You are on page 1of 8

Neuroscience and Behavioral Physiology, Vol. 40, No.

8, 2010

Influence of Music with Different Volumes and Styles


on Recognition Activity in Humans

R. A. Pavlygina, D. S. Sakharov, UDC 612.822.3+612.821


V. I. Davydov, and A. V. Avdonkin

Translated from Zhurnal Vysshei Nervnoi Deyatel’nosti imeni I. P. Pavlova, Vol. 59, No. 5, 557–567,
September–October, 2009. Original article submitted October 27, 2008. Accepted April 28, 2009.

The efficiency of the recognition of masked visual images (Arabic numerals) increased when accompanied
by classical (62 dB) and rock music (25 dB). These changes were accompanied by increases in the coher-
ence of potentials in the frontal areas seen on recognition without music. Changes in intercenter EEG rela-
tionships correlated with the formation a dominant at the behavioral level. When loud music (85 dB) and
music of other styles was used, these changes in behavior and the EEG were not seen; however, the coher-
ence of potentials in the temporal and motor cortex of the right hemisphere increased and the latent periods
of motor reactions of the hands decreased. These results provide evidence that the “recognition” dominant is
formed when there are particular ratios of the levels of excitation in the corresponding centers, which should
be considered when there is a need to increase the efficiency of recognition activity in humans.

KEY WORDS: recognition of masked visual images, EEG, music.

Ukhtomskii regarded the dominant as one of the main A number of studies have been performed [7–9, 11] in
principles of intercenter interactions [18]. The formation of a which a stationary focus of excitation was created in the
stable focus of excitation in the CNS leads to rearrangements CNS using a model based on the recognition of masked
in intercenter relationships such that the focus dominant at a visual images (Arabic numerals) with enhancement of the
given moment in time is enhanced on activation of other ongoing dominant state using music. Recognition during
structures not directly related to the ongoing activity. Studies music at a particular volume led to increased recognition
of the properties of dominants in animals [1, 12–14] and efficiency.
humans [4, 10] have used the light and sound stimuli which Ukhtomskii held that on establishment of intercenter
are standard in experimental conditions for activating the sen- interactions on the principle of the dominant, the ratio of the
sory systems as the stimuli inducing dominant foci. When the levels of excitation in these centers was of great importance.
sensory stimulus used was altered in frequency or intensity, If excitation in the center testing the dominant focus
its effects on the dominant focus were enhanced. This led to increased strongly, the focus could be inhibited. The present
the idea of using music to enhance existing dominant states, study is a logical continuation of these studies. The aim was
as it can be regarded as a stimulus having the most appropri- to compare behavioral and electroencephalographic
ate action on the brain because both musical structure and changes occurring in relation to the volume and style of the
brain electrical activity are non-stationary and polymor- music heard.
phous. The use of music in studies of the dominant in humans
is appropriate because it is experienced in everyday life and,
when positive tests are obtained, is recommended for increas- METHODS
ing the efficiency of many types of work activity.
A total of 19 healthy right-handed subjects aged 18–47
Institute of Higher Nervous Activity and Neurophysiology, Russian years, of both genders, with normal hearing, who were not
Academy of Sciences, Moscow; e-mail: sakharovdm@yandex.ru. professional musicians, took part in the study. In all cases,

877
0097-0549/10/4008-0877 ©2010 Springer Science+Business Media, Inc.
878 Pavlygina, Sakharov, Davydov, and Avdonkin

TABLE 1. Proportions of Experiments (%) in which P and T Changed in Different Environmental Conditions

Types of P T
changes in
P and T w wk1 wk3 wr1 wr3 w wk1 wk3 wr1 wr3

Increase 45 50 67 67 33 55 40 26 33 20

Decrease 45 45 33 33 54 40 55 68 67 67

No change 10 5 0 0 13 5 5 6 0 13

Notes. P is the proportion of correctly recognized stimuli (%); T is the mean decision-taking time, msec; w is work without music; wk1, wk3, wr1, and wr3
are work accompanied by classical or rock music at volume levels 1 and 3.

