You are on page 1of 9

Death Studies

ISSN: 0748-1187 (Print) 1091-7683 (Online) Journal homepage: https://www.tandfonline.com/loi/udst20

Grit and Perseverance in Suicidal Behavior and


Non-Suicidal Self-Injury

Michael D. Anestis & Edward A. Selby

To cite this article: Michael D. Anestis & Edward A. Selby (2015) Grit and Perseverance
in Suicidal Behavior and Non-Suicidal Self-Injury, Death Studies, 39:4, 211-218, DOI:
10.1080/07481187.2014.946629

To link to this article: https://doi.org/10.1080/07481187.2014.946629

Accepted author version posted online: 22


Jan 2015.
Published online: 25 Mar 2015.

Submit your article to this journal

Article views: 622

View Crossmark data

Citing articles: 8 View citing articles

Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at


https://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=udst20
Death Studies, 39: 211–218, 2015
Copyright # Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 0748-1187 print=1091-7683 online
DOI: 10.1080/07481187.2014.946629

Grit and Perseverance in Suicidal Behavior and


Non-Suicidal Self-Injury
Michael D. Anestis
Department of Psychology, University of Southern Mississippi, Hattiesburg, Mississippi, USA

Edward A. Selby
Department of Psychology, Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey, Piscataway,
New Jersey, USA

Data indicate persistence facilitates suicidal behavior. Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI)


is characterized by difficulty persisting while upset. The relationship between NSSI
and suicidal behavior thus may hinge upon persistence. Participants were 604
undergraduates (79.5% women; 42.4% African American; 6.1% with 1 þ prior suicide
attempt). Data were collected online via self-report and analyzed using hierarchical
multiple regression. As expected, higher levels of grit and perseverance predicted
more frequent suicide attempts. Furthermore, grit and perseverance moderated
the relationship between NSSI and suicide attempts, which increased in magnitude as
individuals reported greater persistence. Findings depict suicidal behavior as a deliberate
pursuit of death.

Suicide is the 10th leading cause of death in the United et al., 2010) views suicidal behavior as something
States, with approximately 38,000 individuals engaging individuals must develop the capacity to engage in over
in lethal self-harm each year (Centers for Disease time. The IPTS posits that suicidal behavior is both phy-
Control and Prevention, 2013). The obvious need for sically painful and emotionally distressing, and that
a better understanding of this phenomenon has prompted most individuals who experience suicidal thoughts have
the development of several prominent theoretical not developed the ability to engage in lethal self-harm
frameworks through which researchers and clinicians because they have not yet developed the capacity to
conceptualize risk and develop prevention protocols. overcome such sensations. From this perspective, suici-
One common factor amongst several theoretical dal behavior can thus be seen as a planned effort to pur-
frameworks is the notion that suicidal behavior emerges sue death despite the need to confront fear, pain, and
suddenly and impulsively, potentially serving as an escape mortality that deter most from such behavior. This per-
from momentary aversive states (Baumeister, 1990; spective is inconsistent with escape models of suicide,
Chatard & Selimbegoic, 2011; Mann, Waternaux, Haas, which posit that suicide attempts arise from an inability
& Malone, 1999). From this perspective, suicidal behavior to cope with negative emotion and distress. Thus, escape
can be seen as an inability to persist in life amidst distress. theories may be fundamentally incompatible with sui-
Not all theoretical frameworks share this perception, cide attempts, as such behavior requires individuals to
however. Indeed, the interpersonal-psychological theory confront discomfort rather than immediately escape
of suicidal behavior (IPTS; Joiner, 2005; Van Orden from it. It should be noted that motives to escape long
terms struggles (e.g., chronic unemployment) are not
synonymous with motives to escape momentary affect-
Received 24 April 2014; accepted 17 June 2014.
Address correspondence to Michael D. Anestis, Department of ive states.
Psychology, University of Southern Mississippi, 118 College Drive, Escape models might provide a more precise theoretical
Box #5025, Hattiesburg, MS 39406. E-mail: Michael.anestis@ framework for understanding non-suicidal self-injury
usm.edu
212 M. D. ANESTIS AND E. A. SELBY

