tioning as well. A neuropsychological exam is row interests to pursue a specific major or trade,
generally more lengthy, comprehensive (includ- once that interest has been identified (Hurewitz &
ing more detail about executive function and Berger, 2008). Stephen Shore, an author with
cognitive organization/learning), and expensive. ASD who has written extensively about the sub-
As in all treatment planning, choosing which ject, illustrated the importance of carefully select-
assessments to pursue is highly individualized ing a major or career path for the individual with
based on the individual’s needs. ASD during an interview with Brownell and
The report derived from an evaluation will pro- Walther-Thomas (2001).
vide evidence of areas of weakness warranting In the interview, Shore described enjoying par-
accommodations and thus is likely to be neces- ticipating in music and opting to major in music
sary to receive formal academic accommodations. education during college. However, Shore began
While specific accommodations are needed, to reconsider his major after becoming aware that
another vital piece for individuals with ASD seek- the job prospects for a music major were relatively
ing a postsecondary education is understanding slim. He therefore opted to increase his chances
their strengths and weaknesses via targeted for success postgraduation by double majoring in
assessment. Hume, Boyd, Hamm, and Kucharcyzk accounting (Brownell & Walther-Thomas, 2001).
(2014) reported that in one study, 96% of indi- Shore’s experience illustrates the critical impor-
viduals with ASD scored more than two standard tance for an individual with ASD not only to seek
deviations below the mean on the Scales of postsecondary education in a field that is interest-
Independent Behavior (Bruininks, Woodcock, ing but also to become knowledgeable regarding
Weatherman, & Hill, 1996). This scale, which can how that field can impact their prospects after
be administered via a structured interview or a graduation. A key component of developing such
checklist, aims to assess a person’s adaptive and awareness is to utilize and promote self-advocacy
maladaptive behaviors in certain areas of func- within the ASD community.
tioning (Bruininks et al., 1996). Establishing a The Virginia Commonwealth University’s
baseline level of independent functioning and Rehabilitation Research and Training Center and
assessing which key areas necessitate support Autism Speaks (2016) recently collaborated to
during postsecondary education may be critical to develop a community-based skills assessment to
success. Another assessment that may be useful in measure a person with ASD’s social and personal
determining the appropriate level of support functioning. The assessment helps to gauge a per-
needed is the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale son’s level of competence in domains such as
(Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, 2005). This assess- self-awareness and advocacy, as well as career
ment is administered via a semi-structured inter- path and employment. The TEACCH Transition
view with the person’s caregiver or parents and Assessment Profile-Second Edition (TTAP;
measures independence in both the personal and Mesibov, Thomas, Chapman, & Schopler, 2007)
social domains (Klin et al., 2007). The scale is another measure designed to support a person’s
assesses various domains, such as daily living educational planning by assessing functional
skills, communication, and socialization – com- skills areas such as vocational skills and behavior
ponents all critical to a person with ASD’s func- and independent functioning, among other
tioning in the postsecondary environment. domains. The TTAP measures these domains
Presumably, assessing a person’s level of through direct observation as well as a home and
executive functioning and independence is the school/work scale. Results can be used by disabil-
first step. However, for those individuals with ity services, employment counselors, and other
ASD seeking a postsecondary level of education, caregivers involved in intervention planning.
320 L.W. Williams et al.

Vocational evaluation can be especially help- Postsecondary Supports


ful, particularly if the individual expresses uncer-
tainty about what they wish to do after graduating Many institutions of higher learning are making
high school. Many schools have access to such efforts to meet the needs of individuals with ASD
measures. Additionally, state vocational rehabili- by providing additional support programs. In
tation centers can provide such services to eligi- recent years, some postsecondary education pro-
ble individuals. Students with an IEP often grams have begun to focus on “inclusive postsec-
qualify for some transitional services through the ondary education” to include individuals with
state’s Department of Vocational Rehabilitation. disabilities, particularly intellectual disabilities,
Not only is vocational evaluation helpful for in the normative postsecondary track as much as
those undecided about which direction to pursue, feasible (Uditsky & Hughson, 2012). Two hun-
but it can also be especially helpful when an indi- dred or more programs across the country cur-
vidual’s desired career is a poor match for their rently have programs specifically to provide more
abilities. It is often the case that parents recog- proactive support to individuals with disabilities
nize the mismatch between the demands of a in participating in academic programs, develop-
desired profession and the actual abilities of their ing a career plan, and engaging in campus life
children with ASD; the results of career testing (Blalock, 2014; Grigal & Hart, 2010).
can be an objective springboard for having this
conversation with their children. Individuals with
ASD often have significant difficulty identifying Legal Considerations
their own weaknesses, particularly when it comes
to social communication and interpersonal skills. The Vocational Rehabilitation Act of 1973 and the
They may also have little insight into the actual Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) of 1990
requirements of a given vocation. Working with a improved access for individuals with disabilities
counselor who is knowledgeable about ASD can who wished to pursue higher education (Americans
be invaluable in helping an individual to process with Disabilities Act, 1990; Rehabilitation Act,
feelings of disappointment and identify more 1973); however, the highly variable needs of indi-
viable vocational options, whether or not that viduals with ASD can make it difficult for faculty
includes postsecondary education. and staff to understand how to best meet the needs
Individuals with ASD wishing to gauge their of students with ASD (Farrell, 2004). The most fre-
interest and skills before pursuing postsecondary quently provided academic accommodations (e.g.,
education are not limited to formal assessments. extra time to take exams, alternate testing loca-
Myriad assessments are now available online. tions, tutoring, and assistance from notetakers)
Though these assessments have not been normed may not meet the needs of the individual with
or standardized to the ASD population, they can ASD, who may need more social supports in order
be an efficient way of determining a person’s to succeed a­ cademically. Additionally, the legisla-
skills, interests, and values. For example, tion does not define what qualifies as “reasonable
CareerOneStop is a website supported by the US accommodations,” thus leaving the degree of
Department of Labor and offers a free online responsibility incumbent on institutions open to
skills assessment to measure both technical (e.g., interpretation (Hughes, 2009). The types of sup-
writing computer code, operating equipment) ports identified as often required by individuals
and soft skills (e.g., critical thinking, communi- with ASD in higher education settings are not those
cation). Individuals can develop a skills profile that are typically provided: supports for living on
and then match their skills to various career fields campus or living independently, developing peer
in O*NET, a national database. The assessment is relationships, engaging in self-advocacy, and inter-
free and easily accessible to individuals who are acting effectively with instructors (Dillon, 2007;
comfortable browsing the Internet. Ellison et al., 2013; Hughes, 2009; Smith, 2007).
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 321

Disclosure enter the college environment (Pinder-Amaker,


2014), thus presenting another barrier to accessing
While adolescence is typically a time of increasing supports.
independence, individuals with ASD may find the Some aspects of disclosing an “invisible” dis-
transition to independence difficult and may even ability, like ASD, are uniquely challenging. Pinder-
find their independence decreasing without appro- Amaker (2014) made important connections
priate support (Hume et al., 2014). For example, between the relatively well-established literature
some individuals may lack the ability to success- on college students with psychiatric conditions and
fully problem-solve independently when bus routes disabilities and the lesser-­established literature on
change or when they need to make unexpected college students with ASD. Particularly, students
changes to plans. Postsecondary students with with these types of disabilities will avoid or at least
ASD face other challenges related to their disorder, not seek out resources if they believe that accom-
including critical thinking and difficulties in long- modations will not help them or if they perceive
term planning and organization. Further, group stigma, judgment, or lack of understanding from
work sometimes required in these settings may the school staff and faculty who are supposed to
present a unique challenge for a disorder character- help them (e.g., Eisenberg, Hunt, Speer, & Zivin,
ized by social and emotional obstacles (Gabbo & 2011). Disclosure may feel especially disconcert-
Shmulsky, 2014). Yet, Gabbo and Shmulsky (2014) ing for some individuals who may “pass” as neuro-
also state that people with ASD may have some typical to the casual observer but who may still
advantages when it comes to postsecondary educa- benefit from accommodations (Davidson &
tion, such as adherence to the rules, desire to learn Henderson, 2010). These individuals may have
and be correct, and a passion for their chosen field learned how to suppress certain behaviors typical
or trade. In order to fully take advantage of these of ASD as well as how to increasingly engage in
benefits through appropriate supports, individuals other more neurotypical behaviors such as making
will need to advocate for themselves and consider appropriate eye contact and appropriately express-
disclosing their diagnosis. ing emotions. Therefore, Davidson and Henderson
Once enrolled in a postsecondary setting, indi- (2010) liken the decision to disclose the ASD diag-
viduals with ASD should consider whether they nosis to “coming out.” The lack of understanding
wish to disclose their diagnosis and, if so, how and of social cues typical to ASD can complicate the
when to disclose. Students with ASD, or any other choice of how and to whom to disclose ones’ diag-
disability, are not required to disclose the nature of nosis. Learning how to judge and read these cues
their disability, or that they have a disability at all, may make the choice to disclose somewhat easier,
to educators or administrators at their postsecond- as the individual could feel safer disclosing
ary school. However, if they wish to receive aca- to someone who they can trust (Davidson &
demic adjustments and accommodations, then Henderson, 2010). One suggestion for navigating
they must do so by providing the necessary docu- this path is creating and utilizing a worksheet out-
mentation (U.S. Department of Education, 2011). lining the disclosure process. Individuals with ASD
Choosing to disclose to school officials can be dif- could work with their caregiver or other support
ficult. Some students with disabilities experience professional to develop such a worksheet before
stigma, embarrassment, or other negative feelings they arrive at their chosen postsecondary institu-
around their disability status, and this can become tion. Davidson and Henderson (2010) suggest that
a significant barrier to communicating with dis- the worksheet detail the pros and cons of disclosing
ability services offices and successfully advocat- to a chosen person. For example, if the individual
ing for important supports (Barber, 2012). Further, with ASD is considering disclosing to a peer, they
some students who received special education should consider whether the peer is trustworthy
through high school erroneously may doubt their and whether the peer typically have others’ best
need for accommodations and supports once they interests at heart (Davidson & Henderson, 2010).
322 L.W. Williams et al.

Disclosing to a member of the institution may or privately funded school that receives federal
be the preferred choice, at least initially, in order to funding. While the Family Educational Rights
access certain supports that may prove key to suc- and Privacy Act of 1974 does not directly provide
cess in the postsecondary environment. Gelbar, any supports or accommodations to students with
Shefcyk, and Reichow (2015) conducted a com- disabilities, it does grant all students the right to
prehensive survey of current and former students access and amend their educational records, as
with ASD to assess if and when they chose to dis- well as protection against their educational
close, among other postsecondary experiences. In records (including information about disability
the study, 69% of participants chose to disclose status, accommodations, and communications)
their disability, primarily to disability service coor- being released to third parties without explicit,
dinators and professors – fewer individuals chose written consent (Family Educational Rights and
to disclose to their fellow peers (Gelbar et al., Privacy Act, 1974). The Individuals of Disabilities
2015). The participants who disclosed to members Education Act (IDEA) of 2004 applies to school
of the institution also utilized accommodations, districts (i.e., K-12 schools), but it does not apply
such as extended time for assignments and tests, to postsecondary schools (Individuals with
modified testing environment, social skills courses, Disabilities Education Improvement Act, 2004).
and peer mentors (Gelbar et al., 2015). These par- Thus, postsecondary schools are not required to
ticipants chose to disclose within their first semes- provide free public appropriate education
ters in order to access supports. Clearly, when (FAPE), identify students’ needs through assess-
appropriate support is utilized, students with ASD ments, or provide an Individualized Education
can address the challenges of the disorder and may Plan (IEP).
have a better chance for success. Section 504 and Title II apply both to school
districts and postsecondary schools but in different
ways. According to the Office for Civil Rights of
 cademic Supports for Students
A the US Department of Education (2011), postsec-
with ASD in Postsecondary Schools ondary schools are required to provide academic
adjustments and accommodations, such that stu-
 egally Required Academic Supports
L dents are not discriminated against due to having a
Young adults with ASD are at high risk for sub- disability. These adjustments and accommoda-
optimal educational and professional outcomes: tions depend greatly on the nature of a student’s
Shattuck et al. (2012) found that more than half disability but may include one or more of the fol-
of young people with ASD in the United States lowing: extended time to complete tests and
neither attend college nor obtain competitive assignments, use of different testing locations,
employment during their first 2 years following access to instructors’ slides and outlines before
high school. Thus, it is important to understand class sessions, note-taking services, a reduced or
the key pieces of legislation that affect the post- alternate course load, and access to adaptive tech-
secondary education of students with ASD and nology (Gelbar, Smith, & Reichow, 2014). Also, if
how these aim to improve educational outcomes. a postsecondary school provides student housing,
Per the mandates of Section 504 of the then comparable, convenient, and accessible
Rehabilitation Act of 1973, and Titles II and III options must be made available to students with
of the Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA) of physical disabilities. However, there are some
1990, students with disabilities, including ASD, adjustments and accommodations that postsec-
are entitled to educational supports in postsec- ondary schools are not required to provide. For
ondary schools. This is true regardless of whether instance, they do not have to “lower or substan-
students attend public or private institutions: tially modify essential requirements” of a given
Title II applies to publically funded colleges and course or degree program; they also do not need to
universities, community colleges, and vocational provide services that would entail “undue financial
schools; Title III applies to privately funded or administrative burden” (U.S. Department of
schools; and Section 504 applies to any publicly Education, 2011).
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 323

 onlegally Required Academic


N dents with ASD that students themselves
Supports described as being beneficial, they carefully
As previously discussed, postsecondary schools noted the lack of peer-reviewed experimental
are legally required to provide academic supports research and program evaluation for many of
and housing options (if a school offers any on-­ these supports. Indeed, current recommendations
campus housing) to students with disabilities; for supporting the needs of college students with
there are also some types of accommodations ASD are more based on the clinical impressions
that they are not legally required to provide. and experiences of education professionals, and
However, there are many postsecondary schools research on college students with disabilities
that pride themselves on the range of services and other than ASD (e.g., learning disabilities), than
supports they provide to students with ASD, on empirical research with college students with
which go above and beyond what is legally man- ASD (Pinder-Amaker, 2014). However, as more
dated. In their systematic review of 20 studies in and more young people with ASD enter postsec-
which college students with ASD self-reported ondary education and more programs are imple-
on their educational supports and utilization of mented and evaluated, the research base is very
services, Gelbar and colleagues (2014) found that likely to grow.
academic accommodations, consistent with those
required by law, were discussed in the majority Self-Advocacy
(60%) of the studies. However, a significant The nature of the current laws surrounding dis-
minority (45%) of the studies touched upon addi- ability accommodations in postsecondary set-
tional, nonlegally required services to support tings all but require students with disabilities,
students with ASD. These included peer mentor- including ASD, to assume a prominent self-­
ship programs, in which students with ASD are advocacy role. This represents a significant shift
paired with volunteer neurotypical students; from secondary school, where a great deal of
being assigned to a counselor, aide, or liaison for responsibility falls both on parents and the school
advocacy supports and help navigating the post- district to make sure that students with ASD are
secondary environment; parental involvement; being appropriately educated and supported.
the use of video modeling and Social Stories Pinder-Amaker (2014) suggested that
(therapeutic stories that can be used to help indi- postsecondary students would benefit greatly
­
viduals with ASD navigate familiar and unfamil- from a theoretical “Individualized College Plan,”
iar situations); the provision of disability teams; in which the complex interactions between the
student support groups; and cognitive-behavioral student, family, and school that currently occur in
interventions provided through campus counsel- the context of an IEP could continue into the col-
ing centers. Indeed, in a qualitative study con- lege setting. In the absence of such a service,
ducted by Barber (2012) on graduates with a however, Pinder-Amaker (2014) made the impor-
broad range of disabilities from five New Jersey tant point that students with ASD would benefit
colleges and universities, the participants identi- from gaining self-advocacy experience, while
fied both legally mandated and nonlegally man- they are still in high school. Ultimately, learning
dated supports as being critical to their success. how to be a strong self-advocate takes self-­
These included having close, supportive relation- knowledge, time, and practice. In many cases,
ships with one or more disability services staff this includes having a strong understanding of the
members, as well as with faculty members. Also, supports they need to be successful and commu-
participants noted that while learning about dis- nicating these to school administrators and pro-
ability services and available supports was a viding any documentation requested by their
cumbersome process, actually accessing those postsecondary school. Also, if students find that
services was not. they are not receiving the accommodations that
While Gelbar and colleagues (2014) discov- they were promised, or if those accommodations
ered a range of interventions and supports for stu- are not having their intended impact, then they
324 L.W. Williams et al.

must communicate with school administrators to all the more challenging for young people with
address the issue (U.S. Department of Education, ASD. In the following section, existing supports
2011). Fortunately, in a recent qualitative study to address the social relationships, sexuality, and
among Australian college students with ASD and professional development of college students
their parents, the vast majority of students with ASD are explored.
(90.9%) reported feeling as though they could
talk to a university staff member if they believed  upports Around Social Relationships
S
that their needs were not being met (Cai & College students with ASD are likely to benefit
Richdale, 2016). Regarding the inclusion of par- from supports to increase their social communi-
ents, Wolf, Brown, and Bork (2009) strongly sug- cation skills and to foster relationships with other
gest that with the student’s permission, parents or students, roommates, and faculty and staff. These
primary caregivers remain involved with the stu- may include, but are not limited to, social skills
dent’s on-campus support system. They note that groups, support groups for students with ASD,
for individuals with other disabilities (e.g., physi- participation in student groups not specifically
cal, medical, sensory, learning, psychiatric dis- related to disability (e.g., orchestra, anime club),
abilities), encouraging independence often means on-campus and off-campus social outings for stu-
less contact with parents. However, for some dents with ASD, individual therapy with counsel-
individuals with ASD, some parental involve- ing center providers, and participation in peer
ment can be essential. The authors suggest iden- mentorship programs. In their review of a
tifying a point person to communicate periodically Campus-Based Inclusion Model (CBIM) for high
with parents, particularly if health or safety issues school and college students with ASD, Zager and
arise. A contract outlining discussed and agreed-­ Alpern (2010) stressed the particular importance
upon boundaries of contact can help establish a of supporting students in the area of social com-
professional relationship, reassuring parents munication, which has important implications for
while also fostering emerging independence. social relationships. In particular, they described
students participating in weekly therapy sessions
led by speech-language pathologists, using
 ocial Supports for Postsecondary
S ­standardized measures to assess students’ com-
Students with ASD munication skills, naturalistic observation of stu-
dents to determine their communication skill
While many of the legally mandated and nonle- needs, and measuring students’ self-perceptions
gally mandated supports for college students of their communication skills and abilities.
with ASD focus somewhat narrowly on achiev- Overall, interventions that are focused on the
ing academic success, college is not exclusively domain of social communication, highly individ-
an academic experience. Instead, postsecondary ualized to students’ needs, and implemented
education frequently includes multiple, meaning- before students attend postsecondary school full
ful social relationships, including friendships, time are likely to help students with ASD have
intimate relationships, and preparation for the more successful social relationships during their
professional world (Pinder-Amaker, 2014). college years.
However, the core features of ASD – deficits in
social communication and restricted and repeti-  upports Around Romantic
S
tive behaviors and interests – alongside fre- Relationships and Sexuality
quently co-occurring conditions, including Compared to their neurotypical peers, college-­
deficits in executive functioning and attention age students with ASD overall have less sexual
(e.g., Rosenthal et al., 2013), and internalizing knowledge, fewer sources of sexual education,
symptoms including depression, anxiety, and and less sexual experience (Brown-Lavoie,
obsessive-compulsive disorder (e.g., Lai et al., Viecili, & Weiss, 2014; Mehzabin & Stokes,
2011), make success in any one of these domains 2011). Individuals with ASD also have much
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 325

greater difficulty interpreting the social cues of ing center and/or disability services staff, peer
others to ascertain others’ emotional reactions or mentorship programs, career assessment (while
intent. These disparities may have particular keeping in mind that most measures of career
implications in the college setting, where many aptitude were not designed for use with individu-
students engage in romantic and sexual relation- als with ASD), career preparation supports (e.g.,
ships. Emerging research suggests that adoles- résumé and cover letter advice, mock interviews),
cents and adults with ASD are at increased risk and ultimately taking a strengths-based approach
both for showing inappropriate dating behaviors, to helping students with ASD determine their
such as stalking or being inappropriately asser- career goals (Grandin & Duffy, 2008). These sup-
tive/persistent (Stokes, Newton, & Kaur, 2007), ports will assist students with ASD, so that they
and for being victims of sexual abuse (Brown-­ may pursue competitive and fulfilling careers
Lavoie et al., 2014). Very little research currently after graduation.
exists on the implementation of sexual education
programs and other interventions to support the Attitudes Toward  Postsecondary
healthy sexualities of college students with ASD, Students with ASD
although several research-informed curricula do Many of the interventions and supports described
exist (e.g., Davies & Dubie, 2012; Henault, in the current chapter target individuals with ASD
2005). and help them adapt to postsecondary environ-
ments and their expectations. However, research-
 upports Around Professional
S ers are starting to consider ways in which the
Development responsibility could be shifted from the individual
Professional development and career preparation to the system in order to provide better experi-
are increasingly being recognized as an area ences and outcomes for college students with
where many postsecondary students with ASD ASD. An important first step in designing this
need particular support. Despite increased pro- type of intervention is to understand the attitudes
gramming for students with disabilities more of postsecondary schools and their i­nhabitants
broadly, college graduates with disabilities appear toward students with ASD. For example, Nevill
to be disproportionately unemployed compared to and White (2011) found that neurotypical stu-
their nondisabled peers (U.S. Department of dents who had a first-degree relative with ASD
Labor, 2015), although some studies do suggest were significantly more open toward students
fewer disparities and more optimistic outcomes with ASD symptomatology (presented through
(e.g., Fichten et al., 2012). Similarly, a quarter of hypothetical vignettes), compared to neurotypical
students who exited TPSID programs at the end students without such a relative. Further, no dif-
of the 2013–2014 academic year did not have ferences in openness toward students with ASD
paid employment or participate in any other were found between neurotypical male and
career development (Grigal et al., 2015). female students or between students studying dif-
Recognizing the significant social skills (e.g., ferent majors (categorized as social sciences,
interacting with coworkers and supervisors, busi- physical sciences, engineering, and other sub-
ness etiquette) and executive function skills (e.g., jects). In another recent study, neurotypical col-
time management, decision making) that are often lege students rated written descriptions of students
needed to be successful in the professional world, with ASD symptoms and a formal ASD diagnosis
Dipeolu, Storlie, and Johnson (2015) provided a more positively than written descriptions of stu-
series of recommendations as to how postsecond- dents with ASD symptoms but without a diagnos-
ary schools can better prepare students with tic label (Matthews, Ly, & Goldberg, 2015). In
ASD. These included initiating professional train- this study, however, male students were found to
ing and development early in one’s college career express more positive attitudes, across conditions,
(e.g., internships), group counseling and social than female students. Together, these studies
skills training, individual sessions with counsel- show that neurotypical students have a strong
326 L.W. Williams et al.

capacity to understand and accept peers with ASD independence, a presumption of competence,
and that there may be malleable factors that can understanding, and communication (Robledo &
be targeted via intervention in order to increase Donnellan, 2008). Both students and service pro-
this acceptance. Studies like these provide an viders identified family member involvement as
important framework for future interventions that appreciated in Schlabach’s (2008) study, with stu-
may change the environment, instead of the indi- dents often relying on family for emotional sup-
vidual, to help students with ASD thrive in post- port as well as practical assistance as students
secondary settings. Indeed, the presence of lived with or near their family. Family members
diverse students, including those with ASD, played an integral role in assisting with choosing
increases awareness and reduces stigma, thus an institution, campus visits, time management,
benefitting the entire college community (Pinder- problematic social situations, and serving as
Amaker, 2014). advocates when needed (Schlabach, 2008). Of
course, not every student will welcome the same
degree of familial involvement, and family mem-
Family Involvement bers should respect these differences.

In a survey of studies looking at family involve-


ment and postsecondary outcomes for students Conclusion
with ASD, Dallas, Ramisch, and McGowan
(2015) found few studies looking at this topic. In conclusion, increasing numbers of individuals
Additionally, most of the studies included a low with ASD are choosing to pursue postsecondary
number of participants, such that results should be education. Though there is relatively little research
taken as theoretical in nature. These studies pro- on this topic thus far, it is gaining traction.
vide a glimpse at how supports for the postsec- Additionally, in recent years, a number of resources
ondary education experience are viewed by those and programs have been developed to assist indi-
in varying roles. Findings from the studies indi- viduals with ASD in planning and reaching their
cated that the amount of parent involvement postsecondary education goals. A variety of post-
expected by institutions varies widely but that in secondary paths exist including some programs
general parents are viewed positively. Examples developed specifically to provide supports for
of parental involvement from the institutional individuals with ASD. The decision of whether
standpoint included written progress reports to and where to pursue postsecondary education is a
parents, weekly emails, and parent informational complex one, and planning is best begun years in
sessions about services and expectations (Barnhill, advance including yearly IEP planning throughout
2016). Szentmiklosi (2009) surveyed community the high school years. Students with ASD are
college students and found family members man- likely to need supports beyond the traditional aca-
aged information related to disability services, demic supports for learning disabilities. For exam-
attended multiple disability service appointments, ple, supports to address deficits in executive
acted as class notetaker, and provided transporta- function, daily living skills, and social skills may
tion. Parents identified the type of help provided be needed. However, individuals with ASD can
as including things such as assisting with money also be a notable asset to the campus community.
management and paying bills, assisting with laun- Postsecondary education provides many with not
dry, continuing to manage appointments, and only the opportunity for important educational-
identifying and helping problem-solve difficult and employment-­ related attainments but also a
social situations (Morrison, Sansosti, & Hadley, developmentally normative environment to con-
2009). Feedback from the students themselves tinue developing social skills and independence.
identified several themes that contribute to appre- Many individuals with ASD can successfully
ciated assistance from a caregiver: a strong sense reach their postsecondary goals with the appropri-
of trust, an intimate connection, a shared vision of ate, well-­coordinated supports as programs to
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 327

p­ rovide these supports and research into best prac- with intellectual and developmental disabilities: A
training and mentoring intervention for employment
tices continue to develop.
consultants. Journal of Rehabilitation, 78(2), 20.
Cai, R. Y., & Richdale, A. L. (2016). Educational expe-
riences and needs of higher education students with
References autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 46, 31–41.
Cameto, R., Levine, P., & Wagner, M. (2004). Transition
Adams, C., Green, J., Gilchrist, A., & Cox, A. (2002).
planning for students with disabilities: A special topic
Conversational behaviour of children with Asperger
report from the National Longitudinal Transition
syndrome and conduct disorder. Journal of Child
Study-2 (NLTS2). Menlo Park, CA: SRI International.
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(5), 679–690.
Carlotti, P. (2014, July 31). More colleges expanding
Adreon, D., & Durocher, J. S. (2007). Evaluating the
programs for students on autism spectrum. Forbes.
college transition needs of individuals with high-­
Retrieved from http://www.forbes.com/sites/paigecar-
functioning autism spectrum disorders. Intervention in
lotti/2014/07/31/more-colleges-expanding-programs-
School and Clinic, 42(5), 271–279.
for-­students-on-autism-spectrum/#173d8fd73661
Alpern, C. S., & Zager, D. (2007). Addressing communi-
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2014).
cation needs of young adults with autism in a college-
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder–autism and
based inclusion program. Education and Training in
developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11
Developmental Disabilities, 42, 428–436.
sites, United States, 2010. Surveillance Summaries,
Americans with Disabilities Act of 1990, 42 U.S.C. §§
63(2), 1–21.
12101 et seq (1991).
Chiang, H.-M., Cheung, Y., Hickson, L., Xiang, R., &
Attwood, T. (2007). The complete guide to Asperger syn-
Tsai, L. (2012). Predictive factors of participation
drome. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
in postsecondary education for high school leavers
Autism Speaks (2016). Community-based skills assess-
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ment. Retrieved from http://www.autismspeaks.org/
Disorders, 42, 685–696.
family-services/community-based-skills-assessment
Colle, L., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., & van der
Barber, P. (2012). College students with disabilities: What
Lely, H. K. (2008). Narrative discourse in adults
factors influence successful degree completion? A case
with high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome.
study. New Brunswick, NJ: John J. Heldrich Center
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
for Workforce Development & Kessler Foundation.
38(1), 28–40.
Retrieved from: http://www.heldrich.rutgers.edu/sites/
Dallas, B. K., Ramisch, J. L., & McGowan, B. (2015).
default/files/products/uploads/College_Students_
Students with autism spectrum disorder and the role of
Disabilities_Report.pdf.
family in postsecondary settings: A systematic review
Barnhill, G. P. (2016). Supporting students with asperger
of the literature. Journal of Postsecondary Education
syndrome on college campuses: Current practices.
And Disability, 28(2), 135–147.
Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities,
Davidson, J., & Henderson, V. (2010). ‘Coming out’ on
31(1), 1–15. doi:10.1177/1088357614523121
the spectrum: Autism, identity and disclosure. Social
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Burtenshaw, A., &
& Cultural Geography, 11(2), 155–170.
Hobson, E. (2007). Mathematical talent is linked to
Davies, C., & Dubie, M. (2012). Intimate relationships
autism. Human Nature, 18(2), 125–131.
and sexual health: A curriculum for teaching ado-
Blalock, G. (2014). Expert update: Inclusive post-­
lescents/adults with high-functioning autism spec-
secondary education for individuals with developmen-
trum disorders and other social challenges. Shawnee
tal disabilities. Retrieved March 13, 2016 from http://
Mission, KS: AAPC Publishing.
www.gcdd.org/blogs/gcdd-blog/2639-expert-update-­
Dillon, M. R. (2007). Creating supports for college stu-
inclusive-post-secondary-education-for-individuals-­
dents with Asperger syndrome through collaboration.
with-developmental-disabilities.html
College Student Journal, 41(2), 499.
Brownell, M., & Walther-Thomas, C. (2001). Steven
Dipeolu, A. O., Storlie, C., & Johnson, C. (2015). College
shore: Understanding the autism spectrum – what
students with high-functioning autism spectrum disor-
teachers need to know. Intervention in School and
der: Best practices for successful transition to the world
Clinic, 36(5), 293–299.
of work. Journal of College Counseling, 18, 175–190.
Brown-Lavoie, S. M., Viecili, M. A., & Weiss, J. A.
Eisenberg, D., Hunt, J., Speer, N., & Zivin, K. (2011).
(2014). Sexual knowledge and victimization in adults
Mental health service utilization among college stu-
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
dents in the United States. The Journal of Nervous and
Developmental Disorders, 44(9), 2185–2196.
Mental Disease, 199(5), 301–308.
Bruininks, R., Woodcock, R., Weatherman, R., & Hill,
Ellison, M., Clark, J., Cunningham, M., Hansen, R.
B. (1996). Scales of independent behavior – Revised.
(2013). Academic and campus accommodations that
Rolling Meadows, IL: Riverside.
foster success for college students with Asperger’s
Butterworth, J., Migliore, A., Nord, D., & Gelb, A. (2012).
disorder. Southern Regional Council on Educational
Improving the employment outcomes of job seekers
Administration 2013 Yearbook: Jazzing It Up, 65.
328 L.W. Williams et al.

Family Educational Rights and Privacy Act of 1974, 20 Hurlbutt, K. S., & Handler, B. R. (2010). High school stu-
U.S.C. § 1232g (1974). dents with Asperger syndrome: A career path binder
Farrell, E. F. (2004). Asperger’s confounds colleges. The project. Intervention in School and Clinic, 46(1),
Chronicle of Higher Education, 51(7), 35–36. 46–50.
Fichten, C. S., Jorgensen, S., Havel, A., Barile, M., Hurlbutt, K., & Chalmers, L. (2004). Employment and
Ferraro, V., Landry, M-E., … Asuncion, J. V. adults with Asperger syndrome. Focus on Autism and
(2012). What happens after graduation? Outcomes, Other Developmental Disabilities, 19(4), 215–222.
employment, and recommendations of recent junior/ IDEA Partnership. (2004). IDEA regulations—
community college graduates with and without dis- Individualized education program (IEP). Author.
abilities. Disability and Rehabilitation, 34(11), Retrieved from http://www.ideapartnership.org/
917–924. using-­tools/topic-briefs/regulatoryprovisions/1679-­
Freedman, S. (2010). Developing college skills in stu- individualized-­education-program-regulations.html
dents with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. London, Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act,
England: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. 20 U.S.C. § 1400 (2004).
Gelbar, N., Shefcyk, A., & Reichow, B. (2015). A com- Klin, A., Saulnier, C., Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D., Volkmar,
prehensive survey of current and former college stu- F., & Lord, C. (2007). Social and communication
dents with autism spectrum disorders. Yale Journal of abilities and disabilities in higher functioning indi-
Biology and Medicine, 88, 45–68. viduals with autism spectrum disorders: The Vineland
Gelbar, N. W., Smith, I., & Reichow, B. (2014). and the ADOS. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Systematic review of articles describing experience Disorders, 37, 748–759.
and supports of individuals with autism enrolled in Lai, M., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. V.,
college and university programs. Journal of Autism Wheelwright, S.J., Sadek, S. A.,. .. Baron-Cohen, S.
and Developmental Disorders, 44, 2593–2601. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male and female
Gobbo, K., & Shmulsky, S. (2014). Faculty experience adults with high functioning autism spectrum condi-
with college students with autism spectrum disorders: tions. PloS One, 6(6), e20835.
A qualitative study of challenges and solutions. Focus Le Couteur, A., Lord, C., & Rutter, M. (2003). Autism
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles, CA:
29(1), 13–22. Western Psychological Services.
Grandin, T., & Duffy, K. (2008). Developing talents: Lysaght, R. M., & Cobigo, V. (2014). Enhancing the
Careers for individuals with Asperger syndrome and quality of life of marginalized populations through
high-functioning autism. Shawn Mission, KS: Autism employment. In R. I. Brown, R. M. Faragher, R. I.
Asperger Publishing. Brown, & R. M. Faragher (Eds.), Quality of life and
Grigal, M., & Hart, D. (2010). Think college: intellectual disability: Knowledge application to other
Postsecondary education options for students social and educational challenges (pp. 155–167).
with intellectual disabilities. Baltimore, MD: Paul Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers.
H. Brookes. Matson, J. L. (Ed.). (2015). Comorbid conditions among
Grigal, M., Hart, D., Smith, F. A., Domin, D., Sulewski, children with autism spectrum disorder. New York:
J., & Weir, C. (2015). Think College National Springer.
Coordinating Center: Annual report on the transi- Matthews, N. L., Ly, A. R., & Goldberg, W. A. (2015).
tion and postsecondary programs for students with College students’ perceptions of peers with
intellectual disabilities (2013–2014). Boston, MA: autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
University of Massachusetts Boston, Institute for Developmental Disorders, 45, 90–99.
Community Inclusion. Mazone, L., & Vitiello, B. (Eds.). (2016). Psychiatric
Henault, I. (2005). Asperger’s syndrome and sexuality: symptoms and comorbidities in autism spec-
From adolescence through adulthood. Philadelphia, trum disorder. New York: Springer International
PA: Jessica Kingsley Publishers. Publishing.
Howlin, P., Mawhood, L., & Rutter, M. (2000). Autism Mazzotti, V. L., Rowe, D. A., Kelley, K. R., Test, D. W.,
and developmental receptive language disorder—a Fowler, C. H., Kohler, P. D., & Kortering, L. J. (2009).
follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: Social, Linking transition assessment and postsecondary
behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. Journal of goals key elements in the secondary transition plan-
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(5), 561–578. ning process. Teaching Exceptional Children, 42(2),
Hughes, J. L. (2009). Higher education and Asperger’s 44–51.
syndrome. The Chronicle of Higher Education, Mehzabin, P., & Stokes, M. A. (2011). Self-assessed
55(40), 21. sexuality in young adults with high-functioning
Hume, K., Boyd, B., Hamm, J., & Kucharcyzk, S. (2014). autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5,
Supporting independence in adolescents on the autism 614–621.
spectrum. Remedial and Special Education, 35(2), Mesibov, G., Thomas, J., Chapman, S. M., & Schopler, E.
102–113. (2007). Teacch transition assessment profile (2nd ed.).
Hurewitz, F., & Berger, P. (2008). Preparing students with Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
autism for college, and preparing colleges for students Morningstar, M. E., Frey, B. B., Noonan, P. M., Ng, J.,
with autism. Speaker’s Journal, 8(11), 109–117. Clavenna-Deane, B., Graves, P.,. .. Williams-Diehm,
18  Supports for Postsecondary Education 329

K. (2010). A preliminary investigation of transi- Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S.,
tion preparation and self-determination for students Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disor-
with disabilities in postsecondary education settings. ders in children with autism spectrum disorders:
Career Development for Exceptional Individuals, 33, Prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a
80–94. population-derived sample. Journal of the American
Morrison, J. Q., Sansoti, F. J., & Hadley, W. M. (2009). Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8),
Parent perceptions of the anticipated needs and expec- 921–929.
tations for support for their college-bound students Smith, C. P. (2007). Support services for students with
with Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Postsecondary Asperger’s syndrome in higher education. College
Education and Disability, 22(2), 78–87. Student Journal, 41(3), 515.
Müller, E., Schuler, A., Burton, B. A., & Yates, G. Sparrow, S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla. (2005). Vineland
B. (2003). Meeting the vocational support needs adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX:
of individuals with Asperger syndrome and other PsychCorp/Pearson Publishing.
autism spectrum disabilities. Journal of Vocational Stokes, M., Newton, N., & Kaur, A. (2007). Stalking, and
Rehabilitation, 18(3), 163–175. social and romantic functioning among adolescents
Nevill, R. E., & White, S. W. (2011). College stu- and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal
dents’ openness toward autism spectrum disorders: of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10),
Improving peer acceptance. Journal of Autism and 1969–1986.
Developmental Disorders, 41(12), 1619–1628. Szentmiklosi, J. M. (2009). Self-determination among
Orsmond, G. I., Krauss, M. W., & Seltzer, M. M. (2004). community college students diagnosed with Asperger’s
Peer relationships and social and recreational activi- syndrome: A qualitative study (Doctoral dissertation).
ties among adolescents and adults with autism. Retrieved from Proquest Database (3401105).
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Test, D. W., Mazzotti, V. L., Mustian, A. L., & Fowler,
34(3), 245–256. C. H. (2009). Evidence-based secondary transition
Petner-Arrey, J., Howell-Moneta, A., & Lysaght, R. predictors for improving post-school outcomes for
(2016). Facilitating employment opportunities for students with disabilities. Career Development for
adults with intellectual and developmental disability Exceptional Individuals, 32, 160–181.
through parents and social networks. Disability and Tevrizian, M. (2015). Man with autism becomes first
Rehabilitation: An International, Multidisciplinary student at Cal Western to pass bar exam. NBC
Journal, 38(8), 789–795. San Diego. Retrieved from: http://www.nbc-
Pinder-Amaker, S. (2014). Identifying the unmet needs sandiego.com/news/local/Erik-Weber-Passes-Bar-
of college students on the autism spectrum. Harvard Exam-304505311.html
Review of Psychiatry, 22(2), 125–137. U.S. Department of Education, Office for Civil Rights.
Rehabilitation Act of 1973, 29 U.S.C. § 701 et seq (1973). (2011). Students with disabilities preparing for post-
Robledo, J. A., & Donnellan, A. M. (2008). Properties secondary education: Know your rights and respon-
of supportive relationships from the perspective of sibilities. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of
academically successful individuals with autism. Education, Office for Civil Rights. Retrieved from
Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 46(4), http://www2.ed.gov/about/offices/list/ocr/transition.
299–310. html
Rosenthal, M., Wallace, G. L., Lawson, R., Wills, M. C., U.S. Department of Labor, & Bureau of Labor Statistics.
Dixon, E., Yerys, B. E., & Kenworthy, L. (2013). (2015). Persons with a disability: Labor force charac-
Impairments in real-world executive function increase teristics – 2014. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department
from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum dis- of Labor, Bureau of Labor Statistics. Retrieved from
orders. Neuropsychology, 27(1), 13–18. http://www.bls.gov/news.release/pdf/disabl.pdf.
Schlabach, T. L. (2008). The college experience of students Uditsky, B., & Hughson, E. (2012). Inclusive postsec-
with Asperger’s disorder: Perceptions of the students ondary education – An evidence-based moral impera-
themselves and of college disability service providers tive. Journal of Policy and Practice in Intellectual
who work with these students (Doctoral dissertation). Disabilities, 9, 298–302.
Retrieved from Proquest Database (3323938). Van Bergeijk, E., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2008).
Shattuck, P. T., Narendorf, S. C., Cooper, B., Supporting more able students on the autism spec-
Sterzing, P. R., Wagner, M., & Taylor, J. L. (2012). trum: College and beyond. Journal of Autism and
Postsecondary education and employment among Developmental Disorders, 38(7), 1359–1370.
youth with an autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics, Wagner, M., Newman, L., Cameto, R., Levine, P.,
129(6), 1042–1049. & Marder, C. (2007). Perceptions and expecta-
Shattuck, P. T., Seltzer, M. M., Greenberg, J. S., Orsmond, tions of youth with disabilities: A special topic
G. I., Bolt, D., Kring, S., ..., Lord, C. (2007). Change report of findings from the National Longitudinal
in autism symptoms and maladaptive behaviors in Transition Study-2 (NLTS2). Menlo Park, CA: SRI
adolescents and adults with an autism spectrum disor- International.
der. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Weigand, J. (2011, May 31). Campuses struggle to design
37(9), 1735–1747. programs for autistic students. Pittsburgh Tribune-­
330 L.W. Williams et al.

Review. Retrieved from: http://www.nfvzone.com/ autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
news/2011/07/17/5640875.htm Disabilities, 25(3), 151–157.
Wolf, L. E., Brown, J. T., & Bork, G. R. K. (2009). Students Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U. (2010). Executive function
with Asperger syndrome: A guide for college personnel. in typical and atypical development. In U. Gaswami
Lenexa, KS: Autism Asperger Publishing Company. (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of child-
Zager, D., & Alpern, C. S. (2010). College-based inclu- hood cognitive development (2nd ed.pp. 574–603).
sion programming for transition-age students with Hoboken, NJ: Wiley-Blackwell Publishing.
Sensory Integration Therapy
and DIR/Floortime 19
Jasper A. Estabillo and Johnny L. Matson

tioning across individuals with ASD and emphasis


Introduction on individualized treatment, it may be under-
standable that families may seek out alternative
The increasing prevalence of autism spectrum treatments; however, it is up to clinicians and
disorder (ASD) has also led to an increase in practitioners who work with families to educate
treatments for the disorder. Given the lifelong them on effective interventions.
impairments on an individual’s functioning asso- Sensory integration and DIR/Floortime are
ciated with ASD, families of those affected may two alternative therapies that may be provided for
seek out numerous treatments to assist with individuals with ASD. A few research studies on
developing skills. Although applied behavior both interventions indicate some support; how-
analysis (ABA)-based interventions have been ever, wide-scale efficacy studies are lacking and
shown to be effective for individuals with ASD long-term outcomes are currently unknown. This
(Dawson et al., 2012; Estes et al., 2015; Matson, chapter discusses the background, applications,
Tureck, Turygin, Beighley, & Rieske, 2012; research, and conclusions for each therapy.
Peters-Scheffer, Didden, Korzilius, & Sturmey,
2011; Reichow, 2011), there continues to be a
myriad of alternative treatments purported to aid Sensory Integration
in treating ASD symptoms and related problems.
Interestingly, few other developmental and medi- Background
cal disorders have been subject to such a high
number of controversial treatments as ASD Sensory behaviors have historically been associ-
(Metz, Mulick, & Butter, 2005). It has been sug- ated with ASD. In his classic study, Kanner
gested that ASD has been so prone to fad treat- (1943) included descriptions of children with
ments because of the heterogeneous nature of the sensory fascinations with lights and spinning
disorder, comorbid problems, limited access to objects, as well as hypersensitivity to sounds and
empirically based treatments, and/or lack of moving objects. Although not specified in previ-
knowledge of ASD and effective interventions ous editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical
(Metz et al., 2005). Given the differences in func- Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM) (American
Psychiatric Association [APA], 1980, 1994) for
ASD symptoms, sensory behaviors were typi-
J.A. Estabillo (*) • J.L. Matson, PhD
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA cally captured within the criteria of stereotyped
e-mail: jestab1@lsu.edu and repetitive motor mannerisms and persistent

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 331


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_19
332 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

preoccupation with parts of objects. The DSM-5 (Rogers, Hepburn, & Wehner, 2003); sensory
(APA, 2013) includes “hyper- or hypo-reactivity reactivity has been found to be associated with
to sensory input or unusual interest in sensory the restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviors
aspects of environment; (such as an apparent and interests characteristic of ASD (Boyd et al.,
indifference to pain/heat/cold, adverse response 2010; Boyd, McBee, Holtzclaw, Baranek, &
to specific sounds or textures, excessive smelling Bodfish, 2009; Gabriels et al., 2008). Researchers
or touching of objects, fascination with lights or have also suggested that impairments in sensory
spinning objects)” under criteria B4 for ASD. processing may result in inappropriate behavioral
Researchers have also established that indi- responses, which can affect an individual’s devel-
viduals with ASD often seek or avoid sensory opment of communication, socialization, and
stimuli in the environment, including auditory, adaptive behavior skills (Jasmin et al., 2009;
tactile, and vestibular input (Baranek, Boyd, Poe, Schaaf, Toth-Cohen, Johnson, Outten, &
David, & Watson, 2007; Ben-Sasson, Carter, & Benevides, 2011). Additionally, sensory selectiv-
Briggs-Gowan, 2009; Ben-Sasson et al., 2008; ity may influence an individual’s food aversions
Hazen, Stornelli, O’Rourke, Koesterer, & (Leekam, Nieto, Libby, Wing, & Gould, 2007;
McDougle, 2014; Rogers & Ozonoff, 2005). It is Paterson & Peck, 2011).
estimated that greater than 80% of children with Greater than 60% of children with ASD
ASD also have problems with sensory processing receive sensory interventions, typically com-
(Ben-Sasson et al., 2009). Joosten and Bundy bined with other therapies including OT, and it is
(2010) found that children with ASD and stereo- the most commonly requested service requested
typical behaviors have significantly greater sen- by parents of children with ASD (Green et al.,
sory processing problems (d = 2.00) than typically 2006). Thus, the need to address treatment needs
developing children. In a meta-analysis of studies for sensory behaviors in individuals with ASD is
on sensory impairments in individuals with ASD, warranted.
Ben-Sasson et al. (2009) found that the greatest
difference between children with and without
ASD was for hypo-reactivity (d = 2.02). Definition
Additionally, large effects were also found for
hyper-reactivity (d = 1.28) and sensory-seeking Sensory-based interventions are centered on the
behaviors (d = 0.82). Developmentally, sensory idea that sensory information may be augmented
processing problems appear to be a greater by applying various sensory sensations in order
impairment during childhood and become less to change an individual’s arousal state (Parham &
significant as one ages. Between 0 and 6 years, Mailloux, 2010). As such, sensory processing is
hyper-reactivity and sensory-seeking behaviors described as necessary for a person to receive,
were found to increase, then peak between 6 regulate, integrate, and organize external sensa-
and 9 years, and ultimately decrease after 9 years tions that are received in one’s central nervous
of age. system to produce correct behavioral responses
Sensory behaviors may also be associated (Bundy, Lane, & Murray, 2002). Therefore, the
with other ASD-associated deficits. The most underlying assumptions of sensory integration
common sensory behavior in children with ASD are (1) the central nervous system is plastic, (2)
is hypo-reactivity in social contexts (Baranek the sensory integration process has a develop-
et al., 2007). This refers to deficits in response to mental progression, (3) the brain is composed of
social stimuli, which can be attributed to the systems that are hierarchical which function
social communication impairments characteristic together, (4) adaptive behaviors and sensory inte-
of the disorder. The repetitive movements com- gration have a circular relationship, and (5)
monly seen in individuals with ASD may also humans are innately driven to develop sensory
be attributed to sensory processing problems integration through engaging in sensorimotor
19  Sensory Integration and DIR/Floortime 333

activities (Fisher & Murray, 1991; Metz et al., weighted vest may be worn in order to provide
2005). Sensory interventions have several pressure to the individual’s body. Multiple
­components: use of a variety of modalities, vari- sensory-­based interventions may be incorporated
ous target behaviors, passive and active child par- into an individual’s treatment plan, and a “sen-
ticipation, and applications in various contexts sory diet” may be implemented in which differ-
(Case-Smith, Weaver, & Fristad, 2014). Thus, the ent types of sensory stimulation are given to the
purpose of sensory interventions is to address individual on a specified schedule in order to
underlying sensory dysfunction and improve treat “sensory defensiveness” (Wilbarger &
one’s ability to integrate sensory stimuli (Ayres, Wilbarger, 2002). Sensory defensiveness is
1979; Dawson & Watling, 2000). Clinicians defined as the “overreaction and avoidance of a
using sensory integration therapy report improve- sensation from any sensory modality” (Wilbarger
ments in individuals’ ability to focus attention & Wilbarger, 1991). The sensory diet may
and remain on task, reduction in maladaptive include various activities such as gross motor
behaviors, and general improvements in addi- actions (e.g., jumping on trampoline, swinging)
tional skills such as language (Devlin, Leader, & in addition to dry brushing and deep pressure
Healy, 2009). throughout the day.
Ayres Sensory Integration Therapy is a clinic-­
based, child-centered intervention in which
play-­based activities are provided in order to Applications
contrive opportunities for a child to engage in
adaptive responses (Case-Smith et al., 2014). Sensory integration therapies were developed to
According to Ayres (1979), sensory integration address sensory dysfunctions in individuals. In
therapy is based on “neurological processes that addition to individuals with ASD, the treatment
organize sensation from one’s own body and has been used with individuals with intellectual
from the environment and makes it possible to disabilities, learning disabilities, behavioral
use the body effectively within the environ- problems, and cerebral palsy (Case-Smith &
ment.” It is believed that typical sensory function Miller, 1999; National Board for Certification in
is a natural result of sensorimotor development Occupational Therapy, 2004; Spitzer, Roley,
and the appropriate integration of sensory behav- Clark, & Parham, 1996; Watling, Deitz, Kanny,
iors allows the individual to engage and partici- & McLaughlin, 1999). It is frequently imple-
pate in a variety of meaningful and purposeful mented with individuals on the spectrum, and the
activities. high use of sensory integration in this population
In contrast, sensory-based interventions are may be due to the overlap between sensory ste-
structured, adult-directed sensory strategies that reotypies and sensory stimulation (Davis,
are incorporated into other routines and therapies Durand, & Chan, 2011). Sensory integration
in attempt to improve behavioral regulation therapies are typically implemented by occupa-
(American Academy of Pediatrics, 2012; Case-­ tional therapists; Watling et al. (1999) conducted
Smith et al., 2014). These interventions are based a survey with occupational therapists and found
on the theory that certain types of sensory input that 82% reported always using some form of
(e.g., pressure, rocking) may be calming to indi- sensory integration therapy in their practice.
viduals and promote self-regulation. Examples of However, some sensory integration techniques
sensory-based interventions include dry brush- may also be used by teachers and other practitio-
ing, wearing a weighted vest, and incorporation ners (Worley, Fodstad, & Neal, 2014).
of various gross motor activities. Dry brushing Additionally, sensory integration therapies are
and compression involve providing the individual some of the most commonly recommended treat-
with deep pressure to the skin and joints ments by professionals in the school system
(Wilbarger & Wilbarger, 1991). Similarly, a (Miller, Schreck, Mulick, & Butter, 2012).
334 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

Effectiveness Research Supporting

The difficulty in assessing the effectiveness of Several individual research studies have been
sensory integration therapies is in the heteroge- conducted to examine the effectiveness of sen-
neity of interventions used, as well as the skills sory integration therapy and sensory-based inter-
targeted. Researchers have studied various com- ventions. The following recent reviews of sensory
ponents of sensory integration therapy and integration treatments are discussed to provide
sensory-­ based interventions; however, due to research on the overall effectiveness of the inter-
the differences in therapies used, methodology, vention type.
outcomes, and populations studied, there is lim- Case-Smith et al. (2014) conducted a system-
ited research on the effectiveness of sensory atic review of studies on sensory integration ther-
integration itself. apy and sensory-based interventions published
In order to assist with concerns regarding the between 2000 and 2014. Two randomized con-
research on sensory integration therapy, Parham trolled trials of sensory integration therapy were
and colleagues (2007, 2011) developed the Ayres reviewed, with positive effects for child perfor-
Sensory Integration Fidelity measure to assess the mance found (d = 0.72–1.62). The additional sen-
ten essential elements of Ayres Sensory Integration sory integration therapy studies reviewed also
Therapy. The essential components include (1) showed reduction in sensory problems. For
ensure the individual’s physical safety, (2) present sensory-­based interventions, few positive effects
a range of opportunities for sensory stimulation, were found. These included single-sensory strat-
(3) use activities and arrange the environment to egies used to influence one’s state of arousal
help the child maintain self-­regulation and atten- (e.g., weighted vest). As stated by the authors,
tion, (4) challenge the individual’s motor control, sensory-based interventions may not be effective
(5) challenge praxis and organization of behavior, due to the lack of fidelity to treatment protocols
(6) collaborate with the child on choice of activi- or targeting specific problems with sensory
ties, (7) contrive activities to present the just-right processing.
challenge, (8) ensure success, (9) support the indi- Watling and Hauer (2015) conducted a sys-
vidual’s motivation to play and engage in the tematic review of the literature from 2006 to
activity, and (10) establish rapport with the indi- 2013 on the effectiveness of Ayres Sensory
vidual. Regarding psychometrics, content valid- Integration Therapy and sensory-based inter-
ity, internal consistency, and inter-rater reliability ventions in occupational therapy for individuals
on the total score were found to be high on the with ASD. Moderate evidence was found for
measure (Parham et al., 2011). Use of this mea- the use of Ayres Sensory Integration Therapy,
sure may be helpful for future research because and mixed results were found for sensory-based
the operational definitions of components of sen- interventions. The studies included in the
sory integration therapy will allow researchers to review met with published criteria to be classi-
measure various aspects of the intervention. As fied as Ayres Sensory Integration Therapy.
outlined by the American Occupational Therapy Three of the four Ayres Sensory Integration
Association (2014), research opportunities in sen- Therapy studies reviewed showed positive
sory integration research include using occupa- effects for reducing autism symptoms. For the
tional therapy with sensory integration approaches sensory-based interventions, single-sensory
to develop individual functional goals, motor interventions (e.g., weighted vests) had little or
skills, sensory-­perceptual skills, emotional regu- no effects, but vestibular input (e.g., spinning,
lation skills, communication and social skills, swinging, hanging upside down) had some lim-
mental function, and pain management. ited support.
19  Sensory Integration and DIR/Floortime 335

Research Not Supporting Discussion

A review conducted by Lang et al. (2012) Researchers have suggested that sensory prob-
found no consistently positive effects of sen- lems can affect children’s behavior but the rela-
sory integration therapy for children with tionship between sensory problems, stereotypic
ASD. The authors examined 25 studies and behavior, attention, activity level, and regulation
found 3 studies with positive results, 8 studies is not well understood (Case-Smith et al., 2014).
with mixed findings, and 14 studies with no The underlying approach to sensory integration
benefits of sensory integration therapy. They therapy is improving behaviors associated with
concluded that there was insufficient evidence sensory dysfunction; however, most studies do
for sensory integration to be used for this pop- not use neurophysiological measures to support
ulation. Moreover, the authors stated that it their assertions (Metz et al., 2005). The underly-
may be inappropriate for agencies that are ing mechanisms between nervous system impair-
mandated to use evidence-based interventions ment and functional behavior changes are not
to be using sensory integration therapy. known (Iarocci & McDonald, 2006; Metz et al.,
Agencies such as public schools, which are 2005), and research is needed on the neurobiol-
required under the Individuals with Disabilities ogy of sensory symptoms and how to treat them
Education Improvement Act (2004), were cau- (Hazen et al., 2014). Additionally, it is not clear
tioned against the use of this intervention due whether children with sensory problems have an
to lack of empirical evidence. actual “sensory disorder” of their sensory path-
Baranek (2002) found low-level support that ways or if these impairments are due to other
sensory integration practices improve social developmental and behavioral disorders
skills in children with ASD. Both research on (American Academy of Pediatrics, 2012).
Ayres Sensory Integration Therapy and other Because sensory difficulties are often seen in
sensory-based interventions (e.g., sensory stimu- various developmental and behavioral disorders,
lation techniques, auditory integration and related it is recommended that an evaluation be con-
interventions, visual therapies, physical exercise) ducted for these disorders be completed to deter-
were reviewed. Results were inconsistent across mine where the individual’s deficits lie and how
studies, and in the few controlled studies to treat them.
included, there was limited support. Overall, there is a lack of solid evidence in
Polatajko and Cantin (2010) called for an support of sensory integration therapy for indi-
urgent need for well-controlled studies examin- viduals with ASD (Worley et al., 2014). In addi-
ing the effectiveness of interventions used in tion to the inability to connect sensory integration
occupational therapy with well-defined, homog- therapy with mechanisms underlying sensory
enous populations on outcomes that target adap- dysfunction, there is concern that sensory inte-
tive skills. Additional rigorous randomized gration therapy may be associated with increases
controlled trials using manualized protocols for in challenging behaviors (Devlin et al., 2009;
sensory integration therapy and sensory-based Devlin, Healy, Leader, & Hughes, 2010; Mason
interventions are needed to evaluate their effects. & Iwata, 1990). Without direct behavioral inter-
There is a need for higher-level studies with vention to redirect the individual or provide them
larger sample sizes and operationalized defini- with replacement behaviors, the rate of challeng-
tions with systematic methods to determine the ing behavior may increase.
effect of sensory interventions in individuals with As suggested by Wong et al. (2015), research-
ASD. Additionally, studies examining the effect ers and clinicians should use a combination of
of sensory-based interventions on specific behav- evidence-based practices to address the unique
iors are needed. needs and goals of the individual when planning
336 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

treatment. At present, the field lacks consensus strengths and weaknesses at each stage of devel-
regarding sensory interventions, and there is opment to build healthy foundations for social,
insufficient support for this intervention (Wan emotional, and intellectual abilities (Greenspan
Yunus, Liu, Bissett, & Penkala, 2015). Given & Wieder, 2006; Masse, McNeil, Wagner, &
the limited evidence for sensory integration Chorney, 2007). The child’s functional emotional
therapy and sensory-based interventions, it is skills are assessed and used as a foundation for
not currently considered to be evidence-based teaching new skills. Additionally, the child’s sen-
practice. However, if a family is interested in sory modulation abilities and motor planning
pursuing sensory therapies, the American skills are also taken into consideration for devel-
Academy of Pediatrics also states that sensory- oping interactions (Greenspan & Wieder, 1999).
based interventions may be acceptable but only Greenspan and Wieder (1999) view ASD as an
if used as a component to a comprehensive “inability to relate to others affectively in a recip-
treatment plan (2012). As such, it is unclear rocal fashion in a variety of contexts.” Thus, DIR/
what sensory interventions families should seek Floortime aims to create mutually enjoyable
and what clinicians should recommend (Case- shared experiences between the child and parent
Smith et al., 2014). Because it is unknown how and, therefore, reduce the child’s social isolation
sensory integration therapies may be effective (Masse et al., 2007).
and what components of the interventions are As ASD is characterized by pervasive impair-
effectual, families should be cautioned about ments in social communication, DIR/Floortime
sensory integration therapies. Therefore, fami- is based on the foundation of increasing positive
lies should be informed of the limitations of social interactions to develop more complex
sensory integration therapies and how to evalu- skills. In this model, there is an emphasis on
ate their effectiveness through behavior tracking creating meaningful interpersonal relationships
and rating scales (American Academy of between the adult and child (Wagner, Wallace,
Pediatrics, 2012). & Rogers, 2014). As stated by Greenspan and
Wieder (1999), “the primary goal of DIR-based
intervention program (sometimes referred to as
DIR/Floortime Floortime) is to enable children to form a sense
of themselves as intentional, interactive indi-
Background viduals, develop cognitive language and social
capacities from this basic sense of intentional-
DIR/Floortime is the application of the Develop-­ ity, and progress through the six functional
mental, Individual-differences, and Relationship-­ emotional developmental capacities.” There are
based (DIR) model that focuses on the child’s critical milestones that are central to the DIR/
developmental abilities in the context of his or Floortime approach. They include (1) self-regu-
her individual processing profile and interactions lation and interest in the world, (2) developing
with the family (Greenspan & Wieder, 2006, relationships and attachment with others, (3)
2007). The components of the DIR model are reciprocal communication, (4) complex com-
Development, which emphasizes the child’s munication, (5) emotional ideas (e.g., pretend
developmental level in developing a treatment play, identifying emotions, perspective taking),
plan; Individual-differences, which focuses on and (6) emotional thinking (e.g., connecting
the child’s unique needs; and Relationship-based, one’s actions and feelings, understanding the
which described the learning relationships that relationship between self and others). These
the child has with their caregivers. This model milestones progress sequentially and are impor-
focuses on the individual’s social and emotional tant for individuals to develop communication,
development, sensory processing, and motor thinking, and emotional coping skills (Metz
planning skills by working with the individual’s et al., 2005).
19  Sensory Integration and DIR/Floortime 337

Definition Regarding the settings that DIR/Floortime is


implemented, the intervention is typically con-
Floortime is one component of the DIR model ducted in the family’s home, in clinics, or in
and refers to the specific technique of getting on schools (Wagner et al., 2014). Floortime sessions
to the floor to work with the child and build his or are to take place daily in the family’s home with
her skills. The technique emphasizes a child-lead the caregiver and child (Greenspan & Wieder,
approach and joining the child in his or her world 2006, 2007). These sessions are conducted dur-
to promote their functional emotional develop- ing playtime in which the parent follows the
mental capacities. It is a child-lead intervention child’s lead and facilitates development of vari-
in which the role of the caregiver is to develop the ous skills. Therapists may also attend Floortime
child’s skills by following the child’s lead. During sessions in order to provide feedback and assist
Floortime sessions, the adult follows the child’s the caregiver.
interests and uses their interactions to promote
the child’s progression through socialization
skills (Wieder & Greenspan, 2003). Foundational Effectiveness
skills such as joint attention, engagement, basic
gestures, and problem solving are emphasized to There is very limited research on the effective-
promote development of more complex skills. ness of DIR/Floortime as an intervention for indi-
The primary objective of DIR/Floortime is to viduals with ASD. Current studies are small
encourage parents to meet their child at the scale, and there are a limited number of studies
child’s developmental level (Greenspan & comparing the efficacy of DIR/Floortime to other
Wieder, 2007). Parents direct their child to intervention methods. Therefore, additional
increasingly complex interactions through a pro- research on this therapy is needed.
cess of “opening and closing circles of communi-
cation.” Dedicated DIR/Floortime sessions are
encouraged throughout the day. In addition to Research Supporting
Floortime sessions, parents are also encouraged
to utilize natural opportunities to implement Greenspan and Wieder (1999) examined treat-
DIR/Floortime strategies throughout the day, ment outcomes for 200 children after implement-
such as during the morning routine and meal ing the DIR/Floortime intervention. After at least
times. DIR/Floortime therapists work with care- 2 years of DIR/Floortime treatment, over half of
givers in the family’s home, in clinics, or in the the children showed significant improvements in
child’s school setting in order to provide the care- social, cognitive, and academic skills, as well as
givers with as much support for the child as pos- decreases in social isolation (Greenspan &
sible (Wagner et al., 2014). Wieder, 2006). Clinically, those children no lon-
ger scored in the Autistic range on the Childhood
Autism Rating Scale (Schopler, Reichler, &
Applications Renner, 1986). At follow-up 10–15 years later,
children who had initial gains continued to show
DIR/Floortime was developed for young children treatment gains, such that they continued to show
with ASD. It has primarily been utilized in this little or no impairments in core ASD symptoms.
population. The literature on DIR/Floortime has In a pilot study by Solomon, Necheles, Ferch,
focused on young children (i.e., infants, toddlers, and Bruckman (2007), the DIR/Floortime
and preschool-aged children), and there is no approach was found to be effective at increasing
information on the use of this intervention for social communication skills. The study partici-
older individuals. Given the nature of the inter- pants included 68 children aged 18 months to
vention, it may not be appropriate for individuals 6 years old who completed an 8–12 month pro-
outside of the indicated developmental age range. gram of 15 h per week of 1:1 interaction with
338 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

their parents. About 45% of children made good either DIR/Floortime or ABA intervention. Both
to very good clinically significant developmental children had severe language delays. After
progress. Importantly, parents were found to be 9 weeks, results showed that the child who
effective at implementing the intervention. This received ABA had slight gains in the
study provided preliminary support for the use of Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales
DIR/Floortime as a cost-effective intervention (Wetherby, Watt, Morgan, & Shumway, 2007)
for young children with ASD. composite score, while the DIR/Floortime child
Another pilot study by Pajareya and had slight losses. The ABA child showed
Nopmaneejumruslers (2011) examined the effi- improvements in the areas of gestures, vocal
cacy of DIR/Floortime in preschool children in communicative means, and social-affective sig-
Thailand. Thirty-two children and their parents naling, as well as decreases in the areas of reci-
were enrolled in the study. Parents were trained procity and symbolic behaviors. The DIR/
by the researchers on how to implement DIR/ Floortime child showed improvements in reci-
Floortime sessions. After implementing the inter- procity and symbolic behaviors and had decreases
vention for about 15 h each week over 3 months, in vocal communicative means and social-­
the children in the DIR/Floortime group made affective signaling. The greatest difference
significantly greater gains on measures of emo- between the interventions was that the ABA child
tional development (e.g., engagement, social-­ was found to have greater improvements in
emotional reciprocity, communication) and response to name and completion of one-step
decreases in ASD symptom severity when com- instructions. The DIR/Floortime child had greater
pared to a typical treatment group. improvements over the ABA child in imitation
Casenhiser, Shanker, and Stieben (2013) con- and spontaneous production of words. Data from
ducted a randomized controlled trial to evaluate this study indicate that DIR/Floortime may aid to
the efficacy of a DIR-based intervention. The in improving skills; however, given the small
intervention group received 2 h of coaching and scale of the study (i.e., number of participants as
therapy each week, while a community therapy well as timeline), it is unclear what additional
group received various services for around 4 h factors may have played a role in the outcome
each week. The study examined the quality of and if these treatment gains are maintained over
social interaction (i.e., engagement in play), abil- time.
ity to engage in and initiate joint attention, degree
of enjoyment in interaction, and language ability.
Fifty-one children aged 2–4 years were given Discussion
treatment over 1 year. The children in the DIR-­
based intervention group made significantly Although the few studies on DIR/Floortime indi-
greater gains in social interaction skills over the cate promising treatment gains, there is currently
community treatment group. Specifically, the such limited research on the intervention that it
DIR-based intervention group showed greater is unclear if the intervention is effective and
improvements in enjoyment of interactions, should be recommended for individuals with
engagement in interactions, and initiation of joint ASD. There are several limitations that should
attention. be considered when evaluating DIR/Floortime.
First, because there are so few studies, the
research must be replicated. In order to do so,
Research Not Supporting intervention methods (e.g., treatment dosage,
skills targeted) should be operationalized.
A study by Hilton and Seal (2007) compared Secondly, most studies have small sample sizes,
ABA intervention and DIR/Floortime and found which limit generalization of results. Thirdly,
mixed results. The researchers randomly assigned additional variables that may play a role in the
2-year-old male monozygotic twins with ASD to efficacy of the therapy should also be explored.
19  Sensory Integration and DIR/Floortime 339

Lastly, future research studies should compare themselves on empirically based treatments and
DIR/Floortime to other methods of intervention provide families with valid recommendations.
and assess treatment gains over longer periods of Regarding sensory integration therapies and
time. A major limitation of DIR/Floortime is the DIR/Floortime, there is limited evidence for the
intensive nature of the ­ intervention. DIR/ effectiveness of these interventions for children
Floortime requires caregivers to spend signifi- with ASD. Although some studies show marginal
cant time with their children conducting improvements, the efficacy and effectiveness of
Floortime sessions; as such, families must be these treatments are not to the same level as
highly motivated and able to dedicate the time to ABA-based treatments (Tarbox, Dixon, Sturmey,
implement the intervention (Masse et al., 2007). & Matson, 2014). ABA-based treatments have
While this intervention lacks sufficient the most empirical support in treating ASD
research evidence, there are components of DIR/ (Lofthouse, Hendren, Hurt, Arnold, & Butter,
Floortime that are part of traditional behavioral 2012; Matson & Smith, 2008; Tarbox et al.,
therapy which may benefit children with 2014). Given the growing number of individuals
ASD. The DIR/Floortime model incorporates affected with ASD, it is important that funding
several teaching methods including incidental sources provide services for empirically based
teaching, optimizing interactions, and shaping interventions. Researchers state that the most
and reinforcement techniques. The difference effective therapies for individuals with ASD are
between DIR/Floortime and ABA-based inter- individualized treatments that include functional
ventions is that DIR/Floortime does not use dis- communication training, social skills interven-
crete trials to teach skills. Although DIR/ tion, and behavioral supports (Lofthouse et al.,
Floortime emphasizes child-lead interactions, the 2012). Interventions for individuals with ASD
parent is able to incorporate several teaching should be lifelong, with multidisciplinary ser-
methods that are used in traditionally behavioral vices that are evidence based and found to be
approaches. Additionally, while the focus of effective, including applied behavior analysis,
DIR/Floortime is to develop social-emotional educational programs, speech therapy, occupa-
skills, there are components that may promote tional therapy, social skills training, and physical
the development of the child’s motor, cognitive, therapy.
and language skills (Wagner et al., 2014).