the spontaneous EEG contained an alpha rhythm with dif- 85 dB above the hearing threshold. The method using music
ferent strengths. The subjects’ eyes were open throughout has been described in detail in a previous report [15].
the experiment. EEG recordings were made using 16 leads distributed
The studies were performed in two stages: a prepara- using the standard 10–20% system. A total of 170 experi-
tive stage (subjects were trained to the motor skill and ments were performed. Mean EEG coherence (Coh) values
image recognition) and an experimental period (recognition were analyzed using a fast Fourier transformation in standard
with EEG recording). The masked visual images (MVI) physiological frequency ranges: Δ = 1–4 Hz, θ = 4.25–8 Hz,
consisted of the numerals “5,” “6,” “8,” and “9” presented α1 = 8.25–10 Hz, α2 = 10.25–13 Hz, β1 = 13.25–20 Hz,
as illuminated dots on the screen, along with so-called sham β2 = 20.25–30 Hz, and γ = 30.25–40 Hz. Artifact-free EEG
signals consisting of randomly illuminated dots. The mask- segments were processed in MATLAB 5.2. The analysis
ing process consisted of inactivating a number of the points epoch was 4 sec and the sampling frequency was 256 Hz.
making up the image of the numeral or activating a number Changes in Coh were assessed in 56 intrahemisphere pairs.
of additional points on the screen. The masking level was Only significant changes in EEG Coh were assessed
12%. Subjects had to respond to all stimuli by pressing the (p < 0.05) for each subject using the Mann–Whitney test
corresponding keys with the left hand in response to the and for the whole group using Student’s test. Differences in
numerals “5” and “6” and with the right hand in response to integral coherence levels using music of different styles and
the numerals “8” and “9;” the space bar had to be pressed in volumes were assessed using the χ2 test.
response to sham stimuli. The duration of image presenta-
tion was 200 msec. The mean interstimulus interval was
8–10 sec. In each experiment, the subject was presented RESULTS
with an average of 95 stimuli. The baseline EEG was
recorded during the first 10 min. Subjects then recognized Assessment of MVI recognition efficiency with musi-
masked images for 12.5 min and continued during the next cal accompaniment identified changes in the proportion of
12.5 min with music playing at a selected volume and style. correctly recognized images (P) as compared with the first
Each experiment used a single type of musical accompani- half of the experiment, when recognition took place without
ment. Breaks between experiments lasted 3–7 days. Each music; changes in the mean decision-taking time through
subject took part in one or two control experiments, where the experiment (T) were also assessed.
they continued to recognize visual images without music MVI recognition efficiency changed with musical
during the second half. The efficiency of recognition activ- accompaniment. The nature of the changes depended on the
ity was evaluated using the paired Wilcoxon test. style of the music and its loudness, and individual variations
Rock music consisted of recordings by the Rolling were seen. Table 1 shows that in most experiments (67%),
Stones at two volume levels, music fragments having mean efficiency in terms of the proportion of correctly recognized
intensities of: r1 = 25 dB and r3 = 62 dB above the hearing stimuli (P) increased on recognition accompanied by music
threshold. Classical music was by Mozart, also at two vol- k3 and k1, while no such tendency was seen when subjects
ume levels: k1 = 35 dB and k3 = 62 dB. It should be noted worked with music k1 or r3. When recognition continued in
that professional musicians regard music at volumes of up the second half without music, there were no differences
to 70 dB as quiet. In addition, a series of experiments between the numbers of experiments in which recognition
involving seven subjects using louder music was also per- efficiency increased or decreased (Table 1, w). This type of
formed: piano concertos by Mozart (k4) and Schnittke (a4; change in P was seen on recognition while music k1 was
avant garde) and music by the Rolling Stones (r4) and the playing. Working with music always showed a predomi-
Russian group Ariya (h4, hard rock) with a mean volume of nance of experiments in which the decision time decreased.
Influence of Music with Different Volumes and Styles on Recognition Activity in Humans 879

Fig. 1. Changes in stimulus recognition efficiency with quiet music. P is the proportion of correctly recognized stimuli (%); T is the
decision-taking time. White columns (w) show recognition without music; dark columns show recognition with classical (wk3, 62 dB)
or rock (wr1, 25 dB) music. Significant changes in efficiency: *p < 0.005, **p < 0.0015.