(NSSI), rather than suicidal behavior. Indeed, both indicate the NSSI is more strongly associated with
theoretical and empirical work has emphasized that the suicidal behavior amongst individuals with high distress
most common rationale for engaging in NSSI is to tolerance, rather than low distress tolerance.
immediately escape or at least diminish the intensity of However, these researchers measured distress toler-
aversive states (Linehan & Heard, 1992; Nock & Prinstein, ance via self-report, which is an important consideration
2004; Selby, Anestis, Bender, & Joiner, 2009). as the precise operational definition of distress tolerance
Accordingly, escape motives could represent a potentially depends upon measurement methods. Self-reported dis-
important mechanism differentiating NSSI and suicidal tress tolerance represents the extent to which an individ-
behavior, with individuals motivated primarily by ual is overwhelmed by negative affect and capable of
immediate negative reinforcement opting for NSSI and continuing to function while upset (Simons & Gaher,
individuals motivated to die and capable of pursuing death 2005). Behaviorally indexed distress tolerance, on the
amidst the discomfort opting for suicidal behavior. other hand, represents the extent to which an individual
However, the association between NSSI and suicidal can persist in goal-directed behavior while upset (Nock
behavior is well documented (e.g., Van Orden et al., & Mendes, 2008). The behavioral definition is thus more
2008), indicating that both escape theories and the IPTS consistent with a general construct of persistence, a vital
may be intertwined and relevant to the development of variable to consider in models testing the applicability of
suicidal behavior. One way in which these perspectives escape motives to any behavioral outcome. As such,
may be intertwined is that one motivation has a direct self-report measures of constructs more directly related
influence on suicidal behavior, whereas the other motiv- to persistence may better capture this aspect of the model.
ation has a powerful but indirect influence on suicidal Although consistent with a model in which the ability
behavior. For example, it is posited that NSSI may to pursue death despite discomfort is of great impor-
increase the capacity for enacting lethal self-harm by tance in suicidal behavior, the distress tolerance findings
enabling individuals to habituate to pain and intention- assess a construct that is only tangentially related to per-
ally self-inflicted physical harm. Thus, using NSSI to sistence. Among older adults with prior suicidal beha-
escape from distress may serve as a precursor to suicidal vior, individuals with more lethal attempts persisted
behavior, perhaps through a potential impact on both longer in a delay discounting task, indicating that they
suicidal desire and capacity (Joiner et al., 2005; Van are more willing to persist for a greater duration to
Orden et al., 2010). Nonetheless, if NSSI is primarily obtain a larger reward (Dombrovski et al., 2011).
prompted by escape motives, and suicidal behavior Although such work highlights the importance of per-
requires the ability to suspend the need for immediate sistence, further work considering variables specifically
escape from aversive states in the pursuit of a longer focused on persistence towards goals would be highly
term goal (death), consideration of potential moderating useful and two variables in particular seem to have
variables seems likely to be valuable as a method of potential worth in this regard: grit and perseverance.
refining the interplay between these seemingly conflict- Grit is defined as the ability to strenuously pursue
ing models of suicide risk. long-term goals despite obstacles and adversity (Duck-
Preliminary work along these lines has thus far worth, Peterson, Matthews, & Kelly, 2007) and has been
supported a moderation model. For instance, low dis- linked to greater retention levels in military recruits
tress tolerance is associated with high suicidal desire (Maddi, Matthews, Kelly, Villarreal, & White, 2012),
but low capacity for suicidal behavior (Anestis, Bagge, greater educational attainment (Duckworth et al.,
Tull, & Joiner, 2011) and high distress tolerance 2007), less negative affect (Singh & Jha, 2008), and less
increases the magnitude of the relationship between sui- suicidal ideation (Kleiman, Adams, Kashdan, & Riskind,
cidal behavior and both borderline personality disorder 2013). In contrast, perseverance is defined as a
(Anestis, Gratz, Bagge, & Tull, 2012) and posttraumatic component of impulsivity ([lack of] Perseverance;
stress disorder (Anestis, Tull, Bagge, & Gratz, 2012). Whiteside & Lynam, 2001) assessing the degree to
Building on these findings, Anestis, Knorr, Tull, which an individual exhibits the tendency to quit tasks
Lavender, and Gratz (in press) examined the potential when they become difficult or boring. Difficulties
moderating role of distress tolerance in the relationship in perseverance have been associated with lower
between NSSI and suicide potential. In this study, sui- academic performance (Smith et al., 2007) and greater
cide potential was defined as a continuum extending levels of antisocial behavior (Whiteside & Lynam,
from no suicidal behavior, to suicidal behavior of low 2003). Thus, it is likely that those with elevated grit
lethality, to suicidal behavior of high lethality. In a sam- will also have a greater ability to persevere, and,
ple of 93 adult inpatients receiving treatment for as suggested by the IPTS, both grit and perseverance
substance use disorders, the magnitude of the relation- may contribute to suicidal behavior.
ship between NSSI and suicide potential increased at We hypothesized that the main effects of grit and
higher levels of distress tolerance. Thus, these results perseverance in predicting suicide attempts with clear
GRIT AND PERSEVERENCE IN SUICIDAL BEHAVIOR 213