References
Conclusion
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2012). Sensory inte-
gration therapies for children with developmental and
The combination of professional recommenda- behavioral disorders. Pediatrics, 129(6), 1186–1189.
tions and influence of anecdotal reports of suc- https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0876
cess with various treatments may lead many American Occupational Therapy Association. (2014).
Research opportunities in the area of children and
caregivers to seek multiple therapies for their adolescents with challenges in sensory process-
child (Miller et al., 2012). As a result, most chil- ing and sensory integration. American Journal of
dren with ASD are receiving between four and Occupational Therapy, 68(2), 242–244. https://doi.
six therapies (Goin-Kochel, Myers, & org/10.5014/ajot.2014.682003
American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic
Mackintosh, 2007). Most parents are in the view and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.).
that more therapies are better; however, parents Washington, DC: Author.
do not typically prioritize treatments being evi- American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
dence based, with about half of parents choosing and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
research-supported treatments for their child American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
(Miller et al., 2012). It is, therefore, the responsi- and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
bility of professionals in the field to educate Washington, DC: Author.
340 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

Ayres, A. J. (1979). Sensory integration and the child. Los Dawson, G., Jones, E. J. H., Merkle, K., Venema, K.,
Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. Lowy, R., Faja, S., … Webb, S. J. (2012). Early
Baranek, G. T. (2002). Efficacy of sensory and motor behavioral intervention is associated with normal-
interventions for children with autism. Journal of ized brain activity in young children with autism.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32(5), 397– Journal of the American Academy of Child &
422. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020541906063 Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(11), 1150–1159. https://
Baranek, G. T., Boyd, B. A., Poe, M. D., David, F. J., & doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2012.08.018
Watson, L. R. (2007). Hyperresponsive sensory pat- Devlin, S., Leader, G., & Healy, O. (2009). Comparison
terns in young children with autism, developmental of behavioral intervention and sensory-integration
delay, and typical development. American Journal therapy in the treatment of self-injurious behavior.
on Mental Retardation, 112(4), 233–245. https://doi. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(1), 223–
org/10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[233:HSPIYC]2.0 231. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.06.004
.CO;2 Devlin, S., Healy, O., Leader, G., & Hughes, B. M. (2010).
Ben-Sasson, A., Hen, L., Fluss, R., Cermak, S. A., Comparison of behavioral intervention and sensory-­
Engel-Yeger, B., & Gal, E. (2008). A meta-analysis integration therapy in the treatment of challeng-
of sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with ing behavior. Journal of Autism and Developmental
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Disorders, 41, 1303–1320.
Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 1–11. https://doi. Estes, A., Munson, J., Rogers, S. J., Greenson, J., Winter,
org/10.1007/s10803-008-0593-3 J., & Dawson, G. (2015). Long-term outcomes of
Ben-Sasson, A., Carter, A. S., & Briggs-Gowan, M. J. early intervention in 6-year-old children with autism
(2009). Sensory over-responsivity in elementary spectrum disorder. Journal of the American Academy
school: Prevalence and social-emotional correlates. of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(7), 580–587.
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 37(5), 705– https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.04.005
716. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-008-9295-8 Fisher, A., & Murray, E. A. (1991). Introduction to sen-
Boyd, B. A., McBee, M., Holtzclaw, T., Baranek, G. T., & sory integration theory. In A. G. Fisher, E. A. Murray,
Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Relationships among repetitive & A. C. Bundy (Eds.), Sensory integration: Theory
behaviors, sensory features, and executive functions in and practice (pp. 3–26). Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis.
high functioning autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Miller, L. J., Gralla, J., Pan,
Disorders, 3(4), 959–966. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Z., Goldson, E., … Hooks, E. (2008). Is there a rela-
rasd.2009.05.003 tionship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped
Boyd, B. A., Baranek, G. T., Sideris, J., Poe, M. D., behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response
Watson, L. R., Patten, E., & Miller, H. (2010). Sensory in children with autism spectrum disorders? Research
features and repetitive behaviors in children with in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 660–670. https://
autism and developmental delays. Autism Research, doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2008.02.002
3(2), 78–87. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.124 Goin-Kochel, R. P., Myers, B. J., & Mackintosh, V. H.
Bundy, A. C., Lane, S. J., & Murray, E. A. (2002). (2007). Parental reports on the use of treatments and
Sensory integration: Theory and practice (2nd ed.). therapies for children with autism spectrum disorders.
Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(3), 195–
Casenhiser, D. M., Shanker, S. G., & Stieben, J. (2013). 209. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2006.08.006
Learning through interaction in children with autism: Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly,
Preliminary data from asocial-communication-based M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments
intervention. Autism, 17(2), 220–241. https://doi. used by parents of children with autism. Research
org/10.1177/1362361311422052 in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary
Case-Smith, J., & Miller, H. (1999). Occupational therapy Journal, 27(1), 70–84.
with children with pervasive developmental disor- Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (1999). A functional
ders. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 53, developmental approach to autism spectrum disor-
506–513. ders. Research and Practice for Persons with Severe
Case-Smith, J., Weaver, L. L., & Fristad, M. A. (2014). A Disabilities, 24(3), 147–161.
systematic review of sensory processing interventions Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2006). Engaging autism:
for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism, Using the floortime approach to help children relate,
19(2), 133–148. communicate, and think. Cambridge, MA: Da Capo
Davis, T. N., Durand, S., & Chan, J. M. (2011). The effects Press.
of a brushing procedure on stereotypical behavior. Greenspan, S. I., & Wieder, S. (2007). The developmen-
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(3), 1053– tal individual-difference, relationship-based (DIR/
1058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.11.011 Floortime) model approach to autism spectrum dis-
Dawson, G., & Watling, R. (2000). Interventions to facili- orders. In E. Hollander, E. Anagnostou, E. Hollander,
tate auditory, visual, and motor integration in autism: & E. Anagnostou (Eds.), Clinical manual for the
A review of the evidence. Journal of Autism and treatment of autism (pp. 179–209). Arlington, VA:
Developmental Disorders, 30, 415–421. American Psychiatric Publishing.
19  Sensory Integration and DIR/Floortime 341

Hazen, E. P., Stornelli, J. L., O’Rourke, J. A., Koesterer, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1412–
K., & McDougle, C. J. (2014). Sensory symp- 1417. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2012.02.010
toms in autism spectrum disorders. Harvard Metz, B., Mulick, J. A., & Butter, E. M. (2005). Autism: A
Review of Psychiatry, 22(2), 112–124. ­https://doi. late-20th-century fad magnet. In J. W. Jacobson, R. M.
org/10.1097/01.HRP.0000445143.08773.58 Foxx, & J. A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial therapies
Hilton, J. C., & Seal, B. C. (2007). Brief report: for developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and sci-
Comparative ABA and DIR trials in twin brothers ence in professional practice (pp. 237–263). Mahwah,
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Disorders, 37(6), 1197–1201. https://doi.org/10.1007/ Miller, V. A., Schreck, K. A., Mulick, J. A., & Butter, E.
s10803-006-0258-z (2012). Factors related to parents’ choices of treat-
Iarocci, G., & McDonald, J. (2006). Sensory integration ments for their children with autism spectrum disor-
and the perceptual experience of persons with autism. ders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6(1),
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 87–95.
77–90. National Board for Certification in Occupational Therapy.
Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act (2004). A practice analysis for entry-level occupa-
(2004) of 2004, 20 U.S.C. § 1400. tional therapists registered and certified occupational
Jasmin, E., Couture, M., McKinley, P., Reid, G., therapy assistant practice. Occupational Therapy
Fombonne, E., & Gisel, E. (2009). Sensori-motor Journal of Research, 24(Suppl. 1), 1–31.
and daily living skills of preschool children with Pajareya, K., & Nopmaneejumruslers, K. (2011). A pilot
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and randomized controlled trial of DIR/Floortime par-
Developmental Disorders, 39, 231–241. ent training intervention for pre-school children with
Joosten, A. V., & Bundy, A. C. (2010). Sensory process- autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(5), 563–577.
ing and stereotypical and repetitive behaviour in chil- https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361310386502
dren with autism and intellectual disability. Australian Parham, L. D., & Mailloux, Z. (2010). Sensory integra-
Occupational Therapy Journal, 57(6), 366–372. tion. In J. Case-Smith, A. S. Allen, & P. N. Pratt (Eds.),
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1630.2009.00835.x Occupational therapy for children. (5th ed., pp. 356–
Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con- 411). St. Louis, MO: Elsevier.
tact. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 35(4), 100–136. Parham, L. D., Cohn, E. S., Spitzer, S., Koomar, J. A.,
Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Healy, O., Rispoli, M., Lydon, Miller, L. J., Burke, J. P., … Roley, S. S. (2007).
H., Streusand, W., … Giesbers, S. (2012). Sensory Fidelity in sensory integration intervention research.
integration therapy for autism spectrum disorders: The American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
A systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum 61(2), 216.
Disorders, 6(3), 1004–1018. https://doi.org/10.1016/j. Parham, L.D., Roley, S.S., May-Benson, T. A., Koomar,
rasd.2012.01.006 J., Brett-Green, B., Burke, J.P., … Schaaf, R.C. (2011).
Leekam, S. R., Nieto, C., Libby, S. J., Wing, L., & Gould, Development of a fidelity measure for research on
J. (2007). Describing the sensory abnormalities of the effectiveness of the Ayres Sensory Integration(R)
children and adults with autism. Journal of Autism and Intervention. American Journal of Occupational
Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 894–910. https://doi. Therapy, 65(2), 133–142. https://doi.org/10.5014/
org/10.1007/s10803-006-0218-7 ajot.2011.000745
Lofthouse, N., Hendren, R., Hurt, E., Arnold, L. E., & Paterson, H., & Peck, K. (2011). Sensory processing ability
Butter, E. (2012). A review of complementary and and eating behaviour in children with autism. Journal
alternative treatments for autism spectrum disorders. of Human Nutrition and Dietetics, 24(3), 301–301.
Autism Research and Treatment, 2012, 1–21. https:// https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-277X.2011.01175_31.x
doi.org/10.1155/2012/870391 Peters-Scheffer, N., Didden, R., Korzilius, H., & Sturmey,
Mason, S. A., & Iwata, B. A. (1990). Artifactual effects of P. (2011). A meta-analytic study on the effectiveness
sensory-integrative therapy on self-injurious behavior. of comprehensive ABA-based early intervention pro-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 361–370. grams for children with autism spectrum disorders.
Masse, J. J., McNeil, C. B., Wagner, S. M., & Chorney, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5(1), 60–69.
D. B. (2007). Parent-child interaction therapy and high https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2010.03.011
functioning autism: A conceptual overview. Journal of Polatajko, H. J., & Cantin, N. (2010). Exploring the effec-
Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4(4), 714. tiveness of occupational therapy interventions, other
Matson, J. L., & Smith, K. R. M. (2008). Current sta- than the sensory integration approach, with children
tus of intensive behavioral interventions for young and adolescents experiencing difficulty processing and
children with autism and PDD-NOS. Research in integrating sensory information. American Journal of
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(1), 60–74. https://doi. Occupational Therapy, 64(3), 415–429. https://doi.
org/10.1016/j.rasd.2007.03.003 org/10.5014/ajot.2010.09072
Matson, J. L., Tureck, K., Turygin, N., Beighley, J., & Reichow, B. (2011). Overview of meta-analyses on early
Rieske, R. (2012). Trends and topics in early inten- intensive behavioral intervention for young children
sive behavioral interventions for toddlers with autism. with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
342 J.A. Estabillo and J.L. Matson

Developmental Disorders, 42(4), 512–520. https://doi. behavioral problems: A systematic review. Journal of
org/10.1007/s10803-011-1218-9 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(11), 3565–
Rogers, S. J., & Ozonoff, S. (2005). Annotation: What 3579. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2503-9
do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A Watling, R., & Hauer, S. (2015). Effectiveness of Ayres
critical review of the empirical evidence. Journal of sensory integration and sensory-based interventions
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(12), 1255–1268. for people with autism spectrum disorder: A systematic
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01431.x review. American Journal of Occupational Therapy,
Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S., & Wehner, E. (2003). 69(5), 6905180030p1. https://doi.org/10.5014/
Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers ajot.2015.018051
with autism and those with other developmental Watling, R., Deitz, J., Kanny, E. M., & McLaughlin,
disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental J. F. (1999). Current practice of occupational ther-
Disorders, 33(6), 631–642. https://doi.org/10.1023/ apy for children with autism. American Journal of
B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7 Occupational Therapy, 53(5), 498–505. https://doi.
Schaaf, R. C., Toth-Cohen, S., Johnson, S. L., Outten, org/10.5014/ajot.53.5.498
G., & Benevides, T. W. (2011). The everyday routines Wetherby, A. M., Watt, N., Morgan, L., & Shumway, S.
of families of children with autism: Examining the (2007). Social communication profiles of children
impact of sensory processing difficulties on the fam- with autism spectrum disorders late in the second
ily. Autism, 15, 373–389. 1362361310386505. https:// year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental
doi.org/10.1177/1362361310386505 Disorders, 37(5), 960–975.
Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J., & Renner, B. R. (1986). The Wieder, S., & Greenspan, S. I. (2003). Climbing the sym-
childhood autism rating scale (CARS): For diagnostic bolic ladder in the DIR model through floortime/inter-
screening and classification of autism. New York, NY: active play. Autism, 7(4), 425–435.
Irvington. Wilbarger, P., & Wilbarger, J. (1991). Sensory-­
Solomon, R., Necheles, J., Ferch, C., & Bruckman, D. defensiveness in children ages 2–12. An intervention
(2007). Pilot study of a parent training program for guide for parents and other caretakers. Denver, CO:
young children with autism: The PLAY project home Avant Educational Programs.
consultation program. Autism, 11(3), 205–224. https:// Wilbarger, J., & Wilbarger, P. (2002). Alternative and
doi.org/10.1177/1362361307076842 complementary programs for intervention. In A. C.
Spitzer, S., Roley, S. S., Clark, F., & Parham, D. (1996). Bundy, S. J. Lane, & E. A. Murray (Eds.), Sensory
Sensory integration: Current trends in the United integration: Theory and practice (pp. 333–370).
States. Scandinavian Journal of Occupational Philadelphia, PA: F A Davis.
Therapy, 3, 123–138. Wong, C., Odom, S.L., Hume, K.A., Cox, A.W., Fettig,
Tarbox, J., Dixon, D. R., Sturmey, P., & Matson, J. L. A., Kucharczyk, S., … Schultz, T.R. (2015). Evidence-­
(Eds.). (2014). Handbook of early intervention for based practices for children, youth, and young
autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY: Springer adults with autism spectrum disorder: A comprehen-
New York. sive review. Journal of Autism and Developmental
Wagner, A. L., Wallace, K., & Rogers, S. (2014). Disorders, 45(7), 1951–1966. https://doi.org/10.1007/
Developmental approaches to treatment of young s10803-014-2351-z
children with autism spectrum disorder. In J. Tarbox, Worley, J. A., Fodstad, J. C., & Neal, D. (2014).
D. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & J. L. Matson (Eds.), Controversial treatments for autism spectrum disor-
Handbook of early intervention for autism spectrum ders. In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, P. Sturmey, & J. L.
disorders. New York, NY: Springer. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early intervention for
Wan Yunus, F., Liu, K. P. Y., Bissett, M., & Penkala, S. autism spectrum disorders (pp. 487–509). New York,
(2015). Sensory-based intervention for children with NY: Springer.
Socialization Programs for Adults
with Autism Spectrum Disorder 20
Saray Bonete and Clara Molinero

During the last decades, there has been increasing teenagers feel alone, rejected, and even attacked
recognition of the need to carry out studies to by their peers. Furthermore, given their optimum
ascertain the effectiveness and efficiency of dif- cognitive ability, these youngsters suffer social
ferent types of intervention and programs aimed difficulties and become a risk group for develop-
at training or improving pragmatics, social com- ing low self-esteem, anxiety disorders, and/or
munication, and interpersonal skills – the main depression (Barnhill, 2007; Ghaziuddin,
areas affected in ASD individuals. As scientific Weidmar-Mikhail, & Ghaziuddin, 1998). As they
knowledge regarding the provision of interven- grow into adulthood, social ability deficiencies
tion at first infancy has grown in quality and could give rise to a barrier that diminishes their
quantity over the last decades, the need to focus job prospects (Krasny et al., 2003). Adults with
on adolescents and adults has become more ASD are likely to suffer misemployment or feel-
urgent (Mazefsky & White, 2013). Social deficits ings of dissatisfaction with social relations
in adults with ASD may lead to problems in (García-Villamisar & Hughes, 2007; Szatmari,
important areas of quality of life such as family Bartolucci, Bremmer, Bond & Rich, 1989;
relations (Krasny, Williams, Provencal, & Venter, Lord, & Schopler, 1992). Faced with this
Ozonoff, 2003), friendship (Jobe & White, 2007), background, it is logical that during the last
romantic relationships, and vocational success decade research efforts have been made which
(García-Villamisar & Hughes, 2007). These dif- focus on feasibility and effectiveness of social
ficulties extend over time, and significant skills training programs in order to prevent or at
improvements cannot be made by the mere pas- least mitigate the negative consequences of this
sage of time (Howlin, 2000). When they reach social dysfunction (Reichow & Volkmar, 2010).
adolescence, social interests usually increase, but The literature on social skills is extensive,
deficiencies persist and make proper and normal although there are different approaches and
relationships more difficult (White, 2012). backgrounds (psychometric perspective, clini-
Moreover, it is the time when adolescent con- cal psychology, educational area, cognitive
sciousness develops. During this stage in life in psychology, etc.). Gresham, Sugai, and Horner
which fitting in is of utmost importance, these (2001) grouped the social skills interventions
into deficits in acquisition, performance, and
fluency of social skills. The first type of train-
ing addresses acquisition deficits, giving rise to
S. Bonete (*) • C. Molinero
Universidad Francisco de Vitoria, Madrid, Spain explicit unspoken rules and meanings in social
e-mail: s.bonete.prof@ufv.es situations (e.g., through social stories). The

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 343


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_20
344 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

second, appropriate performance training, according to the theoretical orientation,1 either


focuses on problem-­solving and positive rein- because it was explicitly stated by authors or
forcement. Finally, training that tries to increase extracted from the way they approached the con-
the fluency of appropriate social behavior uses cept of social skills and what each intervention
exposure and practice tasks (e.g., repeating claimed to train (Bonete, Calero, & Fernández-­
social scripts applicable for different interac- Parra, 2015).
tions). White, Koenig, and Scahill (2007) dif- The core of the chapter organization is based
ferentiated between group-based social skills on our proposal of three main approaches to the
training and individual programs. Following construct of social/interpersonal skills according
the same structure, Rao, Beidel, and Murray to what authors believe is the essence and nature
(2008) reviewed programs classifying them of social skills. Thus, depending on each social
according to traditional social skills training skills definition, programs will have specific
and social skills plus generalization skills train- goals to guide the training. To this end, they will
ing. Therefore, the emphasis is on the strategies choose either a group or individual format and
and contexts. Other review articles (McMahon, different psychological strategies (instructional
Lerner, & Britton, 2013; Palmen, Didden, & format or didactic lessons, social performance
Lang 2012; Reichow & Volkmar, 2010) exam- training, ABA, naturalistic interventions, etc.)
ine the empirical evidence within the frame- and techniques (role-playing, modeling, instruc-
work of a best evidence synthesis, sorting by tion, mediation, etc.). In fact, this chapter
the age category and the delivery agent of the describes manualized socialization programs as
intervention (technological, parental, non- intervention packages. We also identify the kind
parental adult delivery, peer delivery, or com- of concrete strategies used in each one of them
bined delivery). Cappadocia and Weiss (2011) (structured-learning approach, social perfor-
meanwhile focus on the different components mance training, naturalistic interventions, peer-­
included in the therapeutic package of each direct training, teaching strategy based on
training program, comparing results between parents or teachers) and the techniques (cartoon-
traditional social skills training, social skills ing, power cards, role-play, visual techniques,
training with explicit cognitive-­behavioral ori- feedback, reinforcement, etc.) which could be
entations, and social skills training groups with implemented as part of different socialization
parent-intervention components. programs and independently of the theoretical
As we can see, variables for classification are backgrounds.
multiple. The lack of consensus regarding the In this sense, for some authors, social skills
definition of social skills limits the comparison are the combination of different discrete behav-
across interventions and conclusions about mea- iors. Others would state that the social cognition
sures of changes, as it depends on the assessment approach is the key to understanding the com-
tools which in turn are a function of the defined plexity of situations and how thoughts play a fun-
constructs. This chapter is an attempt to compile damental role in the interpretation of reality.
the empirical evidence of social skills interven- There is a third group of professionals that defend
tions, synthetizing the remarkable research con- the interpersonal skills concept and focus on
ducted in socialization programs for adolescents interpersonal problem-solving process for train-
and adults. We collected most of the variables ing (for a review, see Bonete & Molinero, 2016).
that were addressed in recent reviews and meta-­
analysis, such as background theory and target
skills, intervention delivery agent, evidence-­
1 
based criteria, participants description, interven- Vocational skills or leisure programs, although somehow
play a role in social skills proficiency, are addressed in
tion type and density, settings, and assessment Chap. 17 of this manual and therefore are not the interest
type. In this case, we first tried to differentiate of this chapter.
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 345

The three approaches mentioned highlight According to Reichow and Volkmar, the six basic
different aspects of the nature of social compe- criteria are (1) description of inclusion/exclusion
tence. They reflect three levels of understanding criteria to ensure homogenous sample and ade-
that should at least be considered when looking quate diagnosis; (2) intervention focus on
for outcomes in social competence, as it is such a improving one or more social skills of ASD indi-
complex and wide construct. Moreover, the three viduals; (3) inclusion of at least one social out-
of them could be combined in order to address come of the participants; (4) the study presents an
different needs throughout social development adequate research design such as a randomized
according to the participants’ level and age or clinical trial, quasi-experimental multiple-group
even simultaneously when needed. comparison, or single-subject experimental
In this chapter, we review the scientific design; (5) published results at peer-refereed
research published during the last decade accord- journals available in English; and (6) the study
ing to these three identified perspectives, address- addresses important variables to be qualified as a
ing the issue of complex socialization skills study with strong methodological rigor ratings
interventions for adolescents and adults with (proper study design, some kind of generalization
ASD that can help them lead a happy, normal life. or maintenance of changes assessment, and some
We hope the reader might find a guideline which of the defined procedural fidelity criteria).
supports his own idea of social skill intervention. Criteria one to three and five are fulfilled in most
With this overview of different treatment studies, of these training programs. The fourth and sixth
our aim is to offer a source from which one may criteria are not always achieved, but some studies
be able to choose the best intervention for a given were included when considering that subsequent
patient, according to their needs and the available studies could be developed to confirm the find-
setting. ings, testing efficacy and effectiveness. The full
This chapter was based on the last published description of fundamental criteria for each pro-
studies, reviews, and meta-analysis (Cappadocia gram is summarized in Tables 20.1, 20.2, and 20.3.
& Weiss, 2011; Hotton & Coles, 2016; Rao
et al., 2008; White et al., 2007); most treatment
manuals were selected for their methodological Social Skills Training Programs
rigor, although they cover different steps of the
four phases described by a NIMH working group As mentioned above, the following classification
concerning the development, testing, and dis- is a focus on the theoretical approach to the social
semination of psychosocial interventions (Smith skills construct. At the same time, important
et al., 2007): (1) formulation and systematic evidence-­based variables must be considered (for
application of a new intervention technique, (2) a complete synthesis, see Reichow & Volkmar,
manualization and protocol development, (3) 2010). To clarify the exposition, each of the
efficacy studies, and (4) community effective- dimensions that guide the presentation of the dif-
ness studies. Some others were chosen due to ferent programs presented across the chapter2 is
their original content, despite being in an initial hereby described:
stage of research. The aim of including these
programs’ description is that new research • Intervention type (IT): Concerning the tech-
groups may subsequently take over where these niques and methods used by the practitioner
studies have left off and do so in an increasingly during the intervention sessions. Reichow and
rigorous manner. Volkmar define eight categories: ABA, natu-
The programs represented in this chapter meet ralistic interventions, parent training, peer
some of the indicators for evidence-based prac- training (and mediation), social skills groups,
tice and well-established treatments in adults
(Reichow & Volkmar, 2010) and were all chosen 2 
Initials and acronyms were specified to explain tables’
because they contained manualized programs. information.
Table 20.1  Summary of social skills interventions focus on discrete behaviors training
Intervention type/ Type of
program Research assessment/
description and Density and Target Age, gender, design G/M outcome
Ref delivery agent Aim/target skills setting sample n and IQ fidelity measures Effects evidence Extras
Mesibov Individual To achieve positive – Adolescents 14–35 years UT Self-report Positive Individual
(1984) psychotherapy plus peer-related social and adults measures (S) changes, no sessions before
60-min group experiences with autism quantitative data each session
sessions diagnosis available
NPA 12 weeks n = 15 No M/G Videotaped
Clinical No F role-play (O)
Howlin and Non-­manualized Understanding 150 min ASD adults 19–44 years UT Therapist Enhancement of Role-play, group
Yates program social difficulties 1×/month 10 adults old No M/G appreciation understanding activities, and
(1999) and improving and social skills structured games
1 year No F
conversational skills were included
based on recent
personal events

Barnhill Teaching your Nonverbal skills in 60 min + Learning 12–17 years UT Child report Five of the seven Activity within
et al. child language of different contexts activity disabilities participants community
(2002) social success within (ASD, improved scores. (2–3 h) after
(Duke et al., 1996) community PDD-NOS, Small changes each session
AHF)
NPA (2–3 h) n = 8 7M, 1F No M/G Good parent
1×/week IQ NR No F satisfaction
8 weeks
Clinical
Webb SCORE Skills Five specific skills: 60 min Children and 12–17 years UT Paper-pencil Positive changes
Miller, Strategy (Vernon share ideas, adolescents test for for 4 of the 5
Pierce, et al., 1996) compliments, offer participants skills
Strawser, commercialized help, recommend
and Jones package with changes,
(2004) guidebook and self-control
videotape. School
settings or
community
environments
Role-playing and Systematic 2×/week N = 10 10 M No M/G SSRS parent No
games instructional report improvements
NPA sequences 10 weeks 74–126 VIQ No F according to
parent report
School
settings
Golan and Mind Reading Emotions and 2 h Study 1: All ages Multi-group Close related Greater
Baron- Program (Baron- mental states comparison measures and improvement in
Cohen Cohen, Golan, generalization intervention
(2006) Wheelwright, & measures group in close
Hill, 2004): related measures
computer-based only
training program
with short video
clips, voice
recording, and
written examples
Technological 1×/week AS and HFA Study 1: No M Study 2:
delivery 10–15 weeks Participants 17–50 years G in final Greater
assessment improvement
Home n = 19 ASD 14 M, 5F No F than CG in close
related measures
Study 2: n = 22 ASD 80–138 VIQ
only
CG
2 h software n = 24 CG Study 2:
+ support
sessions
1×/week Study 2: 17–50 years
10 weeks n = 13 ASD 12 M, 1F
used software
plus
1×/week Group session 76–128 VIQ
Home (and n = 13 social 10 M, 3F
clinic) skills group
n = 13 CG 10 M, 3F
(continued)
Table 20.1 (continued)
Intervention type/ Type of
program Research assessment/
description and Density and Target Age, gender, design G/M outcome
Ref delivery agent Aim/target skills setting sample n and IQ fidelity measures Effects evidence Extras
Tse, Skillstreaming Behavioral 1 + 1/2 h ASD and 13–18 years UT Self-report Significant Individualization
Strulovitch, (Goldstein & problems, not only typical (quasi- and parent improvement
Tagalakis, McGinnis, 2000): ASD samples children and experimental report after treatment
Meng, and modeling, adolescents pre-post no measures
Fombonne role-play, group CG)
(2007) performance, and
transference of
training
NPA Behaviors taught: 1×/week n = 46 ASD 28 M, 18F No M/G
eye contact, 12 weeks (grouped in IQ NR No F
nonverbal 7–8
Clinic
communication, participants)
politeness, etc.
Kandalaft, Virtual Reality Enhancement of 1 h High- 18–26 years Quasi- S, D: Improvement in Virtual reality
Didehbani, Social Cognition social cognition, functioning experimental ToM measures scenarios
Krawczyk, Training social skills and autism adults pre-post and emotion
Allen, and social functioning recognition
Chapman NPA; TD 2×/week n = 8 6 M, 2F UT ACS-SP, Also real-life
(2013) WASI, Eyes social and
5 weeks M age = 21.25 No M/G
and Triangles occupational
M No F
(ToM), Ekman functioning
IQ = 111.88
60F, SSPA
Laugeson, UCLA PEERS Verbal and 1 + 1/2 h ASD 18–24 years RCT Self-report Significant ???
Gantman, adults program: nonverbal social adolescents old and their measures improvement in
Kapp, Program for the skills: conversation, and young parents TASSK self-report and
Orenski, Education and humor, managing adults (Laugeson & parent-report
and Enrichment of embarrassment, Frankel, 2006) measures
Ellingsen Relational Skills dating, dealing with
(2015) rejection, etc.
In community 1×/week n = 12 ASD 9 M, 3F M at 16 week Parent-report
settings (divided in follow-up measure SSRS
groups) (Gresham &
Elliot, 1990)
14–16 weekly N = 10 IQ No G And others
(wait-list M = 107,44
group) 7 M, 3F F in every
IQ session and
M = 102,13 feedback
Morgan, Interview skills Social-pragmatic 1 + 1/2 h Young adults 18–36 years Matched Observation in Improvement of
Leatzow, curriculum (ISC): skills for job with ASD old RCT posttreatment interview skills
Clark, and manualized interviews mock job
Siller group-delivered interview,
(2014) program VABS-II and
NPA 1×/week n = 12 27 M, 1F M at 12 week PHQ-9
randomly follow-up
assigned to
treatment
12 weeks n = 15 or IQ M = 103 No G
wait-list
group
Vernon START Program Discrete social 2 h ASD IQ M = 103 F monthly Self-report General positive Weekly
et al. skills adolescents and parent outcomes (but individualized
(2016) Clinical setting 1×/week n = 6 13–16 years UT, clinical report on heterogeneous homework
old case series SSIS-RS, results between assigned by
and multiple SRS-2, and participants) social facilitators
baseline live
conversational
NPA 20 weeks 3 M, 3F No M/G
measures
81–127 IQ F in every
session
Delivery agent: NPA no parent assisted (research/clinician)
Social outcome measures: P parent report, S self-report questionnaire, C clinician rating, T teacher rating, B blinded rater, O behavioral or observational, D direct assessment
Setting: C clinic or university, S school, O other or unspecified
Extras: individualization (I), parent involvement (PI), homework (H), typical peers (TP)
? unspecified, NR not reported, UT uncontrolled trial
Table 20.2  Summary of social skills interventions focus on cognitive-behavioral training
Intervention type/ Research Type of
program description Aim/target Density and Target Age, gender, design G/M assessment/
Ref delivery agent skills setting sample n and IQ fidelity outcome measures Effects evidence Extras
Provencal Two phases: first Trains specific 75 min ASD 12–16 years Quasi-­ S and P Large symptoms Parents
(2003) sessions with adult social skills adolescents experimental reduction and received
tutor, second combined with pre-post: social skills training on
sessions with peers. CBT treatment improvement parenting
At the same time, group anxiety
parents were trained compared to
in groups not-treatment
group
1×/week n = 10 M, F No G/M ADI-R No gains in
treatment understanding
group friendship or
emotion
recognition
32 weeks n = 10 ASD IQ No F ADOS Effect size
randomized reported
non-­treatment
group
Lerner, SDARI Program Teaches 5 h ASD youth 11–17 years Quasi-­ S and P Social problems Motivators
Mikami, (Lerner & Levine, social- experimental decreased like video
and Levine 2007), sociodramatic pragmatic skills pre-post: games and
(2011) affective-relational training group physical
intervention. uses compared to activities
dramatic techniques: matched
affectively engaging non-­
acting, interaction intervention
with children and group
staff, video games,
and physical
activities
NPA 5×/week n = 9 ASD M age = 14,31 G/M: EDI, CBCL, SRS, In vivo
intervention assessment SSRS, BDI-Y, practices,
group periodically, DANVA-2 satisfaction
every 3 weeks survey
5 times
6 weeks n = 8 8 M, 1 F No F
(summer non-­ M age = 14,32
program) intervention
6 M, 2F
group
(matched in Regular IQ
age and
diagnosis)
White MASSI program: Social 60–70 min ASD 12–17 years Experimental S, P, C No significant Weekly
et al. individual’s competence: adolescents pre-post differences homework
(2013) thoughts, feelings, management of with anxiety between groups
and actions and their anxiety and disorder before treatment
interactions social skills 1×/week n = 15 M age = 14 years No G/M SRS, PARS, CASI- 16% Concurrent
treatment improvement of parent
group social impairment training
20 weeks n = 15 11 M, 4F F: after each Anx, DDCGAS 26% anxiety Satisfaction
randomized session symptoms and
wait-list decreased involvement
group M verbal IQ = 100 Effect size assessment
M age = 15 years reported
12 M, 3F
M verbal IQ = 94
Eack et al. CET program: Social and 1 h ASD adults 18–45 years UT: quasi-­ S, O, C Enhancement of Homework
(2013) Enhancement nonsocial-­ experimental cognitive deficits
Therapy with high cognitive pre-post and social
structured sessions, impairment behavior
cognitive exercises
about specific
behaviors, and
rehearsed social
situations
NPA 1×/week n = 14 M age = 25.29 years No M/G CSQ-8, Large effect size Satisfaction
80 weeks treatment M IQ = 117.70 No F MATRICS, reported and
group CSSCE Interview adherence
60-h
assessment
computer-­
based training
+45 group
sessions
(continued)
Table 20.2 (continued)
Intervention type/ Research Type of
program description Aim/target Density and Target Age, gender, design G/M assessment/
Ref delivery agent skills setting sample n and IQ fidelity outcome measures Effects evidence Extras
De Bruin MYmind: Mindfulness to 90 min ASD 11–23 years old UT pre-post Self-report and Improvement in Weekly
et al. mindfulness training increase social adolescents parent-report aim variables at homework
(2015)  Clinical setting responsiveness, 1×/week n = 23 M age = 15,8 M: 9 weeks measures post-test and Concurrent
communication, adolescents in follow-up follow-up parent
social training training
9 sessions 17 M, 6F No F
cognition, and group group
social No M IQ
motivation,
among other
skills
Chung, Online game for Social 1 h ASD 13–18 years Quasi S: CARS, SCQ, Grater Homework
Han, Shin, social cognition communication, adolescents -experimental fMRI improvement
and emotion pre-post when CBT+
Renshaw  TD recognition 3×/week n = 10 CBT + 8 M, 2F No M/G game
(2016) (words and game group
faces)
6 weeks n = 10 CBT M age = 15.8 No F
group M IQ = 80.0
9 M, 1F
M age = 16.3
M IQ = 80.4
Olsson, KONTAKT: CBT and social Brief ASD children 8–17 years UT quasi-­ S, P, C: Pre-post Parents
Rautio, manualized ongoing cognition to intervention: and experimental DDCGAS, OSU- improvement in sessions
Asztalos, program for social enhance social 1 h adolescents pre-post functional skills
Stoetzer, skills skills training group and less clinical
and Bölte 1×/week n = 22 M age = 12.3 No M/G Aut-CGIS, SRS, symptomatology.
(2016) training SSGT interview No
12 weeks 12 M,8F No F
group communicational
Long IQ >70 skills
intervention: improvement
1 h
1×/week
24 weeks
Hillier, The Aspirations Social and 1 h ASD 18–30 years Pre-post O and S: IPR, AQ, Improvement in Feedback
Fish, program vocational skills adolescents EQ, EQ and number meetings
Cloppert, and young of social with parents,
and interaction, participants,
Beversdorf and staff
(2007)  NPA 1×/week n = 13 M age = 19 No M/G Self-esteem and Satisfaction
8 weeks M IQ = 108.08 No F positive attitude questionnaire
toward others
11 M, 2F
increase; anxiety
and ASD
symptoms
decreased
Delivery agent: NPA no parent assisted (research/clinician), TD technological delivery
Social outcome measures: P parent report, S self-report questionnaire, C clinician rating, T teacher rating, B blinded rater, O behavioral or observational, D direct assessment
Setting: C clinic or university, S school, O other or unspecified
Extras: individualization (I), parent involvement (PI), homework (H), typical peers (TP)
? unspecified, NR not reported, UT uncontrolled trial
Table 20.3  Summary of social skills intervention focus on interpersonal skills and social problem-solving process
Density Research
Intervention type/ and design G/M Outcome
Ref program description Aim/target skills setting Target sample n Age, gender, and IQ fidelity measures Effects evidence Extras
Bauminger Socio-­emotional Three levels of 60 min Children and 8–17 years old UT pre-post Observational Progress in the Peer meeting
(2002) intervention program intervention: adolescents assessment of three levels of 2×/week
 T (in class) and discrete social 3×/week n = 15 11 M, 4F No M/G the interactions, intervention with Parent
peers participate skills, social problem-­solving parents, teachers, involvement
7 months IQ M = 81,36 F 2×/month
cognition and reality and emotion and peers
author’s
interpretation, and understanding
supervision
socio-interpersonal tests, and
problem-solving teachers rating
for social skills
Turner-­ SCIT-A: Cognition Emotion 50 min ASD adults 18–55 years old Quasi-­ Short SCIT: significant
Brown, and Interaction recognition, ToM, experimental questionnaire improvement in
Perry, Training attribution, and design about SCI-A, ToM and
Dichter, social interaction FEIT for communication
Bodfish, skills emotion skills
and Penn Three phases (six 1×/week n = 11 M age = 42,5 No M/G recognition, TAU: no change
(2008) sessions): social cues hinting task for
awareness, socially ToM skills and
relevant facts, and other measures
integration of social
functioning
 Clinical setting 18 weeks n = 6 SCIT 5 M, 1F No F
group
n = 5 TAU M IQ = 113,3
(treatment as M age = 28,8
usual)
5 M
M IQ = 110,6
Liu et al. Workplace training Socialization, 6 h ×/day Adults with 18–24 years old UT pre-post Work Significant
(2013) program communication, and ASD and Personality improvements in
emotion. intellectual Profile, Scales some workplace
Psychoeducation disability of Independent social behavior,
about workplace Behavior-R, and social
principles Observational communication
Emotional skills, and
Inventory-R emotional control
 Occupational 5×/week n = 14 M age = 24,60 years No M
therapist and old
assistants
 Community 6 months 10 M, 4F No F
settings NV IQ = 72,52
Pugliese Problem-­solving Promoting effective 90 min ASD college 18–23 years old Pre-post Social Improvement in Summarizing
and White therapy for ASD problem-­solving students single-subject Problem-­ SPSI-R:L and sheets after
(2014) people skills design Solving OQ only in two each session
 Clinical setting 1×/week n = 5 M age = 21,27 M: 8 weeks Inventory-­ participants Weekly
follow-up/No Revised: Long homework
G Form (SPSI-
R:L), outcome
9 5 M F after each questionnaire
sessions IQ 111–136 session
Bonete SCI-laboral: Training thinking 75 min ASD adults 16–29 years old Quasi-­ Evaluation for Significant Weekly
et al. interpersonal abilities to be experimental the Solutions to improvement in homework
(2015) problem-­solving for applied in different design Interpersonal social problem-­ task in social
workplace adaptation. interpersonal Conflicts solving task and problem-­
Sequential training problem-solving (ESCI), parent-report solving
following ten processes socialization process
particular problem-­ scale and
solving phases
 Community setting 1×/week n = 50 43 M, 7F M: 12 weeks Vineland
SCI-labor follow-up Socialization
group (ASD (general Scale (VABS)
participants) questionnaire) and Osnabrueck
10 weeks n = 50 Control M IQ = 96,26 No G Ability to Work
group (NT Profile (O-AFP)
M NV-IQ = 47,96 No F
participants
M age = 19,54
without
training) 43 M, 7F
M NV-IQ = 51,62
Delivery agent: NPA no parent assisted (research/clinician)
Social outcome measures: P parent report, S self-report questionnaire, C clinician rating, T teacher rating, B blinded rater, O behavioral or observational, D direct assessment
Setting: C clinic or university, S school, O other or unspecified
Extras: individualization (I), parent involvement (PI), homework (H), typical peers (TP)
? unspecified, NR not reported, UT uncontrolled trial
356 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

visual techniques (including video modeling), collected under any other category but never-
and others. It also includes distinguishing theless enriched the intervention. An example
between a structured learning approach, would be individualization, parent involve-
social performance training, and social skills ment, homework, and typical peers in the
support group. group intervention.
• Evidence-based criteria (EBC):Based on the
Evaluative Method for Determining Evidence-­ We tried to picture a full scheme of the differ-
Based Practices in Autism (Reichow, Volkmar, ent kinds of programs professionals can find
& Cicchetti, 2008), whereby four categories are addressing social difficulties in adolescence and
described: overall experimental rigor rating, adulthood. Therefore, range of age is around
study design, inclusion of generalization/main- 13 years old to adulthood. As an exception, we
tenance assessments (G/M), and procedural included some studies which considered earlier
fidelity. Procedural fidelity was described in ages when the mean age of the sample was dur-
detail as fidelity of treatment adherence, treat- ing the adolescence stage, and we believe that the
ment differentiation, and therapist competence. content could be implemented for older subjects.
• Research design (RD): Distinguishing In some cases, tables summarized information of
between a randomized clinical trial (RCT), the relevant studies that were historically impor-
quasi-experimental multiple-group compari- tant (because of their novelty, genuine contribu-
son (Q-E), or single-subject experimental tion, etc., e.g., Mesibov, 1984; Howlin & Yates,
designs (SSED). In this chapter few studies 1999; Webb et al., 2004; Provencal, 2003; Olsson
with single-subject experimental designs were et al., 2016; Bauminger, 2002 and Liu et al.,
included (calculating Reliable Change Index 2013) although they could not be widely
with very small samples, Jacobs & Truax, described in the text as they are not the most rep-
1991), but single-case interventions were not resentative of the specific category (discrete
presented (for a review in this matter, Wang, social skills, cognitive-behavioral approach, or
Parrila, & Cui, 2013) interpersonal skills), they do not fulfill any of the
• Delivery agent of intervention (DAI): Adult current evidence-based standards, or their mean
mediated (parent, P; no parent assisted, NPA), age was mainly pubescent.
peer mediated, a combination of both of them,
and technological delivery.
• Description of sample (DS): Sample size,  nderstanding Social Skills
U
group of age, gender, and IQ. as Behaviors
• Type of assessment: Parent report, child
report, clinician or staff-report questionnaire, A great number of authors present social skills as
social-cognitive assessment, and behavioral specific behaviors that need to be incorporated in
observation. anyone’s repertoire. From this perspective, the
• Intervention density (ID): Concerning a quan- assertive behaviors are considered the most skill-
tification of the amount of direct services pro- ful. Social skills are conceptualized as learned
vided during the intervention. Reichow and skills, with each specific situation determining
Volkmar (2010) specified the quantification of the behaviors deemed adequate. Therefore,
the intervention density: (a) session duration training is focused on increasing positive behav-
(min), (b) number of sessions per week, and iors in social situations (Caballo, 1993). Multiple
(c) total length of the intervention (in weeks). social responses must be practiced. Different
• Settings of implementation (S): Home, clini- programs choose different topics of instruction
cal, school, or community setting. such as conversational skills, verbal and nonver-
• Extras: Following Miller, Vernon, Wu, & bal cues, making friends, paying and receiving
Russo (2014), different extra information was compliments, appropriate use of humor, han-
described, when relevant information was not dling teasing and bullying, handling rumors,
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 357

practicing interviewing, asking and giving help, Systematically organized emotions and
and apologizing. If the training of specific behav- mental states (according to the emotions groups
iors is understood as key, a very wide social and developmental levels) were the content of
skills curriculum will inevitably be the result. this software. The program included short video
From this approach, social competence is clips, films of faces, voice recordings, and writ-
understood as the putting into place of the ten examples of situations that evoke each emo-
acquired skills. A successful implementation tion. To facilitate generalization, the face videos
would be followed by social recognition, occupa- and voice recordings comprised actors of both
tional achievements, etc. Emphasis is given to genders, various ages, and ethnicities. It was
what others actually observe. Socially skilled thought that such a complete guide would be
people are the ones who maximize their profits. useful for exploiting the ASD good systemizing
The skill is measured by what is observed, and, skills.
consequently, the training works on explaining Golan and Baron-Cohen (2006) examined
the skill, practicing it, and increasing the fre- improvements in recognizing complex emotions
quency of each skill through different contexts. A in faces and voices. They compared a group of 19
selection of studies which follow this trend are ASD participants that used Mind Reading at
summarized in Table 20.1.3 home alone (intervention group) to a control
Historically, it is important to highlight the group of 24 ASD matched participants that were
pioneer work in social skills intervention made assessed twice with no intervention in between
by Mesibov (1984). Although most of the rigor and a unique assessment of a typical control
ratings applied nowadays were not included, it group of 24 people with no psychiatric history.
was the first research reporting qualitative data of Between 10 and 15 weeks was the length of time
improvements in a sample of 15 adolescents and between assessments. At the end of the program,
adults with ASD (14–35 years). He combined the intervention group improved significantly
30-min individual psychotherapy sessions fol- more than the control groups based on the
lowed by a 60-min group session for 12 weeks. Cambridge Mindreading (CAM) Face-Voice
Positive results were found through self-report Battery (Golan, Baron-Cohen, & Hill, 2006) and
measures and videotaped role-plays. two closely related measures created by the same
research group (Reading the Mind in the Eyes
task and Reading the Mind in the Voice task) but
Mind Reading4 did not show better performance than the ASD
control group when a test for holistic distant gen-
Baron-Cohen et al. (2004) developed this mate- eralization was implemented (the Reading the
rial as a systematic guide for all ages to teach Mind in the Film). Therefore, improvement fol-
emotions and mental states specifically; there- lowing the intervention was limited to close gen-
fore, it could be considered a part of the discrete eralization tasks.
social skills training approach. It consists of a In a second experiment, a comparison was
computer-based intervention that uses drawings made among a group of ASD/HFA participants
or photographs for teaching emotion recognition who used the Mind Reading approach alone and
focused on basic emotions and using only facial who were also assisted by 10 weekly sessions in
expressions (Golan & Baron-Cohen, 2006). small groups with a tutor (n = 13) and two control
groups, adults with ASD attending social skills
training (n = 13), and a typical control group of
3 
There are some additional studies which were included the general population. As in the first experiment,
in the tables that are not described in the text either
because they were frequently described in the past or the intervention group improved significantly
because they mainly addressed preadolescents. more than the ASD participants who were under-
4 
http://www.jkp.com/mindreading going alternative social skills training, but only in
358 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

the close generalization face task, and moreover feedback, and transference of training (practicing
failed to improve on the distant generalization the skills at home and in the community). It was
task. Thus, the use of systematic software train- conceived to be flexible and easy to use. Each
ing may be beneficial, although more intensive session repeated the same sequence of activities:
training may be needed to achieve generalization. check-in, review of last week’s skill, introduction
This study has received strong methodological of new skill, role-play, snack break, activity, and
rigor ratings, according to evidence-based prac- closing. In the manual, one can find a first part on
tice approach in autism (Reichow & Volkmar, implementation matters, and it includes forms
2010). Meanwhile, results should be taken with and leader and observer checklists to ensure pro-
some reserve due to an important limitation – the gram integrity.
social skills group received much fewer total Part of this program content was tested for
training sessions, which might have accounted children in different studies (Lopata, Thomeer,
for some of the improvement difference. Volker, Nida, & Lee, 2008; Lopata et al., 2010),
Looking at the results and reflecting on the but for the adolescent version, only a preliminary
difficulties in generalizing holistic materials, study was found. Tse et al. (2007) examined the
authors recommended the use of Mind Reading improvements of a social skills training group
as a first step in training trajectories; later, con- based on the skillstreaming curriculum in a sam-
text and integration of different socio-emotional ple of 46 adolescents with ASD (13–18 ages) for
cues should be incorporated into the treatment in 12 weeks. Each group was composed of seven to
order to obtain a wider view of the social eight adolescents, one trained social worker, and
situations. one trained psychologist. They combined psy-
choeducational and experiential methods (espe-
cially role-play) for teaching social skills such as
 killstreaming5 (Goldstein &
S eye contact, introducing oneself to others, aware-
McGinnis, 2000) ness and expression of feelings, nonverbal com-
munication recognition, politeness, listening,
This prosocial skills training program is available conversational skills, negotiation, dealing with
for different instructional levels (early childhood, teasing and bullying, hygiene, and dining eti-
elementary school children, and adolescents). quette. As part of a few sessions, an outdoor
Staff training materials were also developed. In activity was organized responding to the need for
concrete, Skillstreaming the Adolescent was generalization.
developed for use with youths with behavioral Pre- and post-program measures were taken
issues (aggression, immaturity, withdrawal, etc.) (self- and parent-report questionnaires). Data
and not specifically for adolescents with analysis revealed that significant improvements
ASD. The skills areas were presented in different were made in social competence and problem
levels from the easiest skills (asking a question, behaviors. The strength of this study was its large
introducing yourself, etc.) to advanced social sample size, although there was not a control
skills (asking for help, apologizing, convincing group to contrast it with, and therefore we cannot
others, etc.), skills for dealing with feelings and be sure if the improvements were due to the inter-
stress, and alternatives to aggression. The last vention itself or other factors. Taking into account
edition also included some lessons on planning that this program obtained successful outcomes
skills (related to social problem-solving skills). when it was applied to children with ASD under
The work approach included teaching model- rigorous experimental conditions (Lopata et al.,
ing, student role-playing, group performance, 2010), replication of the study under randomized
controlled trials for adolescents should have
5 
http://www.skillstreaming.com/ promising results.
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 359

 CLA PEERS Program: Program


U This program was tested following the recom-
for the Education and Enrichment mendations for evidence-based practice. It was
of Relational Skills6 first tested through pilot studies and properly
manualized. Afterward, efficacy studies were
The PEERS program (Gantman, Kapp, Orenski, carried out (Dolan et al., 2016), and it was also
& Laugeson, 2012; Laugeson & Frankel, 2010; tested in community settings (Gantman et al.,
Laugeson, Frankel, Gantman, Dillon, & Mogil, 2012).
2012) aimed at teaching specific behaviors that Turning again to adolescence, this program
facilitate positive social interactions and dimin- was delivered to 33 adolescents between 13 and
ish socially inappropriate behaviors. In this case, 17 years of age and their parents (Laugeson &
the authors understand social skills as discrete Frankel, 2010) by comparing a treatment group
specific skills (verbal and nonverbal) that are with a delayed treatment group in a randomized
learned by instruction and rehearsal. Lessons control trial. Adolescents improved in knowledge
focused on conversational skills; electronic forms of social skills rules for making and keeping
of communication, developing friendship net- friends, according to a test specifically developed
works and finding sources of friends; appropriate for the program content (Test of Adolescent
use of humor, peer entry strategies, peer exit Social Skills Knowledge, TASSK, Laugeson &
strategies, organizing get-togethers with friends, Frankel, 2006), and parents reported significant
handling teasing and embarrassing feedback, dat- changes compared with the wait-list control
ing etiquette, handling peer pressure and avoid- group on the Social Skills Rating System (SSRS;
ing exploitation, and resolving arguments with Gresham & Elliot, 1990). Looking for efficacy, a
friends (Gantman et al., 2012). A manual was second study was ran (Laugeson et al., 2012)
created for adolescents and subsequently also with a new sample of 28 adolescents with autism
adapted for young adults, in which some addi- from 12 to 17 years of age (treatment group vs.
tional treatment modules were included based on delayed group). Again, significant improvements
developmental appropriateness. It consisted of were found in the SSRSS and the Social
12–14 weekly 90 min sessions in groups of nine Responsiveness Scale (SRS, Constantino &
to ten participants. There were separate sessions Gruber, 2005) according to parents and also in
for young and caregivers and were applied in knowledge of social skills (on the TASSK-R).
community settings. Moreover, 16 weeks later, treatment gains were
The techniques used through the whole pro- maintained for the treatment group for all out-
gram were didactic lessons, role-playing, behav- come measures except one. However, teacher
ioral rehearsal exercises, performance feedback, participation was limited, and this data was far
and weekly socialization homework assignments. less conclusive.
The didactic lessons were based on a presenta- The PEERS adults program was also devel-
tion of concrete rules and steps in order to sys- oped and tested (Gantman et al., 2012; Laugeson
tematize the information collected using Socratic et al., 2015). The first randomized controlled
questioning (Gantman et al., 2012). Each session study was made with a sample of 17 adults with
began with a review of the homework assign- ASD (18–23 years of age; Gantman et al., 2012).
ment, followed by a didactic lesson (parents Using the same questionnaires, significant
received an outline of it). In each session, group improvements were found in overall social skills
leaders modeled the specific social skill, and it knowledge and awareness, social responsiveness,
was practiced through role-playing, with partici- empathy, frequency of get-together, and less self-­
pants rehearsing each new skill learned, and reported loneliness. In the most recent replication
feedback was given from tutor and peers. (Laugeson et al., 2015), the randomized con-
Homework was assigned for the next day. trolled study included 12 adults with ASD in a
treatment group and compared the outcomes with
6 
https://www.semel.ucla.edu/peers/young-adults a delayed control group (n = 10). Again, this was
360 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

based on the SSRS, SRS, the Quality of thing particularly relevant when reaching adult-
Socialization Questionnaire (QSQ, Laugeson & hood. The topics were selected to reflect parts of
Frankel, 2010), and the Empathy Quotient (EQ, an interview process in a structured format. The
Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright, 2004). Results curriculum was organized into three areas, with
showed improvements in overall social skills, four lessons designated for each one: character
frequency of social engagement, and social skills attitude and person, small talk, nonverbal com-
knowledge with a significant reduction of ASD munication and hygiene, and, finally, interview
symptoms and a maintenance in most of the mea- questions, closing interview, and follow-up.
sures at 16-weeks follow-up with gains in Strategies such as discussion, role-play, video
decreased problem behaviors and externalizing feedback, peer review, and games were imple-
behavior and improvements in self-control and mented during the treatment.
social awareness. This is in concordance with Morgan et al. (2014) tested this manual in a
Mandelberg et al. (2014) who found durability of pilot study with a randomized, controlled trial
treatment gains in a sample of adolescents with experimental design. Twenty-eight adults
ASD that had participated in the PEERS program between 18 and 36 years old participated,
from 1 to 5 years earlier. These findings are inter- although only 24 completed the study. Outcomes
preted as being modulated by the parent or care- were analyzed by a proximal outcome, based on
giver involved in the program. the mock job interview, in which sample scores
Schohl et al. (2014) found significant improve- improved. As distal outcomes, authors reported
ments in get-together, higher ratings of friend- scores in the Vinelad Adaptive Behavior Scale
ship skills, fewer reported problem behaviors, (VABS) and the depression scale Patient Health
and lower social anxiety. Their results are of spe- Questionnaire. Although changes were not statis-
cial value as it is the only study independent of tically significant, participants of the experimen-
the authors of the manual, reducing the risk of tal group increased scores on the VABS and had
investigator bias for testing this intervention pro- the same depressed symptoms as the control
gram. Yoo et al. (2014) also observed significant group based on the second one with medium- and
changes in social interaction and communication; large-size effects, while the wait-list group
specifically, they also examined the positive remained stable. The small sample size limited
changes in maternal anxiety, which was reduced, the conclusions to be made, but the results high-
and the depressive symptoms of participants. light the importance of training to enhance
What is also very important about this inter- performance.
vention is the fact that improvements were
obtained through different outcome measures,
including new observational scales such as the  he START Program (Vernon, Miller,
T
Contextual Assessment of Social Skills (Ratto, Ko, & Wu, 2016)
Turner-Brown, Rupp, Mesibov, & Penn, 2011),
which enriched its veracity and relevance due These authors proposed teaching discrete social
to the ecological characteristics (White, Scarpa, behaviors by changing the teaching methodol-
Conners, Maddox, & Bonete, 2015; Dolan ogy. They did not use didactic lessons but rather
et al., 2016). experiential learning as the core of the training.
Their intervention addressed motivational, con-
ceptual, and skills deficits by combining didactic
The Interview Skills Curriculum (ISC) methodology and an experiential treatment
approach. In this way, exposition to the complex
This manualized intervention was developed as a reality of social interactions is made easier with
3-month treatment package to work on increas- the incorporation of a trusted therapist and highly
ing social-pragmatic skills, especially those motivated peers with normal development whose
needed for a successful job interview – some- aim is to “connect” with the participants.
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 361

The START Program sessions always fol- Currently, a randomized, controlled trial is under-
lowed the same structure: First, check-in session way, increasing methodological rigor and explor-
(5 min) followed by unstructured socialization ing long-term follow-up data.
time with self-management between the partici-
pant and an assigned social facilitator (20 min).
During this time, topics were introduced by the  irtual Reality Social Cognition
V
participants without predetermined agenda, and Training
participants tracked their use of individual target
skills. A structured social activity followed Kandalaft et al. (2013) proposed a program that
(20 min), which varied each week and focused on used virtual reality in ASD adults and was sup-
team-building activities and party games. After ported by empirical evidence of its effects. The
that, an interactive social topic discussion aim of this intervention was to enhance theory of
(20 min), where participants shared their reflec- mind (ToM), emotion recognition skills, and
tions and in which social facilitators introduced social and occupational functioning. Eight indi-
the week’s social skill topic, and finally, an indi- viduals between 18 and 26 years old participated
vidual checkout session (5 min) in which partici- in a pilot study testing this program for 5 weeks,
pants rated their level of comfort, the use of their organized into two sessions per week.
identified target skills, and interactions with oth- In each session, different virtual environments
ers. The social facilitator also provided a home- were presented using Second Life software, and
work objective each week to promote additional participants had to interact according to a learning
experiential learning opportunities in their natu- objective for each session. The objectives of each
ral settings (for a complete description see session were discrete social skills (e.g., introduc-
Vernon et al., 2016). tion to others, emotion recognition, conversation),
Preliminary outcomes have only recently been and ToM skills (e.g., conveying social cues, cyni-
published, but they seem promising. We include cism) developed in different scenarios (e.g., inter-
this intervention process because of its pioneer action with a friend, job interview, blind date).
approach, although study limitations (lack of An assessment battery of social cognition,
group of comparison, small sample, etc.) should social skills, and ToM collected pre- and post-­
be addressed in further research. intervention improvement. Data analysis showed
In their research, Vernon et al. (2016) recruited significant increases on the social-cognitive mea-
a group of six adolescents (13–16 years) with sures (Ekman 60 and The Reading the Mind).
individual goals which changed every 5 weeks if Some changes were observed on the conversa-
they showed acquisition. Prior to intervention, tional skills measures although not significantly.
participants were assessed and taught to self-­ A follow-up survey was also carried out to assess
manage, encouraging them to self-manage their the long-term impact of the program, and partici-
use during each START group session. Three pants reported specific social benefits. The results
social facilitators were trained to run the group. suggested this virtual reality software offers
Social competence improvements were promise as a platform and training intervention to
obtained through self-report, parent-report mea- improve social cognition. Further research is
sures (Social Skills Improvement System Rating needed to test its efficacy and social validity.
Scales (SSIS-RS) and the Social Responsiveness Up to now, all studies we have looked at
Scale (SRS-2)), and live conversational measures address training in social skills using a group
for increasing social inquiries, decreasing nega- intervention format. However, there are also
tive statements, increasing verbal contributions, some successfully applied single-case interven-
and decreasing verbal contributions. General tions which fulfill evidence-based standards but
improvements were found based on repeated which are out of the scope of this chapter (Wang
measures for each individual participant. et al., 2013 for an extensive review).
362 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

 ocial Cognition Interventions


S  DARI Program, Sociodramatic
S
to Improve Social Skills Affective-Relational Intervention7

The second approach identifies social skills with Based on the formerly called drama-based social
social cognition, referring to the individual’s pragmatic intervention (Lerner & Levine, 2007),
capacity to process and interpret reality ade- this is an integrative approach for teaching social
quately, avoiding negative thoughts and inade- pragmatics using dramatic principles and tech-
quate responses. Interpretation of reality is niques. It was conceived as an intervention
taken into consideration, becoming a crucial model for small groups of teens with Asperger
part of the training (Mavroveli, Petrides, syndrome. This intervention meets the American
Sangareau, & Furnham, 2009). From this point Psychological Association’s standard for proba-
of view, cognitive-­behavioral interventions are bly efficacious treatment. This group-based
the main representative by training not only dis- manualized intervention has three core compo-
crete skills but also the interactions among emo- nents: (1) the use of affectively engaging
tion, cognition, and behaviors in context as they improvization games and dramatic training
are related to each other (Beck, 1976; Bulter, adapted for the ASD population as techniques,
Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006). In this case, (2) child-child and child-staff relationship for
the target works on the obstacles which impede reinforcement of social interactions, and (3) age-
optimal conditions to the expression of a spe- appropriate motivators such as video games and
cific social skill (e.g., personal insecurity, fear noncompetitive physical activity. It was specifi-
of negative evaluations, etc.), identifying behav- cally devised for adolescents, due to their
ior patterns and uncovering thought processes increased social awareness and motivation that
(e.g., black and white thinking) (Lerner, White, may facilitate active participation. With the
& McPartland, 2012). A skilled person manages increase in social-pragmatic skills, generaliza-
to avoid mistaken thoughts like unjustified tion may then occur more easily. Authors empha-
inferences, selective abstractions, totalitarian sized social engagement more than behavioral
thinking, errors of maximization and positive accuracy. From their point of view, emotionally
qualities minimization, or baseless generaliza- engaging activities with socially simplified com-
tion (Ballester & Gil, 2002). ponents increase successful social experiences,
Concerning the ASD population, this approach which in turn positively affect the outcomes. In
provides participants with a framework for the fact, the authors prioritized social engagement
integration between social-cognitive processes over behavioral accuracy.
and social behavior (Stichter et al., 2010). The initiative included staff training which
Although some modifications of regular CBT consisted of specific intervention activities, char-
need to be done when used with ASD children acteristics of the clinical population and behavior
and adolescents (Attwood & Scarpa, 2013), it has management strategies. Its basis was that drama
been useful to set off positive changes in social activities effectively address social-cognitive
competence (White, Ollendick, Scahill, Oswald, skills and strengthen relationships (Guli, Semrud-­
& Albano, 2009a; Lopata et al., 2008). In fact, Clikeman, Lerner, & Britton, 2013). Therefore,
most of the developed interventions by this we located it under this epigraph, as in this case
approach were primarily conceived to treat social problem-solving is not training as a step-
comorbid conditions such as depression and anx- wise sequence but a skill by itself, together with
iety in adult Asperger participants (Gaus, 2011). others. The program was made up of ten weekly
Some of these programs are described below, sessions during the school year and another pro-
highlighting those that were evidence based or gram at a summer camp, which took place 7 days
had a high-quality intervention design, although a week for 6 weeks. The manual included a daily
unfortunately, the majority of the studies lack schedule, sequence and definition of everyday
some of these aspects (Bishop-Fitzpatrick,
Minshew, & Eack, 2014). 7 
http://spotlightprogram.com/
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 363

goals, and corresponding activities (Lerner & toward peers in adolescents and adults with
Levine, 2007 for an extension). All activities for ASD. It was designed in eight 1-h weekly ses-
each session were related to the day’s goal, but sions, including pre- and post-assessment for
this was not explicitly expressed until the end of empirical support of the program: self-report
the session when adolescents were asked to measures and observation. On this occasion, 13
review when they used the goal during the ses- individuals between 18 and 30 years old with
sion. Among the topics, common adolescent ASD took part in the program. Each session con-
behavioral problems were presented and inter- sisted of sharing experiences, giving advice, and
vention strategies given. creating problem-solving strategies as a group.
Although randomized controlled trials are the Initially, for each session, a facilitator sug-
gold standard method for evaluating the efficacy gests the topic for discussion and the areas to be
of any social skills intervention, SDARI was only covered. At the end of the session, the group
tested this way in children with ASD (Lerner & reviews what has been told and what they have
Mikami, 2012). In youth, the intervention was learned. After the program, participants held
also applied (Lerner et al., 2011) comparing nine monthly meetings where they could strengthen
youths with ASD (11–17 years of age) with their friendships. These meetings facilitated gen-
matched group who did not receive intervention. eralization to real-life problems in a more
Due to its implementation in a psychotherapy set- unstructured setting, although this effect was not
ting, participants were selected through conve- assessed in the study. In addition, there was also
nience sampling. However, many of the indicators a parent support self-directed group – weekly
of quality for group evidence-based practices during the program and monthly when it ended.
research in autistic populations (Reichow et al., Hillier et al. (2007) evaluated the effects of the
2008) were present. As part of a Spotlight intervention for each participant through self-­
Summer Program, participants attended 4 h ses- report measures and structured observation at the
sions every weekday for 6 weeks in the summer. beginning and end of the program. This assess-
Each group was made up of five youths (in which ment showed that some ASD-related symptoms
only one to three were the study participants) and decreased, and attitude toward peers relations
three staff per group (who were unaware of who and empathy improved after the program,
were the study participants). Treatment fidelity although there was no control group.
was controlled. Positive results were found in Later, Hillier, Fish, Siegel, and Beversdorf
some subscales of the Social Skills Rating System (2011a) replicated the program, increasing the
(SRRS) related to social assertion, errors in iden- sample size and strengthening the assessment
tifying emotions decreased compared with those design. This time, 49 ASD participants between 18
youths who did not participate in SDARI during and 28 years old and divided into nine groups were
the same period, and these changes were main- evaluated 2–3 weeks before the program and at the
tained 6 weeks after treatment finished. Moreover, end of it. Moreover, assessment design added
social problems decreased at posttreatment based depression and anxiety measures in order to know
on the Child Behavior Checklist. if the program could help in these areas. As an evi-
dence support for the effects of the program,
results of this assessment showed a ­reduction in
 he Aspirations Programme8
T anxiety and depression symptoms, as well as some
(Hillier et al., 2007) improvement in attitudes toward peers.
At the same time, Hillier, Greher, Poto, and
The Aspirations Programme used semi-­structured Dougherty (2011b) created a variation of this
group conversations to improve self-esteem, program named SoundScape9 that made use of
reduce anxiety, and increase positive attitudes
9 
http://faculty.uml.edu/ahillier/SoundScape_Music_
8 
http://faculty.uml.edu/ahillier/aspirations.htm Program.htm
364 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

music. Again, the target was self-esteem, anxiety the adolescent learning, providing exposure
and depression symptoms, and attitudes toward exercise during the week and encouraging with
peers in ASD adolescents and adults. This time, homework tasks and activities. Parent coaching
22 participants between 13 and 29 years old were took place after each session during approxi-
divided into two groups. The SoundScape pro- mately 15 min (White et al., 2010a).
gram was also a program made up of eight weekly The program was manualized and structured.
music sessions. Each session lasted 90 min, Each session included regular practice, immedi-
where a facilitator directed musical activities ate, direct, and specific feedback on performance,
which also included technology training (e.g., positive social learning experiences, modeling
various types of music listening, playing with dif- skills, and psychoeducation about ASD and
ferent instruments, and composing music, multi- anxiety.
media production, short film creation). Finally, Program feasibility was tested using its initial
pre- and post-assessment measures (self- and version, Multi-component Integrated Treatment,
parent-report questionnaires) showed an improve- with four participants (12–14 years of age) with
ment in attitudes toward peers, self-esteem, and ASD by White et al. (2009a). In that study, thera-
anxiety symptoms. pist treatment integrity, participants’ adherence
to treatment, and consumer satisfaction were all
acceptable. Therefore, a randomized control trial
MASSI Program (White et al., 2010a) was carried out (White et al., 2013) with a sample
of 30 adolescents with ASD and a diagnostic of
A clear example of a behavioral cognitive pro- at least one anxiety disorder. The authors com-
gram for ASD population is the Multimodal pared outcomes of 15 ASD teens who partici-
Anxiety and Social Skills Intervention (MASSI pated in the MASSI program with a wait-list
program; White et al., 2010a), simultaneously group of 15 adolescents. The intervention was
targeting anxiety management and social skills. run by five licensed clinical psychologists.
It addressed the individual’s thoughts, feelings, Without any significant differences between
and actions and also the interactions among groups prior to participation in the program, 9 of
these three domains in order to change social the 15 participants from the intervention group
competence. It was a short intervention (12–16 demonstrated individual reliable change on the
sessions) based on didactic teaching. The pro- SRS, and four of them also significantly changed
gram included parent and family involvement, their anxiety scores. Mean group scores were sig-
individual therapy, and group intervention; all nificantly lower on the SRS, but change was not
of them proved useful working with adolescents statistically significant on the anxiety scores of
and adults with ASD. Individual therapy allows the intervention group. As expected, the waitlist
for the individualization of the program, accord- group did not improve in any aspect. Participant
ing to a particular need, in order to work on adherence and treatment integrity were high, and
behavioral aspects and cognitive influences. The families reported acceptable satisfaction.
program included 12 individual therapy mod-
ules with 50–75-min sessions (plus an extra ses-
sion if needed). The group therapy was designed MYmind: Mindfulness Training
as the place to provide safe opportunities to for Youngsters with ASD (de Bruin,
practice specific social skills and anxiety man- Blom, Smit, van Steensel, & Bögels,
agement strategies with peers. The manual rec- 2015)
ommended starting 3 weeks after the beginning
of the individual therapy. The package included This training is an adaptation and combination
seven 60-min group therapy sessions. Moreover, of the Mymind protocol for children with
parents were involved in the program to follow ­attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 365

(Van der Oord, Bögels, & Peijnenburg, 2012) functioning, and poor academic achievement, for
and mindfulness training for reducing anxiety example. The targets of CET were the cognitive
and depression in adults with ASD (Spek, Van skills under successful interpersonal interacting
Ham, & Nyklíček, 2012). Some modifications and problem-solving. CET was structured in 60 h
were made, such as adding a ninth session to of computer-based neurocognitive training (two
increase repetition. We include this program participants with a coach) and 45 min of group
among this intervention selection as it is a good sessions training social-cognitive abilities over
example of intervention aimed at working on the the course of 18 months.
interrelation of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors Computer-based training (an hour per week)
which results in a positive effect in social inter- was organized into three modules: attention,
actions even though this program does not train memory, and problem-solving, to improve cogni-
social skills specifically. This training focused tion, processing speed, cognitive flexibility, high-­
on enhancing attention, (bodily) awareness, and order cognitive abilities, etc. After some months,
self-control by doing mindfulness exercises in group sessions started with six to eight partici-
typical stressful situations for the ASD popula- pants in each group (1.5 h per week). In these
tion such as the social interactions. Being aimed sessions, spontaneity, others’ perspective, emo-
at individuals with ASD, sessions were highly tions management, etc., were trained. Each ses-
structured and guided to reduce insecurity. At sion was highly structured in stages: introduction
least one of the parents participated in concurrent (welcome back), homework presentation, cogni-
mindful parenting training based on the Mindful tive exercise, feedback, brief psychoeducational
Parenting book (Bögels & Restifo, 2013). lecture, and homework assignment. These exer-
In this study pre- and post-measures were cises integrated several aspects of social cogni-
taken (De Bruin et al., 2015), and adolescents tion, while the group solved everyday social
reported about their ASD core symptoms, the problems from the perspective of more than one
Mindful Attention and Awareness Scale-­ character. They performed the message in pairs
Adolescent version, and two self-reports about and worked on their different intentions and emo-
their own worries and ruminations. For their part, tions, the message that initiates the action, and
parents answered about mindfulness skills, and, how to resolve discrepancies to get a solution.
what is more interesting, they filled the Social Finally, a generalization of the learning was also
Responsiveness Scale about their children, which trained through some homework, although this
measures ASD symptoms in social contexts. effect is not assessed in the study.
Adolescents improved in social cognition and Eack et al. (2013) carried out this program in
social communication, and both parents and ado- a group of 11 adults with ASD for 18 months.
lescents improved their quality of life. Results of cognitive and behavioral assessment
This study is just a first approach to this inno- showed highly significant and large effects of
vative way to address social difficulties. A ran- treatment in neurocognition and social cognition
domized control trial or a comparative training training. Some improvements were particularly
program would be necessary to test its efficacy to impressive: processing speed, social cognition,
address social difficulties in adolescents and and social functioning. Generalization of this
adults with ASD. social cognition training was observed in voca-
tional effectiveness, interpersonal effectiveness,
and their ability to adjust to different conditions.
Cognitive Enhancement Therapy A randomized clinical trial was presented last
(CET) (Eack et al., 2013) year including functional magnetic resonance
imaging (fMRI) with positive results for change.
This program targeted social and nonsocial-­ Similar results have been found lately in other
cognitive functioning in adults with ASD that studies that included technology in cognitive
lead to employment difficulties, limited social training for ASD participants, although usually
366 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

for children or adolescents (for a revision, Wass and each of them uses a different strategy to
& Porayska-Pomsta, 2013); one of them will be address the issue. However, none of them priori-
described below. tizes on the social problem-solving process itself.
This is the pattern of thinking which we encom-
pass under the following epigraph, the approach
 nline Game for Social Cognition
O of social problem-solving training.
(Chung et al., 2016)

Chung et al. (2016), within a CBT framework, I nterpersonal Skills and Social


developed an online prosocial game to improve Problem-Solving Programs
social cognition in 15 Korean ASD adolescents
with ages between 13 and 18 years old. Although the impact of the perspective of inter-
Simultaneously, they compared them with personal skills on developing intervention pro-
another group of 15 ASD adolescents that used grams has been less than others, its reputation is
off-line CBT game. Both groups were assessed at actually rising as part of Gardner’s multiple intel-
the beginning and at the end of the program in ligence model and emotional intelligence scope
ASD symptoms, social communication, emotion (Goleman, 1996). This approach highlights the
recognition of words and faces, and fMRI scan- importance of the role that others play in an inter-
ning while carrying out word and face emotion action where ethical issues are dealt with in con-
recognition exercises. text (Gardner, 1983; Pelechano, 1984). The main
The online game program lasted 6 weeks and difference with discrete social skills is to be
was organized into three 1-h sessions per week. found in the fact that the concept of interpersonal
During these sessions, they played the online skills includes continually considering the oth-
game in a closed room. The game included chat- ers’ perspective, understanding others’ thoughts
ting, presenting virtual gifts, and imitating other and feelings, offering help, giving confidence,
players, in order to improve their avatar and etc. Therefore, this competence is expressed
become friends with other avatars. At the same through social problem-solving skills, which can
time, a trainer interacted online with the partici- also be trained. From this point of view, the
pant to introduce a CBT intervention. emphasis is on perspective taking, consequence
The off-line program consisted of 18 CBT evaluation, cause attribution, and the generation
sessions for 6 weeks, where a psychologist of action alternatives. Each domain is a funda-
trained social cognition skills (e.g., conversa- mental part of the social problem-solving pro-
tional attitudes, verbal skills, assertiveness, stress cess. All domains, different steps of a sequence
management, coping strategies). Again, in this that must be adequately completed to solve con-
case, some aspects of social problem-solving flicts, may be nourished with properly imple-
skills were presented, but the focus was not train- mented discrete skills to obtain a fluid interaction.
ing the process which allows to understand a situ- Interpersonal competence is needed when clashes
ation but rather the different socialization skills of interest appear. Spivack and Shure (1991)
involved. define social problem-solving competence as the
As the reader can appreciate, all of these pro- ability to generate multiple alternatives when a
grams (SDARI, Aspirations, MASSI, MYmind, conflict situation arises, developing a feasible
CET, etc.) aim at socialization in adolescents and plan to reach the desired outcome, anticipate
adults. In this case, discrete social behaviors are ­consequences of a behavior, and manage conse-
in the background but are not the target. All these quences and causes. After reviewing the scien-
programs share their interest in working on social tific literature in social problem-solving (Bonete
cognition from a broader perspective. In each & Molinero, 2016), we agree with D’Zurilla and
case, they focus on different aspects that affect Goldfried (1971) with their definition of different
social cognition (thoughts, behaviors, emotions), domains of social problem-solving process, and,
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 367

like Pelechano (1984), we share the opinion that the adaptation for people with autism, Turner-
passing from a domain to another follows a Brown and her colleagues changed these dimen-
sequence, going forward and back when the solu- sions for three phases, namely, “interest/disinterest
tions seems to fail. Nevertheless, we also recog- and social cues awareness,” “socially relevant/
nize the ability to solve social problems as a irrelevant facts,” and “integration.” Specific vid-
particular process with special connotations. eotapes were created to show social situations that
In the ASD population there is not a great deal were more appropriate for the social challenges of
of literature on the cognitive process of social adults with autism.
problem-solving skills (Solomon, Goodlin-Jones, Turner-Brown et al. (2008) tested the feasibil-
& Anders, 2004; Antshel et al., 2011) despite ity of this intervention comparing (n = 6 adults) a
being highly applicable in daily life interaction. normal treatment group made up of (n = 5) adults
In the following paragraphs, we look at different with ASD. Researchers measured social cogni-
programs that, from our personal point of view, tion based on emotion recognition and ToM tasks
address the issue of fluency in social interactions, and also social functioning by administering two
focusing on the training of the social problem-­ self-report questionnaires on communication and
solving process. It should be borne in mind that social skills performance. Significant improve-
some of these interventions were defined by ments were found in ToM skills and communica-
authors as cognitive-behavioral training. Both tion in the SCIT group with large effect sizes.
perspectives are not incompatible insofar as Group attendance and satisfaction with the pro-
cognitive-­behavioral techniques and contents gram were also very high.
may be implemented, but we propose this cate- This study is very relevant insofar as it prop-
gory as an independent one as these programs erly addressed the range of age after 18 years old.
focus on the different domains needed to picture The manual approach is also a clear example of
a whole social problem which needs to be this holistic perspective of training the social
resolved. problem-solving process which needs to take
place when facing social interactions. However,
the authors did not provide detailed information
 ocial Cognition and Interaction
S regarding the treatment of the control group.
Training for Autism (SCIT-A) (Turner-­ Having access to information regarding the com-
Brown et al., 2008)10 parison treatment’s component would have
enriched the results. It should also be noted that,
This group intervention was originally developed despite being a small sample, there was a wide
and validated for adults with psychotic disorders range of ages, which would mean maturation dif-
to improve social cognition, social skills, and ferences among participants as a potential mod-
community functioning. Successfully used with erator variable.
individuals with schizophrenia (Penn et al., 2005;
Combs et al., 2007), this program was adapted
for adults with autism (18 years old and older)  roup-Based Social Competence
G
based on similarities in social-cognitive function- Intervention (SCI) (Stichter et al.,
ing (Couture, Penn, & Roberts, 2006). 2010)
The program targeted emotion recognition,
theory of mind, attributions, and social interaction This intervention was created specifically to meet
skills, among others. The original SCIT program the social needs and address the complex chal-
consisted of three phases: “emotion training,” lenges of youth with ASD (Stichter et al., 2010).
“understanding situations,” and “integration.” In Based on scaffolding learning, the curriculum
provided opportunities to practice abilities in the
10 
Adaptation of the Social Cognition and Interaction participants’ natural environments in order to pro-
Training by Roberts et al. (2004). mote generalization of learning. This intervention
368 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

focused on the “core deficits” of ASD such as we did not find published studies analyzing the
theory of mind, emotion recognition, and execu- efficacy and effectiveness of the SCI-High
tive functioning. Similar to what other interven- School.
tions tried to target (Solomon et al., 2004; Webb
et al., 2004), in this case, an effort was made to
generalize learning to natural settings by the  roblem-Solving Therapy for Adults
P
introduction of a scaffolded approach. with ASD, PSS:101 (Pugliese & White,
This program worked on thinking patterns. 2014)
Strategies used were metacognitive strategies,
self-monitoring and self-regulation, exposure, Pugliese and White (2014) adapted the evidence-­
and response situations. This intervention con- based problem-solving therapy of D’Zurilla and
sisted of 20 h of group intervention conducted Nezu (2007) for adults with ASD methodology.11
twice weekly for 10 weeks with a range of four to Although their work is only a preliminary
six students per group. The curriculum provided approach to validating this therapy in ASD, we
five modules to be applied in four 1-h sessions. include this study as it is a valuable example of
The modules addressed were (1) facial expres- adjusting the original manual of evidence-based
sion, (2) sharing ideas, (3) turn taking, (4) feel- general therapy to an ASD population.
ings and emotions, and (5) problem-solving. The In order to apply it to their specific population,
first session insisted on the importance of apply- they provided a first psychoeducational session
ing discrete skills (such as using greetings, mak- on ASD and problem-solving. Subsequently,
ing appropriate eye contact, and acknowledging three sessions were focused on positive problem
the presence of others) through the whole pro- orientation in relation to problem-solving, and
gram sessions. Modules from 1 to 4 are prerequi- four sessions were focused on practicing specific
sites needed to solve social problems adequately. dimensions of the problem-solving process
In fact, the authors tried to promote acquisition of through different techniques. A final session was
necessary skill sets by giving multiple opportuni- centered on evaluating attempts to implement
ties for structured and naturalistic practice with and redefine solution plans when necessary. The
the aim to allow generalization to their natural content of the program was preserved while add-
environment. ing specific strategies from CBT and social skill
The structure was maintained in each module: interventions frequently used in ASD population.
reviewing the previous skill learned and intro- Additional examples were included, focusing on
ducing the next one, skill modeling, practicing, the new challenges associated with adaptation to
and a closing activity or review. Each new skill university life.
was built upon previous ones, and maintenance This pilot study involved a sample of five col-
of previous skills was reinforced throughout. The lege students. Improvements were assessed
program included the teaching of specific social through a questionnaire measuring the cognitive
skills on a CBT model, working on interrelations process of social problem-solving, the Problem-­
among thoughts, emotions, behaviors (examples Solving Inventory-Revised: Long Form (SPSI-­
of intervention are given in Stichter et al., 2010), R:L), and an outcome questionnaire to assess
and problem-solving. client status throughout the course of therapy.
At present, there are different versions depend- Problem feasibility was determined by analyzing
ing on age: SCI-Elementary (6–10 years of age), treatment integrity, treatment adherence, and
SCI-Adolescent (11–14 years), SCI-High School consumer satisfaction. The program seemed fea-
(teens aged 14–18), and iSocial (an interactive, sible with good scores in these three measures.
online, three-dimensional virtual learning environ- Homework completion was not homogeneous,
ment for the same program). Some of the groups
where this program was applied included a con- 11 
Based in the original work of D’Zurilla and Nezu
current parent education program. Unfortunately, (2007).
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 369

but most group members highlighted the positive actions and facing failures, and (10) reviewing
experience and the opportunity to meet others the process.
with ASD on campus. However, when outcome The program was made up of ten weekly
measures were examined through reliable change 75-min sessions for groups of four to six adoles-
indices, only two of the five participants achieved cents and adults with ASD assisted by a therapist.
significant clinical change after treatment, and Through a meditational approach and building on
the other three did not show any significant posi- different examples of daily interpersonal prob-
tive change in problem-solving at all. Further lems, the participants worked on the different
research is needed to explore the efficacy of this phases session by session. The aim was to train a
therapy, with different measures to detect thinking process to be applied to different con-
changes. It is well known that only a self-report texts in a flexible way.
questionnaire about problem-solving skills is a This meditational approach was developed
poor measure of change. We encourage research- with Feuerstein’s work on mediated learning
ers to continue working on the validation of this experience. Mediated learning has been also
program. We believe this is one of the best applied to the clinical population (Haywood,
approaches to provide ASD adults with the nec- 2000; Haywood & Lidz, 2007). It consists of
essary holistic tools. This goal probably cannot implicit teaching. Building on the participants’
be reached by training discrete skills alone or knowledge, it introduces different forms of help
only working on interactions between thoughts, to facilitate participants in furthering their knowl-
feelings, and emotions and their consequence on edge of the social problem resolution process.
social behaviors. Particularly in this program, all given examples
were focused on the workplace environment for
solving interpersonal problems due to its impor-
The Interpersonal Problem-Solving tance for adaptation during this stage of life.
Program for Workplace Adaptation; The program was manualized and its imple-
SCI-Laboral (Bonete et al., 2015) mentation structured (Calero, García-Martín, &
Bonete, 2012). Each module followed the same
The intervention called “Interpersonal Problem-­ outline: reviewing the previous day’s learned
Solving Program for workplace adaptation” step of social problem-solving and homework
[Programa de Solución de Conflictos task, a representative drawing was shown at the
Interpersonales para la adaptación laboral, SCI-­ beginning of each session from which partici-
Laboral] is based on an approach called social pants may disclose the phase they were going to
problem-solving in phases (D’Zurilla & work on each day. Subsequently, examples of
Goldfried, 1971; Pelechano, 1995) as the cogni- problematic situations starting from an imper-
tive process involved is based on resolution sonal problem, followed by a conflict of interest
phases such as defining problem, taking perspec- between two people and, finally, an interper-
tives, generating solution, and considering conse- sonal problem affecting a whole group were
quences. Through sequential training, the given. Opportunities to share personal experi-
program centered each session on one particular ences of the same or similar kind, role-playing
step of the interpersonal problem-solving pro- and feedback, and, finally, a review of the prin-
cess. Based on Pelechano (1995), phases were cipal ideas of the session were also addressed.
delimited as (1) introduction to interpersonal At the end of the session a homework task was
problem-solving skills and description of AS’s given consisting of a step-by-step resolution of
characteristics, (2) conversational skills, (3) two interpersonal problems (Bonete, Calero, &
defining a problem, (4) different points of view, Fernández-Parra, 2011). When the program was
(5) thinking of causes, (6) generating solutions, finished, participants received a portfolio with
(7) considering consequences and choosing the their homework and templates for new situa-
best option, (8) planning of action, (9) evaluating tions to come.
370 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