Fig. 2. Changes in intrahemisphere Coh of potentials on recognition of masked visual images. A) Positions of electrodes on subjects’ heads, 10–20%
scheme, used for plotting maps. B) Maps of changes, where continuous lines show significant increases and dotted lines show significant decreased
in Coh functions compared with baseline. α2, β1, β2, and γ are frequency ranges. C) Overall (all leads and frequency ranges) numbers of cases with
significant increases (dark columns) and decreases (light columns) in Coh in the left (LH) and right (RH) hemispheres during image recognition com-
pared with baseline. The vertical axis shows the proportions with increases or decreases in Coh as percentages of the total number of possible
changes. 95% confidence intervals are shown.
880 Pavlygina, Sakharov, Davydov, and Avdonkin

Fig. 3. Maps of changes in intrahemisphere Coh of potentials during recognition of masked visual images accompanied by clas-
sical music at 62 dB. For further details see caption to Fig. 2, B.

TABLE 2. Numbers of Intrahemisphere Pairs with Increased Coh during Recognition of MVI Without and With Music Compared with Baseline

Conditions LH RH LH + RH

w 97 63 160

wk1 94 70 164

wk3 106 89 195*

wr1 115 103 218**

wr3 87 66 153

Notes. LH = left hemisphere; RH = right hemisphere. Experiment types: recognition without music (w); recognition with music wk1, wk3, wr1, wr3.
Significantly different changes compared with baseline: *p = 0.023; **p = 0.0015.

Figure 1 shows data on changes in MVI recognition sure to music [6]. It should be noted that recognition never
efficiency with music k3 and r1. Significant differences were increased Coh between potentials in the temporal and frontal
identified using the paired Wilcoxon test. The proportion of areas of the cortex of the right hemisphere (Fp2–T4).
correctly recognized stimuli increased by an average of Figure 2, B shows overall data for all leads and frequen-
6.7% (n = 38, p = 0.0015) with music k3. Rock music cy ranges in terms of the numbers of cases of significant
increased recognition efficiency by 4.2% (n = 38, p = 0.005). increases (dark columns) and decreases (light columns) in
A significant decrease in reaction time, by an average of Coh in the left and right hemispheres as compared with
70 msec, was seen with music k3 (n = 38, p = 0.0015) and baseline (χ2, n= 72, p < 0.05). The data provide evidence
r1 (n = 38, p = 0.005). that recognition forms a left-sided asymmetry. The number
Recognition of masked visual stimuli produced a par- of cases with increases in combined potentials in the left
ticular structure of coherence connections: Coh of potentials hemisphere was significantly greater than the number with
(in relation to baseline) increased in the frontal areas of the increases in the right hemisphere.
cortex, particularly in the left hemisphere, between all Recognition of images accompanied by music
recording points (Fig. 2, A, Student’s test, n = 72, p < 0.05). increased the extent of the main pattern of Coh connections
This was particularly marked in the α2, β1, β2, and γ fre- formed on recognition without musical accompaniment
quency ranges. In addition, there were increases in Coh of (Fig. 3, Student’s test, n = 72, p < 0.05). It should be noted
potentials in the frontal and occipital areas of the cortex in that music increased Coh between potentials in the frontal
both hemispheres. These changes were accompanied by and temporal areas of the right hemisphere (Fp2–T4) in the
decreases in the connectedness of potentials in the temporal- α2, β1, β2, and γ ranges which had been absent on recogni-
central-occipital areas. As the structure of Coh connections tion without music (compare Fig. 2 and Fig. 3). With the
on recognition of MVI was topographically similar in all fre- aim of obtaining a clearer view of the contribution of music
quency ranges, only changes in the high-frequency oscilla- to the EEG pattern as compared with working without
tion bands (10–35 Hz) are plotted; as shown by the results of music, the numbers of intrahemisphere pairs with increased
previous experiments, these are the most reactive to expo- levels of connectedness were analyzed in comparison with
Influence of Music with Different Volumes and Styles on Recognition Activity in Humans 881