intent to die would be significant after controlling for Measures


relevant demographics, depressive symptoms, and
Short Grit Scale (Grit-S; Duckworth & Quinn, 2009) is
NSSI. Furthermore, we hypothesized that both grit
a 12-item self-report scale measuring the degree to which
and perseverance would moderate the relationship
individuals tend to persist towards long-term goals (e.g.,
between NSSI and suicidal behavior with clear intent
‘‘I finish what I begin’’ [reversed]). Items are scored on
to die, with the relationship increasing in strength at
a Likert scale ranging from 1 (very much like me) to 5
higher levels of persistence.
(not at all like me), with greater scores indicating a great-
er ability to persist. The alpha coefficient in this sample
was .76. Prior support exists for the validity of the
METHOD
Grit-S in undergraduate samples (Kleiman et al., 2013).
UPPS-P Impulsive Behavior Scale (UPPS-P; Cyders
Participants
et al., 2007; Cyders & Smith, 2007) is a 59-item
Participants were 604 undergraduate students ranging in self-report scale assessing various facets of impulsivity.
age from 18 to 55 years (M ¼ 20.72; SD ¼ 4.15). In total, Items are scored on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (agree
79.5% (n ¼ 480) of the sample were women, 52.0% was strongly) to 4 (disagree strongly), with higher scores
White (n ¼ 314), and 45.5% (n ¼ 275) reported a total indicating greater impulsivity. The UPPS-P includes five
annual family income of $50,000 or less. Additional subscales; however, in this study, only the (lack of)
demographic information can be found in Table 1. All Perseverance subscale was utilized. This subscale
procedures were approved by the Institutional Review consists of 10 items that measure the extent to which
Board, and all participants completed informed consent. an individual tends to quit when activities become diffi-
Data were collected as part of a larger, multisite project cult or boring (e.g., ‘‘I generally like to see things
investigating cohort effects on various aspects of suicide through to the end’’). The alpha coefficient in this sam-
risk. All participants were recruited from undergraduate ple was .84. Prior support exists for the validity of the
psychology courses and they received course credit in UPPS-P in undergraduate samples (Anestis et al., 2011).
return for their participation. The entire protocol was Deliberate Self-Harm Inventory (DSHI; Gratz, 2001)
completed with online questionnaires through a secure is a self-report questionnaire that assesses an indivi-
server. dual’s lifetime history of NSSI. The scale assesses the
frequency and duration of 17 forms of NSSI (e.g., cut-
ting, burning, preventing wounds from healing). A total
TABLE 1 count score representing the number of times an individ-
Demographic Information ual has engaged in any method of NSSI over the course
Demographic N (%) of his or her lifetime was used to index total NSSI fre-
quency. Internal consistency is not typically calculated
Gender for the DSHI, as the measure represents a count fre-
Men 121 (20.0%) quency of discrete behaviors across the broad spectrum
Women 480 (79.5%) of NSSI. Prior support exists for the validity of the
Undeclared 3 (0.5%) DSHI in undergraduate samples (Latimer, Meade, &
Tennant, 2013).
Race=Ethnicity Depression Anxiety Stress Scale–21 (DASS-21;
Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995) is a 21-item self-report
White 314 (52.0%)
African American 256 (42.4%)
measure that assess the presence and severity of
Hispanic=Latino 10 (1.7%) depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms over the
Asian=Pacific Islander 6 (1.0%) course of the previous 7 days. Items are rated on a
Other 17 (2.8%) Likert scale ranging from 0 (did not apply to me at all)
Undeclared 1 (0.2%) to 3 (applied to me very much), with higher scores greater
symptoms. In this study, we used only the depression
Income subscale, which had an alpha coefficient of .88. Prior
$0–$10,000 52 (8.6%)
support exists for the validity of the DASS in under-
$10,001–$25,000 89 (14.7%) graduate samples (Osman et al., 2012).
$25,001–$50,000 134 (22.2%) Measure of Episodic Planning of Suicide (MEPOS;
$50,001–$75,000 136 (22.5%) Anestis, Pennings, & Williams, in press) is a 4-item
$75,001–$100,000 95 (15.7%) self-report scale that assesses frequency and charac-
Greater than $100,000 96 (15.9%)
Undeclared 2 (0.3%)
teristics of prior suicidal behavior. Participants are first
asked whether they have ever intentionally harmed
214 M. D. ANESTIS AND E. A. SELBY