This version for adolescents and adults was that effectiveness is about not only empirical
studied in a sample of 50 adolescents and adults evidence but also social validity in order to
(from 16 to 32 years of age) who were trained for make the use of manualized programs more
10 weeks (Bonete et al., 2015). Participants widespread (Callahan et al., 2016).
showed positive outcomes. In general terms, they Some other difficulties we found are that most
improved in the social problem-solving task of the studies included adolescents and adults
Evaluation for the Solutions to Interpersonal with medium to high functioning. Clinical pre-­
Conflicts (ESCI) and the Vineland Socialization intervention assessment increases costs, and
Scale (VABS-S) reported by parents. Participant therefore it is at times difficult. However, the dif-
involvement was considered high based on atten- ferent functioning levels of participants indis-
dance and homework completion, and partici- criminately mixed together affect the validity of
pants and parents’ satisfaction were also recorded the results.
with clear success. In addition, as one may see, different theoreti-
Although it is a valuable study due its hetero- cal approaches lead to different intervention
geneous sample, results are limited because of designs. In our opinion, these three approaches
the methodological issues. Following evidence-­ should neither be dismissed nor overrated. The
based criteria, a randomized control trial or treat- first approach is very useful when specific behav-
ment comparison design would be of great iors need training. In particular, for ASD adults,
importance in determining efficacy. these programs (discrete behavior programs)
All these studies offer a way to train the entire may raise the frequency of adequate social behav-
social and interpersonal problem-solving pro- iors. They can be seen as easy-to-learn skills that
cess. This approach in particular tries to keep a lead to more social or job success.
broader focus for training social competence in When ASD adults have the capability to
order to facilitate interpersonal and vocational understand the cognitive substrate underlying
success. social behavior, and depending on the available
resources for intervention, the second group
approach (social cognition programs) shows the
Conclusions cognitive components that may explain social
behaviors. In addition, some of them might
As can be seen, advances have been made in explain the cognitive elements not only within
addressing interventions for social difficulties of oneself but also in others. These kinds of pro-
adults with ASD. There are different approaches grams train the cognitive-behavior dyad, facili-
and some new and creative initiatives. Moreover, tating the acquisition of social rules and social
some treatments are gradually satisfying impor- competence.
tant evidence-based practice criteria. The third approach (interpersonal skills pro-
One of the conclusions of special note is the grams) aims to go a step further by teaching about
many methodological limits and flaws in design the process underlying social interactions. The
usually found in intervention studies. Reichow, interpersonal problem-solving skills appear as the
Steiner, and Volkmar (2012) and Reichow and competence to picture the situation and under-
Volkmar (2010) highlighted this issue. They stand it. In this way, even when sometimes inter-
found that most studies did not measure either action might have some costs (taking others’
generalization or maintenance criteria. In addi- perspective, surrendering something, fatiguing
tion, many of the papers did not measure the cognitive processes or discussions, etc.), it reports
procedural fidelity of the intervention, and only many benefits (social relations, understanding
a few evaluated treatment adherence. The mea- others, sharing each other’s experiences, caring,
surement of social validation (e.g., consumer affection, friendship, etc.). Interpersonal under-
satisfaction) has gained in importance in the last standing may lead people to a point at which they
years as there is a forever increasing awareness arrive to maturity in the communal living; in other
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 371

words, people may work to solve interpersonal Barnhill, G. P. (2007). Outcomes in adults with Asperger
syndrome. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
conflicts because, even if one conflict might seem
Disabilities, 22(2), 116–126.
unconquerable, it can produce personal growth. Barnhill, G. P., Tapscott Cook, K., Tebbenkamp, K., &
Not only are there differences regarding the Smith Myles, B. (2002). The effectiveness of social
theoretical approach, but there are also different skills intervention targeting nonverbal communication
for adolescents with Asperger syndrome and related
methods in intervention leading to different
pervasive developmental delays. Focus on Autism and
results. Therefore, numerous intervention tech- Other Developmental Disabilities, 17(2), 112–118.
niques can be used when designing a program, Baron-Cohen, S., & Wheelwright, S. (2004). The empa-
either for training or for assessment. We have thy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger
syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal
mentioned some of them: video modeling, in vivo
sex differences. Journal of Autism and Develomental
techniques, technology resources (in assessments Disorders, 34(2), 163–175.
like fMRI or during intervention like virtual Baron-Cohen, S., Golan, O., Wheelwright, S., & Hill, J. J.
reality). (2004). Mind reading: The interactive guide to emo-
tions. London, UK: Jessica Kingsley Limited.
Finally, different social issues can be treated
Bauminger, N. (2002). The facilitation of social-emo-
in intervention programs. We have found that tional understanding and social interaction in high-­
most programs focused on relationships with functioning children with autism: Intervention
peers, while many others centered on job or pro- outcomes. Journal of Autims and Developmental
Disorders, 32(4), 283–298.
fessional success. However, there are a few pro-
Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional
grams aimed at love relationships and dating. disorders. Madison, CT: International University
ASD adults usually have difficulty in this domain, Press.
and it could be helpful to develop programs that Bishop-Fitzpatrick, L., Minshew, N. J., & Eack, S. M.
(2014). A systematic review of psychosocial interven-
take this issue as a primordial aim.
tions for adults with autism spectrum disorders. In
In conclusion, some advances have been made Adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disor-
in scientific evidence of social skills interven- ders (pp. 315–327). New York, NY: Springer.
tions for adults with ASD, but improvements are Bögels, S. M., & Restifo, K. (2013). Mindful parenting: A
guide for mental health practitioners. New York, NY:
still needed. We trust that the reader has found
Springer.
this chapter adequate as a brief guide on what Bonete, S., & Molinero, C. (2016). The interpersonal
manualized programs are available for adoles- problem-solving process: Assessment and interven-
cents and adults with ASD. Our wish is also to tion. In K. Newton (Ed.), Problem-solving: Strategies,
challenges and outcomes. New York, NY: Nova
inspire our peers to continue working on analyz-
Science Publishers.
ing effectivity and effectiveness for these Bonete, S., Calero, M. D., & Fernández-Parra, A. (2011).
interventions. Cuaderno de Trabajo del Programa de resolución de
problemas interpersonales para la adaptación lab-
oral de personas con síndrome de asperger. Granada,
Spain: Sider. D.L. Gr-2975-2011.
References Bonete, S., Calero, M. D., & Fernández-Parra, A. (2015).
Group training in interpersonal problem-solving skills
Antshel, K. M., Polacek, C., McMahon, M., Dygert, for workplace adaptation of adolescents and adults
K., Spenceley, L., Dygert, L., … Faisal, F. (2011). with Asperger syndrome: A preliminary study. Autism,
Comorbid ADHD and anxiety affect social skills 19(4), 409–420.
group intervention treatment efficacy in children with Bulter, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & Beck,
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Developmental A. T. (2006). The empirical status of cognitive-­
and Behavioral Pediatrics, 32(6), 439–446. behavioral therapy: A review of meta-analyses.
Attwood, T., & Scarpa, A. (2013). Modifications of CBT Clinical Psychology Review, 26(1), 17–31.
for use with children and adolescents with high func- Caballo, V. E. (1993). Manual de evaluación y entre-
tioning ASD and their common deficits. In A. Scarpa, namiento de las habilidades sociales. [Manual of
S. W. White, & T. Attwood (Eds.), Promising cogni- social skills assessment and intervention]. Madrid,
tive behavioral interventions for children and adoles- Spain: Siglo XXI de España editores. sa.
cents with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. Calero, M. D., García-Martín, M. B., & Bonete, S.
New York, NY: Guilford. (2012). Programa de entrenamiento en habilidades de
Ballester, R., & Gil, M. D. (2002). Habilidades sociales resolución de problemas interpersonales para niños.
[Social skills]. Madrid, Spain: Síntesis. [Training program in interpersonal problem a solv-
372 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

ing skills for Children]. Granada, Spain: Sider. D.L: Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theory of mul-
Gr2371-2012. tiple intelligences. New York, NY: Basics.
Callahan, K., Hughes, H. L., Mehta, S., Toussaint, Gaus, V. L. (2011). Adult Asperger syndrome and the
K. A., Nichols, S. M., Ma, P. S., ... Wang, H. T. utility of cognitive-behavioral therapy. Journal of
(2016). Social validity of evidence-based prac- Contemporary Psychotherapy, 41, 47–56.
tices and emerging interventions in autism. Focus Ghaziuddin, M., Weidmar-Mikhail, E., & Ghaziuddin, N.
on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. (1998). Comorbidity of Asperger syndrome: A prelim-
doi:10.1177/1088357616632446 inary report. Autism, 42, 279–283.
Cappadocia, M. C., & Weiss, J. A. (2011). Review of Golan, O., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2006). Systemizing empa-
social skills training groups for youth with Asperger thy: Teaching adults with Asperger syndrome and
syndrome and high functioning autism. Research in high functioning autism to recognize complex emo-
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, 70–78. tions using interactive multimedia. Development and
Chung, U., Han, D. H., Shin, Y. J., & Renshaw, P. F. Psychopathology, 18(2), 589–615.
(2016). A prosocial online game for social cogni- Golan, O., Baron-Cohen, S., & Hill, J. J. (2006). The
tion training in adolescents with high-functioning Cambridge Mindreading (CAM) face voice battery:
autism: An fMRI study. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Testing complex emotion recognition in adults with
Treatment, 12, 651–660. and without Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism
Combs, D. R., Adams, S., Penn, D., Roberts, D. L., and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 169–183.
Tiegreen, J., & Stern, P. (2007). Social cognition and Goldstein, A. P., & McGinnis, E. (2000). Skillstreaming
interaction training (SCIT) for patients with schizo- the adolescent : New strategies and perspectives for
phrenia spectrum disorders. Schizophrenia Research, teaching prosocial skills (Program Forms Booklet).
91, 112–116. ISBN: 978-0878223718.
Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2005). Social respon- Goleman, D. (1996). Inteligencia emocional. [Emotional
siveness scale (SRS). Los Angeles, CA: Western intelligence]. Barcelona, Spain: Kairos.
Psychological Services. Gresham, F. M., & Elliott, S. N. (1990). Social skills
Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., & Roberts, D. L. (2006). The rating system (SSRS). Circle Pines, MN: American
functional significance of social cognition in schizo- Guidance Service.
phrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(1), Gresham, F. M., Sugai, G., & Horner, R. H. (2001).
S44–S63. Interpreting outcomes of social skills training for stu-
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Nezu, A. M. (2007). Problem-solving dents with high-incidence disabilities. The Council for
therapy: A positive approach to clinical intervention Excepcional Children, 67, 331–344.
(3rd ed.). New York, NY: Springer. Guli, L. A., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Lerner, M. D., &
de Bruin, E. I., Blom, R., Smit, F. M., van Steensel, F. J., Britton, N. (2013). Use of creative drama as interven-
& Bögels, S. M. (2015). MYmind: Mindfulness train- tion for children with social competence deficits. The
ing for youngsters with autism spectrum disorders and Arts in Psychotherapy, 40, 37–44.
their parents. Autism, 19(8), 906–914. Haywood, H. C. (2000). Cognitive-developmental
Dolan, B. K., Van Hecke, A. M., Carson, A. M., Karst, therapy: Overview. In A. Kozulin & Y. Rand (Eds.),
J. S., Stevens, S., Schohol, K. A., … Hummel, E. Experience of mediated learning: An impact of
(2016). Brief report: Assessment of intervention Feuerstein’s theory in education and psychology
effects on in vivo peer interactions in adolescents with (pp. 292–308). Oxford, UK: Elsevier Science.
Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Journal of Autism Haywood, H. C., & Lidz, C. S. (2007). Dynamic assess-
and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2251–2259. ment in practice: Clinical and educational applica-
D’Zurilla, T. J., & Goldfried, M. R. (1971). Problem solv- tions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
ing and behavior modification. Journal of Abnormal Hillier, A., Fish, T., Cloppert, P., & Beversdorf, D. Q.
Psychology, 78, 107–126. (2007). Outcomes of a social and vocational skills
Eack, S. M., Greenwald, D. P., Hogarty, S. S., Bahorik, support group for adolescents and young adults on
A. L., Litschge, M. Y., Mazefsky, C. A., & Minshew, the autism spectrum. Focus on Autism and Other
N. J. (2013). Cognitive enhancement therapy for Developmental Disabilities, 22, 107–115. doi:10.117
adults with autism spectrum disorder: Results of an 7/10883576070220020201
18-month feasibility study. Journal of Autism and Hillier, A. J., Fish, T., Siegel, J. H., & Beversdorf,
Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2866–2877. D. Q. (2011a). Social and vocational skills training
Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K., & Laugeson, reduces self-reported anxiety and depression among
E. A. (2012). Social skills training for young adults young adults on the autism spectrum. Journal of
with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 23, 267–
randomized controlled pilot study. Journal of Autism 276. doi:10.1007/s10882-011-9226-4
and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1094–1103. Hillier, A., Greher, G., Poto, N., & Dougherty, M.
García-Villamisar, D., & Hughes, C. (2007). Supported (2011b). Positive outcomes following participation in
employment improves cognitive performance in a music intervention for adolescents and young adults
adults with autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability on the autism spectrum. Psychology of Music, 40(2),
Research, 51(2), 142–150. 201–215.
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 373

Hotton, M., & Coles, S. (2016). The effectiveness of for autism spectrum disorders. Dialogues in Clinical
social skills training groups for individuals with Neuroscience, 14(3), 307–318.
autism spectrum disorder. Review Journal of Autism Liu, K. P. Y., Wong, D., Chung, A. C. Y., Kwok, N., Lam,
and Developmental Disorders, 3(68), 68–81. M. K. Y., Yuen, C. M. C., & Kwan, A. C. S. (2013).
Howlin, P. (2000). Outcome in adult life for more able Effectiveness of a workplace training programme
individuals with autism or Asperger syndrome. in improving social, communication and emotional
Autism, 4, 63–83. skills for adults with autism and intellectual disability
Howlin, P., & Yates, P. (1999). The potential effectiveness in Hong Kong- a pilot study. Occupational Therapy
of social skills groups for adults. Autism, 3(3), 299–307. International, 20, 198–204.
Jacobson, N. S., & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: Lopata, C., Thomeer, M. L., Volker, M. A., Nida, R. E., &
a statistical approach to defining meaningful change Lee, G. K. (2008). Effectiveness of a manualized sum-
in psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and mer social treatment program for high-functioning chil-
Clinical Psychology, 59(1), 12–19. dren with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism
Jobe, L., & White, S. W. (2007). Loneliness, social rela- and Developmental Disorders, 38(5), 890–904.
tionships, and a broader autism phenotype in college Lopata, C., Thomeer, M. L., Volker, M. A., Toomey, J. A.,
students. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, Nida, R. E., Lee, G. K., … Rodgers, J. (2010). RCT
1479–1489. of a manualized social treatment for high-functioning
Kandalaft, M. R., Didehbani, N., Krawczyk, D. C., autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Allen, T. T., & Chapman, S. B. (2013). Virtual real- Develompental Disorders, 40, 1297–1310.
ity social cognition training for young adults with Mandelberg, J., Laugeson, E. A., Cunningham, T. D.,
high-­functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Ellingsen, R., Bates, S., & Frankel, F. (2014). Long-­
Developmental Disorders, 43(1), 34–44. term treatment outcomes for parent-assisted social
Krasny, L., Williams, B. J., Provencal, S., & Ozonoff, skills training for adolescents with autism spectrum
S. (2003). Social skills interventions for the autism disorders: The UCLA PEERS program. Journal of
spectrum: Essential ingredients and a model curricu- Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities,
lum. Child Adolescence Psychiatry Clinics, 12(1), 7(1), 45–73.
107–122. Mavroveli, S., Petrides, K., Sangareau, Y., & Furnham,
Laugeson, E.A., & Frankel, F. (2006). Test of adolescent A. (2009). Exploring the relationships between trait
social skills knowledge. UCLA parenting and children’s emotional intelligence and objective socio-emotional
friendship program. Los Angeles: 300 Medical Plaza. outcomes in childhood. British Journal of Educational
Laugeson, E. A., & Frankel, F. (2010). Social skills for Psychology, 79, 259–272.
teenagers with developmental and autism spectrum Mazefsky, C. A., & White, S. W. (2013). Adults with
disorders: The PEERS treatment manual. New York, autism. In S. J. Rogers (Ed.), Handbook of autism
NY: Routledge. spectrum disorders (4th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Laugeson, E. A., Frankel, F., Gantman, A., Dillon, A. R., McMahon, C. M., Lerner, M. D., & Britton, N. (2013).
& Mogil, C. (2012). Evidence-based social skills Group-based social skills interventions for adolescents
training for adolescents with autism spectrum disor- with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorders: A
ders: The UCLA PEERS program. Journal of Autism review and looking to the future. Adolescent Health,
and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1025–1036. Medicine and Therapeutics, 4, 23–38.
Laugeson, E. A., Gantman, A., Kapp, S. K., Orenski, K., Mesibov, G. B. (1984). Social skills training with verbal
& Ellingsen, R. (2015). A randomized controlled trial autistic adolescents and adults: A program model.
to improve social skills in young adults with autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14,
spectrum disorder: The UCLA PEERS program. 395–404.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Miller, A., Vernon, T., Wu, V., & Russo, K. (2014). Social
doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2504-8 skills group interventions for adolescents with autism
Lerner, M. D., & Levine, K. (2007). The spotlight method: spectrum disorders: A systematic review. Review
An integrative approach to teaching social pragmatics Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 1,
using dramatic principles. Journal of Developmental 254–265.
Processes, 2(2), 91–102. Morgan, L., Leatzow, A., Clark, S., & Siller, M. (2014).
Lerner, M. D., & Mikami, A. Y. (2012). A preliminary ran- Interview skills for adults with autism spectrum dis-
domized controlled trial of two social skills interven- order: A pilot randomized controlled trial. Journal
tions for youth with high-functioning autism spectrum of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(9),
disorders. Focus Autism and Other Developmental 2290–2300.
Disabilities, 27(3), 147–157. Olsson, N. C., Rautio, D., Asztalos, J., Stoetzer, U., &
Lerner, M. D., Mikami, A. Y., & Levine, K. (2011). Socio-­ Bölte, S. (2016). Social skills group training in high-­
dramatic affective-relational intervention for adoles- functioning autism: A qualitative responder study.
cents with Asperger syndrome and high functioning Autism, 20, 995–1010. doi:1362361315621885
autism: Pilot study. Autism, 15(1), 21–42. Palmen, A., Didden, R., & Lang, R. (2012). A systematic
Lerner, M. D., White, S. W., & McPartland, J. C. (2012). review of behavioral intervention research on adap-
Mechanisms of change in psychosocial interventions tive skill building in high-functioning young adults
374 S. Bonete and C. Molinero

with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
Spectrum Disorders, 6(2), 602–617. 37(2), 354–366.
Pelechano, V. (1984). Inteligencia social y habilidades Solomon, M., Goodlin-Jones, B. L., & Anders, T. F.
interpersonales. [Social intelligence and interpersonal (2004). A social adjustment enhancement intervention
skills]. Análisis y Modificación de Conducta, 10(26), for high functioning autism, Asperger’s syndrome, and
393–420. pervasive developmental disorder NOS. Journal of
Pelechano, V. (1995). Habilidades interpersonales: con- Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(6), 649–668.
ceptualización y entrenamiento [Interpersonal skills: Spek, A. A., Van Ham, N., & Nyklíček, I. (2012).
Conceptualization and training]. In M. D. Calero Mindfulness based therapy in adults with an autism
(Ed.), Modificación de la inteligencia: Sistemas de spectrum disorder: A randomized controlled trial.
evaluación e intervención [Intelligence modification: Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 246–253.
Systems of assessment and intervention] (pp. 131– Spivack, G., & Shure, M. (1991). Social adjustment of
179). Madrid, Spain: Pirámide. young children: A cognitive approach to solving real
Penn, D., Roberts, D. L., Munt, E. D., Silverstein, E., life problems (p. 212). Ann Arbor, MI: University
Jones, N., & Sheitman, B. (2005). A pilot study of Microfilms International.
social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for Stichter, J. P., Herzog, M. J., Visovsky, K., Schmidt, C.,
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 80, 357–359. Randolph, J., Schultz, T., & Gage, N. (2010). Social
Provencal, S. L. (2003). The efficacy of a social skills competence intervention for youth with Asperger
training program for adolescents with autism spec- syndrome and high-functioning autism: An initial
trum disorders. Unpublished thesis, University of investigation. Journal of Autim and Developmental
Utah, Utah. Disorders, 40, 1067–1079.
Pugliese, C. E., & White, S. W. (2014). Brief report: Szatmari, P., Bartolucci, G., Bremmer, R., Bond, S., &
Problem solving therapy in college students with Rich, S. (1989). A follow-up study of high-functioning
autism spectrum disorders: Feasibility and prelimi- autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental
nary efficacy. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19(2), 213–225.
Disorders, 44, 719–729. Tse, J., Strulovitch, J., Tagalakis, V., Meng, L., &
Rao, P. A., Beidel, D. C., & Murray, M. J. (2008). Social Fombonne, E. (2007). Social skills training for adoles-
skills interventions for children with Asperger’s syn- cents with Asperger syndrome and high-­functioning
drome or high-functioning autism: A review and rec- autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
ommendations. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(10), 1960–1968.
Disorders, 38(2), 353–361. Turner-Brown, L. M., Perry, T. D., Dichter, G. S., Bodfish,
Ratto, A. B., Turner-Brown, L., Rupp, B. M., Mesibov, J. W., & Penn, D. L. (2008). Brief report: Feasibility
G. B., & Penn, D. L. (2011). Development of the of social cognition and interaction training for adults
Contextual Assessment of Social Skills (CASS): with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and
A role play measure of social skill for individuals Developmental Disorders, 38, 1777–1784.
with high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Van der Oord, S., Bögels, S. M., & Peijnenburg, D. (2012).
Developmental Disorders, 41(9), 1277–1286. The effectiveness of mindfulness training for children
Reichow, B., Steiner, A. M., & Volkmar, F. (2012). Social with ADHD and mindful parenting for their parents.
skills groups for people aged 6 to 21 with autism Journal of Child and Family Studies, 21, 139–147.
spectrum disorders (ASD). Cochrane Database Venter, A., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1992). A follow-up
of Systematic Reviews. doi:10.1002/14651858. study of high-functioning autistic children. Journal
CD008511.pub2. of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied
Reichow, B., & Volkmar, F. R. (2010). Social skills Disciplines, 33(3), 489–507.
interventions for individuals with autism: Evaluation Vernon, T. W., Miller, A. R., Ko, J. A., & Wu, V. L. (2016).
for evidence-based practices within a best evi- Social tools and rules for teens (the START program):
dence synthesis framework. Journal of Autism and Program description and preliminary outcomes of an
Developmental Disorders, 40(2), 149–166. experiential socialization intervention for adolescents
Reichow, B., Volkmar, F. R., & Cicchetti, D. V. (2008). with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Development of the evaluative method for evaluating Developmental Disorders, 46, 1805–1823.
and determining evidence-based practices in autism. Wang, S. Y., Parrila, R., & Cui, Y. (2013). Meta-analysis
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, of social skills interventions of single-case research
38(7), 1311–1319. for individuals with autism spectrum disorders:
Schohl, K. A., Van Hecke, A. V., Carson, M., Dolan, Results from three-level HLM. Journal of Autism and
B., Karst, J., & Stevens, S. (2014). A replication and Developmental Disorders, 43(7), 1701–1716.
extension of the PEERS intervention: Examining Wass, S. V., & Porayska-Pomsta, K. (2013). The uses
effects on social skills and social anxiety in adoles- of cognitive training technologies in the treatment of
cents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of autism spectrum disorders. Autism. doi:10.1177/
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, 532–545. 1362361313499827.
Smith, T., Scahill, L., Dawson, G., Guthrie, D., Lord, C., Webb, B. J., Miller, S. P., Pierce, T. B., Strawser, S., &
Odom, S., … Wagner, A. (2007). Designing research Jones, W. P. (2004). Effects of social skill instruction
studies on psychosocial interventions in Autism. for high-functioning adolescents with autism spectrum
20  Socialization Programs for Adults with ASD 375

disorders. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2007). Social
Disabilities, 19(1), 53–62. skills development in children with autism spectrum
White, S. W. (2012). Growing pains: How psychologists disorders: A review of the intervention research.
can help to meet the clinical needs of clients with Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive and Behavioral 2007(37), 1858–1868.
Practice, 19(3), 433–436. Elsevier B.V. White, S. W., Ollendick, T., Albano, A. M., Oswald, D.,
White, S. W., Ollendick, T., Scahill, L., Oswald, D., Johnson, C., Southam-Gerow, M. A., & Scahill, L.
& Albano, A. M. (2009). Preliminary efficacy of a (2013). Randomized controlled trial: Multimodal
cognitive-­behavioral treatment program for anxious anxiety and social skill intervention for adolescents
youth with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and
and Developmental Disorders, 39(12), 1652–1662. Developmental Disorders, 43(2), 382–394.
White, S. W., Albano, A. M., Johnson, C. R., Kasari, White, S. W., Scarpa, A., Conners, C. M., Maddox,
C., Ollendick, T., Klin, A., … Scahill, L. (2010a). B. B., & Bonete, S. B. (2015). Evaluating change in
Development of a cognitive-behavioral intervention social skills in adults with autism spectrum disor-
program to treat anxiety and social deficits in teens der using a laboratory-based observational measure.
with high-functioning autism. Clinical Child and Focus of Autism and Other Developmental Disorders.
Family Psychology Review, 13(1), 77–90. doi:10.1177/1088357614539836
White, S. W., Keonig, K., & Scahill, L. (2010b). Group Yoo, H. J., Bahn, G., Cho, I. H., Kim, E. K., Kim, J. H.,
social skills instruction for adolescents with high-­ Min, J. W., & Laugeson, E. A. (2014). A randomized
functioning autism Spectrum disorders. Focus on controlled trial of the Korean version of the PEERS
Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 25(4), parent-assisted social skills training program for teens
209–219. with ASD. Autism Research, 7, 145–161.
Treatment of Addiction in Adults
with Autism Spectrum Disorder 21
Laurence Lalanne, Luisa Weiner,
and Gilles Bertschy

thing, i.e., a substance. Lifetime DSM-5 SUD


What Are Addictive Disorders? diagnosis requires the presence of at least two
psychological, physical, or social criteria during
Addictive disorders, such as substance abuse and the 12 months preceding the interview, or previ-
dependencies included in the DSM-IV Codes ously, and SUD severity levels are classified as
(American Psychiatric Association [APA], 1994) mild, moderate, or severe (2–3, 4–5, or ≥6 crite-
and DSM-5 substance use disorders, are common ria, respectively) (American Psychiatric
disorders involving either the overuse of sub- Association, 2013).
stances like alcohol or drugs or behavioral addic- Many factors are involved in the development
tions such as gambling, internet gaming, or video of SUD. Among them, genetic vulnerability, social
gaming disorders. Addiction develops over time and family factors, psychological traits (i.e.,
and is a chronic and relapsing disorder. The impulsiveness, sensation seeking), and specific
DSM-5 classification of substance use disorders psychiatric illnesses (e.g., anxiety, mood disor-
combines the psychological, physical, and social ders, schizophrenia, autism) are the most promi-
consequences of SUD and adds craving as a new nent. These vulnerability traits precipitate toward
criterion absent from the DSM-IV classification. addictive disorder if and when subjects come into
Craving is defined as an intense desire for some- contact with a given substance (drug availability).

L. Lalanne, MD, PhD-INSERM1114 (*) G. Bertschy


Department of Psychiatry, Strasbourg University Translational Medicine Federation, Medical School,
Hospital, 1 place de l’hôpital, 67000 Strasbourg, Strasbourg University, 4 rue Kirschleger, 67000
France Strasbourg, France
Translational Medicine Federation, Medical School, Department of Psychiatry, Strasbourg University
Strasbourg University, 4 rue Kirschleger, 67000 Hospital, 1 place de l’hôpital, 67000 Strasbourg,
Strasbourg, France France
e-mail: laurence.lalanne@chru-strasbourg.fr
INSERM 1114, Strasbourg University Hospital,
L. Weiner 1 place de l’hôpital, 67000 Strasbourg, France
Department of Psychiatry, Strasbourg University e-mail: gilles.bertschy@chru-strasbourg.fr
Hospital, 1 place de l’hôpital, 67000 Strasbourg,
France
INSERM 1114, Strasbourg University Hospital,
1 place de l’hôpital, 67000 Strasbourg, France
e-mail: luisa.weiner@chru-strasbourg.fr

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 377


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_21
378 L. Lalanne et al.

Thus, the context of the availability of a given sub- tional cognitive bias and beliefs, and alleviating
stance will be associated with substance use. For cognitive impairment. Cognitive impairment, in
example, if someone is used to drinking alcohol particular, affects an individual’s overall quality
with friends in a bar, this context of the bar may of life and his/her social and functional abilities.
reactivate the craving for drinking through asso- Moreover, functional and social impairment may
ciative learning. Whether substance use initially is be increased if addiction is comorbid with other
pleasurable or provides relief from internal dis- psychiatric disorders such as mood disorders,
comfort, it becomes a problem when it is associ- schizophrenia, or intellectual disability.
ated with a loss of control. According to Goodmann Regarding comorbidity with autism spectrum
(Goodman, 1990), “addiction is employed in a disorders (ASD), many studies have reported a
pattern characterized by 1) recurrent failure to reward system dysfunction in individuals with
control the behavior (powerlessness) and 2) con- ASD, as well as hypoactivation in the mesocorti-
tinuation of the behavior despite negative conse- colimbic circuitry, associated with poor dopa-
quences (unmanageability).” “The essential mine activity in response to social and monetary
feature of this disorder is a cluster of cognitive, reward (Kohls et al., 2011, 2013; Scott-Van
behavioral and physiological symptoms that indi- Zeeland, Dapretto, Ghahremani, Poldrack, &
cate that the person has impaired control of psy- Bookheimer, 2010). Cerebral dopamine dysfunc-
choactive substance use and continues use despite tion has also been reported in ASD, akin to what
adverse consequences.” In addition, the mainte- has consistently been found in individuals with
nance of addiction is characterized by difficulty SUD (Arias-Carrión & Pŏppel, 2007). Thus, neu-
controlling the consumption of a given substance, robiologically speaking, individuals with ASD
which reflects the transition toward compulsive may have a higher risk of developing addictive
consumption (Koob & Le Moal, 2008). disorders. Furthermore, psychiatric comorbidi-
Loss of control is associated with both cogni- ties frequently found in individuals with ASD
tive disorders and neurobiological impairments. (i.e., ADHD, anxiety, mood disorders) are also
Main neuropsychological features associated frequently reported in addictive disorders. Lastly,
with SUD are (i) attention and memory bias psychological and neuropsychological features
focused on the substance and the context with associated with ASD, such as impulsiveness and
which it is associated and (ii) executive dysfunc- cognitive rigidity, may predispose these individu-
tion that impairs both inhibition of behaviors als to developing addictive disorders. In the pres-
associated with substance use and the problem-­ ent work, following inpatients with ASD, and
solving skills needed to avoid substance use and epidemiological reminder, we examine these dif-
to take the decision to end substance use (Lalanne ferent aspects of addictive behaviors specific
et al., 2015). These cognitive disorders reflect treatments that might suit this population.
neurobiological impairments in (i) reward cir-
cuitry, i.e., the mesocorticolimbic system, involv-
ing neuroplasticity modifications in the striatum, Epidemiological Reminder
which is responsible for motivation and pleasure About Addictive Disorders
related to dopamine release; (ii) the hippocampus in Patients with ASD
and amygdala, which are responsible for learning
new associations between drug and context; and  ubstance Use Disorder in Patients
S
(iii) the prefrontal cortex, which is responsible with ASD
for problem-solving and cognitive control as a
whole (Feltenstein & See, 2008; Koob & Volkow, Little is known about the comorbidity of ASD and
2010). These modifications keep substance users SUD, since studies in this field are scarce. In
in their addictive behaviors, and treatment con- ASD, co-occurrent SUD is thought to be rare.
sists in prescribing pharmacological therapies, Impaired social skills and lower-than-average
modifying reinforced behaviors involved in the novelty-seeking behavior in patients with ASD
disorder, identifying and restructuring dysfunc- may reduce their access to their substance-using
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 379

peers and, thus, to the substances themselves Furthermore, adolescents with ADHD and ASD
(Bauminger & Kasari, 2000; Prendeville, Prelock, traits might be at risk of other drug abuse.
& Unwin, 2006; Sizoo, van den Brink, Gorissen Whereas few studies reported the dual diagnosis
van Eenige, & van der Gaag, 2009; Soderstrom, between ASD and SUD in adults and adoles-
Rastam, & Gillberg, 2002). Moreover, Hofvander cents, recent data (e.g., Mulligan et al., 2014)
et al. (2009) reported that while psychiatric disor- suggest that the association between the two dis-
ders (i.e., anxiety, ADHD, depression) are fre- orders might be underestimated. Moreover, there
quently comorbid with ASD, SUD is a comorbidity are no data regarding the prevalence of SUD in
seldom found in this population, due to the fact high-functioning autism (HFA), and it may be
that, psychologically speaking, individuals with that patients with HFA show higher rates of
ASD usually have few sensation-seeking traits comorbid SUD, because they are able to develop
and tend to be more introverted, whereas the strategies for interacting with substance-using
opposite personality traits are thought to predis- peers. Matthew Tinsley, an adult with HFA,
pose individuals to developing SUD. In their reported that he used alcohol “to cope with his
paper, Sizoo et al. (2010) explored the prevalence anxiety, to maintain his friendships, and to give
of comorbid SUD in ADHD and ASD. Although access to a whole host of relationships and even
they revealed a higher rate of comorbid SUD in to sustain careers” (Tinsley & Hendrickx, 2008).
patients suffering from ADHD compared to those He assumed that patients with HFA have the ten-
with ASD (58% versus 30%), they showed that dency superficially to display a “normal” façade
patients with ASD and those with ADHD share when they drink alcohol, which may explain why
similar risk factors for SUD. Compared to a con- their alcohol dependency is not better diagnosed.
trol group, both patient groups reported more Recent studies support this hypothesis. For
adverse family events, had more parental SUD, instance, Clark, Tickle, and Gillott (2016) inter-
and started smoking at a young age. In the context viewed eight adults with Asperger syndrome
of their study, De Alwis et al. (2014) recruited about which factors they considered had led to
3,080 young adult Australian twins (mean age the development and maintenance of their prob-
31.9 years) and assessed them for ADHD symp- lematic substance use. The authors identified six
toms, autistic traits, substance use, and substance main themes and explored two of them – self-­
use disorders via a cross-sectional interview and medication and social facilitation – more thor-
self-report questionnaires. They highlighted that oughly, since they were more often reported and
ASD and ADHD traits were associated with high seemed particularly relevant to ASD. Thus, from
levels of regular smoking and cannabis use. a psychopathological standpoint, alcohol use in
However, ASD and ADHD traits were unrelated this population might be related to some of the
to alcohol use. Individuals with ASD are probably core symptoms associated with autism, namely,
less inclined to alcohol use because of their lim- social anxiety and change-related anxiety.
ited social interactions usually associated with Moreover, from a neuropsychological standpoint,
recreational drinking and alcohol intoxication Lalanne et al. (2015) suggested that patients with
(Santosh & Mijovic, 2006). It is important to note, HFA could use alcohol and psychostimulants to
however, that individuals with ASD have a similar cope better with their social and behavioral diffi-
risk of developing alcohol dependence as patients culties and also to compensate for their neuro-
with anxiety or depression (De Alwis et al., 2014). psychological impairments (i.e., social cognition
In adolescence, ASD diagnosis was associated in impairments, executive dysfunction, and weak
previous studies with a relatively low risk of SUD central coherence). Since cognitive impairment
(Ramos et al., 2013), but in a recent study and psychiatric comorbidities are well-known
Mulligan, Reiersen, and Todorov (2014) showed vulnerability traits that may precipitate individu-
that adolescents with moderate to high ASD als into developing SUD, it seems necessary to
symptoms have an elevated risk of alcohol and gain a better understanding of how they are
tobacco use, especially in the case of comorbid related to the development of this comorbidity in
ADHD disorder (Mulligan et al., 2014). ASD in order to treat SUD in patients with ASD.
380 L. Lalanne et al.

 ehavioral Addictions in Patients


B sure to video games in adults with ASD needs to
with ASD be explored in future studies. Likewise, all behav-
ioral addictions, like gambling, which is widely
Boys and girls with ASD, but especially boys, available via the Internet, need to be explored in
spend 62% of their leisure time watching televi- further research studies. To date, no report has
sion and playing video games (Mazurek & described such problems in adults with ASD.
Wenstrup, 2013). Mazureck and several authors
reported behavioral addiction, especially prob-
lematic video game use in boys and adolescents  omorbid Psychiatric Disorders
C
with ASD (Engelhardt & Mazurek, 2014; in Patients with ASD: Relationship
Mazurek & Engelhardt, 2013; Mazurek & with Addictive Disorders
Wenstrup, 2013). In one of their studies (Mazurek
& Engelhardt, 2013), they explored video game Psychiatric comorbidities are very frequent in
use in boys aged from 8 to 18 suffering from patients suffering from ASD. Among them, mood
ADHD or ASD and compared them to a control disorders and anxiety disorders such as phobia
group. Both groups (ADHD and ASD) showed a and ADHD, in particular, have been often
greater risk of problematic video game use than reported (Ghaziuddin, Ghaziuddin, & Greden,
the control group. In patients with ASD as well as 2002; Gillberg et al., 2016; Hofvander et al.,
in those with ADHD, attentional disorders were 2009; Matson & Cervantes, 2014). This high rate
strongly associated with problematic video game of comorbidities is associated with poorer out-
use. Moreover, the same authors described how come. Moreover, patients with more severe ASD
environmental factors are largely to be blamed symptoms are more likely to experience addi-
for the development of this behavioral addiction tional comorbid symptoms (Gadke, McKinney,
in ASD patients. Indeed, access to video games & Oliveros, 2015). Whether these psychiatric
in the bedroom and the absence of parental rules comorbidities affect social insertion, family rela-
regarding video game use are two causes of prob- tionships, and friendships, or cognitive abilities,
lematic video game use (Engelhardt & Mazurek, they are also associated with a high risk of devel-
2014), with consequences for sleep. Problematic oping addictive disorders. This association,
video game players spent less time per night namely, dual diagnosis (here, the association
sleeping regardless of their diagnosis (ASD, between a neurodevelopmental or psychiatric
ADHD, or typical development), and in ASD disorder and SUD), was reported in data analyses
patients, there was a direct relationship between with regard to the National Epidemiological
the impact on sleep and the number of hours Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions
spent playing video games (Engelhardt, Mazurek, (NESARC-USA), which showed there is to be a
& Sohl, 2013). One study explored the possibility link between major depression and alcohol
that in adults with ASD, as compared to controls, dependence (OR = 3.7, 95% CI = 3.1–4.4) (Grant
exposure to violent video games increases et al., 2007) and, in women, between major
aggressive behavior, aggressive-thought accessi- depression and smoking (Husky et al., 2008).
bility, and aggressive affect. Adults with ASD Mood disorders are often associated with alcohol
were not differentially affected by exposure to use (Odds ratio = 3.5, 99% CI = 2.7–4.5) and
violent video games (Engelhardt, Mazurek, anxiety disorders (Odds ratio = 2.7, 99%
Hilgard, Rouder, & Bartholow, 2015). However, CI = 2.1–3.7) (Compton, Thomas, Stinson, &
in adults, no study has ever explored the propor- Grant, 2007; Grant et al., 2007). Moreover,
tion of problematic gamers among patients with according to Callaghan et al. (2013), the
ASD. Regarding the poor social skills of adults ­prevalence of smoking among individuals with
with ASD, it is likely that many ASD patients psychiatric conditions is approximately two to
who are problematic gamers in adolescence will four times higher than in the general population.
continue as such in adulthood. However, expo- Adults with ADHD use more psychostimulants,
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 381

cannabis, nicotine, and alcohol than their peers all the more true given that patients with ASD are
(Groenman et al., 2013; Katusic et al., 2005; frequently comorbid with ADHD, which
Levy et al., 2014; Upadhyaya et al., 2005; ). increases their attentional disabilities (Ghaziuddin
Consequently, since patients with ASD are often et al., 2002; Gillberg et al., 2016; Hofvander
comorbid with other psychiatric disorders fre- et al., 2009; Matson & Cervantes, 2014). Adaptive
quently associated with SUD, the treatment of functioning, in particular, is highly dependent on
SUD in ASD patients should also take account of self-control, problem-solving, and the ability to
how the other psychiatric comorbidities are being initiate functional strategies and adapt to nonrou-
treated. tine events, e.g., executive functioning. Numerous
studies suggest that these abilities are impaired in
ASD (Banaschewski et al., 2011; Lalanne et al.,
 ognitive Disorders in Patients
C 2015, Rommelse et al., 2015;Wallace et al., 2015;
with ASD: Relationship Zhang et al., 2015). Impulsiveness and cognitive
with Addictive Disorders rigidity, in particular, may be associated with the
onset and maintenance of SUD in ASD (Koob &
Several neurocognitive particularities have been Le Moal, 2008). Indeed, addictive disorders are
associated with ASD. Among them, social cogni- defined as the repetitive use of certain substances,
tion impairment, executive dysfunction (i.e., in usually the same substance, in a routine context.
respect of planning, cognitive flexibility, working Thus, patients with ASD who present ritualized
memory, attention control), and a specific and behavior patterns and, on a neuropsychological
detailed information-processing style have been level, cognitive rigidity may be particularly
the most studied and constitute the main endo- prone to developing and maintaining SUD.
phenotypic traits of the disorder (Hill & Frith, Consequently, treatment of SUD in these patients
2003). These neurocognitive traits have been has to take account of neurocognitive traits that
linked with some of the behavioral disturbances might contribute to maintaining the SUD.
found in ASD, namely, social anxiety and anxiety
associated with unexpected events or transitions.
Thus, as suggested by case studies (Lalanne Interventions
et al., 2015), it is possible that these neurocogni-
tive traits may be vulnerability risk factors for the Psychosocial interventions are the crux of treat-
onset and maintenance of addiction disorders in ment for addiction. The main criterion of effec-
patients with ASD. On the one hand, anxiolytic tiveness is either the reduction of consumption or
substances (i.e., alcohol, benzodiazepines, or abstinence, depending on the goals set by the
marijuana), in particular, may trigger social inter- patient. Improvements across a broad range of
action and be a way of coping with their lack of areas of functioning are also expected (e.g., phys-
social skills and reduced ability to cope with ical and psychological well-being, HIV and hep-
change in their environment (Kronenberg, atitis risk behaviors, interpersonal relationships,
Goossens, van Busschbach, van Achterberg, & employment and criminal behavior). However,
van den Brink, 2015; Kronenberg, Slager-­ psychological, neurocognitive, and psychiatric
Visscher, Goossens, van den Brink, & van comorbidities might be barriers to attaining the
Achterberg, 2014; Sizoo et al., 2009). On the goal of the intervention. Thus, treatment of SUD
other hand, some patients with ASD report that requires multilevel care that includes pharmaco-
tea, coffee, or certain drugs enhance their atten- logical, psychological, and cognitive remediation
tional abilities, helping them to initiate nonrou- therapies. Regarding cognitive impairment, in
tine activities, such as work-related tasks particular, it is important to note that executive or
(Lalanne et al., 2015). Like patients with ADHD, attentional dysfunction might have different and
SUD patients use psychostimulants that enhance sometimes additive etiologies in the case of ASD,
their attentional and concentration abilities. It is such as ADHD, depression, bipolar disorder, and
382 L. Lalanne et al.

anxiety disorders. Therefore, to treat SUD in Cochrane review showed that motivational inter-
patients with ASD, it is important to assess views have an impact on the extent of substance
patients’ neurocognitive profile; psychological use disorders (Smedslund et al., 2011). To the
traits involved in maintaining the SUD, such as best of our knowledge, however, there have been
specific beliefs associated with the SUD (i.e., I no studies regarding the use of motivational
cannot cope socially without alcohol); functional interviewing alone in patients with ASD. Based
abilities (i.e., social skills); social and family on our clinical experience, motivational inter-
environment; and psychiatric comorbidities, viewing can be effective in this population, pro-
since these factors are key to achieving perma- vided some of the neurocognitive and behavioral
nent abstinence or controlled use of substances in particularities found in ASD are taken into
this population. account. For instance, because of patients’ con-
crete information-­processing style, their literal
understanding of communication, and cognitive
Psychosocial Interventions rigidity, clinicians need to use explicit terms, be
wary of abstract goals, help patients verbalize
The most widely used and evidence-based psy- their ambivalence, and favor the use of visual
chosocial therapies with SUD are motivational schema and/or written aids in order to elicit the
interviewing, brief interventions, and cognitive emergence of ambivalence in these patients.
behavioral therapy (CBT) (Jhanjee, 2014). However, even when patients with ASD are able
Motivational interviewing and brief interventions to realize the discrepancy between their behavior
are the therapies most used because they are easy and their goals, they are often too anxious to
to carry out in primary care settings. They were change. This anxiety may be associated with cog-
first developed for alcohol and tobacco use disor- nitive rigidity. Thus, motivational interviewing
ders. Brief intervention consists in brief counsel- may be a first step toward change and could be
ing for patients with addiction disorders but who incremented by family-oriented therapies, the
come under primary care. It might be used, for goal of which is to alleviate stress in the family,
example, in the case of admission to a general insofar as although addiction is an individual
hospital following a car crash or in the case of problem, families are deeply affected a family
acute intoxication (Bien, Miller, & Tonigan, member’s addiction. According to Copello,
1993). Motivational interviewing is a patient-­ Velleman, and Templeton (2005), three types of
centered, semi-directive method for enhancing family-focused interventions may be effective in
intrinsic motivation to change by exploring and alleviating stress in family. First, promoting the
resolving ambivalence (Miller & Rollnick, 2012). substance user’s commitment to his care and his
The principles of motivational interviewing are will to change may be important for encouraging
to help patients come to terms with their ambiva- the family to support a patient faced with his
lence about their behavior and to stimulate choice. Secondly, it may be important to involve
change. Some of the techniques used to reach this family members and substance-misusing rela-
goal include expressing empathy through reflec- tives in the patients’ treatment so that they take
tive listening, developing discrepancy between on an active role. Finally, each member of the
patients’ goals or values and their current behav- family needs to be supported in this situation
iors, avoiding argument and direct confrontation, with suitable follow-­up. In the same way, raising
adjusting to client resistance, and supporting self-­ a child or an adolescent with ASD is a profoundly
efficacy and optimism. Motivational interview- stressful experience (Duarte, Bordin, Yazigi, &
ing is not a stand-alone therapy delivered with the Mooney, 2005; Montes & Halterman, 2007). In
intention of achieving behavior change. Instead, particular, behavioral difficulties are a barrier for
it prepares an individual for change by increasing adult independence and community involvement
contemplation and commitment to change (Smith & Philippen, 2005) which impact the
(Arkowitz, Westra, Miller, & Rollnick, 2007). A whole family. For the families of adult individuals
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 383

with ASD, specific therapies such as multifamily instance), cue exposure, promotion of nondrug-­
group psychoeducation (Smith, Greenberg, & related activities, relaxation training, and long-­
Mailick, 2012) have been developed to help and term prevention of relapses. Other elements of
support them. “Such intervention involves CBT include social skills training and problem-­
weekly group sessions wherein multiple family solving skills which are determinants of SUD in
members are together provided with education ASD. Moreover, the long-term maintenance of
on the nature, course, and management of the treatment goals involves making use of specific
condition as well as training in and activities for problem-solving and planning abilities.
practicing problem-­solving” (Smith, Greenberg, According to Jarvis, Tebbutt, and Mattick (2005),
& Mailick, 2012). The goal of the family psycho- to prevent relapse patients need to identify situa-
education model is to promote education and tions and/or places (like where an alcoholic con-
problem-­solving strategies which impact family sumes alcohol with friends) or states (using
stress and give all family members an active role alcohol to cope with a negative emotion, to make
in the treatment. When SUD and ASD are com- it disappear for example) that trigger a craving
bined, it amounts to a “double burden” for the and make people vulnerable to drugs. Once iden-
family. Whereas, as far as we know, no specific tified, the goal is to develop new strategies to
family therapies have been designed to address avoid high-risk situations that trigger craving and
this combination, therapists and carers must be to develop skills for managing craving in the case
attentive to family suffering and stress. Moreover, of exposure and other painful emotions without
family interventions may increase the generaliz- using substances and learning to cope with
ability of newly learned functional behaviors, lapses. CBT has been found to be effective for
especially in individuals with ASD. treating SUD as a stand-alone therapy. There is
Although combining psychosocial and family evidence, however, that abstinence rates can be
therapies might be helpful for patients with ASD improved by combining psychosocial approaches,
and SUD, they sometimes need a much more which are delivered according to patients’ diffi-
structured and intensive behavioral therapy for culties, with pharmacological treatments for
them to commit to change. alcohol abstinence, like acamprosate (Feeney,
Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is based Young, Connor, Tucker, & McPherson, 2002)
on learning principles and the theory that behav- and naltrexone (O’Malley et al., 1992). In patients
ior may be influenced by cognitive processes with ASD, behavioral therapies such as applied
(Dobson, 2000). CBT is a structured psychologi- behavior analysis (ABA) and social skills train-
cal approach, derived from a cognitive model of ing are the most widely used and evidence-based
drug misuse (Beck, Wright, Newman, & Liese, treatments available (e.g., Virués-Ortega, 2010;
1993; Dobson , 2000). According to this model, Otero, Schatz, Merrill, & Bellini, 2015). CBT, for
distorted cognitive bias and dysfunctional beliefs its part, has been mainly used to treat comorbid
may play a role in the maintenance of addiction. mental illnesses, such as anxiety disorders (e.g.,
There are also overlearned associations between Wood et al., 2015; Ung, Selles, Small, & Storch,
specific contexts and substance use, which are 2015). In such cases, CBT was adapted to suit
associated with craving. Thus, CBT uses a cogni- patients with ASD-specific neurocognitive and
tive and behavioral approach to addiction, relying behavioral styles. Such adaptations include
on both strategies to promote change in the direc- greater use of visualization to assist patients with
tion of the patient’s goals and to prevent relapse. identifying their emotional states, their thoughts,
Cognitive therapy relies on identifying distorted and all “the invisible” aspects involved in
cognitive bias and restructuring dysfunctional ­communication (Ekman & Hiltunen, 2015). To
thoughts that may be associated with seemingly the best of our knowledge, there are no reports
irrelevant decisions that lead to a relapse. The regarding its effectiveness in treating SUD in
behavioral strategies used are coping with sub- patients with ASD. Based on our clinical experi-
stance cravings (through the use of distraction for ence, however, CBT adapted to ASD may be
384 L. Lalanne et al.

effective for treating SUD in this population, tive deficits (Pitel et al., 2009). Thus, the devel-
although executive dysfunction, mainly cognitive opment of strategies to ameliorate executive and
rigidity, may reduce its effectiveness in some memory processes may reduce the risk of relapse,
patients with severe executive impairment. which is especially high in the first year after
Indeed, patients may display greater resistance withdrawal. Cognitive remediation has been
when challenged about their dysfunctional beliefs poorly explored in relation to addiction to date,
(i.e., I am unable to function socially without except for alcohol use disorder. Some studies
alcohol; I cannot deal with change-related stress (Houben, Nederkoon, Wiers, & Jansen, 2011;
without smoking) via Socratic questioning or Houben, Wiers, & Jansen, 2011; Rupp, Kemmler,
may have difficulty generating alternative Kurz, Hinterhuber, & Fleischhacker, 2012; Wiers
thoughts when stimulated to do so. In such cases, et al., 2015a, 2015b) have shown that executive
clinicians may prefer behavioral experiments to deficit remediation, through techniques such as
verbal techniques. However, change-related anx- goal management training (GMT) and inhibition
iety and cognitive rigidity may also be barriers to reinforcing, reduces alcohol consumption for a
performing such experiments in patients with period of 1–2 months in students (Houben,
severe executive impairment. In such cases, cog- Havermans, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2012), as
nitive remediation therapy, aimed at improving well as in patients suffering from a moderate to
executive dysfunction, or behavioral therapy severe alcohol use disorder (Wiers et al., 2015).
alone, may be proposed prior to CBT, with a rea- Moreover, alleviation of executive impairment
sonable chance success (Lalanne et al., 2015). has been shown to be accompanied by activation
changes in the medial prefrontal cortex (Wiers
et al., 2015b). However, these studies suffer from
Cognitive Remediation a number of limitations. First, patients with SUD
may present with memory impairment, especially
Cognitive remediation therapy targets cognitive when they have a history of alcohol, cannabis,
dysfunction that can be associated with both psychostimulant, or heroin consumption (Gould,
SUD and ASD, through training, facilitation, 2010), and memory processes are necessary for
and/or compensation techniques, in order to alle- learning and developing strategies and new rou-
viate the functional burden associated with neu- tines in a process of change. Therefore, it may be
rocognitive impairment. As mentioned above, necessary to develop strategies that target both
neurocognitive impairment, such as executive executive and memory dysfunction. Only one
dysfunction, is involved in the onset and mainte- publication reported memory improvement in
nance of addiction. Moreover, it may also reduce patients suffering from Korsakoff syndrome, a
the effectiveness of psychosocial therapies in chronic neuropsychiatric disorder caused by
SUD. Whereas cognitive disorders have been alcohol abuse and thiamine deficiency (Oudman,
studied extensively in relation to alcohol depen- Nijboer, Postma, Wijnia, & Van der Stigchel,
dence, there are fewer reports in relation to 2015). It is possible to argue that programs
SUD. In alcohol-dependent subjects, executive adapted more specifically to the severity of cog-
and memory deficits impact negatively on the nitive deficits should be better able to enhance
possibility of reducing substance use and increase executive functions and promote functional
the risk of relapse (Pitel et al., 2009). Segobin recovery in patients, depending on the extent of
et al. (2014) showed the spontaneous recovery of their cognitive impairment. It is all the more rel-
cerebral structures (fusiform gyrus, paracingulate evant for dual diagnosis patients, i.e., patients
and anterior cingulate gyri, striatum, cerebellum) suffering from a psychiatric or neurodevelop-
6 months after alcohol withdrawal but only if mental disorder combined with SUD. In ASD
alcohol consumption was reduced to less than patients who have a SUD, cognitive impair-
about one glass per day during this period. ment may be associated with both ASD and the
Memory deficits recover much faster than execu- substance use. Consequently, in the case of dual
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 385

or trial diagnosis patients with ADHD, ASD, and from addictive disorders (Krentzman et al.,
SUD, if the patient presents with executive dys- 2010), but there are no data about the efficiency
function that may interfere with psychosocial of such groups for patients with ASD and SUD.
therapies, cognitive remediation which targets For ASD patients, support groups are an oppor-
planning, problem-solving, and inhibition abili- tunity for adults with ASD and their families to
ties may be particularly helpful as an adjunct share their experience and their difficulties.
therapy to pharmacotherapy and other psychoso- Meetings are proposed as a way of learning
cial approaches. As far as we know, there are no about ASD and how it affects social life. The
reports regarding the effectiveness of cognitive impact of such groups is connected with sound
remediation of executive processes in ASD with psychological and sociological theories and the
SUD. However, our own data suggest that cogni- helper-therapy principle, theories of experiential
tive remediation of executive processes is able to knowledge, and social support theories (Salzer
alleviate working memory, cognitive flexibility, et al., 1994). In the case of ASD and SUD, sup-
inhibition, and planning difficulties and at the port groups may be of invaluable help for the
same time have a positive impact on social func- process of behavioral change (toward absti-
tioning, depressive symptoms, and self-esteem in nence, for instance) and also as social support as
this population (Weiner, Bruckmann, & Bizet, regards efforts to stay sober (relapse prevention).
2010). In this study, cognitive remediation con- Social skills training groups that take account of
sisted in 3 months of twice-weekly sessions that the specific needs of individuals with ASD and
made use of our own adaptation of the flexibility SUD should also be designed. Such groups
module of the CRT program for schizophrenia already exist for other diagnoses frequently
developed by Wykes, Huddy, Cellard, McGurk, found comorbid with SUD (e.g., bipolar disorder
and Czobor (2011), GMT, and a working mem- and SUD). In ASD, it is possible that substance
ory training program developed by Levaux et al. use is at least partially sustained by the beliefs
(2012). Similar positive results were found when these individuals have about their inability to
this composite program was used in two patients cope with social situations. Thus, if the aim is to
with ASD and SUD prior to CBT (Lalanne et al., prevent relapse in the long run, it is crucial to
2015). help patients improve their social skills and their
feeling of self-efficacy.

Group Interventions
Pharmacotherapy
Three groups could be distinguished, one cen-
tered on a specific treatment related to a specific  harmacotherapy for Addictive
P
motivational stage (precomtemplation, contem- Disorders
plation, or action), one centered on the develop- In addictive disorders, there are few certified
ment of social skills, and a support group. Given medications available for abstinence and reduc-
that Alcoholics Anonymous is only for alcohol- tion other than for alcohol addiction. We shall
ics, another group, Narcotics Anonymous, has describe the specificity of prescription in patients
been developed for people suffering from drug with ASD and SUD. For reduction, there are two
addiction. They use a traditional 12-step model medications, one certified nalmefene (Marazziti
which promotes, for example, admitting addic- et al., 2015), which showed a reduction in alcohol
tion or compulsion, learning from past mistakes consumption when accompanied by ­psychosocial
and making amends for these mistakes, and, follow-up, and another, baclofen (Lesouef,
finally, developing a new code of behavior. At Bellet, Mounier, & Beyens, 2014, a meta-­
the end, the treatment involves helping others analysis), the effectiveness of which is mixed.
suffering from addictive disorders. These thera- There is no specificity of their prescription in
pies have proved effective in patients suffering ASD, except that baclofen is not recommended
386 L. Lalanne et al.

in France in the case of severe psychiatric disor- ture are mixed. A meta-analysis carried out by
ders and a high level of precariousness. It is Thomas et al. (2015) showed no increase in sui-
worth noting that some reports point to some cide or attempted suicide, suicidal ideation,
improvements in socialization in ASD individu- depression, or death with varenicline (Thomas
als with arbaclofen, one of the baclofen enantio- et al., 2013, 2015) and bupropion (Thomas et al.,
mers (Frye, 2014). We found no case reports 2013). However, Molero, Lichtenstein,
about the use of baclofen in alcohol-dependent Zetterqvist, Gumpert, and Fazel (2015) suggested
ASD patients. that varenicline is associated with a very slight
For the maintenance of alcohol abstinence, increase in the risk of suicidality and accidents.
three medications are recommended (disulfiram, Given that it is not clearly demonstrated by
acamprosate, and naltrexone), but their efficacy results that smoking cessation drugs do not cause
is poor. Disulfiram is as effective as the other psychiatric symptoms (Davies, 2013), a follow-
treatments but controversial because of its strong ­up with a psychiatrist would be recommended in
side effects, especially cardiovascular disorders tobacco-smoking patients with mood disorders
(Skinner, Lahmek, Pham, & Aubin, 2014). Up to combined with ASD, in the case of varenicline
now, it is considered a treatment of second inten- and bupropion prescription. Again, we found no
tion. In a comparison of naltrexone to acampro- reports of clinical experience with these drugs in
sate, acamprosate seems slightly more effective these tobacco-smoking ASD subjects.
for maintaining abstinence and naltrexone For cocaine addiction, no treatment is cur-
slightly more effective for reducing heavy drink- rently certified (Karila et al., 2011, 2014), but
ing and craving (Maisel et al., 2013). Naltrexone based on publications, some recommendations
has been tested in patients with ASD, and it has have been given about pharmacotherapy in com-
been found that low doses of naltrexone (0.5 mg/ bination with psychosocial follow-up. To manage
kg) have a positive effect on hyperactivity, tan- cocaine withdrawal N-acetylcysteine may be
trums, social isolation, stereotypical behaviors, proposed at the dosage of 1,200 mg per day to
self-mutilation, and irritability in children and alleviate symptoms associated with withdrawal
improve social and reduce aggressive behaviors and reduce craving. Topiramate, an antiepileptic
in adults (Baghdadli, Gonnier, & Aussilloux, drug whose action modulates mesocorticolimbic
2002; Doyle & McDougle, 2012; Rossignol, transmission, may be offered at the dosage of
2009; Roy et al., 2015; Roy et al., 2015b, review; 200 mg per day to prevent relapses. Disulfiram
Wynn & Brunetti, 2009 a review). Subsequently, reduces the high associated with the increased
following these clinical observations, naltrexone levels of dopamine in the case of cocaine intoxi-
was regarded as having potential for treating cation. Since disulfiram has many cardiovascular
ASD. Consequently, with respect to maintaining effects, its prescription should be restricted to
alcohol abstinence, patients with ASD might the- specialist centers, especially in the case of
oretically benefit from being prescribed naltrex- cocaine addiction combined with alcohol addic-
one for both ASD and their alcohol use disorders. tion. Once again we found no reports about the
However, we have not found any studies or even pharmacotherapy of cocaine addiction in ASD
case reports to support this view. patients.
For tobacco addiction, same conventional pre- Although there is no description of opiate
scriptions could be offered to patients with ASD addiction in patients with ASD, replacement
and tobacco use disorder. Chewing gum or nico- treatments, like methadone and buprenorphine/
tine patches could be proposed first as nicotine buprenorphine-naloxone, have been proposed.
replacement therapy and would be combined Follow-up must be very strict for methadone and
with psychosocial follow-up. Bupropion and var- buprenorphine initiation. Finally, for behavioral
enicline could be proposed as a second step but addictions, regardless of whether behavioral
would have to be carefully prescribed in patients treatment should be encouraged first, many phar-
with psychiatric disorders. Results in the litera- macological treatments have been tried. The
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 387

principles of these treatments are to reduce enhancing drugs in patients suffering from addic-
impulsiveness by prescribing antidepressant tion, particularly during protracted abstinence.
treatments such as specific serotonin reuptake According to him, such medications could help
inhibitors (SSRI) and tricyclic and/or to decrease patients maintain their abstinence or cut down on
craving by prescribing, for example, nalmefene, their addictive behavior. Clinical trials have
naltrexone, topiramate, NAC, and memantine. shown that methylphenidate, used to raise atten-
However, none of these treatments are certified tion levels in patients suffering from ADHD, has
(Grant & Kim, 2006). This fact also raises the a tendency to enhance the executive performance,
question of the specific pharmacodynamic effects and especially response inhibition (Levin, Evans,
of such largely used psychotropic drugs in ASD Brooks, & Garawi, 2007; Li et al., 2010), of
patients. For instance, SSRI seem to reduce patients addicted to psychostimulants by reduc-
behavioral disorders such as self-injurious behav- ing their craving for cocaine (Levin et al., 2007).
ior and ADHD-like symptoms in young ASD Adolescents with ASD and ADHD usually ben-
patients when taken at a very low dose (for efit from methylphenidate in that it helps curb
instance, venlafaxine 18.75 mg/day) (Carminati, their impulsiveness, improving their behavioral
Deriaz, & Bertschy, 2006; Carminati et al., 2016). control and hyperactivity (Pearson et al., 2013;
Such effects in ASD patients are difficult to Santosh, Baird, Pityaratstian, Tavare, & Gringras,
match with known effects in non-ASD patients. 2006). There is no description of how it affects
Moreover, regarding the side effects of anti-­ adults. As impulsive behaviors are strongly asso-
addictive treatments, patients with ASD and SUD ciated with addiction onset and the persistence of
should be followed by a therapist skilled in both addictive disorders, it could be very interesting to
addictology and psychiatry. On the psychiatric prescribe methylphenidate in patients with mul-
side, a specific ASD expertise would be expected. tiple comorbidities, i.e., ASD/ADHD/
Some authors reported that cognitive enhanc- SUD. Although this observation has yet to be
ers were beneficial in patients suffering from confirmed in clinical trials, other authors, like
cognitive disorders combined with addictive dis- Kumar, Prakash, Sewal, Medhi, and Modi (2012),
orders. This is of particular interest for patients suggest that cognitive and behavioral impair-
suffering from both psychiatric and addictive dis- ments in patients with autism could be treated
orders and in whom we observe cumulative cog- with psychostimulants such as methylphenidate
nitive disorders. or nicotinic agonistic agents.

 harmacotherapy for Cognitive
P Pharmacotherapy for Dual Diagnosis
Disorders: Cognitive Enhancers
As reported above, patients with ASD regularly
As neuropsychological and clinical observations suffer from other psychiatric disorders like
have shown, patients suffering from SUD develop depressive disorders, ADHD, anxiety disorders,
cognitive impairments, such as executive, mem- and even psychosis (Ghaziuddin et al., 2002;
ory, attentional, and temporal disorders (Gould, Gillberg et al., 2016; Hofvander et al., 2009;
2010), which are also found in other pathologies Matson & Cervantes, 2014). All these psychiatric
such as neurological, neuropsychiatric, and psy- pathologies are associated with a heightened risk
chiatric disorders. Based on these observations, of SUD and require treatment. Recently, litera-
physicians tried to treat patients suffering from ture reported that some pharmacological
different types of addictive behavior with medi- ­treatments might have a positive impact on dual
cation aimed at improving cognitive deficits pre- diagnosis, including mirtazapine, for depressive
viously prescribed in neurological and psychiatric disorders comorbid with SUD (Graves, Rafeyan,
disorders (Sofuoglu et al., 2013). In their article, Watts, & Napier, 2012; Lalanne et al., 2015). On
Sofuoglu et al. (2013) propose testing cognition-­ the other hand, some antipsychotics, like risperi-
388 L. Lalanne et al.

done and haloperidol (especially in Japan), which (2012) described in their paper how clozapine is
are frequently prescribed in autism to manage effective for improving behavioral symptoms in
irritability, behavioral aggressiveness, and mal- patients with ASD. Consequently, it might be of
adaptive behaviors in children, adolescents, and particular interest in ASD patients who suffer
adults (Hsia et al., 2014; Scott & Dhillon, 2008; from both severe behavioral symptoms and
Sharma & Shaw, 2012), might impact SUD. For addictive disorders. As in many other parts of this
example, Dawe, Gerada, Russell, and Gray chapter, we propose building a bridge hypothesis
(1995) showed that a single dose of haloperidol between ASD patient studies and SUD patient
increases nicotine intake in smokers. Moreover, studies, but studies or reports about dual diagno-
nicotine dependence is greater in patients treated sis patients are lacking to date.
with haloperidol (Kim, Han, Joo, & Min, 2010),
owing to pharmacological aspects of haloperidol
as a dopamine receptor type 2 antagonist involved Multidisciplinary and Holistic Cares
in pleasure associated with dopamine action.
DR2 antagonism is also associated with increased Like dual diagnosis patients, for example,
craving, which leads patients to overconsume to schizophrenic patients with substance use disor-
compensate for the effects of antipsychotics. ders, with cannabis, alcohol, and tobacco being
Consequently, prescribing atypical antipsychot- the main substances involved in these patients
ics, which have less DR2 antagonism, particu- (Thoma & Daum, 2013), patients with autism
larly in cases of addiction to opiates (Gerra et al., comorbid SUD need an integrated approach to
1985; Kern, Akerman, & Nordstrom, 2014), their health and substance abuse treatment. In
cocaine (Longo, 2002), and nicotine is generally SUD associated with psychiatric disorders, in
recommended (Wijesundera, Hanwella, & de particular, Torrens, Rossi, Martinez-Riera,
Silva, 2014; Wu, Chen, & Lee, 2013). Also, in Martinez-Sanvisens, and Bulbena (2012) showed
patients with ASD comorbid SUD, haloperidol the superiority of a multidisciplinary approach
should be avoided and replaced by risperidone. combining different therapies delivered in a
Studies have moreover suggested that risperidone coordinated fashion by different therapists:
has some positive effect in the case of cocaine addictologist, psychiatrist, psychologist, social
addiction (Akerele & Levin, 2007; De La Garza, worker, etc. However, the fact is that a specific
Newton, & Kalechstein, 2005) and likewise program has to be defined according to patients’
aripiprazole in the case of alcohol and cocaine difficulties to promote the best outcome. In
addictions (Anton et al., 2008; Brunetti et al., patients with ASD and SUD, it might be impor-
2012; Stoops, Lile, Lofwall, & Rush, 2007). tant to treat vulnerability factors, namely, psy-
Among antipsychotics, clozapine seems to be of chiatric comorbidities like ADHD, anxiety,
real value for treating addiction in patients suffer- mood disorders, and psychotic disorders, and
ing from psychiatric disorders. Clozapine shows taking into account the behavioral traits of ASD
less affinity for DR2 receptors but a greater affin- that might predispose these individuals to devel-
ity for DR3 and DR4 than other antipsychotics oping SUD (e.g., impaired social skills, change-
(McCormick, Wilson, Wilson, & Remington, related anxiety, lack of imagination). Moreover,
2013; Seeman, 2014), and in clinical studies cognitive impairments might be taken into
these effects are associated with a reduction of account before starting CBT. Accordingly, a
addictive disorders (Keltner et al., 2000). In neuropsychological assessment might be useful
patients, addiction decreases significantly (by to identify neurocognitive particularities that
around 85%) when patients suffering from psy- promote substance use d­ isorders, such as execu-
chiatric and addictive disorders switch from clas- tive deficits, as well as specific cognitive reme-
sic antipsychotic drugs to clozapine (Procyshyn, diation of executive dysfunction. As a parallel
Ihsan, & Thompson, 2001; San, Arranz, & approach, CBT, which is very well structured,
Martinez-Raga, 2007). Moreover, Kumar et al. seems to be appropriate in patients with ASD
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 389

and SUD and might be combined with a family her independence and a new identity outside of
approach that promotes familial guidance. Such drug addiction. Specific structures could be
specific care involves many different profession- developed for dual diagnosis patients.
als (e.g., psychiatrist, neuropsychologist, psy-
chologist, addictologist, etc). Alongside such
specific care, psychoeducation programs could The Concept of Personal Recovery
be helpful to these patients in informing them
about the risks associated with drugs, assisting Personal recovery is defined by Anthony (1993)
them with setting their own objectives, adopting as “a deep, personal, unique process of changing
a different lifestyle, and restoring relationships one’s attitude, values, feelings, goals, skills, and
with friends and family. Such programs support roles.” It thus requires developing a new meaning
the idea of integrated care and a multidisci- and purpose of life, beyond the effects of the ill-
plinary approach involving several different ness. Common elements of recovery as identified
types of networks (Lang, Bonnewitz, Kusterer, by Davidson (2005) include renewing hope and
& Lalanne-­Tongio, 2014). commitment, redefining self, incorporating ill-
ness and managing symptoms, involving in
meaningful activities, overcoming stigma and
Social Rehabilitation assuming control, becoming empowered and
exercising citizenship, and being supported by
In dual diagnosis patients, precariousness is not others. In individuals suffering from dual diagno-
rare. Social rehabilitation that promotes patients’ sis, such as ASD and SUD, clinical recovery is
autonomy and accompanies them throughout particularly difficult. One study (Kronenberg
their rehabilitation is an important part of strug- et al., 2015) identified the use of personal recov-
gles with addiction. Involving patients’ families ery strategies in two groups of dual diagnosis
is extremely important as regards care in the case patients (ADHD-SUD vs. ASD-ADHD) via
of both SUD and autism. Patients with ASD semi-structured interviews. Compared to ADHD-­
experience real difficulties finding a job, difficul- SUD patients, patients with ASD and SUD
ties which become even more acute in the case of reported more difficulty becoming active agents
SUD. Consequently, social workers have to take of change, and limited empowerment and hope,
account of difficulties pertaining to both diagno- which makes their personal recovery a difficult
ses when steering the patient toward a suitable concept and complex process. According to the
structure, such as a Center of Assistance by authors, these results are due to the clinical char-
Work, where staff are trained in behavioral diffi- acteristics associated with ASD, i.e., social and
culties relating to psychiatric disorders. Until behavioral difficulties, which make these patients
now, however, there is no specific structure that particularly dependent on support from others
accompanies patients with dual diagnosis on with finding activities and filling their lives. It is
their path to find employment. It could be impor- also possible that these difficulties are due at least
tant to run sheltered employment programs where in part to the neurocognitive particularities
the majority of workers are people with dual reported in ASD (cognitive flexibility, social cog-
diagnosis, namely, with psychiatric disorders or nition, lack of imagination), as we have already
neurodevelopmental disorders and SUD. These suggested before. Moreover, autobiographical
programs should be supervised by workers memory abnormalities have been reported in
trained to deal with patients’ specific disabilities. ASD (Crane, Lind, & Bowler, 2013). Such abnor-
Moreover, in such centers, access to multidisci- malities suggest that the concept of selfhood and
plinary care workers should be promoted, and the setting of future goals based on self-values
direct connections could improve specific care may be impaired in these individuals. Thus, when
given to these patients. All in all, social care is using the concept of personal recovery in patients
important for enabling a patient to regain his or with ASD, it is important to take into account
390 L. Lalanne et al.