Fig. 4. Change in the efficiency of recognition of masked visual images accompanied by loud (85 dB) classical
music, rock music, hard rock music, and avant garde symphonic music (total, wm). Significant changes in effi-
ciency: *p = 0.002. For further details see caption to Fig. 1.

baseline in conditions of isolated recognition of stimuli and (C4–T4). As during recognition subjects responded to MVI
on recognition accompanied by music (Table 2). In all alternately with the left and right hands, differential analy-
recognition conditions, the number of pairs with increased sis of the movement latencies of both hands was performed.
biopotentials Coh levels was greater in the left hemisphere As shown in Fig. 6, most experiments showed reductions in
than in the right. As changes occurred in both hemispheres, recognition latency with the left hand (paired Wilcoxon test,
Table 2 shows the total numbers of pairs with increased Coh n = 15, p = 0.046).
in the left and right hemispheres (LH + RH). The number of
pairs with increased Coh levels was significantly greater
during work accompanied by music k3 (χ2 test, n = 392, DISCUSSION
p = 0.023) and r1 (χ2 test, n = 392, p = 0.0015) as compared
with working without music. Music of the same volume The literature contains reports of studies providing evi-
was effective for MVI recognition (Table 1). dence that hearing music influences the cognitive activity of
Louder music was used in the same experimental con- the human brain. Some cases showed positive effects. Thus,
ditions and the same methodology. No changes in the per- music promotes solution of temporospatial tasks in tests with
centage of correct responses were seen. However, there was mental rotation or recognition of mirror images [24, 28]. Most
a reduction in latent periods (T) by an average of 65 msec schoolchildren prefer to study with the radio or television on
(paired Wilcoxon test, n = 28, p = 0.002). Figure 4 shows [27]. Music improves measures of verbal memory [20]. We
overall data for all subjects and all types of music used. believe that studies in this direction are often constructed
The structure of Coh connections showed no increase methodologically on the principle of the dominant. However,
in Coh between the frontal and temporal cortex in the right unfortunately, the authors use only qualitative analysis of
hemisphere – Fp2–T4 (Fig. 5, A, Student’s test, n = 28, brain states on the basis of tests and verbal responses, with-
p < 0.05). There was also no asymmetry in the number of out EEG studies. Results obtained using music of different
Coh connections in the left and right hemispheres (Fig. 5, B), volumes and styles have not been reported. We believe that
i.e., the changes in Coh seen with quieter music and leading people have long used music as a factor enhancing ongoing
to increases in recognition efficiency, did not occur. Loud dominant states. Music is used before military engagements
music produced an increase in Coh between potentials in to promote the fighting spirit of the combatants and to inten-
the temporal and central cortex of the right hemisphere sify religious feelings during services in temples. A report in
882 Pavlygina, Sakharov, Davydov, and Avdonkin

Fig. 5. Changes in intrahemisphere Coh of potentials during recognition of masked visual images accompanied by loud (85 dB)
music of different styles (overall). A) Maps of changes. B) Total (all leads and frequency ranges) numbers of cases with signifi-
cant increases or decreases in Coh in the left and right hemispheres (compared to baseline) on recognition of images accompa-
nied by loud classical (wk4) and rock (wr4) music. For further details see caption to Fig. 2, B, C.