themselves with at least some intent to die (yes–no). objects under skin (n ¼ 22; 13.6%). Amongst these 162,
Participants who endorse a history of at least one such 26 (16.0%) reported a prior suicide attempt with clear
behavior are then asked a series of follow-up questions intent to die.
regarding intent (no intent to die, ambiguous intent Both NSSI and suicide attempts with clear intent to
to die, clear intent to die), method (open ended), and die exhibited significant skew and kurtosis, so we per-
planning for suicidal behavior—for example, 1 (‘‘I had formed a rank transformation using Blom’s formula.
never considered it for even a moment prior to my life’’) For ease of interpretation, nontransformed descriptive
to 6 (‘‘I thought about it continuously or off and on for data are provided for these variables in Table 2; how-
a period of years prior to my attempt’’ [e.g., even if days, ever, all analyses used the transformed variables.
weeks, months, or a year or more passed between Older participants endorsed significantly more grit,
thoughts]). For the purposes of this study, responses r ¼ .14, p ¼ .001. Those with higher incomes endorsed
to the question assessing the frequency of intentional significantly more NSSI, r ¼ .09, p ¼ .03, and less grit,
self-inflicted injury with clear intent to die were used r ¼ .10, p ¼ .02. Greater levels of depression were asso-
to assess lifetime number of suicide attempts. We opted ciated with more pathological levels of NSSI, suicide
to restrict our definition of suicidal behavior to attempts attempts, grit, and perseverance (r’s > .16; p’s < .001).
with clear intent to die to avoid any potential criterion ANOVAs indicated that men reported greater difficulty
contamination with our measure of NSSI. This measure with perseverance, F(1,533) ¼ 4.47, p ¼ .04, l2p ¼ :01.
has previously been used in undergraduate samples ANOVAs also indicated that significant differences in
(Anestis, Pennings, & Williams, in press). NSSI by race=ethnicity F(1,592) ¼ 2.94, p ¼ .02,
l2p ¼ :02, with Hispanic individuals reporting less NSSI
RESULTS than White participants and participants who endorsed
‘‘other’’ as their race=ethnicity. In addition, Asian part-
In all, 37 (6.1%) participants endorsed a history of at icipants reported less NSSI than did participants who
least one prior suicide attempt with clear intent to die. endorsed ‘‘other’’ as their race=ethnicity. Thus, age,
The average number of such attempts was 1.43 income, gender, race=ethnicity, and depressive symp-
(range ¼ 1–4). Of these 37, 26 (70.3%) endorsed a prior toms were all included as covariates.
history of NSSI. The most common method used in Primary analyses examined the main effects of grit
the most recent suicide attempt was intentional overdose and perseverance on suicide attempts with clear intent
(n ¼ 17; 45.9%). to die, controlling for demographic factors, depression,
In all, 162 (26.8%) participants endorsed a history of and NSSI. With respect to grit, the overall model was
at least prior episode of NSSI. The average number was significant, F(7,554) ¼ 12.95, p < .001, and including grit
26.22 prior episodes (range ¼ 1–1,594) and an average of significantly improved the model, DR2 ¼ .01, p ¼ .02,
1.72 methods (range ¼ 1–7). Amongst these 162, the f2 ¼ .01. Similarly, with respect to perseverance, the
most common method was cutting (n ¼ 65; 40%), fol- overall model was significant, F(7,519) ¼ 12.80, p < .001,
lowed by severely scratching self (n ¼ 39; 24.1%), carving and including perseverance significantly improved the
words in skin (n ¼ 29; 17.9%); carving pictures or model (DR2 ¼ .015, p ¼ .002, f2 ¼ .02). These results are
designs in skin (n ¼ 23; 14.2%), and sticking sharp presented in Table 3.