their neurocognitive profile and the obstacles that American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of mental disorders DSM-IV
may be encountered in this population in relation
(4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric
to its implementation in clinical practice. Association.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
and statistical manual of psychiatric disorders DSM-­
V (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric
Training for Early Identification Association.
Anthony, W. A. (1993). Recovery from mental illness:
In his book, Matthew Tinsley (2008) reported The guiding vision of the mental health service sys-
that for patients with ASD, drugs are a way of tem in the 1990s. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal,
16(4), 11–23.
coping with anxiety and depression and of
Anton, R. F., Kranzler, H., Breder, C., Marcus, R. N.,
improving their social skills. Consequently, Carson, W. H., & Han, J. (2008). A randomized, mul-
drugs may mask their diagnosis for a long time. ticenter, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of
Little is known in psychiatry or in addictology the efficacy and safety of aripiprazole for the treat-
ment of alcohol dependence. Journal of Clinical
about the occurrence of SUD in patients with
Psychopharmacology, 28, 5–12.
ASD. However, there are many quick clinical Arias-Carrión, O., & Pŏppel, E. (2007). Dopamine, learn-
assessment tools available for identifying SUD ing, and reward-seeking behavior. Acta Neurobiologiae
(e.g., for nicotine addiction Fagerström’s ques- Experimentalis (Wars), 67(4), 481–488.
Baghdadli, A., Gonnier, V., & Aussilloux, C. (2002).
tionnaire, for alcohol addiction the Alcohol Use
Review of psychopharmacological treatments in ado-
Disorder Identification or AUDIT, or for canna- lescents and adults with autistic disorders. Encephale,
bis addiction the Cannabis Abuse Screening 28(3 Pt 1), 248–254. Review. French.
Test). In adolescents, specific tests could be use- Banaschewski, T., Poustka, L., & Holtmann, M. (2011).
Autism and ADHD across the life span. Differential
ful. For example, the CRAFFT (Car, Relax,
diagnoses or comorbidity? Nervenarzt, 82(5), 573–
Alone, Forget, Friends, Trouble) Screening Test 580. German.
is a test for screening for substance-related risks Bauminger, N., & Kasari, C. (2000). Loneliness and
and problems in adolescents. Whether or not the friendship in high-functioning children with autism.
Child Development, 71(2), 447–456.
diagnosis of SUD in patients with ASD should
Beck, A. T., Wright, F. D., Newman, C. F., & Liese,
be encouraged, it is likely that patients with ASD B. S. (1993). Cognitive therapy of substance abuse.
and SUD mask their autism for a long time, New York: Guilford Press.
using their SUD to facilitate their social skills. Bien, T. H., Miller, W. R., & Tonigan, J. S. (1993).
Brief interventions for alcohol problems: A review.
Lalanne et al. (2015) reported the cases of two
Addiction, 88, 315–335.
patients with comorbid ASD and SUD, who Brunetti, M., Di Tizio, L., Dezi, S., Pozzi, G., Grandinetti,
were diagnosed a long time after their first medi- P., & Martinotti, G. (2012). Aripiprazole, alcohol
cal visit. Awareness raising and training of care- and substance abuse: A review. European Review
for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences, 16(10),
givers should improve the detection of patients
1346–1354.
with ASD. In particular, it would be very easy to Callaghan, R. C., Veldhuizen, S., Jeysingh, T., Orlan, C.,
raise awareness about the idea that patients with Graham, C., Kakouris, G., et al. (2013). Patterns of
ASD may abuse drugs to enhance their social tobacco-related mortality among individuals diag-
nosed with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or depres-
skills, fill their days, or cope with everyday
sion. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 48(1), 102–110.
stress. Such a profile of substance use disorder Carminati, G. G., Deriaz, N., & Bertschy, G. (2006).
could guide professionals toward this diagnosis Low-­ dose venlafaxine in three adolescents and
with the help of questionnaires and thorough young adults with autistic disorder improves self-
injurious behavior and attention deficit/hyperactiv-
clinical investigation.
ity disorders ­ (ADHD)-like symptoms. Progress
in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological
Psychiatry, 30(2), 312–315.
References Carminati, G. G., Gerber, F., Darbellay, B., Kosel,
M. M., Deriaz, N., Chabert, J., et al. (2016).
Akerele, E., & Levin, F. R. (2007). Comparison of olan- Using venlafaxine to treat behavioral disorders in
zapine to risperidone in substance-abusing individu- patients with autism spectrum disorder. Progress
als with schizophrenia. The American Journal of in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological
Addictions, 16(4), 260–268. Psychiatry, 65, 85–95.
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 391

Clarke, T., Tickle, A., & Gillott, A. (2016). Substance use among adults with and without autism spectrum disor-
disorder in Asperger syndrome: An investigation into der. Psychological Science, 26(8), 1187–1200.
the development and maintenance of substance use Engelhardt, C. R., Mazurek, M. O., & Sohl, K. (2013).
disorder by individuals with a diagnosis of Asperger Media use and sleep among boys with autism spectrum
syndrome. The International Journal on Drug Policy, disorder, ADHD, or typical development. Pediatrics,
27, 154–163. 132(6), 1081–1089.
Compton, W. M., Thomas, Y. F., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, Feeney, G. F., Young, R. M., Connor, J. P., Tucker, J., &
B. F. (2007). Prevalence, correlates, disability, and McPherson, A. (2002). Cognitive behavioural therapy
comorbidity of DSM-IV drug abuse and dependence combined with the relapse-prevention medication
in the United States: Results from the national epi- acamprosate: Are short-term treatment outcomes for
demiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. alcohol dependence improved? The Australian and
Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(5), 566–576. New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 622–628.
Copello, A. G., Velleman, R. D. B., & Templeton, L. J. Feltenstein, M. W., & See, R. E. (2008). The neurocir-
(2005). Family interventions in the treatment of alco- cuitry of addiction: An overview. British Journal of
hol and drug problems. Drug and Alcohol Review, 24, Pharmacology, 154(2), 261–274.
369–385. Frye, R. E. (2014). Clinical potential, safety, and tolerabil-
Crane, L., Lind, S. E., & Bowler, D. M. (2013). ity of arbaclofen in the treatment of autism spectrum
Remembering the past and imagining the future in disorder. Journal of Drug Healthcare and Patient
autism spectrum disorder. Memory, 21(2), 157–166. Safety, 6, 69–76.
Davidson, L. (2005). Recovery in serious mental ill- Gadke, D. L., McKinney, C., & Oliveros, A. (2016).
ness: A new wine or just a new bottle? Professional Autism spectrum disorder symptoms and comorbid-
Psychology: Research and Practice, 36(5), 480–487. ity in emerging adults. Child Psychiatry and Human
Davies, S. P. (2013). Still not clear that smoking cessa- Development, 47(2), 194–201.
tion drugs do not cause psychiatric symptoms. British Gerra, G., Di Petta, G., D'Amore, A., Iannotta, P.,
Medical Journal, 347, f7065. Bardicchia, F., Falorni, F., et al. (1985). Endorphin
Dawe, S., Gerada, C., Russell, M. A., & Gray, J. A. (1995). activity in childhood psychosis: Spinal levels of 24
Nicotine intake in smokers increases following a sin- cases. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 780–783.
gle dose of haloperidol. Psychopharmacology, 117(1), Ghaziuddin, M., Ghaziuddin, N., & Greden, J. (2002).
110–115. Depression in persons with autism: Implications for
De Alwis, D., Agrawal, A., Reiersen, A. M., Constantino, research and clinical care. Journal of Autism and
J. N., Henders, A., Martin, N. G., & Lynskey, M. T. Developmental Disorders, 32, 299–306.
(2014). ADHD symptoms, autistic traits, and substance Gillberg, I. C., Helles, A., Billstedt, E., & Gillberg, C.
use and misuse in adult Australian twins. Journal of (2016). Boys with Asperger syndrome grow up:
Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 75(2), 211–221. Psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders 20
De La Garza, R., 2nd, Newton, T. F., & Kalechstein, A. D. years after initial diagnosis. Journal of Autism and
(2005). Risperidone diminishes cocaine-induced crav- Developmental Disorders, 46(1), 74–82.
ing. Psychopharmacology, 178, 347–350. Goodman, A. (1990). Addiction: Definition and implica-
Dobson, K. S. (2000). Handbook of cognitive-behavioral tions. British Journal of Addiction, 85, 1403–1408.
therapies (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Gould, T. J. (2010). Addiction and cognition. Addiction
Doyle, C. A., & McDougle, C. J. (2012). Pharmacotherapy Science & Clinical Practice, 5(2), 4–14.
to control behavioral symptoms in children with Grant, B. F., Harford, T. C., Muthén, B. O., Yi, H. Y.,
autism. Expert Opinion on Pharmacotherapy, 13(11), Hasin, D. S., & Stinson, F. S. (2007). DSM-IV
1615–1629. alcohol dependence and abuse: Further evidence of
Duarte, C. S., Bordin, I. A., Yazigi, L., & Mooney, validity in the general population. Drug and Alcohol
J. (2005). Factors associated with stress in mothers of Dependence, 86(2–3), 154–166.
children with autism. Autism, 9, 416–427. Grant, J. E., & Kim, S. W. (2006). Medication manage-
Ekman, E., & Hiltunen, A. J. (2015). Modified CBT using ment of pathological gambling. Minnesota Medicine,
visualization for autism Spectrum disorder (ASD), 89, 44–48.
anxiety and avoidance behavior – a quasi-experimental Graves, S. M., Rafeyan, R., Watts, J., & Napier, T. C.
open pilot study. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, (2012). Mirtazapine, and mirtazapine-like compounds
56(6), 641–648. as possible pharmacotherapy for substance abuse
Engelhardt, C. R., & Mazurek, M. O. (2014). Video game disorders: Evidence from the bench and the bedside.
access, parental rules, and problem behavior: A study Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 136(3), 343–353.
of boys with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 18(5), Groenman, A. P., Oosterlaan, J., Rommelse, N., Franke,
529–537. B., Roeyers, H., et al. (2013). Substance use disor-
Engelhardt, C. R., Mazurek, M. O., Hilgard, J., Rouder, ders in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactiv-
J. N., & Bartholow, B. D. (2015). Effects of violent-­ ity disorder: A 4-year follow-up study. Addiction, 108,
video-­game exposure on aggressive behavior, 1503–1511.
aggressive-­thought accessibility, and aggressive affect
392 L. Lalanne et al.

Hill, E. L., & Frith, U. (2003). Understanding autism: dependency in patients with schizophrenia. Human
Insights from mind and brain. Philosophical Psychopharmacology, 25(2), 187–190.
Transacions of the Royal Society Lond B Biological Kohls, G., Peltzer, J., Schulte-Rüther, M., Kamp-Becker,
Sciences, 358(1430), 281–289. I., Remschmidt, H., Herpertz-Dahlmann, B., &
Hofvander, B., Delorme, R., Chaste, P., Nydén, A., Wentz, Konrad, K. (2011). Atypical brain responses to reward
E., Ståhlberg, O., et al. (2009). Psychiatric and psy- cues in autism as revealed by event-related potentials.
chosocial problems in adults with normal-intelligence Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
autism spectrum disorders. BMC Psychiatry, 10, 9–35. 41(11), 1523–1533.
Houben, K., Havermans, R. C., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, Kohls, G., Schulte-Rüther, M., Nehrkorn, B., Müller, K.,
A. (2012). Beer à no-go: Learning to stop responding Fink, G. R., Kamp-Becker, I., et al. (2013). Reward
to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced system dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders.
affective associations rather than increased response Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(5),
inhibition. Addiction, 107(7), 1280–1287. 565–572.
Houben, K., Nederkoon, C., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. Koob, G. F., & Le Moal, M. (2008). Review.
(2011). Resisting temptation: Decreasing alcohol-­ Neurobiological mechanisms for opponent moti-
related affect and drinking behaviour by training vational processes in addiction. Philosophical
response inhibition. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, Transactions of the Royal Society Lond B Biological
116, 132–136. Sciences, 363(1507), 3113–3123.
Houben, K., Wiers, R. W., & Jansen, A. (2011). Getting a Koob, G. F., & Volkow, N. D. (2010). Neurocircuitry
grip on drinking behavior: Training working memory of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology, 35(1),
to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychological Science, 22(7), 217–238.
986–975. Krentzman, A. R., Robinson, E. A. R., Moore, B. C.,
Hsia, Y., Wong, A. Y., Murphy, D. G., Simonoff, Kelly, J. F., Laudet, A. B., White, W. L., et al. (2010).
E., Buitelaar, J. K., & Wong, I. C. (2014). How alcoholics Anonymous (AA) and narcotics
Psychopharmacological prescriptions for people with Anonymous (NA) work: Cross-disciplinary perspec-
autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A multinational tives. Alcoholism Treatment Quartely, 29(1), 75–84.
study. Psychopharmacology, 231(6), 999–1009. Kronenberg, L. M., Goossens, P. J., van Busschbach,
Husky, M. M., Mazure, C. M., Paliwal, P., & McKee, S. J., van Achterberg, T., & van den Brink, W. (2015).
A. (2008). Gender differences in the comorbidity of Coping styles in substance use disorder (SUD) patients
smoking behavior and major depression. Drug and with and without co-occurring attention deficit/hyper-
Alcohol Dependence, 93(1-2), 176–179. activity disorder (ADHD) or autism spectrum disorder
Jarvis, T., Tebbutt, J., & Mattick, R. P. (2005). Treatment (ASD). BMC Psychiatry, 15, 159.
approaches for alcohol and drug dependence: An Kronenberg, L. M., Slager-Visscher, K., Goossens, P. J.
introductory guide (2nd ed.). Chichester, UK: Wiley. J., van den Brink, W., & van Achterberg, T. (2014).
Jhanjee, S. (2014). Evidence based psychosocial interven- Everyday life consequences of substance use disorder
tions in substance use. Indian Journal of Psychological (SUD) and co-occurring attention deficit/hyperactivity
Medicine, 36(2), 112–118. disorder (ADHD) or autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
Karila, L., Reynaud, M., Aubin, H. J., Rolland, B., in adult patients. BMC Psychiatry, 14, 264.
Guardia, D., Cottencin, O., & Benyamina, A. (2011). Kumar, B., Prakash, A., Sewal, R. K., Medhi, B., & Modi,
Pharmacological treatments for cocaine dependence: M. (2012). Drug therapy in autism: A present and
Is there something new? Current Pharmaceutical future perspective. Pharmacological Reports, 64(6),
Design, 17(14), 1359–1368. 1291–1304.
Karila, L., Zarmdini, R., Petit, A., Lafaye, G., Lowenstein, Lalanne, L., Weiner, L., Trojak, B., Berna, F., & Bertschy,
W., & Reynaud, M. (2014). Cocaine addiction: Current G. (2015). Substance-use disorder in high-functioning
data for the clinician. Presse Médicale, 43(1), 9–17. autism: clinical and neurocognitive insights from two
Katusic, S. K., Barbaresi, W. J., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, case reports. BMC Psychiatry, 15(1), 149.
A. L., Leibson, C. L., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2005). Lalanne, L., Laprevote, V., Danion, J. M., & Bacon, E.
Psychostimulant treatment and risk for substance (2016). Theoretical reflection on the place of memory
abuse among young adults with a history of attention-­ and temporal cognitive mechanisms in addictive disor-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A population-based, ders. Encephale, 42(3), 264–269.
birth cohort study. Journal of Child and Adolescent Lang, J. P., Bonnewitz, M. L., Kusterer, M., & Lalanne-­
Psychopharmacology, 15, 764–776. Tongio, L. (2014). Alcohol consumption in patients
Keltner, N. L., Coffeen, H., & Johnson, J. E. (2000). with psychiatric disorders: Assessment and treatment.
Atypical antipsychotic drugs: Part 1. Perspectives in Encephale, 40(4), 301–307. French.
Psychiatric Care, 36(3), 101–104. Lesouef, N., Bellet, F., Mounier, G., & Beyens, M. N.
Kern, A. M., Akerman, S. C., & Nordstrom, B. R. (2014). (2014). Efficacy of baclofen on abstinence and craving
Opiate dependence in schizophrenia: Case presenta- in alcohol-dependent patients: A meta-analysis of ran-
tion and literature review. Journal of Dual Diagnosis, domized controlled trials. Thérapie, 69(5), 427–435.
10(1), 52–57. Levaux, M. N., Larøi, F., Malmedier, M., Offerlin-Meyer,
Kim, S. H., Han, D. H., Joo, S. Y., & Min, K. J. (2010). I., Danion, J. M., & Van der Linden, M. (2012).
The effect of dopamine partial agonists on the nicotine Rehabilitation of executive functions in a real-life set-
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 393

ting: Goal management training applied to a person with autism: A population-based study. Pediatrics,
with schizophrenia. Case Reports in Psychiatry, 2012, 119(5), 1040–1046.
503023. Mulligan, R. C., Reiersen, A. M., & Todorov, A. A.
Levin, F. R., Evans, S. M., Brooks, D. J., & Garawi, F. (2014). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, autis-
(2007). Treatment of cocaine dependent treatment tic traits, and substance use among Missouri adoles-
seekers with adult ADHD: Double-blind comparison cents. Scandinavian Journal of Child and Adolescent
of methylphenidate and placebo. Drug and Alcohol Psychiatry and Psychology, 2(2), 86–92.
Dependence, 87, 20–29. O’Malley, S. S., Jaffe, A. J., Chang, G., Schottenfeld, R. S.,
Levy, S., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., Meyer, R. E., & Rounsaville, B. (1992). Naltrexone
Killian, J. M., Voigt, R. G., & Barbaresi, W. J. (2014). and coping skills therapy for alcohol dependence. A
Childhood ADHD and risk for substance dependence controlled study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 49,
in adulthood: A longitudinal, population-based study. 881–887.
PloS One, 9(8), e105640. Otero, T. L., Schatz, R. B., Merrill, A. C., & Bellini, S.
Li, C. S., Morgan, P. T., Matuskey, D., Abdelghany, O., (2015). Social skills training for youth with autism
Luo, X., Chang, J. L., et al. (2010). Biological mark- spectrum disorders: A follow-up. Child and Adolescent
ers of the effects of intravenous methylphenidate on Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 24(1), 99–115.
improving inhibitory control in cocaine-dependent Oudman, E., Nijboer, T. C., Postma, A., Wijnia, J. W., &
patients. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Van der Stigchel, S. (2015). Procedural learning and
Sciences U S A, 107, 14455–14459. memory rehabilitation in Korsakoff's syndrome – a
Longo, L. P. (2002). Olanzapine for cocaine craving and review of the literature. Neuropsychology Review,
relapse prevention in 2 patients. Journal of Clinical 25(2), 134–148.
Psychiatry, 63(7), 595–596. Pearson, D. A., Santos, C. W., Aman, M. G., Arnold, L. E.,
Maisel, N. C., Blodgett, J. C., Wilbourne, P. L., Casat, C. D., Mansour, R., et al. (2013). Effects of
Humphreys, K., & Finney, J. W. (2013). Meta-analysis extended release methylphenidate treatment on ratings
of naltrexone and acamprosate for treating alcohol use of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)
disorders: when are these medications most helpful? and associated behavior in children with autism spec-
Addiction, 108(2), 275–293. trum disorders and ADHD symptoms. Journal of
Marazziti, D., Presta, S., Baroni, S., Mungai, F., Piccinni, Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 23(5),
A., Mucci, F., & Dell’Osso, L. (2015). Nalmefene: 337–351.
A novel drug for an old disorder. Current Medicinal Pitel, A. L., Rivier, J., Beaunieux, H., Vabret, F.,
Chemistry, 22(27), 3162–3168. Desgranges, B., & Eustache, F. (2009). Changes in
Matson, J. L., & Cervantes, P. E. (2014). Commonly stud- the episodic memory and executive functions of absti-
ied comorbid psychopathologies among persons with nent and relapsed alcoholics over a 6-month period.
autism spectrum disorder. Research in Developmental Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research,
Disabilities, 35, 952–962. 33(3), 490–498.
Mazurek, M. O., & Engelhardt, C. R. (2013). Video game Prendeville, J. A., Prelock, P. A., & Unwin, G. (2006).
use in boys with autism spectrum disorder, ADHD, or Peer play interventions to support the social compe-
typical development. Pediatrics, 132(2), 260–266. tence of children with autism spectrum disorders.
Mazurek, M. O., & Wenstrup, C. (2013). Television, video Seminars in Speech and Language, 27(1), 32–46.
game and social media use among children with ASD Procyshyn, R. M., Ihsan, N., & Thompson, D. (2001). A
and typically developing siblings. Journal of Autism comparison of smoking behaviours between patients
and Developmental Disorders, 43(6), 1258–1271. treated with clozapine and depot neuroleptics. Internal
McCormick, P. N., Wilson, V. S., Wilson, A. A., & Clinical Psychopharmacology, 16(5), 291–294.
Remington, G. J. (2013). Acutely administered anti- Ramos, M., Boada, L., Moreno, C., Llorente, C., Romo,
psychotic drugs are highly selective for dopamine D2 J., & Parellada, M. (2013). Attitude and risk of sub-
over D3 receptors. Pharmacological Research, 70(1), stance use in adolescents diagnosed with Asperger
66–71. syndrome. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 133(2),
Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2012). Meeting in the mid- 535–540.
dle: motivational interviewing and self-determination Rommelse, N., Langerak, I., van der Meer, J., de Bruijn,
theory. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition Y., Staal, W., Oerlemans, A., & Buitelaar, J. (2015).
and Physical Activity, 9(1), 25. Intelligence may moderate the cognitive profile of
Molero, Y., Lichtenstein, P., Zetterqvist, J., Gumpert, patients with ASD. PloS One, 10(10), e0138698.
C. H., & Fazel, S. (2015). Varenicline and risk of Rossignol, D. A. (2009). Novel and emerging treatments
psychiatric conditions, suicidal behaviour, crimi- for autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review.
nal offending, and transport accidents and offences: Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 21(4), 213–236.
Population based cohort study. British Medical Roy, A., Roy, M., Deb, S., Unwin, G., & Roy, A. (2015).
Journal, 350, h2388. Are opioid antagonists effective in attenuating the core
Montes, G., & Halterman, J. S. (2007). Psychological symptoms of autism spectrum conditions in children:
functioning and coping among mothers of children A systematic review. Journal of Intellectual Disability
Research, 59(4), 293–306.
394 L. Lalanne et al.

Roy, A., Roy, M., Deb, S., Unwin, G., & Roy, A. (2015). Smedslund, G., Berg, R. C., Hammerstrøm, K. T.,
Are opioid antagonists effective in reducing self-injury Steiro, A., Leiknes, K. A., Dahl, H. M., et al. (2011).
in adults with intellectual disability? A systematic Motivational interviewing for substance abuse.
review. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 5,
59(1), 55–67. CD008063.
Rupp, C. I., Kemmler, G., Kurz, M., Hinterhuber, H., & Smith, L. E., Greenberg, J. S., & Mailick, M. R. (2012).
Fleischhacker, W. W. (2012). Cognitive remediation Adults with autism: Outcomes, family effects, and the
therapy during treatment for alcohol dependence. multi-family group psychoeducation model. Current
Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 73(4), Psychiatry Reports, 14(6), 732–738.
625–634. Smith, M. D., & Philippen, L. R. (2005). Community inte-
Salzer, M. S., Mcfadden, L., & Rappaport, J. (1994). gration and supported employment. In D. Zager (Ed.),
Professional views of self help groups. Administration Autism spectrum disorders: Identification education
and Policy in Mental Health, 22(2), 85–95. and treatment (pp. 493–514). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
San, L., Arranz, B., & Martinez-Raga, J. (2007). Erlbaum Associates.
Antipsychotic drug treatment of schizophrenic Soderstrom, H., Rastam, M., & Gillberg, C. (2002).
patients with substance abuse disorders. European Temperament and character in adults with Asperger
Addiction Research, 13(4), 230–243. syndrome. Autism, 6(3), 287–297.
Santosh, P. J., Baird, G., Pityaratstian, N., Tavare, E., & Sofuoglu, M., DeVito, E. E., Waters, A. J., & Carroll,
Gringras, P. (2006). Impact of comorbid autism spec- K. M. (2013). Cognitive enhancement as a treat-
trum disorders on stimulant response in children with ment for drug addictions. Neuropharmacology, 64,
attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A retrospec- 452–463.
tive and prospective effectiveness study. Child: Care, Stoops, W. W., Lile, J. A., Lofwall, M. R., & Rush, C. R.
Health and Development, 32(5), 575–583. (2007). The safety, tolerability, and subject-rated
Santosh, P. J., & Mijovic, A. (2006). Does pervasive effects of acute intranasal cocaine administration dur-
developmental disorder protect children and adoles- ing aripiprazole maintenance. The American Journal
cents against drug and alcohol use? European Child & of Drug and Alcohol Abuse, 33, 769–776.
Adolescent Psychiatry, 15, 183–188. Tinsley, M., Hendrickx, S. (2008). Asperger syndrome
Scott, L. J., & Dhillon, S. (2008). Spotlight on risperi- and alcohol: Drinking to cope ? Kingsley SJ. London.
done in irritability associated with autistic disorder in 2008, 9.
children and adolescents. CNS Drugs, 22(3), 259–262. Thoma, P., & Daum, I. (2013). Comorbid substance use
Scott-Van Zeeland, A. A., Dapretto, M., Ghahremani, disorder in schizophrenia: A selective overview of neu-
D. G., Poldrack, R. A., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2010). robiological and cognitive underpinnings. Psychiatry
Reward processing in autism. Autism Research, 3(2), and Clinical Neurosciences, 67(6), 367–383.
53–67. Thomas, K. H., Martin, R. M., Davies, N. M., Metcalfe,
Seeman, P. (2014). Clozapine, a fast-off-D2 antipsychotic. C., Windmeijer, F., & Gunnell, D. (2013). Smoking
ACS Chemical Neuroscience, 5(1), 24–29. cessation treatment and risk of depression, suicide,
Segobin, S. H., Chételat, G., Le Berre, A. P., Lannuzel, C., and self harm in the clinical practice research datalink:
Boudehent, C., Vabret, F., et al. (2014). Relationship Prospective cohort study. British Medical Journal,
between brain volumetric changes and interim drink- 347, f5704.
ing at six months in alcohol-dependent patients. Thomas, K. H., Martin, R. M., Knipe, D. W., Higgins,
Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, J. P. T., & Gunnell, D. (2015). Risk of neuropsy-
38(3), 739–748. chiatric adverse events associated with varenicline:
Sharma, A., & Shaw, S. R. (2012). Efficacy of risperidone systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ, 350,
in managing maladaptive behaviors for children with h1109–h1109.
autistic spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal Torrens, M., Rossi, P. C., Martinez-Riera, R., Martinez-­
of Pediatric Health Care, 26(4), 291–299. Sanvisens, D., & Bulbena, A. (2012). Psychiatric
Sizoo, B., van den Brink, W., Gorissen van Eenige, M., co-morbidity and substance use disorders: Treatment
& van der Gaag, R. J. (2009). Personality characteris- in parallel systems or in one integrated system?
tics of adults with autism spectrum disorders or atten- Substance Use & Misuse, 47(8–9), 1005–1014.
tion deficit hyperactivity disorder with and without Ung, D., Selles, R., Small, B. J., & Storch, E. A.
substance use disorders. The Journal of Nervous and (2015). A systematic review and meta-analysis of
Mental Disease, 197(6), 450–454. cognitive-­behavioral therapy for anxiety in youth
Sizoo, B., van den Brink, W., Koeter, M., Gorissen van with high-­ functioning autism Spectrum disorders.
Eenige, M., van Wijngaarden-Cremers, P., & van der Child Psychiatry and Human Developement, 46(4),
Gaag, R. J. (2010). Treatment seeking adults with 533–547.
autism or ADHD and co-morbid substance use disor- Upadhyaya, H. P., Rose, K., Wang, W., O’Rourke,
der: Prevalence, risk factors and functional disability. K., Sullivan, B., Deas, D., & Brady, K. T. (2005).
Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 107(1), 44–50. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, medica-
Skinner, M. D., Lahmek, P., Pham, H., & Aubin, H. J. tion treatment, and substance use patterns among
(2014). Disulfiram efficacy in the treatment of alcohol adolescents and young adults. Journal of Child and
dependence: A meta-analysis. PloS One, 9(2), e87366. Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15, 799–809.
21  Treatment of Addiction in Adults with ASD 395

Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic inter- Wijesundera, H., Hanwella, R., & de Silva, V. A. (2014).
vention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis, Antipsychotic medication and tobacco use among out-
meta-regression and dose–response meta-analysis patients with schizophrenia: A cross-sectional study.
of multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, Annals of General Psychiatry, 13(1), 7.
30(4), 387–399. Wood, J. J., Ehrenreich-May, J., Alessandri, M., Fujii,
Wallace, G. L., Kenworthy, L., Pugliese, C. E., Popal, C., Renno, P., Laugeson, E., et al. (2015). Cognitive
H. S., White, E. I., Brodsky, E., & Martin, A. (2015. behavioral therapy for early adolescents with autism
Epub ahead of print). Real-world executive functions spectrum disorders and clinical anxiety: A random-
in adults with autism spectrum disorder: Profiles of ized, controlled trial. Behavior Therapy, 46(1), 7–19.
impairment and associations with adaptive function- Wu, B. J., Chen, H. K., & Lee, S. M. (2013). Do atypi-
ing and co-morbid anxiety and depression. Journal of cal antipsychotics really enhance smoking reduction
Autism and Developmental Disorders. more than typical ones?: The effects of antipsychot-
Weiner, L., Bruckmann, J., & Bizet, E. (2010). Cognitive ics on smoking reduction in patients with schizophre-
remediation therapy of working memory and execu- nia. Journal of Clinical Psychopharmacology, 33(3),
tive dysfunction in Asperger Syndrome: Premilinary 319–328.
results from single-case studies. Mid-Year Meeting of Wykes, T., Huddy, V., Cellard, C., McGurk, S. R., &
the International Neuropsychological Society, July the Czobor, P. (2011). A meta-analysis of cognitive reme-
2nd. Krakow, Poland. diation for schizophrenia: Methodology and effect
Wiers, R. W., Houben, K., Fadardi, J. S., van Beek, P., sizes. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(5),
Rhemtulla, M., & Cox, W. M. (2015). Alcohol cog- 472–485.
nitive bias modification training for problem drinkers Wynn, T., & Brunetti, S. (2009). Compounding naltrexone
over the web. Addictive Behaviors, 40, 21–26. for the treatment of autism. International Journal of
Wiers, C. E., Ludwig, V. U., Gladwin, T. E., Park, Pharmaceutical Compounding, 13(4), 296–299.
S. Q., Heinz, A., Wiers, R. W., et al. (2015). Effects Zhang, L., Tang, J., Dong, Y., Ji, Y., Tao, R., Liang, Z.,
of cognitive bias modification training on neural et al. (2015). Similarities and differences in decision-­
signatures of alcohol approach tendencies in male making impairments between autism Spectrum dis-
alcohol-­dependent patients. Addiction Biology, 20(5), order and schizophrenia. Frontiers in Behavioral
990–999. Neuroscience, 9, 259.
Diet and Supplementation
Targeted for Autism Spectrum 22
Disorder

Mark J. Garcia, Pamela McPherson, Stuti Y. Patel,


and Claire O. Burns

supplements as cures for autism; still dietary


 iet and Supplementation Targeted
D interventions remain popular. Survey data that
for ASD evidences this point is found in the parental use
of elimination diets presented by the Autism
Popular culture is replete with stories of cures for Research Institute report titled Parent Rating of
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) based on food. Behavior Effects of Biomedical Interventions
While not a core symptom of ASD, food and (Edelson, 2010). This report documents survey
feeding are prominent concerns. Kanner (1943) data on parental perception of medication, sup-
is among the first to note food-related challenges plements, and elimination diets over the last half
in his study group of children with ASD. Then, century. Thousands of these surveys report chil-
diet-based interventions became topical in 1969 dren with ASD “got better” when parents imple-
when Goodwin and Goodwin reported a success- mented an elimination diet. Depending on the
ful diet-based intervention for a child with both diet implemented, 45–71% of parents rated
celiac disease and ASD using a gluten-free diet. improvement versus only 2–7% rating “got
Since then parents have been drawn to dietary worse,” which is a trend that is significantly dif-
and supplementation interventions with the hope ferent for pharmacological interventions. An
of a cure for ASD. However, prevailing scientific obvious difference between diets and medication
evidence does not support elimination diets or is the familiarity, availability, and ease with
which an elimination diet can be implemented.
This can be taken to mean that parents are willing
to try what is available and do so independent of
professional assistance. In addition, this survey
M.J. Garcia (*) • S.Y. Patel data highlights parent’s willingness to try even
Northwest Resource Center, hazardous interventions. For example, parents
5401 Shed Road, Bossier City, LA 71111, USA reported high use and parental satisfaction with
e-mail: mark.garcia@la.gov chelation therapy, an experimental treatment for
P. McPherson ASD that has been linked to fatalities (Baxter &
Northwest Louisiana Human Services District, Krenzelok, 2008). Overall, this survey data high-
2924 Knight Street, Suite 350, Shreveport,
LA 71105, USA lights parental dedication and commitment to
making significant lifestyle changes for their
C.O. Burns
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State loved ones with the hope of improving ASD
University, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA symptoms.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 397


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_22
398 M.J. Garcia et al.

This chapter seeks to discuss diet and supple- Advances in food science have sparked popu-
mentation and ASD as it relates to nutritional lar dietary movements such as the idea of “func-
health, assessment of nutritional health, empiri- tional foods,” which has been gaining popular
cal evidence, GI disorder impact on ASD, sup- attention since the 1980s (Yao et al., 2012).
port for those considering these types of Functional food is defined as natural or formu-
intervention, and the future direction of this field. lated nutritious foods consumed as part of a daily
Our goal is to acknowledge the following reali- diet, i.e., not a capsule, tablet, or powder, that
ties: (1) nutrition is important; (2) current contain ingredients that may provide a health
research does not support dietary interventions as benefit beyond the traditional nutrients that it
a specific cure for ASD; (3) individuals with contains and thus contribute to the prevention of
ASD are at significant risk of food-related comor- nutrition-related diseases, the increase in physi-
bidities; (4) overall physical, emotional, and cal health, and the promotion of mental well-­
behavioral health can be improved by addressing being (Crowe & Francis, 2013). An example
these food-related comorbidities; (5) families are would be the cardiac benefits of oatmeal. The
willing to try what is available and will need concept of food as medicine is a mainstay of
guidance to match interventions and evaluate complementary, alternative, and integrative
intervention efficacy; (6) well-aware clinicians health approaches, which advocates nontradi-
can offer this guidance; and (7) research is con- tional interventions, such as diet and supplemen-
stantly evolving our understanding of how food is tation, to heal and promote health (Ernst, 2012).
related to ASD. Scientists are beginning to identify how food
and ASD are related. Still, empirical connections
between food, supplements, and dietary interven-
Nutritional Health tions for ASD are premature (Buie et al., 2010).
However, knowledge of this connection is accu-
Food Is Medicine mulating rapidly with the work addressing medi-
cal disorders. The high incidence of
The idea of food as medicine is old, dating from food-related-ASD comorbid medical disorders
ancient cultures as far back as 1000 B.C. (Yao, and related symptoms is informing our under-
Hao, Pan, & Wang, 2012). Since this time, fami- standing of how food may affect behavior. For
lies have been waiting impatiently for science to example, research into gastrointestinal dysfunc-
explain the brain-gut connection. While science tion has recently helped to identify the robust
unlocks these mysteries, families must remember connection between gastrointestinal disorders
that as with all medicine, food can be seen to and ASD. McElhanon, McCracken, Karpen, and
have three effects – the benefits of nourishment, Sharp (2014) recently presented evidence that
growth, and repair; side effects such as gastroin- individuals with ASD are at a higher risk of
testinal upset, insomnia, or cavities; and adverse developing gastrointestinal disorders, which was
reactions such as life-threatening interaction with previously only a hypothesis. Leading gastroen-
medications or allergies. When thinking of food terologists, including Buie et al. (2010), agree on
side effects, consider that side effects are not bad, several points related to gastrointestinal disor-
merely unintended effects of the food. For exam- ders: (1) GI dysfunctions need to be addressed,
ple, weight gain for an under-eater would be a (2) individuals with ASD experience the full
positive outcome. Gaining weight for this person range of GI dysfunction, (3) the remarkable prev-
is a good thing, whereas for others it will be non- alence rate of GI dysfunction in individuals with
optimal. A food adverse reaction will include ASD, and (4) there is no ASD-specific GI distur-
food allergies, metabolic disease like diabetes, bance. Additionally, there is a strong consensus
and gastrointestinal disease like Crohn’s or celiac that comorbid gastrointestinal disorders may be
disease. Here the food is not only having an unin- increasing challenges faced by individuals with
tended effect, it is causing harm. ASD and disorders such as constipation, diar-
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 399

rhea, acid reflux, and food allergies should be Dietary Considerations: Children
routinely assessed.
It is likely that dietary and supplementation The balanced diet provides the necessary energy
interventions will have its greatest benefits for and building materials for the exceptional growth
individuals with ASD in the management of and development of childhood. Growth of the
food-related comorbid conditions. Fortunately, body and brain in childhood is rapid and nonlin-
there is strong empirical evidence that comorbid ear. Therefore, not only are sufficient proteins,
gastrointestinal symptoms are improved by carbohydrates, fats, vitamins, minerals, fiber, and
dietary and supplementary interventions. It fol- water necessary, these building blocks must be
lows that, as the individual returns to health, available during critical growth periods. Specific
improvements in behavioral challenges and to the brain, fatty acids, omega-3 and omega-6,
return of functional skills will also be seen. are required during the rapid brain growth of
Therefore, there is utility in implementing dietary young children, whereas for adults the demand is
and supplementation interventions and thus a relatively less. Researchers have not yet discov-
need to consider what effects, side effects, and/or ered the complex algorithm defining these needs;
adverse reactions might the given individual with therefore, a steady supply of dietary resources
ASD experience when considering food as medi- must be ingested daily to ensure healthy growth
cine. In other words, the goal is to identify what and development (Prado, 2014). Obtaining an
constitutes a healthy diet, or optimal nutrition, adequate supply is further complicated by spe-
for a specific individual. cific nutrient demands through the day, with it
necessary to have sufficient nutritional supply
available during the day to optimize learning and
Nutrition: “The Balanced Diet” then the night to supply rapid growth periods
(Taki, 2012). Attainment of adequate nutrition is
Optimal nutrition for a specific individual further complicated by the core symptoms of
begins with an understanding of basic nutri- ASD. For example, children with ASD are found
tional requirements. Food is simultaneously more likely to have protein and calcium deficien-
basic and complex. At its most basic, eating is cies because of food selectivity (Sharp et al.,
the first milestone in infancy, and food provides 2013). Obesity is also a concern that often pres-
the nutrition for life. The last century heralded ents as a challenge in preschool children due to
unparalleled nutritional discoveries. With these core symptoms of ASD (Presmanes Hill,
discoveries, the concept of a balanced diet has Zuckerman, & Fombonne, 2015). To aid in devel-
evolved. The identification of vitamins, miner- oping a balanced diet for all children and teens,
als, essential amino acids, and essential fatty with and without ASD, the American Academy
acids as critical dietary needs has informed the of Pediatrics website healthychildren.org offers
US Department of Agriculture (USDA) concept basic nutrition information (American Academy
of a healthy diet. For over 120 years, the USDA of Pediatrics, 2016).
has published dietary guidelines (Davis, 1999).
As science has expanded our understanding of
nutrition, the four food groups and food pyra-  ietary Considerations: Adolescents/
D
mid of our youth have given way to My Plate in Young Adults
the most recent revision of the USDA dietary
guidelines (USDA, 2016). The guidelines pro- Dietary needs shift as brain maturation and phys-
mote an understanding of the importance of bal- ical development accelerate during puberty with
ancing the nutritional and energy needs of our increased nutritional demands to fuel rapid bone
body with the nutrition and energy supplied by and muscle growth in the early teens and brain
our diet. reorganization through the early twenties. While
400 M.J. Garcia et al.

calorie demands are greater for young men, these diseases. As adults with ASD achieve inde-
young women entering puberty have greater pendence, guidance on diet and a healthy life-
demands for iron and calcium. Inadequate cal- style is critical. Because nutritional needs vary
cium during adolescence can increase risk for with age, activity level, pregnancy, illness and
osteoporosis later in life. Poor eating habits dur- medication, diet, and lifestyle, education is a life-
ing childhood may result in the emergence of long necessity. For individuals with mental ill-
health issues during the teen years. While obesity ness, years of psychotropic medication use also
is a growing concern for all age groups, teens places them at risk. For example, antipsychotic
with ASD are at increased risk for lifelong strug- medication use increases the risk for obesity, dia-
gles with weight and the associated immediate betes, and related conditions. Therefore, medica-
and long-term health risks (Phillips et al., 2014). tion education should also include suggestions
Obesity is not the only challenge youth with ASD for diet to promote a healthy lifestyle. The
may face. Adolescents with ASD are at increased Academic Autistic Spectrum Partnership in
risk for medical conditions including diabetes, Research and Education (AASPIRE) Healthcare
seizures, muscular dystrophies, and schizophre- Toolkit is a resource designed to improve health
nia (Kohane, 2012). Accordingly, these comorbid care for adults with ASD and their health care
conditions will need special dietary consider- providers (Academic Autistic Spectrum
ations. As mentioned above the American Partnership in Research and Education, 2015).
Academy of Pediatrics website healthychildren. More nutrition information targeting specific
org offers basic nutrition information (American issues faced by adults is available at nutrition.gov
Academy of Pediatrics, 2016). (USDA, 2016).

Dietary Considerations: Adults Assessment of Nutritional Status

Adults with ASD face the challenge of balancing Our bodies and our mental processes are the
caloric intake and activity shared by all adults but result of complex biochemical interactions that
complicated by the core symptoms of ASD. By depend on nutrients and adequate hydration. The
this time food selectivity and sedentary lifestyles morbidity associated with nutritional deficiencies
have become resistant to change at a time when highlights the importance of nutritional assess-
the high calorie demands of physical growth in ment. Nutrients such as protein, fats, carbohy-
the late teens have significantly decreased, pos- drates, vitamins, and minerals provide the
ing an increased risk of poor overall health. As building blocks for cells and the energy to power
adults with ASD age, they are at an increased risk the millions of chemical reactions in the human
of obesity, heart disease, diabetes, immune disor- body, and nearly all these processes need water.
ders, mental illness, and even dental issues Our body is able to synthesize some nutrients,
(Croen & Zerbo, 2015; Tyler et al. 2011). Dental called nonessential nutrients, while the essential
issues are an inconspicuous concern yet relevant nutrients must be consumed by our diet. To aid in
because adults with ASD have reported experi- meeting these nutritional needs, several agencies
encing greater pain and anxiety during dental have published guidelines. The USDA publishes
care which makes diagnosing periodontal disease basic dietary guidelines. Specific guidelines for
more difficult (Blomqvist, Dahllöf, & Bejerot, the assessment of nutritional status are published
2014). Periodontal disease is an additional con- by the American Academy of Pediatrics, the
cern as it raises the risks of poor nutrition, heart American Gastroenterological Association, and
disease, and diabetes (Cullinan, 2013). All the many other specialty organizations (see American
factors placing individuals with ASD at increased Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Nutrition,
health risk are not well understood; however, a 2013). Autism Speaks publishes more informa-
balanced diet may help to lower risk and manage tion such as synthesized nutritional research and
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 401

guidelines, making this critical information Dietary Interventions for ASD


accessible to individuals with ASD and their
families. Empirical evidence is scant for many dietary
interventions implemented in the hope of benefit-
ting individuals with ASD. The gluten-free,
 utritional Assessment of Individuals
N casein-free diet and ketogenic diet are excep-
with ASD tions. While these diets do not cure ASD or treat
core symptoms, the empirical evidence for these
Individuals with ASD are at increased risk of diets is robust.
nutrition-related health problems. Nutritional
deficiencies contribute to impaired growth, obe-
sity, anemia, low bone density, and many other Gluten-Free, Casein-Free Diet
health issues (Adams, Johansen, Powell, Quig,
& Rubin, 2011; McElhanon et al., 2014; Sharp The gluten-free, casein-free (GFCF) diets are
et al., 2013). The assessment of nutrition-related now the most commonly used dietary interven-
problems in individuals with ASD involves both tions for individuals with ASD. However, the
nutritional and medical assessment. The nutri- GFCF diet is an evidence-based medical inter-
tional assessment begins with parent education vention for the management of celiac disease
on feeding behavior of children with autism. For (Murray, Watson, Clearman, & Mitros, 2004). In
an example, see the publication Exploring persons with celiac disease, gluten and casein
Feeding Behavioral Autism: A Parent’s Guide to found in wheat, barley, and rye cause an immune
Exploring Feeding Behavior in Autism presented reaction in the gut that results in damage to the
by Autism Speaks Autism Treatment Network lining of the intestine causing GI distress
(2014). Parental observations are then completed (National Institute of Health, 2015; Tonutti &
to inform well-child pediatric visits that screen Bizzaro, 2014). Regrettably, the GFCF diet has
for nutrition-related concerns. If weight loss/ not been proven to treat ASD symptoms.
gain, restrictive eating, gastrointestinal distress, Researchers have suggested that the positive
or other food related concerns are identified, behavioral change experienced by individuals
other medical assessment is warranted. The with ASD who used the diet is directly related to
medical assessment includes a detailed history, the alleviation of GI distress (Chaidez, Hansen,
completion of a growth chart, physical examina- & Hertz-Picciotto, 2014; Elder et al., 2006;
tion, and indicated laboratory studies. Specialized Johnson, Handen, Zimmer, Sacco, & Turner,
medical or behavioral assessments may be 2010; Seung, Rogalski, Shankar, & Elder, 2007).
ordered to aid in diagnosis and treatment. The use of GFCF diets for the treatment of
Caregivers may assist the medical and behav- ASD has its basis in the 1969 Goodwin and
ioral assessments by completing a 3–5 day food/ Goodwin report of a successful diet-based inter-
eating behavior diary and a list of gastrointesti- vention for a child with both celiac disease and
nal and behavioral concerns. Teens and adults ASD using a gluten-free diet. In exploring this
might track diet and symptoms with apps to bet- relationship, Panksepp (1979) suggested the
ter inform medical decision-making. For exam- symptoms of ASD might be due to excessive
ple, the assessment and treatment of abdominal brain opioid activity. Panksepp hypothesized that
pain, constipation, diarrhea, gastroesophageal individuals with ASD had an abnormally perme-
reflux, and food allergies may be referred to a able intestinal membrane, or “leaky gut,” through
gastroenterologist. After obtaining history and which peptides with opioid activity could be
physical examination, the gastroenterologist absorbed and enter the central nervous system
might order X-rays, stool studies, and additional (D’Eufemia et al., 1996). Once in the brain, pep-
blood tests before considering endoscopy or tides with opioid activity could then result in
colonoscopy. symptoms of ASD. Specific to the GFCF diet,
402 M.J. Garcia et al.

this hypothesis posits that peptides found in glu- the use of this diet to treat behavioral symptoms
ten and casein are responsible for triggering the or improve developmental functioning in indi-
opioid reaction (Whiteley & Shattock, 2002). viduals with ASD. Still, it has been suggested
Thus, this hypothesis supported the use of the that the elimination of gluten and casein from an
GFCF diet to treat autism symptomology, the individual’s diet may positively impact a subpop-
idea being that excluding foods with gluten and ulation of individuals with ASD, but these
casein would improve ASD symptoms. There is improvements should not be generalized to the
some empirical evidence that gut permeability entire population (Pennesi & Klein, 2012) and
may be compromised in some gastrointestinal may more likely be related to the symptom relief
disorders resulting in diarrhea, constipation, and from a GI dysfunction.
gastroesophageal reflux, conditions common in The popularity of the GFCF diet may be
individuals with ASD (Adams et al., 2011). explained by several studies that have found that
However, the opioid excess hypothesis is based parental perception of efficacy can be specious.
on limited and tenuous evidence that (1) animals Researchers have found that parents may report
given exogenous opioids were accurate models improvements despite a lack of statistically sig-
for ASD, (2) abnormalities in opioid levels in nificant findings across measures (Elder et al.,
individuals with ASD could be measured, and (3) 2006). Harris and Card (2012) found that the
naltrexone, a drug that reverses the effects of opi- behavior patterns and GI symptoms of children
oids, could improve ASD symptoms (Leboyer on a GFCF diet did not differ significantly from
et al., 1990). Subsequent researchers have refuted those not on the GFCF diet. However, when
the opioid excess hypothesis by demonstrating asked, 100% of parents with children on the
that individuals with ASD do not have higher lev- GFCF diet reported both GI symptoms and
els of neuropeptides (Cass et al., 2008; Dettmer, behavior had improved on the diet. Hurwitz
Hanna, Whetstone, Hansen, & Hammock, 2007; (2013) suggested that the discrepancy between
Hunter, O’Hare, Herron, Fisher, & Jones, 2003) parent report and empirical data might be due to
and that naltrexone administration rarely parent’s high hopes for improvement and invest-
improves ASD symptoms (Feldman, Kolmen, & ment in the implementation of the GFCF diet. It
Gonzaga, 1999). has also been suggested that when parents have
Of the studies that have found positive effects expectations of improvement, they may misinter-
in behavioral symptoms (Knivsberg, Reichelt, pret variability in their child’s behavior as evi-
Høien, & Nødland, 2003; Pennesi & Klein, dence of effectiveness (Sandler & Bodfish, 2000).
2012), none have established benefit at a signifi- Further, Green (2007) explains that for parents,
cant level. Johnson et al.’s (2010) research on more time involved and more money invested
developmental outcomes and problem behaviors into an intervention result in a more favorable
found that individuals on the GFCF diet showed impression of intervention effectiveness.
some gains in developmental outcomes and prob- Interestingly, young age has been suggested to be
lem behaviors, but these improvements were not a possible predictor for being a responder to
statistically significant, and control group also GFCF diets (Pedersen, Parlar, Kvist, Whiteley, &
showed improvements in some areas. The lack of Shattock, 2013). Perhaps this finding is also
evidence for GFCF diets is further reported by related to parental misinterpretation.
research findings that gluten and casein supple-
mentation, that is, an increased intake of gluten
and casein, was not related to increased challeng- Ketogenic Diet
ing behavior or GI symptom severity
(Pusponegoro, Ismael, Firmansyah, There is a significant body of literature on the
Sastroasmoro, & Vandenplas, 2015). Overall, ketogenic diet as a method to reduce seizures.
studies are unable to find adequate evidence for Because seizures are common in individuals with
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 403

ASD, the effect of the ketogenic diet has been The following tables of dietary restriction and
explored as a treatment for ASD core symptoms supplement interventions give a brief description
(Evangeliou et al., 2003; Herbert & Buckley, of dietary interventions commonly used in the
2013). The ketogenic diet is a high-fat, low-­ ASD community. The tables note the medical
protein, and low-carbohydrate diet that is conditions, for which the dietary intervention is
designed to elicit biochemical changes that are intended, the basis for the intervention in indi-
typically associated with starvation (Freeman viduals with ASD, risks associated with the
et al., 1998). This biochemical response results in dietary restriction or supplementation, and
the replacement of glucose with ketones as the resources to learn more about the diet.
primary source of fuel for the brain (Hartman,
Gasior, Vining, & Rogawski, 2007). The use of Name of diet Gluten-free diet
the ketogenic diet to control seizures is well Basic description A gluten-free diet eliminates
established and has been supported by earlier foods containing gluten, a protein
research (Groesbeck, Bluml, & Kossoff, 2006; commonly found in wheat,
barley, and rye
Kinsman, Vining, Quaskey, Mellits, & Freeman,
Intended use ∙ Celiac disease
1992; Neal et al., 2008). Related to ASD, the
∙ Non-celiac gluten sensitivity
majority of studies that have positive effects on (NCGS)
ASD type symptoms have focused on mouse ∙ Wheat allergy
models (Ahn, Narous, Tobias, Rho, & Mychasiuk, Description of Celiac disease: a gluten-free diet
2014; Ruskin et al., 2013). However, there is still illness is prescribed for individuals with
inadequate evidence to support the effectiveness celiac disease, non-celiac gluten
sensitivity, and wheat allergy.
of this diet for humans with ASD (Castro et al.,
According to the Celiac Disease
2015). A final concern is that use of this diet is Foundation (CDF, 2016), celiac
related to known health risks such as slow growth, disease is an autoimmune
skeletal fractures, and kidney stones (Groesbeck disorder, which occurs in about
1 in 100 people. In individuals
et al., 2006).
with this genetic predisposition,
gluten damages the small
intestine (CDF, 2016)
 ommonly Used Diets in the ASD
C NCGS: the UCLA Divisions of
Community Digestive Diseases reports that
individuals with NCGS have
some of the physical symptoms
Restrictive Diets of celiac disease but a normal
small intestine biopsy
The restrictive diets, i.e., the elimination of ingre- Wheat allergy: individuals with
dients from an individual’s diet, have their place wheat allergy produce an allergic
antibody to wheat (UCLA
in treatment. They are routinely prescribed for Divisions of Digestive Diseases,
physical disorders including food allergies and 2016)
sensitivities, inflammatory bowel disorders, sei- Hypothesis behind In 1979, Jaak Panksepp proposed
zures, and fungal and viral infections (Srinivasan, use in individuals a connection between autism and
2009). Because individuals with ASD are more with autism opiates, noting that injections of
minute quantities of opiates in
prone to these conditions, restrictive diets may be young laboratory animals induce
appropriate to ease the pain and suffering of these symptoms like those observed
comorbid conditions, thereby improving overall among individuals with autism
quality of life. However, eliminating certain (Research Autism, 2015). Then in
1991, Kalle Reichelt suggested a
ingredients from a diet brings the risk of an relationship between autism and
imbalance in the diet. For example, removal of the consumption of gluten and
dairy from a diet due to casein allergy may cause casein (Smeltzer, 2011)
calcium deficiency. (continued)
404 M.J. Garcia et al.

Name of diet Gluten-free diet Name of diet Casein-free diet


Evidence for use in For individuals with ASD who Hypothesis Low levels of lactase (enzyme
treating autism have comorbid celiac disease, behind use in necessary to digest lactose) are found
NCGS, or wheat allergy, the individuals in some individuals with ASD
gluten-free diet may lessen pain with autism (Adams, 2013). Some individuals
and gastrointestinal distress with ASD may have an abnormal
leading to improved physical and immune response to casein (and
behavioral health. In the absence usually simultaneously gluten). In
of these disorders, there is no 1979, Jaak Panksepp proposed that
evidence to support the use of a the brain becomes damaged when
gluten-free diet to treat ASD casein in the form of harmful peptide
Risks Gluten-free diet limits protein (acting like an opioid) enters the
intake. According to the National central nervous system causing
Foundation for Celiac behavioral problems in animal
Awareness (2012), gluten-free models (Research Autism, 2015)
products may not be enriched or Evidence for No studies have investigated the
fortified with micronutrients such use in treating efficacy of a casein-free diet alone.
as folic acid, iron, vitamins D and autism For individuals with ASD who have
K, and calcium. Additionally, the comorbid casein allergies, a diet
gluten-free diet may be higher in free of casein may lessen pain and
fat, sugar, salt, and calories than gastrointestinal distress leading to
regular versions and more improved physical and behavioral
expensive (Research Autism, health. In the absence of this allergy,
2015) there is no evidence to support the
Internet resources ∙ Celiac.org use of a casein-free diet to treat
∙ Gastro.ucla.edu ASD
∙ GFCFdiet.com Risks A casein-free diet limits calcium
intake, and calcium is an important
mineral for musculoskeletal and
Name of diet Casein-free diet bodily functions (Siri & Lyons,
Basic A casein-free diet eliminates dairy 2011). A person on this diet may
description that comes from mammals need to take calcium supplements for
Intended use Casein allergies a balanced diet
Description of A casein-free diet is prescribed for Internet ∙ GFCFdiet.com
illness individuals with casein allergy (not resources ∙ Healthychildren.org
whey or lactose intolerant). A casein
allergy occurs when the body Name of diet Ketogenic
mistakenly thinks the protein in
Basic A diet high in fat, adequate protein, and
casein is dangerous and produces
description low in carbohydrates. This combination
allergic antibodies. National Food
changes the way energy is used in the
Service Management Institute
body (brain uses ketones for energy
(NFSMI, 2014) suggests 2–5% of
instead of glucose) and results in an
individuals under the age of three
elevated level of ketone bodies in the
suffer from milk allergy. Experts also
blood (known as ketosis)
suggest that majority of individuals
with casein allergies will outgrow Intended use Helps control seizures in some patients
this allergy within the first few years with refractory epilepsy
of their life (NFSMI, 2014)
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 405

Name of diet Ketogenic Name of diet Specific carbohydrate diet (SCD)


Description Epilepsy is one of the most common Intended use This diet was initially designed to
of illness neurological disorders after stroke and treat Crohn’s disease, ulcerative
is diagnosed when the individual has colitis, and celiac disease
recurrent, unprovoked seizures. Description of Crohn’s disease and ulcerative colitis
Seizures occur when cortical neuron illness are inflammatory bowel diseases
fires excessively and/or (IBD) that affect the gastrointestinal
hypersynchronously and disrupts tract (inflammation of the intestine).
normal brain function. An estimated 1 The Crohn’s and Colitis Foundation of
of 3 individuals with ASD has epilepsy. America (2016) reports that an
In 30% of cases (Autism Speaks, 2016; estimated 1.4 million American suffer
Levisohn, 2007), epilepsy does not from IBD; however, it is most
respond to pharmacological treatment commonly diagnosed in adolescents
Hypothesis The diet was designed in 1921 by Dr. and young adults. According to
behind use Russell Wilder at the Mayo Clinic to Doshi-Velez et al. (2015), individuals
in treat seizures. Because the individual is with ASD experience increased
individuals consuming so few carbohydrates and so prevalence of IBD
with autism much fat, the fat is converted into Hypothesis In 1950, Dr. Sydney Valentine Haas
ketone, and the ketone bodies replace behind use in laid the foundations of SCD by
glucose as the brain’s energy source, individuals helping a patient with ulcerative
which decreases seizures. It has been with autism colitis maintain lasting remission
suggested that it may also improve ASD through diet and fermented food. This
symptoms. One hypothesis for this is patient was Elaine Gottschall’s
the ketogenic diet increases adenosine daughter. It was Dr. Gottschall, a
compound in the brain, therefore biochemist, who coined the term
decreasing inflammation specific carbohydrate diet and spent
Evidence for There is some evidence that this diet most of her life researching the
use in may decrease some types of seizures. science behind the diet; her findings
treating Related to ASD, the most promising were published in Breaking the
autism studies are on mice. Masino et al. (2011) Vicious Cycle. A recent advance in
found a decrease in ASD symptoms in this hypothesis suggests that
mice eating a ketogenic diet. Evangeliou individuals with ASD lack enzymes
et al. (2003) report limited beneficial use necessary to break down the
but poor tolerability for individuals with carbohydrates into sugars (Siri &
ASD. Additionally, Castro et al., 2015 Lyons, 2011). This can lead to
suggest that there is some potential for abnormal gut flora. SCD restricts the
the ketogenic diet, but there is type of carbohydrates intake by
inadequate evidence to support its allowing the person to eat foods that
effectiveness require minimal digestion and that can
Risks Acidosis, high cholesterol, kidney be quickly absorbed. Removing
problems, osteoporosis, GI hard-to-digest foods can help with GI
disturbances, nutrient and calorie problems and decrease discomfort.
deficiency Additionally, promoting normal gut
Internet ∙ Charliefoundation.org function can lead to improvements in
overall health which includes
resources ∙ Epilepsy.com
behavioral, emotional, and cognitive
∙ Epilepsyfoundation.org health (Research Autism, 2015)
Evidence for No studies have investigated the
Name of diet Specific carbohydrate diet (SCD) use in treating efficacy of the SCD for individuals
Basic This diet eliminates complex autism with ASD. Only anecdotal accounts
description carbohydrates (lactose, fructose, and suggest improvements in eye contact,
starches) and is based on the idea that language, anxiety, and self-stimulatory
if diet is limited to simple carbs, it will behavior in individuals with ASD
“starve out toxins” in GI tract and (Gottschall, 1994)
restore GI and immunological Risks If an individual refrains from dairy
functioning. The diet also uses products while on SCD, calcium
fermented products (yogurt) to supplements are likely necessary for a
repopulate gut with healthy bacteria balanced diet
(continued)
406 M.J. Garcia et al.

Name of diet Specific carbohydrate diet (SCD) Name of diet Feingold diet
Internet ∙ Scdlifestyle.com Evidence for No studies have investigated the
resources ∙ CCFA.org use in efficacy of the Feingold diet to treat
∙ Webmd.com treating ASD; only improvement in ADHD-
autism type symptoms is suggested; however,
this is anecdotal data. Related to
Name of diet Feingold diet ADHD, newer research by Nigg,
Basic The Feingold diet eliminates foods Lewis, Edinger, and Falk (2012)
description containing salicylates, synthetic food found that removing food additives
colors, flavors, fragrances and and dyes reduced ADHD symptoms in
preservatives, and artificial sweeteners 33% of children with
Intended use The Feingold diet may help ADHD. Conversely, the FDA’s
individuals with salicylate and food position on “food-related behavior
color sensitivities problems,” i.e., ADHD, is that these
behaviors appear to be the result of
Description To help them protect against diseases
person-specific food/additive
of illness and insects, plants produce a chemical
intolerance and exhibited by
called salicylate. According to
predisposed individuals with an
Auckland Allergy Clinic (2014), some
unclear etiology although the etiology
adults and children have salicylate
may involve genetic, endocrine, or
sensitivity—when the body is unable
immunologic pathways (Aungst,
to tolerate more than a certain amount
2011)
of salicylate at a time (low tolerance
to salicylate). Salicylate sensitivity Risks There appear to be no medical risks
depends on the individual and the type for this diet however; there is the
of salicylate (Edelkind, 2012) concern that reliance solely on diet
limits the opportunity to receive
Hypothesis The Feingold diet was based on an
appropriate professional help to treat
behind use in allergy diet designed by Dr. Lockey of
ADHD
individuals the Mayo Clinic. In 1965, Dr. Ben
with autism E. Feingold observed a link between Internet ∙ FDA.org
certain foods and synthetic additives resources ∙ Quackwatch.com
and their effect on children with ∙ Reserchaustim.net
hyperactivity and learning. In 1973, ∙ Feingold.org
Dr. Feingold tested his hypothesis—
synthetic additives and certain foods
containing natural salicylate can Name of diet Yeast-free diet
trigger hyperactivity. Related to ASD, Basic The yeast-free diet can help inhibit
it has been suggested that since description overgrowth of yeast, called Candida
symptoms of ADHD are common albicans. Yeast is normally found on
among individuals with ASD, then and in the body. It is hypothesized
this diet may improve attention and that yeast produces harmful toxins
hyperactivity challenges. Additionally, that affect behavioral and mental
a deficiency of phenol sulfur functioning of an individual with
transferase (PST) is noted among ASD. Yeast levels are measured by
individuals with ASD. Salicylates are stool test. Medication, homeopathic
a subgroup of phenols that are remedies, and dietary changes can
occasionally difficult for individuals treat yeast overgrowth
with ASD to break down Intended use Yeast overgrowth
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 407

Name of diet Yeast-free diet metabolic abnormalities. It is important to


Description of Invasive candida can cause remember that the Food and Drug Administration
illness candidemia, a bloodstream infection, (FDA) does not regulate over-the-counter supple-
which is treated with antifungal
ments beyond the stipulation that supplements
medications. This often occurs in
people with weakened immune may not make a claim of treatment efficacy
systems. Typically, these infections unless approved by the FDA (US Food and Drug
are treated with medication. Administration, 1995). Therefore, supplements
Although candidemia does not seem
may present risk, e.g., not containing what they
to be treated with the yeast-free diet,
less severe forms of yeast claim or having impurities (Adams, 2013). The
overgrowth such as thrush or yeast United States Pharmacopeia, a nonprofit organi-
infections can respond to this diet zation that sets standards for the identity, strength,
Hypothesis Rimland claims to have been quality, and purity of medicines, food ingredi-
behind use in investigating the relationship
ents, and dietary supplements (The United States
individuals between candida and autism since
with autism 1966. The “leaky gut” hypothesis Pharmacopeial Convention, 2016), can verify
implies that controlling yeast dietary supplements.
overgrowth can help GI problems.
The yeasts were hypothesized to
create toxic chemicals, which can Name of diet Probiotics
affect the developing brain/mental Basic description Live bacteria and yeasts that are
functioning; it can also damage the good for the digestive system are
intestine, which can lead to digestive given to normalize the gut
problems (Srinivasan, 2009). microbiome
Moreover, yeast overgrowth can Intended use Irritable bowel syndrome,
result in pain and various behavioral inflammatory bowel disease,
symptoms associated with ASD such infectious or antibiotic-related
as hyperactivity, inattention, diarrhea
aggression, self-stimulatory
behavior, food selectivity, sensory Description of GI problems
defensiveness, and high-pitched illness
squealing Hypothesis behind “Preliminary evidence” suggests
use in individuals that there is an imbalance of
Evidence for No studies have investigated the
with autism bacteria in a subgroup of
use in treating efficacy of the yeast-free diet. Only
individuals with ASD (see
autism anecdotal accounts suggest that the
Autism Speaks)
yeast-free diet helps with
concentration and obsessive Evidence for use in Some research suggests that it
behaviors treating autism reduces inflammation and
restores normal gut microbiota,
Risks Occasional diarrhea is reported
thereby helping with GI issues.
among individuals as a reaction due
There is lack of scientific
to elimination of yeast from the
evidence on the use of probiotics
colon. However, no severe adverse
among individuals with ASD
reactions are noted (Levy & Hyman,
2005) Risks Safe for most people, may have
mild side effects such as upset
Internet ∙ Pathfindersforautism.org
stomach, diarrhea, bloating.
resources ∙ Lancommunity.org Probiotics may also cause
allergic reactions.
Immunocompromised
individuals or individuals using
antibiotics should use caution
Supplementation Diets while starting probiotics
(Madzhidova et al., 2015)
The supplementation of diets is the adding of Internet resources ∙ Autismspeaks.org
supplements to the diet to replace verified defi- ∙ Webmd.com
ciencies or in an attempt to treat hypothesized ∙ NCCIH.nih.gov
408 M.J. Garcia et al.

Name of diet General diet supplementation Name of diet General diet supplementation
Basic Vitamins and minerals are necessary Risks Between 2004 and 2103, over
description for health. A wide range of vitamin 23,000 emergency room visits each
(A, B3, B6, B12, C, D, and E) and year were related to supplements
mineral (calcium, iron, magnesium, (Geller et al., 2015). In some
zinc) supplements have been individuals, risks related to fatty
hypothesized to benefit individuals acids include mild GI discomfort,
with ASD. Fatty acids (omega-3) are including diarrhea and nausea
found in the cell membrane and are (Madzhidova et al., 2015).
essential for brain function. Additional scientific evidence is
Dimethylglycine (DMG) is needed
necessary for cell function Internet NIH Office of Dietary Supplements
Intended se Vitamin or mineral deficiencies and resources NIH National Center for
fatty acids are due to deficiency Complementary and Integrative
diseases, poor diet, absorption Health
problems, or other causes
Webmd.com
Description of A description of all illnesses related
University of Maryland Medical
illness to vitamin and mineral deficiencies
Center, Complementary and
is beyond the scope of this article.
Alternative Medicine Guide
Please refer to the resources below
United States Pharmacopeia (USP.
Hypothesis Gastrointestinal issues and food
org)
behind use selectivity may result in nutrient
with deficiencies. In addition,
individuals supplements may improve
with ASD epigenetic vulnerabilities including
inflammation, mitochondrial
dysfunction, oxidative stress, and
abnormal methylation. Individuals
 onsiderations for Diet
C
with ASD are at increased risk for and Supplementation Use
calcium deficiency. Low levels of
omega-3 fatty acids are found in Since efficacy research does not yet support a
neurodevelopmental disorders such
as ADHD, dyspraxia, and
clear connection between dietary interventions
dyslexia—these conditions have and/or supplementation and the core symptoms
similar symptomatic and biological of ASD, the most relevant use of diet and supple-
characteristics as ASD (Kidd, 2002). mentation is to address comorbid symptomatol-
Individuals with autism are at risk of
deficits of omega-3 fatty acids
ogy. Of the well-established comorbid symptoms,
(Lofthouse, Hendren, Hurt, Arnold, those that are related to feeding and gastrointesti-
& Butter, 2012; Kidd, 2002) nal concerns are the most pertinent to a discus-
Evidence for The biomedical subgroup of sion of diet and/or supplementation use.
use in treating complementary and alternative Specifically, feeding and gastrointestinal (GI)
ASD medicine treatments for ASD
reviewed the evidence for function are diet-related concerns that have been
multivitamin supplementation for documented since Kanner (1943) noted food and
individuals with ASD in 2013. The feeding issues in the form of food intolerance in
quality of evidence was rated as his study group of children with ASD. More
“fair” with a daily multivitamin
supplementation being recently, Ming, Brimacombe, Chaaban,
recommended by the committee Zimmerman-Bier, and Wagner’s (2008) study of
(Hendren, 2013). Initial pilot studies 160 children with ASD found half suffered long-­
suggest that omega-3 may be term food intolerance, and in 25%, the onset
beneficial in controlling symptoms
of ADHD (Madzhidova et al., 2015; occurred in infancy with infant formula intoler-
Lofthouse et al., 2012). However, ance. Below are several important considerations
majority of the studies did not show that need to be acknowledged by the clinician
significant benefit of omega-3 fatty looking to start a dietary intervention and/or sup-
acid in treating ASD
plemental addition for an individual with ASD.
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 409

Feeding Challenges more restricted food repertoire, research has not


established significant differences in nutrient
While not a core symptom of ASD, feeding chal- intake between individuals with ASD and typi-
lenges are common in individuals with ASD cally developing individuals (Herndon et al.,
(Emond, Emmet, Steer, & Golding, 2010), often 2008; Hyman et al., 2012; Johnson et al., 2008)
beginning in infancy (Ming et al., 2008). The eti- with the exception of protein and calcium (Sharp
ology of feeding challenges is multifactorial et al., 2013). A debilitating cycle may ensue for
including behavioral, physiological, emotional, some individuals with feeding challenges leading
cognitive, and medical (Vissoker, Latzer, & Gal, to poor nutrition, which in turn impacts GI func-
2015). However, clinicians hypothesize that a tioning, resulting in physical discomfort exacer-
core symptom of ASD, excessive adherence to bating all challenging behaviors including ASD
routines and rituals, may foster the development symptoms (Mannion & Leader, 2014).
of feeding challenges (Matson & Fodstad, 2009). The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Feeding challenges include chewing and swal- Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5; American
lowing problems, eating-/feeding-related aggres- Psychiatric Association, 2013) section on eating
sion and tantrums, food refusal, food selectivity/ disorders comprises feeding disorders common
restricted food intake, eating too quickly, idio- in ASD including pica, rumination disorder, and
syncratic eating rituals, over- or under-eating, avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder
pica, poor appetite, rumination and pocketing, (AFRID). Hospitalizations for pica increased
spitting/eating, vomiting, and gastroesophageal 93% between 1999 and 2009, with 31% occur-
reflux, with food selectivity being the most com- ring in children with ASD in 2009 (Zhao &
mon (Bandini et al., 2010; Beighley, Matson, Encinosa, 2011). Rumination disorders are more
Rieske, & Adams, 2013; Schreck, Williams, & common in individuals with neurodevelopmental
Smith, 2004; Vissoker et al., 2015). Research evi- disorders where it may be a form of self-soothing
dence suggests food refusal may be associated or stimulation (American Psychiatric Association,
with texture (Hubbard, Anderson, Curtin, Must, 2013). Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder,
& Bandini, 2014; Johnson, Handen, Mayer-­ new to DSM-5, is diagnosed when the avoidant
Costa, & Sacco, 2008), which may, in turn, be or restrictive eating leads to malnutrition and
related to sensory sensitivity in this population requires intervention.
(Cermak, Curtin, & Bandini, 2010, 2014). There
are also reported differences in the types of food
individuals with ASD consume, such as fewer Gastrointestinal Symptomatology
vegetables (Bandini et al., 2010; Johnson et al.,
2008) and less dairy (Herndon, DiGuiseppi, Special attention should be made to identify GI
Johnson, Leiferman, & Reynolds, 2008). symptomatology given the high prevalence of
Children with ASD are often reported to prefer comorbid GI symptoms found in individuals with
starches, often called a “beige diet.” Gorrindo ASD (Bauman, 2010). Unfortunately, clinicians
et al.’s (2012) study of the eating habits of chil- treating ASD are not typically looking for GI
dren with ASD found that more than 25% of all symptomatology, and clinical practice guidelines
calories consumed were starchy foods, followed do not routinely consider the potential impact of
by dairy and sweets, with nuts, eggs, and fish GI symptomatology on the ASD presentation
rarely eaten. Many individuals with ASD have (Buie et al., 2010). When the clinician does begin
difficulties with food selectivity and an insistence to assess for GI symptoms, arriving at a diagnosis
on sameness that often includes dietary prefer- is complex as there are no reliable signs or symp-
ence. When this happens, the individual suffers toms that can consistently aid the clinician to dis-
from limited nutritional intake, which may lead tinguish between functional and structural
to a lack of fiber, fluids, or other nutrients (Kuddo disorders (Bauman, 2010). The important differ-
& Nelson, 2003). However, despite a potentially ence here is that structural disorders are seen at a
410 M.J. Garcia et al.

cellular level and cause physical changes that aggression, self-injurious behaviors, and sleep
more easily facilitate diagnosis, whereas the disturbances occur, the clinician may consider
functional symptoms manifest as subjective that the individual with ASD may be attempting
symptoms, i.e., the more-challenging-to-identify to cope with physical discomfort by these behav-
type that relies on effective communication. ioral manifestations (Buie et al., 2010). The risk
Research to improve the diagnosis of GI disor- is that these overt physical gestures such as point-
ders is complicated since the majority of research ing, rubbing, and/or tapping an affected area,
participants are children who generally present such as the belly, may be considered part of the
with functional or subjective symptoms, rather ASD presentation and overlooked as a possible
than the more-easily-identified structural symp- form of communicating a specific location of
toms (Gorrindo et al., 2012). Researchers agree pain or general discomfort. For instance, when
that subjective symptoms are particularly suscep- individuals present with vocal stereotypy or
tible to under-identification or misidentification repetitive behavior, this behavior could be incor-
due to the lack of effective communication rectly attributed to being a symptom of ASD and
(Bauman, 2010; Buie et al., 2010). Clinicians not a comorbid GI symptom (Mannion & Leader,
have noted that even in those who can verbally 2014). Overall, the clinician will have to be con-
communicate, there is still the challenge of sen- scious of these sources of risk for misattribution
sory processing which impairs the ability to of overt behavioral manifestations.
describe the subjective experience, e.g., symp-
toms of pain and accurate localization of discom-
fort (Baron-Cohen, 1991). Thus, the clinician Communicating GI Distress
focusing attention on GI symptomatology will be
met with systemic and diagnostic challenges. Researchers, like Matson and colleagues, have
Atypical GI presentation and diagnostic over- shown a variety of comorbidities that exacerbate
shadowing are other considerations for the clini- symptoms of ASD (Matson et al., 2011; Matson,
cian. Several researchers warn that atypical Boisjoli, & Mahan, 2009; Matson & Rivet, 2008).
behavioral manifestations of GI symptomatology Their work indicates a need for identifying and
are common in individuals with ASD (Buie et al., correctly diagnosing comorbid disorders.
2010; Mannion & Leader, 2014). For example, Identification and diagnosis are made challeng-
GI disorders can present as non-GI dysfunction ing by several factors; however, communicating
such as sleep problems or challenging behavior challenges may be the most significant as many
(Buie et al., 2010). Further, comorbid symptoms as half of those with ASD use nonverbal methods
may be overshadowed when challenging behav- of communication (Leyfer et al., 2006). Often
iors are prominent (Reiss, Levitan, & Szyszko, pain, discomfort, and/or inconvenience persists
1982). Buie (2005) explains how atypical GI pre- when limited verbal and nonverbal communica-
sentation and diagnostic overshadowing might tion impedes diagnosis. Even individuals with
obfuscate diagnosis. The behavioral manifesta- adequate communication are still vulnerable as
tions that may be ascribed to ASD may actually some may lack a theory of mind, which may limit
be an atypical behavioral manifestation of GI communication of mental states and experiences
symptomatology. As an example, Buie describes (Baron-Cohen, 1991). Here, communication def-
that body movements or mannerisms, observed icits compound the distress of GI symptoms by
in ASD, which are suggestive of a seizure or ste- limiting the ability to communicate discomfort.
reotypy, may be due to comorbid medical condi- Bauman offers that identifying and managing
tions, such as gastroesophageal reflux disease. comorbidities will result in an improved quality
When behavioral manifestations such as chest of life and sense of well-being by allowing for
tapping, facial grimacing, intermittent gulping, more effective participation in educational and
chewing on nonedible objects, constant eating or therapeutic programs. Mannion and Leader
drinking, applying pressure to the abdomen, (2014) suggest that identification of comorbidi-
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 411

ties sets the stage for educational and therapeutic symptomatology of participants, use of control
programs by teaching functional communication groups, differing data sources, and variability in
such as communicating pain awareness and the definitions of GI symptoms. The presence of
localization. While communication deficits have GI symptomatology and its impact on ASD
a significant impact on diagnosis and treatment severity are not fully understood; however, the
of GI distress, teaching functional communica- variability of reported research findings stresses
tion skills for distress will also have a consider- the need for future research.
able impact on the individual with ASD.