the German periodical Journal of Marketing noted that peo- performance of various types of cognitive activity [3, 5, 19,
ple’s purchasing tendency increases by 65% if there is music 25]. Asymmetry in the coherence of potentials in the frontal
in the shop [23]. areas of the cortex may provide further support for the view
We interpret our data showing that music of a certain vol- that the left hemisphere is more involved in the analytical
ume and style increased recognition efficiency as a manifesta- process of decision-taking than the right [16]. Increases in
tion of the mechanism of the dominant, whose main property the connectedness of potentials in the frontal-occipital areas
is summation. In our experiments, music was not presented are probably a reflection of decision-taking in relation to
before testing, when it could significantly alter the initial state visual images. The greatest increase in Coh in the high-fre-
of the CNS and influence the results of subsequent work. quency band appears to be related to the fact that recogni-
During the initial 2–3 experiments, the subjects learned to rec- tion reflects brain cognitive activity characterized by these
ognize the images. In the main experiment, the subjects again particular frequencies [2, 17, 22, 29].
had to recognize 95 stimuli, and it was only after this that The main spatial structure of Coh connections of
recognition with music was trialed. The experimental condi- potentials in the neocortex arising on recognition persisted
tions promoted the formation of a stable focus of excitation in when music enhancing the dominant at the moment of the
brain structures associated with recognition activity, music activity was used. Image recognition with music of a par-
only then being used. A certain level of activation of the audi- ticular volume strengthened the main pattern of Coh con-
tory analyzer is required for irradiation of excitation to other nections seen during work without music. Music alone
brain structures, including those in which the excitation focus leads to a right-sided asymmetry in people who are not
was created, to occur. Summation of excitation takes place, musicians [6, 15, 21, 30]. Results from fMRI studies have
one measure of which is an increase in ongoing activity. demonstrated that music in “non-musicians” is addressed to
We will consider the data obtained from analysis of the the right temporal-occipital area of the cortex, i.e., the
spatial organization of neocortical potentials in different “musical brain” [26]. The left-sided asymmetry characteris-
states in humans. Recognition of masked visual images pro- tic of recognition processes persisted and increased during
duced a significant increase (compared with baseline) in recognition accompanied by music. Significant changes
Coh values between potentials in the frontal area and were seen on recognition accompanied by classical (62 dB)
between potentials in the frontal and occipital zones of both and rock (25 dB) music, which correlated with the forma-
hemispheres. Left-sided asymmetry in the connectedness of tion of a dominant at the behavioral level. Music with these
potentials in the frontal cortex was noted. These changes same characteristics increased image recognition efficiency.
were more marked in the high-frequency ranges. Activation The ratio of the levels of excitation in two centers
of the frontal areas of the cortex is known to occur during between which integrative connections are established on
Influence of Music with Different Volumes and Styles on Recognition Activity in Humans 883

motor cortex. We take the view that changes in reaction


time when loud music was used were not associated with
the recognition process but reflect the influences of music
on the motor centers. Increased motor activity is seen in
people when loud music is played in discotheques.