TABLE 2
Descriptive Data and Correlations of Variables

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Gender —
2. Depression .05 —
3. NSSI .05 .31 —
4. Grit .06 .40 .18 —
5. Perseverance .09 .37 .10 .67 —
6. Suicidal Behavior .01 .16 .35 .01 .06 —
M=% women 79.90 8.44 7.10 3.42 19.53 .09
SD — 9.09 78.59 .54 4.79 .40
Minimum — 0.00 0.00 1.42 10.00 0.00
Maximum — 42.00 1594.00 4.92 37.00 4.00

Note: NSSI ¼ number of lifetime episodes of non-suicidal self-injury; Perseverance ¼ (lack of) Perseverance; Suicidal behavior ¼ lifetime number
of suicide attempts with clear intent to die.

p < .05.  p < .01.
GRIT AND PERSEVERENCE IN SUICIDAL BEHAVIOR 215

TABLE 3
Main Effects of Grit and (Lack of) Perseverance Predicting Suicide Attempts With Clear Intent to Die

Variable R2 DR2 t b R2 DR2 t B

.13 .13
Gender .10 .00 Gender .45 .02
Age .42 .02 Age .30 .01
SES 1.93 .08 SES 1.94 .08
Race .82 .03 Race .99 .04
Depression 1.33 .06 Depression 1.35 .06
NSSI 8.14 .34 NSSI 7.77 .34
Grit .14 .01 2.34 .10 Perseverance .15 .02 3.08 .14

Note: SES ¼ annual family income; Race ¼ self-reported race=ethnicity; NSSI ¼ number of lifetime episodes of non-suicidal self-injury;
Perseverance ¼ (lack of) Perseverance.

p < .05.  p < .01.  p < .001.

TABLE 4
Grit and (Lack of) Perseverance as Moderators of the Relationship Between NSSI and Suicide Attempts With Clear Intent to Die

Variable R2 DR2 t b R2 DR2 t B

.03 .03
Gender .37 .02 Gender .80 .04
Age .19 .01 Age .28 .01
SES 1.22 .05 SES 1.18 .05
Race .87 .04 Race 1.03 .05
Depression 3.81 .16 Depression 3.75 .16
.14 .11 .15 .11
Grit 2.34 .10 Perseverance 3.08 .14
NSSI 8.31 .35 NSSI 7.79 .33
.16 .02 .17 .02
Grit  NSSI 3.19 .13 Perseverance  NSSI 4.01 .16

Note: SES ¼ annual family income; Race ¼ self-reported race=ethnicity; NSSI ¼ number of lifetime episodes of non-suicidal self-injury;
Perseverance ¼ (lack of) Perseverance.

p < .05.  p < .01.  p < .001.

Next, we examined the moderating effects of grit and relationship between NSSI and suicide attempts with
perseverance on the relationship between NSSI and clear intent to die increased from high (b ¼ .124,
suicide attempts with clear intent to die, controlling for p ¼ .001) to mean (b ¼ .216, p < .001) to low (b ¼ .307
demographic factors and depression. With respect to p < .001) levels of perseverance. This indicates that, as
grit, the overall model was significant, F(8,553) ¼ 12.79,
p < .001, and adding the Grit  NSSI interaction signifi-
cantly improved the model (DR2 ¼ .015, p ¼ .002,
f2 ¼ .02). Analyses of simple slopes indicated that the
magnitude of the relationship between NSSI and suicide
attempts with clear intent to die increased from low
(b ¼ .169, p < .001) to mean (b ¼ .243, p < .001) to high
(b ¼ .318, p < .001) levels of grit. This indicates that, as
individuals exhibit greater ability to persist toward
long-term goals, the relationship between NSSI and
suicide attempts with clear intent to die becomes stronger.
These results are presented in Table 4 and Figure 1.
With respect to perseverance, the overall model was
significant (F(8,518) ¼ 13.54, p < .001) and adding the
Perseverance  NSSI interaction significantly improved
the model (DR2 ¼ .026, p < .001, f2 ¼ .03). Analyses of FIGURE 1 Grit moderates the relationship between non-suicidal self-
simple slopes indicated that the magnitude of the injury and suicide attempts with clear intent to die.
216 M. D. ANESTIS AND E. A. SELBY