 SD Comorbidities Impacted by GI


A
 I Symptomatology and ASD
G Dysfunction
Severity
As mentioned above, GI symptoms are relevant
Several researchers have indicated that GI dys- to ASD; these physical symptoms have the poten-
function may aggravate the presentation of ASD tial to exacerbate symptomology and the poten-
symptomology. Wang, Tancredi, and Thomas tial to interfere with interventions (Mannion &
(2011) report a positive correlation between GI Leader, 2014). Significant bodies of research
symptom severity and autism severity in their offer evidence that the below comorbid ASD
research group of North American children with conditions may also be related to GI dysfunction:
ASD. Peters et al. (2014) found a specific rela- psychopathology, sleep disturbance, constipa-
tionship between compulsive behavior and the GI tion, and language skills.
symptoms of constipation and diarrhea or soiling
for children with ASD. Chaidez et al. (2014)
found a specific relationship with levels of irrita- Comorbid Psychopathology
bility, social withdrawal, stereotypy, and hyper-
activity and the GI symptoms of constipation, A positive link has been made between ASD,
diarrhea, abdominal pain, and gaseousness for comorbid psychopathology, and GI symptom-
children with ASD. Chaidez et al. also found a atology by several researchers. Mazurek et al.
specific relationship with irritability, social with- (2013) found that children with ASD and GI
drawal, and stereotypy and the GI symptoms of symptoms had significantly higher rates of anxi-
painful stool passing, sensitivity to food, and dif- ety on the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL;
ficulty swallowing. Maenner et al. (2012) found a Achenbach & Rescorla, 2001); in addition they
positive relationship of what might be considered also had higher sensory-over-responsivity scores
associated features of ASD, e.g., argumentative, on the Short Sensory Profile (Dunn, 1999).
oppositional, or destructive behaviors and GI Mazefsky et al. (2014) found significantly higher
dysfunction. Mannion and Leader (2014) have levels of affective problems for children with
noted that comorbid GI dysfunction can increase ASD and no intellectual disability who also had
challenging behaviors. Other ASD researchers GI dysfunction on the CBCL. Nikolov et al.
have suggested that GI pain may increase levels (2009) found that children with GI problems
of escape-maintained challenging behavior, e.g., showed greater irritability and social withdrawal
tantrum behavior (Buie et al., 2010; Mulloy et al., on the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (Aman,
2010). However, several researchers have not Singh, Stewart, & Field, 1985) and greater anxi-
found the same relationship in their research pop- ety on a scale modified by Sukhodolsky et al.
ulations (Chandler et al., 2013; Mazefsky, (2008). Williams, Christofi, Clemmons,
Schreiber, Olino, & Minshew, 2014; Molloy & Rosenberg, and Fuchs (2012) found greater anxi-
Manning-Courtney, 2003; Nikolov et al., 2009). ety in their sample of children with both chronic
Wang et al. (2011)suggest several reasons for this GI symptoms and ASD. They also suggested a
variability, including variability in the ASD positive link between ASD, comorbid psychopa-
412 M.J. Garcia et al.

thology, and GI symptomatology and found that occurred in 92.3% of those with nausea, 91.1%
as age increases so do symptoms of psychopa- of those with abdominal pain, 90.9% of those
thology. They examined CBCLs completed by with bloating, 90% of those with diarrhea, and
parents and found that the young children, ages 83.7% of those with constipation. Johnson,
1–5, with ASD and GI symptoms had higher Giannotti, and Cortesi (2009) recommended that
rates of emotional reactivity, anxious/depressed all children with ASD be screened for potential
mood, somatic complaints, internalizing prob- sleep disturbances.
lems, affective problems, and anxiety problems.
The older, school-aged children, ages 6–18, in
this study had the highest scores on all CBCL Constipation
subscales and higher total scores on the
CBCL. Mannion and Leader (2013) found simi- Constipation is a frequent challenge for individu-
lar results in their study of GI symptoms in chil- als who have poor nutrition and hydration. A
dren with ASD. They report that the total number recent meta-analysis by McElhanon et al. (2014)
of GI symptoms also predicted total scores on the found that constipation was three times more
Autism Spectrum Disorder-Comorbid for likely for individuals with ASD than their peers.
Children (Matson & Gonzalez, 2007). They Gorrindo et al. (2012) found that constipation
found three predictive associations: abdominal was associated with younger age, increased
pain and constipation predicted conduct behav- social impairment, and limited expressive lan-
ior, diarrhea predicted tantrum behavior, and nau- guage. Expert consensus guidelines for individu-
sea predicted worry/depressed behavior, avoidant als with ASD and GI dysfunction specify the
behavior, and conduct behavior (Mannion & need for medical professionals to evaluate their
Leader, 2013). patients for latent constipation and to consider
empiric treatment of constipation for individuals
who are nonverbal (Buie et al., 2010). In a nota-
Sleep Disorders ble study comparing parental reports of GI dys-
function relative to those by pediatric
Sleep disorders are common among individuals gastroenterologists, constipation was the most
with ASD. Buie et al. (2010) suggest that a pos- frequently occurring GI symptom at 85%
sible cause of nighttime awakenings may be a (Gorrindo et al., 2012). An added finding was
gastrointestinal disorder, such as gastroesopha- that parents tend to be poor at discriminating
geal reflux disease. For individuals with ASD, symptom variability, which prompted these
sleep problems are predictive of gastrointestinal researchers to recommend the expertise of a gas-
symptoms (Williams, Fuchs, Furuta, Marcon, & troenterologist to accurately identify GI dysfunc-
Coury, 2010). Individuals with GI dysfunction tion and determine the nature of the GI disorder.
commonly experience increased night awakening Practitioners should also help to evaluate parent-­
and pain (Ming et al., 2008). Mannion and Leader child communication about toileting needs to
(2013) found that breathing-related sleep disor- find the extent that limited expressive language
ders and daytime sleepiness predicted both contributes to constipation by thwarting appro-
abdominal pain and bloating. In addition, under-­ priate toileting skills (Gorrindo et al., 2012).
eating, avoidant behavior, and GI symptoms pre-
dicted sleep problems, abdominal pain predicted
sleep anxiety, and under-eating, avoidant behav- Language Skills
ior, constipation, diarrhea, nausea, abdominal
pain, and bloating predicted parasomnias and Several popular culture sources report that lost
daytime sleepiness. In Mannion and Leader’s expressive language skills have been restored by
(2013) participant group, sleep problems dietary changes. However, empirical evidence
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 413

has not yet made this connection clear. Early Gastrointestinal: Medical Assessment
research by Valicenti-McDermott, McVicar,
Cohen, Wershil, and Shinnar (2008) reported an Evidence does not now support elimination or
association with language regression, a family supplementation diets for ASD in the absence of
history of autoimmune disease, and gastrointes- specific comorbid conditions; therefore, comor-
tinal symptoms. Valicenti-McDermott’s team bid conditions need to be identified. A vast list of
found that children with ASD who presented possible comorbid GI conditions and symptoms
with abnormal stool patterns reported more fre- that have been studied includes abdominal pain,
quent language regression. More recently, abdominal bloating, abnormal stool pattern,
Gorrindo et al. (2012) reported a novel finding; bloody stools, celiac disease, colitis, constipa-
they showed that a large portion of children with tion, Crohn’s disease, diarrhea, encopresis, feed-
comorbid GI dysfunction and ASD lacked ing issues, frequent vomiting, food allergy, food
expressive language. However, several other intolerance, food regurgitation, food selectivity,
researchers have studied this relationship and foul-smelling stools, gaseousness, gastritis,
have not found evidence for a correlation with GERD, esophagitis, inflammatory bowel disease,
GI disorders and loss of language (Baird et al., mouth ulcers, and weight loss ( Buie, 2005;
2008; Chandler et al., 2013; Hansen et al., 2008; Chandler et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2011). Expert
Molloy & Manning-Courtney, 2003; Niehus & consensus (Buie et al., 2010) recommends the
Lord, 2006). While these findings are mixed, following approach to assessing GI disorders:
there appears to be utility in considering lan- medical history and physical examination; anal
guage skill as vulnerable to GI disorders, examination; assessment of the back and spine;
although further research will need to clarify the analysis of a stool specimen including an assess-
relationship with language skills, ASD, and GI ment for parasites, enteric pathogens, stool
disorders. guaiac, electrolytes/osmolarity, and serum elec-
trolytes; liver function tests; assessment of nutri-
tional status; and abdominal roentgenogram to
 upport for Those Considering Diet
S assess bowel gas pattern and the possible reten-
and Supplementation Intervention tion of stool. Navigating the challenges posed by
identifying these conditions may require the inte-
Families’ use of dietary and supplementation gration of the family. Families may help with this
intervention is driving professionals to apply sci- assessment by observing eating behavior, food
entific rigor to evaluate the efficacy of these inter- intake, and physical and behavioral responses to
ventions for the treatment of ASD. However, an food closely while keeping meticulous records.
exact course is premature as current scientific
evidence supports multiple pathways leading to
the disorder. Given the diversity it follows that Gastrointestinal: Indirect Assessment
effective intervention needs to be person specific
and formulated by assessment to maximize the Given the challenges of assessing subjective
benefit. Therefore, the clinicians will need to experience in individuals with ASD, an indirect
understand the complex behavioral and physio- assessment can provide reliable and valid infor-
logical issues common in individuals with mation. Unlike direct and experimental assess-
ASD. Below is a discussion of factors relevant to ment, indirect assessment uses a structured
supporting those considering a diet and/or sup- interview to gather information from parents or
plement intervention; the need for assessment, caregivers. Indirect assessments may be particu-
the need to scrutinize therapeutic claims, and the larly helpful for diagnosing functional disorders,
need to appreciate that families will seek to use those without biological markers, especially
these types of interventions. where the use of other types of tests and
414 M.J. Garcia et al.

p­ rocedures has high cost and low diagnostic yield history of the disorder. Of the standards that sup-
(Dhroove, Chogle, & Saps, 2010). A commonly port scientific merit, replicability is the hallmark
used indirect assessment measure used by gastro- characteristic of valid scientific effort. Poor
enterologists is the ROME protocol. Since 1994, experimental integrity, including a lack of proper
the Rome Foundation has classified functional control groups or procedural control, hampers
gastrointestinal disorders. The fourth generation replicability rendering research too vague for
of this classification system, the Rome IV crite- others to reproduce reliable findings with signifi-
ria, was released in 2016. Clinicians using the cance. Ethically, the scientific process must be
Rome IV, or any other indirect assessment, based on sound experimental design that has
should be aware that parents of children with withstood the scrutiny of an institutional review
ASD tend not to overreport GI dysfunction and in board. Institutional review board approval is a
fact are at risk for underreporting symptomatol- key ethical standard here and is always a part of
ogy (Gorrindo et al., 2012). This caution also sound experimental design, especially research
highlights the clinical necessity of screening for involving vulnerable populations such as chil-
GI disorders with thorough but concrete inter- dren. Given the technical sophistication of these
view questions to ensure that terminologies and standards, perhaps the rule of thumb for clini-
concepts discussed mean the same thing to both cians and families to rely when identifying/evalu-
the clinician and the informant. ating dietary interventions is determining if
research findings to support the intervention are
endorsed by a jury of peers and put forth for com-
Identifying/Evaluating Therapeutic munity review in professional journals and
Options conferences.

The sheer number of sources offering hope for


symptom relief with diet and supplementation is Factors for a Risk/Benefit Analysis
daunting. To navigate the identification and eval-
uation process of these sources, families will Questions of safety and efficacy are the primary
need well-informed clinicians who apply scien- concern. Clinicians can help in analyzing the
tific principles to the proposed dietary interven- benefit to risks ratio by considering a 2 × 2
tion and advise on the risk to benefit ratio. square with safety and efficacy at the top and
Tuzikow and Holburn (2011) offer these basic unsafe and ineffective at the side. An interven-
questions for considering an intervention’s scien- tion that is both safe and effective is ideal, and,
tific merit: (1) Is the intervention approach based of course, interventions that are neither safe nor
on the scientific process and with a valid analy- effective should be discouraged. For an interven-
sis?, (2) Does it have replication that can be found tion that is considered risky but effective, it
in peer-reviewed journals?, and (3) Is it built should be either discouraged or monitored
upon other bodies of knowledge? Myers and closely. Monitoring the response to the interven-
Johnson (2007) offer that the ideal evidence for tion should be d­ edicated to safety concerns. In
supporting or refuting an intervention should the case where the intervention is safe but inef-
include the following: peer-reviewed research fective and has the families’ interest presents a
with appropriately diagnosed, well-defined need for its own analysis. The decision to toler-
homogeneous study populations; randomized, ate this intervention or not is based on secondary
double-blind, placebo-controlled design; ade- concerns such as time, money, effort, and hope.
quate sample size to support the statistical analy- If the cost of these secondary concerns is mini-
sis presented; use of appropriate, validated mal, then Cohen (2002) recommends the practi-
outcome measures; and control for confounding tioner to tolerate the use. For a more thorough
factors, such as the placebo effect and the natural analysis, Cohen recommends considering the
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 415

severity and acuteness of illness, the likely out-  efinition of Complementary


D
come with conventional care, the degree of inva- and Integrative Health
siveness, the toxicities and adverse effects of
conventional treatment, the quality of evidence The NCCIH defines complementary and integra-
for efficacy and safety of the intervention, and tive health approaches (CIHA) as “a group of
the family’s understanding of the risks and ben- diverse medical and health care interventions,
efits of the intervention, voluntary acceptance of practices, products, or disciplines that are not
those risks, and the family’s ability to implement generally considered part of conventional medi-
the intervention with fidelity. In addition, the cli- cine” (Ernst, 2012). Of note is the change in ide-
nician needs to honor the basic principles of bio- ology away from the use of “alternative.” The
medical ethics: (1) respect for the individual’s NCCIH (2015) differentiates complementary
autonomy, (2) beneficence, (3) nonmaleficence, from alternative in these ways: “complementary”
and (4) justice, i.e., fairness in providing access is a coordinated use of nonmainstream practice
to essential care (Cohen, 2002). along with conventional medicine, and “alterna-
tive” is when a nonmainstream practice is used in
place of conventional medicine. The NCCIH
Federal Resources adds that true alternative medicine is uncommon,
most people use nonmainstream approaches
Several federal resources are available to help along with conventional treatments. Further, it
evaluate scientific merit. Of those most related considers “integrative” health care as the coordi-
to dietary and nutritional information, the Food nated use of conventional and complementary
and Drug Administration (FDA) and the approaches together. The use of the term “com-
National Center for Complementary and plementary health approaches” describes prac-
Integrative Health (NCCIH) will provide the tices and products of nonmainstream origin,
best source of information related to dietary whereas the use of “integrative health” describes
interventions for ASD. Where the FDA regu- the incorporation of complementary and main-
lates and monitors foods and drugs that have a stream approaches. Levy and Hyman (2005)
potential for harm, the NCCIH is responsible offer that complementary and integrative inter-
for researching and commenting on products ventions used with ASDs can be further catego-
and practices that are not generally considered rized as “biological” or “nonbiological.”
part of conventional medicine, e.g., dietary Nonbiological interventions include treatments
interventions. Commonly the research findings such as auditory integration training, animal-­
of the NCCIH will prompt the FDA to take reg- assisted therapy, or music therapy. Biological
ulatory action. The NCCIH recognizes that the interventions include diets and supplementation.
public has ready access to enormous amounts of The NCCIH defines biologically based practices
complementary and integrative health informa- to include the use of botanicals, animal-derived
tion; however, most of this information is extracts, vitamins, minerals, fatty acids, amino
incomplete, misleading, inaccurate, or based on acids, proteins, prebiotics and probiotics, diets,
scientifically unproven claims (Ernst, 2012). and functional foods (Ernst, 2012).
Therefore, the NCCIH mission “is to define,
through rigorous scientific investigation, the
usefulness and safety of complementary and Talking with Families
integrative health interventions and their roles
in improving health and health care” (Ernst, Health care practitioners who work with families
2012). For the clinician providing support to and individuals with ASD should recognize that
families, reliable dietary and nutritional infor- many of their patients will use nonstandard thera-
mation may be obtained from the FDA and the pies (NCCIH, 2016). Myers and Johnson (2007)
NCCIH. point out that these families are willing to pursue
416 M.J. Garcia et al.

any interventions believed to present some hope unproven treatments and to identify those that
of helping their child, and this is especially true if have been proven to be ineffective and potentially
the therapies are considered to be safe, e.g., harmful.
touted as natural. As noted in the previous tables,
natural substances are not without risk. In addi-
tion, nonstandard interventions which are often Families Role
unsubstantiated and ineffective (Ernst, 2012) also
compete with validated treatments and may lead The NCCIH (2016) recognizes that despite the
to physical, emotional, or financial harm due to support of efficacy studies, special diets may help
depleting time, effort, and financial resources some people with ASD but follows with the
(Myers & Johnson, 2007). Below is a dissection caveat that nutritional well-being needs to be
of the clinicians and families’ role in the evalua- carefully monitored before and while on the diet.
tion of CIHA. The NCCIH urges families to consult their medi-
cal practitioner before implementing interven-
tions, as some complementary and integrative
Clinicians’ Role approaches have not been studied in children
because children were excluded from older
The American Academy of Pediatrics Committee research studies. Exclusion was due to special
on Children With Disabilities points out the protections mandated for research involving chil-
responsibility of the practitioner to (1) become dren. However, this has changed with new
knowledgeable about CIHA, (2) be able to evalu- requirements from the NIH to include children in
ate the scientific merits of interventions, (3) be all studies unless scientific and ethical reasons
able to identify risks or potential harmful effects prevent inclusion (Ernst, 2012). Additionally, the
of treatment approaches, (4) to provide balanced NCCIH stresses to families the importance of
information and advice about treatment options, talking to the health care practitioner for assess-
(5) to educate families on the evaluation of the ing what, if any, complementary approach would
scientific merit of information about treatment, help since there is very little high-quality research
(6) to avoid dismissal of CIHA in ways that con- on CIHA for ASD (NCCIH, 2016).
vey a lack of sensitivity or concern, (7) to recog-
nize feeling threatened and guard against
becoming defensive, (8) to offer assistance in Dealing with  “Scientific
monitoring and evaluating the CIHA, and (9) to Breakthrough”
actively listen to the family and continue to work
with families even if there is disagreement about Families are vulnerable to any number of fad
treatment choices (Sandler et al., 2001). Kemper, intervention claims. The “scientific break-
Vohra, and Walls (2008) suggest that pediatri- through” claim is especially alluring as it pres-
cians ask parents about all interventions used ents as something new and sounds like valid
because families often do not volunteer informa- science promising life change with this
tion about CIHA. Inquiry will help the pediatri- ­breakthrough. However, caution is advised before
cian assist the family in correctly identifying the implementing interventions based on “scientific
etiology of improvements or adverse effects that evidence” that circumvents the full scientific val-
may arise. Questioning should include asking if a idation process mentioned above. Stanovich
patient is using any vitamins, herbs, supplements, (1985) responds wisely to these claims of sudden
teas, home remedies, special foods, or diets. breakthroughs. He explains that science tends to
Overall, health care practitioners are in the best move slowly and that very few times have we
position to help families in distinguishing empiri- seen great leaps in any field of science. He posits
cally validated treatment approaches from that problems in science are not solved with a
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 417

single experiment, even one that completely nal thinking and action despite having adequate
decides the issue and overturns all previous intelligence called “dysrationalia.” This thinking
knowledge. Specifically, new theories must con- flaw occurs when thinking is made vulnerable by
nect to previously established empirical fact, two processes, “the cognitive miser” and “mind-
building on it with new facts while accounting for ware problems.” The cognitive miser tendency is
the previously held facts. As this is occurring, taking the easy way out when trying to solve
scientists review and replicate findings until a problems by using quick and minimal cognition
critical mass of support is achieved. In other and is vulnerable to overgeneralizing, under
words, the preponderance of the evidence sup- thinking, and abandoning effortful cogitation
ports one theory over any other theory and allows early. Our cognitive miser does not seek to iden-
no theory to be beyond challenge. tify variables that may account for a correlational
Nickel (1996) offers several “red flags” of relationship. Our cognitive miser may be duped
caution for the clinician to share with families to by faulty “person-who-evidence” logic. This is a
help guard against unfounded therapeutic claims. tendency to think “that can’t [/can] be true,
The practitioners can advise families to seek because I know a person who …” leading one to
additional information when they encounter the discount all other variables or evidence
following: (1) treatments that are based on overly (Stanovich, 1985). Stanovich’s concept of “mind-
simplified scientific theories; (2) therapies that ware” is a term used to describe needed cognitive
are claimed to be effective for multiple, different, tools such as specific knowledge, rules, and strat-
and unrelated conditions or symptoms; (3) claims egies used to think rationally. “Mindware prob-
that individuals will respond dramatically and lems” arise when these tools are not available or
some will be cured; (4) use of case reports or are faulty and prevent the rational evaluation of
anecdotal data rather than carefully designed information. Flawed thinking is present when
studies to support claims for treatment; (5) lack things that are too good to be true are not dis-
of peer-reviewed references or denial of the need missed or, worse, invested in heavily.
for controlled studies; and (6) treatments that are Clinicians are tasked with helping identify
said to have no potential or reported adverse and dismantle vulnerabilities that prevent the dis-
effects (Nickel, 1996). missal of too-good-to-be-true therapeutic claims
(Tuzikow & Holburn, 2011). Avoiding the cogni-
tive miser tendency can be done by calling atten-
Dealing with the “Too Good tion to overgeneralizing, under thinking, and
To Be True” tiring of effortful cognition. Overcoming the
mindware problem includes addressing two areas
Clinicians will likely deal with the too-good-to-­ Stanovich calls “mindware gaps” and “mindware
be-true therapeutic claim, a claim that persists as contamination.” The gaps are the absences of
viable despite reasonable evidence to the contrary. strategies or tools for rational decision-making.
One reason that these claims persist is the urge to The contamination is having bad tools or infor-
provide symptom relief. Decisions for therapeutic mation. Mindware contamination is the most
intervention often rest on surrogate decision-mak- ­relevant to too-good-to-be-true therapeutic
ers; mostly these people are acting on behalf of a claims. It is based on four beliefs: beliefs that are
person identified as a loved one. It may be the not based on evidence, beliefs that are attractive
need to “do something” to provide symptom relief and hopeful, beliefs that are easily spread to oth-
for a loved one that stimulates hope in the too- ers, and beliefs that are potentially harmful
good-to-be-true therapeutic claim. (Stanovich, 2009). Particularly relevant to evalu-
Another reason that too-good-to-be-true ther- ating too-good-to-be-true therapeutic claims is
apeutic claims may persist is flawed thinking. that this type of cognitive error spreads easily
Stanovich (2009) hypothesizes a thinking flaw when it promises a benefit to the user. An addi-
that may account for the tendency toward irratio- tional caveat offered by Stanovich is that intelli-
418 M.J. Garcia et al.

gence does not protect us from contaminated reliable health summaries including scientific
mindware and inimically greater intelligence reports, legislative developments, enforcement
may increase the use of rationalization to support actions, related news items, website evaluations,
beliefs. In general, the clinician may have the recommended and non-recommended books,
best outcome to addressing too-good-to-be-true research tips, and other information relevant to
therapeutic claims by recognizing beliefs that are consumer protection and consumer decision-­
not based on evidence and potentially harmful. making (Barrett & London, 2016).

Sites to See: Useful Resources  n the Horizon: The Microbiome-­


O
Gut Brain Axis
In addition to the resources identified above,
there are several well-established government Definition
resources with helpful information to consider
when choosing an intervention. The primary The complex interrelationship between the diges-
resource for CIHA is the National Center for tive and nervous systems has been the subject of
Complementary and Integrative Health, which scientific inquiry for centuries. In fact, Ivan
currently has publications related to ASD treat- Pavlov spoke of the functional interdependence
ments in addition to herbs and supplement effi- of the nervous and digestive systems at his 1904
cacy (e.g., see publication titled 7 Things To Nobel lecture:
Know About Complementary Health Approaches Precise knowledge of what happens to the food
for ASD or publication title Herbs at a Glance). entering the organism must be the subject of ideal
The FDA provides additional information on physiology, the physiology of the future. Present-­
herb and supplement safety (e.g., see publication day physiology can but engage in the continuous
accumulation of material for the achievement of
titled Tips for Dietary Supplement Users). The this distant aim. … It is clear to all that the animal
US National Library of Medicine offers health-­ organism is a highly complex system consisting of
related information via MedlinePlus including an almost infinite series of parts connected both
information on herbs and supplement, food and with one another and, as a total complex, with the
surrounding world, with which it is in a state of
nutrition, and health education materials. The equilibrium. The equilibrium of this system, as of
USDA provides information that is helpful in any other system, is a condition for its existence.
designing a healthy diet at ChooseMyPlate.gov. (Pavlov, 1904)
Information on the current federally funded
research and how to participate in ongoing stud- Pavlov understood that technology would allow
ies is available at ClinicalTrials.gov. In addition the discoveries of the future. He foreshadowed
to government resources, there are several exam- the discovery of intricate biochemical webs we
ples of well-established private resources. For now call the microbiome-gut-brain axis (Cryan,
example, the American Academy of Pediatrics is 2011).
a professional organization that offers resources Historically, Banks used “gut-brain axis” in
for both the professionals and families. A 1980 to describe the relationship of the gut [duo-
resource for efficacy and safety is Quackwatch. denal] hormone, cholecystokinin, plasma, and
com. Launched in 1996 and administrated by cerebrospinal fluid (Banks, 1980). The gut-brain
Stephen Barrett, MD., this website hosts an inter- axis currently refers to the bidirectional signaling
national network whose focus is on consumer between the gut and brain via neural and humoral
protection, medical ethics, and scientific skepti- mechanisms (Bercik, Collins, & Verdu, 2012).
cism. This site offers evidence-based information The neural components include the vagal, spinal,
to consider when evaluating interventions. An and sympathetic nerves as well as the enteric ner-
affiliate resource is the National Council Against vous system (ENS). The ENS is the nervous sys-
Health Fraud, which offers weekly updates of tem of the gut, responsible for intestinal motility,
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 419

hormone secretion, blood flow, and mucosal fluid 2015). Hsiao (2014) suggests the gut-brain axis
exchange (Sassellia, Vassilis, & Burns, 2012). explains the contribution of such GI abnormali-
The humoral components include hormones, ties to the clinical manifestations of ASD-­
neuropeptides, and cytokines. Humoral compo- associated symptoms as well as immune
nents are also produced by the gut microbiota dysregulation and metabolic dysfunction. While
(Bercik et al., 2012). The gut microbiome is the this exciting research has renewed interest in
genetic encoding of the microbiota of the gut. nutrition, probiotics, fermentation, and other spe-
The microbiota of the human body includes bac- cial diets, the current state of our knowledge does
teria, viruses, eukaryotes, and archaea that far not allow interventions tailored to the needs of
outnumber our own cells (Clemente, Ursell, the individual. Promises to cure ASD by elimi-
Wegener Parfrey, & Knight, 2012). nating dysbiosis or normalizing the enterotype
The microbiota of the gut are critical to our are premature.
health. These symbiotes promote absorption,
immunological maturation and health, and even
cognition in ways we are only beginning to Conclusion
understand (Cryan, 2011; Heijtz et al., 2011).
Compounds may be activated or inactivated by It is inevitable that parents of children diagnosed
the gut microbiota. Dysbiosis, an imbalance or with ASD will use the available treatments to
disruption of healthy gut microbiota, has been help alleviate their child’s suffering. Easy access
linked to physical and mental disease states to blogs, forums, medical research, and websites
(Carding, 2015). A healthy gut may be upset by may seem to contain answers, but the data regard-
diet, infection, stress, environmental toxins, and ing dietary interventions is often anecdotal and
other factors (Clemente et al., 2012). insufficient to support efficacy. Our understand-
ing of the vital impact that food has on physical
and mental health is increasing at a rapid rate.
The M-G-B Axis and ASD Technological advances are allowing genetic and
physiologic study of the effects of food, includ-
The high prevalence of gastrointestinal symp- ing supplementation and restriction diets, on
toms in individuals with ASD is well docu- individuals with ASD as well as almost instanta-
mented, but the etiology of these symptoms is neous communication of study results. This com-
less clear (McElhanon et al., 2014). Early hypoth- munication often comes in sound bites promising
eses citing issues such as a “leaky” gut, gut opi- cures, demanding that it must be approached with
oid production, and yeast overgrowth of the a critical eye. What study results mean for the
intestine, while possibly true in some individuals, individual with ASD is a complex question, but it
have not withstood scientific analysis as a hall- remains clear that diet does not cure autism.
mark of ASD. New research focuses on the qua- Sound treatment decisions require behavioral
drillions of bacteria that populate the human assessment of feeding behaviors, dietary
digestive tract. New technologies have allowed ­assessment, and the identification of comorbid
the analysis of gut bacteria at a genetic level in a medical conditions, especially gastrointestinal
fraction of the time required for growing bacteria disorders, for an individualized approach. Dietary
in a petri dish. Exactly what constitutes a healthy interventions should be based on peer-reviewed,
gut has not been fully defined, but common pat- replicated studies and include a careful analysis
terns are emerging. These patterns, or ratios of of potential risks and benefits for the individual.
bacterial types, are called “enterotypes” The expected benefits should be measureable.
(Arumugam et al., 2011). The study of entero- The response to dietary interventions should be
types common in individuals with ASD has monitored to document efficacy and prevent
revealed bacterial ratios that do not fit common harm. Most important, the clinician should be
patterns (Benach, 2012; Krajmalnik-Brown, well informed.
420 M.J. Garcia et al.

References Autism Speaks Autism Treatment Network/Autism


Intervention Research Network on Physical Health-
Behavioral Health Sciences Committee. (2014).
Academic Autistic Spectrum Partnership in Research and
Exploring feeding behavioral autism: A parent’s guide
Education. (2015, 4 4). AASPIRE healthcare toolkit.
to exploring feeding behavior in autism. Retrieved from
Retrieved from AASPIRE: http://autismandhealth.org
http://www.autismspeaks.org/science/find-resources-
/?p=home&theme=ltlc&size=small
programs/autism-treatmentnetwork/tools-you-can-use/
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. A. (2001). Manual for
atn-air-p-guide-exploring-feeding-behavior.
the ASEBA school-age forms & profiles: An integrated
Baird, G., Charman, T., Pickles, A., Chandler, S., Loucas,
system of multi-informant assessment. Burlington,
T., Meldrum, D., … Simonoff, E. (2008). Regression,
VT: Research Center for Children, Youth & Families,
developmental trajectory and associated problems in dis-
University of Vermont.
orders in the autism spectrum: The SNAP study. Journal
Adams, J. B. (2013). Summary of dietary, nutritional, and
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 1827–1836.
medical treatments for autism—based on over 150
Bandini, L. G., Anderson, S. E., Curtin, C., Cermak, S.,
published research studies. Autism Research Institute,
Evans, E. W., Scampini, R., … Must, A. (2010). Food
40, 1–53.
selectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders
Adams, J. B., Johansen, L. J., Powell, L. D., Quig, D.,
and typically developing children. The Journal of
& Rubin, R. A. (2011). Gastrointestinal flora and gas-
Pediatrics, 157(2), 259–264. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.
trointestinal status in children with autism – compari-
jpeds.2010.02.013
sons to typical children and correlation with autism
Banks, W. A. (1980). Evidence for a cholecystokinin gut-­
severity. BMC Gastroenterology, 11(1), 22. http://doi.
brain axis with modulation by bombesin. Peptides, 1,
org/10.1186/1471-230X-11-22
347–351.
Ahn, Y., Narous, M., Tobias, R., Rho, J. M., &
Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The theory of mind deficit
Mychasiuk, R. (2014). The ketogenic diet modifies
in autism: How specific is it? British Journal of
social and metabolic alterations identified in the pre-
Developmental Psychology, 9, 301–314.
natal valproic acid model of autism Spectrum disorder.
Barrett, S., & London, W. E. (2016). Consumer health
Developmental Neuroscience, 36(5), 371–380. http://
digest archive. Retrieved from http://www.ncahf.org/
doi.org/10.1159/000362645.
digest16/index.html
Aman, M. G., Singh, N. N., Stewart, A. W., & Field, C. J.
Bauman, M. L. (2010). Medical comorbidities in
(1985). Psychometric characteristics of the Aberrant
autism: Challenges to diagnosis and treatment.
Behavior Checklist. American Journal of Mental
Neurotherapeutics, 7, 320–327.
Deficiency, 89, 492–502.
Baxter, A. J., & Krenzelok, E. P. (2008). Pediatric fatal-
American Academy of Pediatrics. (2016, 3 13). Nutrition
ity secondary to EDTA chelation. Clinical Toxicology,
and fitness. Retrieved from HealthyChildren: https://
46(10), 1083–1084.
www.healthychildren.org
Beighley, J. S., Matson, J. L., Rieske, R. D., & Adams,
American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Nutrition.
H. L. (2013). Food selectivity in children with and
(2013). Pediatric nutrition. In R. E. Kleinman & F. R.
without an autism spectrum disorder: Investigation
Greer (Eds.), Pediatric nutrition (7th ed.p. 1477). Elk
of diagnosis and age. Research in Developmental
Grove Village, IL: American Academy of Pediatrics.
Disabilities, 34(10), 3497–3503. http://doi.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic
org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.07.026
and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5.
Benach, J. L. (2012). A microbial association with
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
autism. mBio, 3, e00019–12. ­http://doi.org/10.1128/
Arumugam, M., Raes, J., Pelletier, E., Le Paslier, D.,
mBio.00019-12
Yamada, T., Mende, D. R., … Bertalan, M. (2011).
Bercik, P., Collins, S. M., & Verdu, E. F. (2012). Microbes
Enterotypes of the human gut microbiome. Nature,
and the gut-brain axis. Neurogastroenterology and
174–180.
Motility, 24(2), 405–413.
Auckland Allergy Clinic. (2014). Salicylate sensitivity.
Blomqvist, M., Dahllöf, G., & Bejerot, S. (2014).
Retrieved from http://www.allergyclinic.co.nz/salicy-
Experiences of dental care and dental anxiety in adults
late_sensitivity.aspx
with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research and
Aungst, J. (2011). Evaluation of studies on artifi-
Treatment. doi:10.1155/2014/238764
cial food colors and behavior disorders in chil-
Buie, T. (2005). Gastrointestinal issues encountered in
dren. Available at: http://www.fda.gov/downloads/
autism. The neurobiology of autism (pp. 103–117).
AdvisoryCommittees/CommitteesMeetingMaterials/
Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.
FoodAdvisoryCommittee/UCM273033.pdf. Accessed
Buie, T., Campbell, D. B., Fuchs, G. J., III, Furuta,
May 2016.
G. T., Levy, J., VandeWater, J., … Winter, H. (2010).
Autism Speaks Inc. (2016, 2 23). Autism speaks.
Evaluation, diagnosis, and treatment of gastrointesti-
Retrieved from Autism Speaks Web site: https://www.
nal disorders in individuals with ASDs: A consensus
autismspeaks.org/
report. Pediatrics, 125, S1–S18.
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 421

Carding, S. V. (2015). Dysbiosis of the gut microbiota in D’Eufemia, P., Celli, M., Finocchiaro, R., Pacifico, L.,
disease. Microbial Ecology in Health and Disease, 26, Viozzi, L., Zaccagnini, M., … Giardini, O. (1996).
doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.3402/mehd.v26.26191 Abnormal intestinal permeability in children with
Cass, H., Gringras, P., March, J., McKendrick, I., O’Hare, autism. Acta Paediatrica, 85(9), 1076–1079. http://
A. E., Owen, L., & Pollin, C. (2008). Absence of uri- doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.1996.tb14220.x
nary opioid peptides in children with autism. Archives Davis, C. A. (1999). Dietary recommendations and how
of Disease in Childhood, 93(9), 745–750. http://doi. they have changed over time. In E. Frazao (Ed.),
org/10.1136/adc.2006.114389 America’s eating habits: Changes and consequences
Castro, K., Faccioli, L. S., Baronio, D., Gottfried, C., (p. 494). Washington, DC: USDA, Agriculture
Perry, I. S., & dos Santos Riesgo, R. (2015). Effect of a Information Bulletin No. (AIB-750). Retrieved from
ketogenic diet on autism spectrum disorder: A system- USDA: http://www.ers.usda.gov/publications/aib-
atic review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, agricultural-information-bulletin/aib750.aspx
20, 31–38. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2015.08.005 Dettmer, K., Hanna, D., Whetstone, P., Hansen, R., &
Celiac Disease Foundation. (2016). Celiac disease. Hammock, B. D. (2007). Autism and urinary exoge-
Retrieved from https://celiac.org/celiac-disease/ nous neuropeptides: Development of an on-line SPE–
Cermak, S. A., Curtin, C., & Bandini, L. G. (2010). Food HPLC–tandem mass spectrometry method to test the
selectivity and sensory sensitivity in children with opioid excess theory. Analytical and Bioanalytical
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of the American Chemistry, 388(8), 1643–1651. http://doi.org/10.1007/
Dietetic Association, 110(2), 238–246. http://doi. s00216-007-1301-4
org/10.1016/j.jada.2009.10.032 Dhroove, G., Chogle, A., & Saps, M. (2010). A million-­
Cermak, S. A., Curtin, C., & Bandini, L. (2014). dollar work-up for abdominal pain: Is it worth it?
Sensory sensitivity and food selectivity in chil- Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition,
dren with autism Spectrum disorders. In V. B. Patel, 51(5), 579–583.
V. R. Preedy, & C. R. Martin (Eds.), Comprehensive Doshi-Velez, F., Avillach, P., Palmer, N., Bousvaros, A.,
guide to autism (pp. 2061–2076). New York, NY: Ge, Y., Fox, K., … Kohane, I. (2015). Prevalence
Springer . Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/ of inflammatory bowel disease among patients with
referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-1-4614-4788-7_126 autism spectrum disorders. Inflammatory Bowel
Chaidez, V., Hansen, R. L., & Hertz-Picciotto, I. (2014). Diseases, 21(10), 2281–2288.
Gastrointestinal problems in children with autism, Dunn, W. (1999). Sensory profile. San Antonio, TX:
developmental delays or typical development. Journal Psychological Corporation.
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(5), 1117– Edelkind, S. (2012). Feingold bluebook: Behavior, learn-
1127. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1973-x ing, and health: The dietary connection. Retrieved from
Chandler, S., Carcani-Rathwell, I., Charman, T., Pickles, Feingold.orghttp://www.feingold.org/BLUEBOOK.
A., Loucas, T., Meldrum, D., … Baird, G. (2013). pdf
Parent reported gastro-intestinal symptoms in children Edelson, S. (2010). Parent ratings of behavioral effects
with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and of biomedical interventions (vol. 34). San Diego, CA:
Developmental Disorders., 43(12), 2737–2747. Autism Research Institute, Publication.
Clemente, J. C., Ursell, L., Wegener Parfrey, L., & Elder, J. H., Shankar, M., Shuster, J., Theriaque, D., Burns,
Knight, R. (2012). The impact of gut microbiota on S., & Sherrill, L. (2006). The gluten-free, casein-free
human health: An integrative view. Cell, 1258–1270. diet in autism: Results of a preliminary double blind
Cohen, M. H. (2002). Legal issues in complementary clinical trial. Journal of Autism and Developmental
and integrative medicine: A guide for the clinician. Disorders, 36(3), 413–420. http://doi.org/10.1007/
Medical Clinics of North America, 86(1), 185–196. s10803-006-0079-0
Croen, L. A., Zerbo, O., Qian, Y., Massolo, M. L., Rich, Emond, A., Emmet, P., Steer, C., & Golding, J. (2010).
S., Sidney, S., & Kripke, C. (2015). The health sta- Feeding symptoms, dietary patterns, and growth
tus of adults on the autism spectrum. Autism, 19(7), in young children with autism spectrum disorders.
814–823. Pediatrics, 126, 337–342.
Crohn’s and Colitis Foundation of America. (2016). Fact Ernst, E. (2012). Exploring the science of complemen-
sheet about IBD. Retrieved from CCFA website http:// tary and alternative medicine: Third strategic plan,
www.ccfa.org/resources/facts-about-inflammatory.html 2011–2015. Focus on Alternative and Complementary
Crowe, K. M., & Francis, C. (2013). Position of the Therapies, 17(1), e7.
academy of nutrition and dietetics: Functional foods. Evangeliou, A., Vlachonikolis, I., Mihailidou, H., Spilioti,
Journal of the Academy of Nutrition and Dietetics, M., Skarpalezou, A., Makaronas, N., … Smeitink,
113(8), 1096–1103. J. (2003). Application of a ketogenic diet in children
Cryan, J. a. (2011). The microbiome-gut-brain axis: with autistic behavior: Pilot study. Journal of Child
From bowel to behavior. Neurogastroenterology and Neurology, 18(2), 113–118. http://doi.org/10.1177/08
Motility, 187–192. 830738030180020501
Cullinan, M. P. (2013). Periodontal disease and sys- Feldman, H. M., Kolmen, B. K., & Gonzaga, A. M. (1999).
temic illness: Will the evidence ever be enough? Naltrexone and communication skills in young chil-
Peridontology, 2000, 271–276. dren with autism. Journal of the American Academy of
422 M.J. Garcia et al.

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(5), 587–593. http:// and children with typical development? Journal of
doi.org/10.1097/00004583-199905000-00021 Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(2), 212–
Freeman, J. M., Vining, E. P. G., Pillas, D. J., Pyzik, P. L., 222. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0606-2
Casey, J. C., & Kelly LM. (1998). The efficacy of the Hsiao, E. Y. (2014). Gastrointestinal issues in autism
ketogenic diet—1998: A prospective evaluation of spectrum disorder. Harvard Review of Psychiatry,
intervention in 150 children. Pediatrics, 102(6), 1358– 22(2014), 104–111.
1363. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.102.6.1358 Hubbard, K. L., Anderson, S. E., Curtin, C., Must,
Geller, A. I., Shehab, N., Weidle, N. J., Lovegrove, M. C., A., & Bandini, L. G. (2014). A comparison of food
Wolpert, B. J., Timbo, B. B., … Budnitz, D. S. (2015). refusal related to characteristics of food in children
Emergency department visits for adverse events with autism spectrum disorder and typically devel-
related to dietary supplements. The New England oping children. Journal of the Academy of Nutrition
Journal of Medicine, 373(16), 1531–1540. and Dietetics, 114(12), 1981–1987. http://doi.
Goodwin, M. S., & Goodwin, T. C. (1969). In a dark mir- org/10.1016/j.jand.2014.04.017
ror. Mental Hygiene, 53(4), 550. Hunter, L. C., O’Hare, A., Herron, W. J., Fisher, L. A., &
Gorrindo, P., Williams, K. C., Lee, E. B., Walker, L. S., Jones, G. E. (2003). Opioid peptides and dipeptidyl
McGrew, S. G., & Levitt, P. (2012). Gastrointestinal peptidase in autism. Developmental Medicine & Child
dysfunction in autism: Parental report, clinical evalu- Neurology, null(02), 121–128. http://doi.org/10.1017/
ation, and associated factors. Autism Research, 5(2), S0012162203000227
101–108. Hurwitz, S. (2013). The gluten-free, casein-free diet and
Gottschall, E. (1994). Breaking the vicious cycle. Ontario, autism: Limited return on family investment. Journal
Canada: Kirkton Press. of Early Intervention, 1053815113484807. http://doi.
Green, V. A. (2007). Parental experience with treatment org/10.1177/1053815113484807
for autism. Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 19, Hyman, S. L., Stewart, P. A., Schmidt, B., Cain, U.,
91–101. Lemcke, N., Foley, J. T., … Ng, P. K. (2012).
Groesbeck, D. K., Bluml, R. M., & Kossoff, E. H. (2006). Nutrient intake from food in children with autism.
Long-term use of the ketogenic diet in the treat- Pediatrics, 130(Supplement 2), S145–S153. http://doi.
ment of epilepsy. Developmental Medicine & Child org/10.1542/peds.2012-0900L
Neurology, null(12), 978–981. http://doi.org/10.1017/ Johnson, C. R., Handen, B. L., Mayer-Costa, M., &
S0012162206002143 Sacco, K. (2008). Eating habits and dietary status in
Hansen, R. L., Ozonoff, S., Krakowiak, P., Angkustsiri, young children with autism. Journal of Developmental
K., Jones, C., Deprey, L. J., … Hertz-Picciotto, I. and Physical Disabilities, 20(5), 437–448. http://doi.
(2008). Regression in autism: Prevalence and asso- org/10.1007/s10882-008-9111-y
ciated factors in the CHARGE study. Ambulatory Johnson, K. P., Giannotti, F., & Cortesi, F. (2009). Sleep
Pediatrics, 8(1), 25–31. patterns in autism spectrum disorders. Child and
Harris, C., & Card, B. (2012). A pilot study to evalu- Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America,
ate nutritional influences on gastrointestinal symp- 18(4), 917–928.
toms and behavior patterns in children with autism Johnson, C. R., Handen, B. L., Zimmer, M., Sacco, K., &
Spectrum disorder. Complementary Therapies in Turner, K. (2010). Effects of gluten free/casein free diet
Medicine, 20(6), 437–440. http://doi.org/10.1016/j. in young children with autism: A pilot study. Journal
ctim.2012.08.004 of Developmental and Physical Disabilities, 23(3),
Hartman, A. L., Gasior, M., Vining, E. P. G., & Rogawski, 213–225. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10882-010-9217-x
M. A. (2007). The neuropharmacology of the keto- Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective con-
genic diet. Pediatric Neurology, 36(5), 281–292. tact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–230.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2007.02.008 Kemper, K. J., Vohra, S., & Walls, R. (2008). The use of
Heijtz, R. D., Wang, S., Anuar, F. Q., Björkholm, B., complementary and alternative medicine in pediatrics.
Samuelsson, A., Hibberd, M. L., … Pettersson, S. Pediatrics, 122(6), 1374–1386.
(2011). Normal gut microbiota modulates brain devel- Kidd, P. M. (2002). Autism, an extreme challenge to
opment and behavior. Proceedings of the National integrative medicine. Part II: Medical management.
Academy of Sciences, 108, 3047–3052. Alternative Medicine Review, 7(6), 472–499.
Hendren, R. L. (2013). Autism: biomedical complemen- Kinsman, S. L., Vining, E. P. G., Quaskey, S. A., Mellits,
tary treatment approaches. Child and Adolescent D., & Freeman, J. M. (1992). Efficacy of the keto-
Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 22(3), 443–456. genic diet for intractable seizure disorders: Review
Herbert, M. R., & Buckley, J. A. (2013). Autism and of 58 cases. Epilepsia, 33(6), 1132–1136. http://doi.
dietary therapy case report and review of the litera- org/10.1111/j.1528-1157.1992.tb01770.x
ture. Journal of Child Neurology, 0883073813488668. Knivsberg, A.-M., Reichelt, K.-L., Høien, T., & Nødland,
http://doi.org/10.1177/0883073813488668 M. (2003). Effect of a dietary intervention on autistic
Herndon, A. C., DiGuiseppi, C., Johnson, S. L., Leiferman, behavior. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
J., & Reynolds, A. (2008). Does nutritional intake dif- Disabilities, 18(4), 247–256. http://doi.org/10.1177/1
fer between children with autism spectrum disorders 0883576030180040601
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 423

Kohane, I. S. (2012). The co-morbidity burden of chil- adenosine A 1 receptors. The Journal of Clinical
dren and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Investigation, 121(7), 2679–2683.
PLoS ONE, 7(4), e33224. http://doi.org/10.1371/jour- Matson, J. L., & Fodstad, J. C. (2009). The treatment of
nal.pone.00332 food selectivity and other feeding problems in chil-
Krajmalnik-Brown, R. L. (2015). Gut bacteria in children dren with autism spectrum disorders. Research in
with autism spectrum disorders: Challenges and prom- Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3(2), 455–461.
ise of studying how a complex community influences Matson, J. L., & Gonzalez, M. L. (2007). Autism spectrum
a complex disease. Microbial Ecology in Health and disorders – comorbidity – child version. Baton Rouge,
Disease, 26, doi:http://dx.Doi.Org/10.3402/mehd. LA: Disability Consultants, LLC.
v26.26914 Matson, J. L., & Rivet, T. T. (2008). Characteristics of
Kuddo, T., & Nelson, K. B. (2003). How common are challenging behaviors in adults with autistic disor-
gastrointestinal disorders in children with autism? ders, PDD-NOS, and intellectual disability. Journal
Current Opinion in Pediatrics, 15, 339–343. of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 33,
Leboyer, M., Bouvard, M. P., Lensing, P., Launay, J.-M., 313–329.
Tabuteau, F., Arnaud, P., … Panksepp, J. (1990). Opioid Matson, J. L., Boisjoli, J. A., & Mahan, S. (2009). The
excess hypothesis of autism: A double-blind study of relation of communication and challenging behaviors
naltrexone. Brain Dysfunction, 3(5–6), 285–298. in infants and toddlers with autism spectrum disorders.
Levisohn, P. M. (2007). The autism-epilepsy connection. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities,
Epilepsia, 48(s9), 33–35. 21, 253–261.
Levy, S. E., & Hyman, S. L. (2005). Novel treatments for Matson, J. L., Mahan, S., Fodstad, J. C., Worley, J. A.,
autistic spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and Neal, D., & Sipes, M. (2011). Effects of symptoms of
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11(2), comorbid psychopathology on challenging behaviors
131–142. among infants and toddlers with Autistic Disorder
Leyfer, O. T., Folstein, S. E., Bacalman, S., Davis, and PDD-NOS as assessed with the Baby and Infant
N. O., Dinh, E., Morgan, J., … Lainhart, J. E. (2006). Screen for Children with aUtIsm Traits (BISCUIT).
Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 14, 129–139.
autism: Interview development and rates of disorders. Mazefsky, C. A., Schreiber, D. R., Olino, T. M., &
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, Minshew, N. J. (2014). The association between emo-
849–861. tional and behavioral problems and gastrointestinal
Lofthouse, N., Hendren, R., Hurt, E., Arnold, L. E., & symptoms among children with high-functioning
Butter, E. (2012). A review of complementary and autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research
alternative treatments for autism spectrum disorders. and Practice, 18(5), 493–501.
Autism Research and Treatment, 2012, 870391. Mazurek, M. O., Vasa, R. A., Kalb, L. G., Kanne, S. M.,
Madzhidova, S., Dunn, R. D., Lerch, D. L., Blackman, S. K., Rosenberg, D., Keefer, A., … Lowery, L. A. (2013).
Kariman, A., & Sodoro, J. J. (2015). Complementary Anxiety, sensory over-responsivity, and gastrointes-
and alternative medicine in pediatric patients with tinal problems in children with autism spectrum dis-
autism spectrum Disorder. Inside Pharmacy, 3(8). orders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41,
Retrieved, from http://www.insidepatientcare.com/ 165–176.
issues/2015/august-2015-vol-3-no-8/240-comple- McElhanon, B. O., McCracken, C., Karpen, S., & Sharp,
mentary-and-alternative-medicine-in-pediatric- W. G. (2014). Gastrointestinal symptoms in autism
patients-with-autism-spectrum-disorder spectrum disorder: A meta-analysis. Pediatrics,
Maenner, M. J., Arneson, C. L., Levy, S. E., Kirby, R. S., 133(5), 872–883.
Nicholas, J. S., & Durkin, M. S. (2012). Brief report: Ming, X., Brimacombe, M., Chaaban, J., Zimmerman-­
Association between behavioral features and gastro- Bier, B., & Wagner, G. C. (2008). Autism spectrum
intestinal problems among children with autism spec- disorders: Concurrent clinical disorders. Journal of
trum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Child Neurology, 23, 6–13.
Disorders, 42, 1520–1525. Molloy, C. A., & Manning-Courtney, P. (2003). Prevalence
Mannion, A., & Leader, G. (2013). An analysis of the pre- of chronic gastrointestinal symptoms in children with
dictors of comorbid psychopathology, gastrointestinal autism and autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 7,
symptoms and epilepsy in children and adolescents 165–171.
with autism spectrum disorder. Research in Autism Mulloy, A., Lang, R., O’Reilly, M., Sigafoos, J., Lancioni,
Spectrum Disorders, 7(12), 1663–1671. G., & Rispoli, M. (2010). Gluten-free and casein-free
Mannion, A., & Leader, G. (2014). Gastrointestinal diets in the treatment of autism spectrum disorders:
symptoms in autism spectrum disorder: A literature a systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum
review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 4, 328–339.
Disorders, 1(1), 11–17. Murray, J. A., Watson, T., Clearman, B., & Mitros, F.
Masino, S. A., Li, T., Theofilas, P., Sandau, U. S., Ruskin, (2004). Effect of a gluten-free diet on gastrointestinal
D. N., Fredholm, B. B., … Boison, D. (2011). A symptoms in celiac disease. The American Journal of
ketogenic diet suppresses seizures in mice through Clinical Nutrition, 79(4), 669–673.
424 M.J. Garcia et al.

Myers, S. M., & Johnson, C. P. (2007). Management of children with autism spectrum disorders: Behavioural
children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, and psychometric measures of dietary response.
120(5), 1162–1182. Nutritional Neuroscience, 17(5), 207–213. http://doi.
National Center for Complementary and Integrative org/10.1179/1476830513Y.0000000082
Health. (2015). Complementary, alternative, or inte- Pennesi, C. M., & Klein, L. C. (2012). Effectiveness of
grative health: What’s in a name? (NCCIH Publication the gluten-free, casein-free diet for children diagnosed
No. D347). Washington, DC: U.S. Government with autism spectrum disorder: Based on parental
Printing Office. report. Nutritional Neuroscience, 15(2), 85–91. http://
National Center for Complementary and Integrative doi.org/10.1179/1476830512Y.0000000003
Health. (2016). 7 things to know about complemen- Peters, B., Williams, K. C., Gorrindo, P., Rosenberg,
tary health approaches for ASD. Washington, DC: D., Lee, E. B., Levitt, P., & Veenstra-VanderWeele,
U.S. Government Printing Office. J. (2014). Rigid compulsive behaviors are associated
National Food Service Management Institute. (2014). with mixed bowel symptoms in autism spectrum disor-
Food allergy fact sheet: Milk allergies. Retrieved from der. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
NFSMAI website http://www.nfsmi.org/documentli- 44, 1425–1432.
braryfiles/PDF/20140912034919.pdf Phillips, K., Schieve, L., Visser, S., Boulet, S., Sharma,
National Foundation for Celiac Awareness. A. K., Boyle, C., & Yeargin-Allsopp, M. (2014).
(2012). Retrieved from NFCA website http:// Prevalence and impact of unhealthy weight in a
w w w. b e y o n d c e l i a c . o r g / S i t e D a t a / d o c s / national sample of US adolescents with autism and
NFCAWebina/9f1795d8ffd82e77/NFCA_Webinar_ other learning and behavioral disabilities. Maternal
The%20Gluten-Free%20Link_%20ADHD_Autism_ and Child Health Journal, 1964–1975.
Celiac%20Disease_slides.pdf Prado, E. L. (2014). Nutrition and brain development in
National Institute of Health. (2015). Celiac disease. early life. Nutrition Reviews, 267–284.
Retrieved April 22, 2016, from http://www.niddk. Presmanes Hill, A., Zuckerman, K., & Fombonne, E.
nih.gov/health-information/health-topics/digestive- (2015). Obesity and autism. Pediatrics, 1051–1061.
diseases/celiac-disease/Pages/facts.aspx Pusponegoro, H. D., Ismael, S., Firmansyah, A.,
Neal, E. G., Chaffe, H., Schwartz, R. H., Lawson, M. S., Sastroasmoro, S., & Vandenplas, Y. (2015). Gluten
Edwards, N., Fitzsimmons, G., … Cross, J. H. (2008). and casein supplementation does not increase symp-
The ketogenic diet for the treatment of childhood toms in children with autism spectrum disorder.
epilepsy: a randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Acta Paediatrica, 104(11), e500–e505. http://doi.
Neurology, 7(6), 500–506. http://doi.org/10.1016/ org/10.1111/apa.13108
S1474-4422(08)70092-9 Reiss, S., Levitan, G. W., & Szyszko, J. (1982). Emotional
Nickel, R. E. (1996). Controversial therapies for young disturbance and mental retardation: Diagnostic over-
children with developmental disabilities. Infants & shadowing. American Journal of Mental Deficiency,
Young Children, 8(4), 29–40. 86, 657–574.
Niehus, R., & Lord, C. (2006). Early medical his- Research Autism. (2015). Gluten-free, casein-free
tory of children with autism spectrum disorders. diet and autism. Retrieved from Research Autism
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), website http://researchautism.net/autism-interven-
S120–S127. tions/types/diets-and-supplements/special-diets/
Nigg, J. T., Lewis, K., Edinger, T., & Falk, M. (2012). gluten-free-casein-free-diet-and-autism
Meta-analysis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity dis- Ruskin, D. N., Svedova, J., Cote, J. L., Sandau, U., Rho,
order or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder J. M., Kawamura, M., Jr., … Masino, S. A. (2013).
symptoms, restriction diet, and synthetic food color Ketogenic diet improves core symptoms of autism
additives. Journal of the American Academy of Child in BTBR mice. PloS One, 8(6), e65021. http://doi.
& Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(1), 86–97. org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065021
Nikolov, R. N., Bearss, K. E., Lettinga, J., Erickson, C., Sandler, A. D., & Bodfish, J. W. (2000). Placebo
Rodowski, M., Aman, M. G., … Arnold, L. E. (2009). effects in autism: Lessons from secretin. Journal
Gastrointestinal symptoms in a sample of children of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(5),
with pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of 347–350.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 405–413. Sandler, A. D., Brazdziunas, D., Cooley, W. C., de Pijem,
Panksepp, J. (1979). A neurochemical theory of autism. L. G., Hirsch, D., Kastner, T. A., … Ruppert, E. S.
Trends in Neurosciences, 2, 174–177. http://doi. (2001). Counseling families who choose comple-
org/10.1016/0166-2236(79)90071-7 mentary and alternative medicine for their child with
Pavlov IP. (1904). The physiology of digestion (pp. 141– chronic illness or disability. Pediatrics, 107, 598–601.
155). Nobel Lecture (Physiology or Medicine). Sasselli, V., Pachnis, V., & Burns, A. J. (2012). The Enteric
Amsterdam: Elsevier. Nervous System. Developmental Biology, 366, 64–73.
Pedersen, L., Parlar, S., Kvist, K., Whiteley, P., & Schreck, K. A., Williams, K., & Smith, A. F. (2004). A
Shattock, P. (2013). Data mining the ScanBrit study comparison of eating behaviors between children
of a gluten- and casein-free dietary intervention for with and without autism. Journal of Autism and
22  Diet and Supplementation Targeted for ASD 425

Developmental Disorders, 34(4), 433–438. http://doi. adults with autism spectrum disorder: forewarned is
org/10.1023/B:JADD.0000037419.78531.86 forearmed. American Journal on Intellectual and
Seung, H., Rogalski, Y., Shankar, M., & Elder, J. (2007). Developmental Disabilities, 116(5), 371–380.
The gluten-and casein-free diet and autism: UCLA. (2016). Division of digestive diseases: Celiac dis-
Communication outcomes from a preliminary ease program. Celiac vs. gluten-sensitivity vs wheat
double-­blind clinical trial. Journal of Medical Speech allergies. Retrieved from http://gastro.ucla.edu/site.
Language Pathology, 15(4), 337. cfm?id=281
Sharp, W. G., Berry, R. C., McCracken, C., Nuhu, N. N., United States Food and Drug Administration. (1995).
Marvel, E., Saulnier, C. A., … Jaquess, D. L. (2013). Dietary supplement health and education act of 1994.
Feeding problems and nutrient intake in children with December, 1.
autism spectrum disorders: A meta-analysis and com- United States Food and Drug Administration. (2016a).
prehensive review of the literature. Journal of Autism Choose My Plate. Retrieved from Choose My Plate:
and Developmental Disorders, 43(9), 2159–2173. http://www.choosemyplate.gov/
Siri, K., & Lyons, T. (2011). Cutting-edge therapies for United States Food and Drug Administration. (2016b, 2
autism. New York, NY: Skyhorse Publishing. 28). Dietary guidelines. Retrieved from USDA: http://
Smeltzer, B. (2011). The medical side of the GF/CF diet www.cnpp.usda.gov/DietaryGuidelines
for autism: What is being treated and why it isn’t Valicenti-McDermott, M. D., McVicar, K., Cohen, H. J.,
just “autism”. Autism Science Digest: The Journal of Wershil, B. K., & Shinnar, S. (2008). Gastrointestinal
Autism., 1, 14–19. symptoms in children with an autism spectrum dis-
Srinivasan, P. (2009). A review of dietary interventions in order and language regression. Pediatric Neurology,
autism. Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 21(4), 237–247. 39(6), 392–398.
Stanovich, K. E. (1985). How to think straight about Vissoker, R. E., Latzer, Y., & Gal, E. (2015). Eating and
psychology. Glenview, IL: Scott Foresman/Addison-­ feeding problems and gastrointestinal dysfunction
Wesley Publishers. in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Stanovich, K. (2009). What intelligence tests miss: The Spectrum Disorders, 12, 10–21.
psychology of rational thought. New Haven, CT: Yale Wang, L. W., Tancredi, D. J., & Thomas, D. W. (2011).
University Press. The prevalence of gastrointestinal problems in chil-
Sukhodolsky, D. G., Scahill, L., Gadow, K. D., Arnold, dren across the United States with autism spectrum
L. E., Aman, M. G., McDougle, C. J., … Vitiello, B. disorders from families with multiple affected mem-
(2008). Parent-rated anxiety symptoms in children bers. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral
with pervasive developmental disorders: Frequency Pediatrics, 32, 351–360.
and association with core autism symptoms and Whiteley, P., & Shattock, P. (2002). Biochemical
cognitive functioning. Journal of Abnormal Child aspects in autism spectrum disorders: Updating the
Psychology, 36(1), 117–128. opioid-excess theory and presenting new opportu-
Taki, Y. (2012). Brain development in childhood. The nities for biomedical intervention. Expert Opinion
Open Neuroimaging Journal, 6, 103–110.: http://doi. on Therapeutic Targets, 6(2), 175–183. http://doi.
org/10.2174/1874440001206010103 org/10.1517/14728222.6.2.175
Tonutti, E., & Bizzaro, N. (2014). Diagnosis and clas- Williams, K. C., Fuchs, G. J., Furuta, G. T., Marcon,
sification of celiac disease and gluten sensitivity. M. A., & Coury, D. L. (2010). Clinical features associ-
Autoimmunity Reviews, 13(4–5), 472–476. http://doi. ated with GI symptoms in autism spectrum disorders
org/10.1016/j.autrev.2014.01.043 (ASD). Gastroenterology, 138(5), S-74.
Tuzikow, J. E., & Holburn, S. (2011). Identifying fad Williams, K. C., Christofi, F. L., Clemmons, T., Rosenberg,
therapies for autism spectrum disorders and promot- D., & Fuchs, G. J. (2012). Chronic GI symptoms in
ing effective treatment. In J. L. Matson & P. Sturmey children with autism spectrum disorders are associated
(Eds.), International handbook of autism and per- with clinical anxiety. Gastroenterology, 142(5), S-79.
vasive developmental disorders (pp. 307–319). Yao, C., Hao, R., Pan, S., & Wang, Y. (2012). Functional
New York NY/Heidelberg, Germany/Dordrecht, foods based on traditional Chinese medicine.
Netherlands/London, UK: Springer. Luxembourg: INTECH Open Access Publisher.
Tyler, C. V., Schramm, S. C., Karafa, M., Tang, A. S., Zhao, Y., & Encinosa, W. (2011). An update on hospital-
& Jain, A. K. (2011). Chronic disease risks in young izations for eating disorders, 1999 to 2009.
The TEACCH Program for People
with Autism: Elements, Outcomes, 23
and Comparison with Competing
Models

Javier Virués-Ortega, Angela Arnold-Saritepe,
Catherine Hird, and Katrina Phillips

Treatment and education of autistic-related autism found that over 30 % of parents were
communication-­handicapped children (TEACCH) currently using, or had used, a program based
is a family-centered lifelong approach to educa- upon the TEACCH model (Green et al., 2006).
tion for people with autism spectrum disorder. TEACCH is also used extensively in public
The specific services provided by TEACCH prac- schools around the world (Hess, Morrier, Heflin,
titioners depend on the needs of the family and & Ivey, 2008) and is a funded public health
may include assessment and diagnosis, interven- program in North Carolina.
tion, community integration, employment, and
supported living (Lord, Bristol, & Schopler,
1993). A common feature in all TEACCH ser- Historical Development
vices is the focus on utilizing the strengths
and abilities of the client, with its proponents The development of TEACCH began in the
placing an emphasis on TEACCH’s flexibility and 1960s when professionals were viewing autism
person-centered approach (Siegel, 2008). as a mental disorder caused by emotionally frigid
TEACCH has been used to support children, mothers and largely absent fathers. A young
adolescents, and adults with autism for over graduate student, Eric Schopler, came to believe
50 years, and it is widely recognized and imple- that this view was misguided and detrimental to
mented throughout the world (see, e.g., two spe- children and families (Schopler, 1971). In his
cial issues of the International Journal of Mental doctoral work, Schopler sought to demonstrate
Health published in 2000 for a global overview that autism was not an emotional illness caused
of TEACCH services). A multinational Internet by parenting styles but a disorder that affected
survey completed by parents of children with the processing of sensory information (Schopler,
1965, 1966). These studies were followed by
research into the perceptual differences in chil-
dren with autism (Reichler & Schopler, 1971)
J. Virués-Ortega (*) and their tendency to respond better when learn-
School of Psychology, The University of Auckland, ing skills in structured rather than unstructured
Room 335B Level 3 Bldg 301, Science Centre, 23 sessions (Schopler, Brehm, Kinsbourne, &
Symonds St, Private Bag 92019, Auckland 1010,
New Zealand Reichler, 1971). Schopler and colleagues con-
e-mail: j.virues-ortega@auckland.ac.nz cluded that people with autism were much better
A. Arnold-Saritepe • C. Hird • K. Phillips visual leaners than auditory learners and that they
The University of Auckland, Auckland, New Zealand benefited from structured learning environments.

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 427


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_23
428 J. Virués-Ortega et al.

These findings, in conjunction with a change in a way of thinking about the characteristic pat-
professionals’ attitudes toward the parental influ- terns of behavior seen in individuals with autism
ence in the etiology of autism, laid the ground- and used this concept as the basis for interven-
work for the development of TEACCH (Mesibov, tions leading to the long-term goal of community
Shea, & Schopler, 2005). participation (Mesibov et al., 2005).
The early years, of what was to become The notion of culture of autism emphasizes
TEACCH, were somewhat revolutionary in terms the preference toward processing visual informa-
of autism treatment. Schopler and child psychia- tion; strong attention to detail and limited general
trist Robert Reichler interacted directly with the attention; difficulty with organizing ideas, mate-
children working with their parents as co-­ rial, or activities; limited communication; attach-
therapists, rather than treating the parents as the ment to routines; restricted interests; and marked
cause of the problem. Children and parents were sensory preferences and dislikes. The goal for the
observed interacting, and therapists were TEACCH practitioner is then to support parents,
involved to support and model intervention tech- educators, and others who work with persons
niques that were practical. As these early clients with autism to see the world though their eyes.
aged, the developers extended TEACCH to Those working with the person aim to teach them
include adolescent and adult services (Mesibov to function as independently as possible while
et al., 2005). making the environment more understandable
Today the TEACCH model is no longer just for and suited to the needs of these individuals
children and their parents; it offers programs for (Mesibov et al., 2005).
individuals with autism across the life span. For
example, Van Bourgondien, Reichler, and Schopler
(2003) investigated the use of the TEACCH pro- Structured Teaching
gram in a residential program for adults with
autism; Orellana, Martinez-Sanchis, and Silvestre The structure teaching process is believed to
(2014) used a TEACCH-based approach to develop independence and self-esteem (Lord
increase compliance among clients during a dental et al., 1993; Mesibov & Howley, 2003; Schopler,
assessment; and Fischer-Terworth and Probst Mesiboc, & Hearsey, 1995). Structured teaching
(2011) incorporated TEACCH elements into a contains strategies tailored to suit the strengths of
treatment program for people with dementia, to people with autism. For example, the strong
mention a few notable examples. As TEACCH has emphasis on visual strategies and predictability
evolved over the last half century to meet the needs rests upon the assumption of a primarily visual
of its clients, the content of TEACCH programs and routine-based learning style (Schopler et al.,
has also evolved. Yet, TEACCH family-centered 1995; Mesibov & Shea, 2010).
philosophy has remained unaltered.