CONCLUSIONS

1. Recognition of masked visual images (Arabic


numerals) by humans showed increased probabilities of
correct recognition and decreases in decision-making times
in conditions in which work was accompanied by classical
(62 dB) or rock (mean 25 dB) music.
2. Increases in the coherence of potentials in the
frontal areas of the cortex, particularly marked in the left
hemisphere, were seen during recognition of masked visual
images, along with increases in the connectedness of poten-
tials in the frontal-occipital areas. This structure of coher-
ence connections of potentials was more marked in the
high-frequency EEG band. Coherence decreased in the tem-
poral-central-occipital areas in both hemispheres.
3. The extent of the main pattern of coherence connec-
tions formed on recognition without musical accompaniment
Fig. 6. Proportion (%) of experiments in which recognition accompanied increased during recognition of masked visual images with
by loud music decreased the latencies of motor responses of the left (LH)
and right (RH) hands. 95% confidence intervals are shown.
musical accompaniment (classical music at 62 dB, rock
music at a mean 25 dB). In addition, there was an increase in
the connectedness of potentials in the frontal and temporal
areas of the right hemisphere in the high-frequency ranges.
the principle of the dominant is important. If excitation in 4. When recognition was accompanied by classical
the center testing the dominant focus increases strongly, the music at 62 dB or rock music at a mean 25 dB, intercenter
focus can be inhibited [18]. Working from this proposition, EEG ratios correlated with the formation of a dominant at
we performed a series of experiments using louder music – the behavioral level.
at 85 dB. Apart from classical and rock music, hard rock 5. When music of other styles and high volume was
and avant garde symphonic music was also used. This used (classical, rock, hard rock, avant garde symphonic at
music did not lead to increases in correct responses; there a mean 85 dB), there were no changes in the proportions of
was only a reduction in motor reaction times. The structure correct responses. The only significant changes consisted
of coherence connections lacked any increase in Coh of reductions in the latencies of motor reactions, especial-
between potentials in the frontal areas of the cortex; left- ly of the left hand. Music at this volume led to increases in
sided asymmetry in the numbers of coherence connections coherence between the temporal and central cortex in the
in the left and right hemispheres was also not seen. Thus, right hemisphere.
those changes in Coh seen when work was accompanied by 6. Comparison of the results obtained from recognition
quieter music and led to an increase in the proportion of with music of different volumes and styles leads to the con-
correct recognition trials were absent. It was characteristic clusion that the “recognition” dominant forms at a particu-
that in the absence of changes in Coh in the frontal cortex, lar ratio of levels of excitation in the corresponding centers,
there were increases in Coh between potentials in the tem- which must be considered when there is a need to increase
poral and motor cortex of the right hemisphere. Music in the efficiency of recognition activity in humans.
this case appeared to affect the motor area of the cortex, This study was supported by the Russian Humanities
thus decreasing that motor reaction time to stimuli. As sub- Scientific Foundation (Grant No. 08-06-00150a).
jects had to respond with their right and then left hands
alternately during recognition, we performed a separate
analysis of latency for each hand. All subjects were right- REFERENCES
handed; latency of left hand movements showed greater
changes on hearing music (decreases in latency), which is 1. A. V. Bogdanov and A. G. Galashina, “Time distribution of the
consistent with the increase in Coh in the right temporal- linked spike activity of neurons in the sensorimotor cortex in rabbits
884 Pavlygina, Sakharov, Davydov, and Avdonkin