NSSI, and furthermore, frequently engaging in NSSI


may increase one’s ability to tolerate the physical and
emotional distress of bodily harm. Importantly,
although we used NSSI as the linking variable to explain
how escape theories and IPTS can intertwine to predict
worse suicidal behavior, NSSI is not the only behavior
that could function in this manner. Joiner (2005) dis-
cusses multiple avenues through which an acquired
capability for suicidal behavior can be established,
including NSSI, physical fights, injuries from reckless
behavior, and drug use, all of which have been linked
to escaping upsetting emotional experiences (Selby
et al., 2009; Selby & Joiner, in press). Frequently engag-
FIGURE 2 (Lack of) Perseverance moderates the relationship ing in each of these behaviors could result in habituation
between non-suicidal self-injury and suicide attempts with clear intent to pain and fear of death. Thus, there may be many
to die.
routes through which behaviors used to escape emotion-
al distress may result in increased ability to persevere
an individual is better able to persist during tasks that through the distressing aspects of suicidal behavior.
become difficult or boring, the relationship between It should be noted that the motivation to escape an
NSSI and suicide attempts with clear intent to die acute momentary crisis is not synonymous with a motiv-
becomes stronger (lower scores in perseverance indicate ation to escape a broader set of circumstances that take
a greater ability to persist). These results are presented place over an extended period of time. In this sense, an
in Table 4 and Figure 2. individual who decides to engage in suicidal behavior
may simultaneously pursue death while seeking to
escape a long-term struggle. Indeed, the individual’s
DISCUSSION desire to end a lengthy struggle may enhance his or
her ability to persist in the pursuit of death and few
Among this large undergraduate sample in which 37 would argue against the notion that suicidal individuals
students reported previous suicide attempts, greater grit are dissatisfied with life and looking to end their own
and perseverance predicted more frequent suicide struggle. A sudden desire to escape an acute experience
attempts with clear lethal intent. These effects remained of negative affect, however, represents a different
even when accounting for critical covariates. Also, those phenomenon less compatible with approaching a beha-
who reported higher levels of NSSI simultaneously with vior associated with immediate increases in pain and
high grit reported the most frequent suicide attempts fear. The findings of this study indicate that a general
with clear lethal intent. These findings suggest that capacity for persistence may facilitate suicidal behavior,
NSSI, which is a behavior that often occurs in response perhaps by enabling an individual to overcome acute
to motivations to escape aversive emotional states (e.g., fear and pain in pursuit of death.
Nock & Prinstein, 2004), may increase suicidal behavior Present findings highlight problems with some
by facilitating increased persistence in the face of pain common assumptions made by many clinicians and
and death. researchers. For example, if a patient exhibits a desire
These findings highlight the complexity of the to avoid or escape emotional distress that does not
relationships between emotional distress, emotional necessarily mean that he or she is unable to tolerate dis-
avoidance, NSSI, and suicidal behavior. These findings tressing feelings in in all situations. Some patients may
further support Joiner’s (2005) notion that to engage have difficulty experiencing the upsetting feelings that
in a suicide attempt with clear intent to die, one must come along with criticism from others or feelings of fail-
overcome the distress associated with pain and death. ure; some may not experience difficulty with experienc-
Thus, to engage in suicidal behavior, the individual must ing physical pain or embracing the notions of death.
be able to persevere through difficult and frightening Thus, although grit and perseverance are likely positive
emotional experiences. These findings are seemingly at traits for most people in most situations, there may be
odds with escape theories of suicide, wherein the individ- select scenarios in which such positive qualities may
ual engages in suicidal behavior to escape emotional dis- actually confer increased risk toward suicidal behavior.
tress or aversive self-awareness. Yet, in many ways the These suggestions are consistent with recent empirical
IPTS and escape theories complement each other when and theoretical work indicating that the way one
behaviors such as NSSI are factored in. Escape motiva- responds to an emotional situation is highly dependent
tions are highly important for those who engage in on the context of the situation (Aldao, 2013).
GRIT AND PERSEVERENCE IN SUICIDAL BEHAVIOR 217