Eclecticism
The TEACCH Philosophy
TEACCH therapists often merge knowledge
TEACCH developers recognized that, despite the from a range of fields including speech language
variability among people who share the diagnos- therapy, social work, early intervention, special
tic label of autism, there were also many similari- education, and psychology (Lord et al., 1993).
ties. For them, autism involved lifelong challenges TEACCH’s eclectic approach incorporates tech-
in perception, communication, and learning. niques from many perspectives to achieve the end
Interventions should help to balance the unique goal of meeting the crucial needs of families:
characteristics of the person with autism and the managing challenging behaviors and promoting
social conventions of the wider community and learning. Thus, it is not unexpected that the
culture. They coined the term culture of autism as TEACCH approach to structured teaching shares
23 TEACCH 429

common features with other interventions includ- bined effectiveness of the various elements of
ing behavioral, developmental, and ecological structured teaching are rare.
perspectives (Lord et al., 1993). For example, in
the 1970s, TEACCH borrowed elements of the Visual Information  Visual information is
behavioral approach by incorporating contingen- intended to make tasks clear, meaningful, and
cies of reinforcement. Similarly, the behavioral understandable for the learner. Common ele-
approach to preference-based teaching (Reid & ments include instructions (i.e., verbal descrip-
Green, 2005) translates into TEACCH’s empha- tion of what a child has to do with the material
sis on preferred activities. It is expected that tasks provided for a task), a particular task organiza-
that are more meaningful to the learner will be tion (i.e., the separation and distribution of tasks
more easily understood, practiced, and general- into meaningful groups), and strategies to
ized. This emphasis on meaningful behaviors increase clarity (i.e., cues to what is the most
was also influenced in the 1980s by the increas- important aspect of the task). TEACCH practitio-
ing presence of cognitive social learning theory, ners use visual information in lieu of verbal
where meaningfulness was seen as crucial for instructions owing to the limited receptive lan-
generalizing behaviors from one learning envi- guage ability in this population.
ronment to another (Bandura, 1986). Visual information may take many forms
TEACCH owes to developmental psychology depending on the individual: from written check-
its aim to set learning goals after the developmen- lists to visual schedules and actual objects. For
tal level of each individual. The TEACCH model example, visual schedules provide information
has also adopted cognitive constructs to charac- about the order in which a series of tasks ought to
terize the deficits of people with autism in execu- be completed. Visual schedules help with transi-
tive functioning and attention. tions and can help the individual to become inde-
Despite its eclectic nature, there are key ele- pendent from the cues and prompts of others.
ments that are often present in structured teach- Visual schedules, also known as activity sched-
ing programs. These elements comprise the ules, have been studied extensively in the
cornerstone of TEACCH (Mesibov et al., 2005). behavior-­ analytic literature. Activity schedules
have been shown to increase skill acquisition and
on-task behavior (e.g., MacDuff, Krantz, &
Key Elements McClannahan, 1993) while decreasing challeng-
ing behavior (e.g., Dooley, Wilczenski, & Torem,
TEACCH proponents discourage a standardized 2001). However, a review by Lequia, Machalicek,
curriculum for each individual has his or her and Rispoli (2012) noted that setting, autism
unique learning style and skills. However, all severity, and existing verbal abilities moderate
TEACCH programs include some key elements the effectiveness of activity schedules.
of structured teaching: organization of the physi-
cal environment, visual information, task organi- Organization of the Physical Environment  The
zation, and work systems. Individual programs TEACCH practitioner modifies the environment
are developed within the structured teaching in order to clarify what is expected from the
framework and with consideration of the child’s learner and facilitates independence (Mesibov
developmental level and individual needs. Goals et al., 2005). For example, in a classroom setting,
often involve the development of communication the location of the furniture can be used to cue the
and daily living skills that would lead to func- upcoming activity. The discrimination can be
tional independence in adult life (Schopler et al., established by performing specific activities at
1995, Mesibov & Howley, 2003; Mesibov et al., specific locations—boundaries should also exist
2005). Although the TEACCH program has been at home for the learner to discriminate where to
evaluated as an omnibus approach, component eat, sleep, dress, and so forth. Welterlin, Turner-­
analyses to weighing the individual and com- Brown, Harris, Mesibov, and Delmolino (2012)
430 J. Virués-Ortega et al.

found that children’s work skills improved when ner uses one-on-one instruction, hand-over-hand
parents were taught to provide physical structure and visual prompts, social encouragement, and
by organizing furniture, creating boundaries, and access to preferred activities at the end of the ses-
arranging materials. Room arrangement can also sion, whereas for already mastered tasks, the
help to decrease stimulation (e.g., less things on child is expected to work independently in a
the wall), limit distractions (e.g., sitting in cubi- workstation with minimal distractions (Schopler
cles to work rather than at group tables), and pro- et al., 1995).
mote independence (e.g., having free access to
already mastered activities). In this connection,
Hume and Odom (2007) reported that minimiz- The TEACCH Program
ing visual and auditory distractions facilitates in the Outcome Literature
learning.
There have been few attempts to summarize sys-
Task Organization  Predictability within both tematically the TEACCH outcome literature.
activities and their sequences is believed to help Some of the barriers to effectively pool the avail-
the person with autism understand their environ- able evidence have to do with the notion of evi-
ment. Predictability can be achieved by a close dence present among TEACCH circles and with
adherence to activity schedules and routines. the nature of the empirical literature evaluating
These may include pre-planned sequences of the TEACCH program. Prominent commentators
steps used for tasks, chores, and recreation. The tend to present TEACCH as a diverse, lifespan-­
schedule provides information on where, when, long approach that is both a philosophy and a
and what the activity will be. It is recommended method informed by a culture of autism. These
this information be communicated visually. For attributes have been presented as a conceptual
example, Probst, Jung, Micheel, and Glen (2010) basis that places TEACCH above standard quan-
found a decrease in challenging behavior in a titative and meta-analytical methods for weigh-
young girl when schedules and a choice board ing clinical evidence (Mesibov & Shea, 2010).
were provided as part of their social skills train- These arguments, albeit informative, do not seem
ing. TEACCH proposes that when the environ- to justify non-evidence-based clinical practices.
ment does not provide routines, the individual For example, interventions such as those based
with autism will develop his or her own, which on applied behavior analysis have been indepen-
are likely to be less adaptive and acceptable. dently evaluated by psychometric assessments,
Although TEACCH is clear about the importance controlled trials, and meta-analyses, in spite of
of routines as a learning catalyst, it acknowledges the fact that standard methods for quantifying
that flexibility should be incorporated. clinical evidence differ from the single-subject
experimental tradition in which most behavior-­
Work Systems  While organizing the physical analytic research is grounded (see, e.g., Virués-­
space and providing predictable tasks are pre- Ortega, 2010).
sented as prerequisites for engaging the individ- Although the number, samples size, and
ual, a work system (also referred to as structured design of TEACCH program evaluations remain
work or activity systems) would still be needed to limited, advances in meta-analytical methods
convey the specific demands of a task. Work sys- have made it possible to aggregate the evidence
tems are sometimes referred to as structured available in a systematic and cogent manner.
work or activity systems. They tell the person Virues-Ortega, Julio, and Pastor (2013) compiled
what task to engage in, how much is required, the evidence from 12 TEACCH trials conducted
how long the activity will last, how to know when in eight countries. Although these studies did not
the activity is finished, and what happens after it produce the high methodological quality scores—
is finished. The work system may vary for new there were few randomized controlled trials—
and mastered tasks. For novel tasks, the practitio- they did have a number of favorable attributes:
23 TEACCH 431

(a) well-validated standardized assessments; (b) have been the result of outlier studies within the
led by multiple semi-independent international pool included in the meta-analysis or have been a
teams; (c) with few exceptions, those delivering product of a systematically biased publication
the intervention had been trained directly by the trend. While the effect on maladaptive behavior
original TEACCH center in North Carolina; and showed some promise, it required further replica-
(d) methodological quality scores and study tion before evidence-based recommendations
design were not associated with effect magni- could develop from these findings.
tude. The intervention induced very limited gains It is possible that the putative effects of the
in standardized evaluations of cognitive and ver- TEACCH program upon maladaptive behavior
bal performance and developmental status: over- may be parsimoniously characterized as the
all effect sizes fell within the small effect size product of noncontingent reinforcement, which
range. Similar effect magnitudes were reported is a well-documented procedure for attenuating
for communication, motor, daily living, and challenging behavior in the behavior-analytic lit-
social adaptive skills and also for more narrowly erature (see, e.g., Fischer, Iwata, & Mazaleski,
defined behavioral repertoires including eye-­ 1997). Specifically, the presentation of preferred
hand coordination, gross motor functioning, imi- social and leisure activities in a behavior-­
tation, and perceptual discrimination. Effects independent fashion, consistent with the empha-
within the negligible to moderate range deserve sis on preferred activities present in structured
limited commentary, as they are unlikely to be teaching sessions, may attenuate various forms of
compatible with clinically important gains. challenging behaviors.
Figure 23.1 illustrates some of these findings. According to Virues-Ortega et al. (2013),
Maladaptive behavior, as evaluated by the TEACCH studies tend to present greater gains in
Vineland subscale (Sparrow, Cicchetti, & Balla, the areas of perceptual and motor, adaptive, and
2005), may be an exception to the somewhat dis- verbal and cognitive abilities in studies con-
appointing findings offered by the pooled analy- ducted with school-age participants (6–17 years
sis. The outcome demonstrated a moderate-to-large of age), whereas studies targeting younger chil-
overall effect size. However, subsequent analyses dren (0–5 years of age) almost invariably pro-
suggested that the effect may be subject to signifi- duced nonsignificant effects sizes. Interestingly,
cant small-study and publication biases (Egger, important aspects of the interventions evaluated
Smith, Schneider, & Minder, 1997). Specifically, including participants developmental age during
the amelioration of maladaptive behavior may baseline, intervention intensity (hours per week),

Fig. 23.1  Effect sizes and 95 % confidence intervals of from the Psychoeducational Profile and selected IQ tests
selected TEACCH outcomes. Effect sizes above one sug- (Based on Virues-Ortega et al. (2013))
gest favorable treatment effects. Standardized outcomes
432 J. Virués-Ortega et al.

intervention duration (weeks of intervention),  EACCH and Applied Behavior


T
and intervention setting (home vs. school Analysis-Based Interventions
based) were not associated with significantly
larger or smaller effects in any of any of the Siegel (2008) discussed some procedural differ-
key dependent variables, most of them derived ences between the TEACCH model and discrete
from the Psychoeducational Profile (PEP) trial training (DTT), a common behavior-analytic
(Schopler, 1990). strategy. Siegel stated that TEACCH classrooms
While the literature summarized above is not typically utilize visual schedules for classroom
conclusive, it points to the facets of the TEACCH routines and workstations to organize and struc-
program that might be more promising, and it ture individual work, whereas in a classroom
helps to identify the weaknesses in the literature using DTT, adult prompting has a more preemi-
that require further replication. Some fundamen- nent role. The TEACCH model, relative to DTT,
tal aspects of the intervention make it hard to allows for more initiation from the child and
assess and optimize. First, key concepts of the encourages the child to problem solve. In a
intervention are not operationalized, and few TEACCH classroom, if a child is having difficulty
studies report treatment integrity. Second, the with a task, the teacher will wait and allow the
intervention model is fundamentally eclectic. child to complete the task. Similarly, if a child is
The latter added to the lack of experimental off-task, the teacher will allow the child to reen-
research supporting the key intervention ele- gage before presenting any prompts. Siegel sug-
ments make it impossible to build a self-­ gested that the child-directed nature of TEACCH
correcting body of evidence-based practices. was in contrast to DTT, where the child is
According to Virues-Ortega et al. (2013), prompted back to his or her task immediately.
TEACCH could be conceptualized as a targeted Siegel concludes that TEACCH is a more natural
intervention with narrowly defined strengths. way of teaching along the lines of typical child
Namely, the quantitative evaluation of the litera- development. Although Siegel does state that
ture does not support the adoption of TEACCH DTT is a method rather than a comprehensive ser-
as a comprehensive approach to address the vice, the contrast is somewhat misleading in that
wide array of behavioral and social challenges DTT is simply one of many behavioral strategies
posed by clients diagnosed with autism over their and cannot be equated with applied behavior anal-
life span. ysis—a similar error would involve comparing
early intensive behavioral intervention with
TEACCH workstations. Siegel commented only
 EACCH and Alternative
T on procedural differences but provided no formal
Intervention Models comparison of learning outcomes.
Eikeseth et al. (2002) conducted a yearlong
There is a dearth of studies directly comparing study with 4- to 7-year-old children with autism
the TEACCH program with other comprehensive who attended public kindergartens to assess the
psychosocial approaches to intervention for peo- efficacy of early intensive behavioral interven-
ple with autism. A few studies provide an indirect tion. They compared the outcomes of the behav-
preliminary comparison with early intensive ioral intervention with eclectic treatment that
behavioral intervention programs based on utilized aspects of TEACCH. Both groups
applied behavior analysis (Eikeseth, Smith, Jahr, received treatment, from a trained therapist, for at
& Eldevik, 2002; Howard, Sparkman, Cohen, least 20 h a week for the duration of the study.
Green, & Stanislaw, 2005), learning experiences Therapists in the behavioral intervention group
and alternative program for preschoolers (LEAP) were trained specifically for the study and had
(Boyd et al., 2014), and integrated classrooms more direct supervision and feedback than the
(Panerai, Ferrante, & Zingale, 2002; Panerai eclectic group, who received supervision via con-
et al., 2009). sultation. The children in the intensive behavior
23 TEACCH 433

treatment group initially received treatment in a ticipants were not randomly allocated to the
DTT format. These skills were then generalized intervention groups, to mention just a few. Some
to more ecological settings (e.g., peer interaction, of these limitations are inherited to the TEACCH
home). The children in the eclectic treatment approach. For example, TEACCH does not
group received treatment that involved many dif- involve a standardized curricula or a set of tech-
ferent components commonly used with children nologically defined procedures and is often pre-
with autism, which included features of both sented as a heterogeneous, lifelong approach that
TEACCH and applied behavior analysis. At the is both a philosophy and a method and therefore
end of the study, Eikeseth et al. (2002) found sig- not amenable to procedural integrity and stan-
nificant treatment gains for the children in the dardized treatment outcomes (Hume et al., 2011).
behavioral intervention group in comparison to The proposed lack of accountability added to the
the eclectic group. Assessment measures demon- preliminary negative findings by Eikeseth et al.
strated that children in the behavioral group (2002) and Howard et al. (2005) seem to lend
scored higher in areas such as language compre- little support to TEACCH as a comprehensive
hension and adaptive behavior relative to partici- approach to teaching children with autism.
pants in the eclectic group.
Howard et al. (2005) also investigated the
treatment differences between children with Other Treatment Comparisons
autism or pervasive developmental disorders not
otherwise specified receiving intensive behav- The learning experiences and alternative pro-
ioral intervention and those receiving an eclectic gram for preschoolers (LEAP) is a packaged
treatment that incorporated TEACCH. The chil- intervention with a blend of elements from
dren in the intensive behavioral group received applied behavior analysis and social learning
one-on-one treatment for 25–40 h a week across theory and a strong emphasis on social skills and
home, school, and community settings. This mainstream integration (Hollander, Kolevzon, &
group had structured and unstructured lessons Coyle, 2011). Boyd et al. (2014) worked with
delivered by tutors trained by a special education teachers in classrooms to compare the relative
teacher. A certified behavior analyst supervised efficacy of LEAP and TEACCH. The study had
the treatment team. The eclectic treatment group both strengths and weaknesses. In the positive
attended classrooms staffed with a teacher and side, the authors made an effort to evaluate treat-
four to eight assistants. The eclectic treatment ment fidelity. By contrast, lack on randomization
involved a combination of approaches, including and significant differences in autism severity
sensory integration therapy, TEACCH, and spo- across groups were significant shortcomings.
radic DTT sessions. The teacher received consul- While the authors reported significant changes
tation from graduates who had completed during follow-­up in communication, autism char-
coursework in applied behavior analysis. The acteristics, and other standardized outcomes after
children in the intensive behavioral group had a minimum 6-month exposure to either interven-
significantly higher scores on a variety of daily tion, the study failed to identify any differences
living, verbal, and academic measures relative to across groups.
the eclectic group. Both Eikeseth et al. (2002) Panerai et al. (2002) compared TEACCH to
and Howard et al. (2005) reported that the behav- integrated classrooms with support teachers. The
ior treatment group attained scores in standard- key characteristic of an inclusive education is that
ized tests closer to typical performance relative to the classroom and the teaching are not altered
those in the eclectic group. specifically for children with autism. In addition,
A number of methodological concerns limit the child may be supported by teacher aids and
these preliminary comparisons: interventions may receive additional intervention outside of the
were not equated in intensity and duration, proce- classroom environment (e.g., speech therapy).
dural integrity measures were lacking, and par- However, the integrated classroom group in the
434 J. Virués-Ortega et al.

study was not exposed to any autism-specific children a choice between behavioral in one loca-
teaching procedures, and the teachers lack any tion and TEACCH intervention in another loca-
training for working with this population. This tion. The children did not demonstrate a distinct
contrasted sharply with the TEACCH group in preference toward any of the two interventions.
the study, which was led by teachers, specialized Similar findings have been reported when teach-
in working with children with autism. Children in ers, parents, and administrators are identified as
the TEACCH group lived at the facility and went consumers (Callahan, Shukla-Mehta, Magee, &
home at regular intervals. The children in the Wie, 2010).
integrated group were at school during the day Assessing the commitment, satisfaction, and
and went home at the end of the school day. The burnout of practitioners of a given teaching
results showed larger improvements in the model provides an indirect means of studying
TEACCH group compared with the control group acceptability. According to Jennett, Harris, and
in all domains of the PEP. Panerai et al. (2009) Mesibov (2003), teachers using applied behavior
extended their initial analysis by including a analysis tend to be more committed to the under-
group of children who received TEACCH within lying philosophy of the approach when compared
a mainstream classroom and at home. According to teachers using TEACCH. Moreover, the teach-
to the results, children in the TEACCH groups ers that were more committed to a philosophy
demonstrated superior performance in the PEP had higher personal efficacy and satisfaction
relative to children in the inclusive education ratings and were less likely to report burnout in
group. However, the magnitude of the effect was relation to personal accomplishment. Coman
modest (effect size <0.5; see Fig. 23.1). The lack et al. (2013) compared LEAP, TEACCH, and
of improvement for children in inclusive educa- high-­quality special education programs. They
tion in these two studies may be attributable, not found that educators using LEAP were signifi-
to inclusive education per se, but rather to the cantly more committed to the underlying philos-
absence of autism specific approaches. ophy; no such difference was found among
In sum, Panerai et al. (2002, 2009) demon- TEACCH educators. Unsurprisingly teachers in
strated that eclectic interventions that include high-­quality special education programs showed
components tailored to the needs of children with similar level of commitment to both TEACCH
autism lead to superior albeit modest outcomes and LEAP philosophies. There were no differ-
than simply including children with autism into ences in reported burnout.
mainstream classrooms. Moreover, Eikeseth
et al. (2002) and Howard et al. (2005) were able
to demonstrate large intervention effects of early Summary
intensive behavioral intervention well above an
eclectic TEACCH group used as reference for The literature reviewed here suggests that we
comparison. should use caution before drawing any conclu-
sions about the effectiveness of TEACCH as a
comprehensive program, especially in compari-
Treatment Acceptability son to alternative comprehensive interventions.
The literature seems to be limited in two funda-
Treatment acceptability refers to consumers’ per- mental facets. First, there is a dearth of empirical
ceived preference for a particular model. research on the individual components of
Acceptability is important in that it leads to structured teaching and their combination
­
greater client engagement and service demand. (Mesibov & Shea, 2010). In the absence of such
Brower-Breitweiser, Miltenberger, Gross, Fuqua, evidence, it is difficult to gain a clear understand-
and Breitwieser (2008) assessed the treatment ing of the mechanisms underlying structured
acceptability of applied behavior analysis-based teaching and TEACCH more generally. Second,
intervention versus TEACCH by providing three researchers and practitioners lack the tools to
23 TEACCH 435

evaluate the integrity of the TEACCH-based controlled study. Behavior Modification, 26, 49–68.
doi:10.1177/0145445502026001004
intervention. The outcome research currently
Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1997).
available suggests that, while TEACCH may be Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as
as acceptable for consumers as other competing treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of
models, it has not performed well in stand-alone Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249. doi:10.1901/
jaba.1997.30-239
treatment evaluations and does not compare
Fischer-Terworth, C., & Probst, P. (2011). Evaluation
favorably to alternative models. of a TEACCH and music therapy based psycho-
logical intervention in mild to moderate demen-
tia. A controlled trial. Gero Psych, 24, 93–101.
doi:10.1024/1662-9647/a000037
References Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly,
M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treatments
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and used by parents of children with autism. Research in
action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, Developmental Disabilities, 27, 70–84. doi:10.1016/j.
NJ: Prentice-Hall. ridd.2004.12.002
Boyd, B. A., Hume, K., McBee, M. T., Alessandri, M., Hess, K. L., Morrier, M. J., Heflin, L. J., & Ivey, M. L.
Gutierrez, A., Johnson, L., et al. (2014). Comparative (2008). Autism treatment survey: Services received
efficacy of LEAP, TEACCH, and non-model-specific by children with autism spectrum disorders in
special education programs for preschoolers with public school classrooms. Journal of Autism and
autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 961–971. doi:10.1007/
Developmental Disorders, 44, 366–380. doi:10.1007/ s10803-007-0470-5
s10803-013-1877-9 Hollander, E., Kolevzon, A., & Coyle, J. T. (2011).
Braiden, H. J., McDaniel, B., McCrudden, E., Hanes, Textbook of autism spectrum disorders. Arlington, VA:
M., & Crozier, B. (2012). A practice-based evalua- American Psychiatric Publishing.
tion of Barnardo’s forward steps early intervention Howard, J. S., Sparkman, C. R., Cohen, H. G., Green, G., &
programme for children diagnosed with autism. Child Stanislaw, H. (2005). A comparison of intensive behav-
Care in Practice, 18, 227–242. doi:10.1080/1357527 ior analytic and eclectic treatments for young children
9.2012.683773 with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities,
Brower-Breitweiser, C. M., Miltenberger, R. G., Gross, 26, 359–383. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2004.09.005
A., Fuqua, W., & Breitwieser, J. (2008). The use of Hume, K., & Odom, S. (2007). Effects of an individual
concurrent operants preference assessment to evalu- work system on the independent functioning of students
ate choice of intervention for children diagnosed with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
with autism. International Journal of Behavioral Disorders, 37, 1166–1180.
Consultation and Therapy, 4, 270–278. Hume, K., Boyd, B., McBee, M., Coman, D., Gutierrez,
Callahan, K., Shukla-Mehta, S., Magee, S. & Wie, M. A., Shaw, E., et al. (2011). Assessing implementation
(2010) ABA versus TEACCH: The case for defin- of comprehensive treatment models for young chil-
ing and validating comprehensive treatment mod- dren with ASD: Reliability and validity of two mea-
els in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental sures. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 11,
Disorders, 40, 74–88. 1430–1440.
Coman, D. C., Alessandri, M., Gutierrez, A., Novotny, S., Jennett, H., Harris, S., & Mesibov, G. (2003). Commitment
Boyd, B. A., Hume, K., et al. (2013). Commitment to to philosophy, teacher efficacy, and burnout among
classroom model philosophy and burnout symptoms teachers of children with autism. Journal of Autism
among high fidelity teachers implementing preschool and Developmental Disorders, 33, 583–593.
programs for children with autism spectrum disorders. Lequia, J., Machalicek, W., & Rispoli, M. (2012). Effects
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, of activity schedules on challenging behaviour exhib-
345–360. ited in children with autism spectrum disorders: A
Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). systematic review. Research in Autism Spectrum
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle Disorders, 6, 480–492.
River, NJ: Pearson. Lord C., Bristol, M. M., & Schopler, E. (1993). Early
Dooley, P., Wilczenski, F., & Torem, C. (2001). Using an intervention for children with autism and related
activity schedule to smooth school transitions. Journal developmental disorders. In E. Schopler, M. E. Van
of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 57–61. Bourgondien, & M. M. Bristol (Eds.), Preschool issues
Egger, M., Smith, G. D., Schneider, M., & Minder, C. in autism (pp. 199–221). New York: Springer Science
(1997). Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, + Business Media. doi:10.1007/978-1-4899-2441-4
graphical test. British Medical Journal, 315, 629–634. MacDuff, G., Krantz, P., & McClannahan, L. (1993).
doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 Teaching children with autism to use photographic
Eikeseth, S., Smith, T., Jahr, E., & Eldevik, S. (2002). activity schedules: Maintenance and generaliza-
Intensive behavioral treatment at school for 4- to tion of complex chains. Journal of Applied Behavior
7-year-old children with autism: A 1-year comparison Analysis, 26, 89–97.
436 J. Virués-Ortega et al.

McConkey, R., Truesdale-Kennedy, M., Crawford, enjoy learning without problem behavior. Morganton,
H., McGreevy, E., Reavey, M., & Cassidy, A. NC: Habilitative Management Consultants.
(2010). Preschoolers with autism spectrum disor- Schopler, E. (1965). Early infantile autism and recep-
ders: Evaluating the impact of a home-based inter- tor processes. Archives of General Psychiatry, 13,
vention to promote their communication. Early 327–335.
Child Development and Care, 180, 299–315. Schopler, E. (1966). Visual versus tactual receptor prefer-
doi:10.1080/03004430801899187 ence in normal and schizophrenic children. Journal of
Mesibov, G., & Howley, M. (2003). Accessing the cur- Abnormal Psychology, 71, 108–114.
riculum for pupils with autistic spectrum disorders: Schopler, E. (1971). Parents of psychotic children as
Using the TEACCH programme to help inclusion. scapegoats. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy,
London: David Fulton. 4, 17–22.
Mesibov, G. B., & Shea, V. (2010). The TEACCH pro- Schopler, E. (1990). Psychoeducational profile, revised.
gram in the era of evidence-based practice. Journal of Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 570–579. Schopler, E., Brehm, S., Kinsbourne, M., & Reichler, R.
doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0901-6 (1971). Effect of treatment structure on development
Mesibov, G., Shea, V., & Schopler, E. (2005). The in autistic children. Archives of General Psychiatry,
TEACCH approach to autism spectrum disorders. 24(5), 415–421.
New York: Springer. Schopler, E., Mesiboc, G. B., & Hearsey, K. (1995).
Orellana, L. M., Martinez-Sanchis, S., & Silvestre, F. J. Structure teaching in teach system. In E. Schopler
(2014). Training adults and children with an autism & G. B. Mesibov (Eds.), Learning and cognition in
spectrum disorder to be compliant with a clinical autism (pp. 243–268). New York: Plenum.
dental assessment using a TEACCH-based approach. Siegel, B. (2008). Getting the best for your child with
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44, autism: An expert’s guide to treatment. New York: The
776–785. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1930-8 Guilford Press.
Ozonoff, S., & Cathcart, K. (1998). Effectiveness of a home Sparrow, S. S., Cicchetti, D. V., & Balla, D. A. (2005).
program intervention for young children with autism. Vineland adaptive behavior scales (2nd ed.). Circle
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28, Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
25–32. doi:10.1023/A:1026006818310 Tsang, S., Shek, D., Lam, L., Tang, F., & Cheung, P.
Panerai, S., Ferrante, L., & Zingale, M. (2002). Benefits of (2007). Brief report: Application of the TEACCH pro-
the treatment and education of autistic and communi- gram on Chinese pre-school children with autism —
cation handicapped children (TEACCH) programme does culture make a difference? Journal of Autism and
as compared with a non-specific approach. Journal of Developmental Disorders, 37, 390–396. doi:10.1007/
Intellectual Disability Research, 46, 318–327. s10803-006-0199-6
Panerai, S., Zingale, M., Trubia, G., Finocchiaro, M., Van Bourgondien, M. E., Reichler, N., & Schopler, E.
Zuccarello, R., Ferri, R., & Elia, M. (2009). Special (2003). Effects of a model treatment approach on adults
education versus inclusive education: The role with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental
of the TEACCH program. Journal of Autism and Disorders, 33, 131–140.
Developmental Disorders, 39, 874–882. Virués-Ortega, J. (2010). Applied behavior analytic inter-
Persson, B. (2000). Brief report: A longitudinal study of vention for autism in early childhood: Meta-analysis,
quality of life and independence among adult men meta-regression and dose-response meta-analysis of
with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental multiple outcomes. Clinical Psychology Review, 30,
Disorders, 30, 61–66. doi:10.1023/A:1005464128544 387–399. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.01.008
Probst, P., Jung, F., Micheel, J., & Glen, I. (2010). Virues-Ortega, J., Julio, F., & Pastor, R. (2013). The
Tertiary-preventive interventions for autism spectrum TEACCH program for children and adults with autism:
disorders (ASD) in children and adults: An evaluative A meta-analysis of intervention studies. Clinical
synthesis of two TEACCH based outcome studies. Psychology Review, 33, 940–953. doi:10.1016/j.
Life Span and Disability, 13, 129–167. cpr.2013.07.005
Reichler, R. J., & Schopler, E. (1971). Observations on Welterlin, A., Turner-Brown, L. M., Harris, S., Mesibov,
the nature of human relatedness. Journal of Autism G., & Delmolino, L. (2012). The home TEACCHing
and Childhood Schizophrenia, 1, 283–296. program for toddlers with autism. Journal of Autism
Reid, D.H., & Green, C.W. (2005). Preference-based and Developmental Disorders, 42, 1827–1835.
teaching: Helping people with developmental disabilities doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1419-2
Positive Behavior Support
24
Darlene Magito McLaughlin
and Christopher E. Smith

with community-based support efforts (Evans &


History and Definition of PBS Meyer, 1985; Horner et al., 1990).
Positive behavior support (PBS) emerged to
For well over half a century, applied behavior fill this need. In one of the earliest writings on
analysis (ABA) has had a tremendous impact on PBS, Carr (1997) articulated the need to focus on
the field of autism, contributing many of the tools consumer goals pertaining to comprehensive
and strategies that are now routine practice for lifestyle support, long-term change, and direct
individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) support to consumers themselves. Carr predicted
who display problem behavior. Applied behavior that the field of applied behavior analysis would
analysis is specifically credited with the develop- split into two branches in order to address this
ment of functional assessment, which is univer- need. One branch of the field (still referred to as
sally accepted as the “gold standard” of ABA) would maintain a traditional focus on con-
behavioral assessment. It has articulated princi- ceptual purity, emphasizing elegant experimental
ples of reinforcement and consequences, which control, and a microanalysis of cause and effect
are now firmly established as key determinants of from a researcher’s point of view. The second
behavior. Applied behavior analysis has also put and newer branch (now referred to as PBS)
forth learning theory, establishing the key prin- would focus on ecological relevance, emphasiz-
ciples and practices that promote skill acquisi- ing meaningful outcomes in the form of lifestyle
tion. However, in the late 1980s and early 1990s, change, and prioritizing a broader analysis of
the emergence of specific social movements and systems and closer attention to practicality, as
ecological variables, such as the normalization defined by non-researchers (i.e., consumers).
movement, the inclusion movement, and the era The first research monograph on PBS was
of person-centered values created the need for a published in 1999 by Carr and his colleagues in
new approach to intervention that was more conjunction with the American Association on
responsive to consumers (e.g., individuals with Mental Retardation (Carr et al., 1999a). In their
disabilities and their families) and more in line review, the authors elaborated on the defining
characteristics of PBS, referring to PBS as inter-
ventions that are designed to increase the proba-
D.M. McLaughlin, PhD, BCBA-D (*) bility of functional positive behaviors by way of
C.E. Smith, PhD, BCBA-D
Positive Behavior Support Consulting &
building key skills (e.g., communication, self-­
Psychological Resources, P.C, Centerport, NY, USA management, and social skills) and changing key
e-mail: posbeh@positivebehavior.org elements in the environment (e.g., activity

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 437


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_24
438 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

p­ atterns, choice options, prompting procedures). Interventions (JPBI), and the creation of a new
These changes often result in measurable improve- professional organization, the Association for
ments in social, vocational, and educational status Positive Behavior Support (APBS), specifically
(i.e., lifestyle change) and concomitant decreases dedicated to this work.
in problem behavior. Thus, PBS refers to interven-
tions that increase positive behaviors, promote life-
style change, and result in decreases in problem Critical Features of PBS
behavior. Using this framework, the monograph
attempted to answer several research questions In 2002, Carr et al. published a key article in the
including the following: “How widely applicable is Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions enti-
PBS?”; “In what ways is the field evolving?”; tled, “Positive Behavior Support: Evolution of an
“How effective is PBS and what factors modulate applied science.” This paper outlined nine “criti-
its effectiveness?”; and “How responsive is the cal features” of Positive Behavior Support that
PBS literature to the needs of consumers and non- now define this body of work. The nine critical
researchers?” Following their review, the authors features included (1) comprehensive lifestyle
concluded that PBS was a viable approach to inter- change and quality of life, (2) lifespan perspec-
vention that was widely applicable and could be tive, (3) ecological validity, (4) stakeholder par-
implemented by typical intervention agents in typi- ticipation, (5) social validity, (6) systems change
cal settings (Carr et al., 1999a). and multicomponent intervention, (7) prevention,
Over the years, PBS has shown steady and (8) flexibility with respect to scientific practices,
dramatic growth in its principles and procedures, and (9) multiple theoretical perspectives. In the
and interventions are now more likely than ever decade or so since the paper was published, there
before to focus on deficient contexts, rather than have been several new contributions to the PBS
deficient repertoires (Koegel, Koegel, & Dunlap, literature that have illustrated and elucidated
1996). The initial wave of studies that were these features. A review of this progress will
reviewed in the monograph showed that PBS serve as the framework for the present discus-
resulted in substantial improvements in problem sion. In each of the sections below, critical fea-
behavior in approximately two-thirds of the tures of PBS will be defined and illustrated by
cases, and these improvements were generally case studies and research examples from the lit-
maintained over time (Carr et al., 1999a). Thus, erature. In addition, a discussion of measurement
PBS showed promise as an approach for working is presented within each feature to highlight the
with students with serious disabilities, including new data collection strategies that have evolved
autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, as in order to capture the broader concepts of
Carr and his colleagues (1999a) noted, there was PBS. While this information is by no means
still much work to be done. At the conclusion of exhaustive, it is meant to provide an overview of
their review, the authors suggested that the goals the work that has been done to advance this rela-
of PBS could most plausibly be met by an tively new field of endeavor.
increasing emphasis on multicomponent inter-
ventions that are linked to assessment informa-
tion, a broad reorganization of context (systems Comprehensive Lifestyle Change
change), an emphasis on ecological validity
(interventions involving typical agents and typi- Comprehensive lifestyle change is the first criti-
cal settings), an intervention in all relevant con- cal feature of PBS and is aimed at improving a
texts, and the application of practices over variety of quality of life dimensions. These can
protracted periods of time. These recommenda- include improvements in social relationships
tions established the framework for a PBS move- (e.g., friendship formation), personal satisfaction
ment, which resulted in the development of a (e.g., self-confidence, happiness), employment
new journal, the Journal of Positive Behavior (e.g., productivity, job prestige, good job match),
24  Positive Behavior Support 439

self-determination (e.g., personal control, choice Process for four individuals with severe disabili-
of living arrangements, independence), recreation ties and challenging behavior. The five-step
and leisure (e.g., adequate opportunities, good ­lifestyle planning process included vision plan-
quality of activities), community adjustment (e.g., ning, assessing and remediating barriers to partici-
domestic skills, survival skills), and community pation, assembling meaningful routines and
integration (e.g., mobility, opportunities for par- schedules, developing specific intervention strate-
ticipation in community activities, school inclu- gies, and evaluating effectiveness by way of devel-
sion). The person’s daily routines, schedules, and oping a monitoring system. Following intervention,
social interactions are thus important consider- all four participants engaged in a greater number
ations. Outcome success emphasizes improve- of integrated activities during the mid- and post-­
ments in family life, jobs, community inclusion, intervention periods, compared with baseline.
supported living, expanding social relationships, Three of the four participants experienced gains of
and personal satisfaction. more than 200% in the number of preferred, inte-
Turnbull and Turnbull (1996) offered an grated activities they performed at the end of the
example of lifestyle change in their discussion of intervention period. The two adults in the study
JT, a 26-year-old man with autism and behavioral were engaged in the first integrated work opportu-
challenges. Here, the goals of intervention were nities of their lives and two children participated to
not limited to behavioral change per se but were various degrees in regular classroom activities in
rather focused on lifestyle change. Treatment their neighborhood schools. Furthermore, all four
objectives were to have JT continue his paid participants experienced at least slight increases in
work at a university as a clerical aide; to support their unpaid social networks and performed a
JT in living in a home of his own, with room- greater number of integrated activities with people
mates (university students) who were available to who were not paid to spend time with them.
meet his personal support needs; to assist JT in Finally, all four participants showed evidence of
using local public transportation to and from improved behavior and communication skills over
work; and to support JT in maintaining friend- the course of the intervention.
ships and community connections at his favorite In an effort to evaluate lifestyle change, the
community spots–a local bakery, two jazz clubs, field has seen an emergence of empirically vali-
restaurants with live music, a church, a neighbor- dated measures designed to capture this complex
hood grocery store, and a fitness center. The ecology. For example, the Resident Lifestyle
authors noted that for JT, the criteria for “assess- Inventory (Wilcox & Bellamy, 1987) measures
ing success” constantly changed in response to the types of activities that are performed by an
the complex ecology of his emerging lifestyle. individual, how often each activity occurs, where
Following a 6-year planning process referred to each activity typically occurs, which activities
as Group Action Planning (Turnbull & Turnbull, are preferred, and the level of support needed for
1996), JT worked a total of 30 hours per week at participation. The Social Network Analysis
the university as a clerical aide; he moved to a Interview (Kennedy, Horner, & Newton, 1990)
home of his own along with two roommates from elicits information about the persons who are
the university who each provided him with socially important in the life of the target indi-
12–15 hours of support; he learned to take public vidual and the types and frequencies of activities
transportation to work; he joined a fraternity; and in which persons in the social network typically
he made several friends in the community with engage with the individual. The Program Quality
whom he maintained regular contact. Indicators Checklist (Meyer, Eichinger, & Park-­
In a second example, Malette, Mirenda, Jones, Lee, 1987) identifies the “most promising prac-
Bunz, and Rogow (1992) presented a series of tices” in educational programs for persons with
case studies that evaluated lifestyle changes asso- severe disabilities, as gleaned from a literature
ciated with a Lifestyle Development Planning review and survey of nationally recognized
440 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

experts in the field. It can assess the content of a McConnachie, and Pierson (2001) presented a
plan’s goals and objectives and track changes 63-month evaluation of a 35-year-old man who
over time. The Ecocultural Family Interview moved from a developmental center into his own
(Weisner, Coots, & Bernheimer, 1997) assesses home in the community. At baseline, the man
resources in the home environment, family con- demonstrated a high level of problem behaviors
nectedness, social networks, and leisure activi- that included self-injurious behavior (head hits),
ties. Taken together, these tools represent a vomiting, assault, and property destruction.
growing body of research aimed at measuring These behaviors occurred at a high frequency
lifestyle change as an important feature of PBS. and posed an immediate danger to the partici-
pant and to others, requiring the use of mechani-
cal restraints and medications. Following a
Lifespan Perspective comprehensive functional analysis of problem
behavior, the team implemented a series of inter-
The second critical feature of PBS is lifespan per- vention phases that included the following:
spective. A lifespan perspective recognizes that Phase I (7/95–4/96) involved providing commu-
efforts to achieve meaningful change occur over nity access to increase the man’s activity level
time. Thus, intervention is seen as a systemic pro- and provide an opportunity for him to control the
cess that evolves as different challenges arise dur- environment. Phase 2 (5/96–11/96) involved
ing different stages of life (Turnbull, 1988; teaching the man to appropriately request the use
Vandercook, York, & Forest, 1989). Carr et al. of a restraint chair since restraint was identified
(2002) noted that when one follows an individual as a reinforcer. Phase 3 (11/96–10/97) involved
over many years in changing life circumstances, moving to a home in the community, which was
deficient environments and deficient adaptive intended to further minimize the setting factors
skills will almost certainly continue to emerge and antecedents for problem behavior. Phase 4
and be identified. Therefore, new PBS strategies (10/97–2/98) involved introducing a new
may have to be added and old ones modified. In a recliner to replace the restraint chair. Phase 5
truly comprehensive PBS approach, intervention (3/98–4/99) involved removing a medication
never ends and follow-up is measured in decades, that appeared to be exacerbating problem behav-
not months (Carr et al., 2002). Kennedy and ior. Finally, Phase 6 (5/99–10/99) involved mov-
Itkonen (1996) illustrated this principle in their ing to a second home in the community that was
discussion of social relationships across the life purchased by the man’s parents. In the last
span. The authors suggested that support environ- 2 years of the study, the participant was reported
ments vary along a number of dimensions includ- to have had no injuries from self-injurious
ing the manner in which people with severe behaviors and no injuries to staff members. His
disabilities are grouped/clustered, the people who vomiting ceased, and his medication to prevent it
are contacted, the availability of specific types of was discontinued. The authors reported that his
activities, the emphasis and support available for quality of life improved significantly in other
social relationships, and the physical proximity to areas as well. Moving to his own home allowed
typical community settings. Each of these dimen- him to participate in more community activities,
sions can facilitate or inhibit social relationships; to develop relationships, to participate in the hir-
therefore, interventions may vary greatly across ing of support staff, and to have greater choice
an individual’s life span and also within any group over activities, meals, and his own schedule.
of people of a particular age. From a lifespan perspective, the PBS interven-
The lifespan perspective has led to an increase tions that were implemented addressed known
in the number of longitudinal studies that have functions of problem behavior while at the same
been done to evaluate the impact of PBS over time responding to new ­developments and life
time (Carr et al., 1999b; Dunlap et al., 2010; changes that the man encountered over a period
Lucyshyn et al., 2007). A case study by Jensen, of several years.
24  Positive Behavior Support 441

In their longitudinal study, Carr et al. (1999b) ability, willingness to try new things, skill acqui-
applied a multicomponent PBS intervention over a sition, self-confidence, emotional stability, and
period of 1.5–2.5 years with three adults with general health and well-being.
autism who demonstrated severe problem behav- Carr et al. (2002) recommended that measures
ior. The results showed favorable outcomes on of PBS capture three things: problem behavior,
dependent variables (i.e., task engagement, prob- implementation of PBS plans, and quality of life
lem behavior) that were maintained over time. changes over time. Recently, it has been suggested
Importantly, the authors described several changes that a full battery of assessments, including stan-
in the participants’ life circumstances, such as resi- dardized instruments, structured interviews, and
dential and employment status and recreational checklists that are individualized on the basis of
opportunities, as well as the need for follow-up the participant’s age, developmental status, and
assessments and adjustments to the intervention living circumstances, may be required (Dunlap
plans as time passed and new stressors influenced et al., 2010). Thus, the development of an individ-
the participants’ life situations. A more recent lon- ualized “case portfolio” has been identified as a
gitudinal study, conducted by Dunlap et al. (2010), venue for collecting and analyzing data, with some
involved a multisite evaluation of PBS across a instruments administered annually or semiannu-
2-year period. The authors compiled multiple out- ally, some periodically (i.e., on an as-needed
come measures from 22 participants in five areas basis), and some continually (e.g., contact logs,
of the country. They summarized information journals) (Dunlap et al., 2010). Here, quality of
from nearly 20 different data sources and found life was defined along six dimensions: (1) material
modest improvements in nearly all of the partici- well-being, which includes access to materials or
pants’ problem behavior and quality of life relative activities that are preferred by the individual and
to baseline, with improvement generally maintain- may enhance the individual’s pleasure or ability to
ing over the 2 years of the study. function effectively; (2) health and safety, which
Since PBS is designed to produce or facilitate includes health status, medication effects, safety
enduring improvements across the life span, there risks, or physical disturbances; (3) social well-
has been increased attention to the measurement being and interpersonal competence, which refers
of quality of life (Hughes, Hwang, Kim, to social networks, presence of friends, and capa-
Eisenman, & Killian, 1995). As a construct, qual- bilities for interacting; (4) emotional and affective
ity of life pertains to the full breadth of a person’s well-being, which includes outward emotional
existence, across all settings and hours of the day, response, evidence of happiness, mood, and emo-
and for periods of years, rather than the usual tional stability; (5) leisure and recreation, which
weeks or months of intervention research. For the includes activities that the individual engages in to
most part, quality of life has been evaluated using occupy him/herself for pleasure; and (6) personal
subjective rating scales, including the Quality of well-­ being, which includes self-sufficiency and
Life Questionnaire (Schalock & Keith, 1993), the independence, self-determination, and choice
Quality of Life Evaluation (Kincaid, Knoster, regarding personal belongings, activities, clothing,
Harrower, Shannon, & Bustamante, 2002), and food, living arrangements, and relationships
the Family Quality of Life Survey (Park et al., (Dunlap et al., 2010). The authors applied a 9-point
2003). To illustrate, the Quality of Life Evaluation Likert-type rating scale (1 = substantial deteriora-
(Kincaid et al., 2002) uses a 5-point Likert-type tion; 5 = no changes relative to baseline; 9 = sub-
rating scale (1 = much worse, 2 = somewhat stantial improvement) to summarize and evaluate
worse, 3 = no change, 4 = somewhat better, 5 = overall changes in quality of life before and after
much better) to evaluate changes in a person’s PBS strategies were implemented. This approach
life circumstances over the course of PBS inter- allowed information from multiple sources to be
ventions. The survey items tap a variety of synthesized in a meaningful way, creating the
quality of life changes including relationships, potential for an ongoing analysis of quality of life
community contacts, satisfaction, expressive changes across the lifespan.
442 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

Ecological Validity Moes and Frea (2000) presented an elaborate


case study that examined the issue of ecological
Another critical feature of PBS is ecological valid- validity. Their study described a PBS interven-
ity. PBS addresses quality of life issues in natural tion with a 3-year-old boy named Matthew who
contexts, including home, school, and community. had ASD and challenging behavior. The study
In other words, PBS focuses on how the scientific compared the child and family outcomes when
principles underlying the PBS approach can be the intervention was directed solely by the inter-
applied to real-life settings and situations as they ventionist (the prescriptive approach) or in col-
pertain to a particular individual. This is what is laboration with the family (the contextualized
referred to as ecological validity. Ecological valid- approach). The prescriptive approach included
ity involves examining whether typical interven- the use of standardized protocols and structured
tion agents (e.g., parents, teachers) can carry out teaching formats to direct intervention efforts,
interventions in typical settings (e.g., the home, while the contextualized approach included an
the school, the community, the workplace) where assessment of settings, values, and beliefs to
they support individuals with ASD (Carr et al., inform the process and increase compatibility
2002). Singer (2000) has emphasized the need for between intervention elements and known family
service delivery systems to provide PBS in real- routines and practices (Moes & Frea, 2000). All
life settings. Interventions need to be evaluated in assessment and intervention sessions took place
natural settings and implemented by typical inter- in Matthew’s home and were implemented by his
vention agents in those settings. This has also been parents and 4-year-old brother during routines in
referred to as “contextual fit.” Crone and Horner which he was expected to clean up after himself
(2003) defined contextual fit as the congruence (e.g., putting toys away). The prescriptive treat-
between behavior interventions and the values, ment package included treatments derived from
skills, resources, and routines of the individuals the literature, including functional communica-
who will be implementing those interventions. tion training (i.e., requesting a “break”), extinc-
McLaughlin, Denney, Snyder, and Welsh (2012) tion, and demand fading. The contextualized
conducted a review of studies published in the intervention incorporated family preferences
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions (JPBI) gathered during assessment. For example,
to examine the extent to which family-­centered Matthew’s parents indicated that they wanted to
interventions with contextual fit have appeared in reward him for following through with parental
the journal. Contextual fit was defined as (1) requests, so a treatment component was built in
acknowledgement of the cultural and linguistic to enable Matthew to earn a desired item/activity
background of families; (2) collaborative partner- of his choice after spending 5 min cleaning up
ship with families for assessment, planning, imple- after himself. His parents felt that teaching
mentation, and evaluation of the behavior support Matthew to request “help” rather than “break”
intervention; (3) consideration of family ecology; would be more helpful in guiding their interac-
(4) selection of family activity settings as contexts tions during the “cleaning up” routine. His par-
for intervention; (5) parents or familial caregivers’ ents also requested modifications to the prompt
perspectives about acceptability, feasibility, effec- sequence used to facilitate cooperation. They
tiveness, sustainability, and satisfaction; and (6) reported some level of past success with a
family quality of life (McLaughlin et al., 2012). ­three-­step prompting sequence, starting with a
Their review included 18 research studies and verbal prompt to complete the request, then a
demonstrated that family-­ implemented interven- reminder of what could be earned (e.g., “If you
tions were successful in teaching positive behav- pick up your shirt you can play with ____”), and
iors and decreasing problem behaviors in children then, if necessary, a verbal “count to 3” proce-
with ASD. Furthermore, the studies that included dure (“1,2,3…”) to foster cooperation. Finally,
maintenance data showed that positive outcomes Matthew’s parents wanted his older brother to be
were maintained over time. a part of the intervention; he was expected to
24  Positive Behavior Support 443

model and follow the plan that was developed for Goodness-of-Fit Survey which includes 20 items
his brother (Moes & Frea, 2000). that assess the degree to which a support plan is
The results showed that in baseline, Matthew appropriately matched to the environment. The
engaged in high levels of disruptive behavior and authors used a 5-point Likert-type rating scale (1 =
did not use functional communication. When the not at all; 2 = not much; 3 = can’t tell; 4 = well
prescriptive intervention approach was imple- (much); and 5 = very well (very much)) to deter-
mented, Matthew began using the functional mine whether the plan applied to all relevant set-
communication response to request a “break,” but tings and social demands that the child encounters;
was not engaging in on-task behavior. The whether the plan considered the caregiver’s under-
demand-fading procedure was then introduced, standing, expectations, and comfort level with the
and Matthew’s disruptive behaviors returned to child; whether the plan reflected the highest prior-
baseline levels. At the end of intervention, his par- ity goals; whether the plan fit in with the daily rou-
ents reported that they “couldn’t tell” how well tines and successes of the family; and whether the
the plan fit in with their beliefs, values, goals, plan was feasible and sustainable over time. The
abilities, and needs (Moes & Frea, 2000). When Self-Assessment of Contextual Fit (Horner,
the contextualized intervention was implemented, Salentine, & Albin, 2003) is a second instrument
Matthew showed a decrease in disruptive behav- that has been used to evaluate PBS plans at the
ior and an increase in on-task behavior (Moes & statewide level (KIPBS, 2010). This 16-item scale
Frea, 2000). Furthermore, parent ratings indicated uses a 6-point Likert-type rating (1 = strongly dis-
that both parents found the intervention to be agree; 2 = moderately disagree; 3 = barely dis-
highly compatible with their beliefs, values, agree; 4 = barely agree; 5 = moderately agree; 6 =
goals, abilities, and needs (Moes & Frea, 2000). strongly agree) to evaluate eight dimensions of
Fox and Emerson (2001) described the need contextual fit, including knowledge of elements in
for examining the perceived value of outcomes by the support plan, skills needed to implement the
various stakeholders who participate in the inter- plan, values consistent with plan elements,
vention process. They found that reduction in the resources available to implement the plan, overall
severity of challenging behavior was considered support for the plan, effectiveness of the plan, best
the most important outcome of intervention for a interest of the person, and efficiency of plan imple-
child/young adult living with his or her family in mentation. Thus, the inclusion of “goodness of fit”
four out of seven stakeholder groups in the study. measures addresses ecological validity as a critical
For adults living in group homes, only three out of feature of PBS.
seven stakeholder groups identified challenging
behavior as the highest priority. Other outcomes
that were considered “most important” included Stakeholder Participation
increased friendships, increased relationships,
learning alternative ways of getting needs met, The next critical feature of PBS is stakeholder
increased control, and empowerment (Fox & participation. Stakeholder participation assumes
Emerson, 2001). The authors concluded that the that all members of an individual’s support team
evaluation of outcomes should maximize the are relevant stakeholders (e.g., parents, siblings,
“goodness of fit” between intervention, the evalu- neighbors, teachers, job coaches, friends, room-
ation of the intervention, and the beliefs of the mates, and the person with disabilities) and par-
stakeholders involved (Albin, Lucyshyn, Horner, ticipate as partners to build the vision, methods,
& Flannery, 1996). and success criteria pertinent to defining quality
To address ecological validity, several research- of life for everyone concerned. Stakeholders have
ers have begun to define and measure “goodness an active role in providing valuable qualitative
of fit” to evaluate whether a plan fits with the perspectives for assessment purposes; in deter-
overall values and lifestyle of those who are mining whether proposed prevention strategies
implementing it. Albin et al. (1996) developed a are relevant for all of the challenging situations
444 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

that need to be dealt with; in evaluating whether musicians into JT’s life. Finally, the family
the approach taken is practical and in line with looked for strategic community leaders who were
the values, needs, and organizational structures natural “matchmakers” with inclusive commu-
related to the individual with disabilities and his nity opportunities. They reached out to someone
or her support network; and in defining what out- from the church, who also happened to work at
comes are likely to improve the general quality of the local bakery and knew almost everyone in
life and enhance the individual’s personal satis- town. This person had keen communication skills
faction (Carr, 2002). and acted as a group facilitator. JT’s planning
Vaughn, Dunlap, Fox, Clarke, and Bucy (1997) team expanded from just a few professionals and
presented a case study that detailed a community- parents to a rich and extensive arena of family,
based intervention with a 9-year-old boy with sig- friends, and community citizens. These stake-
nificant disruptive and destructive behaviors. A holders were passionately committed to him.
collaborative team that included the boy’s mother Collectively, they created a context for social
designed and implemented functional assessments connectedness and interdependent caring; they
and hypothesis-based interventions in three set- engaged in dynamic and creative problem-­
tings: a drive-through bank, a large grocery store, solving and took action steps in order to identify
and a fast-food restaurant. Data showed that the and achieve shared goals. In other words, critical
interventions reduced problem behaviors in all people were present from all different environ-
three settings and that concomitant increases were ments in which JT participated so there could be
observed in desirable mother-child interactions. coordination and sharing of responsibility. Over
The PBS procedures, which included the presenta- time, waitpersons understood how to provide
tion of competing reinforcers in each environment additional support when JT seemed anxious at
(e.g., toys, picture book/schedule), were conducted the restaurant he frequented; bus drivers knew
by a parent who was also explicitly involved in the how to get him home safely if he got on the
assessment process as well as the design and eval- wrong bus; and people in his neighborhood
uation of the intervention program. Several other watched out for him to make sure he was OK.
studies in the literature have demonstrated the effi- A variety of similar planning and problem-­
cacy of using natural supports, including parents solving processes have been articulated in the
(Clarke, Dunlap, & Vaughn, 1999), siblings literature under the broad umbrella of person-­
(Walton & Ingersoll, 2012), teachers (Lee, Sugai, centered planning. These processes include
& Horner, 1999), and paraprofessionals (Feldman Lifestyle Planning (O’Brien, 1987), Personal
& Matos, 2013) as intervention agents. However, Futures Planning (Mount, 1987; Mount &
far fewer studies to date have incorporated com- Zwernick, 1988), The McGill Action Planning
munity members as natural helpers. System (Forest & Lusthaus, 1987; Vandercook
The earlier example of JT (Turnbull & et al., 1989), Framework for Accomplishment/
Turnbull, 1996) illustrates the potential role of Personal Profile (O’Brien, Mount, & O’Brien,
natural helpers. JT’s family engaged in a collab- 1991), and Essential Lifestyle Planning (Smull &
orative process referred to as Group Action Harrison, 1992). These person-centered planning
Planning to design JT’s PBS plan. The initial activities share many similarities. Most of the
planning group consisted of JT along with his approaches utilize group graphics (large paper
parents and his sisters, family friends, and a few and marker drawings) and facilitation techniques
friends from school. This initial group supported to involve groups in learning more about the per-
him in achieving employment and a home of his son and his or her family and planning for a more
own. At that point, his job coach, a few cowork- positive future (Kincaid, 1996). In addition, these
ers, and his roommates became additional Action approaches share a commitment to five essential
Group members. Later on, because JT deeply goals, outcomes, or valued accomplishments,
enjoyed music, the family included a music ther- which include (1) being present and participating
apy teacher who in turn brought in dozens of in community life, (2) gaining and maintaining
24  Positive Behavior Support 445

satisfying relationships, (3) expressing prefer- able to draw others into the process, whether the
ences and making choices in everyday life, (4) meeting progressed at a comfortable pace, how
having opportunities to fulfill respected roles and responsive the facilitator was to the person’s
to live with dignity, and (5) continuing to develop input and point of view, and how satisfied mem-
personal competencies. Mount (1994) addresses bers of the group were with the outcomes of the
many of the benefits and limits of Personal planning process. Tools such as this hold promise
Futures Planning. Benefits of the process include for addressing stakeholder participation as a criti-
developing a positive view of the person, inspir- cal feature of PBS.
ing motivation in participants, empowering peo-
ple with disabilities as well as their family and
friends, involving and developing community Social Validity
relationships, and producing organizational
change. However, as Mount (1994) pointed out, Social validity has been identified as another crit-
the integrity of the process is challenged if the ical feature of PBS (Carr et al., 2002). Social
emphasis moves away from what the person validity refers to whether or not interventions are
needs and wants and centers on what the system seen by intervention agents as practical (e.g.,
needs and wants. Mount argued that person-­ Can typical intervention agents carry out the
centered planning is not a process that can be strategies?), desirable (e.g., Do typical interven-
standardized, implemented on a large scale, or tion agents view the strategies as being worthy of
molded to fit into the existing structures of a ser- implementation?), and appropriate for the con-
vice system (Mount, 1994). Instead, it is designed texts in which they are to be implemented. Social
to challenge systems to adapt to the unique needs validity has also been defined along the dimen-
of the person served. Kincaid (1996) summarized sions of whether intervention procedures effec-
the process of person-centered planning as tively reduced problem behavior (e.g., Do
follows: stakeholders view the strategies as having made a
The Personal Profile and Futures Plan initiate the meaningful difference in reducing problem
approach, additional information is obtained behavior to acceptable levels?) and whether the
through various activities, a comprehensive plan is strategies were effective in improving quality of
developed, and the team or work group continues life (e.g., Do stakeholders view the strategies as
to work to accomplish the identified goals. (p. 464)
having made a meaningful difference in the life-
Because the process of person-centered plan- style of the individual by increasing ­opportunities
ning is, in and of itself, difficult to standardize to participate in typical community settings?)
and measure, PBS interventionists have instead (Carr et al., 2002).
begun to evaluate stakeholder satisfaction as a An illustration of social validity can be found
means of assessing the person-centered planning in the work of Kemp and Carr (1995). Their
process (Abery, McBride, & Rotholz, 1999). study detailed a multicomponent approach for
This approach has also been applied at the state- remediating problem behavior in three adults
wide level in the evaluation of PBS plans (KIPBS, with autism and severe problem behavior in a
2010), using the Person-Centered Planning community workplace setting, specifically a
Process Satisfaction Survey (Abery et al., 1999). greenhouse. They selected treatments based on
This survey uses a 4-point Likert-type rating hypotheses about the variables controlling the
scale (1 = not at all satisfied; 2 = a little satisfied; problem behavior. The multicomponent inter-
3 = quite a bit satisfied; and 4 = completely satis- vention included functional communication
fied) to evaluate 32 different aspects of stake- training (i.e., requesting help or a break), build-
holders’ experiences with person-centered ing rapport (i.e., delivering reinforcement non-
planning. Items assess how well the focus person contingently), making choices (e.g., choosing
was prepared for the meeting and actively partici- activities, materials), embedding demands (i.e.,
pating, how well the facilitator was prepared and alternating between preferred and non-preferred
446 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

tasks/steps), and building tolerance for delay of In a second example, Binnendyk and
reinforcement. The results showed that follow- Lucyshyn (2009) evaluated the effectiveness of
ing intervention, the participants demonstrated a family-­ centered positive behavior support
both an increase in time spent in the employment approach to the amelioration of food refusal
situation without problem behavior and increases behavior in a child with autism. The study was
in completion of work steps to task completion. conducted with the child and his family in their
Social validation of these results was provided home. It employed an empirical case study
by employment site management. Here, green- design with one meal routine, specifically,
house managers were asked a series of questions snack time. Following training and support
designed to evaluate their confidence level in with the child’s mother, results showed high
supporting the adults in the workplace before levels of child food acceptance, successful
and after intervention. Managers used a 7-point child participation in observed snack routines,
rating scale (7 = very much/always, 4 = some- and high parental ratings of social validity and
what/sometimes, and 1 = not at all/never) to contextual fit. Here, social validity was evalu-
respond to five different items: (1) “I am confi- ated using a 10-item instrument with a 5-point
dent that the job coach can control the employ- Likert-type scale (1 = disagree, 5 = agree).
ee’s behavior”; (2) “ I am confident that my Across four evaluations, the mother’s average
coworkers are safe from harm”; (3)” I am confi- social validity rating was 4.6 (range = 4.3–4.8),
dent that the greenhouse property is safe from suggesting that she consistently believed that
harm”; (4) “The employee’s behavior in the the plan goals, procedures, and outcomes were
greenhouse is severe”; and (5) “The employee acceptable. These improvements maintained up
could make a productive contribution to the to 26 months post-intervention. Implementation
greenhouse.” The social validity outcomes was also associated with generalization of the
showed that the managers reported little confi- child’s eating behavior to new foods and to his
dence that the job coach could control the father’s supporting him during snack time.
employees’ behavior in baseline, but they Child behavioral improvements were also asso-
reported near total confidence at the end of inter- ciated with parental reports of gains in family
vention. At the beginning of the study, the man- quality of life.
agers also reported little confidence that other Brief questionnaires and subjective rating
workers were safe from harm, but they reported scales, such as those described above, represent
total confidence in coworker safety after inter- the current standard for evaluating social validity
vention. Similarly, the managers reported little in PBS interventions. These ratings have been
confidence that the property was safe in baseline applied at various points during intervention and
and reported near total confidence following at the end of intervention to determine whether
intervention. The managers also reported that PBS strategies were reported to have made a dif-
they found the employees’ behavior to be nearly ference. While most social validity ratings are
always severe in baseline and almost never administered in an interview or self-report for-
severe after intervention. Finally, in baseline, the mat, Brookman-Frazee (2004) used real-time
managers reported little or no production by the behavioral observations to examine social valid-
employees. Following intervention, the employ- ity during treatment sessions. In her study, the
ees were reported to be able to make a produc- author examined the effects of a clinician-driven
tive contribution “always.” These results model and a parent/clinician partnership on three
suggested that the intervention strategies were mother-child dyads involving very young boys
socially valid. That is to say, they were generally with autism and their caregivers. The author used
effective and acceptable to the greenhouse staff four different Likert-type scales to assess social
and therefore more likely to be implemented in validity during treatment sessions. Two different
the workplace. 6-point Likert scales (0–5) were used to assess
24  Positive Behavior Support 447

parent stress level and parent confidence during when presented with a task to complete. If the
parent-child interactions, and two 6-point Likert individual is not fatigued, it may be much more
scales (0–5) were used to assess child interest and likely that the individual will successfully com-
affect during the parent-child interactions. plete the task in the absence of problem behavior
Results showed that all three parents in the study (Smith, Carr, & Moskowitz, 2016). Horner et al.
demonstrated decreased levels of observed stress (1996) described several ways in which setting
and increased levels of observed confidence dur- events could be an important part of a multi-
ing the parent-clinician partnership condition component intervention for problem behavior.
compared to the clinician-directed condition. In Interventions might include minimizing the likeli-
addition, all three children in the study demon- hood that the setting event will affect behavior, for
strated more positive affect during the parent-­ example, ensuring a good night’s sleep for an indi-
clinician partnership condition compared to the vidual who experiences increases in problem
clinician-directed condition. The authors con- behavior when fatigued. Another strategy might
cluded that collaborative partnerships between be to neutralize the effects of the setting event. For
parents and professionals had a positive impact example, if a student just had a fight on the play-
on both child target behaviors and overall parent-­ ground and then became uncooperative after re-
child interactions and family quality of life. As entering the building, the teacher might prompt the
such, these interventions were considered to be student to use a known relaxation routine before
socially valid and therefore likely to increase presenting an academic task. A third strategy
treatment success and decrease attrition rates might be to provide additional prompts to facilitate
among parents participating in parent education a desired behavior. This could involve a teacher
programs. following a direction with a prompt such as, “If
you need help doing this, or if you need a break,
tell me by…” (Horner et al., 1996).
Multicomponent Intervention A comprehensive systems change approach
and Systems Change typically involves the use of multicomponent
interventions, with treatment efforts simultane-
PBS emphasizes that efforts should focus on ously focusing on setting events, antecedents,
addressing problem contexts, not problem behav- skill building, and consequences. In illustration,
ior per se. Interventions that directly address prob- Lucyshyn et al. (2007) presented a case study of
lem behavior may not be successful if the context a 5-year-old child, Katherine, who had autism
within which the behavior occurs does not support and severe intellectual disability. Katherine and
the use of the intervention. For meaningful change her family were followed for a 10-year period:
to occur, systems need to be reorganized so that 2 years in baseline, nearly a year and a half of
change can occur and be maintained. Achieving intervention training and support, and 7 years of
meaningful change depends on stakeholders shar- post-intervention. The participant was 5 years old
ing a common vision, ongoing training for staff, when the study began and 15 years old when it
and motivation for people to adopt new or revised concluded. The study targeted four different fam-
ways of looking at problem contexts (Knoster, ily routines at home and in the community: din-
Villa, & Thousand, 2000). Horner, Vaughn, Day, ner, bedtime, fast-food restaurant, and grocery
and Ard (1996) described an expanded way to shopping. The intervention plan incorporated a
look at contexts for problem behavior by including variety of PBS strategies that included four dif-
the role of setting events. They described setting ferent setting event strategies: ensuring that tasks
events as events that momentarily change the like- and activities had meaningful outcomes, using a
lihood of a target behavior at a later point in time. picture schedule, supporting friendships with
For example, if an individual is fatigued, they may nondisabled peers, and decreasing demands
be more likely to engage in problem behavior when ill. The intervention package also included
448 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

five different antecedent strategies: providing interactions, medication changes, and/or illness as
advanced information about events that were potential setting events for problem behavior.
stressful (e.g., tasks, changes, transitions, being Inspired by this work, Carr, Magito McLaughlin,
alone); using natural positive contingencies to Giacobbe-Grieco, and Smith (2003a) developed
motivate cooperation; mediating delays using a and used a mood scale to study the impact of envi-
preferred interaction, item, or activity; ensuring ronmental setting events. The mood scale contains
task success with instruction that matched her a 6-point Likert-­type rating of mood (0–1 = bad
learning style; and using a “safety signal” to pre- mood; 2–3 = neutral mood; 4–5 = good mood), fol-
dict a break. To address skill building, the inter- lowed by a series of open-ended questions aimed at
vention included strategies to teach Katherine to identifying particular setting events that might be
use language to communicate her wants and associated with bad or good mood. Similar rating
needs, to participate in group activities, to wait or scales have been used to evaluate the impact of bio-
accept a delay, and to comply with “stop” and logical setting events such as menses (Carr, Smith,
“come here” cues. Finally, consequence strate- Giacin, Whelan, & Pancari, 2003b) and fatigue
gies included contingent praise for appropriate (Smith, Carr, & Moskowitz, 2016). The Contextual
behaviors such as using language, making prog- Assessment Inventory (McAtee, Carr, & Schulte,
ress toward independence, calmly waiting, and 2004) identifies generic classes of contextual vari-
accepting changes in routine. Planned ignoring of ables that might be associated with problem behav-
low-intensity behaviors was also included as a ior. The inventory includes over 90 individual
consequence-based strategy. Following parent items that are grouped into categories, including
implementation of the multicomponent plan, aspects of the social and cultural environment
Katherine’s problem behaviors decreased to zero that may influence problem behavior (e.g., nega-
or near zero levels, while successful participation tive interactions, disappointments); aspects of the
in routines increased from 0% to 75% of routines task, activity, or routine underway (e.g., rigid,
observed. Most importantly, across 7 years of boring, difficult); aspects of the physical environ-
post-intervention measurement and brief support, ment (e.g., discomfort, change); and aspects of
these changes maintained and showed further the individual’s physical condition or state of
improvement, with successful participation in health (e.g., medication, illness, physiological
routines reaching 100% of routines observed. In states). For each item in the inventory, caregivers
addition, implementation of the support process are asked to rate the likelihood of problem behav-
was associated with a broader range of meaning- iors occurring in the presence of each factor (1 =
ful and durable improvements in Katherine’s never, 3 = half the time, 5 = always). The authors
behavior and quality of life from early childhood concluded that the CAI was an efficient, compre-
to middle adolescence. Katherine’s parents also hensive, and c­ omprehensible means of helping to
reported improvements in the family’s quality of identify context events that could be key compo-
life and in their own personal health that went nents of a multicomponent intervention plan
beyond the immediate focus of the study. As their (McAtee et al., 2004).
skills, confidence, and success in supporting their
daughter grew, the parents reported a decrease in
family fragmentation and social isolation. Emphasis on Prevention
Given the noted importance of multicomponent
interventions, a number of tools have been devel- A PBS approach emphasizes the prevention of
oped to assist PBS interventionists in identifying a problem behavior. From a PBS perspective, “pre-
broad range of factors that can affect problem vention” refers to intervening on problem behav-
behavior. One of the earliest devices to assess ior when the problem behavior is not occurring
global influences was the Setting Events Checklist so that skill building can occur in an effort to pre-
(Gardner et al., 1986), which is a 16-item checklist vent the behavior from occurring again (Carr
that identifies factors such as previous negative et al., 2002). This definition of prevention
24  Positive Behavior Support 449

includes in part an emphasis on the early inter- pants in the study were children with develop-
vention for problem behavior in young children mental delays between the ages of 33 and
with ASD, so that problem behavior later in life 60 months. These children were identified by
for those individuals can be prevented or mini- parents and teachers as engaging in minor prob-
mized. The proactive approach of prevention can lem behaviors such as crying, whining, and light
include interventions such as functional commu- hitting when they wanted to gain someone’s
nication training (e.g., Carr & Durand, 1985), attention. The participants were assigned to one
creating opportunities for choice making (e.g., of two groups. Children in the FCT group inter-
Dunlap et al., 1994), and curricular revisions acted with adults who were trained to teach the
(e.g., Dunlap et al., 1991). children functional communication to request
An illustration of prevention research can be attention (e.g., tapping the adult on the arm, say-
seen in the work of Kay, Harchik, and Luiselli ing the teacher’s name, or saying a phrase such as
(2006). The authors presented a case study of a “Look what I’ve done”). The children in the sec-
17-year-old student with autism named George, ond group, the expressive language training
who attended a public high school. George’s (ELT) group, interacted with adults who were not
drooling was reported to be interfering with his trained in FCT but were trained to teach general
education and his social adjustment at school. expressive language skills (e.g., labeling, answer-
His drooling was thought to be the result of a skill ing questions). Data were collected on the fre-
deficit; thus, one of the components of the inter- quency of functional communication use,
vention involved George participating in three intensity of problem behaviors, and frequency of
pre-teaching sessions, where he was taught to problem behaviors. The results of this study indi-
follow the instructions “swallow” and “wipe cated that the children in each group made few
your mouth” using a tissue. Verbal instructions, requests for attention during baseline. During
partial physical guidance, and praise were used intervention, the children in the FCT group used
during the pre-teaching sessions. Once George functional communication requests more often
demonstrated mastery in following these instruc- than children in the ELT group and did not show
tions, additional intervention components were an increase in the frequency or intensity of prob-
implemented. These interventions included the lem behavior. On the other hand, all of the chil-
following components: (1) George’s paraprofes- dren in the ELT group showed increases in the
sional aide checked him every 5 min, and if his frequency and intensity of problem behavior
mouth was dry, he was praised and provided with from baseline to intervention. Furthermore, after
an edible reinforcer; (2) if saliva was visible out- functional communication training (FCT) was
side his lips, the aide requested George to wipe implemented, there was a reduction in problem
his mouth and swallow. The results showed that behavior in the ELT group. The authors con-
the intervention was associated with a steady cluded that functional communication training
reduction and eventual elimination of drooling may have been effective in preventing increases
across the three locations used in the study (class- in problem behavior in children who are at risk.
room, community vocational site, and cooking Prevention tools can be drawn from the
class). Following the intervention, it was anec- applied behavior analysis literature and include
dotally reported by school staff that George “best practice” strategies for teaching and prompt-
received greater peer acceptance compared to ing. PBS interventionists are actively engaged in
pre-intervention. adapting these procedures to real-­world commu-
In a second example, highlighting early inter- nity settings, using task analyses, incidental
vention research, Reeve and Carr (2000) con- teaching, and a variety of strategies to program
ducted a study that examined whether functional for generalization and maintenance across set-
communication training (FCT) could be a means tings and intervention agents. However, since
of preventing minor problem behaviors from these strategies are not specific to PBS, they will
escalating to more serious behaviors. The partici- not be articulated here.
450 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

 lexibility with Respect to Scientific


F pletion as alternatives to measures of frequency
Practices and time sampling. The rationale for employing
these measures was that, in the community, there
PBS presumes different outcomes than its ABA would be less concern with overall rate or level of
predecessors. For this reason, Carr (1997) sug- problem behavior and more concern with whether
gested the need for a new applied science that an individual could complete a shopping task in a
addresses consumer needs more systematically and reasonable amount of time and do so without
more frequently. With respect to assessment, meth- engaging in problem behavior.
ods must be user-friendly, feasible in the com-
munity, and yield accurate information. PBS
challenges researchers to adopt greater flexibility Multiple Theoretical Perspectives
in their definition of what constitutes acceptable
data (Schwartz & Olswang, 1996). Moving beyond While applied behavior analysis has played a major
observations, PBS challenges researchers to con- role in shaping the development of PBS, as PBS
sider the acceptability of naturalistic observations, has evolved, other branches of psychology, includ-
correlational analyses, and qualitative data, includ- ing organizational management, community/eco-
ing case studies, interviews, subjective rating logical psychology, cultural psychology,
scales, logs, questionnaires, and self-­report mea- biomedical science, and positive psychology, have
sures, many of which have already been described. made significant contributions as well (Carr, 2007).
Flexibility in scientific practice was illustrated These branches of psychology deal with units that
by Carr and Carlson (1993) who presented an are larger than the individual (i.e., systems) and
approach for remediating severe problem behav- emphasize the importance of natural settings for
ior in three adolescents with ASD in a public research and intervention. Carr et al. (2002) identi-
community setting, specifically a supermarket. fied three principles that have long characterized
Here, the authors noted that traditionally, inter- the above fields and have now become dominant
ventions for problem behavior would be evalu- within PBS as well. These are the following: (1)
ated using measures of frequency and time since people in community settings are interdepen-
sampling. These measures are especially appro- dent, clinically significant change occurs in social
priate in home and school settings where parents systems and not just in individuals; (2) producing
or professional staff monitor the problems. In change is not simply a matter of implementing spe-
these settings, there is an understanding that cific techniques; rather, change involves the real-
problem behavior is likely to occur in baseline location of resources such as time, money, and
and must be tolerated, at least in the short run, for political power; and (3) an individual’s behavior is
purposes of assessment. However, no such toler- the result of a continuous process of adaptation,
ance exists in a public supermarket. Instead, even reflecting the interface between competence (a
a relatively small number of instances of property property of individuals) and context (a property of
destruction or aggression against other patrons environments). Therefore, a successful interven-
can result in expulsion from the store or police tion must modulate the goodness of fit between
action. Also, caretakers who accompany individ- competence and context.
uals with disabilities to the store may be embar- These principles are highlighted in a case study
rassed by public displays of problem behavior presented by Clarke, Worcester, Dunlap, Murray,
and therefore unlikely to agree to monitor prog- and Bradley-Klug (2002) who used a multicom-
ress using frequency or time sampling measures. ponent intervention to address the problem behav-
In light of these practical difficulties, Carr and ior of a 12-year-old student named Mindy. Mindy
Carlson (1993) suggested the need for alternative was diagnosed with ASD and attended a public
measures for use in public settings. Accordingly, school. During assessment, the student’s interven-
they evaluated the utility of measures of latency tion team identified specific pre-academic activi-
to problem behavior and percentage of task com- ties as well as transition routines involving
24  Positive Behavior Support 451

physically moving from one location to another staff tend to assume that the children will “out-
location that were associated with the occurrence grow” these behaviors. This can result in staff not
of problem behavior. The team developed a mul- addressing lower-level challenging behaviors,
ticomponent intervention for each of the targeted which may result in a worsening of these behav-
activities and routines. For example, Mindy iors as the children grow older (Reichle et al.,
resisted a routine assembly activity that required 1996). Noting the lack of preservice training for
her to place color-coded foam forms onto match- staff who will eventually work with individuals
ing colored pegs. The intervention involved pro- with challenging behavior, Reichle and his col-
viding an alternate assembly activity that was leagues proposed a training program that focused
more functionally relevant and meaningful to on preventive intervention rather than reactive
Mindy and could serve as a bridge to other, more intervention strategies. This program emphasized
functionally relevant activities. In this case, the need for a transdisciplinary approach to
Mindy was given a new activity of assembling a addressing challenging behavior and described
McDonald’s Happy Meal kit. This activity was how universities and school districts could work
then used as a bridge to the functional skill of collaboratively to improve services through lon-
matching (e.g., matching plastic chicken nuggets gitudinal technical assistance, preservice and in-­
with their containers) and eventually to transition- service coursework, and on-site training in the
ing to the cafeteria. Additional components that prevention of problem behavior.
were included in the intervention were minimiz-
ing distractions for 5 min before making the tran-
sition, inviting a preferred peer to assist Mindy Cultural Sensitivity and PBS
with making the transition, including a preferred
activity (a tape player) for Mindy to enjoy during While cultural sensitivity has not been formally
the transition, and providing Mindy with a pre- identified as a “critical feature” of PBS, as PBS is
ferred object to hold while walking to the cafete- applied to a wider variety of ethnic, racial, social,
ria. The results showed that problem behavior and religious groups, there has been a call for
occurred during a mean of 44% of intervals across increased consideration of cultural diversity in the
all activities during baseline, which decreased to a development and application of PBS ­interventions
mean of 11% during intervention. For transitions, (Carr, 2007). Carr (2007) identified three sets of
problem behavior decreased from an average of cultural issues that are relevant to the future of
75% during baseline to 31% during intervention. PBS: cultural relativism, cultural values, and
Follow-up data 1 year after the study was initiated cross-cultural competence. Each of these will be
were consistent with the levels of problem behav- described in turn.
ior achieved during intervention. Here, interven- In terms of cultural relativism, Carr (2007)
tions for Mindy’s problem behavior recognized noted that problem behavior cannot be fully
that clinically significant change needed to occur understood without considering such things as
in Mindy’s school. Change involved the realloca- the values, beliefs, and norms of the ethnic,
tion of resources such as time, money (items), and racial, religious, or social groups to which an
people (peers, school personnel). Ultimately, individual belongs. Draguns (1997) stated that
Mindy’s progress came as the result of a continu- abnormal behavior concepts vary across cultures.
ous process of adaptation to better balance This implies that a challenging behavior needs to
Mindy’s level of competence with the complex be assessed in relation to its cultural context.
contexts (e.g., work skills, cafeteria) that she Since the way problem behavior is defined can
encountered. vary across cultures, failure to examine the cul-
In a second example, Reichle et al. (1996) tural context and how a particular culture defines
described a model for training early intervention the problem behavior can lead to ineffective
staff who work with preschoolers who engage in interventions and low consumer acceptability of
challenging behavior. These authors noted that the intervention. As an example, Delgado Rivera
452 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

and Rogers-Adkinson (1997) described how with- where the children reside in order to better under-
holding eye contact when interacting with a person stand what the families’ lives and challenges are
in authority is typically seen as respectful in outside of school (Zionts et al., 2003).
Hispanic-American and African-American groups. To illustrate the role of cultural factors, while
Similarly, among Asian-American groups, eye summarizing the critical features of PBS, a final
contact with strangers may be seen as disrespectful case study will be presented. Magito McLaughlin,
(Delgado Rivera & Rogers-­ Adkinson, 1997). Mullen James, Anderson Ryan, and Carr (2002)
Therefore, promoting eye contact in certain situa- presented a case study of Christos, a Greek
tions when working with these cultural groups may immigrant in his early twenties who was multi-
be seen as going against cultural norms. ply diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder,
Regarding cultural values, Carr (2007) seizure disorder, and bipolar disorder. Christos
described how the PBS approach has largely been was unique in that he was of European descent
developed within English-speaking Western cul- and not a native speaker of the English language.
ture. This would suggest that PBS interventions In addition, he experienced medical and psychi-
might reflect the values of this culture, such as atric illnesses, in addition to ASD. Christos was
autonomy, self-reliance, and independence. If these born in Greece, and as a young child, he lived
goals are ascribed to an individual from a different with his family in a Greek enclave in Queens,
culture where values such as group identity and NY. At the age of 19, the intense nature and
mutual dependency are valued, these goals may be severity of Christos’ problem behaviors resulted
seen as going against what the culture values. This in his placement in a residential treatment center
could then result in low consumer satisfaction with out of state. At the center, Christos was heavily
the goals. In a description of culturally sensitive medicated and provided with two staff at all
person-centered planning, Callicott (2003) stated times to address his intense tantrums and destruc-
that self-determination, which is a hallmark of per- tion of property. At age 23, Christos returned to
son-centered planning, may be an unfamiliar con- New York for adult services due to changes in
cept to another culture and may be in opposition to funding r­egulations that no longer allowed him
putting family first. In such situations, person-cen- to remain in an out-of-state placement.
tered planning can provide an opportunity to dis- Christos moved to a temporary crisis home on
cuss expectations and discuss the belief systems of Long Island with 15 other individuals who had
the target individual, the family, and the commu- severe problem behavior. While living at the resi-
nity (Callicott, 2003). dence, his problem behaviors became more
Lastly, Carr (2007) emphasized the need to have intense and more frequent. Staff attempted to
competence in working with a wider variety of cul- exert control over Christos’ behavior by bribing
tures other than White, middle-class groups, and him with food to gain his cooperation. This
such things as family structure, customs, and child- resulted in serious health issues, including
rearing practices will come to play a more impor- becoming overweight and developing high cho-
tant role in determining PBS interventions. For lesterol. Physical and mechanical restraint proce-
example, when working with a family from a cul- dures were also used several dozen times per
ture where extended families rather than nuclear month to keep Christos and others safe from
families may be the norm, interventions will need harm. His problem behavior prevented him from
to consider including extended family members in engaging in classroom activities at his day pro-
assessment and intervention. Using semistructured gram and from accessing the community at large.
interviews, Zionts, Zionts, Harrison, and Bellinger A year later, Christos moved into a smaller
(2003) found that African-American parents of community residence with six other men who
children with disabilities wanted sensitivity train- also had a history of autism and severe problem
ing for teachers working with children from low behavior. His behaviors remained intense, and
socioeconomic levels. They reported that they this became more and more of a concern since
wanted teachers to spend time in the communities Christos was now living in an otherwise quiet
24  Positive Behavior Support 453

neighborhood. A neighbor eventually filed a Problem behavior context Solution


complaint with the state office on developmental Group situations/ Have Christos live alone
disabilities, and the state office mandated that crowded environments and work with a small
group of support staff
Christos be removed from the home.
Have Christos engage in
Following the mandated change in place-
individualized community-­
ment, the agency responsible for providing ser- based supports instead of
vices made a decision to intervene using a traditional day program
positive behavior support (PBS) approach. A supports
support team including Christos and his family, Traditional staffing Hire staff from the local
patterns Greek community who
clinical staff, and direct staff workers who had a could engage Christos in
positive rapport with Christos convened to cre- cultural activities (e.g.,
ate solutions based on Christos’ needs. The team speaking in Greek,
recommended that Christos temporarily return cooking/eating Greek
food)
to the crisis residence, so they could conduct an
Physical and chemical Encourage communication
intensive assessment of his behavior. Central to restraints to express needs
the team’s approach was a reframing of Christos’ Develop a positive rapport
problem behavior: his “noncompliant” behavior with staff
was viewed as having “unmet needs.” Instead of Increase opportunities to
trying to exert power over Christos’ behavior, a exercise self-control (e.g.,
functional behavior assessment (FBA) was con- take a shower to cool down
when angry)
ducted to identify specific contexts that sup-
Implement medication
ported and maintained problem behavior. By changes to decrease side
addressing these contexts, a multicomponent effects (e.g., sedation,
intervention plan could be developed to reduce hunger)
problem behavior and improve his overall qual- Limitations to physical Support verbal
ity of life. mobility communication (e.g., to
leave a situation, to be
As an initial part of the PBS approach, a func- alone)
tional behavior assessment interview was com- Provide healthy snack
pleted, and it indicated that the primary motivation options that were freely
for problem behavior was escape from unwanted available
events. The next priority addressed by the team Lack of access to Use of a photo
community activities communication board so
was to identify the contexts and environments
Christos could choose
that Christos was looking to escape. Using a preferred community
method similar to that described by Carr et al. activities
(1994), the team identified five general contexts Develop a community
in which problem behaviors were most likely to presence with staff support
by introducing Christos to
occur. These included (1) group situations/ local community
crowded environments, (2) traditional staffing destinations (e.g.,
patterns (wherein the staff’s role was to guard churches, parks, Greek
and protect, rather than teach and befriend), (3) restaurants)
physical and chemical restraints, (4) limitations Facilitate ongoing contacts
with family members who
to physical mobility, and (5) lack of access to are still living in the Greek
community activities (Magito McLaughlin et al., community
2002). The team utilized person-centered plan- Create respected roles
ning to brainstorm solutions to each of the five (e.g., part-time work,
contexts identified. The results of this process are strengths-based activities,
preferred activities)
summarized in the table below.
454 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