with a rhythmic motor dominant,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 48, No. 4, 16. P. V. Simonov, The Emotional Brain [in Russian], Nauka, Moscow
630–639 (1998). (1981).
2. N. N. Danilova, “Microstructural analysis of the gamma rhythm as a 17. V. B. Strelets, Zh. V. Garakh, V. Yu. Novototskii-Vlasov, and
method for studying cognitive processes,” in: Questions in Neurocy- R. A. Magomedov, “Relationship between the power and synchro-
bernetics [in Russian], OOO “TsVVR” Press, Rostov-on-Don nization of EEG rhythms in normal conditions and cognitive pathol-
(2005), Vol. 1, pp. 16–18. ogy,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 55, No. 4, 496–504 (2005).
3. E. A. Kostandov, The Psychophysiology of Consciousness and 18. A. A. Ukhtomskii, Collected Works [in Russian], Leningrad State
Unconsciousness [in Russian], Piter, St. Petersburg (2004). University, Leningrad (1950), Vol. 1.
4. N. N. Lebedeva, R. A. Pavlygina, and V. I. Davydov, “Studies of a 19. D. A. Farber and N. V. Dubrovinskaya, “Formation of neurophysio-
motor dominant in humans,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 41, No. 4, logical functions in ontogeny. Mechanisms of human brain activity,”
647–654 (1991). in: Human Neurophysiology [in Russian], Nauka, Leningrad (1988),
5. R. I. Machinskaya, N. O. Machinskii, and E. I. Deryugina, “Functional pp. 426–455.
organization of the right and left hemispheres of the human brain in 20. S. Agnes, Vim-Chi Ho, and Mei-Chun Cheung, “Music training
directed attention,” Fiziol. Cheloveka, 18, No. 6, 77–85 (1992). improves verbal memory,” Nature, 398, 128 (1998).
6. R. I. Pavlygina, V. I. Davydov, A. V. Sulimov, Yu. V. Lyubimova, and 21. M. Besson, F. Faita, and J. Requin, “Brain waves associated with
D. S. Sakharov, “Analysis of EEG coherence on listening to music,” musical incongruities differ for musicians and non-musicians,”
Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 53, No. 4, 402–409 (2003). Neurosci. Lett., 168, No. 1–2, 101–105 (1994).
7. R. A. Pavlygina, D. S. Sakharov, and V. I. Davydov, “Interhemisphere 22. D. Giannitrapani, “Scanning mechanisms and the EEG,” EEG Clin.
EEG interactions on recognition of masked visual images accompa- Neurophysiol., 30, No. 2, 139–146 (1971).
nied by music,” Fiziol. Cheloveka, 34, No. 4, 11–18 (2008). 23. G. J. Gorn, “The effects of music in advertising in choice behavior.
8. R. A. Pavlygina, D. S. Sakharov, and V. I. Davydov, “Spectral EEG A classical conditioning approach,” J. Marketing, 46, No. 1–2,
analysis in humans on listening to musical performances,” Fiziol. 94–101 (1982).
Cheloveka, 30, No. 1, 62–69 (2004). 24. N. Jausovec and K. Habe, “The influence of Mozart’s sonata K.448
9. R. A. Pavlygina, D. S. Sakharov, and V. I. Davydov, “EEG charac- on brain activity during the performance of spatial rotation and
teristics determining the quality of subsequent recognition of visual numerical tasks,” Brain Topogr., 17, No. 4, 207–218 (2005).
images,” Fiziol. Cheloveka, 33, No. 1, 34–40 (2007). 25. M. M. Mesulam, Principles of Behavioral Neurology, Davis Co.,
10. R. A. Pavlygina, A. V. Sulimov, and L. A. Zhavoronkova, “Interhemi- Philadelphia (1985).
sphere EEG relationships in a hunger dominant in humans (analysis 26. T. Nakada, Y. Fujii, K. Suzuki, and I. L. Kwee, “‘Musical brain’
of coherence), Dokl. Ros. Akad. Nauk., 338, No. 6, 833–835 (1994). revealed by high-field (3 Tesla) functional MRI,” Neuroreport, 9,
11. R. A. Pavlygina, M. V. Frolov, V. I. Davydov, G. B. Milovanova, and 3853–3856 (1998).
A. V. Sulimov, “Recognition of visual images in a rich sensory envi- 27. J. E. Patton, D. K. Routh, and T. A. Stinard, “Where do children
ronment: musical accompaniment,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. Deyat., 48, study? Behavioral observations,” Bull. Psychonom. Soc., 24, No. 6,
No. 1, 19–29 (1998). 439–440 (1986).
12. G. Ya. Roshchina, “Intercenter relationships between brain biopo- 28. F. H. Rauscher, G. L. Shaw, and K. N. Ky, “Listening to Mozart
tentials in rabbits on creation of a blink dominant,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. enhances spatial-temporal reasoning: towards a neurophysiological
Deyat., 41, No. 6, 1186–1192 (1991). basis,” Neurosci. Lett., 185, No. 1, 44–47 (1995).
13. V. S. Rusinov, The Dominant. Electrophysiological Studies [in Russian], 29. W. J. Ray and H. W. Cole, “EEG activity during cognitive process-
Meditsina, Moscow (1969). ing: influence of attentional factors,” Int. J. Psychophysiol., 3, No. 1,
14. E. V. Rusinov, “Intercenter relationships between electrical process- 43–48 (1985).
es in the rabbit brain and a polarization dominant,” Zh. Vyssh. Nerv. 30. M. I. Tervaniemi, S. V. Medvedev, K. Alho, S. V. Pakhomov,
Deyat., 36, No. 1, 133–139 (1986). M. S. Roudas, T. L. Van Zuijen, and R. Näätänen, “Lateralized auto-
15. D. S. Sakharov, V. I. Davydov, and R. A. Pavlygina, “Intercenter EEG matic auditory processing of phonetic versus musical information: a
relationships in humans on listening to music,” Fiziol. Cheloveka, PET study,” Hum. Brain Mapping, 10, No. 1, 74–79 (2000).
31, No. 4, 27–32 (2005).

You might also like