Although these findings supported our primary Anestis, M. D., Gratz, K. L., Bagge, C. L., & Tull, M. T. (2012). The
hypotheses, there are some limitations to the current study. interactive role of distress tolerance and borderline personality
disorder in suicide attempts among substance users in residential
First, an important limitation to these data is that they treatment. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 53, 1208–1216. doi:10.1016=
were cross sectional, so this does not allow for causal infer- j.comppsych.2012.04.004
ence on the impact of NSSI and persistence on suicidal Anestis, M. D., Knorr, A. N., Tull, M. T., Lavender, J. M., & Gratz,
behavior with clear lethal intent. Many of the components K. L. (in press). The importance of high distress tolerance in the
of both escape theories and the IPTS involve changes over relationship between non-suicidal self-injury and suicide potential.
Suicide and Life-Threatening Behavior.
time, such as the development of increased acquired Anestis, M. D., Tull, M. T., Bagge, C. L., & Gratz, K. L. (2012). The
capability through repeated NSSI, and additional studies moderating role of distress tolerance in the relationship between
are needed to further clarify the interaction between these posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters and suicidal
variables over time. Second, measures involved self-report behavior among trauma-exposed substance users in residential
of traits and experiences. Although our assessments of treatment. Archives of Suicide Research, 16, 198–211. doi:10.1080=
13811118.2012.695269
NSSI and suicidal behavior have been validated and Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychological
frequently used (e.g., Gratz, 2001), they may be limited Review, 97, 90–113. doi:10.1037=0033-295X.97.1.90
by retrospective recall. Similarly, measures of grit and Centers for Disease Control, & Prevention. (2013). Web-based injury
perseverance are self-report, which are highly but not statistics query and reporting system (WISQARS). Retrieved from
perfectly correlated with behavioral indices of these same http://www.cdc.gov/injury/wisqars/index.html
Chatard, A., & Selimbegovic, L. (2011). When self-destructive
traits. Thus, some participants may have reported having thoughts flash through the mind: Failure to meet standards affects
high grit, but others observing them may not recognize the accessibility of suicide-related thoughts. Journal of Personality
them as being particularly high on this trait. Third, and Social Psychology, 100, 587–605. doi:10.1037=a0022461
this study was conducted with undergraduates. Although Cyders, M. A., & Smith, G. T. (2007). Mood-based rash action and its
an undergraduate sample is appropriate for examining components: Positive and negative urgency. Personality and Individ-
ual Differences, 43, 839–850. doi:10.1016=j.paid.2007.02.008
dimension personality traits such as grit and perseverance, Cyders, M. A., Smith, G. T., Spillane, N. S., Fischer, S., Annus, A. M.,
because these participants may have been high functioning & Peterson, C. (2007). Integration of impulsivity and positive mood
the findings may not generalize to more severe clinical sam- to predict risky behavior: Development and validation of a measure
ples. Finally, our NSSI variable in this study was used as of positive urgency. Psychological Assessment, 19, 107–118.
a proxy for escape motivations due to the strong empirical doi:10.1037=1040-3590.19.1.107
Dombrovski, A. Y., Szanto, K. S., Siegle, G. J., Wallace, M. L.,
link between NSSI and desires to escape from emotional Forman, S. D., Sahakian, B., . . . Clark, L. (2011). Lethal
states. However, because we did not directly examine forethought: Delayed reward discounting differentiates high- and
escape motivations during suicide attempts with clear low-lethality suicide attempts in old age. Biological Psychiatry, 70,
lethal intent, escapist motivations cannot necessarily be 138–144. doi:10.1016=j.biopsych.2010.12.025
ruled out as major motivations for suicide attempts with Duckworth, A. L., Peterson, C., Matthews, M. D., & Kelly, D. R.
(2007). Grit: Perseverance and passion for long-term goals. Journal
clear lethal intent in this sample. of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 1087–1101. doi:10.1037=
There are multiple therapeutic implications that can 0022-3514.92.6.1087
be derived from the current study. Measures of grit Duckworth, A. L., & Quinn, P. D. (2009). Development and validation
and persistence may prove beneficial in risk assessments of the Short Grit Scale (Grit-S). Journal of Personality Assessment,
in that they are not face valid tools for determining 91, 166–174. doi:10.1080=00223890802634290
Gratz, K. L. (2001). Measurement of deliberate self-harm: Preliminary
suicide risk. Similarly, using these measures of persist- data on the Deliberate Self-Harm Inventory. Journal of Psychopath-
ence with implicit measures of suicidal ideation, which ology and Behavioral Assessment, 25, 253–263. doi:10.1023=
can uncover propensity toward suicidal behavior even A:1012779403943
when a participant does not admit to suicidal ideation Joiner, T. E. (2005). Why people die by suicide. Cambridge, MA:
(Nock et al., 2010), could help elucidate not only who Harvard University Press.
Kleiman, E. M., Adams, L. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Riskind, J. H.
has attempted to hide suicidal desire, but who may be (2013). Gratitude and grit indirectly reduce risk of suicidal
most capable of acting on those desires. ideations by enhancing meaning in life: Evidence for a mediated
moderation model. Journal of Research in Personality, 47, 539–546.
doi:10.1016=j.jrp.2013.04.007
REFERENCES Latimer, S., Meade, T., & Tennant, A. (2013). Measuring engagement
in deliberate self-harm behaviours: Psychometric evaluation of six
Aldao, A. (2013). The future of emotion regulation research: scales. BMC Psychiatry, 13, 4. doi:10.1186=1471-244X-13-4
Capturing context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 155–172. Linehan, M. M., & Heard, H. L. (1992). Dialectical behavior therapy
doi:10.1177=1745691612459518 for borderline personality disorder. In J. F. Clarkin, E. Marziali, &
Anestis, M. D., Bagge, C. L., Tull, M. T., & Joiner, T. E. (2011). H. Munroe-Blum (Eds.), Borderline personality disorder: Clinical
Clarifying the role of emotion dysregulation in the interpersonal- and empirical perspective (pp. 248–267). New York: Guilford Press.
psychological theory of suicidal behavior in an undergraduate sample. Lovibond, S., & Lovibond, P. (1995). Manual for the Depression
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45, 603–611. doi:10.1016=j. Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney, Australia: The Psychology
jpsychires.2010.10.013 Foundation of Australia.
218 M. D. ANESTIS AND E. A. SELBY