Prior to these interventions, Christos demon- strategies that were simple and user-friendly.
strated episodes of aggression, self-injury, and Multiple theoretical perspectives were achieved
property destruction between 350 and 1,100 times by carefully weaving cultural factors (i.e., Greek
per month. After PBS was implemented to address staff, food, community) throughout the PBS inter-
specific problem behavior contexts, Christos’ vention plan. Despite many layers of complex
problem behavior episodes decreased to as few as needs, PBS stood as a beacon of hope for Christos
100 episodes per month. In addition, time spent in and his family. This new approach to intervention
the community following the person-­ centered illustrated a broader lifestyle perspective; it was
plan increased significantly (Magito McLaughlin more responsive to Christos’ needs and more in
et al., 2002). line with the community-­ based support efforts
Magito McLaughlin et al. (2002) pointed out that were underway. As a result of PBS, Christos
that Christos’ story illustrated how individuals continues to live in the community today and par-
with problem behavior might not benefit from ticipates in self-directed services to further
large groups or system-oriented services. The improve his quality of life.
authors showed how “thinking outside the box”
and utilizing positive behavior support strategies
that address problem contexts could meet the References
unique needs of this complex young man.
Furthermore, Christos’ story highlights the criti- Abery, B. H., McBride, M. J., & Rotholz, D. A. (1999).
The person-centered planning process satisfaction
cal features of PBS. Comprehensive lifestyle survey (PCP-SS). Minneapolis, MN: University of
change was accomplished by way of a planned Minnesota, Institute of Community Integration.
move out of the group home and to a home of his Albin, R. W., Lucyshyn, J. M., Horner, R. H., & Flannery,
own. In addition, there was a renewed emphasis K. B. (1996). Contextual fit for behavioral support
plans: A model for “goodness of fit.” In L. K. Koegel,
on culturally relevant community membership. R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive behav-
Christos was a young man who was forced to ioral support: Including people with difficult behavior
transition from an out-of-state school where he in the community (pp. 81–98). Baltimore, MD: Paul
was in a highly controlled environment to an adult H. Brookes.
Binnendyk, L., & Lucyshyn, J. M. (2009). A family-­
service placement in New York that was ill-­ centered positive behavior support approach to the
prepared to manage him. A lifespan perspective amelioration of food refusal behavior. Journal of
necessitated the provision of different treatments Positive Behavior Interventions, 11, 47–62.
under different circumstances. Christos’ PBS plan Brookman-Frazee, L. (2004). Using parent/clinician
partnerships in parent education programs for chil-
was ecologically valid in that the interventions dren with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
were matched to hypothesized variables. Relevant Interventions, 6, 195–213.
stakeholders, including Christos’ parents, family Brown, F. (1991). Creative daily scheduling: A nonintru-
members, and support staff, participated in a per- sive approach to challenging behaviors in community
residences. Journal of the Association for Persons with
son-centered planning process to identify relevant Severe Handicaps, 16, 75–84.
contexts and solutions. Social validity was Callicott, K. J. (2003). Culturally sensitive collaboration
ensured in that interventions were readily able to within person-centered planning. Focus on Autism and
be carried out by staff. Multicomponent interven- Other Developmental Disabilities, 18, 60–68.
Carr, E. G. (1997). Invited commentary: The evolution of
tion was accomplished through a simultaneous applied behavior analysis into positive behavior sup-
effort to adjust staffing, housing, communication, port. Journal of the Association for Severe Handicaps,
coping skills, and medication. An emphasis on 22, 208–209.
prevention was illustrated by the use of creative Carr, E. G. (2002). Positive behavior support: Evolution
of an applied science. Journal of Positive Behavior
scheduling (Brown, 1991), choice (Dunlap et al., Interventions, 4, 4–16.
1994), and communication (Carr et al., 1994), as Carr, E. G. (2007). The expanded vision of positive
opposed to crisis management and physical behavior support: Research perspectives on happi-
restraint. Flexibility with respect to scientific ness, helpfulness, and hopefulness. Journal of Positive
Behavior Interventions, 9, 3–14.
practices was evidenced by designing teaching
24  Positive Behavior Support 455

Carr, E. G., & Carlson, J. I. (1993). Reduction of severe and severe problem behaviors. Journal of Applied
behavior problems in the community using a multi- Behavior Analysis, 24, 387–397.
component treatment approach. Journal of Applied Dunlap, G., de Perczel, M., Clarke, S., Wilson, D.,
Behavior Analysis, 26, 157–172. Wright, S., White, R., & Gomez, A. (1994). Choice
Carr, E. G., & Durand, V. M. (1985). Reducing behavior making and proactive behavioral support for students
problems through functional communication training. with emotional and behavioral challenges. Journal of
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 18, 111–126. Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 505–518.
Carr, E. G., Levin, L., McConnachie, G., Carlson, J. I., Dunlap, G., Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Koegel, R. L.,
Kemp, D. C., & Smith, C. E. (1994). Communication-­ Sailor, W., Clarke, S., … Fox, L. (2010). A descriptive
based intervention for problem behavior: A user’s multi-year examination of positive behavior support.
guide for producing positive change. Baltimore: Paul Behavioral Disorders, 35, 259–279.
H. Brookes. Evans, I. M., & Meyer, L. H. (1985). An educative
Carr, E. G., Horner, R. H., Turnbull, A. P., Marquis, J. G., approach to behavior problems: A practical decision
Magito McLaughlin, D., McAtee, M. L., … Doolabh, model for interventions with severely handicapped
A. (1999a). Positive behavior support for people with learners. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
developmental disabilities: A research synthesis. Feldman, E. K., & Matos, R. (2013). Training parapro-
Washington, D.C.: American Association on Mental fessionals to facilitate social interactions between
Retardation. children with autism and their typically developing
Carr, E. G., Levin, L., McConnachie, G., Carlson, J. I., peers. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 15,
Kemp, D. C., Smith, C. E., & Magito McLaughlin, D. 169–179.
(1999b). Comprehensive multi-situational interven- Forest, M., & Lusthaus, E. (1987). The kaleidoscope:
tion for problem behavior in the community: Long-­ Challenge to the cascade. In M. Forest (Ed.), More
term maintenance and social validation. Journal of education/integration (pp. 1–16). Downsview,
Positive Behavior Interventions, 1, 5–25. Canada: G. Allan Roeher Institute.
Carr, E. G., Dunlap, G., Horner, R. H., Koegel, R. L., Fox, P., & Emerson, E. (2001). Socially valid outcomes
Turnbull, A. P., Sailor, W., … Fox, L. (2002). Positive of intervention for people with MR and challenging
behavior support: Evolution of an applied science. behavior: Views of different stakeholders. Journal of
Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, 4–16. Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 183–189.
Carr, E. G., Magito McLaughlin, D., Giacobbe-Grieco, Gardner, W. I., Cole, C. I., Davidson, D. P., & Karan,
T., & Smith, C. E. (2003a). Using mood ratings and O. C. (1986). Reducing aggression in individuals with
mood induction in assessment and intervention for developmental disabilities: An expanded stimulus con-
severe problem behavior. American Journal on Mental trol, assessment, and intervention model. Education
Retardation, 108, 32–55. and Training of the Mentally Retarded, 21, 3–12.
Carr, E. G., Smith, C. E., Giacin, T. A., Whelan, B. M., & Horner, R. H., Dunlap, G., Koegel, R. L., Carr, E. G.,
Pancari, J. (2003b). Menstrual discomfort as a biologi- Sailor, W., Anderson, J., … O’Neill, R. E. (1990).
cal setting event for severe problem behavior. American Toward a technology of nonaversive behavioral sup-
Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 117–133. port. Journal of the Association for Persons with
Clarke, S., Dunlap, G., & Vaughn, B. (1999). Family-­ Severe Handicaps, 15(3), 125–132.
centered, assessment-based intervention to improve Horner, R. H., Vaughn, B. J., Day, H. M., & Ard, W. R.
behavior during an early morning routine. Journal of (1996). The relationship between setting events and
Positive Behavior Interventions, 1, 235–241. problem behavior. In L. K. Koegel, R. L. Koegel,
Clarke, S., Worcester, J., Dunlap, G., Murray, M., & & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive behavioral support:
Bradley-Klug, K. (2002). Using multiple measures Including people with difficult behavior in the com-
to evaluate positive behavior support: A case exam- munity. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
ple. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 4, Horner, R. H., Salentine, S., & Albin, R. W. (2003). Self
131–145. assessment of contextual fit in schools. Unpublished
Crone, D. A., & Horner, R. H. (2003). Building posi- instrument. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon,
tive behavior support systems in schools. New York, College of Education, Educational and Community
London, England: Guilford Press. Support.
Delgado Rivera, B., & Rogers-Adkinson, D. (1997). Hughes, C., Hwang, B., Kim, J. H., Eisenman, L. T.,
Culturally sensitive interventions: Social skills train- & Killian, D. J. (1995). Quality of life in applied
ing with children and parents from culturally and research: A review and analysis of empirical mea-
linguistically diverse backgrounds. Intervention in sures. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 99,
School and Clinic, 33, 75–80. 623–641.
Draguns, J. (1997). Abnormal behavior patterns across Jensen, C. C., McConnachie, G., & Pierson, T. (2001).
cultures: Implications for counseling and psychother- Long-term multicomponent intervention to reduce
apy. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, severe problem behavior: A 63-month evaluation.
21, 213–248. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 3, 225–236.
Dunlap, G., Kern-Dunlap, L., Clarke, S., & Robbins, F. R. Kansas Institute for Positive Behavior Support (KIPBS)
(1991). Functional assessment, curricular revision, (2010). Attachments online. www.kipbsmodules.org
456 D.M. McLaughlin and C.E. Smith

Kay, S., Harchik, A. E., & Luiselli, J. K. (2006). ior. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 6,
Elimination of drooling by an adolescent student 148–165.
with autism attending public high school. Journal of McLaughlin, T. W., Denney, M. K., Snyder, P. A., &
Positive Behavior Interventions, 8, 24–28. Welsh, J. L. (2012). Behavior support interven-
Kemp, D. C., & Carr, E. G. (1995). Reduction of severe tions implemented by families of young children:
problem behavior in community employment using Examination of contextual fit. Journal of Positive
an hypothesis-driven multicomponent intervention Behavior Interventions, 14, 87–97.
approach. Journal of the Association for Persons with Meyer, L. H., Eichinger, J., & Park-Lee, S. (1987). A vali-
Severe Handicaps, 20, 229–247. dation of program quality indicators in educational
Kennedy, C. H., & Itkonen, T. (1996). Social relation- services for students with severe disabilities. Journal
ships, influential variables, and change across the of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps,
lifespan. In L. K. Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap 122, 251–263.
(Eds.), Positive behavioral support: Including people Moes, D. R., & Frea, W. D. (2000). Using family con-
with difficult behavior in the community (pp. 287– text to inform intervention planning for the treatment
304). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. of a child with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
Kennedy, C. H., Horner, R. H., & Newton, S. J. (1990). Interventions, 2, 40–46.
The social networks and activity patterns of adults Mount, B. (1987). Personal futures planning: Finding
with severe disabilities: A correlational analysis. directions for change (Doctoral dissertation, University
Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe of Georgia). Ann Arbor, MI: UMI Dissertation
Handicaps, 15, 86–90. Information Service.
Kincaid, D. (1996). Person-centered planning. In L. K. Mount, B. (1994). Benefits and limitations of personal
Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.), Positive futures planning. In V. J. Bradley, J. W. Ashbaugh,
behavioral support: Including people with difficult & B. Blaney (Eds.), Creating individual supports for
behavior in the community (pp. 439–465). Baltimore: people with developmental disabilities: A mandate for
Paul H. Brookes. change at many levels (pp. 97–108). Baltimore: Paul
Kincaid, D., Knoster, T., Harrower, J. K., Shannon, P., & H. Brookes.
Bustamante, S. (2002). Measuring the impact of posi- Mount, B., & Zwernick, K. (1988). It’s never too early,
tive behavior support. Journal of Positive Behavior it’s never too late, Publication No. 421-88-109. St.
Interventions, 4, 109–117. Paul, MN: Metropolitan Council.
Knoster, T. P., Villa, R. A., & Thousand, J. S. (2000). A O’Brien, J. (1987). A guide to lifestyle planning: Using the
framework for thinking about systems change. In R. A. activities catalog to integrate services and natural sup-
Villa & J. S. Thousand (Eds.), Restructuring for car- port systems. In B. Wilcox & G. T. Bellamy (Eds.), A
ing and effective education (pp. 93–128). Baltimore: comprehensive guide to the activities catalog: An alter-
Paul H. Brookes. native curriculum for youth and adults with severe dis-
Koegel, L. K., Koegel, R. L., & Dunlap, G. (1996). abilities (pp. 175–189). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
Positive behavioral support: Including people with O’Brien, J., Mount, B., & O’Brien, C. (1991). Framework
difficult behavior in the community. Baltimore: Paul for accomplishment: Personal profile. Decatur, GA:
H. Brookes. Responsive Systems Associates.
Lee, Y., Sugai, G., & Horner, R. H. (1999). Using an Park, J., Hoffman, L., Marquis, J., Turnbull, A. P., Poston,
instructional intervention to reduce problem and D., Mannan, H., … Nelson, L. L. (2003). Toward
off-task behaviors. Journal of Positive Behavior assessing family outcomes of service delivery:
Interventions, 1, 195–204. Validation of a family quality of life survey. Journal of
Lucyshyn, J. M., Albin, R. W., Horner, R. H., Mann, J. C., Intellectual Disability Research, 47, 367–384.
Mann, J. A., & Wadsworth, G. (2007). Family imple- Reeve, C. E., & Carr, E. G. (2000). Prevention of severe
mentation of positive behavior support for a child behavior problems in children with developmental
with autism: Longitudinal, single-case, experimental, disorders. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions,
and descriptive replication and extension. Journal of 2, 144–160.
Positive Behavior Interventions, 9, 131–150. Reichle, J., McEvoy, M., Davis, C., Rogers, E., Feeley, K.,
Magito McLaughlin, D., Mullen James, K., Anderson Johnston, S., & Wolff, K. (1996). Coordinating preser-
Ryan, K., & Carr, E. G. (2002). Best practices: Finding vice and in-service training of early interventionists to
a new direction for Christos. Journal of Positive serve preschoolers who engage in challenging behav-
Behavior Interventions, 4, 156–164. ior. In L. K. Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap (Eds.),
Malette, P., Mirenda, P., Jones, P., Bunz, T., & Rogow, S. Positive behavioral support: Including people with
(1992). Application of a lifestyle development process difficult behavior in the community (pp. 227–259).
for persons with severe intellectual disabilities: A case Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.
study report. Journal of the Association for Persons Schalock, R. L., & Keith, K. D. (1993). Quality of life
with Severe Handicaps, 17, 179–191. questionnaire. Worthington, OH: IDS.
McAtee, M. L., Carr, E. G., & Schulte, C. (2004). Schwartz, I. S., & Olswang, L. B. (1996). Evaluating
Contextual assessment inventory for problem behav- child behavior change in natural settings: Exploring
24  Positive Behavior Support 457

alternative strategies for data collection. Topics in ing the vision. Journal of the Association for Persons
Early Childhood Special Education, 16, 82–101. with Severe Handicaps, 14, 205–215.
Singer, G. H. S. (2000). Ecological validity. Journal of Vaughn, B. J., Dunlap, G., Fox, L., Clarke, S., & Bucy,
Positive Behavior Interventions, 2, 122–124. M. (1997). Parent-professional partnership in behav-
Smith, C. E., Carr, E. G., & Moskowitz, L. J. (2016). ioral support: A case study of community-based inter-
Fatigue as a biological setting event for severe prob- vention. Journal of the Association for Persons with
lem behavior in autism spectrum disorder. Research in Severe Handicaps, 22, 186–197.
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 23, 131–144. Walton, K. M., & Ingersoll, B. R. (2012). Evaluation of
Smull, M. W., & Harrison, S. B. (1992). Supporting people a sibling-mediated imitation intervention for young
with severe retardation in the community. Alexandria, children with autism. Journal of Positive Behavior
VA: National Association of State Mental Retardation Interventions, 14, 241–253.
Program Directors. Weisner, T. S., Coots, J., & Bernheimer, L. (1997). The
Turnbull, A. P. (1988). The challenge of providing ecocultural family interview manual. Los Angeles:
comprehensive support to families. Education and UCLA Center for Culture and Health.
Training in Mental Retardation, 23, 261–272. Wilcox, B., & Bellamy, T. (1987). A comprehensive guide
Turnbull, A. P., & Turnbull, R. (1996). Group action plan- to the activities catalog: An alternative curriculum for
ning as a strategy for providing comprehensive family youth and adults with severe disabilities. Baltimore:
support. In L. K. Koegel, R. L. Koegel, & G. Dunlap Paul H. Brookes.
(Eds.), Positive behavioral support: Including people Zionts, L. T., Zionts, P., Harrison, S., & Bellinger, O.
with difficult behavior in the community (pp. 99–114). (2003). Urban African American families’ perceptions
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes. of cultural sensitivity within the special education
Vandercook, T., York, J., & Forest, M. (1989). The McGill system. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental
action planning systems (MAPS): A strategy for build- Disabilities, 18, 41–50.
Psychotropic Medications
as Treatments for People 25
with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Alan Poling, Kristal Ehrhardt, and Anita Li

criteria for other disorders, including attention defi-


Introduction cit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), oppositional
defiant disorder (ODD), and intellectual disability
To be diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ID) (Charman et al., 2011; Lecavalier, Kaat, &
(ASD) according to the criteria described in Stratis, 2014; Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007b).
the current version of the Diagnostic and Helping people with ASD change their behavior in
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorder (DSM- desired ways is an invaluable strategy for enhanc-
V), a person must exhibit restricted, repetitive ing the quality of their lives, and professionals from
patterns of behavior and deficits in social com- many disciplines use the tools of their trade in
munication and social interaction (American attempts to do so. Psychotropic drugs, which are
Psychiatric Association, 2013). The reported medications prescribed with the intent of improv-
prevalence of ASD has increased over the last ing mood, cognitive status, or overt behavior, are
30 years, probably due to increased public the behavior change tools of psychiatrists and other
awareness of the condition and broadening of physicians. It is unsurprising that they frequently
the diagnostic category (Elsabbagh et al., prescribe such drugs for people with ASD. This
2012). For example, the Center Disease Control chapter provides a skeptical appraisal of this
(2016) indicates that 1 in 150 children was practice.
identified with ASD in 2000, but 1 in 68 was so
identified 12 years later.
Many people diagnosed with ASD exhibit chal-  he Prevalence of Pharmacological
T
lenging behaviors that are not part of the defining Interventions
features of the disorder, as well as the kinds of
behavioral excesses and deficits required for the Several studies have examined the prevalence of
diagnosis (Huete, Schmidt, & Lopez-­Arvizu, 2014; psychotropic drug use in people with
Matson & Nebel-Schwalm, 2007a). Moreover, ASD. Findings differed across studies, with prev-
some people diagnosed with ASD exhibit behav- alence rates ranging from 19.5% (Witwer &
iors similar to those required to meet the diagnostic Lecavalier, 2005) to 65% (Schubart, Camacho, &
Leslie, 2014), but most found that approximately
40–50% of sampled individuals were receiving
A. Poling (*) • K. Ehrhardt • A. Li
or had received at least one psychotropic medica-
Western Michigan University,
Kalamazoo, MI 49008, USA tion (e.g., Aman, Lam, & Collier-Crespin,
e-mail: alan.poling@wmich.edu 2003; Croen, Najjar, Ray, Lotspeich, & Bernal,

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 459


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_25
460 A. Poling et al.

2006; Goin-Kochel, Myers, & Mackintosh,  rug Treatment as Evidence-Based


D
2007; Green et al., 2006; Gringras, 2000; Practice
­Langworthy-­Lam, Aman, & Van Bourgondien,
2002; Logan et al., 2015; Sheehan et al., 2015; Professionals in medicine, psychology, and
Williams et al., 2012; Witwer & Lecavalier, other helping disciplines agree that widespread
2005). Although most researchers examined only adoption of evidence-based practice is the cor-
relatively young people with ASD, substantially nerstone of effective clinical treatment (e.g., APA
higher prevalence rates have been reported in Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based
adults compared to children and adolescents Practice, 2006; Institute of Medicine, 2001;
(Park et al., 2016; Seltzer, Shattuck, Abbeduto, & Montori & Guyatt, 2008). Although there is
Greenberg, 2004; Tsakanikos et al., 2006). no consensus as to what, exactly, constitutes
Additionally, lower levels of social competence evidence-­based practice, it is widely acknowl-
and adaptive behavior, like the presence of chal- edged that clinicians should select and administer
lenging behaviors, are associated with increased treatments for a given problem based on three
likelihood of pharmacological treatment (Myers factors. Those factors are (a) their own training
& Johnson, 2007). and expertise, (b) the characteristics and prefer-
It is not unusual for people with ASD to simul- ences of the client being treated, and (c) the
taneously receive two or more medications scientific evidence supporting the effectiveness
intended to improve their behavior, a practice we of various treatments for the problem at hand.
will term polypharmacy. For example, in a study Pharmacology is a major part of medicine,
of 33,565 children with ASD conducted by and medical doctors from all specialties receive
Spencer et al. (2013), 35% of the individuals extensive training in selecting drugs to deal with
were prescribed two or more psychotropic medi- diverse health issues, arranging appropriate doses
cations simultaneously, while 30% of them were of those drugs, monitoring their effects, and alter-
prescribed a single drug. Very similar results ing treatment as appropriate to achieve desired
were reported by Schubart et al. (2014), who outcomes (e.g., by altering dosage or changing to
examined psychotropic drug use among another medication). Psychiatrists are specialists
Medicaid-enrolled children and adolescents with in the use of psychotropic drugs, but many other
ASD in 41 states over a 4-year period. They physicians also have the training and experience
“found that 65% of children with ASD were pre- necessary to use drugs as tools for managing
scribed one or more psychotropics and approxi- behavior. In so doing they are operating within
mately 30% were prescribed medications in more the ethical and legal boundaries of their disci-
than one class with at least a 60-day overlap” pline and are offering what is often the only ten-
(p. 634). As a third example, Mandell et al. able treatment option given their training and the
(2008) used Medicaid claims to examine the psy- limited time they have to spend with individual
chotropic medications prescribed for 60,641 chil- clients. It is natural and appropriate for physi-
dren with ASD. They found that 56% of them cians who are asked to help in improving some-
received at least one such medication and 20% one’s behavior to prescribe psychotropic drugs,
received three or more (data for two or more regardless of whether or not the clients are diag-
drugs were not reported). nosed with ASD. In so doing they are using tools
Clearly, the use of medications in an attempt that are both familiar and arguably the best at
to improve the behavior of people with ASD, their disposal.
and thereby benefit them, is widespread. An Although some people with ASD are old and
important, and obvious, question is “why is competent enough to make legally binding deci-
this so?” Matson and Konst (2015) provided a sions, in most cases treatments for people with
partial answer, which we expand in the next ASD are sought and selected by their parents or
section. legal guardians. There are five obvious reasons
25 Medications 461

for parents and guardians to view p­ harmacotherapy currently approved by the US Food and Drug
as a preferred option for dealing with behavioral Administration (FDA) for treating the defining
challenges. One is that pharmaceutical compa- behavioral features (i.e., “core symptoms”) of
nies have been hugely successful in convincing autism, and, as discussed in the section entitled
the public at large that most behavioral chal- “Research Findings,” there is no compelling evi-
lenges are the result of underlying biochemical dence that any medication is effective in this
anomalies that respond favorably to drug treat- regard.
ments (Whitaker, 2010). A second, related reason Two drugs, risperidone (Risperdal®) and
is that viewing challenging problems as the result aripiprazole (Abilify®), are FDA approved for
of an underlying neurochemical issue, rather than treating “irritability” in children and adolescents
as learned behaviors, frees caregivers of the nag- diagnosed with ASD (United States Food and
ging fear that they are responsible, albeit uninten- Drug Administration, 2006, 2009). “Irritability”
tionally, for the occurrence of those behaviors. A is a shorthand label for several forms of challeng-
third is that insurance companies, and Medicare, ing behavior, including crying, self-injury,
typically pay for pharmacological interventions, aggression directed toward others, and property
while other intervention strategies may not be destruction. The term is commonly used in arti-
covered. A fourth is that drug treatments are easy cles evaluating drug effects in people with ASD
to administer, especially when compared to alter- but rarely used in other contexts. Its popularity in
natives such as behavior-­analytic interventions. the drug literature stems from the widespread use
A fifth is that a concerned individual looking for of a particular behavior rating scale, the Aberrant
an effective treatment for any of a range of chal- Behavior Checklist (ABC; Aman, Singh, Stewart,
lenging can easily find endorsements for pharma- & Field, 1985), to index drug effects.
cological interventions. The ABC is a 58-item symptom checklist that
If, for example, a parent consults the National is completed by a caregiver. The instrument was
Autism Center’s (2011) well-regarded book, A first used with children and adults diagnosed with
Parent’s Guide to Evidence-Based Practice and cognitive impairment (and then termed “mental
Autism, she or he will learn that risperidone retardation”), but it is now widely employed to
(Risperdal®) is an effective for treating “core study drug effects in people with ASD. Each item
symptoms [of ASD] (generally), maladaptive is scored on a four-point scale (0, not a problem;
behavior, hyperactivity, irritability” (p. 54). In through 3, problem is severe in degree). The
addition, methylphenidate (Ritalin®) is deemed items are categorized into five subscales revealed
an effective treatment for addressing the symp- through factor analysis: (1) irritability, agitation,
toms of “inattention and hyperactivity (but and crying (commonly termed “irritability,” 15
response rate may be lower in children with items), (2) lethargy and social withdrawal (16
ASD)” (p. 53). Given these endorsements, and items), (3) stereotypic behavior (7 items), (4)
the other reasons for favoring drug treatments, it hyperactivity and noncompliance (16 items), and
is perfectly reasonable for parents to support (5) inappropriate speech (4 items). Although the
administering one of these drugs to their ABC is easy to use and is reported by its devel-
children. oper to be a reliable and valid behavior rating
instrument (Aman, 2002), it is also a crude instru-
ment that yields ordinal data and provides no
I rritability: Creation and Treatment detailed information about how a person with
of a Make-Believe Disease ASD is behaving. Moreover, it provides data that
are based on raters’ subjective opinions and
Even though risperidone is classified as an effec- memories and provides no information about
tive treatment for the core symptoms of ASD in A contextual variables that affect behavior.
Parent’s Guide to Evidence-Based Practice Perhaps the worst problem in using “irritabil-
(National Autism Center, 2011), no medication is ity” to describe certain kinds of challenging
462 A. Poling et al.

behaviors emitted by people with ASD is that one Behavioral Mechanisms


can all too easily reify the term and then assert of Drug Action
that it causes the occurrence of the behaviors the
label was initially used to describe. It appears, in What psychotropic drugs actually do is to perturb
fact, that FDA administrators did exactly this neurochemical processes. These perturbations
when they approved risperidone for treating sometimes influence an individual’s sensitivity to
people with ASD. Consider the full prescribing environmental events, and in such cases it is
information for risperidone (Risperdal®), which possible to specify the drug’s behavioral mecha-
includes the following statement: nism of action. In contrast to neurochemical
RISPERDAL® [risperidone] is indicated for the mechanisms of drug action, which relate to the
treatment of irritability associated with autistic dis- effects of drugs in the brain, behavioral mecha-
order in children and adolescents aged 5–16 years, nisms of action refer to the stimulus functions of
including symptoms of aggression towards others, drugs in the context of operant and classical con-
deliberate self-injuriousness, temper tantrums, and
quickly changing moods [see Clinical Studies ditioning and to the effects of the drugs on the
(14.4)]. (downloaded from http://www.accessdata. capacity of other stimuli to control behavior.
fda.gov/drugsatfda_docs/label/2009/020272s056, The stimulus properties of drugs involve their
020588s044,021346s033,021444s03lbl.pdf. ability to serve as conditional stimuli, uncondi-
tional stimuli, discriminative stimuli, positive
Note that risperidone is indicated for the treat- reinforcers (conditioned or unconditioned), and
ment of “irritability associated with autistic dis- negative reinforcers (conditioned or uncondi-
order” and that “aggression towards others, tioned). Drugs also can serve as motivational
deliberate self-injuriousness, temper tantrums, operations, increasing or decreasing the reinforc-
and quickly changing moods” are specifically ing or punishing effects of certain other stimuli. In
described as symptoms of that irritability. In fact, addition, they can alter sensitivity to particular
there is no evidence for the existence of “irritabil- dimensions of reinforcement (e.g., amount, prob-
ity,” save for the behaviors described as symp- ability, delay), influence sensory acuity (hence
toms of it (e.g., self-injury). Irritability is not a discrimination), and elicit responses incompatible
disease or a behavior disorder, and it is utter fool- with required operants. Finally, drugs and their
ishness to contend that risperidone, or any other effects can be described in statements (rules) that
psychotropic drug, alleviates “irritability” in peo- alter behavior through rule governance. These and
ple with ASD, which in turn reduces their chal- other behavioral mechanisms of drug action are
lenging behavior. described elsewhere (Poling & Byrne, 2000).
In most cases, challenging behaviors emit- Little is known regarding the relation between
ted by people with ASD are operant responses behavioral mechanisms of action and the benefi-
controlled by their consequences. For example, cial (as well as adverse) effects of psychotropic
a review of 173 studies in which functional medications in people with autism (Poling,
assessment techniques were used to isolate Ehrhardt, Wood, & Bowerman, 2010), but some
environmental variables related to the chal- recent progress has occurred. To determine behav-
lenging behavior of people with ASD revealed ioral mechanisms of drug action, one must first
that, in most participants, attention or escape identify the environmental variables which typi-
from demands maintained the responses of cally regulate the behavior in question. Functional
interest (Matson et al., 2011). In such cases, analysis (and functional assessment in general)
operant conditioning, not an internal state of provides a tool for doing so and has been used in
irritability, was responsible for the challenging a few studies to examine the variables controlling
behaviors. Although researchers and clinicians challenging behavior and how risperidone inter-
frequently discuss drug effects on “irritability” acts with those variables (Crosland et al., 2003;
(e.g., Elbe & Lalani, 2012), doing so is at best Valdovinos et al., 2002; Zarcone et al., 2004).
misleading. Unfortunately, those studies failed to disclose a
25 Medications 463

characteristic behavioral mechanism of drug ASD, current knowledge is inadequate to provide


action for risperidone. In many participants, the a sound rationale for the use of pharmacotherapy
environmental variables controlling destructive (Bethea & Sikich, 2007; Buxbaum & Hof, 2013;
behavior could not be isolated. In some partici- Thompson, 2007). That is, there is no known dis-
pants, however, the functions of the response class ease process that is responsible for the behaviors
were apparent, and risperidone appeared to pro- that lead to a diagnosis of autism, or for co-­
duce consequence-dependent effects, specifically, occurring challenging behaviors, which can be
to weaken escape-­maintained responding. corrected by administering a drug with a particu-
A more recent study that used analogue func- lar mechanism of action. At this point in time, the
tional analysis and other methods to examine the only valid justification for prescribing behavior-­
effects of several drugs on the rate and function modifying drugs for people with ASD is sound
of problem behaviors exhibited by four children empirical evidence that such drugs are effective.
with ASD also revealed that atypical antipsychot- As Heute et al. (2014) point out, “psychopharma-
ics often reduce escape-maintained responding cological interventions may be used to treat [all
(Valdovinos, Nelson, Kuhle, & Dierks, 2009). In behaviors indicative of] an entire suspected psy-
this study, rates of problem behaviors exhibited chiatric disturbance, a specific behavior as a
by two students decreased in the demand condi- symptom of a psychiatric diagnosis, or a behav-
tion (where responding was escape-maintained), ior occurring in the absence of a psychiatric diag-
but not in other conditions, when risperidone or nosis” (p. 735).
olanzapine was discontinued. A drug would be used to treat a psychiatric
In another recent study, Danov, Tervo, Meyers, disturbance in a person with ASD if that person
and Symons (2012) examined the effects of aripip- was to be dually diagnosed, that is, properly iden-
razole on the problem behaviors of four people tified as having both ASD and a recognized psy-
with severe developmental disabilities, one of chiatric condition (e.g., one identified in DSM-V),
whom was diagnosed with ASD. Aripiprazole such as schizophrenia, a mood disorder, an anxi-
“had some apparent differential effects across ety disorder, or ADHD. As noted previously,
behavioral function and behavioral topography for many people with ASD also are concurrently
3 of 4 participants” (p. 286), but not for the partici- diagnosed with ADHD, ODD, or ID (Charman
pant with ASD, whose behavior worsened in all et al., 2011; Lecavalier et al., 2014; Matson &
conditions when the drug was administered. The Nebel-Schwalm, 2007b). Matson and Konst
reason for the difference in drug effects across par- (2015) suggest that, while psychotropic drugs are
ticipants is not clear, but the results of this and generally overused to treat people with autism,
other studies suggest that, as others have argued “the use of pharmacological interventions is
(e.g., Schaal & Hackenberg, 1994; Thompson, appropriate for some symptoms of co-occurring
Egli, Symons, & Delaney, 1994), functional analy- psychopathology such as anxiety, depressions,
sis methodology may be useful in isolating behav- and schizophrenia” (p. 35). That may be true, but
ioral mechanisms of drug action. Discerning such it is important to recognize that the behavioral
mechanisms may prove useful in consistently characteristics that cause a person to be diag-
matching clients with ASD to effective pharmaco- nosed with ASD also make it hard to diagnose
logical interventions, a task which is currently comorbid conditions (Mason & Scior, 2004;
impossible. Shaw, Bruce, Ouimet, Sharma, & Glaser, 2009),
and studies reveal that drugs are commonly
prescribed in an effort to reduce challenging
­
 SD and Comorbidity: Dual
A behaviors (Bamidele & Hall, 2013; Canitano &
Diagnosis Scandurra, 2011; Medeiros, Kozlowski, Beighley,
Rojahn, & Matson, 2012; Tureck, Matson,
Although progress is being made in understand- Turygin, & Macmillan, 2013; West, Waldrop, &
ing how brain structure and function differ in Brunssen, 2009). Unfortunately, these responses are
people who are and who are not diagnosed with rarely defined carefully or measured precisely,
464 A. Poling et al.

and, as Matson and Konst contend, “...a very medication for years, even decades, but no
large segment of psychotropic drug use involves studies have examined drug effects over such
prescribing for extremely vague and/or ill-defined long periods. Given that psychotropic drugs
target behavior” (p. 35). are often prescribed for children with ASD,
whose brains and bodies are rapidly devel-
oping, long-term studies are especially
Limitations of Published Research important. As others have noted (e.g.,
Anderson et al., 2007; Haddad & Sharma,
Researchers have been rightly critical of the 2007), almost nothing is known about the
quantity and quality of drug studies involving long-term side effects of antipsychotics in
people with developmental disabilities, including young people with ASD, even though these
ASD, for more than 40 years (e.g., Sprague & drugs are widely used and are known to pro-
Werry, 1971; Gadow & Poling, 1988; Matson duce several adverse effects. Relevant
et al., 2000). The number of studies examining research is both badly needed and difficult to
the effects of psychotropic drugs in people with conduct.
ASD has increased greatly in recent years, and 2. The possibility of gender differences in drug
the quality of research in this area arguably has effects has been largely ignored. Although
improved with time. For example, the number of there are differences in the behavior of males
studies of children with ASD that used a random- and females with ASD (Rivet & Matson,
ized between-group design with a placebo-­ 2011), gender often influences drug effects
control condition, which is typically (but not (Poling et al. 2009), and the importance of
necessarily wisely) considered as the “gold stan- examining possible gender differences is
dard” in clinical psychopharmacology, increased widely recognized in psychopharmacology
dramatically from 1981–1990 to 2001–2010 (Volkow, 2005–2008), the usual practice in
(Siegel & Beaulieu, 2012). Nevertheless, as regu- drug studies involving people with ASD is to
larly emphasized in reviews of the literature and include relatively few female participants
other articles (e.g., Courtemanche, Schroeder, & and to aggregate data across females and
Sheldon, 2011; Farmer, Thurm, & Grant, 2013; males.
Matson & Hess, 2011; Mohiuddin & Ghaziuddin, 3. The effects of psychotropic drugs in people
2013; Poling et al. 2010; Siegel & Beaulieu, past young adulthood remain to be deter-
2012), many published studies are not method- mined. Although ASD is nearly always a
ologically strong, and several important research lifelong condition, people with ASD con-
questions have not been adequately addressed. tinue to emit challenging behaviors as they
These limitations are understandable given the age (although the form of the behaviors often
practical and ethical challenges that are an inevi- changes with time), and as drugs are fre-
table part of conducting drug research with a pro- quently prescribed in response to those chal-
tected population, but they also seriously limit lenging behaviors, researchers have paid
the conclusions supported by the current research very little attention to drug effects in older
base. Ten limitations of the research base are con- people with ASD (see Dove et al., 2012).
sidered in this section. It should be noted that Most published studies involve children, and
other limitations, such as failure to standardize Dove et al. found only eight studies of
drug dosages and studying heterogeneous and ill-­ ­medications that focused on 13- to 30-year-
defined samples of people with ASD, are also olds with ASD, four of fair quality and four
significant. of poor quality. Given the quantity and qual-
ity of the studies examining drug effects in
1. There are no long-term studies of the value or adolescents and young adults with ASD, no
adverse effects of drug treatments. People with compelling conclusions can be drawn con-
ASD often receive one or more psychotropic cerning the value of pharmacotherapy in this
25 Medications 465

population. Even less can be concluded interventions (which are often behavior-
regarding the value of psychotropic drugs for analytic) with the same intended outcome,
older people with ASD, including those who typically the reduction of challenging behav-
are elderly, because relevant research is lack- ior (Frazier, 2012). As Courtemanche,
ing. The absence of research examining the Schroeder, and Sheldon (2011) point out,
effects of pharmacotherapy in older people very little is known about the effects of
with ASD is vexing, given that data reported such combinations. They provide an excel-
by Shimabukuro, Grosse, and Rice (2008) lent discussion of strategies for examining
indicate that “individuals with an ASD are drug combinations and the importance of
utilizing increasingly intense pharmacother- doing so.
apy to control behavioral symptoms as they 7. Measures of desired and side effects are
grow older” (p. 550). often weak. As Zarcone, Naolitano, and
4. Very little is known about the effects of poly- Valdovinos (2008) discuss, one of the most
pharmacy. As noted, people with ASD often important issues in designing a drug study is
receive two or more psychotropic drugs determining which behaviors to measure and
simultaneously. There are very few data to the best way to measure them. Checklists
provide empirical support for this practice. and rating scales, such as the ABC, are used
For instance, a recent review of polyphar- to index beneficial changes in behavior in
macy involving risperidone or aripiprazole in most studies that examine drug effects in
combination with other drugs revealed that people with ASD. Although they are widely
few relevant articles have appeared, and none accepted and easy to use, such assessments
of them provide compelling support for com- yield limited information and have been
monly used drug combinations (in press). soundly criticized. For example, Huffman,
5. Drug treatments are rarely compared to Sutcliffe, Tanner, and Feldman (2011) found
alternative treatments. Other interventions, that the Clinical Global Impression (CGI)
notably those characteristic of applied scale was the most commonly used general
behavior analysis, have proven useful in measure of drug effects in the 89 studies they
reducing the kinds of challenging behaviors evaluated (it was used in 23 of them), even
that are commonly treated with psychotropic though, as they note, “its shortcomings have
drugs, but head-to-head comparisons of the been recognized in criticisms of the scale on
two intervention modalities rarely, if ever, semantic, logical, and statistical grounds and
appear. For example, research shows that in recommendations for its improvements
both risperidone and behavior-analytic inter- [references omitted]” (p. 63). Alternatives to
ventions can be effective in reducing chal- the ABC and CGI for quantifying drug
lenging behaviors, but a review indicates that effects are sorely needed and summarized
no direct comparison of risperidone and a elsewhere (e.g., Gadow & Poling, 1988;
nondrug treatment has appeared (Weeden, Zarcone et al., 2008). Moreover, as Matson
Ehrhardt, & Poling, 2010a). Moreover, dif- and Hess (2011) emphasize, side effects are
ferent research strategies are typically used rarely assessed adequately, even though such
to evaluate behavior-analytic and pharmaco- effects can be quite serious. They offer a
logical interventions, which make it nearly number of useful suggestions for improving
impossible to compare findings across the measurement of side effects.
studies. 8. Data analysis is often weak. Three separate
6. The effects of combinations of psychotropic issues bear mention. One is that inferential
medication and non-pharmacological statistics, in which group means (e.g., on the
interventions are largely unknown. People with irritability subscale of the ABC) for a pla-
ASD often are simultaneously exposed to both cebo and drug group (or condition) are com-
pharmacological and non-­ pharmacological pared, are widely used in an attempt to
466 A. Poling et al.

determine whether drug treatment produced Unfortunately, studies of drug effects in


a beneficial change in behavior. Statistical participants with ASD that compare medica-
significance is not the same as clinical sig- tion to an active placebo have not appeared.
nificance, which must be assessed using a 10. Predictors of positive responses to drugs
social validation procedure (Poling & have not been isolated. Studies repeatedly
Ehrhardt, 1999; Poling, Methot, & LeSage, show that there are important individual dif-
1995). A second is that the number of par- ferences in how people with ASD respond to
ticipants in many studies is relatively small, a given psychotropic drug, even when the
which compromises the power of statistical dose for each is equivalent (or optimized)
analyses. A third is that meaningless data are and the condition being treated appears to be
sometimes analyzed, as when researchers comparable. For this reason, researchers
consider total ABC scores rather than sub- should routinely distinguish “responders,”
scale scores (e.g., Fung, Chahal, Libove, who are people who respond favorably to a
Bivas, & Hardan, 2012; Thompson, Zarcone, drug, from “nonresponders,” who fail to ben-
& Symons, 2004). efit from the medication, and many (but by
9. The potential for bias to affect findings is no means all) do so. When this is done, a sig-
high in many studies. As Matson and Konst nificant proportion of patients inevitably
(2015) point out, many studies of the phar- proves to be nonresponders. For example, in
macological treatment of people with ASD a study of the effects of risperidone in adults
are funded by the companies that manufac- that used scores on the Clinical Global
ture the drugs being evaluated. Such funding Impression of Improvement scale to index
automatically raises the issue of researcher drug effects, 8 of 14 participants who
bias, both intentional and unintentional. received risperidone were rated as respond-
Knowledge of the conditions to which indi- ers, defined as people whose scores were
vidual participants are exposed is another “much improved” or “very much improved”
source of potential bias, and it is convention- when they received risperidone (McDougle
ally controlled through the use of double-­ et al., 1998). It stands to reason that the
blind conditions, in which neither the patients who responded favorably to risperi-
participants in a study nor the researchers (or done differed in some important way or ways
others) who evaluate them know whether from patients who did not benefit. If empiri-
particular participants are receiving drug or cal variables that reliably distinguish
placebo when data are collected. These con- responders from nonresponders could be
trols are absent in open-label drug trials, identified, then it would be possible to accu-
which should always be viewed with extreme rately match patients to effective treatments,
skepticism, especially in view of data sug- which is the essence of sound clinical prac-
gesting that placebo responses are especially tice. Although researchers have searched for
strong in studies of participants with ASD valid predictor variables at several levels of
(Sandler & Bodfish, 2000). Even when a pla- analysis and have made some progress, as in
cebo is given, it may be easy to tell whether the studies (previously overviewed) suggest-
or not a participant is receiving active medi- ing that antipsychotic drugs weaken
cation, because such medication produces ­escape-­maintained behavior, it is not pres-
obvious changes in that participant’s physi- ently possible to accurately predict individ-
ological status or behavior. In such cases, an ual responses to a given medication. Until
active placebo, that is, a substance that pro- this is accomplished, if ever, it is imperative
duces some detectable effects similar to that every treated individual’s response to
those of the medication of interest, but has medication be carefully monitored, as dis-
no psychotropic action, should be used cussed in the section entitled “Everyday
(Khan & Brown, 2015; Moncrieff, 2015). Medication Monitoring.”
25 Medications 467

Research Findings classes, several with no recognized psychotropic


applications, have also been examined. For
Hundreds of studies have examined the effects of example, a review by Bertelli et al. summarized
various drugs on the behavior of people with the effects of the following drug classes (and
ASD. Although there are serious limitations to individual drugs): antipsychotics (risperidone,
this body of research, as discussed in the forego- palperidone, aripiprazole, clozapine, olanzapine,
ing section, “Limitations of Published Research,” quetiapine, ziprasidone, and asenapine), antide-
published findings support some conclusions, pressants (tricyclics, notably clomipramine, nor-
and dozens of scholarly reviews have summa- triptyline; serotonin-specific reuptake inhibitors,
rized these findings. Table 25.1 lists 18 peer-­ notably fluvoxamine, fluoxetine, sertraline, cital-
reviewed reviews, all published in the past opram, escitalopram, venlafaxine, trazodone, and
decade. Readers seeking informed summaries of mirtazapine), anticonvulsants and mood stabiliz-
the published literature are advised to consult ers (valproic acid, topiramate, levetiracetam, and
these sources. Useful information is also avail- lamotrigine), central nervous system stimulants
able in book chapters and books not specifically (methylphenidate and atomoxetine), other com-
devoted to the psychopharmacology of autism pounds (clonidine, guanfacine, naltrexone, and
(e.g., Huete et al., 2014; Thompson, 2007) and in secretin), new frontier pharmacotherapy (cholin-
a good but somewhat outdated book concerned ergic drugs, notably tacrine, rivastigmine, galan-
solely with the topic (Tsai, 2001). It is important tamine, donepezil, and mecamylamine;
to recognize, however, that authors differ from glutamatergic agents, notably d-cyclosterine,
one another with respect to their general orienta- amantadine, memantine, acamprosate, arba-
tion toward pharmacological interventions, with clofen, and bumetanide), melatonergic agents
some being more skeptical than others. Moreover, (melatonin and agomelatin), and oxytocin.
there is no consensus concerning the specific That list comprises 42 individual drugs.
characteristics that enable a drug evaluation to Published studies are inadequate to support com-
yield creditable findings (Courtemanche et al., pelling conclusions about the benefits or risks of
2011; Higgins & Green, 2006); therefore, review- the vast majority of them. Nonetheless, regard-
ers can legitimately differ with respect to the less of the drug evaluated or what it is prescribed
weight they assign to the findings of particular to treat, most original investigations report a ben-
studies and the conclusions that they draw from eficial outcome in at least some patients, and
them. Despite these considerations, the conclu- many reviews echo these reports. For example,
sions of most reviews are similar. Based on our methylphenidate is often reported to be effective
reading of these reviews and most of the original in treating “hyperactivity,” although some review-
articles upon which they are based, it is our opin- ers view the supporting evidence as compelling
ion that the following conclusions are justified at (e.g., Huffman et al. 2011), while others view it
this time. as suggestive (e.g., Siegel & Beaulieu, 2012).
Such disagreements make it clear that extant data
are inadequate to provide adequate guidance for
A Wide Range of Medications Have physicians who are contemplating the use of psy-
Been Evaluated, Inadequately chotropic medications to treat a person with
ASD, even if they are familiar with the relevant
In descending order of frequency, the drug classes studies and committed to the use of scientifically
most commonly prescribed for people with ASD verified practices.
appear to be antipsychotics, antidepressants, As Heute et al. (2014) point out, “...psychia-
stimulants, and anticonvulsants (Bertelli, Rossi, trists are challenged with basing their under-
Keller, & Lassi, 2016). Multiple drugs from each standing of medication utility on a less than
of these classes have been evaluated in one or optimal body of research and more often on case
more studies. In addition, drugs from many other study reports, and sometimes must refer to
468 A. Poling et al.

Table 25.1  Summary of published reviews in the last 10 years (listed alphabetically)
Published reviews Drug or drug classes Demographica Target symptomsb
Aman et al. (2014) Atomoxetine Children (19 or younger) Hyperactivity
Baribeau and Multiple drug agents Children and adults Social communication
Anagnostou (2013)
Broadstock, Doughty, Multiple drug agents Children and adults Core symptoms of ASD
and Eggleston (2007) Comorbid symptoms
Dove et al. (2012) Multiple drug classes Adolescent and young Core symptoms of ASD
adults (13–30 years old) Comorbid symptoms
Doyle and McDougle SRIs Child and adults Core symptoms of ASD
(2012) Antipsychotics Comorbid symptoms
Elbe and Lalani (2012) Antipsychotics Children and adults Irritability
Misc. drug agents
Fung et al. (2012) Aripiprazole Children (4–18 years old) Sensory abnormalities
Ghanizadeh (2012) Atomoxetine Children and adults ADHD symptoms
Krishnaswami et al. Secretin Children (12 or younger) Core symptoms of ASD
(2011)
McPheeters et al. (2011) Multiple drug classes Children (12 or younger) Challenging and
repetitive behaviors
Mohiuddin and Multiple drug classes Children and adults Hyperactivity
Ghaziuddin (2013) Irritability
Aggression
Parikh, Kolevzon, and Multiple drug agents Children and adolescents Aggression
Hollander (2008) Self-injurious behaviors
Preti et al. (2014 Oxytocin Children and adults Emotion recognition
Eye gaze
Reichow, Volkmar, and Methylphenidate Children ADHD symptoms
Bloch (2013) Atomoxetine
Clonidine
Rossignol and Frye Multiple drug agents approved Children and adults Core symptoms of ASD
(2014) for Alzheimer’s disease Comorbid symptoms
Roy, Roy, Deb, Unwin, Naltrexone Children Core symptoms of ASD
and Roy (2015) Comorbid symptoms
Siegel and Beaulieu Alpha-2 agonists Children (18 or younger) Core symptoms of ASD
(2012) Antipsychotics Comorbid symptoms
West et al. (2009) SSRIs Children Core symptoms of ASD
Comorbid symptoms
a
Demographics as reported by the authors in the review
b
Target symptoms and/or areas as reported by the authors in the review

reported results and clinical trials of medications such as depression of ADHD, responds to medica-
used in the general population for similar symptoms tion. Both assumptions are reasonable. But, as
to guide their decisions” (p. 736). They assume, discussed previously, it is hard to diagnose
for example, that people with ASD can experi- comorbid psychiatric conditions in people with
ence all of the psychiatric conditions (or behavior ASD, and medications are usually prescribed to
disorders) exhibited by other people and that the reduce specific challenging behaviors in people
presence of ASD does not fundamentally alter with ASD, not to reduce established symptoms of
how a person with a given psychiatric condition, recognized psychiatric conditions (e.g., DSM-V
25 Medications 469

criteria for specific disorders). Put simply, the substantial evidence that aripiprazole, also
rationale for prescribing most psychotropic approved for reducing challenging behavior, is
drugs for people with ASD is weak, and the com- often effective.
mon practice of using polypharmacy to manage Antipsychotic drugs can produce a range of
supposed coexisting psychiatric conditions in troublesome and potentially serious side effects,
people with ASD is fraught with difficulty. including sedation, weight gain, metabolic
changes, and motor disturbances. Although anti-
psychotic drugs are often efficacious, we agree
 o Drug Significantly Improves
N with McPheeters et al. (2011), who contend that
the Core Symptoms of Autism “caution is warranted regarding their use in
patients without severe impairments or risk of
Although a few authors might disagree, most injury” (p. 1319).
people who reviewed the relevant literature
reached conclusions similar to the two that fol-
low, as do we: Secretin Is Useless
In summary, despite their widespread use, there
exist no medications that are specific to the core Although secretin, which is a gastrointestinal
symptoms of autism. At best medications result peptide, was once a popular treatment for young
only in modest symptomatic response. (Mohiuddin people with ASD, several methodologically
& Ghaziuddin, 2013, p. 652)
[T]his review finds that there are no definitely sound studies show beyond reasonable doubt that
effective or efficacious pharmacologic treatments it is of no value whatsoever (Huffman et al.,
for the core symptoms of autism. (Farmer et al., 2011; Krishnaswami, McPheeters, & Veenstra-­
2013, p. 310) Vaderweele, 2011).

No medication that substantially reduces the


core symptoms of ASD is currently available, but Everyday Medication Monitoring
researchers continue to search for one. Like
everyone concerned with the well-being of peo- As noted, there are substantial differences in how
ple with ASD, we hope they find it soon. what appear to be similar people with ASD
respond to a particular psychotropic drug. Some
are responders, others nonresponders, and, more-
 ntipsychotic Drugs Often Reduce
A over and importantly, there are substantial
Challenging Behaviors in Children ­individual differences in the form and severity of
and Adolescent the side effects produced by a given drug and
dose. Moreover, people with ASD, and especially
Although several antipsychotic drugs appear to children, may not be able to self-monitor and
reduce challenging behaviors, the evidence is report the effects of medications to their physi-
best for risperidone, unsurprising because the cians and other caregivers. Given these consider-
drug has been approved for a decade for reducing ations, every person with ASD who receives a
such behaviors. As noted, many but not all chil- psychotropic medication should be carefully
dren and adolescents treated with appropriate monitored to ensure that they are receiving sig-
doses of risperidone show substantial reductions nificant benefit from it.
in challenging behaviors, such as self-injury, We have repeatedly argued (e.g., Poling, 1994;
temper tantrums, and aggression directed toward Poling & Ehrhardt, 1999; Poling, Laraway,
property and other people. This outcome can be Ehrhardt, Jennings, & Turner 2004; Poling,
of great value to the treated individuals and those Methot,  & LeSage, 1995; Weeden, Ehrhardt, &
who love and care for them. There is also Poling, 2010b), and argue again, that accountable,
470 A. Poling et al.

hence appropriate, pharmacotherapy requires that are worth making. First, some people with ASD
(a) treatment goals (i.e., the desired changes in lack sufficient communication skills to partici-
target behaviors) are clear and in the participant’s pate in certain types of assessments. Second,
best interest, (b) treatment procedures (i.e., who people are inclined to see (and report) what they
does what to whom) are unambiguous and imple- expect (and hope) to see, so the potential for
mented with fidelity, and (c) treatment decisions observer bias affecting results is always a
(i.e., whether the intervention is continued, consideration.
altered, or terminated) are made on the basis of Third, some of the strategies necessary to col-
actual changes in target behaviors and other rele- lect important data are invasive and will not be
vant characteristics of the participant (e.g., evi- well tolerated by some people with ASD. For
dence of significant side effects). In fact, instance, when patients are prescribed an anti-
caregivers who are committed to using evidence- psychotic, like risperidone, their blood lipids
based practice – and all of them should be so com- and fasting blood glucose should be regularly
mitted – have two essential obligations. One is to monitored (Panagiotopoulos, Ronsley, Elbe,
select interventions based on scientific evidence Davidson, & Smith, 2010) with blood collec-
indicating that those interventions are likely to be tions. But, as Elbe and Lalani (2012) indicate,
effective in the patients that receive it. The other is “for some children with autism spectrum disor-
to provide compelling evidence that the interven- der, attempts at blood collection can lead to
tions actually are effective in the patients that severe behavioural outbursts and intervention
receive them. may be required to complete appropriate moni-
As Sprague and Werry (1971) emphasized toring” (Davit, Hundley, Bacic, & Hanson, 2011,
many years ago, every prescription of a psychotro- p. 145). Rather than arranging such an interven-
pic medication is in essence an experiment in tion, caregivers may simply forego the
which the physician and other caregivers hypoth- monitoring.
esize that administering a specific drug will pro- Fourth, most physicians are not trained in
duce a desired change in one or more aspects of a behavioral assessment, and even those who are
client’s behavior. They hope and expect that the well trained do not have the time to collect rele-
hypothesis will be confirmed but must collect rel- vant data. Therefore, if physicians’ decisions
evant data to validate their expectation. If they do regarding the behavioral effects of psychotropic
not, patients may be exposed indefinitely to inter- medications are to be data based, other people
ventions that fail to help, and may even hurt, them. must collect appropriate data. We have suggested
Depending on the desired effects of the drug that behavior analysts, by virtue of their training
in question, in a particular situation, checklists, and professional functions, are in an especially
rating scales, interviews, and direct observations good position to collect such data (Poling &
may be useful in quantifying drug effects. Good Ehrhardt, 1999; Weeden et al., 2010b), but,
assessment procedures are easy to use, provide a regardless of who actually collects data, it is
meaningful index of the behaviors of clinical essential that all concerned parties decide before
concern, and are acceptable to parents, other rel- medication is prescribed what the drug is intended
evant caregivers, the prescribing physician, and to do and how its effects will be measured and
(insofar as possible) the person with ASD. Several evaluated. Strategies for detecting possible
articles provide good coverage of issues relating untoward drug effects should also be selected at
to quantifying the behavioral effects of drugs in this time. Collecting multiple measures of drug
people with ASD in clinical research (e.g., effects in different situations, such as at home
Arnold et al., 2000; Courtemanche et al., 2011; and at school, is typically desirable, because
McDougle et al., 2000; Matson & Nebel-­ challenging behaviors are often situation spe-
Schwalm, 2007a; Zarcone et al., 2008), and the cific. Having multiple individuals collect data
same general issues pertain to the everyday also is desirable, because doing so reduces (but
assessment of medication effects. It is beyond our does not eliminate) the likelihood of observer
purpose to discuss these issues, but four points bias confounding results.
25 Medications 471

The rigor with which drug effects can be in people with ID to their effects in people with
assessed in different individuals with ASD varies ASD. It is sadly ironic that many of the same
substantially, depending on the situation and the concerns that were expressed years ago, as in
caregivers involved. In our experience, it is com- reviews of the literature by Baumeister and Sevin
mon to have little or no formal assessment. That is, (1990) and Matson et al. (2000), remain relevant
no data relevant to drug effects are collected, and today. Consider the following comment on the
the value of the intervention is assessed based on methodology of published studies involving people
the global impressions of parents, teachers, or other with ID (once termed “mental retardation”),
caregivers. It is unsurprising that this is the case – which appeared more than three decades age:
most people are not committed to data-­based deci- Thirty-nine articles (1970–1982) on drug effects in
sion-making and even those who are may find it mentally retarded participants were evaluated on
difficult to collect appropriate information regard- 14 methodological dimensions. Methodological
ing drug effects. It is also unfortunate. shortcomings were evident in most, but not all,
studies. The relative scarcity of methodologically
Consider the study by Zarcone et al. (2004), sound studies has significant implications for clini-
summarized previously. In that study, risperidone cians, whose decisions concerning drug use with
did not appear to reduce destructive behavior in 3 the mentally retarded should be data-based.
of 13 participants. Nonetheless, the parents of (Poling, Picker, & Wallace 1983, p. 110)
two of three of the persons with ASD who did not
show a beneficial response to the drug during As we have discussed, methodological limita-
functional analysis sessions elected to continue tions also characterize recent studies of the
their children on medication after the study effects of psychotropic drugs in people with
ended. Zarcone et al. noted that “They [the par- ASD. It is easy to bemoan the shortcomings of
ents] felt that although their children continued to the research that has appeared, but it is hard to
engage in some destructive behavior, the inten- improve upon it because funding to support rele-
sity was reduced, and the medication was helpful vant studies is limited. Moreover, both practical
in reducing behaviors that were not captured by and ethical considerations limit the kind of work
the functional analysis, such as hyperactivity, that can be done. Evidence adequate to support
perseverative, and obsessional behavior” (p. 319). strong conclusions concerning the value of many
This may be the case, but it is not clear whether psychotropic drugs commonly prescribed for
risperidone actually improved aspects of the chil- people with ASD will not appear soon, if ever.
dren’s behavior not adequately captured by the Nonetheless, such drugs are routinely prescribed.
researchers’ assessments or whether the parents In the absence of such evidence, the known
believed there were improvements where none adverse effects of many medications, and the
really existed. This distinction is far from trivial availability of safer and better-documented alter-
because risperidone can produce a range of sig- native treatments, a good case can be made that
nificant adverse effects. Therefore, people who psychotropic medications are routinely overpre-
do not receive real and direct benefit from risperi- scribed for people with ASD (Matson & Konst,
done should not receive it. The same is true of all 2015; Matson & Hess, 2011).
other psychotropic medications. Early in this chapter, we discussed some of the
reasons why psychotropic drugs are so often pre-
scribed for people with ASD. The best reason, of
Concluding Comment course, is that some members of this population
derive benefits from a drug treatment that no
For more than half a century, prescribing psycho- other intervention can provide. Prescribing medi-
tropic drugs for people with developmental dis- cation is the primary tool that physicians have
abilities has been a common, and controversial, available to improve the mood, cognitive status,
practice. It remains so today, although the focus or overt behavior of people with ASD, and this
has largely shifted from the effects of such drugs tool is neither intrinsically good nor bad.
472 A. Poling et al.