Maddi, S. R., Matthews, M. D., Kelly, D. R., Villarreal, B., & White, personality disorder. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and
M. (2012). The role of hardiness and grit in predicting performance Treatment. doi:10.1037=a0029933
and retention of USMA cadets. Military Psychology, 24, 19–28. Simons, J. R., & Gaher, R. M. (2005). The Distress Tolerance Scale:
doi:10.1080=08995605.2012.639672 Development and validation of a self-report measure. Motivation
Mann, J. J., Waternaux, C., Haas, G. L., & Malone, K. M. (1999). and Emotion, 29, 83–102. doi:10.1007=s11031-005-7955-3
Toward a clinical model of suicidal behavior in psychiatric patients. Singh, K., & Jha, S. D. (2008). Positive and negative affect, and grit as
American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 181–189. predictors of happiness and life satisfaction. Journal of the Indian
Nock, M. K., & Mendes, W. B. (2008). Physiological arousal, distress Academy of Applied Psychology, 34, 40–45.
tolerance, and social problem-solving deficits among adolescent Smith, G. T., Fischer, S., Cyders, M. A., Annus, A. M., Spillane, N. S.,
self-injurers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, & McCarthy, D. M. (2007). On the validity and utility of discrimi-
28–38. doi:10.1037=0022-006X.76.1.28 nating among impulsivity-like traits. Assessment, 14, 155–170.
Nock, M. K., & Prinstein, M. J. (2004). A functional approach to the doi:10.1177=1073191106295527
assessment of self-mutilative behavior. Journal of Consulting and Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Cukrowicz, K. C., Braithwaite, S. R.,
Clinical Psychology, 72, 885–890. doi:10.1037=0022-006X.72.5.885 Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E. (2010). The interpersonal theory of
Nock, M. K., Park, J. M., Finn, C. T., Deliberto, T. L., Dour, H. J., & suicide. Psychological Review, 117, 575–600. doi:10.1037=a0018697
Banaji, M. R. (2010). Measuring the suicidal mind: Implicit Van Orden, K. A., Witte, T. K., Gordon, K. H., Bender, T. W., &
cognition predicts suicidal behavior. Psychological Science, 21, Joiner, T. E. (2008). Suicidal desire and the capability for suicide:
511–517. doi:10.1177=0956797610364762 Tests of the interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal behavior
Osman, A. W., Wong, J., Bagge, C. L., Freedenthal, S., Gutierrez, P. among adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76,
M., & Lozano, G. (2012). The Depression Anxiety Stress Scales–21 72–83. doi:10.1037=0022-006X.76.1.72
(DASS-21): Further examination of dimensions, scale reliability, Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2001). The Five Factor Model and
and correlates. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 68, 1322–1338. impulsivity: Using a structural model of personality to understand
doi:10.1002=jclp.21908 impulsivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 669–689.
Selby, E. A., Anestis, M. D., Bender, T. W., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2009). doi:10.1016=S0191-8869(00)00064-7
An exploration of the emotional cascade model in borderline per- Whiteside, S. P., & Lynam, D. R. (2003). Understanding the role of
sonality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 375–387. impulsivity and externalizing psychopathology in alcohol abuse:
doi:10.1037=a0015711 Application of the UPPS Impulsive Behavior Scale. Experimental
Selby, E. A., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (in press). Emotional cascades as and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 11, 210–217. doi:10.1037=
prospective predictors of dysregulated behaviors in borderline 1949-2715.S.1.69

You might also like