Appropriate drug treatment requires that the right Toward a more rational approach. Neuroscience &
Biobehavioral Reviews, 14, 253–262.
people receive medication and that their medica-
Bertelli, M. O., Rossi, M., Keller, R., & Lassi, S. (2016).
tion regimen is managed to produce optimal Update on psychopharmacology for autism spectrum
benefit. When this occurs, a psychotropic medi- disorders. Advances in Mental Health and Intellectual
cation can provide quick, effective, and cost-effi- Disabilities, 10, 6–26.
Bethea, T. C., & Sikich, L. (2007). Early pharmacologi-
cient benefits. Ensuring that it occurs consistently
cal treatment of autism: A rationale for developmental
is a worthy goal for everyone who cares for treatment. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 521–537.
people with ASD. Broadstock, M., Doughty, C., & Eggleston, M. (2007).
Systematic review of the effectiveness of pharma-
cological treatments for adolescents and adults with
autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 11(4), 335–348.
References Buxbaum, J., & Hof, P. R. (Eds.). (2013). The neurosci-
ence of autism spectrum disorders. New York, NY:
Aman, M. G. (2002). Annotated bibliography on the Elsevier.
aberrant behavior checklist (ABC). Washington, DC: Canitano, R., & Scandurra, V. (2011).
Aman, M. G.. Psychopharmacology in autism: An update. Progress
Aman, M. G., Lam, K. S., & Collier-Crespin, A. (2003). in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological
Prevalence and patterns of use among individuals Psychiatry, 35(1), 18–28.
with autism in the Autism Society of Ohio. Journal Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2016).
of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 525–534. Autism spectrum disorder: Facts and statistics.
Aman, M. G., Singh, N. N., Stewart, A. W., & Field, C. J. Downloaded from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/
(1985). The aberrant behavior checklist: A behav- autism/data.html.
ior rating scale for assessment of treatment effects. Charman, T., Pickles, A., Simonoff, E., Chandler, S.,
American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 89, 485–491. Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2011). IQ in children with
Aman, M.G., Smith, T., Arnold, L.E., Corbett-Dick, P., autism spectrum disorders: Data from the special
Tumuluru, R., Hollway, J.A., … Handen, B. (2014). needs and autism project (SNAP). Psychological
A review of atomoxetine effects in young people with Medicine, 41, 619–627.
developmental disabilities. Research in Developmental Courtemanche, A. B., Schroeder, S. R., & Sheldon, J. B.
Disabilities, 35(6), 1412–1424. (2011). Designs and analyses of psychotropic and
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic behavioral interventions for the treatment of problem
and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed., behavior among people with intellectual and develop-
text revision). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric mental disabilities. American Journal on Intellectual
Association. and Developmental Disabilities, 116(4), 315–328.
Anderson, G. M., Scahill, I., McCracken, J. T., et al. Croen, L. A., Najjar, D. V., Ray, G. T., Lotspeich, L., &
(2007). Effects of short- and long-term risperidone Bernal, P. (2006). A comparison of health care utili-
treatment on prolactin levels in children with autism. zation and costs of children with and without autism
Biological Psychiatry, 61, 545–550. spectrum disorders in a large group-model health plan.
APA Presidential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. Pediatrics, 118(4), 1203–1211.
(2006). American Psychologist, 61, 271–285. Crosland, K. A., Zarcone, J. R., Lindauer, S. E.,
Arnold, L. E., Aman, M. G., Martin, A., Collier-Crespin, Valdovinos, M. G., Zarcone, T. J., Hellings, J. A., &
A., Vitiello, B., Tierney, E., ..., Klin, A. (2000). Schroeder, S. R. (2003). Use of functional analysis
Assessment in multisite randomized clinical trials methodology in the evaluation of medication effects.
of patients with autistic disorder: The Autism RUPP Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 33,
Network. Journal of Autism and Developmental 271–279.
Disorders, 30(2), 99–111. Danov, S. E., Tervo, R., Meyers, S., & Symons, F. J.
Bamidele, K., & Hall, I. (2013). The place of medica- (2012). Using functional analysis methodology to
tion for challenging behaviour: A whole system per- evaluate effects of an atypical antipsychotic on severe
spective. Advances in Mental Health and Intellectual problem behavior. Journal of Mental Health Research
Disabilities, 7(6), 325–332. in Intellectual Disabilities, 5(3), 286–308.
Baribeau, D. A., & Anagnostou, E. (2013). Social com- Davit, C. J., Hundley, R. J., Bacic, J. D., & Hanson, E. M.
munication is an emerging target for pharmacotherapy (2011). A pilot study to improve venipuncture compli-
in autism spectrum disorder – a review of the literature ance in children and adolescents with autism spectrum
for potential agents. Journal of the Canadian Academy disorders. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral
of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 23(1), 20–30. Pediatrics, 32(7), 521–525.
Baumeister, A. A., & Sevin, J. A. (1990). Pharmacologic Dove, D., Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Taylor, J. L.,
control of aberrant behavior in the mentally retarded: Sathe, N. A., & Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2012).
25 Medications 473

Medications for adolescents and young adults with toms in children with autism spectrum disorders:
autism spectrum disorders: A systematic review. A comprehensive review of pharmacologic and
Pediatrics, 130, 717–726. complementary-­ alternative medicine treatments.
Doyle, C. A., & McDougle, C. J. (2012). Pharmacologic Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics,
treatments for the behavioral symptoms associated 32(1), 56–68.
with autism spectrum disorders across the lifes- Institute of Medicine. (2001). Crossing the quality chasm:
pan. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(3), A new health system for the 21st century. Washington,
263–279. DC: National Academy Press.
Elbe, D., & Lalani, Z. (2012). Review of the pharmacother- Khan, A., & Brown, W. A. (2015). Antidepressants ver-
apy of irritability of autism. Journal of the Canadian sus placebo in major depression: An overview. World
Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 21(2), Psychiatry, 14(3), 294–300.
130–146. Krishnaswami, S., McPheeters, M., & Veenstra-­
Elsabbagh, M., Divan, G., Koh, Y. H., Kim, Y.S., Kauchali, Vaderweele, J. (2011). A systematic review of secre-
S., Marcín, C., … Fombonne, E. (2012). Global prev- tin with children with autism spectrum disorders.
alence of autism and other pervasive developmental Pediatrics, 127(5), 1322–1325.
disorders. Autism Research, 5, 160–179. Langworthy-Lam, K. S., Aman, M. G., & Van Bourgondien,
Farmer, C., Thurm, A., & Grant, P. (2013). M. E. (2002). Prevalence and patterns of use of psy-
Pharmacotherapy for the core symptoms in autistic choactive autism society of North Carolina. Journal
disorder: Current status of the research. Drugs, 73(4), of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 12(4),
303–314. 311–321.
Frazier, T. W. (2012). Friends not foes: Combined ris- Lecavalier, L., Kaat, A. J., & Stratis, E. A. (2014). Mental
peridone and behavior therapy for irritability. Journal health disorders in young children with autism
of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent spectrum disorders. In I. J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon,
Psychiatry, 51, 129–131. P. Sturmey, & J. L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early
Fung, L. K., Chahal, L., Libove, R. A., Bivas, R., & intervention for autism spectrum disorders (pp. 753–
Hardan, A. (2012). A retrospective review of the 780). New York, NY: Springer.
effectiveness of aripiprazole in the treatment of sen- Li, A., MacNeill, B., Curiel, H., & Poling, A. (in press).
sory abnormalities in autism. Journal of Child and Prescribing risperidone or aripiprazole with other psy-
Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 22, 245–248. chotropic drugs: A literature review. Experimental and
Gadow, K. D., & Poling, A. (1988). Pharmacotherapy Clinical Psychopharmacology.
and mental retardation. Boston, MA: College-Hill. Logan, S. L., Carpenter, L., Leslie, R. S., Garrett-Mayer,
Ghanizadeh, A. (2012). Atomoxetine for treating ADHD E., Hunt, K. J., Charles, J., & Nicholas, J. S. (2015).
symptoms in autism: A systematic review. Journal of Aberrant behaviors and co-occurring conditions as
Attention Disorders, 17(8), 635–640. predictors of psychotropic polypharmacy among
Goin-Kochel, R. P., Myers, B. J., & Mackintosh, V. H. children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
(2007). Parental reports on the use of treatments and Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 25(4),
therapies for children with autism spectrum disor- 323–336.
ders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(3), Mandell, D. S., Morales, K. H., Marcus, S. C., Stahmer,
195–209. A. C., Doshi, J., & Polsky, D. E. (2008). Psychotropic
Green, V. A., Pituch, K. A., Itchon, J., Choi, A., O’Reilly, medication use among Medicaid-enrolled children
M., & Sigafoos, J. (2006). Internet survey of treat- with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 121,
ments used by parents of children with autism. 441–448.
Research in Developmental Disabilities, 27(1), 70–84. Mason, J., & Scior, K. (2004). ‘Diagnostic overshadow-
Gringras, P. (2000). Practical paediatric psychopharmaco- ing’ amongst clinicians working with people with
logical prescribing in autism. Autism, 4(3), 229–247. intellectual disabilities in the UK. Journal of Applied
Haddad, P. M., & Sharma, S. G. (2007). Adverse effects of Research in Developmental Disabilities, 17, 85–90.
atypical antipsychotics: Differential risk and clinical Matson, J. L., Bamburg, J. W., Mayvlle, E. A., Pinkston,
implications. CNS Drugs, 21, 911–936. J., Bielecki, J., Kuhn, D., … Logan, J. R. (2000).
Higgins, J., & Green, S. (2006). Cochrane handbook Psychopharmacology and mental retardation: A 10
for systematic reviews of interventions 4.2.6, The year review (199–1998). Research in Developmental
Cochrane library (vol. 4). Chichester, UK: Wiley. Disabilities, 21, 263–296.
Huete, J., Schmidt, J., & Lopez-Arvizu, D. (2014). Matson, J. L., & Hess, J. A. (2011). Psychotropic drug
Behavioral disorders in young children with autism efficacy and side effects for persons with autism
spectrum disorder. In J. Tarbox, D. R. Dixon, spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum
P. Sturmey, & J. L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of early Disorders, 5(1), 230–236.
intervention for autism spectrum disorders (pp. 717– Matson, J. L., & Konst, M. J. (2015). Why pharmacother-
752). New York, NY: Springer. apy is overused among persons with autism spectrum
Huffman, L. C., Sutcliffe, T. L., Tanner, I. S., & disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 9,
Feldman, H. M. (2011). Management of symp- 34–37.
474 A. Poling et al.

Matson, J. L., Kozlowski, A. M., Worley, J. A., an evidenced-based approach to atypical antipsy-
Shoemaker, M. E., Sipes, M., & Horovitz, M. (2011). chotic use in children and adolescents. Journal of
What is the evidence for environmental causes of the Canadian Academy of Child and Adolescent
challenging behaviors in persons with intellectual dis- Psychiatry, 19(2), 124–137.
abilities and autism spectrum disorders? Research in Parikh, M. S., Kolevzon, A., & Hollander, E. (2008).
Developmental Disabilities, 32, 693–698. Psychopharmacology of aggression in children and
Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. (2007a). Assessing adolescents with autism: A clinical review of effi-
challenging behaviors in children with autism spec- cacy and tolerability. Journal of Child and Adolescent
trum disorders: A review. Research in Developmental Psychopharmacology, 18(2), 157–178.
Disabilities, 28, 567–579. Park, S.Y., Cerversi, C., Galling, B., Molteni, S.,
Matson, J. L., & Nebel-Schwalm, M. (2007b). Comorbid Walyzada, F., Ameis, S. H. … Correll, C.U. (2016).
psychopathology with autism spectrum disorders in Antipsychotic use trends in youth with autism spec-
children: An overview. Research in Developmental trum disorder and/or intellectual disability: A meta-­
Disabilities, 28, 341–352. analysis. Journal of the American Academy of Child
McDougle, C. J., Holmes, J. P., Carlson, D. C., Pelton, and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(6), 456–468.
G. H., Cohen, D. J., & Price, L. H. (1998). A double-­ Poling, A. (1994). Pharmacological treatment of behav-
bind, placebo-controlled study of risperidone in adults ioral problems in people with mental retardation:
with autistic disorder and other pervasive develop- Some ethical considerations. In L. J. Hayes, G. J.
mental disabilities. Archives of General Psychiatry, Hayes, S. C. Moore, & P. M. Ghezzi (Eds.), Ethical
55(7), 633–641. issues in developmental disabilities (pp. 149–177).
McDougle, C. J., Scahill, L., McCracken, J. T., Aman, Reno, NV: Context Press.
M. G., Tierney, E., Arnold, L. E., …, Walson, P. Poling, A., & Byrne, T. (2000). Principles of pharmacol-
(2000). Research units on pediatric psychopharma- ogy. In A. Poling & T. Byrne (Eds.), Introduction to
cology (RUPP) Autism Network: Background and behavior pharmacology (pp. 43–63). Oakland, CA:
rationale for an initial controlled study of risperidone. Context Press.
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North Poling, A., & Ehrhardt, K. (1999). Applied behavior
America, 9, 201–224. analysis, social validation, and the psychopharmacol-
McPheeters, M. L., Warren, Z., Sathe, N., Bruzek, ogy of mental retardation. Mental Retardation and
J. L., Krishnaswami, S., Jerome, R. N., & Veenstra-­ Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 5,
Vanderwheele, J. (2011). A systematic review of 342–347.
medical treatments for children with autism spectrum Poling, A., Ehrhardt, K., Wood, A., & Bowerman, R.
disorder. Pediatrics, 127, 1312–1321. (2010). Psychopharmacology and behavior analysis
Medeiros, K., Kozlowski, A. M., Beighley, J. S., Rojahn, in autism treatment. In E. A. Mayville & J. A. Mulick
J., & Matson, J. L. (2012). The effects of develop- (Eds.), Behavioral foundations of effective autism
mental quotient and diagnostic criteria on challenging treatment (pp. 257–275). New York, NY: Sloan.
behaviors in toddlers with developmental disabili- Poling, A., Laraway, S., Ehrhardt, K., Jennings, L., &
ties. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 33(4), Turner, L. (2004). Pharmaceutical interventions and
1110–1116. developmental disabilities. In W. L. Williams (Ed.),
Mohiuddin, S., & Ghaziuddin, M. (2013). Developmental disabilities: Etiology, assessment,
Psychopharmacology of autism spectrum disorders: A intervention, and integration (pp. 105–124). Reno,
selective review. Autism, 17(6), 645–654. NV: Context Press.
Moncrieff, J. (2015). Antidepressants: Misnamed and Poling, A., Methot, L., & LeSage, M. (1995). Fundamentals
misrepresented. World Psychiatry, 14(3), 302–303. of behavior analytic research. New York, NY: Plenum
Montori, V. M., & Guyatt, G. H. (2008). Progress in Press.
evidence-based medicine. Journal of the American Poling, A., Picker, M., & Wallace, S. (1983). Some
Medical Association, 300(15), 1814–1816. methodological characteristics of psychopharmaco-
Murray, M. L., Hsia, Y., Glaser, K., Simonoff, E., logical research with the mentally retarded. Mental
Murphy, D. G. M., Asherson, P. J., … Wong, I. (2014). Retardation and Developmental Disability Bulletin,
Pharmacological treatments prescribed to people with 11, 110–121.
autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in primary health Poling, A., Durgin, A., Bradley, K. P., Porter, L. K.,
care. Psychopharmacology, 231, 1011–1021. Van Wagner, K., Weeden, M., & Panos, J. J. (2009).
Myers, S., & Johnson, C. (2007). Management of children Females’ participation in psychopharmacology
with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 120(5), research as authors, editors, and subjects. Experimental
1162–1182. and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 17, 105–112.
National Autism Center. (2011). A parent’s guide to Preti, A., Melis, M., Siddi, S., Vellante, M., Doneddu, G.,
evidence-based practice and autism. Randolph, MA: & Fadda, R. (2014). Oxytocin and autism: A system-
National Autism Center. atic review of randomized controlled trials. Journal
Panagiotopoulos, C., Ronsley, R., Elbe, D., Davidson, J., of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 24(2),
& Smith, D. H. (2010). First do no harm: Promoting 54–68.
25 Medications 475

Reichow, B., Volkmar, F. R., & Bloch, M. H. (2013). children with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics,
Systematic review and meta-analysis of pharmaco- 132, 833–840.
logical treatment of the symptoms of attention-­deficit/ Sprague, R. L., & Werry, J. L. (1971). Methodology of
hyperactivity disorder in children with pervasive psychopharmacological studies with the retarded. In
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in
Developmental Disorders, 43, 2435–2441. mental retardation (vol. 5, pp. 147–219). New York,
Rivet, T. T., & Matson, J. L. (2011). Review of gender dif- NY: Academic.
ferences in core symptomatology in autism spectrum Thompson, T. (2007). Making sense of autism. Baltimore,
disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 5, MD: Brookes.
957–976. Thompson, T., Egli, M., Symons, F., & Delaney, D.
Rossignol, D. A., & Frye, R. E. (2014). The use of medi- (1994). Neurobehavioral mechanisms of drug action
cations approved for Alzheimer’s disease in autism in developmental disabilities. In T. Thompson & D. B.
spectrum disorder: A systematic review. Frontiers in Gray (Eds.), Destructive behavior in developmental
Pediatrics, 2, 1–8. disabilities: Diagnosis and treatment (pp. 133–188).
Roy, A., Roy, M., Deb, S., Unwin, G., & Roy, A. (2015). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Are opioid antagonists effective in attenuating the core Thompson, T., Zarcone, J., & Symons, F. (2004).
symptoms of autism spectrum conditions in children: Methodological issues in psychopharmacology for
A systematic review. Journal of Intellectual Disability individuals with intellectual and developmental dis-
Research, 59(4), 293–306. abilities. In E. Emersen, C. Hatton, T. Parmeter,
Sandler, A. D., & Bodfish, J. W. (2000). Placebo & T. Thompson (Eds.), International handbook of
effects in autism: Lessons from secretin. Journal of applied research in intellectual disabilities (pp. 549–
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(5), 580). Chichester, UK: Wiley.
347–350. Tsai, L. V. (2001). Taking the mystery out of medications
Schaal, D. W., & Hackenberg, T. (1994). Toward a func- in autism/Asperger’s syndrome. Arlington, TX: Future
tional analysis of drug treatment for behavior problems Horizons.
of people with developmental disabilities. American Tsakanikos, E., Costello, H., Holt, G., Bouras, N., Sturmey,
Journal on Mental Retardation, 99(2), 123–140. P., & Newton, T. (2006). Psychopathology in adults with
Schubart, J. R., Camacho, F., & Leslie, D. (2014). autism and intellectual disability. Journal of Autism and
Psychotropic medication trends among children and Developmental Disorders, 36(8), 1123–1129.
adolescents with autism spectrum disorder in the Tureck, K., Matson, J. L., Turygin, N., & Macmillan, K.
Medicaid program. Autism, 18(6), 631–637. (2013). Rates of psychotropic medication use in chil-
Seltzer, M. M., Shattuck, P., Abbeduto, L., & Greenberg, dren with ASD compared to presence and severity
J. S. (2004). Trajectory of development in adoles- of problem behaviors. Research in Autism Spectrum
cents and adults with autism. Mental Retardation and Disorders, 7(11), 1377–1382.
Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 10(4), U.S. Food and Drug Administration.(2006). FDA approves
234–247. the first drug to treat irritability associated with
Shaw, S. R., Bruce, J., Ouimet, T., Sharma, A., & Glaser, autism, Risperdal. Oct 6, 2006. http://www.fda.gov/
S. (2009). Young children with developmental dis- NewsEvents/Newsroom/PressAnnouncements/2006/
abilities and atypical antipsychotic medications: Dual ucm108759.htm. Accessed 27 Jan 2016.
diagnosis, direction, and debate. Journal of Early U. S. Food and Drug Administration/Center for Drug
Childhood & Infant Psychology, 5, 37–55. Evaluation and Research. (2009). Aripiprazole clinical
Sheehan, R., Hassiotis, A., Walters, K., Osborn, D., addendum. Washington, DC: Otsuka Pharmaceutical
Strydom, A., & Horsfall, L. (2015). Mental illness, Co, Ltd.
challenging behaviour, and psychotropic drug pre- Valdovinos, M. G., Napolitano, D. A., Zarcone, J. R.,
scribing in people with intellectual disability: UK pop- Hellings, J. A., Williams, D. G., & Schroeder, S. R.
ulation based cohort study. British Medical Journal, (2002). Multimodal evaluation of risperidone for destruc-
351, 1–9. tive behavior: Functional analysis, direct observations,
Shimabukuro, T. T., Grosse, S. D., & Rice, C. (2008). rating scales, and psychiatric impressions. Experimental
Medical expenditures for children with an autism and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 10, 268–275.
spectrum disorder in a privately insured population. Valdovinos, M. G., Nelson, S. M., Kuhle, J. L., & Dierks,
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, A. M. (2009). Using analogue functional analysis to
546–552. measure variations in problem behavior rate and func-
Siegel, M., & Beaulieu, A. A. (2012). Psychotropic medi- tion after psychotropic medication changes: A clinical
cations in children with autism spectrum disorders: demonstration. Journal of Mental Health Research in
A systematic review and synthesis for evidence-­ Intellectual Disabilities, 2, 279–293.
based practice. Journal of Autism and Developmental Volkow N. (2005–2008). Excerpts from the director’s
Disorders, 42(8), 1592–1605. report to the National Advisory Council on Drug
Spencer, D., Marshall, J., Post, B., Kulakodlu, M., Abuse: Women, sex/gender differences and drug
Newschaffer, C., Dennen, T., & Jain, A. (2013). abuse. Washington, DC: National Institute on Drug
Psychotropic medication use and polypharmacy in Abuse.
476 A. Poling et al.

Weeden, M., Ehrhardt, K., & Poling, A. (2010a). Williams, P. G., Woods, C., Stevenson, M., Davis, D. W.,
Conspicuous by their absence: Studies comparing and Radmacher, P., & Smith, M. (2012). Psychotropic
combining risperidone and applied behavior analy- medication use in children with autism in the
sis to reduce challenging behavior in children with Kentucky Medicaid population. Clinical Pediatrics,
autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 51, 923–927.
905–912. Witwer, A., & Lecavalier, L. (2005). Adolescents with
Weeden, M., Ehrhardt, K., & Poling, A. (2010b). autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child and
Psychotropic drug treatments for people with autism Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 15(4), 671–681.
and other developmental disabilities: A primer for Zarcone, J. R., Lindauer, S. E., Morse, P. S., Crosland,
practicing behavior analysts. Behavior Analysis in K. A., Valdovinos, M. G., McKerchar, T. L., …
Practice, 3, 4–12. Schroeder, S. R. (2004). Effects of risperidone on
West, L., Waldrop, J., & Brunssen, S. (2009). destructive behavior of persons with developmental
Pharmacologic treatment for the core deficits and disabilities: III. Functional analysis. American Journal
associated symptoms of autism in children. Journal of on Mental Retardation, 109, 310–321.
Pediatric Health Care, 23(2), 75–89. Zarcone, J., Napolitano, D., & Valdovinos, M. (2008).
Whitaker, R. (2010). Anatomy of an epidemic: Magic Measurement of problem behaviour during medica-
bullets, psychiatric drugs, and the astonishing rise of tion evaluations. Journal of Intellectual Disability
mental illness in America. New York, NY: Crown. Research, 52(12), 1015–1028.
Current Status and Future
Directions 26
Rachel L. Goldin and Johnny L. Matson

Introduction  eterogeneity and Pretreatment


H
Characteristics
The preceding chapters provide a comprehensive
overview of the many factors associated with the Response to treatment among individuals with
treatment of individuals with autism spectrum ASD is varied (Bailey, 2014; Damiano,
disorder (ASD). Current practices and service Mazefsky, White, & Dichter, 2014; Stahmer,
deliveries are discussed along with implications Schreibman, & Cunningham, 2011). Much of
for future clinical practice. As the field of ASD the heterogeneity observed in response to treat-
research advances, and our understanding of the ment can be attributed to the heterogeneity of the
disorder and co-occurring conditions improves, disorder itself. Exploring variables that predict
our practices must progress as well. It is esti- response to treatment is an important area of
mated that only 50% of individuals with ASD research. Some predictive variables identified
demonstrate substantial positive gains as a result include age, time of symptom onset, intellectual
of evidence-based interventions (Stahmer et al., quotient (IQ), language skills, and level of func-
2011). Researchers must capitalize on the recent tioning (Harris & Handleman, 2000; Perry,
discoveries about the genetics and neuroscience Blacklock, & Dunn Geier, 2013). Findings from
of ASD, along with the significant increase in the this line of inquiry suggest that children with
number of well-designed and controlled treat- typical intellectual functioning, who a milder
ment studies, to develop more effective develop- presentation, and who begin treatment by age 4
mental and behavioral interventions. The purpose respond the best to early intensive behavioral
of this chapter is to highlight directions for future intervention (Dawson, 2008; De Giacomo &
research and clinical practice. Fombonne, 1998). However, research explaining
why less able children have poorer treatment
outcomes is almost nonexistent.
In order to fill this gap in knowledge, focusing
research attention on pretreatment characteristics
To Appear In: Johnny L. Matson (Eds.), Handbook of
that influence response to treatment is recom-
Autism Treatments.
mended. Pretreatment characteristics related to
R.L. Goldin (*) • J.L. Matson
differential treatment outcomes include child
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State
University, Baton Rouge, LA, USA (e.g., age, sex, race, level of functioning), family
e-mail: rgoldi3@lsu.edu (e.g., SES, level of education, level of support),

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 477


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1_26
478 R.L. Goldin and J.L. Matson

and practitioner (e.g., training, openness, cultural 2011), continuation of treatment is needed to
sensitivity) variables (Stahmer et al., 2011). Such maintain progress made and address new devel-
findings have a multitude of implications. The opmental challenges. Skills learned in childhood
presentation of certain characteristics can be used may not be sufficient for adolescence and adult-
to guide a child into a treatment that will have the hood. For example, in childhood, treatment gen-
greatest degree of effectiveness and reduce time erally addresses social development and academic
spent on ineffective treatment. Also, it will allow skills. In adulthood, however, independence
for individualization of treatment procedures to skills and vocational training are necessary. Thus,
improve treatment outcomes, which in turn can focusing on developing treatments for all stages
impact family and child attitudes and adherence of life is critical.
to treatment. Finally, focus can be shifted to One way of tackling this would be to identify
studying the children who have the poorest important developmental milestones throughout
response to treatment and determining how treat- the lifespan and use those to guide intervention
ments may be redesigned to meet their needs. development. Intervention methods should assist
Given the heterogeneity of ASD, it is clear that an individual in learning developmentally appro-
one method of treatment will not be effective for priate skill sets. That being said, specific treat-
all. Research is needed to determine characteris- ment goals and methods should be individualized
tics that predict treatment response so that clini- according to the level of functioning and severity
cians can further individualize treatment methods of symptoms, which vary widely among individu-
to meet the specific needs of their patient an als with ASD. ASD requires lifelong management
improve prognosis. and researchers must create treatment strategies
that pertain to all stages of development.

Developmental Differences
over the Lifespan Adult-Specific Interventions

ASD is a lifelong disorder. Expectations for As discussed above, ASD persists throughout
behavior and skills change throughout develop- the lifespan. Seventy percent of individuals with
ment; thus, interventions appropriate for children ASD are under the age of 14 but are quickly
are often not appropriate for adolescents or approaching adulthood (Autism and
adults. As such, treatments must be adapted as Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network
individuals with ASD not only face changes in Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators &
societal expectations and demands (e.g., inde- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
pendent living, transition out of school, develop- 2012). Despite this, few studies to date have
ing relationships) but also experience changes in included participants above the age of 20 (Jang
their biological and mental development. et al., 2014). In a review of individuals with ASD
Symptoms of ASD often change in presentation included in ASD research, Jang et al.(2014)
and severity as an individual moves into adoles- found that only 20% of the studies included par-
cence and adulthood, requiring modification of ticipants 20 years or older. Further, in a review of
existing services and supports (Bailey, 2014; interventions for adults with ASD, only 13 of
Maglione et al., 2012). 1,217 studies identified could be classified as
Most treatments for ASD to date are devel- randomized controlled trials. Most of the other
oped and researched for children (National studies were either single-case design or nonran-
Autism Center, 2015). Though early intervention domized controlled trials (Bishop-­ Fitzpatrick,
during childhood is associated with significant Minshew, & Eack, 2013). This is problematic for
improvements in prognosis (Sallows, Graupner, many reasons, but especially because federally
& MacLean Jr, 2005; Smith & Iadarola, 2015; mandated special education services are termi-
Smith, Klorman, & Mruzek, 2015; Warren et al., nated when an individual with a disability
26  Current Status and Future Directions 479

reaches age 21, leaving them with few easily children. As more and more individuals with
accessible service options. ASD move into adulthood, treatments designed
In a review of available interventions for ASD specifically for the needs of adults will become
released by the National Autism Center, as part increasingly more important. It is imperative that
of their National Standards Project (NSP), only professionals working in this field focus on
two interventions were categorized as either developing evidence-­based treatments and sup-
established (i.e., sufficient evidence of effective- ports for adults with ASD.
ness) or emerging (i.e., some evidence of effec-
tiveness) for adults (National Autism Center,
2015). The only established interventions are Gender Discrepancy in Research
behavioral interventions, and the only emerging
interventions are vocational training interven- The majority of research on treatment effective-
tions. For comparison, 14 established and 18 ness has been conducted with male-dominated
emerging interventions were listed for children. samples. This factor may be attributed to the 4:1
Established interventions include behavioral ratio of males to females with ASD and/or to the
interventions, cognitive behavioral intervention higher estimated sex bias in individuals with
package, comprehensive behavioral treatment for higher IQs (e.g., below average IQ is often an
young children, language training (production), exclusionary criterion for participation in
modeling, natural teaching strategies, parent research studies; Fombonne, 2009; Halladay
training, peer training package, pivotal response et al., 2015). More males with higher IQs are
training, schedules, scripting, self-management, available for research, and as such, females may
social skills package, and story-based interven- be excluded to increase homogeneity of research
tion. Emerging interventions include augmenta- samples or to deal with concerns about statistical
tive and alternative communication devices, power. Since most research is male dominated,
developmental relationship-based treatment, there is limited research into possible gender dif-
exercise, exposure package, functional commu- ferences in the brain basis of ASD. This also has
nication training, imitation-based intervention, implications for the generalizability findings on
initiation training, language training (production research effectiveness (Bailey, 2014).
and understanding), massage therapy, multicom- There is a growing body of research indicating
ponent package, music therapy, picture exchange that there may be differences in the behavioral
communication system, reductive package, sign and cognitive phenotypes of females with ASD,
instruction, social communication intervention, especially those considered higher functioning.
structured teaching, technology-based interven- This knowledge further underscores the need to
tion, and theory of mind training (National focus on including more females in ASD research.
Autism Center, 2015). From this report, it is clear Males and females often face differing develop-
there is disproportionately more attention going mental and environmental challenges, and thus
into treatment for children than adults. the emphasis and implementation of treatment
Interventions designed for adults may need to may need to be different (Bailey, 2014). One way
vary from those designed for children not only in to addressing this issue would be to identify doc-
the content but also in the focus. For adults, umented differences observed between males
some important areas of treatment focus should and females with ASD. This information could
include vocational training, independent living, serve as a starting point for figuring out how
transition out of school, and sexuality (Bishop- treatments might need to be adjusted based on
Fitzpatrick et al., 2013; Damiano et al., 2014). gender. For example, a higher percentage of
When designing treatments for adults, research- females diagnosed with ASD fall on the lower
ers should take into account that the treatment end of the IQ distribution but show lower rates
decisions often may come from the adult rather of repetitive behavior (Lai et al., 2012; Mandy
than their family, as is common when treating et al., 2012; Volkmar, Szatmari, & Sparrow, 1993).
480 R.L. Goldin and J.L. Matson

On the other hand, females with ASD who fall in research samples are crucial as published studies
the average IQ range are found to exhibit better guide what services are most widely available.
functional social behavior and language abili- In order to improve the treatment of individu-
ties than male peers with similar IQs (Head, als with ASD from minority populations, research-
McGillivray, & Stokes, 2014; Lai et al., 2011). ers must better understand how minority
Halladay et al. (2015) noted that in research populations perceive, seek, and respond to treat-
conducted on populations without ASD, inherent ment. For example, when conducting research
sex differences are acknowledged, and these dif- with minority populations, community-based
ferences are seen as different baselines for com- strategies (e.g., partnering with community agen-
parison. This technique may be useful for cies) for recruitment, arranging for child care and
studying individuals with ASD. Additionally, transportation, bilingual research staff, providing
little research exists comparing females with research material in the native language of par-
ASD to those without, meaning we know little ticipants, and providing a flexible schedule are
about the influence of being a female. Gaining recommended. This has the potential to improve
this understanding is vital for further improving not only recruitment and retention but also
treatment practices, along with developing treat- improve research integrity (Brown, Fouad, Basen-
ments that address the unique challenges facing Engquist, & Tortolero-Luna, 2000; Zamora et al.,
females with ASD. Factoring gender into inter- 2016). By diversifying research samples, a better
ventions may be a means to improving treatment understanding will be gained on how multicul-
outcomes. tural issues impact treatment response and how
culture must be integrated into treatment develop-
ment and implementation (Dyches, Wilder,
Inclusion of Minorities in Research Sudweeks, Obiakor, & Algozzine, 2004).

Despite a consistent prevalence of ASD across


racial and ethnic groups, significant racial and I mpact and Management
ethnic disparities in regard to early diagnosis and of Comorbidities
access to services exist (Begeer, Bouk, Boussaid,
Terwogt, & Koot, 2009; Mandell, Listerud, Levy, The rate of co-occurring symptoms in individu-
& Pinto-Martin, 2002; Mandell et al., 2009; als with ASD is around 70% (Matson et al.,
Mandell & Novak, 2005; Zamora, Harley, Green, 2013; Simonoff et al., 2008). Despite this high
Smith, & Kipke, 2014). This may be partially rate, little research is available on interventions
attributable to the lower rates of recruitment and for comorbid symptoms in individuals with ASD
retention of minority populations in research (Damiano et al., 2014). Treating comorbidities
(Zamora, Williams, Higareda, Wheeler, & Levitt, and secondary symptoms in individuals with
2016). Zamora et al. (2016) note three common ASD can provide symptom relief, allow for more
research practices that perpetuate the exclusion targeted treatment, motivate the individual and/
of minority populations in studies: (1) exclusion or family for further treatment, and improve
of non-English-speaking participants, (2) quality of life. However, treatments that are
research samples that lack representation of effective in the typically developing population
minority populations, and (3) classification of may not produce similar outcomes in individuals
Latinos and other nonwhite participants as with ASD. For example, reduction of symptoms
“other.” Other issues that may impact research of inattention and hyperactivity in response to
participation of minority include barriers to par- stimulant medication is reported to be poorer in
ticipation (e.g., child care, travel expenses), children with ASD than peers with ADHD only.
active participation invitation, and location of This may indicate that mechanisms underlying
research site (Wendler et al., 2006). Remediating ADHD are not the same in individuals with ASD
these issues and increasing the diversity of compared to those with ADHD. When studying
26  Current Status and Future Directions 481

treatment outcomes, different neurobiological based on social communication, which is very


substrates or additional mechanisms must be context dependent, along with our limited under-
considered in the context of ASD (Bailey, 2014; standing of the pathophysiology of ASD and the
Damiano et al., 2014). In order to best study this unclear link between etiology and clinical pre-
phenomena, it has been suggested that recruit- sentation, adds to the difficulty of developing
ment be based on the target mechanism, rather pharmacological treatments for ASD (Damiano
than behavioral criteria (Damiano et al., 2014). et al., 2014). Volkmar (2001) also noted that
For instance, recruitment should focus on studying medication effectiveness in individuals
impaired emotion regulation or social process- with ASD frequently suffers from sampling
ing. This approach stems from the NIMH’s issues as a result of diagnostic uncertainty, differ-
Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) initiative. ent associated comorbidities, and differences in
RDoC aims to move away from the current prac- symptom expression.
tice of grouping observable symptoms for defin- Another reason developing pharmacological
ing diagnoses and instead developing a treatments for ASD that is so difficult is the cur-
classification system for disorders that is dimen- rent lack of animal models expressing multiple
sional and links to neurobiological systems. The characteristics of ASD. Having robust animal
application of RDoC for treatment in clinical models is a necessary starting point for the devel-
settings is far off; however, it has the potential to opment of effective and safe medications (Metz,
shape how mental health issues are categorized Mulick, & Butter, 2005). Animal models that
and diagnosed. This inevitability will have an currently exist only exhibit some of the charac-
impact on research. Regardless of whether teristics of ASD, and thus findings are not gener-
researchers pursue the RDoC initiative or con- alizable. Metz et al. (2005), for example, note
tinue using more traditional techniques, studying that no animal models address why children with
interventions for comorbid conditions will rely ASD in a typical family environment fail to attain
on sound methods for defining the target popula- language normally, because no other animal
tion and recruiting the sample. learns to talk. As such, medication use in this
population has targeted reducing stereotypies,
aggression, and self-injurious behaviors but has
Pharmacology not been found to be effective for treating the
core features of ASD (e.g., social and communi-
Currently, no drugs are approved by the Food and cation deficits). Therefore, medication may be
Drug Administration (FDA) to treat the core useful as part of a treatment plan to reduce prob-
symptoms of ASD (Damiano et al., 2014). lem behaviors so that individuals with ASD may
Risperidone and aripiprazole are the only two be more responsive to behavioral interventions
drugs approved by the FDA for use in ASD but (Metz et al. 2005; Volkmar, 2001).
both are approved only to treat irritability. The Until more is known about the neurobiology
dearth of research on drugs to treat the core of ASD, behavioral interventions should always
symptoms of ASD can be attributed to several be the primary intervention method, but medica-
factors, one being that ASD onsets in childhood. tion may be a useful adjunct treatment. As the
Studying the effectiveness of drugs in pediatric field progresses, genes that are associated with
populations is difficult due to the complex devel- ASD are being discovered and have the potential
opmental pathways, which poses a problem for to provide information on the pathogenesis of the
the development of biological treatments. In disorder (Volkmar, 2001). Though this research
addition, the heterogeneity of the disorder makes is still in the early stages, it provides better oppor-
it unlikely that one treatment will be effective to tunities for researching pharmacological treat-
all or even a majority of individuals with the dis- ments for ASD that may address the core features
order (Damiano et al., 2014). Further, diagnosis of the disorder.
482 R.L. Goldin and J.L. Matson

Conclusion effects. Additionally, most people are not trained


in identifying evidenced-based interventions and
A half century ago, ASD was underdiagnosed the principles of scientific inquiry. Research on
and considered untreatable. Major advances in ASD is progressing at a rapid rate with new lines
treatment have been made in recent years; how- of inquiry emerging through the advancement of
ever, large service gaps remain; what works for techniques for studying the brain and genes asso-
some individuals does not necessarily work for ciated with ASD. It is important that profession-
others, and many unsubstantiated treatment prac- als continue to educate themselves and follow the
tices are still occurring. Diversifying study sam- progress of the field of ASD closely. We will not
ples (e.g., race, gender, age) is a critical move forward unless we look for new and more
component of decreasing service gaps. The more effective treatments, but it is essential that new
we know about factors that affect compliance treatments be viewed critically and subjected to
with, perception of, and response to treatment of the rigors of scientific testing. It is the role of pro-
ASD, the more individual professionals can reach fessionals to guide individuals with ASD and
and help. Additionally, ASD is not just a child- their caregivers toward evidence-based treat-
hood disorder. ASD persists through the lifespan, ments that best meet the needs of that specific
and as a result, supports and services are needed individual.
as an individual ages. It is important that treat-
ment not only continue into adolescence and
adulthood but that it is modified to meet the dif- References
fering needs and expectations as a person ages.
Similarly, ASD is a heterogeneous disorder Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring
Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal
varying in symptom presentation and severity. Investigators, & Centers for Disease Control and
One treatment is not effective for all. Prevention. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum
Acknowledging the heterogeneity of ASD and disorders – Autism and Developmental Disabilities
the need for highly individualized and targeted Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008.
Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report. Surveillance
intervention is key to achieving the highest rate Summaries (Washington, D.C.: 2002), 61(3), 1–19.
of success for the most individuals. Treatment Bailey, A. (2014). Autism treatment research: Autism
methods such as applied behavioral analysis treatment research. Autism Research, 7(1), 1–3. http://
(ABA) are highly effective when individualized. doi.org/10.1002/aur.1358
Begeer, S., Bouk, S. E., Boussaid, W., Terwogt, M. M., &
With greater knowledge on how specific charac- Koot, H. M. (2009). Underdiagnosis and referral bias
teristics (e.g., IQ, language abilities, multicul- of autism in ethnic minorities. Journal of Autism and
tural issues) impact treatment outcomes, Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 142–148.
professionals will be better equipped to choose Bishop-Fitzpatrick, L., Minshew, N. J., & Eack, S. M.
(2013). A systematic review of psychosocial interven-
intervention that meet the needs of their client tions for adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal
depending on where they are developmentally of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 687–
(i.e., mentally and physically) and their level of 694. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1615-8
functioning. Brown, D. R., Fouad, M. N., Basen-Engquist, K., &
Tortolero-Luna, G. (2000). Recruitment and retention
Providing the best care possible is only achiev- of minority women in cancer screening, prevention,
able through conducting well-designed research and treatment trials. Annals of Epidemiology, 10(8
and using evidence-based treatments. Suppl), S13–S21.
Unfortunately, the field of ASD has been flooded Damiano, C. R., Mazefsky, C. A., White, S. W., &
Dichter, G. S. (2014). Future directions for research in
with unsubstantiated, controversial, and fad treat- autism Spectrum disorders. Journal of Clinical Child
ments. The nature of ASD (e.g., lifelong, variable & Adolescent Psychology, 43(5), 828–843. http://doi.
prognosis, impacts many areas functioning, org/10.1080/15374416.2014.945214
impacts the entire family) leaves caregivers very Dawson, G. (2008). Early behavioral intervention, brain
plasticity, and the prevention of autism spectrum
susceptible to unsubstantiated treatments that are ­disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 20(03),
advertised as cures or to produce immediate 775–803. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579408000370
26  Current Status and Future Directions 483

De Giacomo, A., & Fombonne, E. (1998). Parental rec- autism spectrum disorders. Mental Retardation and
ognition of developmental abnormalities in autism. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 11(2),
European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 7(3), 131– 110–115. http://doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20061
136. http://doi.org/10.1007/s007870050058 Mandell, D., Wiggins, L., Carpenter, L., Daniels, J.,
Dyches, T., Wilder, L., Sudweeks, R., Obiakor, F., DiGuiseppi, C., Durkin, M., … Kirby, R. (2009).
& Algozzine, B. (2004). Multicultural issues in Racial/ethnic disparities in the identification of chil-
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental dren with autism spectrum disorders. American
Disorders, 34(2), 211–222. http://doi.org/10.1023/B:J Journal of Public Health, 99(3), 493–498. http://doi.
ADD.0000022611.80478.73 org/10.2105/AJPH.2007.131243
Fombonne, E. (2009). Epidemiology of pervasive Mandy, W., Chilvers, R., Chowdhury, U., Salter, G.,
developmental disorders. Pediatric Research, 65(6), Seigal, A., & Skuse, D. (2012). Sex differences in
591–598. autism spectrum disorder: Evidence from a large sam-
Halladay, A. K., Bishop, S., Constantino, J. N., Daniels, ple of children and adolescents. Journal of Autism and
A. M., Koenig, K., Palmer, K., … Szatmari, P. (2015). Developmental Disorders, 42(7), 1304–1313. http://
Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum dis- doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1356-0
order: Summarizing evidence gaps and identifying Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T., LoVullo, S. V., Fodstad, J. C.,
emerging areas of priority. Molecular Autism, 6(1). Knight, C., Sevin, J. A., & Sharp, B. (2013). The mod-
http://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0019-y erating effects of intellectual development on core
Harris, S., & Handleman, J. (2000). Age and IQ at intake symptoms of autism and PDD-NOS in toddlers and
as predictors of placement for young children with infants. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34(1),
autism: A four- to six-year follow-up. Journal of 573–578. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2012.03.031
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(2), 137–142. Metz, B., Mulick, J. A., & Butter, E. M. (2005). Autism:
Head, A. M., McGillivray, J. A., & Stokes, M. A. A late-20th-century fad magnet. In J. W. Jacobson,
(2014). Gender differences in emotionality and R. M. Foxx, J. A. Mulick, R. M. Jacobson, Foxx,
sociability in children with autism spectrum dis- & J. A. Mulick (Eds.), Controversial therapies for
orders. Molecular Autism, 5(1), 19. http://doi. developmental disabilities: Fad, fashion and science
org/10.1186/2040-2392-5-19 in professional practice (pp. 237–263). Mahwah, NJ:
Jang, J., Matson, J. L., Adams, H. L., Konst, M. J., Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Retrieved
Cervantes, P. E., & Goldin, R. L. (2014). What are the from http://libezp.lib.lsu.edu/login?url=http://search.
ages of persons studied in autism research: A 20-year ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh
review. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 8(12), &AN=2004-­22398-015&site=ehost-live&scope=site
1756–1760. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.08.008 National Autism Center. (2015). Findings and conclu-
Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Pasco, G., Ruigrok, A. N. sions: National standards project, phase 2. Randolph,
V., Wheelwright, S. J., Sadek, S. A., … MRC AIMS MA: Author. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedi-
Consortium. (2011). A behavioral comparison of male rect.com/science/article/pii/S0140673613609964
and female adults with high functioning autism spec- Perry, A., Blacklock, K., & Dunn Geier, J. (2013). The
trum conditions. PloS One, 6(6), e20835. http://doi. relative importance of age and IQ as predictors of out-
org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020835 comes in Intensive Behavioral Intervention. Research
Lai, M.-C., Lombardo, M. V., Ruigrok, A. N. V., in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7(9), 1142–1150.
Chakrabarti, B., Wheelwright, S. J., Auyeung, B., … http://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2013.06.004
MRC AIMS Consortium. (2012). Cognition in males Sallows, G. O., Graupner, T. D., & MacLean, W. E., Jr.
and females with autism: Similarities and differences. (2005). Intensive behavioral treatment for children
PloS One, 7(10), e47198. http://doi.org/10.1371/jour- with autism: Four-year outcome and predictors.
nal.pone.0047198 American Journal on Mental Retardation, 110(6),
Maglione, M. A., Gans, D., Das, L., Timbie, J., Kasari, 417–438.
C., For the Technical Expert Panel, & HRSA Autism Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Charman, T., Chandler, S.,
Intervention Research – Behavioral (AIR-B) Network. Loucas, T., & Baird, G. (2008). Psychiatric disorders in
(2012). Nonmedical interventions for children with children with autism spectrum disorders: Prevalence,
ASD: Recommended guidelines and further research comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-­
needs. Pediatrics, 130(Suppl), S169–S178. http://doi. derived sample. Journal of the American Academy
org/10.1542/peds.2012-0900O of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(8), 921–929.
Mandell, D., Listerud, J., Levy, S. E., & Pinto-Martin, http://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f
J. (2002). Race differences in the age at diagno- Smith, T., & Iadarola, S. (2015). Evidence base update for
sis among Medicaid-eligible children with autism. autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Clinical Child &
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychology, 44(6), 897–922. ­http://doi.org
Adolescent Psychiatry, 41(12), 1447. http://doi. /10.1080/15374416.2015.1077448
org/10.1097/00004583-200212000-00016 Smith, T., Klorman, R., & Mruzek, D. W. (2015).
Mandell, D. S., & Novak, M. (2005). The role of cul- Predicting outcome of community-based early inten-
ture in families’ treatment decisions for children with sive behavioral intervention for children with autism.
484 R.L. Goldin and J.L. Matson

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 43(7), 1271– autism Spectrum disorders. Pediatrics, 127(5), e1303–
1282. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-015-0002-2 e1311. http://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0426
Stahmer, A. C., Schreibman, L., & Cunningham, A. B. Wendler, D., Kington, R., Madans, J., Wye, G. V., Christ-­
(2011). Toward a technology of treatment individu- Schmidt, H., Pratt, L. A., … Emanuel, E. (2006). Are
alization for young children with autism spectrum racial and ethnic minorities less willing to participate
disorders. Brain Research, 1380, 229–239. http://doi. in health research? PloS Medicine, 3(2). http://doi.
org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.043 org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0030019
Volkmar, F. R. (2001). Pharmacological interventions in Zamora, I., Harley, E. K., Green, S. A., Smith, K., & Kipke,
autism: Theoretical and practical issues. Journal of M. D. (2014). How sex of children with autism spec-
Clinical Child Psychology, 30(1), 80–87. trum disorders and access to treatment services relates
Volkmar, F. R., Szatmari, P., & Sparrow, S. S. (1993). to parental stress. Autism Research and Treatment,
Sex differences in pervasive developmental disor- 2014, e721418. http://doi.org/10.1155/2014/721418
ders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Zamora, I., Williams, M. E., Higareda, M., Wheeler,
23(4), 579–591. B. Y., & Levitt, P. (2016). Brief report: Recruitment
Warren, Z., McPheeters, M. L., Sathe, N., Foss-Feig, and retention of minority children for autism research.
J. H., Glasser, A., & Veenstra-VanderWeele, J. (2011). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(2),
A systematic review of early intensive intervention for 698–703. ­http://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2603-6
Index

A Antidepressants, 467
Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), 461 Antipsychotic drugs, 467, 469
Academic Autistic Spectrum Partnership in Research Anxiolytic substances, 381
and Education (AASPIRE) Healthcare Toolkit, APA’s ethical principles of psychologists, 42
400 Applied behavior analysis (ABA), 1, 10, 20, 42, 43, 85,
ACC systems. See Augmentative and alternative 195, 262, 331, 437, 482
communication (ACC) systems empirical demonstration of, 110
Acceptance and commitment therapy-based training field of, 109
intervention (ACTr), 100 principles of, 122
Accurate variability, 249 Applied verbal behavior (AVB), 133
Addictive disorders, 378, 379, 382–384 Aripiprazole (Abilify®), 461
cognitive disorders, 381 ASD core symptoms, 127–139
cognitive remediation, 384, 385 communication (see Communication skills)
comorbidity, 378 RRBI (see Restricted, repetitive behaviors and
definition, 377 interests (RRBI))
factors, 377 social skills
group interventions, 385 behavioral interventions, 131
loss of control, 378 BST, 131
multidisciplinary and holistic cares, 388, 389 differential reinforcement, 128
neurobiological impairments, 378 DSM-V diagnostic criteria, 127
personal recovery, 389, 390 eye contact, 127, 128
psychiatric comorbidities, 380, 381 joint attention (JA), 128, 129
psychosocial interventions MET, 131
brief interventions, 382 participants and procedures, 130
CBT, 383, 384 peer initiations, 130
motivational interviewing, 382 play, child development, 129
problem-solving strategies, 383 RFT, 131
social rehabilitation, 389 social scripting, 130
SUD (see Substance use disorder (SUD)) social stories and video modeling, 128, 130
training, early identification, 390 and training, 128
video games, 380 visual activity schedules, 130
vulnerability traits, 377 ASD interventions
Affordable Care Act (ACA), 86 acquisition and maintenance tasks, 260
Aided language stimulation (ALS), 283 child choice, 260
Alcohol Use Disorder Identification (AUDIT), 390 facilitation of professionals, 109
Alternative methods of communication. See modeling, 260
Augmentative and alternative communication motivation, 260
(ACC) natural reinforcement, 260
American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Children parent-mediated intervention, 110
With Disabilities, 416 prompts, 260
American Psychological Association (APA), 41 reinforcement, 260, 261
Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA), 320, 322 ASD research, future directions
Anecdotal assessment, 150, 151 adult specific interventions, 478, 479
Anticonvulsants, 467 comorbidities, 480, 481

© Springer International Publishing AG 2017 485


J.L. Matson (ed.), Handbook of Treatments for Autism Spectrum Disorder,
Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-3-319-61738-1
486 Index

ASD research, future directions (cont.) visual and picture-based interventions


developmental differences, lifespan, 478 choice-making, 279
evidence-based interventions, 477 classroom visual schedule, 277, 278
gender discrepancy, 479, 480 cookies, 278
genetics and neuroscience, 477 FCT, 277
heterogeneity and pre-treatment characteristics, picture symbols, 277
477, 478 portable visual activity schedules, 278
minority populations, 480 visual schedules, 277
pharmacology, 481 visual memory and visual processing, 276
professionals, 482 Autism
treatment methods, 482 children, 427, 433
unsubstantiated, controversial and fad treatments, 482 culture of, 428, 430
Asperger’s disorder, 316 diagnostic label, 428
The Aspirations Programme, 363, 364 etiology of, 428
Association for Behavior Analysis International revolutionary, 428
(ABAI), 19 Autism Curriculum Encyclopedia (ACE©), 202
Association for Positive Behavior Support (APBS), 438 Autism diagnostic observation schedule
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 102, (ADOS), 211, 220
459 Autism social skills profile (ASSP), 234
Auditory integration therapy (AIT), 31, 32 Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), 66, 109, 110, 145, 377
Augmentative and alternative communication (ACC) addiction (see Addictive disorders)
systems, 271–281 APA Ethics Code, 81
aided, 276–277 authorized representatives, 80
and ASD, 271 behavior therapy, 5–13
assessments challenging behaviors (see Challenging behaviors)
cognitive, linguistic, physical, language and vs. childhood schizophrenia, 2, 3
sensory capabilities, 274 children, 42, 45, 49, 52
communication needs, 273 chronic autistic disease, 2
complexity, 271 clients, 48, 51
direct and indirect speech and language cognitive ability, 343
assessment tools, 275 core symptoms, 48
follow-up, 276 decision-making, 80
intervention planning, 273 diagnosis, 205
language and communication, 275 DIR/floortime, 331
opportunity and access barriers, 274, 275 ethical guidelines, 81
Participation Model, 273–275 ethical issues, 42
partner interaction strategies, 273 historical context, 81
PECS, 280, 281 individuals, 42–44, 46, 48, 50, 54
referral, 273, 274 maltreatment, 81
skills, 273, 275 markers, 192, 193
team member roles, 272 parent training (see Parent training)
decision-making process, 285 professionals, 41
definitions, 133, 269, 270 psychodynamic treatments, 2, 4, 5
evidence-based treatment approaches, 286 refrigerator mother theory, 2
graphic symbols, 134 research-supported treatments, 339
hardware and software, 285 scientific knowledge, 343
high-technology, 270 social communication abilities, 43
language input, 282, 283 social deficits, 343
low-technology, 270 support and education, 3
manual signs, gestures and total communication, symptoms, 331
133, 134 treatments, 80, 153–159, 331
matrix training, 136 AVB. See Applied verbal behavior (AVB)
medium-technology, 270 Avoidant/restrictive food intake disorder (AFRID), 409
PECS, 135, 136 Ayres Sensory Integration Therapy, 334
PODD, 281, 282
pointing, 133
resources, 286 B
SGD, 134, 135, 283–285 BACB’s Model Act, 88
tablets and iPads®, 270 Baseline design, 258
VIP, 285 Behavior analysis
Index 487

BCBAs, 91 characteristics, 145, 146


behavior analysis, 85, 91 functional assessment, 150–153
consumer support, 88 functions, 149, 150
deficits and behaviors, 85 impact, 147, 148
licensing, 86–88 practical considerations, 159, 160
licensure boards, 89, 90 prevalence, 146, 147
occupational regulation, 86, 90 punishment, 156–159
political will, 87 Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), 411
professional consensus, 87 Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS), 220
reciprocity provisions, 90 Children with autism (CWA), 193
states to license, 86 Clinical Global Impression (CGI) scale, 465
Behavior Analysis Certification Board (BACB), 43, 90, Code of Federal Regulations (CFR), 61
205, 206 Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), 383, 384, 388
BCaBA credentials, 88 Cognitive enhancement therapy (CET), 365, 366
behavior analysis, 88 Cognitive remediation, 384, 385
behavior analysts, 87–89 Cognitive-behavioral training, 350–353
certificants, 88 Communication skills, 133–135
certification, 88 AAC (see Augmentative and alternative
classroom arrangements, 262 communication (AAC))
disciplinary action, 89 AVB, 133
extinction, 261 behavioral intervention principles, 132
family members, 89 learner’s vocabulary, 132
functional analysis, 261 PRT, 133
Model Act, 90 speech-language development, 132
restrictive language, 89 Competency Interview Schedule (CIS), 78
third-party payer, 89 Confidentiality, 75
Behavior therapy, 5–10 Constipation, 412
Behavioral skills training (BST), 112, 131 Criterion design, 259
Behavioral treatment Curriculum
age, 195 ASD, 256
conceptual framework, 195–198 assessments, 258–259
culmination, 195 behavior analysis, 201–203, 255
curriculum, 201–203 clients’ needs, 256
family participation, 198, 199 definition, 255
generalization, 200, 201 goals, 256
instructional format, 199, 200 ID, 257
integration, 200, 201 informal procedures, 258
intensity and duration, 203, 204 instructional methods, 260
service delivery quality, 204 intervention program, 255, 257
single-case analysis, 195 language comprehension, 256
Behaviour analysts lesson/instructional plans, 259
BCBA, 95 materials, 255
level training, 95 multiple skills, 262, 263
in multidisciplinary teams, 97 skill generalization/maintenance, 261
staff training, 97 strength cultivation, 258
BLISS framework, 244, 246 TST lesson plan, 264, 265
Board Certified Assistant Behavior Analyst (BCaBA), verbal skills, 256
46, 88, 95
Board Certified Behavior Analyst (BCBA), 43, 88,
89, 95 D
Delivery agent of intervention (DAI), 356
Department of Health and Human Services
C (DHHS), 61
Campus-Based Inclusion Model (CBIM), 324 Description of sample (DS), 356
Capacity assessments, 76, 77 Developmental, Individual-differences, Relationship-­
Capacity to Consent to Treatment Instrument (CCTI), 79 based (DIR) model. See DIR/floortime
Casein-free diet, 404 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders
CBT. See Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) (DSM), 331, 409
Challenging behaviors Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,
antecedent events, 148, 149 Third Edition (DSM-III), 3
488 Index

Diet and supplementation, 413–415, 418, 419 child’s sensory modulation, 336
adolescents/young adults, 399–400 components, 336
ASD, 397, 398 definition, 337
balanced diet, 399 impairments, 336
casein-free diet, 404 research, 337, 338
children with ASD, 408, 419 social-emotional skills, 339
clinicians’ role, 416 treatment, 338
comorbid psychopathology, 411–412 Discrete trial teaching (DTT), 26, 27, 100, 112, 198, 432
constipation, 412
dietary interventions, 401–403, 419
families role, 416 E
FDA, 407 Early and intensive behavioral intervention (EIBI), 20,
feeding and gastrointestinal, 408, 409 21, 42, 127, 129, 195–204
Feingold diet, 406 ASD, 192, 193, 222, 223
food, 398, 399 autism, 191
gastrointestinal behavioral treatment (see Behavioral treatment)
CIHA, 415 chronological age at intake, 210, 211
distress, 410–411 cognitive and adaptive skills, 220
dysfunction, 411–413 community-based treatment models, 191
federal resources, 415 discrete trial vs. naturalistic teaching, 217
indirect assessment, 413, 414 discrimination training, 216
medical assessment, 413 follow-up research, 219
risk/benefit analysis, 414–415 identification markers, 191
symptomatology, 409–411 individual differences, 209–212
therapeutic options, 414 intensity and quality, 214, 215
genetic and physiologic study, 419 intensity of treatment, 212, 213
GFCF, 401, 402 language interventions, 218, 219
Gluten-free diet, 403 in language, social and cognitive function, 209
health care practitioners, 415 length of intervention, 213, 214
individuals with ASD, 401 measures of change, 204, 205
intervention, 413–415 neurobiology, 223, 224
ketogenic diet, 402–403 outcome studies, 219
language skills, 412–413 parameters, 205
microbiome-gut brain axis (see Microbiome-gut brain parental involvement and stress, 215
axis) populations, 221, 222
nutritional status, 400–401 preschool years, 219
probiotics, 407 pretreatment variables, 210, 211
resources, 418 principles, 191
restrictive diets, 403–407 prospective studies, 191
SCD, 405 quasi-random studies, 224
scientific breakthrough, 416, 417 reinforcement-based approaches, 220
sleep disorders, 412 research, 193–195, 212
yeast-free diet, 406 social engagement, 211
Dietary considerations, 399, 400 social stimuli, 217, 218
Differential reinforcement (DR), 139 stereotyped behavior, 211
Differential reinforcement of other behavior (DRO), 158 training, 205
Differential-reinforcement-of-alternative-behavior treatment, 211, 216–219
(DRA), 155, 179, 180 treatment components, 196, 197
control over reinforcement, 176 treatment settings, 221
escape-maintained problem behavior, 178 university-managed EIBI program, 219
extinction, 180, 181 Early intervention
eye gouging, 178 early-diagnosed children, 192
noncontingent escape (NCE), 178 public school, 194
procedures, 176, 178 treatment models, 195
punishment, 181, 182 Early start Denver model (ESDM), 21, 22, 115, 116, 220
self-injurious/destructive behaviors, 177 Ecocultural family interview, 440
social negative reinforcement, 178 Emotion Regulation and Social Skills Questionnaire
DIR/floortime (ERSSQ), 234
applications, 337 Environmental enrichment (EE), 137, 138
caregivers, 339 Ethics, 44–51
Index 489

additional factors, 48 H
APA and the BACB guidelines, 43 Helsinki Declaration, 68
applied behavior analysis, 42, 43 Higher education
ASD treatments, 41, 42 cognitive potential, 312
client characteristics, 43, 44 individual with ASD, 311, 317
evidence-based treatment, 55 less-structured setting, 313
individual and family, 54 History of treatment
mass of information and misinformation, 53 autism, 1
minimal empirical support, 51–53 behavioral orientations, 2
potential effects, 54 psychodynamic theory, 1
psychopharmacology (see Psychopharmacology) Hopemont Capacity Assessment Interview (HCAI), 78
quality of the evidence, treatment, 54 Hopkins Competency Assessment Test (HCAT), 78
treatment (see Treatment)
Evaluation for the Solutions to Interpersonal Conflicts
(ESCI), 370 I
Evidence-based criteria (EBC), 356 Individualized Education Program (IEP), 257, 318
Expressive language training (ELT) group, 449 Individuals of Disabilities Education Act (IDEA), 255,
Extinction, 24, 261 312, 322
Informed consent
agreement, 69
F APA ethics code, 70
Family participation, 198, 199 ASD, 82
Feeding, 409 Belmont report, 68
Feingold diet, 406 capacity to consent, 77, 78
Floortime. See DIR/floortime caregivers, 75
Fluency deficits, 232 client rights, 69–71
Food and Drug Administration (FDA), 415 clinician competence, 73
Free public appropriate education (FAPE), 322 cognitive abilities, 68
Frontier pharmacotherapy, 467 confidentiality, 75
Functional analytic procedures, 151, 152 decision-maker, 73, 81
Functional behavior assessment (FBA), 22, 23, 101, 453 description, 74
Functional communication response (FCR). See developmental disabilities, 76
Functional communication training (FCT) ethical and effective practice, 82
Functional communication training (FCT), 117, 138, ethical and legal obligations, 81
154–156, 449 ethical principles, 69–71
benefits, 176 evolution, 67
chained schedules, 178, 182–184 factors, 81
control over reinforcement, 176 fixed and essential, 67
description, 173 guidelines, 74
extinction, 180 historical components, 68
generalization and maintenance, treatment, 176 human radiation, 68
multiple schedules, 183, 184 individuals and families, 75
punishment, 180, 181 legal obligations, 71, 72
rapid schedule thinning, multiple schedules, 184 medical and psychological treatments, 75
reinforcement schedule thinning, 182 obsolete practices, 68
resurgence, problem behavior, 185, 186 questions, 74
selection and teaching, FCR, 176, 177 radioactive chemicals, 68
treatment effects, 185 recognition and protection, 67
regulations/guidelines, 82
researchers and clinicians, 82
G risk factors, 76
Gastrointestinal symptomatology, 409, 410 risks and benefits, 73, 82
General diet supplementation, 408 strategies, 82
Gluten-free and casein-free (GFCF), 30, 31 therapeutic services, 67
Gluten-free diet, 403 treatment, 69, 76, 81
Goodness-of-Fit Survey, 443 Institutional review boards (IRBs)
Group-Based Social Competence Intervention (SCI), approval, 61
367, 368 children, 65
490 Index

Institutional review boards (IRBs) (cont.) Melatonergic agents, 467


cognitively impaired individuals, 65–66 MET. See Multiple exemplar training (MET)
composition, 62 Microbiome-gut brain axis (M-G-B axis), 418, 419
exemption, 63 Mind Reading
expedited review, 62, 63 19 ASD participants, 357
full/convened committee review, 62 24 ASD matched participants, 357
goals, 62 ASD/HFA participants, 357
history, 59, 61 emotions and mental states, 357
human fetuses, 64 faces and voices, 357
neonates, 64 social skills training, 357
pregnant women, 64 systematic software, 358
prisoners, 64–65 Mindfulness-Based Positive Behaviour Support
protections, 63, 64 (MBPBS) training, 101
reports, 63 Mindy’s problem behavior, 451
requirements, 62 The Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE), 78, 79
review committees, 61 Multimodal Anxiety and Social Skills Intervention
Intellectual disability (ID), 3, 356, 459 (MASSI) program, 364
Intellectual functioning, 210 Multiple exemplar training (MET), 131
Interpersonal problem-solving program for workplace Mymind protocol, 364, 365
adaptation, 369
Interpersonal skills, 366–370
Intervention type (IT), 345 N
Interview Skills Curriculum (ISC), 360 NAL. See Natural aided language (NAL)
National Center for Complementary and Integrative
Health (NCCIH), 415
J National Commission for Certifying Agencies (NCCA),
JASPER parent-mediated model, 114 88
The Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions (JPBI), National Professional Development Center, 17
438, 442 Natural aided language (NAL), 283
Junior Detective Training Program, 247 NCCIH urges families, 416
Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR), 137, 155–157,
175, 176
K DRA (see Differential-reinforcement-of-alternative-
Ketogenic diet, 404 behavior (DRA))
FCT (see Functional communication training (FCT))
initial schedule, 175
L motivation, 179
Language Acquisition through Motor Planning response-independent, 175
(LAMP®), 284 self-injurious behavior, 175
Latency analysis, 152 time-based, 175
Learning Experiences and Alternative Program (LEAP), Non-TPSID programs, 317
21, 432 Nuremberg Code, 41, 68
Licensing, 86–88 Nutrition, 399
Licensure, 90
BACB certificants, 88, 89
behavior analysts, 86, 87 O
health-care services, 88 Occupational licensing, 85, 86
health-care systems, 86 The Office for Civil Rights of the US Department
laws and regulations, 92 of Education, 322
occupational regulation, 86 Online game, 366
professionals, 88 Oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), 459
reimbursement, 87
requirements mirror, 88
resistance, 87 P
Licensure boards, 89, 90 Parent education, 109, 113, 118
Parent Rating of Behavior Effects of Biomedical
Interventions, 397
M Parent support, 109
MacArthur Competence Assessment Tool for Treatment Parent training, 10, 115–118
(MacCAT-T), 78, 79 active coaching, 113, 114
Maladaptive behavior, 9, 10 benefits of, 110
Index 491

child targets ecological validity, 442, 443


aberrant behavior, 118 emphasis on prevention, 448, 449
ASD diagnosis, 117 flexibility in scientific practice, 450
language, 117 history and definition, 437, 438
social skills, 118 lifespan perspective, 440, 441
demographics, 119, 120 multicomponent intervention, 447, 448
demonstration and role-play, 112 social validity, 445–447
didactic instruction, 113 stakeholder participation, 443–445
evidence-based procedure, 122 theoretical perspectives, 450, 451
instructional formats, 114, 115 Postsecondary education
measurement, 119, 120 advantage of technical/vocational programs, 316
methods, 111–114 age of 20 with ASD, 311
parent demographics, 111 assets, 312, 313
parent education, 109 childhood ASD diagnosis, 311
parent targets comorbidities, 314
discrete trial teaching, 115 considerations, 314–315
early start Denver model, 116 disclosure, 321, 322
functional communication training, 117 DSM-5’s collapsed ASD diagnosis, 316
picture exchange communication system, 116 educational- and employment-related
pivotal response training, 116 attainments, 326
procedures, 115 executive function, 314
pyramidal approach, 116 family involvement, 326
parent-mediated intervention, 109 high school students with ASD, 312
Per the Center for Disease Control and Prevention, IEP planning, 326
109 individuals with ASD, 311, 312
progressive model, 121, 122 intellectual disabilities and ASD, 316–320
research, 110, 111, 119 legal considerations, 320
social validity, 120 legally required academic supports, 322
trainers, 121 nonlegally required academic supports, 323
varieties of, 109 online classes, 316
video modeling, 112, 113 proactive support, 320
Parent’s Guide to Exploring Feeding Behavior in professional development, 325
Autism, 401 psychoeducational and vocational evaluation,
Parental Stress Index, 114 318–320
Parent-mediated intervention, 109, 110 research on school-aged children, 311
PECS®. See Picture Exchange Communication System® romantic relationships and sexuality, 324, 325
(PECS®) self-advocacy, 323, 324
Performance deficits, 232 skill building, 317, 318
Performance management social communication, 313
generalisation and maintenance, 102, 103 social supports, 324–326
Personal recovery, 389, 390 students with ASD, 325, 326
The Person-Centered Planning Process Satisfaction supports around social relationships, 324
Survey, 445 symptoms, 313–314
Pharmacology, 460, 481 theory of mind, 313, 314
Pharmacotherapy, 461, 463, 464, 467, 470 transition plans, 315
addictive disorders, 385–387 Program for the Education and Enrichment of Relational
cognitive disorders, 387 Skills (PEERS), 114, 246, 247
dual diagnosis, 387, 388 Pragmatic organization dynamic display (PODD),
Physical exercise, 159 281, 282
Picture Exchange Communication System® (PECS)®, 27, Precursor behaviors, 153
28, 101, 116, 135, 136, 280, 281 Pretest-posttest group experimental design
Pivotal behavior, 236 control-group design, 259
Pivotal response training (PRT), 27, 101, 133, 262 one-group, 259
PODD. See Pragmatic organization dynamic display Princeton Child Development Institute model, 220
(PODD) Probiotics, 407
Political will, 87 Problem behavior, 175–179, 182–186
Positive behavioral supports (PBS) approaches, 174
comprehensive lifestyle change, 438–440 consequences, 173
critical features, 438 description, 171
cultural sensitivity, 451–454 discriminative stimuli, 172
492 Index

Problem behavior (cont.) Reversal/withdraw design, 258


FCT (see Functional communication training (FCT)) Reynell receptive language test, 114
functional analysis, 171 RISPERDAL® (risperidone), 461, 462
motivating operations, 172, 173 ROME protocol, 414
NCR (see Noncontingent reinforcement (NCR)) RRBI. See Restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests
Professional Consensus, 87 (RRBI)
The Program Quality Indicators Checklist, 439
Prosocial behavior, 8, 9
Proximity, 244 S
Prsonality disorder (PD), 100 The Scales of Independent Behavior, 319
PRT. See Pivotal response training (PRT) SDARI Program, 362, 363
Psychodynamic treatments, 4, 5 Secret Agent Society program, 247, 248
Psychoeducation training (PETr), 100 Self-assessment of contextual fit, 443
Psychoeducational intervention (PEI), 114 Self-control performance deficits, 232
Psychopharmacology Self-control skills deficits, 232
adults with ASD, 49 Self-injurious behaviour (SIB), 100
guidelines, 50, 51 Self-management, 157, 158
research, 49, 50 Sensory integration
Psychotropic medications applications, 333
antipsychotic drugs, 469 ASD, 331, 332, 335
ASD and comorbidity, 463, 464 children’s behavior, 335
core symptoms of autism, 469 components, 334
diagnostic criteria, 459 definition, 332, 333
drug action, 462, 463 DSM-5, 332
drug classes, 467 effectiveness, 336
DSM-V, 459 evidence-based practices, 335
evidence-based practice, 460, 461 heterogeneity, 334
irritability, 461, 462 hypo-reactivity, 332
methylphenidate, 467 neurobiology, 335
monitoring, 469–471 occupational therapy, 332, 334
people with ASD, 459, 468 research, 334, 335
people with ID, 471 sensory-seeking behaviors, 332
pharmacological interventions, 459, 460 sensory stimuli, 332
physicians, 471 Sensory Integration Therapy (SIT), 31
practical and ethical considerations, 471 SGDs. See Speech-generating devices (SGDs)
research, 464–469 Single-function test, 152
secretin, 469 Single-subject design, 258
Skill acquisition
DTT, 26, 27
R PECS, 27, 28
Randomized controlled trial (RCT), 193 prompts, 28–30
Reciprocity provisions, 90 PRT, 27
Registered behavior technician (RBT), 46, 95, 205 Skills deficits, 232
Relational frame theory (RFT), 131 Skillstreaming, 358
The Resident Lifestyle Inventory, 439 Sleep disorders, 412
Restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests (RRBI), Social Cognition and Interaction Training for Autism
137–139 (SCIT-A), 367
antecedent treatments Social cognition interventions, 362–366
EE, 137, 138 Social cognition training, 361
embedding interests, 138 Social deficits
FCT, 138 ASD, 231, 232
NCR, 137 individuals with ASD, 240, 243
pre-session access, 138 students, 249
stimulus control, 138 Social engagement, 211, 212
teaching new skills, 137 Social reinforcement. See Problem behavior
categories, 136 Social skills
consequence-based treatments approaches, 343
DR, 139 as behaviors, 356–361
punishment procedures, 139 classification, 344
punishment procedures, 139 cognitive-behavioral training, 350–353
treatments, 136–137 criteria, 345
Index 493

discrete behaviors, 344, 346–349 behaviour analytic technology, 96


evidence-based practice, 345 BSA, 95
interpersonal skills, 366–370 complex staff skills, 103
intervention, 343, 344 in content areas, 103
levels, 345 cross-disciplinary work, 98
manualized programs, 345 didactic post-qualifying staff training, 104
meta-analysis, 345 didactic teaching, 98
online game, 366 eclectic approach, 97
problem-solving and positive reinforcement, 344 employers, 98
psychoeducational session, 368 in-service training, 103
scientific research, 345 interdisciplinary work, 98
social cognition interventions, 362–366 interventions, 97
social problem-solving process, 354, 355 multidisciplinary work, 96–98
strategies, 344 overview of, 96
structured-learning approach, 344 parent training, 97
Social skills improvement system (SSIS), 234 performance management, 98, 99, 102, 103
Social skills rating system (SRRS), 363 post-qualifying and in-service training, 96
Social skills training, 233–237 procedures, 99–101
and academic skills, 231 self-reported knowledge, 96
assessment of, 233–237 staff skills, 104
behavioral skills training, 237 staff turnover, 95
beneficial and reinforcing, 231 technology, 101, 102
BLISS, 244 traditional didactic methods, 97
categories of, 232 transdisciplinary work, 98
direct assessment well-defined staff competencies, 98
ecologically valid behavior, 236, 237 The START Program, 360, 361
frequency, 235 Stereotypy, 242
interresponse time, 235 Strength cultivation, 257
and interfering behaviors, 235 Structured interview for competency/incompetency
observational recording procedures, 235, 236 assessment testing and ranking inventory
rating scale data, 235 (SICIATRI), 79
temporal locus, 235 Substance abuse, 377, 388
evidence-based variables, 345 Substance dependency, 379
generalization, 248 Substance use disorder (SUD), 385
indirect assessment, , , , s, 233, 234 ADHD traits, 379
interventions, 237–248 CBT, 383
manualized interventions, 246 cerebral dopamine dysfunction, 378
peer-mediated intervention, 243, 246 cognitive disorders, 384
presentation, 345, 356 cognitive remediation, 384, 385
professionals, 356 development, 377
pullout training, 249 neuropsychological features, 378
secret agent society, 247 pharmacotherapy (see Pharmacotherapy)
self-management, 242 psychiatric disorder, 379, 380, 388
social deficits, 231, 232 psychosocial therapies, 382
social narratives, 240 psychostimulants, 381
superheroes social skills, 247 severity levels, 377
variability of behaviors, 249 social rehabilitation, 389
video modeling, 239 social skills, 378, 385
Specific carbohydrate diet (SCD), 405 symptoms, 379
Speech-generating devices (SGD), 134, 135 Substantiated intervention
Compass®, 284 ASD, 18
eye tracking, 285 behavioral supports, 22
and iPads®, 270, 284 data collection, 34, 35
LAMP®, 284 domains, 17
PECS®, 277 DRI, 25, 26
social visual information, 284 evidence-based, 17
VOCAs, 283 functional communication training, 26
Staff training interventions, 19, 20, 33
applied behaviour analysis, 103 meta-analyses, 18
BACB autism competency, 98 registration, 18
BCBA, 95 state and federal organizations, 18
494 Index

T adaptation, 303
TEACCH program. See Treatment and education of behavioral strategies, 303
autistic and related communication-­ cognitive benefits, 301
handicapped children (TEACCH) program comparative research, 305
Teaching interaction procedure (TIP), 112 components, 303
Temporal locus, 235 cost-effectiveness, 306
The TEACCH Transition Assessment Profile-Second demand, 305
Edition (TTAP), 319 development, 302, 306
Therapeutic horseback riding (THR), 32, 33 diagnosis, 289
Training procedures direct and indirect benefits, 303
in classroom, 99 employment
in Hong Kong, 101 comorbid conditions, 291
in-service staff, 104 databases, 305
tiered-peer, 103 rate of, 291, 292
variety of, 99 symptoms, 290
video vs. live staff, 101 factors, 289, 306
Transition Programs for Students with Intellectual heterogeneity, 303
Disabilities (TPSID), 316, 325 job coach, 303
Treatment job-based supports, 295–296
ABA programming, 44 job placement, 302
intensity and duration, 44, 45 participant selection and randomization, 301
intervention components, 46–48 primary education
setting and intervention agent, 45 benefits, 294
training and supervision, 45, 46 comorbid conditions and problem behaviors, 294
Treatment and education of autistic-related components, 294
communication-handicapped children difficulties, 293
(TEACCH) program, 262, 299, 304 discontinuation of supports, 293
applied behavior analysis-based interventions, 432, 433 factors, 294
autism, 427 IQ levels, 292
eclecticism, 428, 429 job placement, 294
elements, 429–430 job-related skills, 293
history, 427, 428 legislative efforts, 292
intervention models, 432–434 participations, 292
multinational Internet survey, 427 stereotypic behaviors, 294
organization, 430 work environment, 293
in outcome literature, 430–432 primary employment industries, 302
philosophy, 428 Project SEARCH, 300, 304
physical environment, 429 quality of life, 301
researchers and practitioners, 434 quality research design, 306
services, 427 self-sufficiency and improvements, 302
structured teaching, 428–430 severity, problem behaviors, 303
treatment, 433, 434 social and communication skills, 301
visual information, 429 social interactions, 306
work systems, 430 surveys, 289
Trial-Based Conditions, 153 TEACCH program, 299, 304
technology, 296
training methodology, 295
U work environment
UCLA PEERS Program, 359, 360 competitive employment, 298, 299, 304
USDA publishes basic dietary guidelines, 400 placement, 296
The US Department of Agriculture (USDA), 399 sheltered workshops, 297, 304
supported employment, 298, 304
Voice output communication aids (VOCAs), 283
V
The Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale, 319
Vineland Socialization Scale (VABS-S), 370 Y
Virtual reality, 361 Yeast-free diet, 406
Visual Immersion Program (VIP), 285 York Measure of Quality of Intensive Behavioural
Vocational training, adult ASD, 290–294, 296–299, 304 Intervention (YMQI), 98

You might also like